Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Jan 1.
Published in final edited form as: Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019 Nov 8;108:322–340. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.11.004

Opposite-sex and same-sex twin studies of physiological, cognitive and behavioral traits

Linda Juel Ahrenfeldt 1,2, Kaare Christensen 1,2,3,4, Nancy L Segal 5, Yoon-Mi Hur 6,*
PMCID: PMC6949417  NIHMSID: NIHMS1545889  PMID: 31711815

Abstract

A scientific interest in opposite-sex (OS) twins comes from animal studies showing hormone transfer between fetuses in utero. A parallel effect in humans may occur, especially for OS females who may be exposed to androgens, in particular testosterone, from the male co-twin. Conversely, OS males may be exposed to lower levels of prenatal testosterone than do same-sex (SS) males. In this special issue, we reviewed published studies investigating potential differences between OS and SS twins in physiological, cognitive and behavioral traits focusing on the Twin Testosterone Transfer (TTT) hypothesis. Sixty articles fulfilled the eligibility criteria including 23 studies published since the review by Tapp et al. (2011). In general, studies of cognition are conflicting, but it is the phenotype for which most support for the TTT hypothesis is found. Less consistent evidence has been found regarding physiological and behavioral traits. We hope that this special issue will stimulate a discussion about how an investigation of the TTT hypothesis should continue in future research.

Keywords: Opposite-sex, Same-sex, Twins, Sex differences, Testosterone, Masculinization, Physiology, Cognition, Behavior

1. Twins

Twin pregnancies are characterized by simultaneous development of two fetuses sharing the uterus. There are two kinds of twins: monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) twins. MZ twins are derived from one fertilized egg, from which two separate embryos later emerge and are genetically identical at conception (Hall, 2003). However, there are different subtypes of MZ twins, possible due to the timing of the initial zygotic division (Loos et al., 1998): dichorionic diamniotic (DCDA), monochorionic diamniotic (MCDA), and monochorionic monoamniotic (MCMA) twins. DCDA twins, who have separate chorions (placenta) and amnions, form when cleavage takes place within 72 hours of fertilization (about 1/3 of MZ twins). MCDA twins, who share the same chorion (placenta) but have two amnions, form when cleavage occurs within 4 to 7 days of fertilization (about 2/3 of MZ twins). MCMA twins, who share the same chorion and amnion, occurs when the split takes place within 8 to 14 days of fertilization (about 1–2% of MZ twins). As MCMA and MCDA twins share the same chorion, they have an increased risk for twin-to-twin transfusion syndrome as well as for perinatal mortality and morbidity compared with DCDA twins, who have separate chorions and amniotic sacs (Hall, 2003; Lewi, 2010; Loos et al., 1998). As DZ twins emerge from two different egg cells and two different sperm cells, they are always DCDA twins. Like ordinary siblings, DZ twins share about 50% of the segregated genes identical by decent (Hall, 2003). Contrary to MZ twins, who are always of the same sex, DZ twins can be either same-sex (SS) or opposite-sex (OS). Thus, the OS twins are always DZ whereas the SS twin pairs, consisting of two boys or two girls, can be either MZ or DZ.

2. The Twin Testosterone Transfer hypothesis

2.1. Sex hormones

Gonadal hormones, particularly androgens, play an important role in early human development, influencing both physical and behavioral characteristics (Hines, 2011). Testosterone is the major androgenic hormone produced by the testes. The fetal testes begin to produce testosterone prenatally, but the ovaries do not (Wilson et al., 1981). As a consequence, male fetuses are exposed to higher levels of testosterone than are female fetuses (Hines, 2008). Testosterone production in males is highest from 8–24 weeks of gestation, with peak levels occurring between 12–18 gestational weeks (Abramovich, 1974; Nagamani et al., 1979; Warne et al., 1977), and these periods are times of rapid brain development. Gonadal hormones can induce a masculinizing influence, enhancing the development of male-typical traits, or a feminizing influence, which enhances the development of female-typical traits. Gonadal hormones can also have a demasculinizing influence (suppression of male-typical characteristics) or a defeminizing influence (suppression of female-typical characteristics) (Collaer and Hines, 1995). Prenatal testosterone exposure has been linked to the masculinization of a variety of traits, including behavioral changes such as childhood play behavior, sexual orientation and gender identity, as well as some cognitive, motor and personality characteristics that show sex differences (Hines, 2006, 2010). In general, estrogen does not promote female-typical development. This occurs in the absence or reduction of testicular hormones (Hines, 2011).

2.2. Animal studies

Human studies of prenatal hormone effects were initially motivated by experimental studies in non-human animals. The pioneering study by Phoenix et al. found that female guinea pigs that were exposed to testosterone prenatally showed masculinized behavior in adulthood (Phoenix, 2009). Since then, numerous studies in non-human mammals have demonstrated effects of testosterone on neurobehavioral sexual differentiation (Constantinescu and Hines, 2012). Moreover, studies in, for example, rats and mice have demonstrated that exposure to sex hormones is influenced by the intrauterine fetal position of the animal, e.g. the proximity to fetuses of the same or opposite sex. Females are considered to be more sensitive to intrauterine position effects than males (Ryan and Vandenbergh, 2002) and female rodents developing between male fetuses in utero present masculinized anatomical (e.g. increased anogenital distance), behavioral (e.g. more aggressive behavior, less attractive to males) and reproductive characteristics (e.g. less reproductive) in adulthood compared with females developing between female fetuses (Ryan and Vandenbergh, 2002). Irrespective of sex, a fetus located between two male fetuses has higher blood concentrations of testosterone and lower concentrations of estradiol than fetuses located between two females (Ryan and Vandenbergh, 2002; vom Saal, 1989). However, although the effects of intrauterine positions on both sexes are well documented in model species, recent literature emphasizes the importance of examining intrauterine position effects in species with different life histories (Fishman et al., 2019). A recent study investigating the intrauterine position effects in a feral animal model (Myocastor coypus), which is characterized by long gestation and precocious offspring, found the opposite, namely that in rodent model species females adjacent to males in utero did not show increased testosterone levels. To the contrary, they showed a reduction in testosterone immunoreactivity, while the testosterone levels of females not positioned next to a male did not differ from those of males (Fishman et al., 2019).

2.3. Congenital Adrenal Hyperplasia

Evidence that testosterone influences human neurobehavioral development comes largely from studies of humans who develop in atypical hormonal environments (Constantinescu and Hines, 2012). The best-studied clinical condition is Congenital Adrenal Hyperplasia (CAH) (Cohen-Bendahan et al., 2005b). Individuals with CAH produce high levels of androgens from early in gestation, due to an enzymatic defect caused by a single gene (Pang et al., 1980). Females with CAH are suggested to differ from unaffected females in a number of domains including activity interests, personality, cognitive abilities, handedness, and sexuality (Hines et al., 2003). However, the most convincing evidence comes from studies of childhood play with CAH females, showing increased male-typical toy, playmate, and activity preferences (Hines, 2011). Males with CAH are generally found to be similar to their unaffected brothers with regard to most aspects of behaviors, but there is some suggestion that they have lower spatial ability than control males (Hampson et al., 1998; Hines et al., 2003).

2.4. Maternal-fetal and feto-fetal route

An ideal study from which to obtain information about the prenatal environment would be a study that directly measures fetal hormones at many points in gestation, and then follows the children into childhood and beyond. However, because there is risk associated with the collection of serum from living fetuses, this is not feasible (Cohen-Bendahan et al., 2005b). Alternative measures have included the investigation of androgen concentrations in maternal serum during pregnancy (Hickey et al., 2009) and perinatal hormones obtained from umbilical cord blood at birth (Whitehouse et al., 2010). It has also been suggested (Cohen-Bendahan et al., 2005b) that studies of prenatal hormones from the amniotic sac during the second trimester of pregnancy provide the most accurate measure of fetal androgen exposure, but this procedure is only performed when there is a medical reason, and the participant sample may not be representative of the general population (Tapp et al., 2011).

OS twins are thought to provide another opportunity to test the effects of prenatal testosterone exposure (Miller, 1994). The Twin Testosterone Transfer (TTT) hypothesis assumes that human sex hormones are transferred between fetuses in the same pregnancy of twins. Transfer of testosterone may occur via the maternal circulation or more directly between fetuses (Miller, 1994). In the first route, testosterone is suggested to pass from fetus to fetus through maternal circulation. Empirical evidence for this pathway comes from animal studies showing that testosterone injected into a pregnant mother increased circulating testosterone concentrations in the fetus and exerted a masculinizing influence on offspring behavior (Miller, 1994; Phoenix et al., 1959). In humans, however, the suggestion of a maternal route is not supported because hormone levels in maternal blood and amniotic fluid do not correlate (Nagamani et al., 1979). Moreover, other human studies have found that the sex of a fetus cannot be predicted from serum androgen concentrations (Glass and Klein, 1981; Hines et al., 2002; van de Beek et al., 2004) suggesting that maternal-fetal hormone transfer may be unidirectional, with only passage of hormones from the mother to the fetus (Tapp et al., 2011). The other potential route goes directly from fetus to fetus (diffusion across fetal membranes). Amniotic fluid can permeate the fetal skin and the placenta until 18 weeks of gestation (Abramovich and Page, 1972), and testosterone production in males is at its highest before this gestational age (Abramovich, 1974; Nagamani et al., 1979). These facts, combined with the considerable evidence of intrauterine position in animals, suggest that there might be exposure to elevated levels of testosterone in twins with male co-twins. However, because females produce little amounts of testosterone themselves, the effect of gestating with a male co-twin is expected to be more pronounced in females than in males (Tapp et al., 2011).

3. Postnatal socialization

An alternative explanation to prenatal hormone effects is differences in postnatal socialization. Socialization effects could result from different experiences including different exposure to sex-typed toys and activities due to having a co-twin of the opposite sex vs. having one of the same sex (Cohen-Bendahan et al., 2005b; Henderson and Berenbaum, 1997; Pulkkinen et al., 2003). Thus, it is possible that OS females are raised in a more male-typical environment than SS females and that this may affect their behavior. In addition, it is also possible that the behavior of OS male twins is influenced by growing up with a twin sister (Cohen-Bendahan et al., 2005b). Studies on twins’ social interactions and the parental treatment of twins are limited (Lundborg, 2008; Pulkkinen et al., 2003), but twin relationships, albeit identical twin relationships have been suggested to be some of the most unique and intimate kinds of interpersonal bonds (Neyer, 2002; Segal, 1999). For instance, a large Finnish study of 1,874 11–12 year-old twins and their 23,200 non-twin classmates reported differences in peer-assessed socio-emotional behavior between twins and singletons (Pulkkinen et al., 2003). Twins of both sexes were rated higher than singletons in adaptive behaviors, especially in socially active behavior. However, this seemed to occur mainly in the OS twins, who were rated higher than their singleton classmates in social interaction, popularity and leadership.

4. Findings from studies comparing OS and SS twins

In this special issue the overall aim is to provide an overview of published studies investigating potential differences between OS and SS twins, focusing on the TTT hypothesis. Three papers have previously reviewed the evidence on OS twins. Miller’s review concluded that comparison of OS and same-sex dizygotic (SSDZ) female twins provides a reasonable model for studies of prenatal testosterone exposure (Miller, 1994). Cohen-Bendahan’s review in 2005 focused on the effects of prenatal sex hormones on sex-typed behavior, and stated that there is good evidence that human sex-typed behavior is influenced by sex hormones during prenatal development with increasing evidence from the normal population (Cohen-Bendahan et al., 2005b). In 2011, Tapp et al. provided an overview of human studies of phenotypic differences in several domains between OS and SS twins and concluded that, although the evidence is inconsistent there is enough support for the TTT hypothesis to motivate further research (Tapp et al., 2011). Since the last review in 2011, several studies on different phenotypes have been conducted using the OS vs. SS twin design. This paper summarizes studies published up to April 2019 investigating potential differences between OS and SS twins on physiological, morphological, and reproductive traits (Table 1), cognitive and perceptual traits (Table 2), behavioral traits (Table 3) and other health outcomes (Table 4).

