Skip to main content
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences logoLink to Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
. 2019 Dec 16;375(1791):20190305. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0305

Phase synchronization varies systematically with linguistic structure composition

Jonathan R Brennan 1, Andrea E Martin 2,3,
PMCID: PMC6939345  PMID: 31840584

Abstract

Computation in neuronal assemblies is putatively reflected in the excitatory and inhibitory cycles of activation distributed throughout the brain. In speech and language processing, coordination of these cycles resulting in phase synchronization has been argued to reflect the integration of information on different timescales (e.g. segmenting acoustics signals to phonemic and syllabic representations; (Giraud and Poeppel 2012 Nat. Neurosci. 15, 511 (doi:10.1038/nn.3063)). A natural extension of this claim is that phase synchronization functions similarly to support the inference of more abstract higher-level linguistic structures (Martin 2016 Front. Psychol. 7, 120; Martin and Doumas 2017 PLoS Biol. 15, e2000663 (doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.2000663); Martin and Doumas. 2019 Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 29, 77–83 (doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2019.04.008)). Hale et al. (Hale et al. 2018 Finding syntax in human encephalography with beam search. arXiv 1806.04127 (http://arxiv.org/abs/1806.04127)) showed that syntactically driven parsing decisions predict electroencephalography (EEG) responses in the time domain; here we ask whether phase synchronization in the form of either inter-trial phrase coherence or cross-frequency coupling (CFC) between high-frequency (i.e. gamma) bursts and lower-frequency carrier signals (i.e. delta, theta), changes as the linguistic structures of compositional meaning (viz., bracket completions, as denoted by the onset of words that complete phrases) accrue. We use a naturalistic story-listening EEG dataset from Hale et al. to assess the relationship between linguistic structure and phase alignment. We observe increased phase synchronization as a function of phrase counts in the delta, theta, and gamma bands, especially for function words. A more complex pattern emerged for CFC as phrase count changed, possibly related to the lack of a one-to-one mapping between ‘size’ of linguistic structure and frequency band—an assumption that is tacit in recent frameworks. These results emphasize the important role that phase synchronization, desynchronization, and thus, inhibition, play in the construction of compositional meaning by distributed neural networks in the brain.

This article is part of the theme issue ‘Towards mechanistic models of meaning composition’.

Keywords: compositionality, language, electroencephalography, neural oscillations, phase synchronization, naturalistic language processing

1. Introduction

A comprehensive account of linguistic compositionality must face up to difficult questions, namely, how such a powerfully expressive formal property can be achieved in a neurophysiological system like the human brain. Language is the canonical externally measurable human perception-action system that exhibits compositionality [1]. From a theorist's-eye view, a principal challenge is to consider how language comprehension might occur from speech or sign, while both obeying the constraints of neurophysiology and staying faithful to the boundary conditions of language [24]. To meet this challenge, the brain must transform the sensory correlates of speech and sign into structured meaning. Gathering evidence for how the latter might occur is the focus of this paper. Specifically, we test here the hypothesis that the phase of neural oscillations, as measured with electroencephalography (EEG), is systematically related to composition in natural language, operationalized in terms of the number of phrases completed at a particular word.

The neurobiology of language has been concerned not only with the identification of network(s) of brain regions implicated in language processing, but also with inferring the computations that might take place across those networks even while the exact nature of those computations remains ill-defined [5].1 Whatever computations are being carried out by the language network(s), it is likely that the coordination of activity across distributed populations plays a key role in bringing linguistic knowledge to bear on incoming speech- or sign-related sensory processing.2 As such, our focus in this paper is to contribute to an emerging constellation of results that tries to understand how oscillatory patterns that reflect such coordinated neural activity might relate to the hypothesized states of the structured linguistic representations undergoing composition.

Here we examine the relationship between the oscillatory dynamics of induced neural activity and incremental composition, or parsing decisions [812]. While the majority of the literature has focused on the relationship between amplitude (in the evoked signal) or power (in the induced signal) and psycholinguistic manipulations; we turn our attention to the relationship between phase and a linguistic structure annotation that reflects compositionality on a word-by-word basis.

There are compelling reasons to believe that the phase of activation across distributed neural populations, not just amplitude or power, is critical for neural information coding, transmission and generation. Some of this evidence comes from physiology [7,1318], but also some comes from the few formalized implementations of symbol-processing systems in artificial neural networks. See, for example, the notion of phase set in the model Learning and Inference with Schemas and Analogies (LISA; [19,20]) and Discovery of Relations by Analogy (DORA; [3,4,2123]). In the latter model, for example, phase synchronization, desynchronization and yoked inhibitory ‘interneurons’ are mechanistically important for carrying and controlling binding information while also maintaining independence (see also [24]).

One way to explore the role of phase in the construction of higher-level linguistic representations from speech is to look at phase coherence; this measure tracks how consistent the phase of an oscillation is across repeated trials. Tracking this allows us to test whether phase in a particular frequency band becomes more or less consistent for more complex compositional linguistic representations. Another way is to examine cross-frequency coupling (CFC; [25,26]). Measures of CFC track the alignment or synchronization between oscillations at different frequencies. Neuronal assemblies putatively carry and exchange information through phase synchronization and CFC [7,13]. In models of cortical speech processing, for example, CFC links acoustic signals to phonemic and syllabic representations, which occur on different timescales [2729].

