Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2020 Jan 6;9:e52990. doi: 10.7554/eLife.52990

Cell-specific non-canonical amino acid labelling identifies changes in the de novo proteome during memory formation

Harrison Tudor Evans 1, Liviu-Gabriel Bodea 1,, Jürgen Götz 1,
Editors: Sacha B Nelson2, Laura L Colgin3
PMCID: PMC6944461  PMID: 31904341

Abstract

The formation of spatial long-term memory (LTM) requires the de novo synthesis of distinct sets of proteins; however, a non-biased examination of the de novo proteome in this process is lacking. Here, we generated a novel mouse strain, which enables cell-type-specific labelling of newly synthesised proteins with non-canonical amino acids (NCAAs) by genetically restricting the expression of the mutant tRNA synthetase, NLL-MetRS, to hippocampal neurons. By combining this labelling technique with an accelerated version of the active place avoidance task and bio-orthogonal non-canonical amino acid tagging (BONCAT) followed by SWATH quantitative mass spectrometry, we identified 156 proteins that were altered in synthesis in hippocampal neurons during spatial memory formation. In addition to observing increased synthesis of known proteins important in memory-related processes, such as glutamate receptor recycling, we also identified altered synthesis of proteins associated with mRNA splicing as a potential mechanism involved in spatial LTM formation.

Research organism: Mouse

Introduction

Neurons are highly complex, compartmentalized cells that respond to a wide range of physiological and pathological signals by regulated changes to their transcriptome and proteome. A critical component in this process is the synthesis of new proteins, both in the cell body and at the synapse (Davis and Squire, 1984; Hafner et al., 2019; Li and Götz, 2017; Schanzenbächer et al., 2016). New protein synthesis is required for the formation, retrieval, and updating of long-term memories (LTMs), distinguishing this process from short-term memory (Costa-Mattioli et al., 2009; Jarome and Helmstetter, 2014; Lopez et al., 2015). One form of LTM is spatial LTM, which is used by organisms to recall spatial information about the environment, and is formed through the process of spatial LTM consolidation (Squire et al., 2015). During this process, neurons undergo a series of changes in different brain regions, with the hippocampus being identified as one of the most important brain structures in both spatial learning and LTM (Clopath, 2012; Mayford et al., 2012; Ziegler et al., 2015). Furthermore, inhibition of protein synthesis in the hippocampus, especially within the first 24 hr following spatial learning, has been demonstrated to prevent the formation of spatial LTM (Alkon et al., 2005; Inda et al., 2005; Jarome and Helmstetter, 2014). Therefore, identifying precisely which proteins need to be newly synthesised during spatial LTM consolidation is crucial in dissecting the underlying molecular mechanisms.

Several proteins and pathways have been linked to spatial LTM formation, as shown by candidate-based studies in which rodents were subjected to behavioural paradigms, such as the active place avoidance (APA) or Morris water maze test in order to induce spatial LTM formation (Merlo et al., 2015; Paul et al., 2009; Plath et al., 2006). However, a non-biased proteomic analysis of de novo protein synthesis during the formation of spatial LTM has not been previously achievable, because de novo synthesised proteins are chemically indistinguishable from those that are already present in the cell. This limitation can, however, be overcome through non-canonical amino acid (NCAA) labelling of newly synthesised proteins. In this technique, NCAAs can be administered for a given period, during which they are integrated into the nascent polypeptide chain (Figure 1A) (Hinz et al., 2013). Unlike most other de novo protein tagging techniques, NCAA incorporation enables newly synthesised proteins to be visualised using fluorescent non-canonical amino acid tagging (FUNCAT), or to be purified using bio-orthogonal non-canonical amino acid tagging (BONCAT) (Figure 1B) (Hinz et al., 2013). This is achieved by reacting the azide group of the NCAA with a dibenzocyclooctyne (DIBO)-bearing tag, using strain-promoted azide-alkyne cycloaddition (Figure 1B) (Beatty et al., 2010).

Figure 1. Long-term memory formation following training using the 30 min APA paradigm results in increased hippocampal de novo protein synthesis.

(A) AHA is recognised by mouse methionine tRNA synthetase, MetRS (Mars1), and labels de novo synthesised proteins at amino-terminal and internal methionine residues using the endogenous translational machinery. (B) NCAA-labelled proteins can be covalently bonded to various tags through reaction of the azide group (orange) of the NCAA with the alkyne group (purple) of the tag. This enables NCAA-labelled proteins to either be visualised using fluorescent non-canonical amino acid tagging (FUNCAT) or to be purified using bio-orthogonal non-canonical amino acid tagging (BONCAT). (C) The 30 min APA paradigm results in spatial long-term memory formation. Mice trained over 30 min learned to avoid a designated shock zone (red), with significantly fewer entries into the shock zone being recorded between 25–30 min compared to between 0–5 min. In a 5 min probe trial held 24 hr after training, mice continued to avoid entering the shock zone, even in the absence of shocks, indicative of the formation of spatial LTM (n = 6 mice, one-way ANOVA, Dunnett’s MCT, *p≤0.05, **p≤0.01). (D) Scheme of 30 min APA task for trained and non-trained mice. Trained mice received foot shocks upon entry into the designated shock zone, while non-trained mice received foot shocks at the same time as their trained partner and were therefore unable to undergo spatial LTM formation. Upon completion of the 30 min APA, mice were administered AHA and were perfused 16 hr later without undergoing a probe trial. (E) A significant increase in protein synthesis was observed in the hippocampus of trained compared to non-trained mice using FUNCAT (n = 4 mice, three sections per mouse, Student’s paired t-test, *p≤0.05). Scale bar = 400 µm.

Figure 1—source data 1. WT 30 minute APA behavioural data.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. AHA treatment does not inhibit spatial LTM formation.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

Both mice treated with either AHA or PBS immediately after training using the 30 min APA test could recall the location of the shock zone in a probe trial conducted 16 hr after training (n = 4 mice, two way ANOVA, Dunnett’s MCT, ***p≤0.001). There was no significant difference in performance in this probe trial between the two groups (two-way ANOVA, Sidak’s MCT, n.s. = non significant, p=0.85).
Figure 1—figure supplement 1—source data 1. AHA treated 30 minute APA behavioural data.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Trained and non-trained mice show similar levels of stress, as measured by plasma corticosterone levels.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

Both trained and non-trained mice show increased plasma levels of corticosterone (a marker of increased stress) compared to a naïve controls which exposed to the 30 min APA arena without receiving foot shocks (n = 3 mice, one-way ANOVA, Tukey’s MCT, **p≤0.01). There was no significant difference in plasma corticosterone levels between trained and non-trained mice (n.s. = non significant, p=0.78).

Two widely used NCAAs are the methionine surrogates azidohomoalanine (AHA) and azidonorleucine (ANL). In AHA labelling, the endogenous translational machinery is used to tag newly synthesised proteins with AHA (Figure 1A) (Dieterich et al., 2006; Hinz et al., 2013; Ullrich et al., 2014). ANL, on the other hand, allows for cell-type-specific NCAA labelling, as it is not recognized by the eukaryotic methionine tRNA synthetase and therefore does not natively integrate into de novo synthesised proteins (Link et al., 2006). ANL instead requires the presence of a mutant tRNA synthetase, such as NLL-MetRS, for its incorporation (Figure 2A) (Ngo et al., 2013). Genetically restricting NLL-MetRS expression enables cell-type- or tissue-specific incorporation of ANL. Both AHA and ANL labelling have been used in a wide range of cell-types, tissues and organisms (Alvarez-Castelao et al., 2019; Alvarez-Castelao et al., 2017; Erdmann et al., 2015; Liang et al., 2014; Lopez et al., 2015; McClatchy et al., 2015; Ullrich et al., 2014). However, even though AHA and ANL labelling display an enhanced versatility compared to other de novo protein labelling techniques, such as stable isotype labelling with amino acids in cell culture (SILAC) or puromycin labelling, they are yet to be more widely used to examine the de novo proteomic changes which occur during complex rodent behaviour.

Figure 2. Rosa26 Cre Click-chemistry (RC3) mice enable cell type-specific labelling of newly synthesised proteins.

Figure 2.

(A) Incorporation of ANL requires the expression of NLL-MetRS, which allows this NCAA to be incorporated at the amino-terminus as proteins are being synthesized. (B) In RC3 mice, the expression of NLL-MetRS-EGFP is prevented by an upstream FLOX-STOP cassette. In RC3xCamk2a-Cre mice, Cre-recombinase is expressed in the hippocampus, resulting in the excision of the FLOX-STOP cassette from the RC3 transgene. This enables expression of NLL-MetRS-EGFP restricted to hippocampal neurons. (C) Immunohistochemical analysis confirmed Cre-specificity of NLL-MetRS-EGFP expression in RC3xCamk2a-Cre mice, with the EGFP signal being confined to neurons in the CA1, CA2, CA2 and dentate gyrus regions of the hippocampus. Scale bar = 400 µm (D) FUNCAT staining confirms that ANL integration was restricted to hippocampal neurons in RC3xCamk2a-Cre mice. Scale bar = 40 µm. (E) BONCAT-WB analysis of RC3xCamk2a-Cre mice reveals maximal ANL labelling when mice were administered 100 µg ANL/gbw via i.p. injection (n = 3 mice, one way ANOVA, Tukey’s multiple comparison test, *p≤0.05). (F) BONCAT-WB analysis of RC3xCamk2a-Cre mice reveals maximal ANL labelling approximately 16 hr post injection (n = 3 mice, one way ANOVA, Tukey’s multiple comparison test **p≤0.01, ***p≤0.001, ****p≤0.0001).

Here, we detail the use of NCAA labelling to identify changes in de novo protein synthesis which occur during the formation of spatial LTM in mice. For this, we generated the RC3 mouse strain, which enables Cre recombinase-dependent targeting of NLL-MetRS expression for cell-type- or tissue-specific incorporation of ANL. Upon crossing the RC3 strain with Camk2a-Cre mice, we successfully restricted ANL labelling of newly synthesised proteins to hippocampal neurons. We combined this labelling technique with SWATH-MS (sequential window acquisition of all theoretical fragment ions mass spectrometry) to examine how the hippocampal de novo proteome is altered in mice following spatial LTM formation. Our results confirmed a number of previously established memory-related proteins to be altered in synthesis during spatial LTM formation, and also highlight potential novel memory mechanisms, such as alterations in mRNA splicing.

Results

AHA labelling reveals increased hippocampal protein synthesis during spatial LTM formation

Given the well-established role of de novo protein synthesis in the process of spatial LTM consolidation (Davis and Squire, 1984), we sought to use NCAA labelling in combination with an unbiased proteomic analysis to identify which proteins were altered in synthesis during spatial LTM formation in mice, using a simple and robust behavioural paradigm.

To achieve this, we subjected mice to an accelerated version of the active place avoidance (APA) test, referred to as '30 min APA'. Mice were trained over 30 min to use spatial cues to avoid entering a designated shock zone (Figure 1C). Wild-type mice undertaking this task demonstrated spatial learning as revealed by their ability to significantly decrease the number of entries into the shock zone over the training period (Figure 1C). We also found that the mice were able to recall the location of the shock zone in a probe trial 24 hr later (Figure 1C), indicative of spatial LTM formation (Plath et al., 2006).

Having established this behavioural paradigm, we next sought to use AHA labelling to examine brain-wide de novo protein synthesis during spatial LTM formation. We have previously demonstrated that optimal AHA labelling following intraperitoneal (i.p.) injection occurs over a time-frame of 16 hr at a concentration of 50 µg AHA/gram body weight (gbw) (Evans et al., 2019). We therefore first confirmed that mice treated with 50 µg AHA/gbw immediately after training performed similarly to PBS-treated controls in a probe trial conducted 16 hr post-training, demonstrating that AHA treatment does not interfere with spatial LTM formation (Figure 1—figure supplement 1).

To differentiate spatial LTM-induced protein synthesis from background levels, we compared mice trained in the 30 min APA (‘trained mice’) to 'non-trained' controls (Figure 1D). Each non-trained mouse underwent a yoked version of the 30 min APA protocol, where instead of being shocked upon entry into the designated shock zone, the animal received shocks at the same time as a trained mouse with which it was paired (Figure 1D). These control mice were therefore unable to undergo spatial learning, while still being exposed to the same environment, number of foot shocks, and a similar level of stress. The latter was confirmed by analysing plasma levels of the stress-related hormone, corticosterone (Gong et al., 2015), which were found to be similar between trained and non-trained mice (Figure 1—figure supplement 2).

Immediately following the behavioural task, both trained and non-trained mice were administered 50 µg AHA/gbw intraperitoneally and perfused 16 hr later without undergoing a probe test (Figure 1D). Using FUNCAT to visualise AHA-labelled proteins, we observed an increase in this signal in the hippocampus of trained mice compared to non-trained controls (Figure 1E), indicating that protein synthesis is increased in this brain region during the formation of spatial LTM. Our findings, in combination with the wealth of data demonstrating that hippocampal protein synthesis is essential for spatial LTM formation and storage (Jarome and Helmstetter, 2014; Kleinknecht et al., 2012; Poucet et al., 2003), led us to examine in more detail how spatial LTM formation alters the de novo proteome in the hippocampus.