Table 1 -.

Studies on physiological, morphological and reproductive traits in opposite-sex and same-sex twins

Trait Publication Setting Numbers of opposite-sex (OS) and same-sex (SS) twins Is there a comparison between OS and SS dizygotic (DZ) twins only?a Age Assessment Findings OS vs. SS females Findings OS vs. SS males Suggests evidence for masculiniza-tion of OS femalesb Suggests evidence for demasculiniza-tion of OS malesb
Age at menarche Kaprio et al., 1995 Finland 434 OSF
378 SSF (DZ)
468 SSF (MZ)
Yes 16 years Self-reported age at menarche OSF higher mean age at menarche than SSF (DZ) but not when compared with SSF (MZ) NA + NA
Sorensen et al., 2013 Denmark 1147 OSF
1250 SSF (DZ)
1466 SSF (MZ)
Yes 12–22 years Self-reported age at menarche No significant differences NA NA
Anthropome-tric measures Gaist et al., 2000 Denmark 700 OSF
655 SSF (DZ)
703 SSF (MZ)
722 OSM
709 SSM (DZ)
734 SSM (MZ)
Yes Mean = 56.9 years Self-reported weight and height, waist circumference and handgrip strength measured by interviewers No significant differences No significant differences
Anthropome-tric measures and metabolic aberrations Alexanderson et al., 2011 Sweden 8,409 OSF
9,166 SSF
DZ twins only OSF: 42–93 years, mean = 59 years SSF: 42–103 years, mean = 61 years Self-reported body mass index (BMI), hyperlipidemia and diabetes mellitus type II BMI, body weight and rate of dyslipidemia moderately higher in OSF than in SSF but only in those over 60 years of age NA +/− NA
Anthropome-tric measures, disease status and reproductive history Korsoff et al., 2014 Finland Sample A:
789 OSF
679 SSF
Sample B (subsample for serum lipid and lipoprotein subclass concentrations):
169 OSF
226 SSF
DZ twins only A: 32–37 years, mean = 34.0 years
B: 21–29, mean = 24 years
Obesity measures: BMI from self-reported height and weight and waist circumference.
Disease status: self-reported hypertension and diabetes. Serum lipid and lipoprotein subclass concentrations measured by nuclear magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Self-reported reproductive history (e.g. current pregnancy, number of children, abortions)
No significant differences NA NA
Anthropome-tric measures Bogl et al., 2017 CODATwins 19 countries 27,100 OSF
39,856 SSF
26,708 OSM
28,638 SSM
DZ twins only 20 years and older
Males: median age = 44 years
Females: median age = 42 years
Height and weight mainly self-reported (97%), BMI calculated based on weight and height No significant differences for BMI and overweight/obesity but OSF 0.31 cm taller than SSF No significant differences for BMI and overweight/obesity but OSM 0.14 cm taller than SSM
Birth size, birth weight and gestational age Tul et al., 2012 Slovenia 1097 OSF
1554 SSF
1097 OSM
1638 SSM
No Infants Mean birth weight obtained from the Slovenian national information perinatal system No significant differences OSM heavier than SSM
Jelenkovic et al., 2018 CODATwins 15 countries 16,417 OSF
17,432 SSF
16,417 OSM
17,584 SSM
DZ twins only Infants Mean birth weight, birth size and gestational age – mainly parentally reported No significant differences OSM on average 31 g heavier and 0.16 cm longer than SSM. OSM longer gestation than SSM
Brain size Peper et al., 2009 The Netherlands Sample A (children):
19 OSF
41 SSF
16 OSM
43 SSM
Sample B (adults):
21 OSF
40 SSF
11 OSM
44 SSM
DZ twins only A: mean = 9.2 years
B: mean = 30.0 years
Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) brain scans Larger total brain and cerebellum volumes for OSF than SSF children. No significant differences in adults Larger total brain and cerebellum volumes for SSM than OSM children. No significant differences in adults +/− +/−
Brain size and craniofacial morphology Marečková et al., 2015 Quebec, Canada 35 OSF
28 SSF
36 OSM
20 SSM
DZ twins only 8 years Magnetic resonance images (MRI) Brain size: No significant differences
Principal components: 5/8 PC3-related distances smaller for SSF than OSF
No significant differences +/−
Circulating hormone levels/Cerebral lateralization (right ear advantage) Cohen-Bendahan et al. 2004 The Netherlands 67 OSF
53 SSF
DZ twins only 10.4–11.8 years, mean = 11.0 years Testosterone levels in saliva from radioimmunoassay. Auditory-verbal dichotic listening task No significant differences regarding hormone levels. OSF greater right ear advantage of cerebral lateralization (more masculinized) than SSF NA +/− NA
Circulating hormone levels/Handedness Vuoksimaa et al., 2010a Finland Testosterone/estradiol levels
117/114 OSF
254/244 SSF
109/108 OSM
291/278 SSM
Handedness
755 OSF
749 SSF (MZ)
706 SSF (DZ)
730 OSM
697 SSM (MZ)
784 SSM (DZ)
No 14 years Testosterone and estradiol levels in saliva from radioimmunoassay. Self-reported handedness (based on two questions) No significant differences regarding hormone levels. Lower prevalence of left-handedness in OSF than in SSF No significant differences +/−
Finger-length ratios (second to fourth finger; 2D:4D) Van Anders et al., 2006 Canada 9 OSF
16 SSF
9 OSM
22 SSM
DZ twins only 4–15 years, mean = 10.2 years 2D:4D measures from photocopies of hands 2D:4D lower (more masculinized) in OSF than in SSF for the left-hand digit ratio but not the right No significant differences +
Voracek and Dressler, 2007 Australia 10 OSF
18 SSF
DZ twins only Adults 2D:4D measures from photocopies of hands 2D:4D lower (more masculinized) in OSF than in SSF NA + NA
Medland et al., 2008 Australia 212 OSF
237 SSF
199 OSM
219 SSM
DZ twins only 11–24 years, mean = 15.5 years 2D:4D measures from photocopies of hands No significant differences No significant differences
Hiraishi et al., 2012 Japan 9 OSF
150 SSF (MZ)
44 SSF (DZ)
9 OSM
58 SSM (MZ)
28 SSM (DZ)
No 16–32 years, mean = 22.5 years 2D:4D measures from photocopies of hands No significant differences No significant differences
Handedness Elkadi et al., 1999 Australia 59 OSF
40 SSF
59 OSM
21 SSM
DZ twins only 18–26 years, OS: mean = 24.4 years
SSF: mean = 24.7 years
SSM: mean = 21.6 years
Edinburgh Handedness Inventory questionnaire (preferred hand for 10 everyday activities) No significant differences No significant differences
Handedness/Footedness Ooki, 2006 Japan Handedness
Sample A:
125 OSF
138 SSF (DZ)
125 OSM
150 SSM (DZ)
Sample B:
203 OSF
209 SSF (DZ)
203 OSM
182 SSM (DZ)
Footedness
Sample A:
113 OSF
130 SSF (DZ)
114 OSM
144 SSM (DZ)
Sample B:
175 OSF
172 SSF (DZ)
177 OSM
150 SSM (DZ)
Yes A: 11–12 years
B: 1–15 years, mean = 5.9 years
Self-reported hand use for writing and foot use for kicking a ball (single question) No significant differences in prevalence except for footedness for females in sample A No significant differences +/−
Handedness Medland et al., 2009 Australia and the Netherlands 54,270 twins and their non-twin siblings (no breakdown provided) Yes Children and adults Handedness assessed by writing/drawing or self-report No significant differences No significant differences
Leukocyte telomere length (LTL) Benetos et al., 2014 Denmark 72 OSF
196 SSF (MZ)
176 SSF (DZ)
72 OSM
172 SSM (MZ)
132 SSM (DZ)
Yes Mean (baseline) = 36.0–39.8 years LTL measured by Southern blots of the terminal restriction fragments Shorter LTL in OSF than in SSF No significant differences +
Polycystic ovary syndrome (PCOS) Kuijper et al., 2009 The Netherlands 480 OSF
711 SSF (DZ)
1325 SSF (MZ)
Yes Adults Self-reported PCOS based No significant differences NA NA
Reproductive function Christensen et al., 1998 Denmark 4269 OSF
3650 SSF (MZ)
4455 SSF (DZ)
No, but differences tested between zygosity groups 18 years or older Self-reported outcome of first try ever to become pregnant and waiting time to pregnancy No significant differences NA NA
Reproductive function/Height Loehlin and Martin, 1998 Australia Reproductive function:
600–700 OSF
1400–1500 SSF
Height
685 OSF
1642 SSF
No Reproductive function:
OSF: mean = 42.6 years
SSF: mean = 39.7 years
Height:
OSF: mean = 39.7 years
SSF: mean = 42.6 years
Self-reported 90-item questionnaire related to reproductive functions and self-reported height No significant differences NA NA
Lummaa et al., 2007 Finland 31 OSF
35 SSF
17 OSM
26 SSM
No Adults Number of offspring obtained from Finnish Church registers OSF had fewer offspring compared with SSF No significant differences +
Medland et al., 2008b Australia, the Netherlands and Virginia USA 913 OSF
1979 SSF
No Adults Self-reported number of children and age at first pregnancy No significant differences NA NA
Reproductive function, marriage, graduating from high school/completing college and socioeconomic outcomes Bütikofer et al., 2019 Norway 13,717 twins including 6,808 female twins (breakdown not provided) No At least one twin observed to age 32 Objective information on birth, household composition, schooling and labor market records from Norwegian Register Data OSF decreased probability of graduating from high school (15.2%), completing college (3.9%) and being married (11.7%). OSF have lower fertility (5.8%) and lower life-cycle earnings (8.6%) than SSF No significant differences +
Tooth size Dempsey et al., 1999 Australia 56 OSF
98 SSF (DZ)
166 SSF (MZ)
56 OSM
88 SSF (DZ)
132 SSF (MZ)
No 7–62 years, mean = 16.5 years Tooth crown diameters of 28 permanent teeth recorded OSF had larger tooth crown size than SSF for 26/28 teeth No significant differences +
Ribeiro et al., 2013 Australia 43 OSF
39 SSF (DZ)
52 SSF (MZ)
Yes 4–16 years Tooth crown size determined by image analysis Larger tooth crown size in OSF than in SSF NA + NA

NA: Not available, OS: opposite-sex, SS = same-sex, F = females, M = males, MZ = monozygotic, DZ = dizygotic, UZ = unknown zygosity

a

Yes: The study makes a separate comparison of OS vs SSDZ twins, although it includes both MZ and DZ twins.

No: The study does not make a separate comparison of OS vs SSDZ twins, thus, including MZ twins in all analyses.

DZ twins only: The study includes DZ twins only. Thus, no MZ twins are included in the study.

b

+: The study provides evidence for masculinization of OS females/demasculinization of OS males.

−: The study provides no evidence for masculinization of OS females/demasculinization of OS males.

+/−: The study provides evidence for masculinization of OS females/demasculinization of OS males in some cases (e.g. in some investigated measures, in some age groups or in some statistical analyses), but not in all.

Table 2 -.