We adapt from those models the leading idea that when more representations are inferred, phase between assemblies will need to be more consistent, in terms of synchronization and desynchronization, in order to coordinate across populations. We do not directly address the lively debate of whether ongoing neural entrainment is causal to perception or neural computation (see endnote 2), a debate which is also related to the controversy about whether neural oscillations are the product of ‘true’ oscillators or, instead, reflect punctate local field potentials (LFPs). The literature connecting neural computations at this level of detail to linguistic composition is in its infancy, but we do not know any current tenable models for which linguistic structures3 arise from any kind of true oscillators. Rather, Martin [2] describes a hybrid architecture based on an ongoing speech-envelope-driven oscillation that is transformed or biased in state space by punctate LFPs. These LFPs reflect perceptual inferences of abstract linguistic structures (viz., phonetic features, phonemes, words, morphemes, syntactic structures, compositional units) cued by the oscillator. We note that the details of such a model are underspecified and the data in support of aspects of the theory are only beginning to emerge (e.g. [31,32]).

Against this nascent theoretical background, we address the basic question of how phase relationships at different timescales relate to compositional structure. We quantify the contribution of structure to compositional meaning as a function of the number of phrases that are completed at a given word. We ask how phase synchronization within and between high-frequency (i.e. gamma) bursts and lower-frequency carrier signals (i.e. delta, theta) correlates with compositional structure quantified in this way.

2. Methods

(a). Stimulus and electroencephalography data

We use a publicly available set of 33 EEG datasets collected while adult participants passively listen to an audiobook of the first chapter of Alice in Wonderland [12,33].4 This stimulus is 12.4 min long and comprises 2129 words. Participants listened to the story over in-ear headphones in an enclosed booth at a loudness of 45 dB above their hearing threshold. The EEG data were recorded at 500 Hz using 61 actively amplified electrodes with impedances kept below 25 kOhm referenced to the left mastoid electrode.

The data are processed using the FieldTrip toolbox in Matlab in two stages. In the first stage, artefacts are identified and removed using previously published procedures [33]: The raw EEG data are (i) re-referenced to the average of two mastoid electrodes, (ii) high-pass filtered at 0.1 Hz, and (iii) divided into epochs spanning −300 to 1000 ms around each word onset. Ocular artefacts are isolated and removed using independent component analysis and remaining epochs or channels containing artefacts are removed based on visual inspection (an average of 13.5% of epochs are marked as containing artefacts across participants; see [33] for further details on data pre-processing).

In a second stage of data processing, we extract band-specific power and phase information for each word in the stimulus. First, each participant's raw data is re-loaded from disc, re-referenced to the average of two mastoid electrodes, high-pass filtered at 0.1 Hz, and previously identified ocular independent components are subtracted. Second, data from missing or artefactual electrodes are reconstructed using surface-spline interpolation. Third, the continuous data are band-pass filtered in three bands: delta (1–4 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz) and gamma (30–50 Hz) (4th order, butterworth) and then converted to their respective analytic signal using the Hilbert transformation. Finally, the instantaneous power and phase as measured at the vertex electrode (Cz) is extracted for each band and for each word at the time-point corresponding to that word's onset, excluding epochs marked as artefactual. We focus on word onset because our main focus includes relatively low frequencies (e.g. 1–4 Hz) where temporal resolution is the same order of magnitude as the rate of word presentation (2–4 words s−1).

(b). Compositional annotation

Following previous work, we quantify composition by counting the number of phrases completed by a given word in the story ([9,34]; see also [8,11]). Phrase completion was derived from a bottom-up tree traversal that enumerates mothers before daughters based on the Penn Treebank annotation scheme [35]. As described in Brennan et al. [34], the story-book text was annotated by the Bikel implementation of the Collins parser trained on the Penn Treebank and the resulting phrase-structure annotation was manually reviewed. This annotation yields an estimate for each word in the story of the number of phrases completed by that word. Phase synchronicity must be assessed across sets of epochs. Accordingly, in the last annotation step we create three bins: words that complete a single phrase, words that complete two phrases, and words that complete three or more phrases. For each bin, we derive several phase synchronicity measures as described below.

Table 1 shows the count of words for each of the three phrase bins, divided into content words and function words. Several reasons motivate this division. Function words and content words probably affect online processing in distinct ways. This is evidenced, in part, by differential effects of unexpectedness that have been observed in both the eye-tracking record (e.g. [36]), and in EEG [33]. Separating these two word categories is further motivated in the present study by the different average length between categories. As shown in figure 1, function words average about 0.2 s long in our stimulus; thus a single function word occupies about one theta cycle (4–8 Hz). By contrast, longer content words average about 0.4 s in length, which is about one delta cycle (1–4 Hz). As previous work postulates a systematic link between the size of a linguistic unit and associated oscillatory responses (e.g. [27,28]), we test the prediction that these two word classes may have distinct effects on the theta- and delta-band responses, respectively.

Table 1.

Number of function words and content words within each syntactic phrase bin in the audiobook stimulus.

function words content words
one 694 396
two 272 201
three+ 64 181

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Histograms of word length from the audiobook stimulus, divided into content words and function words. Annotations indicate predominant neural rhythms that have been associated with linguistic representations of different sizes (e.g. [27]).

(c). Phase synchronicity and cross-frequency coupling

We assess phase synchronicity within and between three frequency bands: delta (1–4 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz) and gamma (30–50 Hz).

Within-band phase synchronicity is assessed using inter-trial phase clustering (ITPC a.k.a. phase-locking value or phase coherence). This value quantifies the uniformity of phases across trials. This value is higher when different epochs show similar phases, as is the case, for example, if the phase is reset at each word. If phase synchronicity plays a role in syntactic composition, we expect increased ITPC when words are completing larger numbers of phrases. This follows if phase synchronization mediates the processing of additional compositional structure, as predicted by the perceptual inference account of Martin [2]. ITPC is calculated for a given electrode and time-point following Cohen [37, p. 244]:

ITPC=|n1r=1neiϕfr|,

where eiϕfr provides the polar representation of phase angle ϕ at frequency f for epoch r.