Novel MetRS mutant transgenic mice enable cell-type-specific in vivo labelling of newly synthesised proteins

Following our observation that hippocampal protein synthesis is increased during spatial LTM formation, we sought to refine our approach by developing an experimental system for cell-specific NCAA labelling in order to examine protein synthesis specifically in neurons of the hippocampus. To restrict protein labelling to hippocampal neurons, we exploited the fact that ANL can only be incorporated into nascent proteins in cells expressing mutant tRNA synthetases, such as NLL-MetRS (Figure 2A). We therefore generated a mouse strain that expresses NLL-MetRS in a Cre-dependent manner, allowing tissue- or cell-type-specific incorporation of ANL. This mouse strain, referred to as ROSA26a Cre-inducible Click Chemistry (RC3) strain, was generated by inserting the RC3 transgene (consisting of NLL-MetRS fused to EGFP downstream of a floxed stop cassette) into the permissive ROSA26a locus (Bouabe and Okkenhaug, 2013) using CRISPR/CAS9-mediated genome editing (Figure 2B). Thus, upon expression of Cre-recombinase in mice, the floxed-STOP cassette is excised, enabling expression of NLL-MetRS-EGFP and incorporation of ANL into newly synthesised proteins (Figure 2B).

For our study, we crossed the RC3 mice with the Camk2a-Cre (T29-1) strain that constitutively expresses Cre recombinase specifically in hippocampal neurons (Tsien et al., 1996). We validated confinement of NLL-MetRS-EGFP expression to hippocampal neurons in the resulting double transgenic strain by immunohistochemistry (Figure 2C), and, following ANL injection, revealed a similarly confined labelling of de novo synthesised proteins (Figure 2D). We next used BONCAT followed by western blotting (BONCAT-WB) to examine the dynamics of ANL labelling in the hippocampus. This allowed us to confirm that ANL labelling followed a similar time course to that of AHA labelling, with a dosage of 100 µg ANL/gbw resulting in maximal labelling at 16 hr (Figure 2E&F). We also confirmed that this labelling did not prevent spatial LTM formation, with ANL-treated RC3xCamk2a-Cre mice being able to recall the location of the shock-zone a week after training (Figure 3—figure supplement 1).

De novo proteomic analysis identifies altered hippocampal synthesis of select proteins and pathways during spatial LTM formation

We next sought to identify de novo proteomic changes which occur in the hippocampal neurons during spatial LTM consolidation. We therefore induced spatial LTM formation in 5 month-old female RC3xCamk2a-Cre mice using the 30 min APA protocol (Figure 3B). This was immediately followed by intraperitoneal injection of 100 µg ANL/gbw of both trained and non-trained mice, which were perfused 16 hr later (Figure 3A). Using FUNCAT and immunohistochemistry, we observed an increase in total protein synthesis in the hippocampal neurons of the trained mice compared to non-trained controls (Figure 3C), recapitulating the above AHA findings. Moreover, we confirmed that blocking protein synthesis through the administration of anisomycin prevented spatial LTM formation (Figure 3—figure supplement 2).

Figure 3. De novo proteomic analysis reveals altered hippocampal synthesis of selected proteins during spatial LTM formation.

(A) Schematic of the 30 min APA task for trained and non-trained (yoked) RC3xCamk2-Cre mice. Trained mice received foot shocks upon entry into the designated shock zone, while non-trained mice were paired by receiving foot shocks at the same time as their trained partner. Upon completion of the 30 min APA task, mice were administered ANL and then perfused 16 hr later. (B) RC3xCamk2a-Cre mice trained in the 30 min APA task reduced the number of entries (red circles) into the shock zone (red) over the 30 min training period (n = 4 mice, one-way ANOVA, Dunnett’s MCT, **p≤0.01). (C) Following spatial LTM formation, total protein synthesis was significantly increased in the hippocampal neurons of RC3xCamk2a-Cre mice. This was reflected by the increased FUNCAT signal observed in trained compared to non-trained mice (n = 4 mice, 30 neurons per mouse, Student’s paired t-test, *p≤0.05). Scale bar = 40 µm. (D) Volcano plot representing the relative abundance of de novo synthesised proteins in the hippocampus of trained and non-trained RC3xCam2ka-Cre mice. In total, 1782 proteins were quantified in four trained and non-trained mice each, using BONCAT-SWATH-MS. Proteins which were significantly increased in synthesised in trained mice (fold-change ≥1.5, p≤0.05) are shown in red, whereas proteins which exhibited significantly decreased synthesis (fold-change ≤0.66, p≤0.05) are shown in blue (n = 4 mice, Student’s t-test). Subsequently validated proteins are encircled in green (see Figure 5).

Figure 3—source data 1. ANL treated 30 minute APA behavioural data for protemic analysis.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. RC3xCamk2a-Cre mice treated with ANL and trained in the 30 min APA task were able to recall the location of the shock zone a week after training.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

RC3xCamk2a-Cre treated with either ANL or PBS immediately after training showed a reduced number of shock zone entries in probe trials held 16 hr and one week following training (n = 4 mice, two way ANOVA, Dunnett’s MCT, ***p≤0.001, ****p≤0.0001). There was no significant difference in probe trial performance between ANL- and PBS-treated mice (two-way ANOVA, Sidak’s MCT, n.s. = non significant, p=0.99).
Figure 3—figure supplement 1—source data 1. ANL treated 30 minute APA behavioural data.
Figure 3—figure supplement 2. The spatial long-term memory formation observed in the 30 min APA task is dependent upon protein synthesis.

Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

RC3xCamk2a-Cre mice were trained with the 30 min APA task, then administered ANL and then either the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin, or PBS. Mice were subsequently tested for their ability to form spatial LTMs using a probe trial held 16 hr following training. Unlike the PBS-treated control, anisomycin-treated mice did not show a reduced number of shock zone entries in the probe trail (n = 4 mice, two way ANOVA, Dunnett’s MCT, n.s. = non significant, p=0.51), and had significantly more shock zone entries than the PBS control (two-way ANOVA, Sidak’s MCT, **p≤0.01), indicating that anisomycin-treatment blocked spatial LTM formation. Anisomcyin-treatment was confirmed to block hippocampal protein synthesis using FUNCAT staining.
Figure 3—figure supplement 2—source data 1. Anisomycin treated 30 minute APA behavioural data.
Figure 3—figure supplement 3. Proteomic analysis of Anl labelled proteins from trained and non-trained RC3xCamk2a-Cre mice.

Figure 3—figure supplement 3.

(A) The overlap of peptides purified by BONCAT from ANL-treated RC3xCamk2a mice compared to background. Using 2D LC MS/MS, 7105 unique peptides were identified from ANL-treated trained and non-trained RC3xCamk2a-Cre mice following BONCAT purification. In order to control for background purification, we removed 119 peptides which had been identified in BONCAT purification from PBS-treated negative control animals. The remaining 6986 peptides were then used to form a custom peptide library for SWATH-MS analysis. (B) Heatmap of the relative abundance in individual samples of the 1782 de novo synthesised proteins from trained and non-trained mice. Euclidian cluster analysis revealed that trained RC3xCamk2a-Cre mice cluster separately from their non-trained RC3xCamk2a-Cre pairs.

Next, we identified which proteins were altered in synthesis in hippocampal neurons during spatial LTM formation using mass spectrometry-based proteomics. For this, ANL-labelled proteins from trained and non-trained mice were purified using BONCAT, and analysed via sequential window acquisition of all theoretical fragment ions mass spectrometry (SWATH-MS). Of the 1782 proteins quantified, 156 were identified to have significantly altered synthesis (|FC| ≥ 1.5, p≤0.05) in trained mice compared to non-trained controls, comprising of 88 proteins showing increased and 68 proteins showing decreased synthesis (Figure 3D). Euclidian clustering analysis revealed that the de novo proteomes of trained mice were more similar to each other than those of their paired non-trained controls, demonstrating a distinctive change in the proteome composition of the experimental groups (Figure 3—figure supplement 3B).

The 156 proteins with significantly altered synthesis during spatial LTM formation were next analysed using STRING network analysis. Of these proteins, 125 (≈80%) formed a highly interconnected, large network, with a median of four protein-protein interactions per node (Figure 4). Using the Molecular Complex Detection (MCODE) clustering algorithm (Bader and Hogue, 2003), we identified five distinct clusters of de novo synthesised proteins (Figure 4). Gene ontology (GO), KEGG and Reactome analysis revealed that the proteins in these clusters were associated with mRNA splicing, ATP hydrolysis coupled proton transport, vesicle-mediated transport, biogenesis of complex I, and Rho GTPase effectors (Figure 4).

Figure 4. Network analysis reveals that the synthesis of distinct clusters of proteins is significantly altered during spatial LTM formation.

Figure 4.

Network analysis using the STRING database reveals that of the 156 proteins identified to be altered in synthesis during spatial LTM formation (|fold-change| ≥ 1.5, p≤0.05), 125 (≈80%) showed evidence of interaction (STRING score cut off ≥0.4) with at least one other significantly regulated protein, forming a highly interconnected network. Within this network, there was a median of 4 interactions per node. MCODE cluster analysis revealed the presence of 5 distinct clusters which were associated with mRNA splicing, ATP hydrolysis-coupled proton transport, vesicle-mediated transport, biogenesis of mitochondrial complex I, and Rho GTPase effectors. Proteins in clusters are depicted by a coloured border and are magnified in the inserts. The distance between each node is representative of the STRING score. Proteins which did not display interactions are not shown. The absolute fold-change is represented by the node size, and the directionality of the fold-change by the node colour.

Validation of key proteins identified in the de novo proteomic analysis using BONCAT western blotting

Given that our de novo proteomic analysis revealed distinct changes in hippocampal protein synthesis during LTM formation, we next performed a validation of a subset of proteins within our identified clusters using BONCAT-WB. This revealed increased synthesis of α-adaptin (Ap2a1), neuron specific enolase (NSE: Eno2), V-ATPase subunit B2 (V-ATPase B2: Atp6v1b2), and the α isoform of the structural subunit A of protein phosphatase 2A (PP2A-A: Ppp2r1a), in the hippocampal neurons of trained mice (Figure 5). We also confirmed that the synthesis of ARE/poly(U)-binding/degradation factor 1 (AUF-1: Hnrnpd) was decreased 16 hr following training (Figure 5). Lastly, as an unaltered control, we examined the synthesis of the housekeeping protein glyceraldehyde 3-phosphate dehydrogenase (GAPDH). As expected from our BONCAT-SWATH-MS analysis, BONCAT-WB revealed that its synthesis was not altered during spatial LTM formation (Figure 5). Together, these results validate our de novo proteomic analysis and provide further evidence that the formation of spatial LTM is associated with regulated changes in the synthesis of specific proteins in hippocampal neurons.

Figure 5. Validation of de novo proteomic comparison of trained vs non-trained.

RC3xCamk2a-Cre mice using BONCAT western blotting BONCAT-WB confirms that in trained mice, there is increased hippocampal synthesis of α- α-adaptin (Ap2a1), neuron specific enolase (NSE: Eno2), V-ATPase subunit B2 (V-ATPase B2: Atp6v1b2), and the α isoform of the structural subunit of protein phosphatase 2A (PP2A-A: Ppp2r1a) compared to non-trained controls. Synthesis of the ARE binding protein ARE/poly(U)-binding/degradation factor 1 (AUF-1: Hnrnpd) was also confirmed to be decreased during spatial LTM formation. The synthesis of the housekeeping gene, glyceraldehyde 3-phosphate dehydrogenase (GAPDH), was unchanged in the hippocampal neurons of trained RC3xCamk2a-Cre mice compared to non-trained controls (p=0.48), together validating the SWATH analysis (n = 4 mice, Student’s paired t-test, *p≤0.05, **p≤0.01, ***p≤0.001).

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Complete images of the western blots shown in Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

Discussion

In our study, we used NCAA labelling in combination with FUNCAT, BONCAT and SWATH quantitative proteomics to examine how de novo protein synthesis is altered in the initial stages of spatial LTM consolidation. Using FUNCAT labelling with the non-canonical amino acid AHA, we first observed that as expected, protein synthesis was increased in the hippocampus of mice during spatial LTM formation (Jarome and Helmstetter, 2014). We further refined our approach by generating the RC3 mouse strain, which, upon crossing with the Camk2a-Cre strain and administration of ANL, allowed for neuron-specific labelling of de novo synthesised proteins specifically in the hippocampus. Using BONCAT in combination with SWATH-MS quantitative de novo proteomics, we identified a total of 1782 proteins which were newly synthesised in hippocampal neurons, 156 of which were found to be significantly altered in synthesis following spatial LTM formation, with BONCAT-WB being used to independently validate a subset of these proteins. These regulated proteins are involved in a wide range of cellular functions, including mRNA splicing, ATP hydrolysis-coupled proton transport, vesicle-mediated transport, biogenesis of mitochondrial complex I, and signalling through Rho GTPases. Our de novo proteomic data suggest a role for these proteins, and the pathways they are involved in, in spatial LTM formation and, more generally, demonstrate that NCAA labelling is a robust, non-biased experimental strategy to examine how the de novo proteome is altered during complex rodent behaviour.