Studies on perceptual and cognitive traits in opposite-sex and same-sex twins

Trait Publication Setting Numbers of opposite-sex (OS) and same-sex (SS) twins Is there a comparison between OS and SS dizygotic (DZ) twins only?a Age Assessment Findings OS vs. SS females Findings OS vs. SS males Suggests evidence for masculiniza-tion of OS femalesb Suggests evidence for demasculiniza-tion of OS malesb
Academic performance Ahrenfeldt et al., 2015a Denmark 968 OSF
2198 SSF
966 OSM
2194 SSM (breakdown not provided)
Yes 15–16 years Ninth grade test scores obtained from registers OSF obtained slightly lower test scores in mathematics than SSF – opposite to what was expected No significant differences
Auditory system functioning McFadden, 1993 Texas, USA 17 OSF
32 SSF (DZ)
46 SSF (MZ)
17 OSM
24 SSM (DZ)
34 SSM (MZ)
Yes OS: mean = 20.8 years
SS (DZ): mean = 18.7 years
SS (MZ): mean = 21.3 years
Spontaneous otoacoustic emissions (SOAEs) SOAE frequency lower for OSF than for SSF No significant differences +
McFadden et al., 1996 Texas, USA 18 OSF
32 SSF (DZ)
42 SSF (MZ)
17 OSM
24 SSM (DZ)
34 SSF (MZ)
Yes OS: mean = 20.7 years
SS (DZ): mean = 18.2 years
SS (MZ): mean = 21.3 years
Click-evoked otoacoustic emissions (CEOAEs) No significant differences, but spectral power of CEOAEs marginally lower (more masculinized) for OSF than for SSF No significant differences +
Expressive vocabulary Galsworthy et al., 2000 England and Wales 974 OSF
994 SSF (DZ)
1126 SSF (MZ)
974 OSM
992 SSM (DZ)
952 SSM (MZ)
Yes 2 years Parent report: MacArthur Communica-tive Developmental Inventory (MCDI) - Short form OSF lower MCDI scores (more masculinized) than SSF OSM higher MCDI scores than SSM + +
Van Hulle et al., 2004 Wisconsin, USA 112 OSF
61 SSF 8DZ)
64 SSF (MZ)
112 OSM
64 SSM (DZ)
52 SSF (MZ)
Yes 20–38 months Parent report: MCDI - Short form OSF lower MCDI scores than SSF OSM higher MCDI scores than SSM + +
Visuospatial cognition Vuoksimaa et al., 2010b Finland 120 OSF
351 SSF
110 OSM
223 SSM
Yes Mean = 22.4 years Mental Rotation Test (MRT) OSF performed better than SSF No significant differences +
Heil et al., 2011 Germany 100 OSF
100 SSF (DZ)
Yes 19–39 years, mean = 23.6 years Mental rotation test from an MRT questionnaire OSF performed better than SSF NA + NA

NA: Not available, OS: opposite-sex, SS = same-sex, F = females, M = males, MZ = monozygotic, DZ = dizygotic, UZ = unknown zygosity

a

Yes: The study makes a separate comparison of OS vs SSDZ twins, although it includes both MZ and DZ twins.

No: The study does not make a separate comparison of OS vs SSDZ twins, thus, including MZ twins in all analyses.

DZ twins only: The study includes DZ twins only. Thus, no MZ twins are included in the study.

b

+: The study provides evidence for masculinization of OS females/demasculinization of OS males.

−: The study provides no evidence for masculinization of OS females/demasculinization of OS males.

+/−: The study provides evidence for masculinization of OS females/demasculinization of OS males in some cases (e.g. in some investigated measures, in some age groups or in some statistical analyses), but not in all.

Table 3 -.

Studies on behavioral traits in opposite-sex and same-sex twins

Trait Publication Setting Numbers of opposite-sex (OS) and same-sex (SS) twins Is there a comparison between OS and SS dizygotic (DZ) twins only?a Age Assessment Findings OS vs. SS females Findings OS vs. SS males Suggests evidence for masculiniza-tion of OS femalesb Suggests evidence for demasculiniza-tion of OS malesb
Alcohol use Lenz et al., 2012 A Sweden
B Australia
Sample A:
29,933 females
29,271 males
Sample B:
2,041 males
3,848 females (Breakdown not provided)
Yes Not provided Hospitalization rate due to alcohol dependence from the Swedish Twin Registry. Self-reported lifetime prevalence of alcohol dependence obtained from telephone interviews No significant differences Higher prevalence of alcohol dependence for OSM than SSM +
Ellingson et al., 2013 Australia 621 OSF
915 SSF (DZ)
1,191 SSF (MZ)
No 32–43 years, mean = 37.7 years Self-reported alcohol use, alcohol dependence and abuse symptoms obtained from telephone interviews OSF slightly more lifetime alcohol use disorder (AUD) symptoms than SSF. No other differences NA +/− NA
Autistic symptoms/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Ho et al., 2005 Missouri, USA 250 OSF
142 SSF (DZ)
177 SSF (MZ)
250 OSM
128 SSM (DZ)
91 SSM (MZ)
Yes 7–15 years Parent report: sub-threshold autistic symptomatolo-gy assessed using the Social Responsive-ness Scale (SRS) No significant differences OSM lower mean score on the SRS (indicating less autistic symptomatolo-gy) compared with SSM +
Atterman et al., 2012 Denmark 2947 OSF
3412 SSF
Yes 3–15 years Parental responses to items from the Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL) OSF scored lower than SSF for both ADHD and total scores – contrary to expected NA NA
Eriksson et al., 2016 Sweden 4219 OSF
1808 SSF (DZ)
4219 OSM
2129 SSM (DZ)
DZ twins only 9 or 12 years Parental telephone interview examining attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and autistic traits using Autism-Tics, AD/HD, and other Comorbidities Interventory (ATAC) OSF displayed fewer traits related to attention deficit and repetitive and stereotyped behaviors than SSF – opposite to what was expected OSM displayed a larger number of traits related to attention deficit and repetitive and stereotyped behaviors than SSM
Disordered eating (DE) Culberg et al., 2008 Michigan, USA 59 OSF
132 SSF (DZ)
172 SSF (MZ)
54 OSM
62 SSM (DZ)
103 SSM (MZ)
Yes Mean = 20.8 years Self-report from the Minnesota Eating Behavior Survey Less DE for OSF compared with SSF More DE for OSM compared with SSM + +
Raevuori et al., 2008 Finland 793 OSF
765 SSF (DZ)
868 SSF (MZ)
717 OSM
705 SSM (DZ)
540 SSM (MZ)
Yes 22–28 years, mean = 24.4 years Self-reported current and lifetime diagnoses of anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa obtained from Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV – short version by telephone No significant differences No significant differences
Baker et al., 2009 Sweden 371 OSF
213 SSF (DZ)
439 SSF (MZ)
361 OSM
344 SSM (DZ)
461 SSM (MZ)
Yes 15–17 years Self-report from Eating Disorder Inventory-2 (questionnaire) No significant differences No significant differences
Lydecker et al., 2012 A: Virginia, USA
B: Norway
C: Sweden
Sample A:
481 OSF
408 SSF (DZ)
614 SSF (MZ)
317 OSM
295 SSM (DZ)
492 SSM (MZ)
Sample B:
345 OSF
530 SSF (DZ)
900 SSF (MZ)
341 OSM
235 SSM (DZ)
445 SSM (MZ)
Sample C:
2433 OSF
2901 SSF (DZ)
4099 SSF (MZ)
2423 OSM
1918 SSM (DZ)
2684 SSM (MZ)
Yes A:
Females: mean = 40.4 years
Males: mean = 42.3 years
B:
Females: mean = 28.2 years
Males: mean = 28.3 years
C:
Females: mean = 33.5 years
Males: mean = 33.4 years
Self-reported eating disorders from interviews based on DSM-IV criteria No significant differences when comparing OS and SS (DZ) twins but co-twin sex was associated with broadly defined bulimia nervosa in sample C when MZ twins were included No significant differences
Culbert et al., 2013 Michigan, USA 64 OSF
178 SSF
No Pre-Early puberty
OSF: mean = 11.7 years,
SSF: mean = 11.4 years
Mid-Late puberty
OSF: mean = 14.0 years
SSF: mean = 13.2 years
Self-reported DE attitudes assessed by the Minnesota Eating Behavior Survey and the Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire OSF exhibited lower levels of DE attitudes (more masculinized) during mid-late puberty compared with SSF. No significant differences during pre-early puberty NA +/− NA
Feminity - masculinity Loehlin and Martin, 2000 Australia Sample A:
691 OSF
3158 SSF
634 OSM
1390 SSM
Sample B:
447 OSF
1773 SSF
399 OSM
1041 SSM
No A: 24–87 years, mean = 41.2 years
B: 1728 years, mean = 23.2 years
Masculini ty-femininity scales “Worried”, “Reserved” and “Breaks Rules” scales, derived from the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire and Cloninger Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire (short versions) A: No significant differences
B: OSF higher scores on Breaks-Rules subscale than SSF but not on the other subscales
A: OSM higher than SSM on Worried subscale and Breaks-Rules subscale but not for Reserved subscale.
B: OSM higher than SSM on the Breaks-Rules subscale. No significant difference on Worried and Reserved subscale
+/− +/−
Feminity – masculinity/Pubertal development/Fertility in early adulthood Rose et al., 2002 Finland 972 OSF
967 SSF
931 OSM
1060 SSM
Sample A:
783/486 OSF
762/466 SSF
Sample B:
772/464 OSF
749/454 SSF (Number of males not provided)
Sample C:
4767 OSF
7528 SSF
No 16 years
A: 11 years
B: 14 years (follow up)
C: 15–28 years
Self-report from Feminine Interest (FEM) Scale
A and B: Post-menarche frequencies.
B: Self-reported Pubertal Development Scale (females and males)
C: Fertility in early adulthood from Central Population Records
No significant differences No significant differences
Verweij et al., 2016 Sweden 536 OSF
392 SSF (DZ)
695 SSF (MZ)
536 OSM
248 SSM (DZ)
374 SSM (MZ)
No 27–54 years, mean = 40.7 years Self-reported Big-Five personality inventory data used to create a bipolar masculinity-femininity (M-F) scale OSF scored higher (more masculine) on the M-F scale than SSF OSM scored higher (more masculine) on the M-F scale than SSM +
Masculine Attitudes (Conservatism) Miller and Martin, 1995 Australia 905 OSF
3964 SSF
905 OSM
1832 SSM
No Adults Self-report from Wilson–Patterson Conservatism Scale (questionnaire) OSF reported more masculine (socially conservative) views compared with SSF No significant differences +
Religiousness and religious coping Ahrenfeldt et al., 2016 Denmark 408 OSF
1383 SSF
350 OSM
856 SSM (Number of MZ twins not provided)
Yes – comparison between OS and SS (DZ)+UZ twins 20–40 years, mean = 29.8 years Self-reported survey including questions on religiousness and religious coping No significant differences No significant differences except that OSM reported attending church more often than SSM in childhood
Sensation seeking/agression Resnick et al., 1993 England 51 OSF
286 SSF
51 OSM
85 SSM
No (scores for MZ and SSDZ twins did not differ) 16–70 years Self-report from Sensation Seeking Scale IV (SSS-IV) – an overall score and four subscales scores (questionnaire) OSF reported increased disinhibition, experience seeking and overall sensation seeking compared with SSF No significant differences +/−
Cohen-Bendahan et al., 2005a The Netherlands 74 OSF
55 SSF
DZ twins only 13 years Self-report from Sensation Seeking Scale V (SSS-V) questionnaire and from Olweus Multifaceted Aggression Inventory (OMAI)
Test of child: Reinisch Aggression Inventory (RAI)
OSF lower experience seeking behavior – opposite to expected. OSF rated more aggressive than SSF on two of the RAI but no differences on OMAI subscales NA −/+ NA
Slutske et al., 2011 Australia 564 OSF
836 SSF (DZ)
1,111 SSF (MZ)
Yes 32–43 years, mean = 37.7 years Telephone interview and questionnaire: The Zuckerman Sensation Seeking Scale-Form V (SSS-V) OSF showed higher experience-seeking and thrill- and – adventure seeking scores compared with SSF. Other group differences were non-significant NA +/− NA
Sex-typed play Elizabeth and Green, 1984 New York and Long Island area 156 OSF
270 SSF (DZ)
320 SSF (MZ)
156 OSM
240 SSM (DZ)
262 SSM (MZ)
Yes 4–12 years, mean = 6.8 years Parental report: a 17-item questionnaire used to discriminate between typical and atypical sex-role development No significant differences OSM scored in a more feminine direction compared with SSM +
Henderson and Berenbaum, 1997 Illinois, USA 35 OSF
36 SSF
DZ twins only 3–8 years, OSF: mean = 5.1 years
SSF: mean = 5.5 years
Observational: time child played with toys typically preferred by boys/girls. Mothers’ reports of game preferences No significant differences NA NA
Rodgers et al., 1998 Oregon, USA 16 OSF
54 SSF
16 OSM
48 SSM
DZ twins only 7–12 years, OS: mean = 8.3 years
SSF: mean = 8.1 years
SSM: mean = 8.5 years
Observational: toy preferences during 5-minute free-play interactions between mothers and twins No significant differences No significant differences
Social skills Laffey-Ardley and Thorpe, 2006 Australia 36 OSF
28 SSF (DZ)
36 OSM
22 SSM (DZ)
DZ twins only 3–6 years, OS: mean = 4.4 years
SS: mean = 4.7 years
Parent and teacher reports of social co-operation and compete nce using the Preschool and Kindergarten Behavior Scale – 2nd Edition (PKBS-2) Parent reports: Mixed result for social co-operation: OSF rated higher than SSF when using adjusted ANCOVA analysis but not for simple ANOVA analysis Teacher-reports: SSM rated higher than OSM on social co-operation +/−