We assess CFC using two commonly applied measures: power–power correlation (P-P) and phase-amplitude coupling (PAC). P-P is simply the Pearson's correlation between power at two frequency bands. We compute this pairwise measure for each combination of frequency bins in our analysis: delta–theta, delta–gamma and theta–gamma. PAC quantifies the degree to which the phase of a lower-frequency oscillation affects the amplitude of a higher-frequency oscillation. We calculate this following Cohen [37, p. 413]:

PAC=|n1r=1nareiϕr|,

where ar is the power at word onset for epoch r at the higher of two frequency bands, and eiϕr is the polar representation of phase for the lower of two frequency bands. Similar to ITPC, if increased composition is mediated by CFC, for example, between delta and gamma bands (cf. [2]), we expect to find higher P-P and/or PAC values at words that complete a larger number of phrases.

These two measures of CFC have been linked with distinct neurobiological mechanisms [25,38,39]. P-P coupling is more likely to be detectable if there is a direct relationship between activation in one cell assembly with another, for example, if power in a network tracking the speech envelope is amplified by power in a network involved in structural inference as the sentence unfolds. PAC, on the other hand, requires that the phase of one network is affected by the power of the other. This latter might hold, for example, when a lower-frequency signal is used to synchronize (perhaps by resetting) a higher-frequency cell assembly.

Several of these measures, especially ITPC and PAC, are sensitive to the number of epochs entered into the calculation. As this value varies across different phrase bins for our naturalistic stimulus (table 1), we normalize each measure to allow for comparison. To do this, we compute a ‘null’ variant of each measure by shifting the phrase bin assigned to each word by 100 epochs. This removes any potential relationship between phrase count and any of the phase measures. We then recompute the target measure within each phrase bin. These offer an estimate of what we would expect of each measure under the null hypothesis, taking into account the different numbers of epochs per phrase bin and word category. Finally, we compute a z-score by subtracting the mean null value from the target values and dividing the result by the standard deviation of the null variant.

We carry out two additional analyses to complement these assessments of phase. First, we quantify power across phrase bins by simply averaging the single-trial power estimates within each frequency band. Second, test the relationship between phrase completion and the evoked signal using single-trial regression in the following way (e.g. [33,4042]). Starting with the same artefact-cleaned epochs, described above, the data are low-pass filtered at 40 Hz and then subject to a linear regression, by participant, testing the scalp voltage at each electrode and time-point (0–1 s after word onset) as a function of the number of completed phrases as well as a set of control variables: sound power at word onset, epoch order and word at the target word, the previous word, and following word frequency (HAL corpus, log transformed). A control regression is also carried out per participant in which the rows of the design matrix are randomly permuted. Single-subject regression coefficients are pooled at the group level using the cluster-based permutation test of Maris & Oostenveld [43]. This test returns clusters of electrodes and time-points where the test coefficient for phrase completions is reliably different than the matched term from the control regression. Following Pallier et al. [8], we conduct this analysis on the count of phrase completions and also the log10 transformation of that count.

(d). Statistical analysis

The z-scored ITPC, P-P, PAC and power values are statistically analysed using Bayesian mixed-effects linear regression, implemented in the STAN programming language using the brms package in R. For each measure, we model the z-scored value as a function of the number of phrases (one, two or three+) frequency band (delta, theta or gamma; or frequency band-pair for CFC), and word category (content, function) and all higher-order interactions between these terms. We include a full by-participants random-effects structure that includes each of these terms and their interactions.

Our research question concerns whether phase relationships change systematically as a function of syntactic composition. But, the direction or magnitude of such changes are underspecified by current theories. We statistically test for any such relationship in two stages. First, we conduct a joint test for a main effect or any higher-order interaction involving the number of phrases. This is done with χ2 tests implemented in the emmeans package. Given the exploratory nature of these tests, we set α = 0.01. To unpack any higher-order interactions, we conduct post-hoc pairwise tests for the effect of phrases within each frequency band and word category. These are evaluated with Bayes factors (BF), calculated with the bayestestR package, that indicate the relative likelihood of the data under the target hypothesis—that there will be difference in phase synchronization—compared to the point null hypothesis that there is no such difference. We focus our attention on differences with a BF > 3 (‘positive’ to ‘strong’ evidence for the target hypothesis; [44]). The estimated effect size and BF value for every pairwise comparison is given in appendix A.

3. Results

(a). Power

We observe little difference in power as a function of syntactic phrases (figure 2a). The statistical analysis suggests a reliable interaction between word category and phrases (χ22=5.96, p = 0.003) and also a three-way interaction between those terms and frequency (χ42=3.98, p = 0.003). But, post-hoc tests for power differences as a function of phrases within each frequency band did not show any pairwise differences with positive or strong support (all BF < 2). Accordingly, we do not see substantial statistical support for a relationship between phrase count and power in these three frequency bands. The full set of pairwise comparisons is given in appendix A, table 2.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

(a) Power at word onset in three frequency bands, divided into function and content words. Points indicate individual averages per participant, and boxes indicate the group mean ± 1 s.e. (b) Inter-trial phase consistency (ITPC) across three frequency bands for function and content words. (c) Circular histograms of single-trial phase in each of three phrase bins (rows) for three frequency bands separated by function and content words (columns). (d–e) CFC as measured by P-P correlations (d) or PAC (e) across three frequency pairs.

Table 2.

Pairwise estimates for differences in power as a function of phrase count along with 95% credibility intervals and Bayes factors.

word category frequency band comparison estimate lower bound upper bound Bayes factor
functional delta one–two −0.038 −0.094 0.021 0.022
one–three+ 0.088 0.001 0.159 0.142
two–three+ 0.126 0.050 0.207 0.798
theta one–two 0.012 −0.045 0.070 0.006
one–three+ 0.031 −0.063 0.124 0.011
two–three+ 0.021 −0.082 0.112 0.006
gamma one–two 0.028 −0.043 0.103 0.009
one–three+ 0.003 −0.116 0.117 0.012
two–three+ −0.025 −0.152 0.096 0.009
lexical delta one–two −0.003 −0.065 0.056 0.006
one–three+ −0.130 −0.205 −0.054 1.467
two–three+ −0.127 −0.203 −0.054 0.972
theta one–two −0.015 −0.077 0.045 0.004
one–three+ −0.073 −0.156 0.010 0.024
two–three+ −0.057 −0.146 0.027 0.009
gamma one–two 0.117 0.036 0.189 0.397
one–three+ 0.023 −0.075 0.134 0.007
two–three+ −0.095 −0.204 0.022 0.020

(b). Inter-trial phase clustering

ITPC increases with more phrases for function words but decreases for content words (figure 2b). This pattern is the strongest in the delta band and is supported by interactions between phrases and word category (χ22=39.3, p < 0.001) and phrases and frequency band (χ42=3.4, p = 0.009). Pairwise comparisons indicate strong support for a difference in ITPC for function words in the delta band (one-versus-three+ phrases, BF = 4513.2; two-versus-three+ phrases, BF = 47.0) and in the theta band (one-versus-three+ phrases, BF = 5836.6; two-versus-three+ phrases, BF = 295.96). Support is also found for differences for content words, where more phrases lead to lower ITPC (delta band, one-versus-two BF = 14.4; one-versus-three+ BF = 72.7; gamma band, one-versus-three+ BF = 10.01). The full set of pairwise comparisons is given in appendix A, table 3.

Table 3.

Pairwise comparisons for differences in ITPC as a function of phrase count along with 95% credibility intervals and Bayes factors. (Bayes factors with values greater than 3 are indicated in italics.)

word category frequency band comparison estimate lower bound upper bound Bayes factor
functional delta one–two −0.371 −0.877 0.129 0.209
one–three+ −1.577 −2.151 −1.056 4513.206
two–three+ −1.205 −1.805 −0.637 47.038
theta one–two −0.258 −0.761 0.295 0.090
one–three+ −1.680 −2.229 −1.102 5836.647
two–three+ −1.422 −2.017 −0.840 295.966
gamma one–two 0.119 −0.421 0.624 0.055
one–three+ −0.549 −1.149 −0.008 0.368
two–three+ −0.671 −1.260 −0.048 0.400
lexical delta one–two 0.987 0.500 1.545 14.448
one–three+ 1.146 0.606 1.736 72.657
two–three+ 0.146 −0.412 0.741 0.040
theta one–two −0.156 −0.744 0.405 0.041
one–three+ 0.307 −0.281 0.866 0.057
two–three+ 0.463 −0.148 1.115 0.076
gamma one–two 0.096 −0.426 0.672 0.038
one–three+ 1.000 0.446 1.595 10.010
two–three+ 0.904 0.296 1.547 1.218

Polar histograms are shown in figure 2c that pool single-trial phase angles from all participants. These appear to show relative uniformity in the distribution of phase angles even for higher numbers of syntactic phrases. This appearance of uniformity seems to be at odds with the differences in ITPC indicated in figure 2b, but this summary at the group level masks individual differences. The electronic supplementary material, figure S1 shows that numerous individual participants show increased phase consistancy for more phrases, but the peak phase differs across participants, which in turn leads to the more uniform distribution of phases at the group level.

(c). Power–power coupling

There is a small trend for increased correlation between power from delta to gamma as the number of syntactic phrases increases for content words (figure 2d). But, this trend is not statistically reliable and there is no evidence for reliable pairwise differences in P-P coupling. The full set of pairwise comparisons is given in appendix A, table 4.

Table 4.

Pairwise comparisons for differences in P-P as a function of phrase count along with 95% credibility intervals and Bayes factors.

word category frequency band comparison estimate lower bound upper bound Bayes factor
functional delta–gamma one–two 0.082 −0.359 0.506 0.077
one–three+ 0.343 −0.098 0.763 0.216
two–three+ 0.265 −0.203 0.687 0.084
delta–theta one–two 0.011 −0.442 0.458 0.045
one–three+ 0.501 0.062 0.969 0.532
two–three+ 0.489 −0.004 0.954 0.227
theta–gamma one–two −0.048 −0.479 0.421 0.044
one–three+ −0.011 −0.825 0.789 0.078
two–three+ 0.035 −0.812 0.803 0.050
lexical delta–gamma one–two −0.606 −1.096 −0.104 0.844
one–three+ −0.698 −1.158 −0.211 2.743
two–three+ −0.096 −0.613 0.391 0.031
delta–theta one–two −0.122 −0.640 0.386 0.033
one–three+ 0.100 −0.354 0.597 0.033
two–three+ 0.229 −0.368 0.756 0.032
theta–gamma one–two 0.213 −0.335 0.777 0.047
one–three+ −0.202 −0.801 0.444 0.046
two–three+ −0.411 −1.114 0.279 0.057

(d). Phase-amplitude coupling

We observe a decrease in PAC at higher phrase counts for content words, as shown on the right-hand side in figure 2e. This decrease contrasts with an increase in PAC for function words with higher phrase counts. These observations are supported by a statistical interaction between phrases and word category (χ22=27.8, p < 0.001) and a three-way interaction between phrases, word category and frequency (χ42=6.4, p < 0.001). Post-hoc pairwise tests for content words strongly support a decrease in PAC as a function of phrase between delta and gamma (one-versus-three + BF = 7355.2; one-versus-two BF = 1702.8). For function words, also between delta and gamma, there is positive support for increased PAC with higher phrase counts (one-versus-three + BF = 62.2); this also obtained between theta and gamma (function words one-versus-three + BF = 46.1; two-versus-three + BF = 23.9). The full set of pairwise comparisons is given in appendix A, table 5.

Table 5.

Pairwise comparisons for differences in PAC as a function of phrase count along with 95% credibility intervals and Bayes factors. (Bayes factors with values greater than 3 are indicated in italics.)

word category frequency band comparison estimate lower bound upper bound Bayes factor
functional delta–gamma one–two −0.426 −0.933 0.069 0.300
one–three+ −1.058 −1.532 −0.482 58.450
two–three+ −0.635 −1.204 −0.066 0.593
delta–theta one–two −0.219 −0.728 0.312 0.073
one–three+ −0.657 −1.149 −0.085 0.802
two–three+ −0.425 −0.969 0.168 0.102
theta–gamma one–two 0.013 −0.505 0.482 0.049
one–three+ −1.049 −1.555 −0.510 42.104
two–three+ −1.053 −1.628 −0.513 22.727
lexical delta–gamma one–two 1.435 0.944 1.968 1703.729
one–three+ 1.462 0.963 2.011 6501.473
two–three+ 0.022 −0.543 0.543 0.033
delta–theta one–two 0.245 −0.293 0.777 0.050
one–three+ 0.578 0.063 1.130 0.336
two–three+ 0.330 −0.219 0.925 0.043
theta–gamma one–two −0.287 −0.894 0.331 0.058
one–three+ 0.461 −0.046 1.032 0.141
two–three+ 0.752 0.113 1.385 0.398

(e). Evoked amplitude

We complement our primary analyses of phase synchronization with an analysis of how the evoked signal changes as a function of phrase count. While we needed to bin the single-trial data for the phase-based analyses above, no such constraint holds for the evoked signal. Accordingly, we compute a regression between phrase counts and the evoked signal across all electrodes and time-points.

Two reliable effects are observed: a late positivity associated with more phrases found primarily on central electrodes (figure 2, top), and an earlier negativity associated with more phrases found primarily on central-anterior electrodes (figure 2, bottom). Aside from slight changes in topography and precise timing, these results are the same for both content words and for function words. A similar pattern is found when phrase count is log transformed, following Pallier et al. [8] (electronic supplementary material, figure S2).

4. Discussion

Using EEG data from a naturalistic story-listening experiment, we explore the relationship between a measure of linguistic composition and phase synchronicity across three bands that have been implicated in sentence-level processing: delta, theta and gamma. Broadly, we see evidence for a systematic relationship between changes in phase synchronicity and CFC that vary as a function of the number of phrases completed by a word. But, the observed patterns neither follow a simple ‘more composition equals more synchronization’ account, nor is there a tight alignment between the ‘size’ of a particular linguistic unit and the frequency bands showing changes in synchronicity (e.g. between content words and slower delta waves). We unpack these observations below.

Evidence for increased phase synchronicity with increased composition was observed for function words in several measures: ITPC in the delta band (with similar trends in theta and gamma), and also between delta and gamma bands as measured with PAC. But, both ITPC and PAC showed decreases in the same bands when considering content words. These patterns are not consistent with a simple word-by-word mapping between phrase completion and phase synchronization. Concerning content words, there is a wealth of psycholinguistic data which suggests that different mechanisms might be engaged at the end of large phrases and sentences than during other moments in structure building and sentence processing (e.g. [4547]), and that these differences affect event-related brain potentials (e.g. [48]). Some of these theories link phrase boundaries with increased discourse-level integration, which may be producing the difference we observed here because content words that complete three or more phrases include a number of clause and sentence final items. A decrease in ITPC at sentence boundaries could also be consistent with the notion that activity builds up as a phrase or sentence is structured but does not persist in the same way across phrase and sentence boundaries, as reported for power in the gamma band in Nelson et al. [11]. More broadly, various sentence-related phenomena occur more often at clause boundaries. These include syntactic and semantic operations associated with identifying and interpreting phrases, but also other interpretive processes like the resolution of referential dependencies. Further, our annotations do not allow us to distinguish the perceptual cues that might correlate with these syntactic and semantic properties, such as acceleration or deceleration of speech rate and segment lengthening [49,50].

The effects for phase synchronization indicated by changes in ITPC and PAC do not pattern, in a simple way, with the evoked response for phrase completion. Our control evoked analysis identified two distinct components that correlated with phrases: an early anterior negativity and a later central positivity. These components were observed on both content words and function words (figure 3), and were also qualitatively similar when treating phrase counts on a linear or log scale (see the electronic supplementary material, figure S2). Both of these evoked components align well with prior literature on event-related potentials associated with syntactic processing (e.g. the LAN, or the P600; see [51] for an overview). These complexities point towards a need in future work to develop annotations that tease apart these distinct phenomena.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Regression event-related potential plots of spatio-temporal clusters that show a reliable linear effect of brackets on evoked EEG data from (a) content words or (b) function words. Two statistically reliable clusters are observed in both sets of data. In all panels, black traces and grey shading indicate the grand-averaged regression coefficient (±CI95%) from electrodes contributing to that effect; insets show those electrodes along with a topographic plot of activity averaged across the cluster's time-window (dark-grey shading). The top plots for both content and function words show a reliable cluster of positive-going activity over central posterior-electrodes starting about 700–800 ms after word onset (p = 0.009, p = 0.001, respectively); the bottom plot shows a reliable negative-going cluster over anterior electrodes starting around 200 ms (p = 0.013, p = 0.025, respectively). (Online version in colour.)

One question that follows from the literature is whether the mechanism by which the pattern of ITPC arises is phase resetting as linguistic structure is being inferred or built [52]. Our answer from the present analysis is: not obviously. In figure 2c, we see a uniformity across bands that does not provide evidence for coordination around any one phase angle. It is still logically possible that phase resetting is occurring, but our results indicate that if it is, then participants must unsystematically vary in the phase angle expressing the change that the ITPC results reflect. Indeed, such individual differences in preferred phase is at least tentatively supported by per-participant phase-angle distributions shown in the electronic supplementary material, figure S1. No account of sentence processing makes any statements about individual variation in neural oscillations. However, the finding that that phase consistency across trials within participants does not extend to consistency across individuals is largely in line with an analysis-by-synthesis view. In an architecture like that of Martin [2], there may be differences across individuals in the time course of perceptual inference or the invocation of a higher-level structure via the LFP. Such an account aligns with observations linking, for example, behavioural outcomes with differences in the peak alpha rhythm between individuals (e.g. [53]). Further, measurable differences between individuals could be driven by differences in brain morphology alone.

Another question from the literature concerns links between the size of linguistic units (phonemes, syllables, phrases) and the oscillatory responses that are associated with cognitive operations over those units [2729]. There are at least two ways such a link, if it were to extend to higher levels of syntactic composition, might be born out in our analysis. First, we might expect to find that function words, which are shorter and thus occur primarily on the same timescale as theta oscillations, might associate primarily with theta activity. Likewise, content words, which occur more on the same timescale as delta oscillations, might thus associate primarily with delta activity. We do not see evidence for any such links between word category and specific frequency bands: both function words and content words show the most statistically reliable effects in the delta band, with trends observed in all three bands that we investigate here.

A related prediction that we examine is whether words that complete larger phrases are associated with activity at lower, longer timescale oscillations. This prediction comes from observations like those of Nelson et al. [11], already discussed above, where the build-up of high-gamma band activity is observed within phrases but not between phrases of different lengths. Our data are consistent with such a hypothesis in a limited way: there is indeed increased low-frequency delta phase synchronicity at function words that complete a larger number of phrases (see the left-hand side of figure 2b). However, our results again do not paint a simple picture: there is also increased phase synchronicity in the theta band for such words; and, more strikingly, increased coupling between the gamma and delta bands as a function of phrase count (figure 2e). This pattern of increased synchronicity does not align with a simplistic mapping between the size of a linguistic unit and oscillatory neural responses associated with that unit.

Another way to frame these observations is that the problem the brain faces when it must create linguistic structure from speech or sign is much more complex than the discrete frequency band analyses that has sometimes been assumed in the field (and, indeed, adopted in this study). Syllables might occur at a timescale that corresponds to the theta-rate, but syllables can be morphemes or words, and can cue complex relations in information and discourse structure; words certainly span theta and delta (e.g. in our dataset, the correlation between delta for content words and theta for function words), as do phrases. At some times, a phrase is a single word lasting a few hundred milliseconds, and at other times, it is tens of words, and lasts a few seconds. At the extremes, entire clauses may be elided with silence. The take-away message is that ‘size’ or length in time, and ergo frequency, are not directly predictive of linguistic structure and content. Consequently, it may not be fruitful to probe for functional data patterns which assume that fixed, structure-related computations can be associated with a given band or coupling pattern.

We are far from the first to point out that we have only a limited understanding of linking hypothesis between our measurements and the compositional processes we are trying to do inference about (e.g. [5,54]). Recognizing this challenge, we attempt to meet a low standard of rhetoric: we assert that, minimally, phase synchronization reflects some flow of information between distributed populations functioning as neuronal assemblies. This minimalistic hypothesis, from a cue-integration and perceptual inference perspective [2], is that as more linguistic representations and structures are inferred or transformed from sensory signals, the more synchronization (at certain time points) and desynchronization (at other time points) occurs. The ITPC and PAC patterns we observe are consistent with the synchronization of excitatory and inhibitory cycles across assemblies, synchronization which increases with the unfolding structure and concomitant accrual of compositional meaning. Larger and more distributed networks oscillate at lower frequencies [17], this is consistent with an interpretation where the formation of higher-level linguistic structures as indicated by phrase count draws upon distributed networks operating in phase. Speculatively, phase synchronization may reflect the transfer of information between ‘reader-integrator’ ensembles [13] that detect or infer [2,13] linguistic structure and meaning.

5. Conclusion

Our results indicate that neural oscillations show changes in phase synchronization within and between frequency bands in response to linguistic compositional demands. These results highlight the importance of neuronal synchronization and desynchronization to levels of linguistic structure. Current theoretical models hold that desynchronization and inhibition are key in the function of computational models that learn and represent a predicate calculus, (see DORA; [3,4,21,22]), and for maintaining variable-value independence (see [24] for discussion). In this way, our results suggest the importance of inhibition and cortical interneurons in controlling the flow of information in across assemblies to derive compositional structure and meaning.

Supplementary Material

Supplemental anayses & results
rstb20190305supp1.pdf (847.5KB, pdf)

Supplementary Material

Phrase annotations
rstb20190305supp2.csv (92.7KB, csv)

Supplementary Material

Single-trial derived measures aligned with phrase annotations
rstb20190305supp3.zip (3.8MB, zip)

Appendix A

See tables 25.

Endnotes

1

However see Martin [2] for an argument that they must be based on, composed of, or at least bounded by, summation and divisive normalization, two operations neurons have been shown to do [6].

2

Our study does not speak directly to the debate about whether neural oscillations are causal or epiphenomenal to neural computation. However, implicit in the theoretical background for this study (e.g. [2,4]) is the idea that cycles of excitation and inhibition/refraction are what carry out the computations in question [7].

3

Which, interestingly, further could be said to descend to below phonemic and phonetic levels given recent results suggesting that auditory cortex might represent a generative model of articulator dynamics for constructing linguistic representations [30].

4

Stimuli and data are available for download at: https://dx.doi.org/10.7302/Z29C6VNH.

Data accessibility

The raw data we analyse are openly available at http://dx.doi.org/10.7302/Z29C6VNH. We have provided the specific annotations used in this analysis and also the derived quantities (phase, power) as electronic supplementary material.

Authors' contributions

A.E.M. developed the research question, and contributed to the data analysis plan and manuscript writing. J.R.B. developed and implemented the data analysis and contributed to the manuscript writing.

Competing interests

We declare we have no competing interests.

Funding

This study was funded, in part, by grant no. 1607251 from the National Science Foundation of the United States to J.R.B. A.E.M. was supported by the Max Planck Research Group ‘Language and Computation in Neural Systems’ and by the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (grant 016.Vidi.188.029).

References

  • 1.Partee B. 1975. Montague grammar and transformational grammar. Linguistic Inquiry 6, 203–300. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Martin AE. 2016. Language processing as cue integration: grounding the psychology of language in perception and neurophysiology. Front. Psychol. 7, 120 ( 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00120) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Martin AE, Doumas LA. 2017. A mechanism for the cortical computation of hierarchical linguistic structure. PLoS Biol. 15, e2000663 ( 10.1371/journal.pbio.2000663) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Martin AE, Doumas LA. 2019. Predicate learning in neural systems: using oscillations to discover latent structure. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 29, 77–83. ( 10.1016/j.cobeha.2019.04.008) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Poeppel D. 2012. The maps problem and the mapping problem: two challenges for a cognitive neuroscience of speech and language. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 29, 34–55. ( 10.1080/02643294.2012.710600) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Carandini M, Heeger DJ. 2012. Normalization as a canonical neural computation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 51–62. ( 10.1038/nrn3136) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Buzsáki G. 2006. Rhythms of the brain. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Pallier C, Devauchelle A-D, Dehaene S. 2011. Cortical representation of the constituent structure of sentences. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 2522–2527. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Brennan J, Nir Y, Hasson U, Malach R, Heeger DJ, Pylkkänen L. 2012. Syntactic structure building in the anterior temporal lobe during natural story listening. Brain Lang. 120, 163–173. ( 10.1016/j.bandl.2010.04.002) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Brennan JR, Pylkkänen L. 2017. MEG evidence for incremental sentence composition in the anterior temporal lobe. Cogn. Sci. 41, 1515–1531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Nelson MJ, et al. 2017. Neurophysiological dynamics of phrase-structure building during sentence processing. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114, E3669–E3678. ( 10.1073/pnas.1701590114) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Hale J, Dyer C, Kuncoro A, Brennan JR.2018. Finding syntax in human encephalography with beam search. arXiv 1806.04127. (http://arxiv.org/abs/1806.04127. )
  • 13.Buzsáki G. 2010. Neural syntax: cell assemblies, synapsembles, and readers. Neuron 68, 362–385. ( 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.023) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Fries P. 2015. Rhythms for cognition: communication through coherence. Neuron 88, 220–235. ( 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.034) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Gray CM, König P, Engel AK, Singer W. 1989. Oscillatory responses in cat visual cortex exhibit inter-columnar synchronization which reflects global stimulus properties. Nature 338, 334–337. ( 10.1038/338334a0) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Klimesch W, Sauseng P, Hanslmayr S, Gruber W, Freunberger R. 2007. Event-related phase reorganization may explain evoked neural dynamics. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 31, 1003–1016. ( 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.03.005) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Sauseng P, Klimesch W. 2008. What does phase information of oscillatory brain activity tell us about cognitive processes? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 1001–1013. ( 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.03.014) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.von der Malsburg C, Buhmann J. 1992. Sensory segmentation with coupled neural oscillators. Biol. Cybern. 67, 233–242. ( 10.1007/BF00204396) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hummel JE, Holyoak KJ. 1997. Distributed representations of structure: a theory of analogical access and mapping. Psychol. Rev. 104, 427–466. ( 10.1037/0033-295X.104.3.427) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Hummel JE, Holyoak KJ. 2003. A symbolic-connectionist theory of relational inference and generalization. Psychol. Rev. 110, 220-–264. ( 10.1037/0033-295X.110.2.220) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Doumas LA, Hummel JE, Sandhofer CM. 2008. A theory of the discovery and predication of relational concepts. Psychol. Rev. 115, 1–43. ( 10.1037/0033-295X.115.1.1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Doumas LA, Martin AE. 2018. Learning structured representations from experience. Psychol. Learn. Motiv. 69, 165–203. ( 10.1016/bs.plm.2018.10.002) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Hummel JE. 2011. Getting symbols out of a neural architecture. Connect. Sci. 23, 109–118. ( 10.1080/09540091.2011.569880) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Martin AE, Doumas LAA. 2019. Tensors and compositionality in neural systems. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 375, 20190306 ( 10.1098/rstb.2019.0306) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Canolty RT, Knight RT. 2010. The functional role of cross-frequency coupling. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 506–515. ( 10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.001) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Lisman JE, Jensen O. 2013. The theta-gamma neural code. Neuron 77, 1002–1016. ( 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.03.007) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Arnal LH, Poeppel D, Giraud AL. 2015. Temporal coding in the auditory cortex. In Handbook of clinical neurology, vol. 129 (eds Celesia GG, Hickok G), pp. 85–98. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Ding N, Melloni L, Zhang H, Tian X, Poeppel D. 2016. Cortical tracking of hierarchical linguistic structures in connected speech. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 158–164. ( 10.1038/nn.4186) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Giraud AL, Poeppel D. 2012. Cortical oscillations and speech processing: emerging computational principles and operations. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 511–517. ( 10.1038/nn.3063) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Anumanchipalli GK, Chartier J, Chang EF. 2019. Speech synthesis from neural decoding of spoken sentences. Nature 568, 493–498. ( 10.1038/s41586-019-1119-1) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Kaufeld G, Ravenschlag A, Meyer AS, Martin AE, Bosker HR. In press. Knowledge-based and signal-based cues are weighted flexibly during spoken language comprehension. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. ( 10.1037/xlm0000744) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Martin AE. 2018. Cue integration during sentence comprehension: electrophysiological evidence from ellipsis. PLoS ONE 13, e0206616 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0206616) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Brennan JR, Hale JT. 2019. Hierarchial structure guides rapid linguistic predictions during naturalistic listening. PLoS ONE 14, e0207741. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Brennan JR, Stabler EP, Van Wagenen SE, Luh W-M, Hale JT. 2016. Abstract linguistic structure correlates with temporal activity during naturalistic comprehension. Brain Lang. 157-158, 81–94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Marcus M, Kim G, Marcinkiewicz M, MacIntyre R, Bies A Ferguson M, Katz K, Schasberger B. 1994. The Penn Treebank: annotating predicate argument structure. ARPA human language technology workshop, pp. 114–119. Philadelphia, PA: Department of Computer and Information Science, University of Pennsylvania. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Roark B, Bachrach A, Cardenas C, Pallier C. 2009. Deriving lexical and syntactic expectation-based measures for psycholinguistic modeling via incremental top-down parsing. In Proc. of the Conf. on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing (EMNLP), Singapore, pp. 324–333. Stroudsberg, PA: Association for Computational Linguistics. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Cohen MX. 2014. Analyzing neural time series data. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Helfrich RF, Herrmann CS, Engel AK, Schneider TR. 2016. Different coupling modes mediate cortical cross-frequency interactions. Neuroimage 140, 76–82. ( 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.035) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Watrous AJ, Fell J, Ekstrom AD, Axmacher N. 2015. More than spikes: common oscillatory mechanisms for content specific neural representations during perception and memory. Curr. Opin Neurobiol. 31, 33–39. ( 10.1016/j.conb.2014.07.024) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Hauk O, Davis MH, Ford M, Pulvermuller F, Marslen-Wilson WD. 2006. The time course of visual word recognition as revealed by linear regression analysis of ERP data. Neurolm-age 30, 1383–1400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Solomyak O, Marantz A. 2009. Lexical access in early stages of visual word processing: a single-trial correlational MEG study of heteronym recognition. Brain Lang. 108, 191–196. ( 10.1016/j.bandl.2008.09.004) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Lewis G, Poeppel D. 2014. The role of visual representations during the lexical access of spoken words. Brain Lang. 134, 1–10. ( 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.03.008) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Maris E, Oostenveld R. 2007. Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG- data. J. Neurosci. Meth. 164, 177–190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Kass RE, Raftery AE. 1995. Bayes factors. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 90, 773–795. ( 10.1080/01621459.1995.10476572) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Hirotani M, Frazier L, Rayner K. 2006. Punctuation and intonation effects on clause and sentence wrap-up: evidence from eye movements. J. Mem. Lang. 54, 425–443. ( 10.1016/j.jml.2005.12.001) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Rayner K, Kambe G, Duffy SA. 2000. The effect of clause wrap-up on eye movements during reading. Quart. J. Exp. Psychol. Sect. A 53, 1061–1080. ( 10.1080/713755934) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Stowe LA, Kaan E, Sabourin L, Taylor RC. 2018. The sentence wrap-up dogma. Cognition 176, 232–247. ( 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.03.011) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Hagoort P. 2003. Interplay between syntax and semantics during sentence comprehension: ERP effects of combining syntactic and semantic violations. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 883–899. ( 10.1162/089892903322370807) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Turk AE, Shattuck-Hufnagel S. 2007. Multiple targets of phrase-final lengthening in American English words. J. Phonet. 35, 445–472. ( 10.1016/j.wocn.2006.12.001) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Wightman CW, Shattuck-Hufnagel S, Ostendorf M, Price PJ. 1992. Segmental durations in the vicinity of prosodic phrase boundaries. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 91, 1707–1717. ( 10.1121/1.402450) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Swaab TY, Ledoux K, Camblin CC, Boudewyn MA. 2012. Language-related ERP components. In The Oxford handbook of event-related potential components (eds Luck SJ, Kappenman ES), pp. 397–439. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Meyer L. 2018. The neural oscillations of speech processing and language comprehension: state of the art and emerging mechanisms. Eur. J. Neurosci. 48, 2609–2621. ( 10.1111/ejn.13748) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Gulbinaite R, van Viegen T, Wieling M, Cohen MX, VanRullen R. 2017. Individual alpha peak frequency predicts 10 Hz flicker effects on selective attention. J. Neurosci. 37, 10 173–10 184. ( 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1163-17.2017) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Krakauer JW, Ghazanfar AA, Gomez-Marin A, MacIver MA, Poeppel D. 2017. Neuroscience needs behavior: correcting a reductionist bias. Neuron 93, 480–490. ( 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.12.041) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplemental anayses & results
rstb20190305supp1.pdf (847.5KB, pdf)
Phrase annotations
rstb20190305supp2.csv (92.7KB, csv)
Single-trial derived measures aligned with phrase annotations
rstb20190305supp3.zip (3.8MB, zip)

Data Availability Statement

The raw data we analyse are openly available at http://dx.doi.org/10.7302/Z29C6VNH. We have provided the specific annotations used in this analysis and also the derived quantities (phase, power) as electronic supplementary material.


Articles from Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences are provided here courtesy of The Royal Society

RESOURCES