Previously, NCAA labelling has been used in rodents to observe changes in de novo protein synthesis during development (Calve et al., 2016), to compare different cell-types following environmental enrichment (Alvarez-Castelao et al., 2017), and to examine changes which occur in mouse models of disease (Evans et al., 2019; McClatchy et al., 2015). To the best of our knowledge, the current study represents the first use of NCAA labelling to examine spatial LTM formation in mice.

NCAAs, such as AHA and ANL, have typically been delivered to mice through diet, with labelling periods ranging from 6 to 21 days (Alvarez-Castelao et al., 2017; McClatchy et al., 2015). Spatial LTM consolidation is dependent on multiple temporally spaced phases of protein synthesis (Fioriti et al., 2015; Meiri and Rosenblum, 1998; Ozawa et al., 2017), with the first 24 hr following training being the most sensitive to the inhibition of protein synthesis (Freeman et al., 1995; Quevedo et al., 1999). Given that the majority of changes to the hippocampal de novo proteome are thought to occur within this time frame, we considered the longer labelling periods used in previous studies not to be suitable for examining spatial LTM-induced protein synthesis. We therefore delivered AHA and ANL via intraperitoneal injection rather than dietary administration, enabling us to label newly synthesised proteins within a 16 hr time window (Figure 2F). An additional advantage of labelling for this shorter period is the increased temporal specificity, with a higher proportion of tagged proteins being synthesised only in response to the behavioural paradigm studied, compared to those that would be synthesised after a longer period of time.

Using ANL labelling in combination with SWATH-MS, we obtained a similar degree of proteome coverage to the only other study we are aware of that has used ANL labelling in mice (Alvarez-Castelao et al., 2017). However, given that the neuronal proteome is believed to consist of around 6000 proteins (Schanzenbächer et al., 2016), more invasive delivery techniques such as cranially implanted osmotic pumps may be required to achieve a deeper degree of labelling than observed in either study.

In order to investigate protein synthesis specifically during spatial LTM formation, we established an accelerated version of the five-day APA paradigm, which relies upon a singular training event (Figure 1C), making it easier to capture the time-period during which spatial LTM formation occurs. We confirmed that this 30 min APA task induces spatial LTM by examining two defining characteristics of long-term memory; the ability to persist for long periods of time and the requirement of new protein synthesis (Figure 3—figure supplement 1, Figure 3—figure supplement 2) (Costa-Mattioli et al., 2009; Rosenberg et al., 2014).

Mice that are exposed to a novel environments and stimuli could potentially experience increased levels of stress while undertaking behavioural tasks, which may alter protein synthesis (Chandran et al., 2013; Moncada and Viola, 2007). We controlled for this by using paired, yoked controls and confirmed that these non-trained mice showed similar levels of stress as their trained counterparts (Figure 1—figure supplement 2). We therefore conclude that the observed changes in protein synthesis between trained and non-trained mice were likely induced by spatial LTM formation, rather than confounding factors including training and stress.

In our analysis we focused on the hippocampus, as protein synthesis in this region plays a critical role in spatial LTM (Burgess et al., 2002; Jarome and Helmstetter, 2014; Kleinknecht et al., 2012; Merlo et al., 2015). As our initial AHA labelling and FUNCAT analysis confirmed that protein synthesis is altered in the hippocampus during spatial LTM formation (Figure 1E), we chose to use cell-type-specific NCAA labelling in order to restrict de novo protein labelling to hippocampal neurons in vivo.

To restrict the expression of NLL-MetRS to hippocampal neurons, we crossed RC3 mice with the Camk2a-Cre T29-1Stl/J mouse strain. When originally generated, this mouse strain expressed Cre-recombinase only in the CA1 pyramidal neurons of the hippocampus and the testes (Tsien et al., 1996); however, more recent analyses revealed expression in other regions of the hippocampus (Alvarez-Castelao et al., 2017; McGill et al., 2017), possibly reflecting a genetic drift. Our results are consistent with these more recent studies, with RC3xCamk2a-Cre mice expressing NLL-MetRS-EGFP in the neurons of the CA1, CA2, CA3, as well as dentate gyrus regions of the hippocampus (Figure 2D).

Recently, another Cre-dependent mouse strain was established that expresses the mutant tRNA synthetase MetRS-L247G (Alvarez-Castelao et al., 2017). Unlike NLL-MetRS, which is considered to label proteins with ANL only at their amino-terminus (Ngo et al., 2013), MetRS-L247G also replaces internal methionine residues with ANL (Müller et al., 2015; Yang et al., 2018), which likely results in a wider range of protein labelling.

By combining hippocampal neuron-specific ANL labelling with BONCAT-SWATH-MS, we identified 156 proteins which were significantly altered in synthesis during spatial LTM formation induced by the 30 min APA paradigm (Figure 3D). While these changes represent a small proportion of the total-ANL proteome, these findings are in line with previous studies assessing the total proteome in different stages of memory consolidation (Borovok et al., 2016). STRING analysis of the 156 significantly altered proteins revealed that the vast majority of these proteins interact with a least one other significantly altered protein, forming a large, highly interconnected network (Figure 4). This high degree of interconnection would suggest that, as expected, spatial LTM formation induces changes not only in the synthesis of specific proteins but also pathways.

Interestingly, while FUNCAT analysis revealed an overall increase in the amount of new protein synthesis during spatial LTM formation (Figure 3B), our de novo proteomic analysis identified a similar number of proteins with decreased and increased synthesis (68 and 88, respectively) in the 16 hr following training. This would suggest that rather than simply inducing the synthesis of memory-related proteins, spatial LTM memory formation causes neurons to selectively modulate certain pathways and molecular processes.

MCODE analysis of the network-regulated proteins identified five distinct clusters of newly synthesised proteins. These were associated with mRNA splicing, ATP hydrolysis-coupled proton transport, vesicle-mediated transport, biogenesis of mitochondrial complex I, and Rho GTPase effectors. We confirmed the validity of our de novo analysis by examining a number of proteins from these clusters using BONCAT-WB, with all of the examined proteins showing similar changes in synthesis for both detection methods (Figure 5).

Many of the proteins and pathways identified by our analysis have previously been associated with memory formation. Examples are α- and β-adaptin (Ap2b1), which were increased in synthesis during spatial LTM formation (Figure 4 and Figure 5). These two proteins are subunits of the clathrin adaptor protein 2 (AP2) complex, which regulates synaptic levels of GluR1- and GluR2/GluR3-containing AMPA receptors (Garafalo et al., 2015). This process is important in the memory-related processes of long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression (LTD) (Barth and Wheeler, 2008; Hardt et al., 2014), as well as complex rodents behaviours such as visual recognition memory, which is disrupted when the interaction between AP2 and the GluR2 subunit is blocked (Griffiths et al., 2008).

Another memory-related protein identified to be altered in synthesis in our de novo proteomic analysis was the structural subunit A of protein phosphatase 2A, PP2A-A. Both BONCAT-SWATH-MS and BONCAT-WB found that the α isoform of PP2A-A was increased in synthesis during the 16 hr following training (Figure 3D and Figure 5). Inhibition of PP2A has been demonstrated to block LTP (Belmeguenai, 2005), with hippocampal PP2A knock-out animals presenting altered extinction of long-term memory (Wang et al., 2019).

Our de novo proteomic analysis also identified several molecular processes which have yet to be robustly linked to spatial LTM formation. One such process is the regulation of mRNA splicing. We observed that during memory and learning, there was increased synthesis of pre-mRNA-processing factor 31 (Prp31) and β-catenin-like protein 1 (Ctnnbl1), both of which are required for the formation and activation of the spliceosome (Ganesh et al., 2011; Yuan et al., 2005) (Figure 4), while the synthesis of the pre-mRNA-splicing factor ATP-dependent RNA helicase DHX15 (DHX15), which is involved in the disassembly of spliceosomes, was decreased (Wen et al., 2008) (Figure 4). We further observed decreased synthesis of serine/arginine-rich splicing factor 6 (SRSF6), which inhibits the splicing of exon 10 of the microtubule associated protein tau (Yin et al., 2012). Taken together, these results suggest that during spatial LTM formation, there may be an alteration the activity of spliceosomes, leading to altered splicing of certain mRNAs such as MAPT, although further examination will be required to confirm if these molecular process are altered in spatial LTM.

The current working model of spatial LTM formation suggests that following spatial training, hippocampal neurons undergo protein synthesis in response to certain stimuli (Kandel et al., 2014; Squire et al., 2015). In our study, we used cell-type-specific NCAA labelling, in combination with a novel 30 min APA protocol and quantitative SWATH-MS de novo proteomics, to examine how the hippocampal de novo proteome is changed during spatial LTM formation. We found that in hippocampal neurons, there was altered synthesis of specific sets of proteins associated with a diverse, yet interconnected set of neuronal pathways and cellular processes following spatial training. In addition to observing altered synthesis of a number proteins already associated with memory, such as α-adaptin, β-adaptin and PP2A-A, we also identified alterations in mRNA splicing as a potential neuronal mechanism which underpins spatial LTM formation. More generally, our findings highlight the potential of cell-type specific NCAA labelling using transgenic mouse strains such as the RC3 mice as a robust tool for identifying and characterising cell-type-specific changes in de novo protein synthesis that occur in response to a wide range of both physiological and pathological stimuli, including complex rodent behaviours.

Materials and methods

Animals and ethics

4–5 month old female C57BL/6, ROSA26a Cre-inducible Click chemistry (RC3), and RC3 mice crossed with Camk2a-Cre T29-1 Stl/j mice (Jackson Labs, 0005359) were used. Mice were provided access to food and water and housed on a 12 hr light/dark cycle. All experiments were approved by and carried out in accordance with the guidelines of the Animal Ethics Committee of the University of Queensland (QBI/554/17/NHMRC).

Generation of RC3 mouse strain

The RC3 donor plasmid was obtained by subcloning the NLL-MetRS-EGFP transgene into the ROSA26A-targetting mammalian expression vector Ai2 (Ai2 was a gift from Hongkui Zeng, Addgene plasmid #22796). The RC3 transgenic mouse strain was generated by CRISPR/CAS9-mediated insertion of the linearized RC3 donor into the ROSA26 locus using a previously published sgRNA (Chu et al., 2015). The CAS9-sgRNA complexes and linearized donor plasmid were introduced into fertilised eggs by pronuclear injection as previously described, with minor modifications (Ittner and Götz, 2007). Offspring were genotyped by PCR using EGFP genotyping primers, with positive pups being confirmed by Sanger sequencing conducted at the Australian Equine Genetics Research Centre (AEGRC).

Non-canonical amino acid treatment of mice

The non-canonical amino acid AHA (ThermoFisher, C10102) was dissolved in phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) and administered to wild-type mice as previously described (Evans et al., 2019). Optimal ANL (Jena Bioscience, CLK-AA009) labelling conditions were examined using BONCAT-WB. A dosage of 100 µg Anl/gbw and a labelling period of 16 hr resulted in maximal labelling and was used for all further experiments.

After treatment, mice were deeply anaesthetised with pentobarbitone sodium and then intracardially perfused with 25 mL of PBS. The brains were subsequently dissected, with one hemisphere being processed for immunohistochemistry and FUNCAT, and the other for BONCAT.

Behavioural analysis

Spatial memory was assessed using a modified APA test. In brief, mice were trained over 30 min to use spatial cues to avoid a shock zone within an arena rotating at 1 rpm. Mice were handled daily for 2 min over a seven day period, prior to being habituated to the rotating arena during a 30 min exploration session. 24 hr after habituation, mice were placed into the arena with a fixed 60° shock zone extending from the centre point of the arena to the southern side of the room.

Mice were separated into two groups, trained and non-trained. Trained mice received a 0.5 mA shock upon entry into the shock zone, with an entrance shock delay of 0.5 s, and a 1.5 s interval between shocks. The number of shocks received and the number of entries into the shock zone were analysed in 5 min intervals. In the non-trained group, mice were paired to individual littermates from the trained group, receiving shocks at the same time as the trained mouse, irrespective of the spatial location of the non-trained mouse. Behavioural analysis was performed at the same time on sequential days in order to control from differences in protein synthesis due to circadian rhythm. Following sacrifice, further sample preparation and analysis for trained and non-trained mice was performed simultaneously.