NA: Not available, OS: opposite-sex, SS = same-sex, F = females, M = males, MZ = monozygotic, DZ = dizygotic, UZ = unknown zygosity

a

Yes: The study makes a separate comparison of OS vs SSDZ twins, although it includes both MZ and DZ twins.

No: The study does not make a separate comparison of OS vs SSDZ twins, thus, including MZ twins in all analyses.

DZ twins only: The study includes DZ twins only. Thus, no MZ twins are included in the study.

b

+: The study provides evidence for masculinization of OS females/demasculinization of OS males.

−: The study provides no evidence for masculinization of OS females/demasculinization of OS males.

+/−: The study provides evidence for masculinization of OS females/demasculinization of OS males in some cases (e.g. in some investigated measures, in some age groups or in some statistical analyses), but not in all.

Table 4 -.

Studies on other health traits in opposite-sex and same-sex twins

Trait Publication Setting Numbers of opposite-sex (OS) and same-sex (SS) twins Is there a comparison between OS and SS dizygotic (DZ) twins only?a Age Assessment Findings OS vs. SS females Findings OS vs. SS males Suggests evidence for masculiniza-tion of OS femalesb Suggests evidence for demasculiniza-tion of OS malesb
Cancer Ahrenfeldt et al., 2015b Denmark and Sweden 44,650 OSF
84,721 SSF
44,660 OSM
88,261 SSM
Yes – comparison between OS and SS (DZ)+UZ twins 0–73 years
Denmark: mean = 25 years at start of follow up
Sweden: mean = 24 years at start of follow up
All-cause cancer and sex-specific cancers identified from the Danish and Swedish twin and cancer registries No significant differences No significant differences
Early life mortality risks Ahrenfeldt et al., 2017 Denmark 12,033 OSF
12,051 OSM
21,644 SSF
22,901 SSM
No 0–15 years Perinatal mortality, neonatal and postneonatal deaths and child mortality identified through the Danish Twin Registry OSF lower perinatal and early neonatal mortality than SSF OSM lower perinatal, neonatal and postneonatal mortality than SSM - may be due to MZ twins in the SS twin group +/−
Epilepsi Mao et al., 2018 Denmark 11,078 OSF
19,186 SSF
11,080 OSM
20,207 SSM
Yes 0–34 years Epilepsy identified through the Danish National Patient Registry No significant differences No significant differences

NA: Not available, OS: opposite-sex, SS = same-sex, F = females, M = males, MZ = monozygotic, DZ = dizygotic, UZ = unknown zygosity

a

Yes: The study makes a separate comparison of OS vs SSDZ twins, although it includes both MZ and DZ twins.

No: The study does not make a separate comparison of OS vs SSDZ twins, thus, including MZ twins in all analyses.

DZ twins only: The study includes DZ twins only. Thus, no MZ twins are included in the study.

b

+: The study provides evidence for masculinization of OS females/demasculinization of OS males.

−: The study provides no evidence for masculinization of OS females/demasculinization of OS males.

+/−: The study provides evidence for masculinization of OS females/demasculinization of OS males in some cases (e.g. in some investigated measures, in some age groups or in some statistical analyses), but not in all.

4.1. Physiological, morphological and reproductive traits

Several studies have compared OS and SS female twins regarding physiological, morphological, and reproductive traits (Table 1). Differences between OS and SS females supporting the TTT hypothesis have been found regarding tooth size (Dempsey et al., 1999; Ribeiro et al., 2013), leukocytes telomere length (Benetos et al., 2014), and brain size in nine-year-old twins, although the latter was not replicated in adult twins (Peper et al., 2009). A recent study using magnetic resonance images to study craniofacial features among eight-year old twins was not able to replicate the findings of Peper et al. regarding brain size, but found that SS females differed from all other twin groups in craniofacial morphology mainly by having a longer and wider jaw and a longer chin (Mareckova et al., 2015) (Table 1). The results of studies of anthropometric measures were largely inconsistent. No differences were identified for self-reported height (Gaist et al., 2000; Loehlin and Martin, 1998), weight, body mass index (BMI), and waist circumference (Gaist et al., 2000; Korsoff et al., 2014), but a large recent study found that BMI, body weight and the rate of dyslipidemia were moderately higher in OS than in SS females, but only among individuals age 60 and above (Alexanderson et al., 2011). A large study based on twins from 19 countries found no differences for BMI and overweight/obesity, but found that OS females were 0.31 cm taller than SS females. As the authors suggested, this difference was detected due to the large sample size used in the study and is unlikely to reflect meaningful differences of public health relevance (Bogl et al., 2017). Neither a Slovenian sample (Tul et al., 2012) nor a sample of twins from 15 countries (Jelenkovic et al., 2018) showed significant differences in birth size or gestational age (Table 1).

The Geschwind-Behan-Galaburda (GBG) hypothesis (Geschwind and Behan, 1982) postulates that high levels of testosterone may inhibit development of the left hemisphere and enhance development of the right hemisphere, resulting in increased left-handedness. In contrast to the GBG hypothesis, the callosal theory proposes that low prenatal testosterone levels result in less regressive development of temporo-parietal regions of the brain, resulting in a larger isthmus of the corpus callosum and less functional asymmetry, thus increasing left-handedness (Witelson, 1991). Thus, while the GBG hypothesis predicts a higher prevalence of left-handedness, the callosal theory predicts a lower prevalence of left-handedness among OS than SS females. Most studies on handedness have found no differences between OS and SS twins (Elkadi et al., 1999; Medland et al., 2009; Ooki, 2006), but one study found a lower prevalence of left-handedness in OS than in SS females (Vuoksimaa et al., 2010a), supporting the callosal theory of the TTT hypothesis. The second-to-fourth-finger ratio (2D:4D) is a measure of the relative length of the second finger to that of the fourth finger. The development of the ratio has been suggested to be affected by testosterone, with males having a lower 2D:4D than females on average (Manning, 2002). The four studies on finger length ratios showed conflicting results with the most recent and largest studies (Hiraishi et al., 2012; Medland et al., 2008a) finding no differences. In contrast, the two other studies (van Anders et al., 2006; Voracek and Dressler, 2007) found 2D:4D lower, e.g. more masculinized in OS than in SS females although this result was only found for the left-hand digit ratio in the Canadian study (van Anders et al., 2006) (Table 1). Some inconsistencies in findings of reproductive characteristics have been reported. A Finnish study including twins born 1734–1888 found that OS females were 25% less likely to reproduce compared with SS females (Lummaa et al. 2007). The authors suggested that the results might provide support for the TTT hypothesis that testosterone from male co-twins is associated with the impaired fertility of females. A recent population-based study from Norway supported this conclusion by finding that OS females had 11.7% lower probability of ever having being married at age 32 and that OS females had 5.8% fewer children on average (Butikofer et al., 2019) (Table 1). However, other studies within this field have not reported any differences between OS and SS twins, either for waiting time to pregnancy (Christensen et al., 1998), or for self-reported reproductive functions including age at first pregnancy and number of children (Korsoff et al., 2014; Loehlin and Martin, 1998; Medland et al., 2008b; Rose et al., 2002). One study reported slightly later menarche for OS compared with SS females (Kaprio et al., 1995), whereas more recent studies did not find any differences (Rose et al., 2002; Sorensen et al., 2013). No differences were found either for self-reported pubertal development (Rose et al., 2002) nor for the prevalence of polycystic ovary syndrome (Kuijper et al., 2009) (Tables 1 and 3). Two studies measuring testosterone (Cohen-Bendahan et al., 2004; Vuoksimaa et al., 2010a) and estradiol levels (Vuoksimaa et al., 2010a) in saliva from radioimmunoassay among children and adolescents did not find differences between OS and SS twins (Table 1).

Among studies investigating differences in physiological and morphological traits in males (Table 1), only one study investigating brain volume in nine-year-old male twins reported findings in the direction predicted by the TTT hypothesis (Peper et al., 2009). However, the results of larger total brain and cerebellum volumes for SS than for OS male children were not replicated for adult twins. Few other studies have found differences between OS and SS male twins, but not in the expected direction predicted by the TTT hypothesis (Bogl et al., 2017; Jelenkovic et al., 2018; Tul et al., 2012).

4.2. Perceptual and cognitive traits

Studies of otoacoustic emissions, which are sounds produced by the inner ear either in response to a sound (click-evoked otoacoustic emissions – CEOAEs) or in the absence of any stimulus (spontaneous otoacoustic emissions - SOAEs), have reported that SOAEs are more numerous and CEOAEs stronger in females compared with males (McFadden and Shubel, 2003; Talmadge et al., 1993). Two twin studies of predominantly the same samples found that OS females produced significantly fewer SOAEs than SS females (McFadden, 1993) and that there was a marginally significant reduction in CEOAEs for OS compared with SS females (McFadden et al., 1996), supporting the TTT hypothesis (Table 2). Also, in support of the TTT hypothesis, OS females showed a more masculine pattern of cerebral lateralization compared with SS females (Cohen-Bendahan et al., 2004) (Table 1). Moreover, studies of visuo-spatial abilities found significantly better mental rotation test (MRT) performance for OS than SS females in the direction towards the male mean (Heil et al., 2011; Vuoksimaa et al., 2010b) (Table 2). Two studies of young twins (approximately age two years) investigated expressive vocabulary by parental reports, and reported that SS females had a larger vocabulary than OS females (Galsworthy et al., 2000; Van Hulle et al., 2004), also consistent with the TTT hypothesis. However, a large Danish register study investigating academic performance measured as ninth grade test scores and teacher ratings confirmed the known sex differences from previous literature, but contrary to the hypothesis, OS females did not perform better than SS females in mathematics, nor did they perform worse in Danish or English (Ahrenfeldt et al., 2015a) (Table 2). The recent Norwegian register-based study found that OS females had 15.2% decreased probability of graduating high school and 3.9% lower probability of graduating from college than SS females. The study also showed that OS females had 8.6% lower life cycle-earning at age 32 compared with SS females (Butikofer et al., 2019) (Table 1). Interestingly, the same differences were observed among subsets of females whose male co-twins died during the first postnatal year, suggesting that the differences are due primarily to prenatal exposure rather than to postnatal socialization effects of being raised with a male co-twin

Among studies investigating cognitive and perceptual traits for males (Table 2), the two twin studies investigating expressive vocabulary in young twins reported that OS males had a larger vocabulary than SS males, in accordance with the TTT hypothesis (Galsworthy et al., 2000; Van Hulle et al., 2004). The finding of no differences in academic performance (Ahrenfeldt et al., 2015a; Butikofer et al., 2019) agreed with the other studies investigating perceptual and cognitive traits among males, showing no differences between OS and SS male twins (McFadden, 1993; McFadden et al., 1996; Vuoksimaa et al., 2010b).

4.3. Behavioral traits

Evidence from studies of behavioral traits in OS and SS female twins comes from two large studies in which the results of sensation-seeking, including experience-seeking in OS compared with SS females, tended towards the male mean (Resnick et al., 1993; Slutske et al., 2011). Notably, another study found lower experience seeking among OS females compared with SS females among 13-year old female twins – opposite to what was predicted (Cohen-Bendahan et al., 2005a). Single studies have reported more masculinized scores for OS than SS females on measures of masculine attitudes (Miller and Martin, 1995), masculinity-femininity (Verweij et al., 2016), rule-breaking behavior (for the youngest of the two subsamples) (Loehlin and Martin, 2000) and aggression (Cohen-Bendahan et al., 2005a). In contrast, other studies did not find support for the TTT hypothesis in females in self-reported feminine interests (Rose et al., 2002), for Worried and Reserved masculinity-femininity subscales (Loehlin and Martin, 2000), for some aspects of social behavior and friendships (Laffey-Ardley and Thorpe, 2006), for hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and autistic symptomatology (Attermann et al., 2012; Eriksson et al., 2016; Ho et al., 2005) or for religiousness (Ahrenfeldt et al., 2016). Moreover, other reports have been negative including a comparison of parental reports of play activities and playmate preferences (Elizabeth and Green, 1984) and two observational studies of sex-typed childhood play (Henderson and Berenbaum, 1997; Rodgers et al., 1998). In the latter studies, however, the sample sizes were small (Table 3). Using the same methodology, four studies investigated disordered eating. One study found that OS females reported less disordered eating than SS females (Culbert et al., 2008), but the other three studies reported no differences (Baker et al., 2009; Lydecker et al., 2012; Raevuori et al., 2008). One recent study found lower levels of disordered eating for OS than for SS females during mid-late puberty but no differences during pre-early puberty (Culbert et al., 2013). No difference in the prevalence of alcohol-dependence was found between OS and SS females in large samples from Sweden and Australia (Lenz et al., 2012), but another study from Australia found that OS females had slightly more lifetime alcohol use disorder symptoms than SS females (Ellingson et al., 2013) (Table 3).

Overall, the evidence for prenatal hormone transfer on behavioral traits for males is sparse with most studies showing no differences between OS and SS males on various sensation-seeking scores (Resnick et al., 1993), feminine interests (Rose et al., 2002) and masculine attitudes (Miller and Martin, 1995). However, one study (Ho et al., 2005) reported that sub-threshold autistic symptomatology rated by parents was higher in SS than in OS male twins. Moreover, a single study reported more disordered eating for OS than for SS males (Culbert et al., 2008). In an older adult cohort, Loehlin and Martin found that OS males scored more feminine than SS males on a worried subscale. This scale contrasts individuals who describe themselves as fearful and worried with those describing themselves as calm and confident. Women score on average higher than men. Thus, the results from the older cohort suggest that having a female co-twin makes the male co-twin more anxious (Loehlin and Martin, 2000). However, for the younger twin cohort, results for this worried scale pointed in the opposite direction, and results for rule-breaking were also in contrast to what was expected, with OS males having more masculine scores than SS males (Loehlin and Martin, 2000). For alcohol dependence, OS males were more likely to become alcohol-dependent than SS males, and this was interpreted as indirect evidence for the role of prenatal testosterone on alcohol dependence (Lenz et al., 2012). One study on sex-typed behavior based on parental reports suggested that OS males might be demasculinized on sex-typical behavior (Elizabeth and Green, 1984). However, no differences were found in an observational study on two-year old children regarding toy preferences (Rodgers et al., 1998) (Table 3).

4.4. Other health traits

Recent register-based twin studies on epilepsy (Mao et al., 2018) and hormone-related cancers (Ahrenfeldt et al., 2015b) did not show differences between OS and SS twins, either for females or for males. In addition, a recent study on early life mortality risks found that OS girls did not have higher mortality risks than SS girls. However, in line with what was hypothesized, OS boys showed lower mortality than SS boys persisting within the first year of life. This finding might provide evidence for the TTT hypothesis, but according to the authors this was at least partly due to the inclusion of MZ twins in the SS twin group (Ahrenfeldt et al., 2017) (Table 4 and chapter 6.2).

5. Summary of main findings

5.1. Recent findings in relation to the TTT hypothesis

In this special issue, we summarize published studies investigating potential differences between OS and SS female and male twins, respectively focusing on the TTT hypothesis. A total of 60 articles fulfilled the eligibility criteria including 23 studies published since the review by Tapp et al. in 2011. Among recent studies of physiological, morphological and reproductive traits in OS and SS females, three studies were interpreted as providing evidence for the TTT hypothesis (Benetos et al., 2014; Butikofer et al., 2019; Ribeiro et al., 2013), whereas six studies (Bogl et al., 2017; Hiraishi et al., 2012; Jelenkovic et al., 2018; Korsoff et al., 2014; Sorensen et al., 2013; Tul et al., 2012) did not find support for prenatal hormone transfer in utero, and two studies (Alexanderson et al., 2011; Mareckova et al., 2015) reported some differences between OS and SS female twins. Mareckova et al. (2015) found that SS females differed from OS females in craniofacial morphology, but did not replicate previous findings on brain size. Alexanderson et al. (2011) found differences on anthropometric measures, but the findings were limited to individuals aged 60 and above.

In general, outcomes from OS twin studies on perception (mainly otoacoustic emissions) and cognition (expressive vocabulary and visuo-spatial ability) are mainly supportive of the TTT hypothesis especially for females, but studies of these traits have not been replicated in recent years. One recent study on cognitive traits did not provide evidence for the TTT hypothesis on academic performance in adolescence (Ahrenfeldt et al., 2015a), but a recent Norwegian register-based study found that OS females had lower educational and socioeconomic attainments than SS females (Butikofer et al., 2019). Among recent studies of behavioral traits, five studies did not report differences between OS and SS females (Ahrenfeldt et al., 2016; Attermann et al., 2012; Eriksson et al., 2016; Lenz et al., 2012; Lydecker et al., 2012), whereas two studies showed some differences in the expected direction on lifetime alcohol use disorder symptoms (Ellingson et al., 2013) and disordered eating (Culbert et al., 2013). One twin study found differences regarding masculinity-femininity in the hypothesized direction for females, but in the opposite direction for males (Verweij et al., 2016). Few studies have investigated whether health outcomes (including epilepsy, hormone related cancers and early life mortality risks) differ between OS and SS twins, but recent register-based studies on larges samples of Nordic twins did not find differences between OS and SS twins in the expected directions (Ahrenfeldt et al., 2017; Ahrenfeldt et al., 2015b; Mao et al., 2018).

Among the 13 recent studies including males, only few studies have provided some evidence for the TTT hypothesis (Ahrenfeldt et al., 2017; Lenz et al., 2012). Nevertheless, although some differences between OS and SS twins have been reported for several traits, there seems to be a growing body of research challenging the manifestation of the TTT hypothesis in observational studies.

5.2. Findings in relation to postnatal socialization

A concern in interpreting findings from OS twin studies is that the effects of the social environment can be interpreted as support for the TTT hypothesis (Tapp et al., 2011). However, although it may be difficult to separate postnatal psychosocial from prenatal hormonal influences, in general, there is sparse evidence regarding twins’ social interactions. Several studies of phenotypes for which psycho-social influences could be expected, for instance in toy preferences and aggression, did not show differences between OS and SS twins, whereas other domains in which social influences are expected to be minimal, such as cerebral lateralization, otoacoustic emissions and tooth size, are consistent with the TTT hypothesis. Few studies have used siblings as a control group for the psychosocial environment, but these studies have failed to find evidence of socialization effects (Ellingson et al., 2013; Heil et al., 2011; Henderson and Berenbaum, 1997; Slutske et al., 2011). In general, investigations of OS female twins have considered a hormonal, rather than a social explanation of possible sex-typing effects (Miller, 1994).

6. Methodological considerations

6.1. The value of twins for testing testosterone effects

So far, there is no direct evidence that OS females are exposed to higher testosterone levels during prenatal development than SS females. While some of the results could be interpreted as providing supporting evidence for the TTT hypothesis, it should be emphasized that hormonal exposure was not tested directly. Transfer of testosterone is assumed, based on animal studies with placentation patterns which are different from those of human twin pregnancies (Ryan and Vandenbergh, 2002). Moreover, the non-human animal studies on intrauterine position that forms the basis of OS studies find that masculinization is most likely for female fetuses that gestate between two males, with smaller effects for those positioned next to just one male (Ryan and Vandenbergh, 2002). Thus, even if hormone transfer exists between twins, exposure from one male co-twin may not be high enough to induce masculinization of the female or the effects might be counteracted by other hormones (Cohen-Bendahan et al., 2005b). In general, to make a valid investigation of the TTT hypothesis, the investigated phenotypes in OS/SS twin studies should be correlated with testosterone. However, the association with androgen exposure for some of the investigated outcomes are uncertain, for instance regarding autistic traits (Kung et al., 2016a; Kung et al., 2016b) and 2D:4D ratio (Berenbaum et al., 2009). Thus, the null findings for OS twin studies on these traits could also suggest that there is no influence of potential, excessive testosterone exposure in utero on these traits.

Available evidence suggests that prenatal testosterone influences human behaviors that show large differences between males and females (Hines et al., 2016), and the size of the sex difference should be considered in interpreting studies of OS and SS twins. The most common measure of the magnitude of gender differences in psychological research is Cohen’s d, which is calculated as the mean of males minus the mean of females, divided by the pooled within-groups standard deviation (Cohen, 1988). According to Cohen’s guidelines, d = 0.20 is interpreted as a small difference, d = 0.50 is a moderate difference and d = 0.80 is interpreted as a large difference. The most consistent sex difference in cognitive abilities favoring males exists for visuospatial abilities and is found in MRT performance with large effect sizes (Voyer et al., 1995). Among OS twin studies on cognitive function, Vuoksimaa et al. found, in a study of 804 Finnish twins that OS females outperformed SS females in MRT performance (d = 0.30), also significant after controlling for possible confounding variables including age, birth weight, gestational age, maternal age, and computer game experience (Vuoksimaa et al., 2010b). Heil et al. replicated the results of better MRT performance for OS than SS females (d = 0.38) in a sample of 200 female twins and 200 non-twin controls. They also found that OS females had higher MRT performance than females raised with a slightly older brother, which according to the authors highlighted the organizational effects of prenatal testosterone (Heil et al., 2011). MRT ability is thought to have implications for standardized exams in mathematics (Halpern et al., 2007). However, a recent register-based study on academic performance did not find differences between OS and SS twins in mathematics in the predicted direction, based on the TTT hypothesis (Ahrenfeldt et al., 2015a). This may be because the tasks on which the ninth-grade test scores and teacher ratings were based rely upon many other abilities than the MRT performance, or because sex differences in average mathematical tests are small (Else-Quest et al., 2010; Lindberg et al., 2010; Stoet and Geary, 2013). However, the size of the difference in mathematics scores (d = 0.06–0.15) was comparable with the sex difference in mathematics, but in the opposite direction (Ahrenfeldt et al., 2015a). In general, among OS twin studies reporting effect sizes (Cohen’s d), several studies found only small differences between OS and SS twins, and some of these findings are unlikely to be of clinical relevance e.g. the findings by Bogl et al. (2017) and by Kaprio et al. (1995). As with any review or meta-analysis, the findings in this study may be influenced by publication bias. Because statistically significant results are more likely to be published than null findings (Dickersin et al., 1987), the evidence against the TTT hypothesis may be even more pronounced than suggested in this review paper.

However, a problematic issue with a number of these studies is their inability to disentangle the OS twins’ prenatal hormonal environment from their postnatal social environment. In other words, finding more masculinized traits among OS female twins, relative to SS female twins, is consistent with both hormonal exposure and socialization. Two recent studies have been able to accomplish this task using unique genetically and environmentally informative kinships. One such study compared the nonverbal cognitive performance of OS female twins to that of OS virtual female twins (Segal et al., 2019). Virtual twins are same-age unrelated siblings raised together since birth, who replicate the twin relationship, but without the biological link. The sample size was small, but among the younger participants a trend toward higher scores on the Block Design subscale (a test of visuospatial ability) of the Wechsler IQ Test was found among the OS female twins who additionally shared their prenatal hormonal environment. This finding provides some support for the prenatal masculinization of these female twins. A second recent study reported reduced fertility and socioeconomic achievement among OS female twins, relative to SS female twins (Butikofer et al., 2019). More importantly, these findings were replicated using OS female twins whose cotwins had died during the first year of life, effectively separating the prenatal and postnatal environments. The addition of raised apart OS twins would bring an additional dimension to this work, because these co-twins share their prenatal circumstances, but not their rearing environment (Segal, 2012).

6.2. The monozygotic twins

Ideally, the TTT hypothesis should be tested using only DZ twins because the intrauterine environment and, thus, possible androgen exposure in twins may be affected by other factors of importance in twin pregnancies that may differ between MZ and DZ twins. They include, for example, left-right position in the uterus, placentation, number of chorions and twin differences in growth (Cohen-Bendahan et al., 2005b). Although several studies have restricted their samples to DZ twin pairs, it was not always possible to allocate all the SS twins to zygosity groups. For instance, 51% of twins in the Danish study investigating early life mortality risks (Ahrenfeldt et al., 2017) were twins with unknown zygosity (UZ). It is generally stated that twins whose zygosity is not accessible are disadvantaged compared with twins of known zygosity (Petersen et al., 2011) and that they are not representative of the twin population at large (Madsen et al., 2010). For example, UZ twins have lower socio-economic status and poorer school performance compared with the other twin groups (Petersen et al., 2009), and they have lower marriage rates and higher divorce rates compared with twins whose zygosity is known (Petersen et al., 2011). Zygosity information is available for all OS twins, i.e. twins who are less privileged are also included in this group. Thus, if the UZ twins are excluded, the SS twin group may not be comparable with the OS twins and bias may be present. It is possible, however, to exclude twins with known MZ status by comparing OS with SSDZ and UZ twins combined. Most studies have investigated DZ twins only, but most often there is no information about UZ twins in the OS twin studies. It may be preferable, however, to keep MZ twins within the comparisons if the MZ and DZ twins do not differ on the outcome variables.

7. Conclusion and future directions

The overall aim of this paper was to provide an overview of published studies investigating potential differences between OS and SS twins focusing on the TTT hypothesis. A total of 60 articles fulfilled the eligibility criteria, including 23 studies published since the last review article (Tapp et al., 2011). Overall, studies of cognition are conflicting, but cognition is the phenotype for which most support for the TTT hypothesis is found. Evidence regarding physiological and behavioral traits is less consistent. Most studies have failed to identify differences between OS and SS twin males. Since the last review, only a limited number of differences in physiological, cognitive and behavioral outcomes between OS and SS twins have been reported, and there seems to be a growing body of research challenging the manifestation of the TTT hypothesis in observational studies. Thus, although the value of twins for testing hormone effects is uncertain, findings are generally reassuring as they fail to find evidence for important differences between OS and SS twins on, for instance, health outcomes such as cancer risks later in life. We hope that this contribution to the special issue will stimulate a discussion about how an investigation of the TTT hypothesis should continue in future research.

To date, the outcome measures in the comparisons of OS and SS twins are mainly self-reported and only few studies have used registry-based outcomes. The large, worldwide twin registries linked to several health and administrative registers may be a useful tool for studying the effects of prenatal hormones as frequency of DZ twinning is increasing globally (Hoekstra et al., 2008). Nevertheless, future twin studies should take into account the role of the social environment. To do that, it would be best to examine virtual twins (unrelated siblings of the same sex raised together from early infancy) or twins reared apart (Segal, 2000; Segal et al., 2019). However, these subjects are rare. Another method would be to include singleton sibling pairs or siblings of twins (Cohen-Bendahan et al., 2005b; Henderson and Berenbaum, 1997) for instance by comparing OS females with SS females having a slightly older or younger brother. Differences between siblings may be related to the postnatal environment, contrary to twin pairs who share both a prenatal and a postnatal environment. In this way, the comparison group of siblings can help to differentiate between behavioral effects due to prenatal hormones and to the social environment, although twin pairs may affect each other differently than siblings of different ages (Cohen-Bendahan et al., 2005b). Only few studies (Ellingson et al., 2013; Heil et al., 2011; Henderson and Berenbaum, 1997; Slutske et al., 2011) have included such a comparison group. Moreover, age effects are important to consider in future studies, because prenatal testosterone effects may diminish with age as the influences of other hormonal or environmental factors become predominant (Cohen-Bendahan et al., 2005b). To study age effects, ideally longitudinal studies of OS vs. SS twins with sibling controls are needed. However, cross-sectional investigations may be helpful too. Furthermore, most twin studies examining the TTT hypothesis have been based on Caucasian samples. Given racial/ethnic differences in levels of hormone (Agurs-Collins et al., 2012; Richard et al., 2014), future twin studies should examine non-Caucasian samples, as well, to generalize the TTT effect across human populations. Lastly, a possible extension of the traditional twin study design to investigate the TTT hypothesis is to include triplets and perhaps also quadruplets because the effects of prenatal hormone transfer in animals are most pronounced for female fetuses positioned between two males. Recently, higher order (3+) multiple births and pregnancies have become increasingly common with the use of reproductive technology, and a few higher order multiple birth registries have been developed e.g. Yokoyama (2019). Thus, it would be possible in future studies to compare females positioned in utero between two males with those positioned between one male and one female or between two females.

Highlights.

  • Sex hormones may be transferred between fetuses in the same pregnancy (the Twin Testosterone Transfer hypothesis).

  • The TTT hypothesis was evaluated using 60 published studies investigating potential differences between opposite-sex and same-sex twins.

  • Cognition is the phenotype for which most support for the TTT hypothesis is found.

  • Studies of physiology and behavior are less supportive of the TTT hypothesis.

  • Overall, a growing body of research is challenging the manifestation of the TTT hypothesis.

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by research grants from the National Institute on Aging (NIA-PO1-AG08761, NIAP01-AG031719) and from the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007–2011) under grant agreement no 259679.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Abramovich DR, 1974. Human sexual differentiation--in utero influences. J. Obstet. Gynaecol. Br. Commonw 81, 448–453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Abramovich DR, Page KR, 1972. Pathways of water exchange in the fetoplacental unit at mid-pregnancy. J.Obstet.Gynaecol.Br.Commonw 79, 1099–1102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Agurs-Collins T, Rohrmann S, Sutcliffe C, Bienstock JL, Monsegue D, Akereyeni F, Bradwin G, Rifai N, Pollak MN, Platz EA, 2012. Racial variation in umbilical cord blood sex steroid hormones and the insulin-like growth factor axis in African-American and white female neonates. Cancer Causes Control 23, 445–454. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Ahrenfeldt L, Petersen I, Johnson W, Christensen K, 2015a. Academic performance of opposite-sex and same-sex twins in adolescence: A Danish national cohort study. Horm. Behav. 69, 123–131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Ahrenfeldt LJ, Larsen LA, Lindahl-Jacobsen R, Skytthe A, Hjelmborg JV, Moller S, Christensen K, 2017. Early-life mortality risks in opposite-sex and same-sex twins: a Danish cohort study of the twin testosterone transfer hypothesis. Ann. Epidemiol 27, 115–120.e112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Ahrenfeldt LJ, Lindahl-Jacobsen R, Moller S, Christensen K, Hvidtjorn D, Hvidt NC, 2016. Differences in Religiousness in Opposite-Sex and Same-Sex Twins in a Secular Society. Twin research and human genetics : the official journal of the International Society for Twin Studies 19, 35–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Ahrenfeldt LJ, Skytthe A, Moller S, Czene K, Adami HO, Mucci LA, Kaprio J, Petersen I, Christensen K, Lindahl-Jacobsen R, 2015b. Risk of sex-specific cancers in opposite-sex and same-sex twins in Denmark and Sweden. Cancer Epidemiol. Biomarkers Prev. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Alexanderson C, Henningsson S, Lichtenstein P, Holmang A, Eriksson E, 2011. Influence of having a male twin on body mass index and risk for dyslipidemia in middle-aged and old women. Int. J. Obes. (Lond.) 35, 1466–1469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Attermann J, Obel C, Bilenberg N, Nordenbaek CM, Skytthe A, Olsen J, 2012. Traits of ADHD and autism in girls with a twin brother: a Mendelian randomization study. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 21, 503–509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Baker JH, Lichtenstein P, Kendler KS, 2009. Intrauterine testosterone exposure and risk for disordered eating. Br.J.Psychiatry 194, 375–376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Benetos A, Dalgard C, Labat C, Kark JD, Verhulst S, Christensen K, Kimura M, Horvath K, Kyvik KO, Aviv A, 2014. Sex difference in leukocyte telomere length is ablated in opposite-sex co-twins. Int. J. Epidemiol 43, 1799–1805. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Berenbaum SA, Bryk KK, Nowak N, Quigley CA, Moffat S, 2009. Fingers as a marker of prenatal androgen exposure. Endocrinology 150, 5119–5124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bogl LH, Jelenkovic A, Vuoksimaa E, Ahrenfeldt L, Pietilainen KH, Stazi MA, Fagnani C, D’Ippolito C, Hur YM, Jeong HU, Silberg JL, Eaves LJ, Maes HH, Bayasgalan G, Narandalai D, Cutler TL, Kandler C, Jang KL, Christensen K, Skytthe A, Kyvik KO, Cozen W, Hwang AE, Mack TM, Derom CA, Vlietinck RF, Nelson TL, Whitfield KE, Corley RP, Huibregtse BM, McAdams TA, Eley TC, Gregory AM, Krueger RF, McGue M, Pahlen S, Willemsen G, Bartels M, van Beijsterveldt T, Pang Z, Tan Q, Zhang D, Martin NG, Medland SE, Montgomery GW, Hjelmborg JVB, Rebato E, Swan GE, Krasnow R, Busjahn A, Lichtenstein P, Oncel SY, Aliev F, Baker LA, Tuvblad C, Siribaddana SH, Hotopf M, Sumathipala A, Rijsdijk F, Magnusson PKE, Pedersen NL, Aslan AKD, Ordonana JR, Sanchez-Romera JF, Colodro-Conde L, Duncan GE, Buchwald D, Tarnoki AD, Tarnoki DL, Yokoyama Y, Hopper JL, Loos RJF, Boomsma DI, Sorensen TIA, Silventoinen K, Kaprio J, 2017. Does the sex of one’s co-twin affect height and BMI in adulthood? A study of dizygotic adult twins from 31 cohorts. Biol. Sex Differ. 8, 14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Butikofer A, Figlio DN, Karbownik K, Kuzawa CW, Salvanes KG, 2019. Evidence that prenatal testosterone transfer from male twins reduces the fertility and socioeconomic success of their female co-twins. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 116, 6749–6753. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Christensen K, Basso O, Kyvik KO, Juul S, Boldsen J, Vaupel JW, Olsen J, 1998. Fecundability of female twins. Epidemiology 9, 189–192. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cohen-Bendahan CC, Buitelaar JK, van Goozen SH, Cohen-Kettenis PT, 2004. Prenatal exposure to testosterone and functional cerebral lateralization: a study in same-sex and opposite-sex twin girls. Psychoneuroendocrinology 29, 911–916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cohen-Bendahan CC, Buitelaar JK, van Goozen SH, Orlebeke JF, Cohen-Kettenis PT, 2005a. Is there an effect of prenatal testosterone on aggression and other behavioral traits? A study comparing same-sex and opposite-sex twin girls. Horm.Behav 47, 230–237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cohen-Bendahan CC, van de Beek C, Berenbaum SA, 2005b. Prenatal sex hormone effects on child and adult sex-typed behavior: methods and findings. Neurosci.Biobehav.Rev 29, 353–384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cohen J, 1988. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences 2nd ed. Hillsdale NJ; Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
  20. Collaer ML, Hines M, 1995. Human behavioral sex differences: a role for gonadal hormones during early development? Psychol. Bull 118, 55–107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Constantinescu M, Hines M, 2012. Relating Prenatal Testosterone Exposure to Postnatal Behavior in Typically Developing Children: Methods and Findings. Child Development Perspectives 6, 407–413. [Google Scholar]
  22. Culbert KM, Breedlove SM, Burt SA, Klump KL, 2008. Prenatal hormone exposure and risk for eating disorders: a comparison of opposite-sex and same-sex twins. Arch.Gen.Psychiatry 65, 329–336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Culbert KM, Breedlove SM, Sisk CL, Burt SA, Klump KL, 2013. The emergence of sex differences in risk for disordered eating attitudes during puberty: a role for prenatal testosterone exposure. J. Abnorm. Psychol 122, 420–432. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Dempsey PJ, Townsend GC, Richards LC, 1999. Increased tooth crown size in females with twin brothers: Evidence for hormonal diffusion between human twins in utero. Am.J.Hum.Biol 11, 577–586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Dickersin K, Chan S, Chalmers TC, Sacks HS, Smith H Jr., 1987. Publication bias and clinical trials. Control. Clin. Trials 8, 343–353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Elizabeth PH, Green R, 1984. Childhood sex-role behaviors: similarities and differences in twins. Acta Genet.Med.Gemellol.(Roma.) 33, 173–179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Elkadi S, Nicholls ME, Clode D, 1999. Handedness in opposite and same-sex dizygotic twins: testing the testosterone hypothesis. Neuroreport 10, 333–336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Ellingson JM, Slutske WS, Richmond-Rakerd LS, Martin NG, 2013. Investigating the influence of prenatal androgen exposure and sibling effects on alcohol use and alcohol use disorder in females from opposite-sex twin pairs. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res 37, 868–876. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Else-Quest NM, Hyde JS, Linn MC, 2010. Cross-national patterns of gender differences in mathematics: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull 136, 103–127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Eriksson JM, Lundstrom S, Lichtenstein P, Bejerot S, Eriksson E, 2016. Effect of co-twin gender on neurodevelopmental symptoms: a twin register study. Mol. Autism 7, 8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Fishman R, Vortman Y, Shanas U, Koren L, 2019. Non-model species deliver a non-model result: Nutria female fetuses neighboring males in utero have lower testosterone. Horm. Behav [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Gaist D, Bathum L, Skytthe A, Jensen TK, McGue M, Vaupel JW, Christensen K, 2000. Strength and anthropometric measures in identical and fraternal twins: no evidence of masculinization of females with male co-twins. Epidemiology 11, 340–343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Galsworthy MJ, Plomin R, Dionne G, Dale PS, 2000. Sex differences in early verbal and non-verbal cognitive development. Developmental Science 3, 206. [Google Scholar]
  34. Geschwind N, Behan P, 1982. Left-handedness: association with immune disease, migraine, and developmental learning disorder. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 79, 5097–5100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Glass AR, Klein T, 1981. Changes in maternal serum total and free androgen levels in early pregnancy: lack of correlation with fetal sex. Am. J. Obstet. Gynecol 140, 656–660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hall JG, 2003. Twinning. Lancet 362, 735–743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Halpern DF, Benbow CP, Geary DC, Gur RC, Hyde JS, Gernsbacher MA, 2007. The Science of Sex Differences in Science and Mathematics. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 8, 1–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Hampson E, Rovet JF, Altmann D, 1998. Spatial reasoning in children with congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to 21‐hydroxylase deficiency. Dev. Neuropsychol 14, 299–320. [Google Scholar]
  39. Heil M, Kavsek M, Rolke B, Beste C, Jansen P, 2011. Mental rotation in female fraternal twins: Evidence for intra-uterine hormone transfer? Biol.Psychol 86, 90–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Henderson BA, Berenbaum SA, 1997. Sex-typed play in opposite-sex twins. Dev.Psychobiol 31, 115–123. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Hickey M, Sloboda DM, Atkinson HC, Doherty DA, Franks S, Norman RJ, Newnham JP, Hart R, 2009. The relationship between maternal and umbilical cord androgen levels and polycystic ovary syndrome in adolescence: a prospective cohort study. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab 94, 3714–3720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Hines M, 2006. Prenatal testosterone and gender-related behaviour. Eur. J. Endocrinol 155 Suppl 1, S115–121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Hines M, 2008. Early androgen influences on human neural and behavioural development. Early Hum. Dev 84, 805–807. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Hines M, 2010. Sex-related variation in human behavior and the brain. Trends Cogn. Sci 14, 448–456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Hines M, 2011. Gender development and the human brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci 34, 69–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Hines M, Fane BA, Pasterski VL, Mathews GA, Conway GS, Brook C, 2003. Spatial abilities following prenatal androgen abnormality: targeting and mental rotations performance in individuals with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Psychoneuroendocrinology 28, 1010–1026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Hines M, Golombok S, Rust J, Johnston KJ, Golding J, 2002. Testosterone during pregnancy and gender role behavior of preschool children: a longitudinal, population study. Child Dev. 73, 1678–1687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hines M, Spencer D, Kung KT, Browne WV, Constantinescu M, Noorderhaven RM, 2016. The early postnatal period, mini-puberty, provides a window on the role of testosterone in human neurobehavioural development. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol 38, 69–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hiraishi K, Sasaki S, Shikishima C, Ando J, 2012. The second to fourth digit ratio (2D:4D) in a Japanese twin sample: heritability, prenatal hormone transfer, and association with sexual orientation. Arch. Sex. Behav 41, 711–724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Ho A, Todd RD, Constantino JN, 2005. Brief report: autistic traits in twins vs. non-twins--a preliminary study. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 35, 129–133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Hoekstra C, Zhao ZZ, Lambalk CB, Willemsen G, Martin NG, Boomsma DI, Montgomery GW, 2008. Dizygotic twinning. Hum.Reprod.Update 14, 37–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Jelenkovic A, Sund R, Yokoyama Y, Hur YM, Ullemar V, Almqvist C, Magnusson PK, Willemsen G, Bartels M, Beijsterveldt CEV, Bogl LH, Pietilainen KH, Vuoksimaa E, Ji F, Ning F, Pang Z, Nelson TL, Whitfield KE, Rebato E, Llewellyn CH, Fisher A, Bayasgalan G, Narandalai D, Bjerregaard-Andersen M, Beck-Nielsen H, Sodemann M, Tarnoki AD, Tarnoki DL, Ooki S, Stazi MA, Fagnani C, Brescianini S, Dubois L, Boivin M, Brendgen M, Dionne G, Vitaro F, Cutler TL, Hopper JL, Krueger RF, McGue M, Pahlen S, Craig JM, Saffery R, Haworth CM, Plomin R, Knafo-Noam A, Mankuta D, Abramson L, Burt SA, Klump KL, Vlietinck RF, Derom CA, Loos RJ, Boomsma DI, Sorensen TIA, Kaprio J, Silventoinen K, 2018. Birth size and gestational age in opposite-sex twins as compared to same-sex twins: An individual-based pooled analysis of 21 cohorts. Sci. Rep 8, 6300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Kaprio J, Rimpela A, Winter T, Viken RJ, Rimpela M, Rose RJ, 1995. Common genetic influences on BMI and age at menarche. Hum. Biol 67, 739–753. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Korsoff P, Bogl LH, Korhonen P, Kangas AJ, Soininen P, Ala-Korpela M, Rose RJ, Kaaja R, Kaprio J, 2014. A comparison of anthropometric, metabolic, and reproductive characteristics of young adult women from opposite-sex and same-sex twin pairs. Front. Endocrinol. (Lausanne) 5, 28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kuijper EA, Vink JM, Lambalk CB, Boomsma DI, 2009. Prevalence of polycystic ovary syndrome in women from opposite-sex twin pairs. J.Clin.Endocrinol.Metab 94, 1987–1990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Kung KT, Constantinescu M, Browne WV, Noorderhaven RM, Hines M, 2016a. No relationship between early postnatal testosterone concentrations and autistic traits in 18 to 30-month-old children. Mol. Autism 7, 15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Kung KT, Spencer D, Pasterski V, Neufeld S, Glover V, O’Connor TG, Hindmarsh PC, Hughes IA, Acerini CL, Hines M, 2016b. No relationship between prenatal androgen exposure and autistic traits: convergent evidence from studies of children with congenital adrenal hyperplasia and of amniotic testosterone concentrations in typically developing children. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 57, 1455–1462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Laffey-Ardley S, Thorpe K, 2006. Being opposite: is there advantage for social competence and friendships in being an opposite-sex twin? Twin research and human genetics : the official journal of the International Society for Twin Studies 9, 131–140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Lenz B, Muller CP, Kornhuber J, 2012. Alcohol dependence in same-sex and opposite-sex twins. J. Neural Transm 119, 1561–1564. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Lewi L, 2010. Monochorionic diamniotic twin pregnancies pregnancy outcome, risk stratification and lessons learnt from placental examination. Verh. K. Acad. Geneeskd. Belg 72, 5–15. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Lindberg SM, Hyde JS, Petersen JL, Linn MC, 2010. New trends in gender and mathematics performance: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull 136, 1123–1135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Loehlin JC, Martin NG, 1998. A comparison of adult female twins from opposite-sex and same-sex pairs on variables related to reproduction. Behav.Genet 28, 21–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Loehlin JC, Martin NG, 2000. Dimensions of psychological masculinity-femininity in adult twins from opposite-sex and same-sex pairs. Behav.Genet 30, 19–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Loos R, Derom C, Vlietinck R, Derom R, 1998. The East Flanders Prospective Twin Survey (Belgium): a population-based register. Twin Res. 1, 167–175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Lundborg P, 2008. The Health Returns to Education: What can we learn from twins?, pp. 1–32.
  66. Lydecker JA, Pisetsky EM, Mitchell KS, Thornton LM, Kendler KS, Reichborn-Kjennerud T, Lichtenstein P, Bulik CM, Mazzeo SE, 2012. Association between co-twin sex and eating disorders in opposite sex twin pairs: evaluations in North American, Norwegian, and Swedish samples. J. Psychosom. Res 72, 73–77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Madsen M, Andersen AM, Christensen K, Andersen PK, Osler M, 2010. Does educational status impact adult mortality in Denmark? A twin approach. Am.J.Epidemiol. 172, 225–234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Manning JT, 2002. Digit ratio: a pointer to fertility, behavior and health. Rutgers University Press, New Brunswick, New Jersey, and London. [Google Scholar]
  69. Mao Y, Ahrenfeldt LJ, Christensen K, Wu C, Christensen J, Olsen J, Sun Y, 2018. Risk of epilepsy in opposite-sex and same-sex twins: a twin cohort study. Biol. Sex Differ 9, 21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Mareckova K, Chakravarty MM, Lawrence C, Leonard G, Perusse D, Perron M, Pike BG, Richer L, Veillette S, Pausova Z, Paus T, 2015. Identifying craniofacial features associated with prenatal exposure to androgens and testing their relationship with brain development. Brain Struct. Funct 220, 3233–3244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. McFadden D, 1993. A masculinizing effect on the auditory systems of human females having male co-twins. Proc.Natl.Acad.Sci.U.S.A 90, 11900–11904. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. McFadden D, Loehlin JC, Pasanen EG, 1996. Additional findings on heritability and prenatal masculinization of cochlear mechanisms: click-evoked otoacoustic emissions. Hear. Res 97, 102–119. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. McFadden D, Shubel E, 2003. The relationships between otoacoustic emissions and relative lengths of fingers and toes in humans. Horm. Behav 43, 421–429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Medland SE, Duffy DL, Wright MJ, Geffen GM, Hay DA, Levy F, van-Beijsterveldt CE, Willemsen G, Townsend GC, White V, Hewitt AW, Mackey DA, Bailey JM, Slutske WS, Nyholt DR, Treloar SA, Martin NG, Boomsma DI, 2009. Genetic influences on handedness: data from 25,732 Australian and Dutch twin families. Neuropsychologia 47, 330–337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Medland SE, Loehlin JC, Martin NG, 2008a. No effects of prenatal hormone transfer on digit ratio in a large sample of same- and opposite-sex dizygotic twins. Pers. Individ. Dif 44, 1225–1234. [Google Scholar]
  76. Medland SE, Loehlin JC, Willemsen G, Hatemi PK, Keller MC, Boomsma DI, Eaves LJ, Martin NG, 2008b. Males do not reduce the fitness of their female co-twins in contemporary samples. Twin.Res.Hum.Genet 11, 481–487. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Miller EM, 1994. Prenatal sex hormone transfer: A reason to study opposite-sex twins. Pers. Individ. Dif 17, 511–529. [Google Scholar]
  78. Miller EM, Martin N, 1995. Analysis of the effect of hormones on opposite-sex twin attitudes. Acta Genet.Med.Gemellol.(Roma.) 44, 41–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Nagamani M, McDonough PG, Ellegood JO, Mahesh VB, 1979. Maternal and amniotic fluid steroids throughout human pregnancy. Am. J. Obstet. Gynecol 134, 674–680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Neyer FJ, 2002. Twin Relationships in Old Age: A Developmental Perspective. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 19, 155–177. [Google Scholar]
  81. Ooki S, 2006. Nongenetic factors associated with human handedness and footedness in Japanese twin children. Environ.Health Prev.Med 11, 304–312. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Pang S, Levine LS, Cederqvist LL, Fuentes M, Riccardi VM, Holcombe JH, Nitowsky HM, Sachs G, Anderson CE, Duchon MA, Owens R, Merkatz I, New MI, 1980. Amniotic fluid concentrations of delta 5 and delta 4 steroids in fetuses with congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to 21 hydroxylase deficiency and in anencephalic fetuses. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab 51, 223–229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Peper JS, Brouwer RM, Van Baal GC, Schnack HG, van LM, Boomsma DI, Kahn RS, Hulshoff Pol HE, 2009. Does having a twin brother make for a bigger brain? Eur.J.Endocrinol 160, 739–746. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Petersen I, Jensen VM, McGue M, Bingley P, Christensen K, 2009. No evidence of genetic mediation in the association between birthweight and academic performance in 2,413 Danish adolescent twin pairs. Twin.Res.Hum.Genet 12, 564–572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Petersen I, Martinussen T, McGue M, Bingley P, Christensen K, 2011. Lower marriage and divorce rates among twins than among singletons in Danish birth cohorts 1940–1964. Twin.Res.Hum.Genet 14, 150–157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Phoenix CH, 2009. Organizing action of prenatally administered testosterone propionate on the tissues mediating mating behavior in the female guinea pig. Hormones and behavior 55, 566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Phoenix CH, Goy RW, Gerall AA, Young WC, 1959. Organizing action of prenatally administered testosterone propionate on the tissues mediating mating behavior in the female guinea pig. Endocrinology 65, 369–382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Pulkkinen L, Vaalamo I, Hietala R, Kaprio J, Rose RJ, 2003. Peer reports of adaptive behavior in twins and singletons: is twinship a risk or an advantage? Twin.Res 6, 106–118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Raevuori A, Kaprio J, Hoek HW, Sihvola E, Rissanen A, Keski-Rahkonen A, 2008. Anorexia and bulimia nervosa in same-sex and opposite-sex twins: lack of association with twin type in a nationwide study of Finnish twins. Am.J.Psychiatry 165, 1604–1610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Resnick SM, Gottesman II, McGue M, 1993. Sensation seeking in opposite-sex twins: an effect of prenatal hormones? Behav.Genet 23, 323–329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Ribeiro DC, Brook AH, Hughes TE, Sampson WJ, Townsend GC, 2013. Intrauterine Hormone Effects on Tooth Dimensions. J. Dent. Res [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Richard A, Rohrmann S, Zhang L, Eichholzer M, Basaria S, Selvin E, Dobs AS, Kanarek N, Menke A, Nelson WG, Platz EA, 2014. Racial variation in sex steroid hormone concentration in black and white men: a meta-analysis. Andrology 2, 428–435. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Rodgers C, Fagot B, Winebarger A, 1998. Gender-Typed Toy Play in Dizygotic Twin Pairs: A Test of Hormone Transfer Theory. Sex Roles 39, 173–184. [Google Scholar]
  94. Rose RJ, Kaprio J, Winter T, Dick DM, Viken RJ, Pulkkinen L, Koskenvuo M, 2002. Femininity and fertility in sisters with twin brothers: prenatal androgenization? Cross-sex socialization? Psychol.Sci 13, 263–267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Ryan BC, Vandenbergh JG, 2002. Intrauterine position effects. Neurosci.Biobehav.Rev 26, 665–678. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Segal N, 1999. Entwined Lives: Twins and what they tell us about Human Behavior. Twin Res. 2, 291–292. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Segal NL, 2000. Virtual twins: New findings on within-family environmental influences on intelligence. J. Educ. Psychol 92, 442–448. [Google Scholar]
  98. Segal NL, 2012. Born together-reared apart: The landmark Minnesota twin study. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, USA. [Google Scholar]
  99. Segal NL, Orozco EN, Preston KJS, Gerkens DR, 2019. Nonverbal cognition across genetic and environmentally informative kinships: The case for opposite-sex twin pairs and virtual twins. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 182, 144–150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Slutske WS, Bascom EN, Meier MH, Medland SE, Martin NG, 2011. Sensation seeking in females from opposite- versus same-sex twin pairs: hormone transfer or sibling imitation? Behav.Genet 41, 533–542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Sorensen K, Juul A, Christensen K, Skytthe A, Scheike T, Kold Jensen T, 2013. Birth size and age at menarche: a twin perspective. Hum. Reprod 28, 2865–2871. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Stoet G, Geary DC, 2013. Sex differences in mathematics and reading achievement are inversely related: within- and across-nation assessment of 10 years of PISA data. PLoS One 8, e57988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Talmadge CL, Long GR, Murphy WJ, Tubis A, 1993. New off-line method for detecting spontaneous otoacoustic emissions in human subjects. Hear. Res. 71, 170–182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Tapp A, Maybery MT, Whitehouse AJ, 2011. Evaluating the twin testosterone transfer hypothesis: A review of the empirical evidence. Horm.Behav [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Tul N, Lucovnik M, Novak Z, Verdenik I, Blickstein I, 2012. No “masculinization” effect of a male on birth weight of its female co-twin. J. Perinat. Med 40, 255–257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. van Anders SM, Vernon PA, Wilbur CJ, 2006. Finger-length ratios show evidence of prenatal hormone-transfer between opposite-sex twins. Horm.Behav 49, 315–319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. van de Beek C, Thijssen JH, Cohen-Kettenis PT, van Goozen SH, Buitelaar JK, 2004. Relationships between sex hormones assessed in amniotic fluid, and maternal and umbilical cord serum: what is the best source of information to investigate the effects of fetal hormonal exposure? Horm. Behav. 46, 663–669. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Van Hulle CA, Goldsmith HH, Lemery KS, 2004. Genetic, environmental, and gender effects on individual differences in toddler expressive language. J. Speech. Lang. Hear. Res 47, 904–912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Verweij KJ, Mosing MA, Ullen F, Madison G, 2016. Individual Differences in Personality Masculinity-Femininity: Examining the Effects of Genes, Environment, and Prenatal Hormone Transfer. Twin research and human genetics : the official journal of the International Society for Twin Studies 19, 87–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. vom Saal FS, 1989. Sexual differentiation in litter-bearing mammals: influence of sex of adjacent fetuses in utero. J.Anim Sci. 67, 1824–1840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Voracek M, Dressler SG, 2007. Digit ratio (2D:4D) in twins: heritability estimates and evidence for a masculinized trait expression in women from opposite-sex pairs. Psychol.Rep 100, 115–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Voyer D, Voyer S, Bryden MP, 1995. Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: a meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychol. Bull 117, 250–270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Vuoksimaa E, Eriksson CJ, Pulkkinen L, Rose RJ, Kaprio J, 2010a. Decreased prevalence of left-handedness among females with male co-twins: evidence suggesting prenatal testosterone transfer in humans? Psychoneuroendocrinology 35, 1462–1472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Vuoksimaa E, Kaprio J, Kremen WS, Hokkanen L, Viken RJ, Tuulio-Henriksson A, Rose RJ, 2010b. Having a male co-twin masculinizes mental rotation performance in females. Psychol.Sci 21, 1069–1071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Warne GL, Faiman C, Reyes FI, Winter JS, 1977. Studies on human sexual development. V. Concentrations of testosterone, 17-hydroxyprogesterone and progesterone in human amniotic fluid throughout gestation. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab 44, 934–938. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Whitehouse AJO, Maybery MT, Hart R, Mattes E, Newnham JP, Sloboda DM, Stanley FJ, Hickey M, 2010. Fetal androgen exposure and pragmatic language ability of girls in middle childhood: Implications for the extreme male-brain theory of autism. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35, 1259–1264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Wilson JD, George FW, Griffin JE, 1981. The hormonal control of sexual development. Science 211, 1278–1284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Witelson SF, 1991. Neural sexual mosaicism: sexual differentiation of the human temporo-parietal region for functional asymmetry. Psychoneuroendocrinology 16, 131–153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Yokoyama Y, 2019. The New West Japan Twins and Higher Order Multiple Births Registry. Twin Research and Human Genetics (in press). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES