Abstract
Objectives:
Higher socioeconomic status (SES) individuals report more social activities than their lower SES counterparts. Yet, SES and racial health disparities are often confounded. Here, we tested whether the frequency of engagement in social activities contributed to the association between SES and daily cortisol secretion among urban African American older adults.
Methods:
Ninety-two community-dwelling African Americans aged 55 years and older reported what they were doing at regular intervals across the day on an Android smartphone for seven consecutive days. They also provided four saliva samples at four time points a day during the same period.
Results:
Higher SES older adults engaged in proportionally more social activities than their lower SES counterparts. A greater relative frequency of weekly social activities was associated with a steeper diurnal cortisol decline. Higher SES was indirectly linked to a steeper cortisol decline via increased relative frequency of weekly social activities.
Discussion:
Our findings suggest that engagement in weekly social activities represents a behavioral intermediary for SES health disparities in endocrine function among older urban African American adults.
Keywords: ecologically momentary assessment (EMA), social participation, minority aging, dysregulation
The socioeconomic gradient—the linear and positive relationship between socioeconomic status (SES) and health—is one of the most robust phenomena in epidemiology (Adler et al., 1994), yet one of the least understood (Sapolsky, 2005). Part of this challenge is identifying the complex mechanisms responsible for this association (Matthews & Gallo, 2011). SES effects are evident in health-related biological processes (Cohen, Doyle, & Baum, 2006; Zilioli, Imami, & Slatcher, 2017). Mechanistically, brain areas involved in the appraisal of environmental threats and challenges project to the hypothalamus, which initiates a hormonal cascade along the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis that ultimately results in cortisol secretion. Cortisol plays a crucial role in regulating many homeostatic systems in the body (e.g., cardiovascular, respiratory, immune; Sapolsky, Romero, & Munck, 2000). For this reason, dysregulation in the diurnal pattern of cortisol release compromises the proper functioning of various homeostatic systems, increasing morbidity and mortality over the long term (for a review, see Adam et al., 2017). Stressors involved in dysregulation are arguably more prevalent and acute for low-SES groups (Matthews & Gallo, 2011).
Cortisol secretion follows a diurnal rhythm characterized by high morning levels, an acute increase approximately 30 min after awakening (known as the cortisol awakening response [CAR]), and a steady decline throughout the day, referred to as the cortisol slope or cortisol decline (Tsigos & Chrousos, 2002). These parameters can be reliably estimated by assessing cortisol concentrations in saliva collected at several time points throughout the day, for several consecutive days. Among these parameters, cortisol decline has been found to be the most robust predictor of psychological and health outcomes (Adam et al., 2017; Saxbe, 2008), while the literature has been found to be largely inconsistent with regard to CAR associations with psychological well-being and physical health (Chida & Steptoe, 2009). Current evidence indicates that low-SES individuals experience more abnormalities (e.g., flattened cortisol declines) in their diurnal cortisol decline (Cohen et al., 2006; Zilioli, et al., 2017). Relatively little is known about how SES leads to cortisol dysregulation in older adults, especially in minority groups. Identifying protective factors that build resilience, potentially ameliorate the insidious effects of SES, and reduce known health disparities is a public health priority in the United States and would assist efforts to enhance healthy aging.
Recent scientific efforts have focused on psychosocial and behavioral pathways to better understand the socioeconomic gradient in health-related biological processes, such as immune function impairment (Chiang et al., 2015; Needham et al., 2013), elevated allostatic load (McCurley et al., 2017), and dysregulation of stress-responsive neuroendocrine axes (Cohen et al., 2006; Janicki-Deverts et al., 2007). Most of these studies have focused on health-related behaviors, such as smoking, exercise, and diet (Cohen et al., 2006; Needham et al., 2013), but they have largely neglected social participation despite clear evidence linking it to physical health (Chiao et al., 2013; Sundquist, Lindström, Malmström, Johansson, & Sundquist, 2004) and psychological well-being (Ellaway & Macintyre, 2007; Hong, Hasche, & Bowland, 2009).
The literature on social participation can be framed in the broader literature on social integration. Theories on social integration find their progenitors in the work of Durkheim and Faris who emphasized the role of poor social integration in the etiology of suicide (Durkheim, 1897/1951) and mental illness (Faris, 1934). More recent and detailed theories on the relationship between social integration and health have been developed by Thoits (1983) and Cohen (1988), who emphasized the behavioral and psychological mechanisms through which social integration exerts its beneficial effects on health. These theories also recognize that social integration is a multifaceted construct, including both behavioral and cognitive components. Social participation—the extent and frequency to which people engage in social activities—has been proposed as one of the multiple components of social integration (Brissette, Cohen, & Seeman, 2000). Among older adults, higher levels of social participation have been linked to increased physical activity (Legh-Jones & Moore, 2012), fewer depressive symptoms (Chiao, Weng, & Botticello, 2011; Roh et al., 2015), lower functional disability (Kanamori et al., 2014), better dental health (Takeuchi, Aida, Kondo, & Osaka, 2013), better self-reported health (Sirven & Debrand, 2008; Veenstra, 2000), and survival (Glass, De Leon, Marottoli, & Berkman, 1999).
More engagement in social activities might be particularly beneficial for minority older adults living in urban areas who are at greater risk of social isolation (Aneshensel et al., 2007; Nicholson, 2012). Although previous work has found a link between a person’s SES and other components of social integration (e.g., social network diversity, Kahn, 1994; Krause & Borawski-Clark, 1995; social network size, Ajrouch, Blandon, & Antonucci, 2005, but see Kubzansky, Berkman, Glass, & Seeman, 1998), very few studies have linked SES to social participation (e.g., Herzog, Franks, Markus, & Holmberg, 1998), especially in the form of what people do—their daily activities that foster interaction and social integration. Furthermore, most of the existing studies have relied on global, retrospective self-reported measures of social participation (e.g., Glass et al., 1999; Kanamori et al., 2014; Veenstra, 2000). Using ecological momentary assessment (EMA) represents an innovative way to measure real-time participation and its link to SES and health. Finally, no work, to our knowledge, has examined how social participation relates to diurnal cortisol fluctuations in older adults (for similar work among younger adults, see Stetler, Dickerson, & Miller, 2004) and whether it acts as a mediator of the expected association between SES and diurnal cortisol. Studying the dynamics between SES, social participation, and physiological stress might be particularly relevant for urban African American older adults, who, compared with other racial groups, tend to experience higher rates of physical disability (Dunlop, Song, Manheim, Daviglus, & Chang, 2007), more financial burdens (Williams, Mohammed, Leavell, & Collins, 2010), and are more likely to dwell in disadvantaged neighborhoods (Williams et al., 2010) that might deter or impede social contacts (Ross & Jang, 2000).
To summarize, we used data from a sample of older urban-dwelling African Americans to test whether (a) higher SES participants report higher rates of daily social activities, (b) higher rates of social activities are associated with a more normative (i.e., steeper) cortisol daily decline, and (c) higher rates of social activities mediate the expected association between higher SES and steeper cortisol declines. In addition to these hypotheses, we also tested whether other diurnal cortisol parameters (i.e., morning cortisol and CAR) are associated with social activities. Our study focuses exclusively on African Americans. Doing so allows us to focus on the variability in the psychosocial and health variables within this population, which might go underexamined in multiracial studies. Moreover, because certain psychosocial stressors, such as socioeconomic disadvantage, are incommensurate across races (Kaufman, Cooper, & McGee, 1997), a single-race study might help avoid problems related to residual confounding. This work contributes to the growing literature on the psychosocial and behavioral pathways to health and resilience, particularly among higher risk older minority populations.
Method
Participants
The current study included 100 adults aged 55 and older (M = 69.83, SD = 8.73, 79.4% women) in Detroit who were capable of participating in activities without assistance from another person. Furthermore, to be eligible, participants did not need to have Cushing’s disease, Addison’s disease, or have used oral steroids in the past 2 weeks. Participants were recruited from a volunteer registry of approximately 1,400 African Americans aged 55 and older willing to participate in research of interest to them. From the registry, we drew a stratified random sample based on a census tract index of neighborhood quality (Table 1). The response rate for our random sample was 21%, meaning that among the 323 people who were initially contacted by phone, 69 ultimately participated in the study. We augmented the random sample with snowball sampling. The latter approach contributed 31% of the full sample. Eight people did not provide saliva samples and were therefore excluded from the analyses. The analytic sample consisted of 92 participants self-identifying as African Americans. We conducted a series of bivariate analyses, chi-squares for categorical and t tests for continuous measures, to test for differences in demographic characteristics between participants who provided cortisol data and who did not. Our results showed that the two groups did not differ in any of the demographics (i.e., gender, age, living status, income, education, and employment status; lowest p value = .651). The Wayne State University Institutional Review Board approved all study procedures.
Table 1.
Neighborhood quality clusters |
Tracts | 2010 Population |
Cluster proportion |
Sample | Excluded participants |
Analytic sample |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
First (best) | 44 | 112,690 | 15.8 | 19 | 1 | 18 |
Second | 79 | 228,512 | 32 | 43 | 3 | 40 |
Third | 80 | 198,342 | 27.8 | 20 | 1 | 19 |
Fourth | 54 | 115,823 | 16.2 | 12 | 2 | 10 |
Fifth (worst) | 40 | 58,410 | 8.2 | 6 | 1 | 5 |
Total | 297 | 713,777 | 100 | 100 | 8 | 92 |
Procedure
We used a smartphone-based EMA protocol as part of a multi-method data collection approach that included psychosocial instruments and saliva samples 4 times per day for seven consecutive days. The EMA data collection protocol has been described previously (Fritz, Tarraf, Saleh, & Cutchin, 2017). The average number of days EMA data were collected was 6.62 (SD = 1.25), with only one participant out of 92 not providing EMA data. Briefly, participants were asked to provide data about their activities and other dimensions of daily experience through the use of movisensXS, a smartphone-based EMA application. After training, all participants were lent a smartphone loaded with the application to use during the data collection period. Participants were asked to carry the phone with them during their waking hours. The EMA application was designed to sound an alarm 4 times per day (9 a.m., 1 p.m., 5 p.m., and 9 p.m.) to alert participants that it was time to complete the EMA question sets. At each alarm, participants were asked to respond to questions about the present. With the exception of the 9 a.m. alarm, participants were then also immediately asked corresponding questions about what they had been doing 2 hr prior to the alarm. By combining “current time” questions with “2 hr prior” questions, we were able to collect information specific to seven equally spaced time points across the day.
Measures
Social activities.
To measure this dimension of social participation, the EMA protocol included a question that asked participants to describe what they were doing at the referent time using text (or voice converted to text). Participants’ descriptions of their activities were brief, ranging in length from a word or two to one sentence. We used the International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health (ICF; World Health Organization, 2001) to code all activity responses into discrete nonoverlapping categories (Table 2). We coded participants’ responses using the most fine-grained coding provided in the ICF, which has four levels of codes and subcodes. Because there is no universally accepted criterion for coding self-reports of activity as either “social” or “individual,” we categorized each chapter within the ICF as being “social” or “individual” activity based on the likelihood that it represented activities that involved social interaction and therefore the type of doing in everyday life that denotes social participation. For example, Chapter 9 of the ICF includes “actions and tasks required to engage in organized social life outside the family, in community, social and civic areas of life” (World Health Organization, 2001). Based on the ICF description, we labeled activities falling under Chapter 9 as “social activity” because they are likely to include some degree of interaction with others. One trained coder assigned codes to each reported activity, and infrequent reports that were too difficult to classify or understand were decided by a majority vote among a team of three authors or coded as missing. For each participant, we then calculated the percentage of social activities from all reported activities during the week. One subject provided no data on this variable, and the rest of the participants provided an average of 36.75 (SD = 10.82) responses. Three participants provided responses at or below 3 SD (i.e., 4) of the average number of responses: Two participants provided only a single report during the week and a third one provided only three. The calculated percentages for these participants were treated as missing values. Thus, the total number of people with missing data per this criterion on the social activities variable was four. Overall, the average percentage of weekly social activities among all activities reported was 26.24% (SD = 13.47).
Table 2.
Major ICF categories |
General description | Second level subcategories | Individual/social designation |
Examples of coded text |
---|---|---|---|---|
Learning and applying knowledge | Learning, applying the knowledge that is learned, thinking, solving problems, and making decisions | Purposeful sensory experiences Basic learning Acquiring skills Applying knowledge Solving problems |
Individual | “Watching TV” “Reading the newspaper” |
General tasks and demands | General aspects of carrying out single or multiple tasks, organizing routines, and handling stress. These items can be used in conjunction with more specific tasks or actions to identify the underlying features of the execution of tasks under different circumstances | Undertaking a single task Undertaking multiple tasks Carrying out daily routine Handling stress and other psychological demands |
Individual | “Making appointments” “Sorting out mail” |
Communication | General and specific features of communicating by language, signs, and symbols, including receiving and producing messages, carrying on conversations, and using communication devices and techniques | Receiving communication Producing communication Conversation and use of communication devices |
Social | “Reading my email on my computer” “Talking on the phone” |
Mobility | Moving by changing body position or location or by transferring from one place to another; by carrying, moving or manipulating objects; by walking, running, or climbing; and by using various forms of transportation | Changing and maintaining body position Carrying, moving, and handling objects Walking and moving Moving around using transportation |
Individual | “I’m sitting on the bed” “Driving” |
Self-care | Caring for oneself, washing and drying oneself, caring for one’s body and body parts; dressing, eating and drinking, and looking after one’s health | Washing Caring for body Toileting Dressing Looking after one’s health |
Individual | “Getting dressed” “Napping” “Washing” |
Domestic life | Carrying out domestic and everyday actions and tasks. Areas of domestic life include acquiring a place to live, food, clothing and other necessities, household cleaning and repairing, caring for personal and other household objects, and assisting others | Acquisition of necessities Household tasks Caring for household objects and assisting others |
Individual (except for assisting others subcategory) | “Making morning coffee” “Household chores” |
Interpersonal interactions and relationships | Carrying out the actions and tasks required for basic and complex interactions with people (strangers, friends, relatives, family members and lovers) in a contextually and socially appropriate manner | General interpersonal interactions Particular interpersonal relationships |
Social | “Playing with my grandbaby” “Visiting a friend in the hospital” |
Major life areas | Carrying out the tasks and actions required to engage in education, work, and employment and to conduct economic transactions | Education Work and employment Economic life |
Social | “At the bank” “Working” |
Community social and civic life | Actions and tasks required to engage in organized social life outside the family, in community, social, and civic areas of life | Community life Recreation and leisure Religion and spirituality |
Social | “Church meeting” “Celebrating a birthday” |
Note. ICF = International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health.
Presence of Others.
Distinct from what respondents were doing, we asked participants to use the EMA interface and report whether they were alone or with someone at each of the seven time points across the day. This variable provides a measure of being in the presence of others but is not about doing, and it allows a control for effects of sociality outside the nature of the activity one is doing. For instance, one might be washing dishes (an individual activity) while someone else is in the next room watching television (being with someone). For each participant, we calculated the percentage of time spent with someone from all reports during the week. Eighteen subjects provided no data on this variable, and the rest of the participants provided an average of 40.31 (SD = 10.94) responses. Two participants provided responses at or below 3 SD (i.e., 7) of the average number of responses: One participant provided only a single report during the week and another one provided only two. The calculated percentages for these participants were treated as missing values. The total number of people with missing reports on the Presence of Others variable for whom we were able to impute missing values was 20. Overall, the average percentage of time spent with someone from all Presence of Others reports during the week was 44.83% (SD = 26.34).
Smoking and medication status.
During saliva collection, participants answered a series of questions, including whether they smoked or took any medications. These data were used to create two person-level variables: smoking status (0 = currently not smoking, 1 = currently smoking) and medication status (0 = currently not taking medication, 1 = currently taking medications). In our sample, 26.1% of participants reported currently smoking, and 90.2% reported currently using medications.
SES and demographics.
Education was measured on a scale from 1 (i.e., eighth grade or less) to 6 (i.e., graduate degree), whereas income was measured on a scale from 1 (i.e., less than US$5,000 a year) to 10 (i.e., US$70,000 or more a year). In our sample, 17.6% of participants reported having completed high school or having passed the General Educational Development (GED) test, 51.6% reported having attended some college or technical school, 17.6% reported having completed an undergraduate degree, and 13.2% reported having completed a graduate degree. Slightly more than a third (34.6%) of participants reported having an average yearly income of less than US$20,000, 49.4% reported having an average yearly income between US$20,000 and US$50,000, and 16% reported having an average yearly income greater than US$50,000. Income and education were first z-scored and then added together to derive an SES Index. Daily-level covariates included day of the week (0 = weekday, 1 = weekend) and daily wake-up time, which have been shown to be related to diurnal cortisol (Kudielka & Kirschbaum, 2003; Kunz-Ebrecht, Kirschbaum, Marmot, & Steptoe, 2004). Person-level covariates included age (M = 69.83, SD = 8.73), gender (0 = male, 1 = female), medication status, smoking status, as well as two important demographic characteristics related to individuals’ social life, living status (59.3% living alone), and employment status (9.1% currently employed), which correlated with our measure of social participation (r = .344, p = .001).
Cortisol.
For seven consecutive days, participants self-collected saliva samples at four time points each day: immediately upon waking, 30 min later to assess CAR, before dinner, and at bedtime. Participants were given verbal and written instructions about cortisol collection and storage in their own refrigerators. Our research team collected samples at their home and stored them in a cooler for transport to the laboratory where they were frozen at −20°C until shipment for analyses. Cortisol concentrations were quantified with a commercially available luminescence immunoassay (IBL International, Hamburg, Germany) at the laboratory of Dr. Kirschbaum at the Technical University of Dresden. Concordant with standard practice, cortisol values were log-transformed to correct for positive skew in the cortisol distribution (Adam & Kumari, 2009; Zilioli et al., 2017). To ensure that all transformed scores were positive, we added a constant of 1 before the transformation. Participants provided 2,387 saliva samples out of the 2,576 potential ones (92.7%). CAR compliance was high; of the available 610 CAR cortisol values, 111 self-reported deviating by 10 min or more from the requested 30-min interval (Adam, Hawkley, Kudielka, & Cacioppo, 2006). These cortisol values were treated as missing values at Level 1 in our multilevel models. Notably, hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) handles missing data at the lowest level of the hierarchy (in this case, the cortisol level) by using the maximum likelihood estimation method (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002).
Statistical Analyses
When considering all variables measured at the person level, the percentage of missing data in our sample was 4.46%. To impute missing values, we used the expectation maximization (EM) algorithm, which provides unbiased parameter estimates and improves statistical power of analyses (Enders, 2001). All variables with missing data were continuous except for one individual who had a missing value for living status and four individuals who provided no data as to their employment status. Mode replacement was used to replace these missing data.
We used HLM to account for the nested structure of the data and to model diurnal cortisol as a function of the covariates of interest (Adam & Kumari, 2009). In line with standard practice, we modeled time (including Time Since Waking, Time Since Waking2, and CAR, which was dummy coded 0 or 1) as a Level 1 measurement, daily-level covariates (e.g., day of the week) as Level 2, and person-level variables (e.g., age) as Level 3. The Time Since Waking variable reflected cortisol decline. First, we ran the model using SES only as the main predictor. Second, we refit the model adding percentage of weekly social activities as a predictor. Both models were fit first without and then controlling for covariates (i.e., daily wake-up time, day of the week, age, sex, living status, medication status, employment status, and percentage of presence of others). Continuous variables were left uncentered if they had a meaningful zero (i.e., percentage of social activities, percentage of presence of others, SES). We recoded age so that a zero value would represent the age of the youngest adults in the sample. We recoded education and income so that a zero value would indicate the lowest level of education and the minimum income reported in the sample. Daily wake-up time was grand-mean centered following recommendations by Enders and Tofighi (2007). HLM models fit without controlling for covariates are reported in the main text, whereas results of models that control for covariates are reported in Table 4. All HLM significance tests were two-tailed with robust standard errors. Third, we regressed social activities on SES with and without controlling for appropriate covariates (i.e., age, sex, living status, percentage of presence of others, and employment status). Finally, we estimated the 95% confidence interval for the indirect effect linking SES to cortisol parameters via percentage of weekly social activities using Monte Carlo simulation (Preacher & Selig, 2012).
Table 4.
Model 1 |
Model 2 |
|||||
Fixed effect (independent variable) |
Estimate | SE | p | Estimate | SE | p |
Time Since Waking, π2 | ||||||
Average linear slope, β20, γ200 | −0.1176 | 0.0244 | <.001 | −0.1085 | 0.0251 | <.001 |
SES, γ201 | −0.0030 | 0.0023 | .190 | −0.0017 | 0.0025 | .514 |
Social activities (%), γ202 | — | — | — | −0.0005 | 0.0002 | .036 |
Female, γ203 | −.0004 | 0.0083 | .957 | 0.0002 | 0.0083 | .978 |
Age, γ204 | −0.0002 | 0.0005 | .609 | −0.0002 | 0.0005 | .582 |
Living alone, γ205 | 0.0135 | 0.0087 | .124 | 0.0145 | 0.0079 | .071 |
Currently employed, γ206 | −0.0015 | 0.0110 | .896 | 0.0058 | 0.0110 | .600 |
Presence of others (%), γ207 | 0.0000 | 0.0002 | .836 | 0.0001 | 0.000 | .667 |
Taking medications, γ208 | 0.0075 | 0.0093 | .424 | 0.0085 | 0.0100 | .399 |
Smoker, γ209 | 0.0279 | 0.0102 | .008 | 0.0284 | 0.0101 | .006 |
Wake-up time, (β21, γ210 | −0.0001 | 0.0020 | .960 | −0.0004 | 0.0019 | .817 |
Weekend, (β22, γ220 | 0.0007 | 0.0038 | .844 | 0.0008 | 0.0038 | .824 |
Note. Intercepts indicate average cortisol values at wake up; average slopes of Time Since Waking indicate change in cortisol per 1-hr change in time; average slopes of Time Since Waking2 indicate change in cortisol per 1-hr change in time2. Although the table reports only predictors for the Time Since Waking variable, the same predictors were also included for Morning Cortisol and CAR, as described in the “Statistical Analyses” section. HLM = hierarchical linear modeling; SES = socioeconomic status; CAR = cortisol awakening response.
Results
SES and Diurnal Cortisol
Bivariate correlations are reported in Table 3. HLM analyses without controlling for covariates showed that higher SES was associated with a steeper cortisol decline (b = −0.006, SE = 0.002, p = .008). The direction and magnitude of the association were consistent when considering income (b = −0.005, SE = 0.002, p = .004) and education separately (b = −0.008, SE = 0.005, p = .065), even though education did not reach statistical significance. We found no significant direct association between SES and morning cortisol (intercept) or CAR (lowest p value = .161).
Table 3.
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Female | 1 | .185† | .129 | .062 | −.029 | −.168 | −.492** | .093 | .111 | .118 | .103 |
2. Age | 1 | −.045 | −.198† | −.205* | −.015 | −.222* | .190† | .160 | .203† | −.008 | |
3. Living alone | 1 | −.062 | −.722** | .028 | −.068 | −.273** | .056 | −.125 | −.063 | ||
4. Currently employed | 1 | .200† | −.288** | −.095 | −.018 | .084 | .038 | .344** | |||
5. Presence of others (%) | 1 | −178† | −.016 | .185† | −.037 | .086 | .139 | ||||
6. Taking medication | 1 | .112 | −.027 | .011 | −.010 | −.073 | |||||
7. Smoker | 1 | −.345** | −.244* | −.342** | −.173† | ||||||
8. Income | 1 | .489** | .863** | .340** | |||||||
9. Education | 1 | .863** | 292** | ||||||||
10. SES | 1 | .366** | |||||||||
11. Social activities (%) | 1 | ||||||||||
M (SD) or % | 79.4 | 69.8 (8.7) | 59.3 | 9.1 | 44.8 | 90.2 | 26.1 | 5.4 (2.2) | 4.3 (0.9) | 0 (1.7) | 26.2 |
Note. Except for SES, descriptive statistics for study variables are based on non-imputed data. SES = socioeconomic status.
p < . 10.
p < .05.
p < .01.
After controlling for covariates, we found that SES was no longer associated with cortisol decline (Table 4, Model 1). This attenuation was likely due to the inclusion of potential mediators of the SES gradient in cortisol decline among our covariates. For example, previous studies have shown that smoking status is a robust mediator of the link between SES and cortisol decline (Matthews, Schwartz, Cohen, & Seeman, 2006). Notably, in our analyses, when smoking status was not included as a covariate in the analyses, the association between SES and cortisol was significant (b = −0.005, SE = 0.002, p = .036), confirming the possibility of a mediating role played by smoking status. Furthermore, although SES was not directly linked to cortisol decline after controlling for covariates, this does not preclude the possibility of mediation through social activities because indirect effects can exist in the absence of a significant total effect (Zhao, Lynch, & Chen, 2010).
SES, Social Activities, and Diurnal Cortisol
When SES and percentage of social activities were both included in an HLM with no covariates, we found that higher rates of social activities were also associated with a steeper cortisol decline (b = −0.0005, SE = 0.0002, p = .022). These associations remained significant after controlling for covariates (Table 4, Model 2). The estimates were consistent when income (b = −0.0005, SE = 0.0002, p = .019; b = −0.0005, SE = 0.0002, p = .022 after controlling for covariates) or education (b = −0.0006, SE = 0.0002, p = .003; b = −0.0005, SE = 0.0002, p = .024 after controlling for covariates) was used in place of SES. Regression analyses without controlling for covariates showed that SES was positively associated with percentage of weekly social activities (b = 2.798, SE = 0.750, p < .001; consistent for income: b = 2.085, SE = 0.609, p = .001; for education: b = 4.185, SE = 1.444, p = .005). The same effects were found in the covariates-controlled model (for SES, b = 2.677, SE = 0.745, p = .001; for income: b = 2.184, SE = 0.615, p = .001; for education: b = 3.736, SE = 1.426, p = .010). Monte Carlo analyses indicated a significant indirect effect of SES on cortisol decline via percentage of weekly social activities (95% CI = [−0.002964, −0.00009278]; 95% CI = [−0.00284, −0.0001927], without controlling for covariates). The same effects were consistent for income (95% CI = [−0.002556, −0.0001589]; 95% CI = [−0.002224, −0.0001431], without controlling for covariates) and education (95% CI = [−0.004551, −0.0001304]; 95% CI = [−0.005031, −0.0005009], without controlling for covariates).
Discussion
Several findings emerged from this work. First, both higher SES and higher rates of social activities were associated with a more normative daily cortisol decline. Second, higher SES was also directly and positively linked to higher participation in social activities. Finally, SES effects on cortisol fluctuations were partially realized through higher rates of social activities.
The positive association between SES and the relative percentage of social activities is in line with the broader literature on SES and social networks. For example, older adults with lower SES tend to have more modest (Ajrouch et al., 2005; Van Groenou & Van Tilburg, 2003) and less diverse (Krause & Borawski-Clark, 1995) social networks compared with their higher SES counterparts. Loneliness, the psychological distress associated with the perception of a withered social network, is more common among people living under conditions of socioeconomic disadvantage (Hawkley et al., 2008). Furthermore, individuals with lower educational attainment and income report participating in fewer social activities (de Leon, Glass, & Berkman, 2003; Herzog et al., 1998; Vroman, Arthanat, & Lysack, 2015).
We also discovered that social activity levels—as derived through our coding schema of self-reported activities—mediated the association between SES and diurnal HPA activity, such that older adults with a greater proportion of weekly social activities experienced a steeper cortisol decline throughout the day than older adults who had lower rates of social activities. This finding remained significant after controlling for the percentage of time our participants were in the presence of others. Interestingly, percentage of time spent with others and percentage of social activities were not correlated, suggesting that these two measures tap into two different constructs; to wit, social participation is about being engaged in social activities and is not the same as being in the presence of others. A handful of studies have linked social participation to physiological activity, such as blood pressure, total cortisol output (Pressman et al., 2009), and chronic inflammation (Friedman, 2011). However, these reports have not specifically looked at weekly social activities; rather, they defined social participation or engagement in terms of either self-reported leisure activities (Pressman et al., 2009) or self-reported social well-being (Friedman, 2011). An exception is the work by Stetler and colleagues who found that, among young adults, regular engagement in daily social activities (Stetler et al., 2004) and daily contact with familiar others (Stetler & Miller, 2008) were associated with steeper cortisol declines. Notably, our investigation extends findings of these previous studies to older adults, and older African Americans in particular. It is reasonable to suggest that social participation should be investigated further as a possible mechanism of reducing the burden of SES inequality and other social harms that affect older African American health (Forrester, Gallo, Whitfield, & Thorpe, 2018).
It is plausible to consider HPA modulation by social participation as one broad biological mechanism responsible for the protective effects attributed to social participation. Higher rates of social participation have been linked to better physical (Chiao et al., 2013; Sundquist et al., 2004) and mental health (Ellaway & Macintyre, 2007; Hong et al., 2009). For example, among adults of various ages, Sundquist et al. (2004) found that low social participation predicted risk of coronary heart disease (but see Ellaway & Macintyre, 2007), whereas Chiao et al. (2013) found that social participation positively predicted life expectancy in older adults. Recent evidence strengthens the hypothesis that dysregulation of daily cortisol secretion acts as a biological intermediary of these poor health outcomes (Zilioli et al., 2017; for a review, see Adam et al., 2017).
To our knowledge, the present study represents one of the first that speaks to how social activities—measured multiple times daily for a week via EMA—are related to daily activity of the HPA axis among minority older adults. Although lower SES is a well-established factor in older adult health (Grundy & Holt, 2001; Schoeni, Martin, Andreski, & Freedman, 2005) and racial health disparities (Williams & Mohammed, 2013; Williams et al., 2010), this study provides evidence of how the role of lower SES in health might be partially offset by higher social participation—a goal of many healthy and productive aging initiatives and interventions. We acknowledge that our coding schema for activities could have resulted in errors of classification of self-reported activity types, yet it was systematic, using a formal classification system designed to measure activities and participation. Another measurement limitation of our study was to ask participants whether they were in the presence of others only with regard to specific points in time and not over longer periods. Thus, we could have underestimated the proportion of time being with others, yet we used repeated measures for each day across a week for all participants. As such, we believe that the weekly average measure that we used captures a meaningful objective and is more precise relative to subjective recollection of the overall time spent in the presence of others.
Because cortisol decline has been prospectively associated with poor objective (Kumari, Shipley, Stafford, & Kivimaki, 2011) and subjective (Zilioli et al., 2017) clinical end points, an important inference from the current findings is that HPA activity may serve as one of the biological intermediaries through which SES and social participation disparities manifest their deleterious effects on physical health outcomes among older adults. In this regard, future longitudinal work should formally test this hypothesis. Future investigations might also benefit from implementing multiracial research designs. Although our study leverages the advantages of a single-race study design, multiracial research designs have the intrinsic value of clearly quantifying racial disparities. Moreover, because our participants were predominantly women, future investigation would benefit from including a more gender-balanced sample, with the added advantage of testing gender differences, which have been previously documented when linking social engagement to physiological activity (Friedman, 2011). Finally, although we recruited only adults capable of participating in activities without assistance from another person, our results do not rule out the possibility that certain health and functional problems may play a role in the association observed between social participation and cortisol decline. Unfortunately, because no measure of physical health was collected in the study, we could not control for this important confounder in our analyses. That said, this concern is mitigated by our inclusion of medication use and smoking, which have been found to be reliable predictors of physical heath.
In sum, in a sample of 92 African Americans between the age of 55 and 95 years, higher SES was associated with a greater percentage of weekly social activities reported through EMA. In turn, weekly engagement in social activities predicted a steeper diurnal cortisol decline. Evidence for an indirect effect linking SES to steeper cortisol decline via increased relative frequency of weekly social activities was found, suggesting that engagement in weekly social activities represent a behavioral intermediary for SES health disparities in endocrine function among older urban African American adults.
Acknowledgments
The authors gratefully acknowledge Daniel Saleh for his assistance in data collection and Dr. Richard B. Slatcher for his assistance with the study design.
Funding
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This work was supported by funds from the Wayne State University Office of the Vice President for Research and by the Wayne State University Center for Urban Responses to Environmental Stressors (CURES) (NIEHS Grant P30 ES020957).
Footnotes
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
References
- Adam EK, Hawkley LC, Kudielka BM, & Cacioppo JT (2006). Day-to-day dynamics of experience–cortisol associations in a population-based sample of older adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103, 17058–17063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adam EK, & Kumari M (2009). Assessing salivary cortisol in large-scale, epidemiological research. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34, 1423–1436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adam EK, Quinn ME, Tavernier R, McQuillan MT, Dahlke KA, & Gilbert KE (2017). Diurnal cortisol slopes and mental and physical health outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 83, 25–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adler NE, Boyce T, Chesney MA, Cohen S, Folkman S, Kahn RL, & Syme SL (1994). Socioeconomic status and health: The challenge of the gradient. American Psychologist, 49, 15–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ajrouch KJ, Blandon AY, & Antonucci TC (2005). Social networks among men and women: The effects of age and socioeconomic status. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 60, S311–S317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aneshensel CS, Wight RG, Miller-Martinez D, Botticello AL, Karlamangla AS, & Seeman TE (2007). Urban neighborhoods and depressive symptoms among older adults. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 62, S52–S59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brissette I, Cohen S, & Seeman TE (2000). Measuring social integration and social networks In Underwood LG, & Gottlieb BH (Eds.), Social support measurement and intervention: A guide for health and social scientists (pp. 53–85). New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Chiang JJ, Bower JE, Almeida DM, Irwin MR, Seeman TE, & Fuligni AJ (2015). Socioeconomic status, daily affective and social experiences, and inflammation during adolescence. Psychosomatic Medicine, 77, 256–266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiao C, Weng L-J, & Botticello AL (2011). Social participation reduces depressive symptoms among older adults: An 18-year longitudinal analysis in Taiwan. BMC Public Health, 11(1), Article 292. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiao C-Y, Lee S-H, Liao W-C, Yen C-H, Lin Y-J, Li C-R, … Lee M-C (2013). Social participation and life expectancy—the case of older adults in Taiwan from 1996 to 2003. International Journal of Gerontology, 7, 97–101. [Google Scholar]
- Chida Y, & Steptoe A (2009). Cortisol awakening response and psychosocial factors: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Biological Psychology, 80, 265–278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen S (1988). Psychosocial models of the role of social support in the etiology of physical disease. Health Psychology, 7, 269–297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen S, Doyle WJ, & Baum A (2006). Socioeconomic status is associated with stress hormones. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68, 414–420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Leon CFM, Glass TA, & Berkman LF (2003). Social engagement and disability in a community population of older adults the new haven EPESE. American Journal of Epidemiology, 157, 633–642. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunlop DD, Song J, Manheim LM, Daviglus ML, & Chang RW (2007). Racial/ethnic differences in the development of disability among older adults. American Journal of Public Health, 97, 2209–2215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Durkheim E (1951). Suicide: A study in sociology (Spaulding JA & Simpson G, trans.). Glencoe, IL: Free Press; (original Work Published 1897) [Google Scholar]
- Ellaway A, & Macintyre S (2007). Is social participation associated with cardiovascular disease risk factors? Social Science & Medicine, 64, 1384–1391. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Enders CK (2001). A primer on maximum likelihood algorithms available for use with missing data. Structural Equation Modeling, 8(1), 128–141. [Google Scholar]
- Enders CK, & Tofighi D (2007). Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel models: A new look at an old issue. Psychological Methods, 12, 121–138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faris REL (1934). Cultural isolation and the schizophrenic personality. American Journal of Sociology, 40, 155–164. [Google Scholar]
- Forrester SN, Gallo JJ, Whitfield KE, & Thorpe RJ (2018). A Framework of Minority Stress: From physiological manifestations to cognitive outcomes. The Gerontologist. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1093/geront/gny104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedman EM (2011). Sleep quality, social well-being, gender, and inflammation: An integrative analysis in a national sample. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1231, 23–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fritz H, Tarraf W, Saleh DJ, & Cutchin MP (2017). Using a smartphone-based ecological momentary assessment protocol with community dwelling older African Americans. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 72(5), 876–887. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glass TA, De Leon CM, Marottoli RA, & Berkman LF (1999). Population based study of social and productive activities as predictors of survival among elderly Americans. British Medical Journal, 319, 478–483. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grundy E, & Holt G (2001). The socioeconomic status of older adults: How should we measure it in studies of health inequalities? Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health, 55, 895–904. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hawkley LC, Hughes ME, Waite LJ, Masi CM, Thisted RA, & Cacioppo JT (2008). From social structural factors to perceptions of relationship quality and loneliness: The Chicago health, aging, and social relations study. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 63, S375–S384. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herzog A, Franks MM, Markus HR, & Holmberg D (1998). Activities and well-being in older age: Effects of self-concept and educational attainment. Psychology and Aging, 13, 179–185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hong S-I, Hasche L, & Bowland S (2009). Structural relationships between social activities and longitudinal trajectories of depression among older adults. The Gerontologist, 49(1), 1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Janicki-Deverts D, Cohen S, Adler NE, Schwartz JE, Matthews KA, & Seeman TE (2007). Socioeconomic status is related to urinary catecholamines in the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 69, 514–520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kahn RL (1994). Social support: Content, causes, and consequences In Abeles RP, Gift HC, & Ory MG (Eds.), Aging and quality of life (pp. 163–184). New York: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Kanamori S, Kai Y, Aida J, Kondo K, Kawachi I, Hirai H, … Suzuki K (2014). Social participation and the prevention of functional disability in older Japanese: The JAGES cohort study. PLoS ONE, 9(6), e99638. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaufman JS, Cooper RS, & McGee DL (1997). Socioeconomic status and health in blacks and whites: The problem of residual confounding and the resiliency of race. Epidemiology, 8, 621–628. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krause N, & Borawski-Clark E (1995). Social class differences in social support among older adults. The Gerontologist, 35, 498–508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kubzansky LD, Berkman LF, Glass TA, & Seeman TE (1998). Is educational attainment associated with shared determinants of health in the elderly? Findings from the MacArthur Studies of Successful Aging. Psychosomatic Medicine, 60, 578–585. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kudielka BM, & Kirschbaum C (2003). Awakening cortisol responses are influenced by health status and awakening time but not by menstrual cycle phase. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 28, 35–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumari M, Shipley M, Stafford M, & Kivimaki M (2011). Association of diurnal patterns in salivary cortisol with all-cause and cardiovascular mortality: Findings from the Whitehall II study. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 96, 1478–1485. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kunz-Ebrecht SR, Kirschbaum C, Marmot M, & Steptoe A (2004). Differences in cortisol awakening response on work days and weekends in women and men from the Whitehall II cohort. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29, 516–528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Legh-Jones H, & Moore S (2012). Network social capital, social participation, and physical inactivity in an urban adult population. Social Science & Medicine, 74, 1362–1367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthews K, Schwartz J, Cohen S, & Seeman T (2006). Diurnal cortisol decline is related to coronary calcification: CARDIA study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68, 657–661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthews KA, & Gallo LC (2011). Psychological perspectives on pathways linking socioeconomic status and physical health. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 501–530. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCurley JL, Penedo F, Roesch SC, Isasi CR, Carnethon M, Sotres-Alvarez D, … Gallo LC (2017). Psychosocial factors in the relationship between socioeconomic status and cardio metabolic risk: The HCHS/SOL Sociocultural ancillary study. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 51, 477–488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Needham BL, Adler N, Gregorich S, Rehkopf D, Lin J, Blackburn EH, & Epel ES (2013). Socioeconomic status, health behavior, and leukocyte telomere length in the national health and nutrition examination survey, 1999–2002. Social Science & Medicine, 85, 1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicholson NR (2012). A review of social isolation: An important but underassessed condition in older adults. The Journal of Primary Prevention, 33, 137–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preacher KJ, & Selig JP (2012). Advantages of Monte Carlo confidence intervals for indirect effects. Communication Methods and Measures, 6, 77–98. [Google Scholar]
- Pressman SD, Matthews KA, Cohen S, Martire LM, Scheier M, Baum A, & Schulz R (2009). Association of enjoyable leisure activities with psychological and physical well-being. Psychosomatic Medicine, 71, 725–732. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raudenbush SW, & Bryk AS (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods (Vol. 1). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE. [Google Scholar]
- Roh HW, Hong CH, Lee Y, Oh BH, Lee KS, Chang KJ, & Son SJ (2015). Participation in physical, social, and religious activity and risk of depression in the elderly: A community-based three-year longitudinal study in Korea. PLoS ONE, 10(7), e0132838. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ross CE, & Jang SJ (2000). Neighborhood disorder, fear, and mistrust: The buffering role of social ties with neighbors. American Journal of Community Psychology, 28, 401–420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sapolsky RM (2005). The influence of social hierarchy on primate health. Science, 308, 648–652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sapolsky RM, Romero LM, & Munck AU (2000). How do glucocorticoids influence stress responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions. Endocrine Reviews, 21, 55–89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saxbe DE (2008). A field (researcher’s) guide to cortisol: Tracking HPA axis functioning in everyday life. Health Psychology Review, 2, 163–190. [Google Scholar]
- Schoeni RF, Martin LG, Andreski PM, & Freedman VA (2005). Persistent and growing socioeconomic disparities in disability among the elderly: 1982–2002. American Journal of Public Health, 95, 2065–2070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sirven N, & Debrand T (2008). Social participation and healthy ageing: An international comparison using SHARE data. Social Science & Medicine, 67, 2017–2026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stetler C, Dickerson SS, & Miller GE (2004). Uncoupling of social zeitgebers and diurnal cortisol secretion in clinical depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29 1250–1259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stetler CA, & Miller GE (2008). Social integration of daily activities and cortisol secretion: A laboratory based manipulation. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 31, 249–257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sundquist K, Lindström M, Malmström M, Johansson S-E, & Sundquist J (2004). Social participation and coronary heart disease: A follow-up study of 6900 women and men in Sweden. Social Science & Medicine, 58, 615–622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takeuchi K, Aida J, Kondo K, & Osaka K (2013). Social participation and dental health status among older Japanese adults: A population-based cross-sectional study. PLoS ONE, 8(4), e61741. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thoits PA (1983). Multiple identities and psychological well-being: A reformulation and test of the social isolation hypothesis. American Sociological Review, 48, 174–187. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsigos C, & Chrousos GP (2002). Hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis, neuroendocrine factors and stress. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 53, 865–871. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Groenou MIB, & Van Tilburg T (2003). Network size and support in old age: Differentials by socio-economic status in childhood and adulthood. Ageing and Society, 23, 625–645. [Google Scholar]
- Veenstra G (2000). Social capital, SES and health: An individual-level analysis. Social Science & Medicine, 50, 619–629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vroman KG, Arthanat S, & Lysack C (2015). “Who over 65 is online?” Older adults’ dispositions toward information communication technology. Computers in Human Behavior, 43, 156–166. [Google Scholar]
- Williams DR, & Mohammed SA (2013). Racism and health I: Pathways and scientific evidence. American Behavioral Scientist, 57, 1152–1173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams DR, Mohammed SA, Leavell J, & Collins C (2010). Race, socioeconomic status, and health: Complexities, ongoing challenges, and research opportunities. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1186, 69–101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization; (2001). International classification of functioning, disability and health: ICF. Author. Geneva, Switzerland: https://www.who.int/classifications/icf/en/ (accessed 6 May 2019). [Google Scholar]
- Zhao XS, Lynch JG, & Chen QM (2010). Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: Myths and Truths about Mediation Analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, 37, 197–206. [Google Scholar]
- Zilioli S, Imami L, & Slatcher RB (2017). Socioeconomic status, perceived control, diurnal cortisol, and physical symptoms: A moderated mediation model. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 75, 36–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]