In order to determine if the mice trained with the 30 min APA underwent spatial LTM formation, they were assessed for their ability to recall the location of the shock zone in 5 min probe trials, where mice did not receive shocks, conducted either 16 hr, 24 hr or 1 week after training. Probe trials were also used to confirm that NCAA treatment did not interfere with spatial LTM formation. In experiments where protein synthesis was inhibited, mice were administered anisomycin (Sigma-Aldrich, A9789) 150 μg/gwb via subcutaneous injection immediately after training as this has been previously demonstrated to inhibit hippocampal protein synthesis for >9 hr (Wanisch and Wotjak, 2008). For all trained and non-trained experiments, mice were administered 50 µg AHA/gbw or 100 µg ANL/gbw immediately after training and were perfused 16 hr later without undergoing a probe trial.

Plasma corticosterone quantification

In order to assess stress levels in mice during the 30 min APA, trained, non-trained and naïve (habituated but not shocked) mice were sacrificed immediately following their respective behaviour tasks. Blood was then collected via cardiac puncture and left to clot for 1 hr, with plasma then being collected via centrifugation. Plasma corticosterone levels were quantified in three mice from each group via ELISA which was performed in triplicate (Enzo Life Sciences, ADI-900–097).

FUNCAT and immunohistochemical analysis

Following PBS perfusion, brain hemispheres were fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde for 24 hr and then placed in cryoprotectant solution (30% glycerol, 30% ethylene glycol in 1x PBS) for 48 hr at 4°C. 25 μm thick free floating sections were then cut between Bregma −1.34 and −2.06 μm using a vibratome (Leica VT1000).

Prior to FUNCAT staining sections were placed in blocking solution (1% bovine serum albumin (BSA), 0.05% Tween in PBS) for 1 hr at room temperature, with three sections per mouse being analysed. AHA- and ANL- labelled proteins were then visualised by incubating sections with 6.25 μM Alexa555-DIBO (ThermoFisher, C20021) in blocking solution overnight at 4°C under constant agitation. Neurons were visualised by staining with a MAP2 antibody (Abcam, ab5392, 1:1000) and anti-chicken Alexa Fluor647 (ThermoFisher, A21449, 1:1000). Sections were washed repeatedly with 0.05% Tween in PBS and then stained with DAPI. As negative controls, sections of PBS-treated mice stained with Alexa555-DIBO were used throughout all experiments. Images were taken using a Zeiss 710 laser scanning confocal microscope.

BONCAT purification

Following PBS perfusion the hippocampus of RC3xCamk2a-Cre mice was dissected. The samples were snap-frozen and then extracted in radioimmunoprecipitation assay (RIPA) buffer (Cell Signalling, 9806) as previously described (Bodea et al., 2017), with protein concentrations being determined using the bicinchoninic acid (BCA) assay (ThermoFisher, 23225).

BONCAT purification was carried out as previously described (Evans et al., 2019). Briefly, for samples to be analysed via western blot following BONCAT purification (BONCAT-WB), 100 μg of protein was used. Proteins were first alkylated with iodoacetamide (IAA) as this has been shown to reduce non-specific reactions when using the strain-promoted azide-alkyne cycloaddition (van Geel et al., 2012). Anl labelled proteins were then reacted with 100 μM DIBO-biotin (Click Chemistry Tools, A112) for 2 hr at room temperature. 40 μg of streptavidin-coated Dynabeads (ThermoFisher, 11205D) were then used to purify biotinylated proteins, with beads being washed multiple times with IP wash buffer (0.1% SDS and 0.05% Tween in Tris-buffered saline (TBS)). Bound proteins were removed from the beads by boiling in 1x Laemmli buffer.

For BONCAT-SWATH-MS analysis, samples were purified as above, but using 250 μg of protein and 100 μg of streptavidin-coated Dynabeads per sample. Beads were then washed in IP wash buffer and resuspended in TBS.

Western blot analysis

Following BONCAT purification, equal volumes of the elution fraction were loaded and separate by SDS-PAGE and analysed via western blotting as previously described (Evans et al., 2019). The total amount of newly synthesised proteins was quantified using the REVERT total protein stain (LI-COR, 926–11010), with representative proteins for each cluster were detected using the following primary antibodies: α adaptin (ThermoFisher, MA3-061, 1:500), neuron specific enolase (NSE) (Abcam, ab53025, 1:500), AT6V1B2 (Abcam, ab73404, 1:500), PP2A-A (Sigma-Aldrich, 07–250, 1:1000), AUF-1 (Sigma-Aldrich, 07–260, 1:500), and GAPDH (Millipore, MAB374, 1:1000).

BONCAT-SWATH-MS analysis

In order to identify proteins newly synthesised during spatial memory formation, Anl-labelled proteins from both trained and non-trained RC3xCamk2a-Cre mice were analysed by BONCAT-SWATH-MS mostly as previously described (Evans et al., 2019). Briefly, BONCAT-purified proteins bound to beads from four trained and four untrained samples were placed in Triethylammonium bicarbonate (TEAB) buffer and subsequently reduced with DTT, followed by alkylation with iodoacetamide. Samples were then digested with 80 ng of trypsin overnight. For generation of the custom ion library, samples were then pulled and resuspended in 5 mM ammonium hydroxide solution (pH 10.5). Peptides were then fractionated using high pH RP-HPLC, with the resulting 17 fractions being analysed via non-LC MS/MS to form the custom ion library used for SWATH analysis. In order to control for background purified proteins, BONCAT purification was performed on an RC3xCamk2a-Cre PBS-treated negative control. Peptides identified from this negative control were excluded from further analysis, in addition to peptides identified in previous BONCAT-SWATH-MS negative control experiments (Figure 3—figure supplement 3A, Supplementary file 1).

Using this custom ion library, samples were then analysed using SWATH-MS using a false discovery rate (FDR) of 1% for peptide and protein identification as previously described (Evans et al., 2019).

Bioinformatic analysis SWATH-MS data

Proteins identified using SWATH-MS were statistically compared using paired t-tests. We opted to use an unadjusted p-value cut off of p≤0.05 and an absolute fold-change cut off of ≥1.5 to identify proteins with significantly altered synthesis. This is because we observed that correcting for multiple comparisons greatly reduced the number of significantly altered proteins from 156 to 27, resulting in the exclusion of a number of true positives, such as NSE and AUF-1, which were confirmed to be altered in synthesis using BONCAT-WB (Figure 5Supplementary file 2). This culling effect on true positives is commonly observed in proteomics studies with smaller sample sizes using multiple comparisons (Pascovici et al., 2016). Previous SWATH-MS experiments using spiked lysates have demonstrated that the cut offs used in our study result in estimated quantitative FDRs of <10% (Wu et al., 2016). Similar cut offs were also used in other studies (Ganief et al., 2017; Liu et al., 2019). Network analysis was performed using Cytoscape (v3.6.0). Data from the SWATH-MS analysis were mapped to the STRING protein query database for Mus musculus using the UniProt identifier. A confidence of interaction score cut-off of 0.4 was used. A network map of the 156 proteins which exhibited significantly altered synthesis in trained mice was then generated using the edge-weighted spring-embedded layout. Clusters of regulated proteins were identified using Molecular Complex Detection (MCODE). The proteins in these clusters where then analysed using the GO, KEGG and Reactome databases. Cluster names, which were both informative and contained a majority of proteins within the cluster, were manually assigned. Heatmapper was used to perform a Euclidian clustering analysis of the relative abundance of all 1782 quantified proteins for each sample (Babicki et al., 2016).

Image analysis

Analysis of images obtained by confocal microscopy was performed blinded and carried out using ImageJ. Protein synthesis in AHA-treated mice was quantified by measuring the median FUNCAT intensity in rectangular regions of interest of the same size around the CA1 neurons of the hippocampus, with each data point representing the mean of three sections analysed per animal. For RC3xCamk2a-Cre mice, the presence of NLL-MetRS-EGFP expression enabled FUNCAT intensity to be quantified per neuron. ANL labelling was quantified by measuring the median FUNCAT intensity, with each data point representing the mean of 30 hippocampal CA1 neurons analysed per animal. Western blots were analysed using the LI-COR Light Studio software.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was performed in GraphPad Prism 7.0 software using Student’s paired t-test, Student’s unpaired t-test, one way ANOVA, or two-way ANOVA, with Tukey’s multiple comparisons test (MCT), Sidak’s MCT, or Dunnett’s MCT being used as appropriate. All values are given as mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM).

Data availability

The RAW mass spectrometry proteomics data used in this study has been deposited to the ProteomeXchange Consortium via the PRIDE partner repository (Perez-Riverol et al., 2019) with the dataset identifier PXD015820.

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank Daniel Blackmore, Jessica Barbizzi, Tishila Palliyaguru, Linda Cumner and Trish Hitchcock for their excellent technical support along with Rowan Tweedale for critical reading of the manuscript. We also like to thank Xiaomin Song and the Australian Proteomic Analysis Facility for performing the SWATH-MS analysis. This research was supported by the Estate of Dr Clem Jones AO, the State Government of Queensland, the Federal Government of Australia (ACT900116), and by grants from the Australian Research Council (ARC DP160103812). LGB is supported by the Peter Hilton Fellowship.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Liviu-Gabriel Bodea, Email: l.bodea@uq.edu.au.

Jürgen Götz, Email: j.goetz@uq.edu.au.

Sacha B Nelson, Brandeis University, United States.

Laura L Colgin, University of Texas at Austin, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • The Estate of Dr Clem Jones AO to Jürgen Götz.

  • Australian Research Council ARC DP160103812 to Jürgen Götz.

  • Queensland Government ACT900116 to Jürgen Götz.

  • The Peter Hilton Fellowship to Liviu-Gabriel Bodea.

  • National Health and Medical Research Council GNT11457569 to Liviu-Gabriel Bodea, Jürgen Götz.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All experiments were approved by and carried out in accordance with the guidelines of the Animal Ethics Committee of the University of Queensland (QBI/554/17/NHMRC).

Additional files

Supplementary file 1. List of peptides identified by 2D-LC MS/MS from BONCAT purified from ANL-treated RC3xCamk2a-Cre mice and PBS-treated negative controls.
elife-52990-supp1.xlsx (140.6KB, xlsx)
Supplementary file 2. SWATH-MS data from quantification of BONCAT purified proteins from trained and non-trained RC3xCamk2a-Cre.

For each of the 1782 proteins quantified, the name, Uniprot identifier, gene name, fold change in trained compared to non-trained mice, p-value, Benjamini and Hochberg adjusted p-value, number of peptides sequences used for quantified, and the normalised protein peak area are given. Proteins contained within clusters identified by MCODE analysis are highlighted.

elife-52990-supp2.xlsx (373.1KB, xlsx)
Transparent reporting form

Data availability

All data in this study is presented in the included manuscript, supporting files, and source data files. The proteomics data set has been uploaded to the PRIDE repository with the dataset identifier PXD015820.

The following dataset was generated:

Evans HT, Bodea LG, Götz J. 2019. Cell-specific non-canonical amino acid labelling identifies changes in the hippocampal de novo proteome during memory formation. PRIDE. PXD015820

References

  1. Alkon DL, Epstein H, Kuzirian A, Bennett MC, Nelson TJ. Protein synthesis required for long-term memory is induced by PKC activation on days before associative learning. PNAS. 2005;102:16432–16437. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0508001102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alvarez-Castelao B, Schanzenbächer CT, Hanus C, Glock C, Tom Dieck S, Dörrbaum AR, Bartnik I, Nassim-Assir B, Ciirdaeva E, Mueller A, Dieterich DC, Tirrell DA, Langer JD, Schuman EM. Cell-type-specific metabolic labeling of nascent proteomes in vivo. Nature Biotechnology. 2017;35:1196–1201. doi: 10.1038/nbt.4016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Alvarez-Castelao B, Schanzenbächer CT, Langer JD, Schuman EM. Cell-type-specific metabolic labeling, detection and identification of nascent proteomes in vivo. Nature Protocols. 2019;14:556–575. doi: 10.1038/s41596-018-0106-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Babicki S, Arndt D, Marcu A, Liang Y, Grant JR, Maciejewski A, Wishart DS. Heatmapper: web-enabled heat mapping for all. Nucleic Acids Research. 2016;44:W147–W153. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkw419. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bader GD, Hogue CW. An automated method for finding molecular complexes in large protein interaction networks. BMC Bioinformatics. 2003;4:2. doi: 10.1186/1471-2105-4-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Barth AL, Wheeler ME. The barista on the bus: cellular and synaptic mechanisms for visual recognition memory. Neuron. 2008;58:159–161. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Beatty KE, Fisk JD, Smart BP, Lu YY, Szychowski J, Hangauer MJ, Baskin JM, Bertozzi CR, Tirrell DA. Live-Cell imaging of cellular proteins by a Strain-Promoted Azide-Alkyne cycloaddition. ChemBioChem. 2010;11:2092–2095. doi: 10.1002/cbic.201000419. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Belmeguenai A. A role for protein phosphatases 1, 2A, and 2B in cerebellar Long-Term potentiation. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:10768–10772. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2876-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bodea LG, Evans HT, Van der Jeugd A, Ittner LM, Delerue F, Kril J, Halliday G, Hodges J, Kiernan MC, Götz J. Accelerated aging exacerbates a pre-existing pathology in a tau transgenic mouse model. Aging Cell. 2017;16:377–386. doi: 10.1111/acel.12565. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Borovok N, Nesher E, Levin Y, Reichenstein M, Pinhasov A, Michaelevski I. Dynamics of hippocampal protein expression during Long-term spatial memory formation. Molecular & Cellular Proteomics. 2016;15:523–541. doi: 10.1074/mcp.M115.051318. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bouabe H, Okkenhaug K. Gene targeting in mice: a review. Methods in Molecular Biology. 2013;1064:315–336. doi: 10.1007/978-1-62703-601-6_23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Burgess N, Maguire EA, O'Keefe J. The human Hippocampus and spatial and episodic memory. Neuron. 2002;35:625–641. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00830-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Calve S, Witten AJ, Ocken AR, Kinzer-Ursem TL. Incorporation of non-canonical amino acids into the developing murine proteome. Scientific Reports. 2016;6:e32377. doi: 10.1038/srep32377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chandran A, Iyo AH, Jernigan CS, Legutko B, Austin MC, Karolewicz B. Reduced phosphorylation of the mTOR signaling pathway components in the amygdala of rats exposed to chronic stress. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 2013;40:240–245. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Chu VT, Weber T, Wefers B, Wurst W, Sander S, Rajewsky K, Kühn R. Increasing the efficiency of homology-directed repair for CRISPR-Cas9-induced precise gene editing in mammalian cells. Nature Biotechnology. 2015;33:543–548. doi: 10.1038/nbt.3198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Clopath C. Synaptic consolidation: an approach to long-term learning. Cognitive Neurodynamics. 2012;6:251–257. doi: 10.1007/s11571-011-9177-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Costa-Mattioli M, Sossin WS, Klann E, Sonenberg N. Translational control of long-lasting synaptic plasticity and memory. Neuron. 2009;61:10–26. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.10.055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Davis HP, Squire LR. Protein synthesis and memory: a review. Psychological Bulletin. 1984;96:518–559. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.96.3.518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dieterich DC, Link AJ, Graumann J, Tirrell DA, Schuman EM. Selective identification of newly synthesized proteins in mammalian cells using bioorthogonal noncanonical amino acid tagging (BONCAT) PNAS. 2006;103:9482–9487. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601637103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Erdmann I, Marter K, Kobler O, Niehues S, Abele J, Müller A, Bussmann J, Storkebaum E, Ziv T, Thomas U, Dieterich DC. Cell-selective labelling of proteomes in Drosophila Melanogaster. Nature Communications. 2015;6:7521. doi: 10.1038/ncomms8521. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Evans HT, Benetatos J, van Roijen M, Bodea LG, Götz J. Decreased synthesis of ribosomal proteins in tauopathy revealed by non-canonical amino acid labelling. The EMBO Journal. 2019;38:e101174. doi: 10.15252/embj.2018101174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Fioriti L, Myers C, Huang Y-Y, Li X, Stephan JS, Trifilieff P, Colnaghi L, Kosmidis S, Drisaldi B, Pavlopoulos E, Kandel ER. The persistence of Hippocampal-Based memory requires protein synthesis mediated by the Prion-like protein CPEB3. Neuron. 2015;86:1433–1448. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Freeman FM, Rose SP, Scholey AB. Two time windows of anisomycin-induced amnesia for passive avoidance training in the day-old chick. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 1995;63:291–295. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1995.1034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Ganesh K, Adam S, Taylor B, Simpson P, Rada C, Neuberger M. CTNNBL1 is a novel nuclear localization Sequence-binding protein that recognizes RNA-splicing factors CDC5L and Prp31. Journal of Biological Chemistry. 2011;286:17091–17102. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M110.208769. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Ganief T, Gqamana P, Garnett S, Hoare J, Stein DJ, Joska J, Soares N, Blackburn JM. Quantitative proteomic analysis of HIV-1 Tat-induced dysregulation in SH-SY5Y neuroblastoma cells. Proteomics. 2017;17:1600236. doi: 10.1002/pmic.201600236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Garafalo SD, Luth ES, Moss BJ, Monteiro MI, Malkin E, Juo P. The AP2 clathrin adaptor protein complex regulates the abundance of GLR-1 glutamate receptors in the ventral nerve cord of Caenorhabditis elegans. Molecular Biology of the Cell. 2015;26:1887–1900. doi: 10.1091/mbc.E14-06-1048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gong S, Miao YL, Jiao GZ, Sun MJ, Li H, Lin J, Luo MJ, Tan JH. Dynamics and correlation of serum cortisol and corticosterone under different physiological or stressful conditions in mice. PLOS ONE. 2015;10:e0117503. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0117503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Griffiths S, Scott H, Glover C, Bienemann A, Ghorbel MT, Uney J, Brown MW, Warburton EC, Bashir ZI. Expression of long-term depression underlies visual recognition memory. Neuron. 2008;58:186–194. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hafner A-S, Donlin-Asp PG, Leitch B, Herzog E, Schuman EM. Local protein synthesis is a ubiquitous feature of neuronal pre- and postsynaptic compartments. Science. 2019;364:eaau3644. doi: 10.1126/science.aau3644. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hardt O, Nader K, Wang Y-T. GluA2-dependent AMPA receptor endocytosis and the decay of early and late long-term potentiation: possible mechanisms for forgetting of short- and long-term memories. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2014;369:20130141. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hinz FI, Dieterich DC, Schuman EM. Teaching old NCATs new tricks: using non-canonical amino acid tagging to study neuronal plasticity. Current Opinion in Chemical Biology. 2013;17:738–746. doi: 10.1016/j.cbpa.2013.07.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Inda MC, Delgado-García JM, Carrión AM. Acquisition, consolidation, reconsolidation, and extinction of eyelid conditioning responses require de novo protein synthesis. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:2070–2080. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4163-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Ittner LM, Götz J. Pronuclear injection for the production of transgenic mice. Nature Protocols. 2007;2:1206–1215. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2007.145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Jarome TJ, Helmstetter FJ. Protein degradation and protein synthesis in long-term memory formation. Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience. 2014;7:1–12. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2014.00061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kandel ER, Dudai Y, Mayford MR. The molecular and systems biology of memory. Cell. 2014;157:163–186. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kleinknecht KR, Bedenk BT, Kaltwasser SF, Grünecker B, Yen Y-C, Czisch M, Wotjak CT. Hippocampus-dependent place learning enables spatial flexibility in C57BL6/N mice. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2012;6:87. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2012.00087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Li C, Götz J. Somatodendritic accumulation of tau in Alzheimer's disease is promoted by Fyn-mediated local protein translation. The EMBO Journal. 2017;36:3120–3138. doi: 10.15252/embj.201797724. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Liang V, Ullrich M, Lam H, Chew YL, Banister S, Song X, Zaw T, Kassiou M, Götz J, Nicholas HR. Altered proteostasis in aging and heat shock response in C. elegans revealed by analysis of the global and de novo synthesized proteome. Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences. 2014;71:3339–3361. doi: 10.1007/s00018-014-1558-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Link AJ, Vink MKS, Agard NJ, Prescher JA, Bertozzi CR, Tirrell DA. Discovery of aminoacyl-tRNA synthetase activity through cell-surface display of noncanonical amino acids. PNAS. 2006;103:10180–10185. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601167103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Liu C, Oveissi S, Downs R, Kirby J, Nedeva C, Puthalakath H, Faou P, Duan M, Chen W. Semiquantitative proteomics enables mapping of murine neutrophil dynamics following lethal influenza virus infection. The Journal of Immunology. 2019;203:1064–1075. doi: 10.4049/jimmunol.1900337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Lopez J, Gamache K, Schneider R, Nader K. Memory retrieval requires ongoing protein synthesis and NMDA receptor Activity-Mediated AMPA receptor trafficking. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:2465–2475. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0735-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Mayford M, Siegelbaum SA, Kandel ER. Synapses and memory storage. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology. 2012;4:a005751. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a005751. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. McClatchy DB, Ma Y, Liu C, Stein BD, Martínez-Bartolomé S, Vasquez D, Hellberg K, Shaw RJ, Yates JR. Pulsed azidohomoalanine labeling in mammals (PALM) Detects changes in Liver-Specific LKB1 knockout mice. Journal of Proteome Research. 2015;14:4815–4822. doi: 10.1021/acs.jproteome.5b00653. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. McGill BE, Barve RA, Maloney SE, Strickland A, Rensing N, Wang P, Wong M, Head R, Wozniak DF, Milbrandt J. Abnormal microglia and enhanced Inflammation-Related gene transcription in mice with conditional deletion of ctcf in Camk2a-Cre -Expressing neurons. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2017;38:0936. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0936-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Meiri N, Rosenblum K. Lateral ventricle injection of the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin impairs long-term memory in a spatial memory task. Brain Research. 1998;789:48–55. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(97)01528-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Merlo E, Bekinschtein P, Jonkman S, Medina JH. Molecular mechanisms of memory consolidation, reconsolidation, and persistence. Neural Plasticity. 2015;2015:1–2. doi: 10.1155/2015/687175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Moncada D, Viola H. Induction of long-term memory by exposure to novelty requires protein synthesis: evidence for a behavioral tagging. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:7476–7481. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1083-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Müller A, Stellmacher A, Freitag CE, Landgraf P, Dieterich DC. Monitoring astrocytic proteome dynamics by cell Type-Specific protein labeling. PLOS ONE. 2015;10:e0145451. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0145451. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Ngo JT, Schuman EM, Tirrell DA. Mutant methionyl-tRNA synthetase from Bacteria enables site-selective N-terminal labeling of proteins expressed in mammalian cells. PNAS. 2013;110:4992–4997. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1216375110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Ozawa T, Yamada K, Ichitani Y. Differential requirements of hippocampal de novo protein and mRNA synthesis in two long-term spatial memory tests: spontaneous place recognition and delay-interposed radial maze performance in rats. PLOS ONE. 2017;12:e0171629. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0171629. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Pascovici D, Handler DC, Wu JX, Haynes PA. Multiple testing corrections in quantitative proteomics: a useful but blunt tool. Proteomics. 2016;16:2448–2453. doi: 10.1002/pmic.201600044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Paul CM, Magda G, Abel S. Spatial memory: theoretical basis and comparative review on experimental methods in rodents. Behavioural Brain Research. 2009;203:151–164. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.05.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Perez-Riverol Y, Csordas A, Bai J, Bernal-Llinares M, Hewapathirana S, Kundu DJ, Inuganti A, Griss J, Mayer G, Eisenacher M, Pérez E, Uszkoreit J, Pfeuffer J, Sachsenberg T, Yilmaz S, Tiwary S, Cox J, Audain E, Walzer M, Jarnuczak AF, Ternent T, Brazma A, Vizcaíno JA. The PRIDE database and related tools and resources in 2019: improving support for quantification data. Nucleic Acids Research. 2019;47:D442–D450. doi: 10.1093/nar/gky1106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Plath N, Ohana O, Dammermann B, Errington ML, Schmitz D, Gross C, Mao X, Engelsberg A, Mahlke C, Welzl H, Kobalz U, Stawrakakis A, Fernandez E, Waltereit R, Bick-Sander A, Therstappen E, Cooke SF, Blanquet V, Wurst W, Salmen B, Bösl MR, Lipp HP, Grant SG, Bliss TV, Wolfer DP, Kuhl D. Arc/Arg3.1 is essential for the consolidation of synaptic plasticity and memories. Neuron. 2006;52:437–444. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.08.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Poucet B, Lenck-Santini PP, Paz-Villagrán V, Save E. Place cells, neocortex and spatial navigation: a short review. Journal of Physiology-Paris. 2003;97:537–546. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2004.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Quevedo J, Vianna MR, Roesler R, de-Paris F, Izquierdo I, Rose SP. Two time windows of anisomycin-induced amnesia for inhibitory avoidance training in rats: protection from amnesia by pretraining but not pre-exposure to the task apparatus. Learning & Memory. 1999;6:600–607. doi: 10.1101/lm.6.6.600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Rosenberg T, Gal-Ben-Ari S, Dieterich DC, Kreutz MR, Ziv NE, Gundelfinger ED, Rosenblum K. The roles of protein expression in synaptic plasticity and memory consolidation. Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience. 2014;7:1–14. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2014.00086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Schanzenbächer CT, Sambandan S, Langer JD, Schuman EM. Nascent proteome remodeling following homeostatic scaling at hippocampal synapses. Neuron. 2016;92:358–371. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.09.058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Squire LR, Genzel L, Wixted JT, Morris RG. Memory consolidation. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology. 2015;7:a021766. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a021766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Tsien JZ, Chen DF, Gerber D, Tom C, Mercer EH, Anderson DJ, Mayford M, Kandel ER, Tonegawa S. Subregion- and cell Type–Restricted Gene Knockout in Mouse Brain. Cell. 1996;87:1317–1326. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(00)81826-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Ullrich M, Liang V, Chew YL, Banister S, Song X, Zaw T, Lam H, Berber S, Kassiou M, Nicholas HR, Götz J. Bio-orthogonal labeling as a tool to visualize and identify newly synthesized proteins in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nature Protocols. 2014;9:2237–2255. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2014.150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. van Geel R, Pruijn GJ, van Delft FL, Boelens WC. Preventing thiol-yne addition improves the specificity of strain-promoted azide-alkyne cycloaddition. Bioconjugate Chemistry. 2012;23:392–398. doi: 10.1021/bc200365k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Wang J, Xie R, Kou X, Liu Y, Qi C, Liu R, You W, Gao J, Gao X. A protein phosphatase 2A deficit in the hippocampal CA1 area impairs memory extinction. Molecular Brain. 2019;12:51. doi: 10.1186/s13041-019-0469-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Wanisch K, Wotjak CT. Time course and efficiency of protein synthesis inhibition following intracerebral and systemic anisomycin treatment. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2008;90:485–494. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Wen X, Tannukit S, Paine ML. TFIP11 interacts with mDEAH9, an RNA helicase involved in spliceosome disassembly. International Journal of Molecular Sciences. 2008;9:2105–2113. doi: 10.3390/ijms9112105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Wu JX, Song X, Pascovici D, Zaw T, Care N, Krisp C, Molloy MP. SWATH mass spectrometry performance using extended peptide MS/MS assay libraries. Molecular & Cellular Proteomics : MCP. 2016;15:2501–2514. doi: 10.1074/mcp.M115.055558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Yang AC, du Bois H, Olsson N, Gate D, Lehallier B, Berdnik D, Brewer KD, Bertozzi CR, Elias JE, Wyss-Coray T. Multiple Click-Selective tRNA synthetases expand mammalian Cell-Specific proteomics. Journal of the American Chemical Society. 2018;140:7046–7051. doi: 10.1021/jacs.8b03074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Yin X, Jin N, Gu J, Shi J, Zhou J, Gong CX, Iqbal K, Grundke-Iqbal I, Liu F. Dual-specificity tyrosine phosphorylation-regulated kinase 1A (Dyrk1A) modulates serine/arginine-rich protein 55 (SRp55)-promoted tau exon 10 inclusion. Journal of Biological Chemistry. 2012;287:30497–30506. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M112.355412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Yuan L, Kawada M, Havlioglu N, Tang H, Wu JY. Mutations in PRPF31 inhibit pre-mRNA splicing of rhodopsin gene and cause apoptosis of retinal cells. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:748–757. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2399-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Ziegler L, Zenke F, Kastner DB, Gerstner W. Synaptic consolidation: from synapses to behavioral modeling. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:1319–1334. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3989-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Sacha B Nelson1
Reviewed by: Sacha B Nelson2, Erin Margaret Schuman3

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Thank you for submitting your work entitled "Cell-specific non-canonical amino acid labelling identifies changes in the de novo proteome during memory formation" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, including Sacha B Nelson as the Reviewing Editor and Reviewer #1, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Senior Editor. The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal their identity: Erin Schuman (Reviewer #3).

As you will see below, the reviewers identified a number of distinct and overlapping problems with the present manuscript that preclude its acceptance despite significant enthusiasm for the overall topic and several features of the study. Performing the required additional experiments and analyses is likely to require significant additional effort, but the reviewers were in agreement that several of these issues would need to be addressed to support the conclusions of the paper. It is eLife policy to reject manuscripts for which significant additional work is required. In this case, if key issues can be addressed, we would encourage a resubmission, and would endeavor to ensure that it is handled by the same reviewers, although we cannot guarantee this.

In an effort to make this process as transparent as possible, we are including the original reviews, but note that some of the issues were viewed as more critical than others. Specifically:

1) All reviewers agreed that identifying background proteins associated with the purification was a critical control (e.g. Rev 3 point 3).

2) In addition, showing that the task requires protein synthesis was felt to be important.

3) Improvement of the statistical analysis was also felt to be critical.

4) Finally, clarifying what was learned about the underlying biology was also deemed important.

In contrast, after discussion, reviewers agreed that characterization of the specificity of the driver mouse could potentially be addressed by referring to other studies. It was also felt that the small number of proteins identified could be discussed without requiring additional experiments. Finally, the concern of reviewer 1 about the possibility of stress-dependent effects rather than learning being required for the changes seen could be addressed by the purity controls and demonstration that the task requires protein synthesis.

Reviewer #1:

The authors describe creation of a transgenic mouse strain permitting cre-dependent expression of a mutant tRNA synthetase from the ROSA26 locus. The transgene permits BONCAT and FUNCAT labeling of newly synthesized proteins in specific cell types. Using this strain, they demonstrate a change in the synthesis of 167 proteins in the hippocampus following a spatial learning paradigm. Although there is a long history of showing that hippocampal learning depends on synthesis of new proteins this is one of the first attempts to provide the list of which new proteins contribute.

Despite significant enthusiasm for the overall goal, for the tool the authors have generated and for some aspects of their experiments, I am concerned about one aspect of their experimental design and one aspect of their statistical analyses.

The design compares animals that learn to avoid a shock in a spatial region with yoked control animals that receive the same shocks but cannot (by design) learn to avoid them. This is in many respects a wonderful control for the stress of animals receiving shocks. However a weakness of the design is that it may actually be much more stressful to receive repeated shocks that one cannot avoid by altering behavior, than to receive the same number of shocks but which one comes to avoid by altering one's behavior. If this is true, the change in protein synthesis could either be due to the learning or could reflect the heightened stress in the control animals. An additional set of untrained controls would resolve this.

The statistical concern has to do with the multiple comparisons that come from simultaneously examining many proteins. The authors do not use a threshold for fold-change and do not do any correction for multiple comparisons across proteins.

Reviewer #2:

Here the authors use non-canonical amino acid labeling, proteomics, immunohistochemical labeling and Western blots to identify changes in the makeup of newly-synthesized proteins in the mouse hippocampus following learning paradigm ('accelerated' active place avoidance). They first establish the learning paradigm. They then demonstrate that incorporation of AHA in hippocampi of trained mice is greater than that observed in non-trained mice. They then describe and characterize a novel mouse strain (RC3 mice) that conditionally expresses a mutant tRNA synthetase. These are crossed with CaMK2a-Cre mice resulting in selective expression of mutant tRNA synthetase in the hippocampus. They then compare the population of newly synthesized proteins in trained and non-trained mice by labeling with ANL and analyzing the ANL-containing proteins by FUNCAT, BONCAT, MS and Western blots. They report training associated changes in the synthesis of 167 proteins, which are clustered into several groups; changes for a subset of these were then verified using western blots.

The approach taken here is valid and has the potential to address many interesting and open questions related to changes in protein synthesis associated with a variety of phenomena, in this case, training on a specific task. As far as I can tell, the behavioral tasks were performed appropriately and the controls are in place. My enthusiasm is dampened, however, by the superficial analysis and treatment of the results. Consequently, the findings, as they stand, are not particularly insightful or useful. Having said this, this can be remedied by 1) deeper and better analyses and; 2) contrasting the findings with the literature.

1) The bioinformatics analyses and conclusions are rather superficial and not very convincing. First, attribution of 'change' is based on the statistical significance of a paired Students t-test (4 pairs), regardless of the magnitude of the change. Thus, for about half of such proteins, the measured change was less than 30%. Such low change thresholds are uncommon in such studies as they are rarely reliable. A reasonable way to set a threshold would be to estimate the variance within each group, and set a threshold based on, say, 2 standard variations of the internal variance. Second, the clustering into functional groups (the four distinct clusters mentioned) is questionable. For example, one of these is 'synaptic vesicle recycling'. This cluster contains only 3 protein (complexes): the vacuolar ATPase, WD Repeat Domain 7, and Dmx Like 2. These proteins have been associated with synaptic vesicles but not exclusively so, thus referring to this tiny group as a 'synaptic vesicle recycling' cluster is not justified. Third, proteins that exhibited the strongest changes are kind of ignored (for example a sodium-coupled bicarbonate transporter, 5-fold increase), or Glyoxalase I (14-fold change).

2) The details on the 167 proteins identified are scant. No table is provided in the main text that lists these proteins, their common names, the degree of change following training, the statistical significance of the change, etc. Such a table is provided as supplementary data, but no details are provided to explain it (for example, what does 'NumberPeptides' mean? How can 8 measurements be made from 1 peptide?). This being so, it is difficult to appreciate what proteins change beyond those mentioned explicitly in the text.

3) The literature on protein synthesis and learning is vast. How do prior expectations and findings compare with those described here? Given the importance attributed to local protein synthesis in synaptic plasticity, are any relationships observed between the proteins identified here and dendritic (and axonal) mRNAs identified so far? Along these lines, were postsynaptic proteins and receptors identified (I noted, for example that newly synthesized GluA1 was markedly increased in 3 of the 4 experiments)? Presynaptic proteins? (I noted changes in Synataxin 1B, Munc-18-1, Α-SNAP). Were any of the identified proteins unique in the sense that they were synthesized only after training (so called 'plasticity proteins')? This is not to criticize the veracity of the findings, but contrasting the findings with prior studies and common hypotheses on protein synthesis and memory would help clarify the insights the study provides beyond a catalog of protein names. Unfortunately, the Discussion is not enlightening in this regard as it is, for the most part, a reiteration of the results.

4) Figure 2F is rather surprising as it suggests that half of newly synthesized protein is lost within 8 hours (from 16 to 24 hours). How do such rapid degradation rates compare with the long half-lives (many days and even weeks) reported for most neuronal proteins (e.g. Fornasiero et al., 2018, PMID30315172)? What might be the explanation?

Reviewer #3:

Summary:

The authors analyze the newly-synthesized proteome of hippocampal neurons using ANL and SWATH; they investigate changes in treated/untreated animals in an active place avoidance test. They identified 700 proteins in total, observing differential expression in 167. To validate their findings, they used BONCAT-WB finding similar trends in most candidate proteins.

Scope:

LTM consolidation is of general interest. Cell-type specific proteome labeling using ncAAs has been described previously, with a much higher overall proteome coverage than reported here. From a neurobiological point of view, the topic is potentially interesting but enthusiasm is diminished by the very low proteome coverage and the absence of many important controls. Hopefully the authors conducted the control experiments but just left them out of the current manuscript. There are several major issues/suggestions, as outlined below.

1) Does long-term memory in the active place avoidance task require protein synthesis? This is an important experiment to include since it would establish that the changes in protein synthesis observed are important for the long-term change in behavior.

2)The authors are using a new mouse line that they created using CRISPR/CAS to mutagenize the methionyl t-RNA synthetase (NLL-MetRS). There is no characterization of the mouse line in the paper. While the crossed mouse uses a well-established CRE driver, each new line can be slightly different, and ectopic expression of the CRE drivers is not unusual. It is thus highly recommended to include least one staining showing the co-localization of the GFP with a neuronal marker. (In Figure 2E the authors report that GFP is expressed in neurons, but no neuronal marker is shown, just a region rich in neurons and ectopic expression cannot be evaluated.

3) Re: proteomics. There were no controls for the purification of newly-synthesized proteins included in the manuscript. Have these purification tests been done? Data should be included that compares non-specific protein absorption in control samples (from e.g. methionine-injected mutant mice or ANL injected wild-type mice, but ideally both) to specifically purified newly-synthesized proteins. This is an essential point that needs to be addressed as the purification of proteins always results in some background proteins associated with the purification procedure (stickiness of the beads). In the present manuscript, all differentially regulated proteins described in the Results section could be derived from non-specifically adsorbing proteins also from other cell types. The authors need to show their enrichment efficiency and thresholds for accepting proteins for inclusion.

4) Re: proteomics: only a very limited number of proteins were identified and quantified, likely representing the "tip of the iceberg" of the cellular proteome. Previous studies showed more than triple the numbers described in this manuscript. Can the authors comment on their relatively low protein numbers?

5) Also, data upload to an online repository is common practice nowadays, and its commendable that the authors plan to do this. PRIDE offers anonymous reviewer accounts after uploading- this allows the possibility that the reviewers can look at the data.

6) It is very interesting and surprising that the authors report a global change in protein translation 16h after a relatively short learning paradigm. Given the big increase in translation that is shown in the FUNCAT experiment (Figure 1E), it is quite surprising that the final number of differential expressed proteins is only 167, and in this regulated pool only 99/167 increased expression, while the remainder showed a decrease in expression. This regulation seems inconsistent with the global FUNCAT measurement. The proteome of single cells is estimated to include ~ 5000 proteins- making it difficult to understand how regulation of 167- with about half going up and half going down- would have a visible effect on the total visualized new proteome. How do the authors explain this?

7) How were the experimental and control MS samples handled? were the 4 control mice samples obtained at the same time, clicked and then analyzed at the same time as the experimental samples (or with interleaving of experimental and control samples)?

8) Data analysis: The authors should provide an analysis of the overlap in replicates for the MS experiments with ANL labelled proteins and the negative control.

9) How were the MS data normalized?

[Editors’ note: what now follows is the decision letter after the authors submitted for further consideration.]

Thank you for submitting your article "Cell-specific non-canonical amino acid labelling identifies changes in the de novo proteome during memory formation" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, including Sacha B Nelson as the Reviewing Editor and Reviewer #1, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and Laura Colgin as the Senior Editor. The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal their identity: Erin Margaret Schuman (Reviewer #3).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Essential revisions:

The statistical issues raised by reviewer 1 and 3 have not been adequately addressed. It is necessary to compute a false discovery rate and to provide the additional replicate information requested by reviewer #3.

Reviewer #1:

The authors have revised their manuscripts and performed some of the requested additional control experiments and analyses. Importantly, they have addressed the key issues of background labeling, the dependence of the learning task on new protein synthesis, and to some degree, the statistical analysis of the data.

I leave it to other more expert reviewers to address the question of whether the issues of background have been adequately dealt with, and to assess whether or not the question of what has been learned from the study has been adequately discussed. The experiments do appear to confirm that the learning requires new protein synthesis. The statistical approach is improved but still lacks adequate control for multiple comparisons. The expected false discovery rate has not been addressed.

In the future it would be helpful for the authors to include the specific changes in the manuscript made with explicit references to where in the manuscript these are to be found.

Reviewer #2:

The authors have addressed the points I raised in my original review. I have no further major comments.

Technology-wise, the approach and genetic model developed will probably be useful for many projects in the future.

As to what has been learned on relationships between protein synthesis and spatial learning, matters remain fuzzy. Perhaps this might be expected in such unbiased studies, and explanatory context might emerge in future studies. Alternatively, perhaps the fuzziness is part of the answer (in analogy to realizations emerging from genome-wide association studies aiming to identify genetic sources of complex traits/diseases).

Time will tell.

Reviewer #3:

The authors have improved the manuscript and now show the data from control experiments. On a positive note, their S/N ratio for ANL-containing proteins compared to PBS-treated control is very good (6986 peptides vs. 207 shared peptides, Figure 3—figure supplement 3A).

Two major remaining points.

Re: statistical analysis of significantly regulated proteins (reviewer 1 comment 2 and reviewer 2 comment 1). The authors have used an absolute fold-change cut-off when the standard in the proteomics field is to use a false-discovery-rate. The papers that the authors cite in their response to the reviewers actually use an FDR- not a simple threshold. I don't understand the reluctance to use an FDR- the Volcano plot looks "ok" (evenly distributed clouds, reasonable shape). The authors should use the FDR method to report statistical significance.

• Biological replicates and experiment numbers: The authors report that they have 3 replicates per experiment (3 mice), but the information on whether the mice belong to the same litter or how many independent experiments were conducted is still missing. This is a key point to understand the power of the data shown. The authors should clearly state litters, biological and technical replicates. Example; In the slice experiment Figure 1E, it is stated that there are 4 mice per experiment, is not clear if they imaged one slice per mouse or more. Again not clear if they are from the same litter and/or experiment. This should be clarified. Related to this- it is desirable if the data uploaded on PRIDE can also be clearly recognized as biological and technical replicates and if the file names used make sense and are easy to cross-walk with the manuscript.

eLife. 2020 Jan 6;9:e52990. doi: 10.7554/eLife.52990.sa2

Author response


[Editors’ note: the author responses to the first round of peer review follow.]

[…] In contrast, after discussion, reviewers agreed that characterization of the specificity of the driver mouse could potentially be addressed by referring to other studies. It was also felt that the small number of proteins identified could be discussed without requiring additional experiments. Finally, the concern of reviewer 1 about the possibility of stress-dependent effects rather than learning being required for the changes seen could be addressed by the purity controls and demonstration that the task requires protein synthesis.

Reviewer #1:

The authors describe creation of a transgenic mouse strain permitting cre-dependent expression of a mutant tRNA synthetase from the ROSA26 locus. The transgene permits BONCAT and FUNCAT labeling of newly synthesized proteins in specific cell types. Using this strain, they demonstrate a change in the synthesis of 167 proteins in the hippocampus following a spatial learning paradigm. Although there is a long history of showing that hippocampal learning depends on synthesis of new proteins this is one of the first attempts to provide the list of which new proteins contribute.

Despite significant enthusiasm for the overall goal, for the tool the authors have generated and for some aspects of their experiments, I am concerned about one aspect of their experimental design and one aspect of their statistical analyses.

The design compares animals that learn to avoid a shock in a spatial region with yoked control animals that receive the same shocks but cannot (by design) learn to avoid them. This is in many respects a wonderful control for the stress of animals receiving shocks. However a weakness of the design is that it may actually be much more stressful to receive repeated shocks that one cannot avoid by altering behavior, than to receive the same number of shocks but which one comes to avoid by altering one's behavior. If this is true, the change in protein synthesis could either be due to the learning or could reflect the heightened stress in the control animals. An additional set of untrained controls would resolve this.

We share this reviewer’s concern that stress levels may be increased in the yoked compared to learning mice because the former cannot avoid the shocks by altering their behaviour due to learning. However, when we analyse plasma levels of corticosterone, a known stress-related hormone (Gong, et al.,2015), we find that while stress levels were higher in mice which received shocks compared to a naïve non-shocked control group, there was no difference in corticosterone levels between trained and yoked controls (that both received shocks (see updated Figure 1—figure supplement 1). This suggests that the observed changes in protein synthesis are likely due to spatial LTM formation and not heightened stress levels.

The statistical concern has to do with the multiple comparisons that come from simultaneously examining many proteins. The authors do not use a threshold for fold-change and do not do any correction for multiple comparisons across proteins.

We agree with the reviewer that a more stringent statistical analysis is required for our de novoproteomic analysis. In the revised manuscript, instead of using 1D-LC SWATH-MS, we used 2D-LC SWATH-MS which allowed us to identify 1,782 newly synthesised proteins. We used a p value cut-off of ≤0.05 and an absolute fold-change cut-off of ≥1.5, as widely used in SWATH-MS (Wu, et al., 2016, Pascovici et al., 2016). Using these cut-off values, we identified 156 proteins which were significantly altered in synthesis during the 16 hours following training (updated Figure 3D).

Reviewer #2:

[…] The approach taken here is valid and has the potential to address many interesting and open questions related to changes in protein synthesis associated with a variety of phenomena, in this case, training on a specific task. As far as I can tell, the behavioral tasks were performed appropriately and the controls are in place. My enthusiasm is dampened, however, by the superficial analysis and treatment of the results. Consequently, the findings, as they stand, are not particularly insightful or useful. Having said this, this can be remedied by 1) deeper and better analyses and; 2) contrasting the findings with the literature.

1) The bioinformatics analyses and conclusions are rather superficial and not very convincing. First, attribution of 'change' is based on the statistical significance of a paired Students t-test (4 pairs), regardless of the magnitude of the change. Thus, for about half of such proteins, the measured change was less than 30%. Such low change thresholds are uncommon in such studies as they are rarely reliable. A reasonable way to set a threshold would be to estimate the variance within each group, and set a threshold based on, say, 2 standard variations of the internal variance.

As discussed in our second response to reviewer 1, we agree that a more stringent statistical analysis was required for our de novoproteomic analysis. In the revised manuscript we used a p value cut-off of ≤0.05 and an absolute fold-change cut-off of ≥1.5.

Second, the clustering into functional groups (the four distinct clusters mentioned) is questionable. For example, one of these is 'synaptic vesicle recycling'. This cluster contains only 3 protein (complexes): the vacuolar ATPase, WD Repeat Domain 7, and Dmx Like 2. These proteins have been associated with synaptic vesicles but not exclusively so, thus referring to this tiny group as a 'synaptic vesicle recycling' cluster is not justified.

We agree that the way we initially discussed the clusters may have been misleading. We used cluster names to inform about the possible neuronal function of the proteins in these cluster. Unfortunately, GO analysis and other similar techniques are general tools that are not designed to examine changes in individual cell-types, such as neurons. Also, given that we are examining newly synthesised proteins, rather than the whole proteome, it is unlikely that we will identify a high proportion of any given GO category. Therefore, in the revised manuscript, we used the GO, Reactome, and KEGG databases to manually assign informative names and possible neuronal functions to these clusters. We also clarify how these names were selected in the Materials and methods section of the revised manuscript.

Third, proteins that exhibited the strongest changes are kind of ignored (for example a sodium-coupled bicarbonate transporter, 5-fold increase), or Glyoxalase I (14-fold change).

We agree that the potential role in spatial LTM of individual proteins which displayed large changes in synthesis, such as those pointed out by this reviewer, cannot be disregarded. In our initial analysis we selected candidate proteins using two criteria, fold-change and number of interactions with other regulated proteins. Many of the proteins with the strongest changes, such as those pointed out be the reviewer, did not show evidence of interaction with any other changed proteins, leading us to focus on pathways where multiple proteins were altered in synthesis. However, this lack of observed interaction was likely due to the overly stringent STRING score cut-off used (≥0.7). In the revised manuscript, we used the far more commonly used STRING score cut-off of ≥0.4, resulting in many proteins with large fold-changes being observed in our clusters. Two examples of these are pre-mRNA-processing factor 31 (Prp31) (FC=4.3) and dynactin subunit 3 (Dctn3) (FC=-6.9) (updated Figure 4).

2) The details on the 167 proteins identified are scant. No table is provided in the main text that lists these proteins, their common names, the degree of change following training, the statistical significance of the change, etc. Such a table is provided as supplementary data, but no details are provided to explain it (for example, what does 'NumberPeptides' mean? How can 8 measurements be made from 1 peptide?). This being so, it is difficult to appreciate what proteins change beyond those mentioned explicitly in the text.

We apologise for not clarifying the meaning of the data in the supplementary tables and have fixed this in the revised manuscript. In regards to the reviewer’s question about the number of peptides, we like to clarify that this number represents the number of peptide sequences that were used to identify and then quantify a particular protein. This means that for proteins where the number of peptides was one, one unique peptide sequence was used to identify this protein, with the levels of this peptide being quantified across all 8 samples. We have added this information in the new submission.

3) The literature on protein synthesis and learning is vast. How do prior expectations and findings compare with those described here? Given the importance attributed to local protein synthesis in synaptic plasticity, are any relationships observed between the proteins identified here and dendritic (and axonal) mRNAs identified so far? Along these lines, were postsynaptic proteins and receptors identified (I noted, for example that newly synthesized GluA1 was markedly increased in 3 of the 4 experiments)? Presynaptic proteins? (I noted changes in Synataxin 1B, Munc-18-1, Α-SNAP).

Indeed, in addition to the proteins pointed out by the reviewer, we also observed changes in α- and β-adaptin, as well as PP2A, all of which have been shown to be involved in memory, further confirming the validity of our analysis. We expand on this in the Discussion of the revised manuscript. However, the main advantage of our approach is the nonbiased nature of the de novoproteomic analysis which allowed an identification of both known and potential novel memory-related proteins and mechanisms. One such novel mechanism which is hinted at by our proteomics finding is mRNA splicing, as discussed in detail in the revised manuscript.

Were any of the identified proteins unique in the sense that they were synthesized only after training (so called 'plasticity proteins')? This is not to criticize the veracity of the findings, but contrasting the findings with prior studies and common hypotheses on protein synthesis and memory would help clarify the insights the study provides beyond a catalog of protein names. Unfortunately, the Discussion is not enlightening in this regard as it is, for the most part, a reiteration of the results.

Using SWATH-MS, a “presence vs. absence” type of analysis typically seen in non-quantitative proteomic analysis method is not feasible. We do not discount the presence of proteins which are exclusively synthesised during memory formation. However, in our study, we identified fairly even numbers of proteins that were increased and decreased in synthesis (Figure 3D), which would suggest that instead of merely synthesising specific proteins to enable memory formation, neurons must instead dynamically adjust selected pathways and processes during spatial LTM formation. We discuss this in the tenth paragraph of the Discussion.

4) Figure 2F is rather surprising as it suggests that half of newly synthesized protein is lost within 8 hours (from 16 to 24 hours). How do such rapid degradation rates compare with the long half-lives (many days and even weeks) reported for most neuronal proteins (e.g. Fornasiero et al., 2018, PMID30315172)? What might be the explanation?

In Figure 2F, we do find a 30% reduction in the BONCAT signal between 16 and 24 hours, reminiscent of what we had observed earlier for AHA labelling. Together, this suggests that NCAA availability becomes limited at later time points (Evans et al., 2019). While neuronal proteins have an average half-life of around 4 days, there are many proteins which are degraded much faster (Dörrbaum et al., eLife, 2018). It is also likely that these shorter-lived proteins are synthesised more regularly, and as such are more likely to incorporate ANL (Schanzenbächer et al., 2016), which would explain the observed decrease in signal after 16 hours. This bias towards shorter-lived proteins is inherent in all techniques which rely upon protein labelling to examine de novoprotein synthesis.

Reviewer #3:

[…] LTM consolidation is of general interest. Cell-type specific proteome labeling using ncAAs has been described previously, with a much higher overall proteome coverage than reported here. From a neurobiological point of view, the topic is potentially interesting but enthusiasm is diminished by the very low proteome coverage and the absence of many important controls. Hopefully the authors conducted the control experiments but just left them out of the current manuscript. There are several major issues/suggestions, as outlined below.

1) Does long-term memory in the active place avoidance task require protein synthesis? This is an important experiment to include since it would establish that the changes in protein synthesis observed are important for the long-term change in behavior.

We agree with this reviewer that it is important to confirm that the spatial LTM formation induced by our accelerated APA as behavioural paradigm is dependent on protein synthesis. For the revised manuscript, we therefore administered the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin to RC3xCamk2a-Cre mice immediately after training with the 30 minute APA test. In the probe trail 16 hours later, unlike PBS-treated mice, mice that had been administered anisomycin were unable to recall the location of the shock zone (Figure 3—figure supplement 2). This confirms that the spatial LTM formation observed in the 30 minute APA test requires new protein synthesis.

2)The authors are using a new mouse line that they created using CRISPR/CAS to mutagenize the methionyl t-RNA synthetase (NLL-MetRS). There is no characterization of the mouse line in the paper. While the crossed mouse uses a well-established CRE driver, each new line can be slightly different, and ectopic expression of the CRE drivers is not unusual. It is thus highly recommended to include least one staining showing the co-localization of the GFP with a neuronal marker. (In Figure 2E the authors report that GFP is expressed in neurons, but no neuronal marker is shown, just a region rich in neurons and ectopic expression cannot be evaluated.

We agree with this reviewer. In the revised manuscript we have included microscopy images revealing that the NLL-MetRS-EGFP and FUNCAT signal is restricted to hippocampal neurons, using MAP2 as a neuronal marker (Figure 2D).

3) Re: proteomics. There were no controls for the purification of newly-synthesized proteins included in the manuscript. Have these purification tests been done? Data should be included that compares non-specific protein absorption in control samples (from e.g. methionine-injected mutant mice or ANL injected wild-type mice, but ideally both) to specifically purified newly-synthesized proteins. This is an essential point that needs to be addressed as the purification of proteins always results in some background proteins associated with the purification procedure (stickiness of the beads). In the present manuscript, all differentially regulated proteins described in the Results section could be derived from non-specifically adsorbing proteins also from other cell types. The authors need to show their enrichment efficiency and thresholds for accepting proteins for inclusion.

We apologise to this reviewer for not clearly communicating the use of purification controls in our initial submission. We have presented these important controls in the revised manuscript. For all de novoproteomic experiments, we controlled for background-purified proteins using PBS-treated controls. In the revised manuscript, we used 2D/LC MS/MS to identify peptides purified by BONCAT from both ANL- and PBS-treated RC3xCamk2a-Cre mice. Any peptides identified in the PBS-negative control were then excluded from the custom ion library used for SWATH-analysis. In addition to this, we excluded any peptides that were identified in the negative control of our previous BONCAT analysis (Evans et al., 2019). In total, 6,986 peptides were identified exclusively in ANL-treated samples, with 119 peptides being found in both ANL- and PBS-treated samples, and 86 peptides being exclusively found in PBS-treated negative controls (Figure 3—figure supplement 3A). We included this information in the Materials and methods section of the revised manuscript. We also added a list of the peptides found in the experimental and negative control groups to Supplementary file 1.

4) Re: proteomics: only a very limited number of proteins were identified and quantified, likely representing the "tip of the iceberg" of the cellular proteome. Previous studies showed more than triple the numbers described in this manuscript. Can the authors comment on their relatively low protein numbers?

In the revised manuscript we have now used 2D-LC SWATH-MS which increased the number of de novosynthesised proteins to 1,782 (updated Figure 3D). This is similar to the number of proteins identified in other in vivoexperiments which have used ANL labelling (Alvarez-Castelao et al., 2017). We do agree with the reviewer that this likely only represents the “tip of the iceberg”, as in vitroexperiments using primary neurons have identified ≈6,000 proteins (Schanzenbächer et al., 2016). This deviation is likely due to the concentration of ANL being higher in these experiments compared to our in vivolabelling. We have added these considerations to the fourth paragraph of the Discussion.

5) Also, data upload to an online repository is common practice nowadays, and its commendable that the authors plan to do this. PRIDE offers anonymous reviewer accounts after uploading- this allows the possibility that the reviewers can look at the data.

We included the PRIDE repository information in the revised version of the manuscript. The dataset identifier is PXD015820.

6) It is very interesting and surprising that the authors report a global change in protein translation 16h after a relatively short learning paradigm. Given the big increase in translation that is shown in the FUNCAT experiment (Figure 1E), it is quite surprising that the final number of differential expressed proteins is only 167, and in this regulated pool only 99/167 increased expression, while the remainder showed a decrease in expression. This regulation seems inconsistent with the global FUNCAT measurement. The proteome of single cells is estimated to include ~ 5000 proteins- making it difficult to understand how regulation of 167- with about half going up and half going down- would have a visible effect on the total visualized new proteome. How do the authors explain this?

As pointed out by this reviewer, FUNCAT analysis revealed an overall increase in newly synthesised proteins during the 16 hour after training. This was also confirmed by BONCAT-WB, by using the total protein stain REVERT (Figure 5). In the revised manuscript, we used a more stringent statistical cut-off for our BONCAT-SWATH-MS de novoproteomics analysis and identified 156 proteins, which were significantly altered in synthesis during spatial LTM formation (Figure 3D), with 88 proteins being increased in synthesis and 68 proteins being decreased in synthesis.

We do not consider these two methods being directly comparable, because FUNCAT and BONCAT-WB are able to detect and measure the total amount of ANL labelled proteins, whereas BONCAT-SWATH-MS can only compare the relative levels of individual proteins. Therefore, proteins which have a higher initial copy number will have a larger impact on the FUNCAT signal that those with a lower initial copy number, even though BONCAT-SWATH-MS may find a similar alteration in fold-change.

We do, however, find it interesting that we observed many proteins which were decreased in synthesis in our de novoproteomic analysis. As pointed out in the sixth response to reviewer 2, this would suggest that instead of merely synthesising small sets of proteins required for memory formation, neurons may modulate selected proteins and pathways during spatial LTM formation. We discuss this in the tenth paragraph of the Discussion.

7) How were the experimental and control MS samples handled? were the 4 control mice samples obtained at the same time, clicked and then analyzed at the same time as the experimental samples (or with interleaving of experimental and control samples)?

Trained and non-trained mice undertook the 30 minute APA at similar times but on sequential days, in order to rule out any changes in protein synthesis due to differences in circadian cycle. Once mice were sacrificed and proteins extracted in RIPA buffer, the samples were clicked and analysed simultaneously.

8) Data analysis: The authors should provide an analysis of the overlap in replicates for the MS experiments with ANL labelled proteins and the negative control.

As pointed out in our response to comment 3, in the new submission we have provided a list of the peptides that were identified in the experimental and negative control samples.

9) How were the MS data normalized?

In regards to the SWATH-MS data, they were normalised by log transforming the protein peak area and normalising this to the total protein peak area for each run, as previously described by us (Evans et al., 2019).

[Editors' note: the author responses to the re-review follow.]

Essential revisions:

The statistical issues raised by reviewer 1 and 3 have not been adequately addressed. It is necessary to compute a false discovery rate and to provide the additional replicate information requested by Reviewer #3.

Reviewer #1:

The authors have revised their manuscripts and performed some of the requested additional control experiments and analyses. Importantly, they have addressed the key issues of background labeling, the dependence of the learning task on new protein synthesis, and to some degree, the statistical analysis of the data.

I leave it to other more expert reviewers to address the question of whether the issues of background have been adequately dealt with, and to assess whether or not the question of what has been learned from the study has been adequately discussed. The experiments do appear to confirm that the learning requires new protein synthesis. The statistical approach is improved but still lacks adequate control for multiple comparisons. The expected false discovery rate has not been addressed.

We agree with this reviewer that, in the proteomics field, it is common to use corrections for multiple comparisons using methods such as the Benjamini and Hochberg procedure. However in our dataset, only 27 proteins remained after adjusting the p value ≤ 0.05. This is common in SWATH-MS studies with low power, such as ours, with multiple testing corrections being recorded to often exclude a high proportion of true positives and thus masking the outcomes of a given study (Ganief et al., 2017; Liu et al., 2019). This notion is further supported by experiments using spiked lysates, which have demonstrated that the corrections can dramatically blunt the SWATH-MS analysis (Wu et al., 2016). This would explain why in our study, we found proteins with adjusted p values > 0.05 (such as NSE, α-adaptin, and AUF1) to be significantly altered in synthesis during spatial LTM formation as shown by western blot analysis (Figure 5). In fact, studies which have directly compared the use of unadjusted and adjusted p-values in the context of cost-prohibitive, low-power proteomic analysis have found that there is little advantage in using FDR-adjusted p-values compared to unadjusted p-values in combination with fold-change cut-offs (Pascovici et a., 2016). We therefore feel that our decision to use unadjusted p-values in combination with a fold-change cut-off is justifiable. For transparency, we have now addressed this issue and included a methodological justification in the revised manuscript (subsection “Bioinformatic analysis SWATH-MS data”).

In the future it would be helpful for the authors to include the specific changes in the manuscript made with explicit references to where in the manuscript these are to be found.

The manuscript went through re-submission and thus, we have treated the manuscript as a new submission, i.e. we have not highlighted the modifications to the original submission. In this resubmission, modifications have been highlighted.

Reviewer #3:

The authors have improved the manuscript and now show the data from control experiments. On a positive note, their S/N ratio for ANL-containing proteins compared to PBS-treated control is very good (6986 peptides vs. 207 shared peptides, Figure 3—figure supplement 3A).

Two major remaining points.

Re: statistical analysis of significantly regulated proteins (Reviewer 1 comment 2 and Reviewer 2 comment 1). The authors have used an absolute fold-change cut-off when the standard in the proteomics field is to use a false-discovery-rate. The papers that the authors cite in their response to the reviewers actually use an FDR- not a simple threshold. I don't understand the reluctance to use an FDR- the Volcano plot looks "ok" (evenly distributed clouds, reasonable shape). The authors should use the FDR method to report statistical significance.

We refer the reviewer to our response to reviewer #1 comment 1.

• Biological replicates and experiment numbers: The authors report that they have 3 replicates per experiment (3 mice), but the information on whether the mice belong to the same litter or how many independent experiments were conducted is still missing. This is a key point to understand the power of the data shown. The authors should clearly state litters, biological and technical replicates. Example; In the slice experiment Figure 1E, it is stated that there are 4 mice per experiment, is not clear if they imaged one slice per mouse or more. Again not clear if they are from the same litter and/or experiment. This should be clarified. Related to this- it is desirable if the data uploaded on PRIDE can also be clearly recognized as biological and technical replicates and if the file names used make sense and are easy to cross-walk with the manuscript.

We have addressed these concerns in our revised manuscript by stating more clearly when technical and biological replicates were used. Regarding the PRIDE repository of our data, we have uploaded more files to this repository to make it easier for readers to use our data.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Evans HT, Bodea LG, Götz J. 2019. Cell-specific non-canonical amino acid labelling identifies changes in the hippocampal de novo proteome during memory formation. PRIDE. PXD015820 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 1—source data 1. WT 30 minute APA behavioural data.
    Figure 1—figure supplement 1—source data 1. AHA treated 30 minute APA behavioural data.
    Figure 3—source data 1. ANL treated 30 minute APA behavioural data for protemic analysis.
    Figure 3—figure supplement 1—source data 1. ANL treated 30 minute APA behavioural data.
    Figure 3—figure supplement 2—source data 1. Anisomycin treated 30 minute APA behavioural data.
    Supplementary file 1. List of peptides identified by 2D-LC MS/MS from BONCAT purified from ANL-treated RC3xCamk2a-Cre mice and PBS-treated negative controls.
    elife-52990-supp1.xlsx (140.6KB, xlsx)
    Supplementary file 2. SWATH-MS data from quantification of BONCAT purified proteins from trained and non-trained RC3xCamk2a-Cre.

    For each of the 1782 proteins quantified, the name, Uniprot identifier, gene name, fold change in trained compared to non-trained mice, p-value, Benjamini and Hochberg adjusted p-value, number of peptides sequences used for quantified, and the normalised protein peak area are given. Proteins contained within clusters identified by MCODE analysis are highlighted.

    elife-52990-supp2.xlsx (373.1KB, xlsx)
    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    The RAW mass spectrometry proteomics data used in this study has been deposited to the ProteomeXchange Consortium via the PRIDE partner repository (Perez-Riverol et al., 2019) with the dataset identifier PXD015820.

    All data in this study is presented in the included manuscript, supporting files, and source data files. The proteomics data set has been uploaded to the PRIDE repository with the dataset identifier PXD015820.

    The following dataset was generated:

    Evans HT, Bodea LG, Götz J. 2019. Cell-specific non-canonical amino acid labelling identifies changes in the hippocampal de novo proteome during memory formation. PRIDE. PXD015820


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES