Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Apr 1.
Published in final edited form as: Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2019 Apr;19(2):309–326. doi: 10.3758/s13415-018-00670-3

Socioeconomic Disadvantage, Brain Morphometry, and Attentional Bias to Threat in Middle Childhood

Alexander J Dufford 1, Hannah Bianco 1, Pilyoung Kim 1
PMCID: PMC6951020  NIHMSID: NIHMS1513154  PMID: 30460484

Abstract

Socioeconomic disadvantage is associated with higher rates of psychopathology as well as hippocampus, amygdala and prefrontal cortex structure. However, little is known about how variations in brain morphometry are associated with socio-emotional risks for mood disorders in children growing up in families experiencing low income. In the current study, using structural MRI, we examined the relationship between socioeconomic disadvantage and gray matter volume in the hippocampus, amygdala, and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex in a sample of children (n = 34) in middle childhood. Using an affective dot probe paradigm, we examined the association between gray matter volume in these regions and attentional bias to threat, a risk marker for mood disorders including anxiety disorders. We found that lower income-to-needs ratio was associated with lower bilateral hippocampal and right amygdala volume, but not prefrontal cortex volumes. Moreover, lower attentional bias to threat was associated with greater left hippocampal volume. We provide evidence of a relationship between income-related variations in brain structure and attentional bias to threat, a risk for mood disorders. Therefore, these findings support an environment-morphometry-behavior relationship that contributes to the understanding of income-related mental health disparities in childhood.

Keywords: family income, hippocampus, amygdala, attentional bias, middle childhood, morphometry

1. Introducation

Growing up in families experiencing socioeconomic disadvantage (SED) has multiple implications including behavioral (Hanson et al., 2015; Pachter, Auinger, Palmer, & Weitzman, 2006) and emotional dysregulation in childhood (Evans & Kim, 2013; Raver, Roy, Pressler, Ursache, & Charles McCoy, 2016) and anxiety and mood disorders later in life (Barch et al., 2016; Najman et al., 2010). The link between childhood SED and socioemotional difficulties is well supported (Green & Benzeval, 2013). Children, between the ages of 10 and 15 that are experiencing SED are 2.5 times more likely to develop anxiety or depressed mood than their peers experiencing middle or high socioeconomic position (Lemstra, 2008). Growing literature suggests that SED is associated with altered brain development which may further be associated with negative childhood outcomes. However, most studies examining childhood SED and the brain focus on the relationship with cognitive outcomes (Hair, Hanson, Wolfe, & Pollak, 2015; Noble, Wolmetz, Ochs, Farah, & McCandliss, 2006), thus little is known about potential neurobiological mechanisms for the socioemotional outcomes. SED is associated with greater exposure to adverse psychosocial and physical environment (Brooks-Gunn & Duncan, 1997; Evans, 2004; Raver, Blair, & Garrett-Peters, 2015). Thus, the higher levels of exposure to threats may be associated with increased vigilance to negative cues in the environment. Attentional biases to threat refer to tendencies to direct attention towards negative stimuli and are central to mechanistic models of anxiety and mood disorders (Beck, 2008; Dalgleish & Watts, 1990; Hankin, Snyder, & Gulley, 2016; Mathews & MacLeod, 2005). Thus, in the current study, we examine whether SED and brain morphometry may be associated with attentional bias to threat in middle childhood.

Previous studies suggest a relationship between family income and brain morphometry in adulthood. While the specific mechanisms of this association in humans remains unclear, evidence from non-human animal models suggest exposure to chronic stress may lead to alterations in brain morphometry. The hippocampus, amygdala, and prefrontal cortex have emerged as regions that are particularly susceptible to effects of chronic stress (Davidson & McEwen, 2012). Specifically, chronic stress is associated with altered spine density, dendritic length, and dendritic branching in these regions (Bloss, Janssen, McEwen, & Morrison, 2010; McEwen, 2007). These neuronal changes may be due to the association between chronic stress and increased glucocorticoid levels in the brain (Hall, Moda, & Liston, 2015) which play a critical role in the regulation of dendritic development (Liston & Gan, 2011). In the short term, the increased glucocorticoids act to mobilize resources to adapt to the stressor. However, long-term exposure to the increased glucocorticoid levels may have maladaptive effects on brain development and long-term consequences on behavior (McEwen, 1998, 2003; Oitzl, Champagne, van der Veen, & de Kloet, 2010; Welberg & Seckl, 2001).

Neuroimaging studies have identified a relationship between family income and gray matter volume, particularly in the hippocampus and amygdala in childhood (Johnson, Riis, & Noble, 2016). In studies examining childhood, lower family income was consistently associated with reduced gray matter volume in the hippocampus (Hanson, Chandra, Wolfe, & Pollak, 2011; Luby et al., 2013a). However, for the amygdala, there have been inconsistent findings regarding the direction of the association. Some studies have found a positive relationship between family income and amygdala gray matter volume (Hanson et al., 2015; Luby et al., 2013a), while other studies show a negative relationship (Noble, Houston, Kan, & Sowell, 2012) or no association (Noble et al., 2015). Others have also reported a negative association between family income and gray matter volume of other brain regions including the prefrontal cortex (Hair et al., 2015; Hanson et al., 2013). Studies have begun to examine how SED and brain regions that are associated with it are further related to behavioral outcomes. These studies have primarily focused on cognitive outcomes such as learning/memory (Hair et al., 2015), language (Noble et al., 2006), and academic achievement (Hair et al., 2015). However, the associations between SED, the brain, and behavioral risks for socioemotional outcomes are unclear.

Middle childhood is a period when children are increasingly vulnerable to the adverse effects of low family income on emotional development (Blair & Raver, 2012; Evans & English, 2002). While clinically significant symptoms may not be present until adolescence, it is possible that children exhibit an early socioemotional and neural risk marker for anxiety disorders in middle childhood (Dia & Bradshaw, 2008; Hankin et al., 2016). Attentional bias to threatening cues are a risk marker for emotion dysregulation, and may play a role in the development of anxiety disorders (Bar-Haim, Lamy, Pergamin, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & Van Ijzendoorn, 2007; Cisler & Koster, 2010) and depression (Mogg, Bradley, & Williams, 1995). Environmental adversity such as maltreatment has been associated with greater attentional bias to threatening cues (Pine et al., 2005; Pollak & Tolley-Schell, 2003). Experiencing low family income is associated with increased exposure to adversity including harsh parenting (Repetti, Taylor, & Seeman, 2002) and exposure to violence (Sampson, Raudenbush, & Earls, 1997). Thus, experiencing low family income may increase threat bias, which in turn is a possible socioemotional risk marker for the development of psychopathology later in life.

Middle childhood is also when the structure of the brain including the hippocampus, amygdala, and prefrontal cortex may be vulnerable to adverse environmental influences (Dannlowski et al., 2012; Hanson et al., 2010; Hanson et al., 2015). Further, variations in brain development due to exposure to adversity may be linked to the variations in socio-emotional processing such as attentional bias to threat. Based upon the connectivity and functional activity of the amygdala it has been conceptualized to be a hub for cognitive-emotional integration in the brain (Pessoa, 2013). Traditionally thought to be primarily involved in affective processes such as fear processing, the amygdala has been demonstrated to be involved in several cognitive processes including attention and associative learning (Holland & Gallagher, 1999) and evidence suggests the amygdala is a center for integrating cognitive appraisal of affective information in the brain (Pessoa, 2008). Variations in amygdala volume are observed in several disorders that are associated with increased attentional bias to threat, specifically among patients with anxiety disorders (Bar-Haim et al., 2007) including pediatric anxiety (Milham et al., 2005).

There is also evidence that the hippocampus is involved in attentional bias to threat. Neurobiologically, the role of the hippocampus in threat bias has been hypothesized to be due to its involvement in contextual threat processing and memory (Alvarez, Biggs, Chen, Pine, & Grillon, 2008; Grupe, Wielgosz, Davidson, & Nitschke, 2016; Maren, Phan, & Liberzon, 2013). First, altered hippocampal morphometry was associated with increased attentional bias to threat in carriers of the rs1360780 T allele of FKBP5 (Fani et al., 2013). Further, hippocampal microstructure was shown to be associated with abnormal threat processing in clinical anxiety and comorbid depression (Grupe et al., 2016). This study suggests hippocampal microstructure to be related to threat processing and may be a common neural pathway for the comorbidity of anxiety and depression (Grupe et al., 2016). Alterations in hippocampal volume have also been linked to threat bias in individuals experiencing posttraumatic stress disorder (Grupe et al., 2016). While limbic regions such as the amygdala and hippocampus are involved in attentional allocation to emotional stimuli such as threats, the prefrontal cortex (specifically the lateral prefrontal cortex) has been demonstrated to be involved in top-down regulation when confronting threats (Bishop, Duncan, Brett, & Lawrence, 2004; MacDonald, Cohen, Stenger, & Carter, 2000). Evidence suggests that the hippocampus, amygdala, and prefrontal cortex are involved in fear acquisition and extinction that may be underlying attentional biases to threat .(Hutcheson, Clark, Bolding, White, & Lahti, 2014). However, the associations between SED, morphometry of the hippocampus, amygdala, and prefrontal cortex and threat bias are unclear.

While more studies focused on family income, some examined parental education to observe the role of socioeconomic status (SES) in child brain structure. The two SES indicators, family income and parental education, are often correlated but some argue that they may have a unique role in brain development. For example, while family income is more closely linked to access to resources and environmental stressors (Evans & English, 2002), parental education is more closely tied to cognitive stimulation in the home environment (Hoff-Ginsberg & Tardif, 1995). For the hippocampus, two studies found no associations (Hanson et al., 2011; Noble, Houston, et al., 2012) while one study reported that left hippocampal volumes were positively associated with paternal education (Noble et al., 2015). For the amygdala, one study found a negative correlation between maternal education and gray matter volume (Noble, Houston, et al., 2012) while two studies have found no association (Hanson et al., 2011; Luby et al., 2013b). Thus, although there are mixed results for parental education, we included testing the association between maternal education and brain structure as a secondary analysis to capture the multidimensional nature of SES.

Thus, in the current study, we examine associations among SED (as defined by INR and maternal education), brain morphometry, and attentional bias to threat in middle childhood. We focused our analysis on the following research questions: (1) Is SED related to gray matter volume in the hippocampus, amygdala, and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex? (2) Are SED and gray matter volumes in the hippocampus, amygdala, and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex related to higher attentional bias to threat? In accordance with previous studies, we hypothesized SED, particularly lower INR, would be associated with lower gray matter volume in the hippocampus and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Given the mixed evidence in the literature, we hypothesize that SED will be associated with amygdala volume, but in either a positive or negative direction. Importantly, we hypothesized that SED and gray matter volumes in the hippocampus, amygdala, and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex would be associated with greater attentional bias to threat.

We conducted additional post-hoc analyses to examine the specificity of the results. First, we tested the relative strength of the association of INR versus maternal education with brain morphometry and threat bias. Second, we tested additional ‘control’ regions to demonstrate that the relationships were specific to the certain brain regions and was not evident in regions that we did not expect to have a relationship with SED such as the precentral gyrus, basal ganglia, and lateral occipital gyrus. Lastly, we tested if other factors may explain the relationship between SED and the regions of interest. We examined if child’s IQ (Blair, 2006; Reiss, Abrams, Singer, Ross, & Denckla, 1996), anxiety levels (De Bellis et al., 2000; Pine, 2007), or pubertal status (Blakemore, Burnett, & Dahl, 2010; Peper, Pol, Crone, & Van Honk, 2011) were potential confounds of the relationship between INR and the hypothesized regions.

1. Materials and methods

2.1. Participants

Participants and their biological mothers were recruited from a volunteer database at the University of Denver as well as via flyers placed in Denver metro area public schools and antipoverty programs. We obtained a socioeconomically diverse sample by initially screening 149 families and oversampling low-income families (approximately 50% were low-income). Of the 47 families that participated in the home visit, 35 were eligible for and agreed to participate in the MRI scan visit. Of 35 children at age 8–10 (see Table 1 for demographic information); one child was excluded from all analysis due to motion artifacts in the MRI data. Therefore, the total number of children included in the analysis was 34. Only one child per household could participate. Participants were included if they were fluent in English, lived with their biological mother at least 50% of the time, and their family income-to-needs ratio was below 7 (to target low- and middle-income families). Participants were excluded if they had (1) ferrous metal or other MRI contraindications in their body; (2) received a psychiatric disorder diagnosis and treatment and/or psychotropic medication (to avoid a confounding effect of psychiatric disorder treatment and medication; based on mother-report); (3) a history of a neurological disorder; or (4) an IQ score below 80 as assessed by the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (Wechsler, 1999).

Table 1.

Demographic and affective dot probe information for the sample (RT = reaction time; NT = neutral-threat; NH = neutral-happy, WASI = Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, SCARED = Screen for Child Anxiety Related Disorders, PPDS = Petersen Physical Development Scale).

N (%) Mean ± SD Range

Child age (years) 8.76 ± 0.65 8 – 10
Child sex (female) 18 (52.9)
Child race/ethnicity
 White/Caucasian 21 (61.8)
 Black/African American 4 (11.8)
 Hispanic 7 (20.6)
 Multi-racial 2 (5.9)
Income-to-needs ratio 2.1 ± 1.5 0 – 6.6
Maternal Education (years) 14.7 ± 2.9 8 – 20
Threat Bias Score (ms) −3.5 ± 68.4 −187.6 – 161.5
Happy Bias Score (ms) −12.43 ± 53.0 −113.7 – 113.0
Dot Probe Accuracy (%) 94.7 ± 0.04 84 – 100
Dot Probe RT (ms) 808.8 ± 209.8 511 – 1263
RT for Neutral NT trials (ms 810.4 ± 214.3 509 – 1296
RT for Threat NT trials (ms) 814.3 ± 215.5 521 – 1230
RT for Neutral NH trials (ms) 800.9 ± 208.8 516 – 1313
RT for Happy NH trials (ms) 813.4 ± 221.5 492 – 1292
IQ (WASI) 100.5 ± 13.1 80 – 126
Anxiety Symptoms (SCARE 32.6 ± 16.2 6 – 70
Pubertal Status (PPDS) 4 ± 1.9 1 – 8

2.2. Procedure

After determining eligibility via a phone call, the study was comprised of a home visit and an MRI scan visit. During the home visit, researchers collected demographic information including a detailed history of family income via extensive interview. Children completed the affective dot probe paradigm during the home visit on a laptop computer. Soon after the home visit, participants were asked to visit the University of Colorado, Boulder Intermountain Neuroimaging Consortium (INC) at the Center for Innovation and Creativity. During the MRI scan visit, participants first underwent a ‘mock’ scanning session in which they were trained to stay still in an MRI simulator. After completing the mock session, participants underwent an MRI scan to obtain structural images. Family income information was updated at the MRI visit if the interval between the home visit and MRI visit was more than 3 months. Therefore, the mean interval between the measurement of family income and the MRI visit was 0.97 months (SD = 2.00). Written informed consent was obtained from the mother of each participant and written informed and verbal assent was obtained from each child participant. The protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Denver.

2.3. Measures

2.3.1. Income-to-needs Ratio and Maternal Education.

We used income-to-needs ratio (INR) to assess family income. INR more accurately captures family economic status by considering how many people are in each family. During the home visit, a standardized interview was conducted to collect parent-reported family income for the last 12 months from the date of the home visit. INR was calculated by computing the ratio of family income to the federal poverty threshold, which is adjusted for the number of family members (Table 1). Consistent with the method the US Census Bureau uses to calculate family income the following were included: earnings, unemployment compensation, workers’ compensation, Social Security, alimony, and child support. However, family income does not included money from welfare programs or financial support from extended family members. Fifty-one percent of the children’s families were considered low income [defined as in poverty (INR ≤ 1) or near poverty (INR ≤ 2)]. We assessed maternal education via an interview in which each mother reported the amount of years of education in which they had completed (Table 1).

2.3.2. Affective Dot Probe Paradigm.

Children completed one session of the affective dot probe paradigm (Abend, Pine, & Bar-Haim, 2014) to measure attentional biases to threat; the task was compiled and presented using a laptop computer through E-Prime 2.0 software (Schneider & Zuccoloto, 2007). Participants completed the task using the mouse to make responses. In between each trial a fixation cross was displayed on the screen for 500 ms. For each trial, pairs of neutral, angry, and happy faces appeared on the screen vertically. Face stimuli were selected in pairs from the NimStim, Face Stimulus Set (Tottenham et al., 2009). Face stimuli were presented on the screen for 500 ms. Following the stimulus presentation, the face stimuli disappeared from the screen and a ‘probe’, in the form of a bracket pointing right or left (< or >), appeared on the screen in the previous location of either the neutral, happy, or angry faces until a response was made. After each response was made there was an inter-trial interval of 500 ms. Trials always contained at least one neutral face stimuli and therefore could be NN (both neutral), NT (one neutral, one threat), or NH (one neutral, one happy). Participants completed 120 trials in which 48 were NT, 48 were NH, and 24 were NN. For the task, there were 48 congruent trials (24 in which the probe replaced the location of the angry face and 24 in which the probe replaced the happy face), 48 incongruent trials (the probe replaced the location of the neutral face), and 24 were neutral trials (in which the probe replaced either of the neutral faces in a counterbalanced fashion). The location of the angry face, the probe location, the probe type, and the facial identity were counterbalanced across the task. Participants were instructed to respond as quickly and as accurately as possible, using their dominant hand, to the direction of the bracket using the mouse buttons (left mouse button for < and right mouse button for >).

Behavioral data analysis was conducted using the TAU/NIMH ABMT Analysis Tool v2.0 which is a standalone tool to analyze dot probe paradigm data files and provide both quality control and quantification of the attentional bias to threat scores (http://tau.ac.il/~yair1/ABMT.html). This toolbox was designed as an initiative to provide a standardized tool for the analysis of dot probe paradigm data and has been used in several studies to clean the data and quantify attention bias scores, including a study by the authors of the toolbox (Goldman, Shulman, Bar-Haim, Abend, & Burack, 2016), as well as several other studies (De Voogd et al., 2016; Fitzgerald, Rawdon, & Dooley, 2016; Fu, Taber-Thomas, & Pérez-Edgar, 2015; Thai, Taber-Thomas, & Pérez-Edgar, 2016). The tool removes trials with reaction times shorter than 150 ms, trials with reaction times longer than 2000 ms, and error trials. For each trial type, z-scores are calculated and trials with scores above 2.5 are removed from the analysis. After the removal of trials, the average percentage of trials that remained (M= 94.85, SD = 4.31, Range = 82.5 – 99.1) was similar to the suggested amount of 94% (Abend et al., 2014). The threat bias score was calculated by subtracting the mean reaction time on congruent trials (in which the probe appeared behind the angry face) from incongruent trials (in which the probe appeared behind the neutral face). A positive threat bias score reflects a bias towards threatening stimuli (the angry face) and captures increased orientation toward the threatening stimuli, disengaging attentional allocation to threat, or both (Salemink, van den Hout, & Kindt, 2007). The behavioral results were similar to other studies using this paradigm with a pediatric sample (Fitzgerald et al., 2016; Pine et al., 2005; Troller-Renfree, McDermott, Nelson, Zeanah, & Fox, 2015). Additionally, we calculated attentional bias to happy stimuli by subtracting the mean reaction time on congruent trials (in which the probe appeared behind the happy face) from the mean reaction time on incongruent trials (in which the probe appeared behind the neutral face). A positive attentional bias to happy stimuli score would reflect an increased attentional allocation towards happy stimuli while a negative score would reflect attentional allocation away from the happy face to the neutral face. See Table 1 for a description of the behavioral performance from the dot probe paradigm.

2.3.3. Measurements of IQ, Anxiety, and Pubertal Status.

To examine the specificity of the results, we also measured child IQ, anxiety, and pubertal status to test them as possible confounds. We used the WASI (Wechsler, 2014) to examine child’s IQ. For anxiety symptoms, we used the SCARED (Birmaher et al., 1997). Lastly, to measure pubertal status, we used the Petersen Physical Development Scale (Petersen, Crockett, Richards, & Boxer, 1988). Refer to Table 1 for more information about IQ, anxiety, and pubertal status.

2.4. MRI Acquisition

T1-weighted MPRAGE (magnetization prepared rapid gradient-echo) images were acquired using a 3T Siemens Magnetom Tim Trio scanner with a 32-channel phase-array coil. The MPRAGE sequence acquired high resolution images with the following parameters: 192 axial slices each at 1.00 mm thick, TR = 2530 ms, TE = 1.64 ms, skip = 0 mm, flip angle = 7°, FOV = 256 mm and voxel size = 1.0 × 1.0 × 1.0 mm.

2.5. Structural Image Processing

Visual inspection was conducted for each image to check for excessive motion artifacts, such as “ringing”, using a 4-point visual scale, for example 1 = no motion; 2 = mild motion; 3 = moderate motion; 4 = severe motion. According to this framework, scans scoring a ‘1’ have little or no detectable motion artifact (51.4% of the sample); a score of ‘2’ has mild but detectable ‘bands’ of motion (37.1% of the sample). A score of ‘3’ is considered moderate as far as detectable motion and can be included in the data analysis (8.6% of the sample) (Blumenthal, Zijdenbos, Molloy, & Giedd, 2002). One participant scored a ‘4’ on the rating scale in which motion artifact is extreme; this participant was excluded reducing the sample size from 35 to 34 participants. Several studies have used these rating scales for quantifying motion artifact in both adult and pediatric samples (Lin et al., 2012; Lyall et al., 2015; Schweren et al., 2016; L. Wang et al., 2015; Zhang et al., 2015). Furthermore, the motion scores were not associated with INR, maternal education, and attentional bias to threat scores.

Volumetric segmentation of the images was conducted using the Freesurfer Image Analysis Suite 5.3 (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/). This method has been demonstrated to be appropriate for pediatric images (Ghosh et al., 2010) and several studies have used the Freesurfer pipeline to segment hippocampal and amygdala volumes in a similar age range as the current sample (Geller et al., 2009; Ghassabian et al., 2014; Nardelli, Lebel, Rasmussen, Andrew, & Beaulieu, 2011; Pagliaccio et al., 2014; Teicher, Anderson, & Polcari, 2012). We carefully examined the segmentation accuracy for each structure for each participant using a similar 4- point rating skill (1 = very accurate; 2 = accurate; 3 = less accuracy; 4 = severe problems with accuracy). Based upon the ratings of the segmentation, accuracy no participants were excluded as no segmentation scored a ‘4’ indicating severe problems with the accuracy; images scoring a 3 (only 2 participants) were included in the study based upon the recommendations of the rating scale (Blumenthal et al., 2002). No images scoring a 4, indicating extremely poor segmentation, were included in the analysis.

The specifics of the procedure for the automatic segmentation of the subcortical structures have been previously documented (Fischl et al., 2002). The automated pipeline begins with motion and intensity non-uniformity correction in the data followed by affine transformation using the MNI305 atlas and intensity normalization. Images are skull stripped and an automatic subcortical segmentation computes volumetric data for subcortical structures including the amygdala and hippocampus. For the analysis of the amygdala, hippocampus, we used the calculation of gray matter volume from the ‘aseg’ output in Freesurfer. However, since Freesurfer does not provide a ‘ventrolateral prefrontal cortex’ volumetric measurement we combined the gray matter volumes calculated for the pars opercularis, pars orbitalis, and pars triangularis. This method has been used in other studies to provide a volumetric measurement of the vlPFC (Ding et al., 2015; Vijayakumar et al., 2014). For the post hoc analysis of the specificity of the results we also calculated gray matter volume for the precentral gyrus, basal ganglia, and lateral occipital gyrus. For the precentral gyrus and lateral occipital gyrus volume, we used the gray matter volume calculated by the automatic segmentation and output in ‘aseg’. Freesurfer automatic segmentation does not have an explicit segmentation for the basal ganglia; however, we calculated the basal ganglia volume by taking the sum of the volume of the caudate, putamen, and globus pallidus (Hutcheson et al., 2014; Qiu et al., 2009; Wyciszkiewicz & Pawlak, 2014).

2.6. Regional Gray Matter Analysis: Regression Models

After conducting quality control on the segmented images from Freesurfer, volumetric information from the subcortical structures were transferred into SPSS (Statistical Package for the Social Sciences, IBM) for analysis. Based up on our a priori hypothesis, the analysis focused on the volumes of the hippocampus, amygdala, and vlPFC. Hippocampal and amygdala volumes were examined for outliers using a protocol designed for outlier detection for Freesurfer volumetric analysis (http://enigma.loni.ucla.edu/protocols/), consistent with other studies (Guadalupe et al., 2014; Stein et al., 2012; Wang et al., 2017). Based on these criteria, no participants’ data for the hippocampus, amygdala, or vlPFC was detected as an outlier. It has been discussed that controlling for age and sex is critical when studying the relationship between family income and gray matter volume (Brito & Noble, 2014) and total gray matter volume is associated with both age and sex (Gur et al., 1999; Taki et al., 2011). Total gray matter volume was calculated during the processing pipeline (retrieved from the ‘aseg’ output) and reflects the sum of the gray matter volume in the left cortex, right cortex, subcortical regions, and cerebellum.

Therefore, to test the relationships between INR, amygdala/hippocampal/vlPFC volume, and threat bias we used separate multiple regressions for each hypothesis and controlled for age, sex, race/ethnicity, and total gray matter volume. Similarly, we used separate multiple regressions to test the association between maternal education, amygdala/hippocampal/vlPFC volume, and threat bias which controlled for age, sex, race/ethnicity, and total gray matter volume and a False Discovery Rate (FDR) correction of q < 0.05. For these regressions, covariates were all entered in the same block as we did not have a specific a priori reason to use a stepwise regression and this procedure may introduce bias (Harrell, 2001). However, we have included the regression results using a stepwise elimination processes (see the Supplementary Material). To further examine the specificity of the INR vs maternal education results, we conducted a multiple regression in which INR, maternal education, and threat bias were simultaneous predictors of the gray matter volumes. We also used a Williams’ test (Wilcox & Tian, 2008) to examine if there was a statistically significant difference between the correlation between INR and our hypothesized brain region’s volume and maternal education and the hypothesized region’s volume. Further, we used an additional Williams’ test to examine if there was a significant difference between the associations between INR and threat bias and maternal education and threat bias. These tests were implemented using R’s ‘psych’ package (Revelle, 2014).

Results

3.1. Demographic Variables

INR did not differ by child sex or child race/ethnicity (ps > 0.05). INR was correlated with age (r = 0.34 p < 0.05) for the sample; the older participants in the sample tended to have a higher INR. INR was not significantly correlated with total gray matter volume or intracranial volume. Maternal education also did not differ by child sex; and did not differ by child ethnicity (ps > 0.05). There was no significant correlation between maternal education and child age, total gray matter volume, or total intracranial volume (ps < 0.05) The time interval between the home visit and MRI visit was not correlated with INR nor threat bias scores. There was a significant correlation between maternal education and the interval between the home visit and lab visit (r = 0.41, p < 0.05). Age, sex, and ethnicity were not significantly related to the attentional bias to threat scores. There was a positive association between INR and maternal education (r = 0.42, p < 0.05). In the post hoc analyses, we examined the specificity of the results to INR by including child IQ, anxiety symptoms, and pubertal status in the models. There was a significant correlation between INR and child IQ (r = 0.050, p < 0.01) but not significant correlation between INR and anxiety symptoms (as measured by the SCARED) and pubertal status (p < 0.05). There was a significant correlation between maternal education and child IQ (r = 0.60, p < 0.0001) and significant correlation between maternal education and anxiety symptoms (r = −0.45, p < 0.01); the correlation between maternal education and pubertal status was not significant (p > 0.05).

3.2. SED and Volumes of a priori Brain Regions

A multiple regression with income-to-needs ratio (INR) as a regressor and participant age, sex, and total gray matter volume as covariates revealed a significant regression equation (F(4, 29) = 7.04, p < 0.001) with an R2 of 0.49. There was a significant relationship between INR and left hippocampal volume (β = 0.61, p < 0.0001, f2 = 0.96, Figure 1 and 2a). A similar regression model revealed a significant regression equation for the relationship between the INR and right hippocampal volume (F(4, 29) = 4.66, p < 0.01) with an R of 0.39. The regression indicated that higher INR was associated with higher right hippocampal volume (β = 0.39, p < 0.05, f2 = 0.63; Figure 2b). For the left amygdala (Figure 2c), the association with INR was not significant (p > 0.05). The regression equation for the right amygdala was significant, (F(4, 29) = 5.91, p < 0.01) with an R2 of 0.44. Therefore, higher INR was associated with higher right amygdala volume (β = 0.41 , p < 0.01, f2= 0.78). There was not a significant relationship between INR and left (p > 0.05) or right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (p > 0.05). Scatter plots of the relationship between raw INR and the raw gray matter volumes for the hippocampus and amygdala are available as Supplementary Figure 2.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Example of the automatic segmentation of the amygdala (blue color) and hippocampus (green color).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

(a) Partial regression plot of the relationship between residual income-to-needs ratio (INR) and residual left hippocampal volumes (b) partial regression plot of the relationship between residual INR and residual right hippocampal volumes (c) partial regression plot of the relationship between residual INR and residual right amygdala volumes. For all the figures, the values reflect residuals from the models with age, sex, and total gray matter volume as covariates. Scatter plots of the raw data can be found in the Supplementary Material (Supplementary Figure 1).

A multiple regression with maternal education as a regressor and participant age, sex, and total gray matter volume did not find a significant relationship between maternal education for left hippocampal volume (p > 0.05), right hippocampal volume (p > 0.05), left amygdala volume (p > 0.05), right amygdala volume (p > 0.05), left vlPFC (p > 0.05), or right vlPFC (p > 0.05).

3.2. Associations among SED, Brain Volumes, and Threat Bias Scores

As the associations between maternal education and brain volumes were not significant, we focused our analysis on INR. First, we examined the relationship between INR and threat bias scores on the dot probe paradigm. We used a multiple regression controlling for participants’ age, and sex and INR was significantly related to threat bias (β = −0.39, p < 0.05, f2 = 0.32) despite a regression equation that was not significant (F(3, 30) = 1.93,p > 0.05). A multiple regression controlling for the sex, age, and total gray matter volume of the participants was used to examine the association between hippocampal volumes and threat bias. The regression equation was significant F(4, 29) = 3.63, p < 0.05) and an R2 = 0.33, threat bias was associated with left hippocampal volumes (β = −0.42,p < 0.05, f2 = 0.49). Therefore, lower left hippocampal volume was associated with increased attentional bias to threatening faces on the affective dot probe paradigm (see Figure 3a for the partial regression plot controlling for age, sex, and total gray matter volume; see Supplementary Figure 2 for the scatter plot of the relationship between raw hippocampal volumes and raw threat bias scores). The nonsignificant F test suggests that there is a better fitting model for data. However, to keep consistent with the other regression models we included the same covariates. While mediation testing is not suggested for cross-sectional data (Maxwell & Cole, 2007), we tested the mediation in an exploratory analysis to be followed up with future longitudinal data (see Supplementary Material). While we cannot establish directionality, we provide a path diagram of the main results of the study, e.g. the associations between INR, left hippocampal volumes, and threat bias (Figure 3b).

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

(a) Partial regression plot of the relationship between residual INR and residual threat bias scores controlling for age and sex (b) Partial regression plot of the relationship between residual attentional bias to threat scores and residual left hippocampal volumes. The values reflect residuals from the model with age, sex, and total gray matter volume as covariates. A scatter plot of the raw data can be found in the Supplementary Material (Supplementary Figure 2). (c) A path diagram of the main findings of the study. The solid line indicates significant associations (p < 0.05) controlling for age, sex, and total gray matter volume. The dotted line indicates a relationship that did not survive correction for multiple comparisons (q < 0.05). * Indicates that this relationship did not survive multiple comparisons correction.

There was no significant relationship between threat bias scores and volume in the right hippocampus, bilateral amygdala, or bilateral vlPFC. We also examined the relationship among INR, brain volumes, and attentional bias to happy stimuli scores using the same covariates and found no significant association.

3.3. Testing the Specificity of SED Results

First, as several separate regressions were used, we corrected for multiple testing using the Benjamini-Hochberg procedure (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995) treating INR models and maternal education models as separate analyses. We used a False Discovery Rate (FDR) correction of q < 0.05. At this threshold, all the results included in section 3.2 and 3.3 remained significant except for the relationship between INR and threat bias (Benjamini-Hochberg p = 0.057).

Second, we conducted each multiple regression using INR, maternal education, and attentional bias to threat scores as simultaneous predictors. In a multiple regression including INR, maternal education, threat bias scores, age, sex, and total gray matter volume, INR was significantly associated with the left hippocampus (β = 0.56, p < 0.01,f2 = 1.22) and right hippocampus (β = 0.48, p < 0.05, f2 = 0.75). INR was not significantly associated with the left amygdala, (p > 0.05), and significant for the right amygdala (β = 0.40, p < 0.05, f2 = 0.88). Consistent with the main analysis findings, for the left and right vlPFC, INR was not significant (ps > 0.05). Maternal education and threat bias scores were not significantly associated with gray matter volume for each region of interest (ps > 0.05).

Third, we used a Williams’ test to examine if there was a significant difference between two correlations (INR and brain structure and maternal education and brain structure). The Williams’ test indicated that there was a significant difference among the correlations between INR and left hippocampal volume and between maternal education and left hippocampal volume (t = 2.2, p < 0 .05). The Williams’ test indicated that there was a difference at a trend level in the right hippocampus (t = 1.94, p = 0.06). The Williams’ tests for the bilateral amygdala were not significant (ps > 0.05). Therefore, particularly in the left hippocampus, a region that is also associated with threat bias, we had the strongest evidence that INR but not maternal education was associated with smaller volume. We also used a Williams’ test to examine if there was a significant difference amid the correlations between INR and threat bias and maternal education and threat bias. The Williams’ test indicated that the correlations were not significantly different (p > 0.05).

Also, as an exploratory analysis to test the specificity of the results, we examined the relationship between INR and volumes of the precentral gyrus, lateral occipital gyrus, and basal ganglia using multiple regressions controlling for age, sex, and total gray matter volume. We specifically tested regions that we did not expect to have an association with INR. Consistent with our hypothesis, INR was not associated with gray matter volumes for the precentral gyrus, lateral occipital gyrus, or basal ganglia volume (ps > 0.05). These exploratory analyses confirm the specificity of the results to the hippocampus and amygdala (by demonstrating it is not a global effect) and provide evidence for future studies of the specific relationship between INR and gray matter volume in reward processing regions.

Fourth, we ran a post hoc multiple regression with child’s IQ included as a covariate. This regression also included sex, age, and total gray matter volume as covariates. INR remained significant for the left hippocampus (β = 0.62, p < 0.001, f2 = 0.96), right hippocampus (β = 0.42, p < 0.05, f2 = 0.63). Consistent with the other models the left amygdala was not significant (p > 0.05), however the association remained for the right amygdala, (β = 0.45, p < 0.01, f2 = 0.85). Despite not surviving the multiple comparisons correction in the main analysis, the association between INR and threat bias remained significant with IQ included in the model along with sex and age (β = −0.41, p < 0 .05, f2 = 0.19). The association between left hippocampal volume and threat bias score remained significant when age, sex, and total gray matter volume and IQ were included as covariates (β = −0.42, p < 0.05, f2 = 0.51).

Fifth, we conducted an additional post hoc multiple regression including SCARED (Birmaher et al., 1997) scores (a measure of pediatric anxiety symptoms) as a covariate in addition to age, sex, and total gray matter volume. In this model, INR remained significant for the left hippocampus (β = 0.65, p < 0.001, f2 = 1.00), right hippocampus (β = 0.45, p < 0.05, f2 = 0.72), and right amygdala (β = 0.45 p < 0.05, f2 = 0.85). Consistent with other models, the left amygdala was not significant (p > 0.05). The relationship between INR and threat bias was not significant, but trending when including SCARED scores, age, and sex in the model (β = −0.39, p = 0.0501, f2 = 0.19). The association between left hippocampal volume and threat bias remained significant when SCARED scores, age, sex, and total gray matter volume were included as covariates (β = −0.41, p < 0.05, f2 = 0.49)

Lastly, we conducted an additional post hoc regression to include pubertal status as measured by the Petersen Physical Development Scale (Petersen et al., 1988) as a covariate. In the model including pubertal status, age, sex, and total gray matter volume, INR remained significant for the left hippocampus (β = 0.62, p < 0.001, f2 = 1.12), right hippocampus (β = 0.40, p < 0.01, f2 = 0.96), and right amygdala (β = 0.41, p < 0.01, f2 = 0.78). Similarly, the left amygdala was not significantly associated with INR (p < 0.05). The relationship between INR and threat bias remained significant when puberty status was included in the model (β = −0.39, p < 0.05, f2 = 0.66) despite not surviving the correction for multiple comparisons in the main analysis. The association between left hippocampal volume and threat bias scores remained significant when pubertal status, age, sex, and total gray matter volume were entered as covariates (β = −0.43, p < 0.05, f2 = 0.61). Therefore, the main results largely independent of the child’s IQ, current anxiety symptoms, and puberty status.

4. Discussion

In this study we address the following research questions: (1) Is SED related to gray matter volume in the amygdala, hippocampus, and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex? (2) Are SED and gray matter volumes in the hippocampus, amygdala, and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex related to higher attentional bias to threat? For our first research question, we found that lower income-to-needs ratio was associated with lower gray matter volume in the bilateral hippocampus and right amygdala, but not in the vlPFC. Additionally, we found that lower INR was associated with greater attentional bias to threat and lower left hippocampal volume was associated with greater attentional bias to threat. Overall, these findings extend the current knowledge on SED-related variations in brain structure and their relation to risks for negative emotional outcomes. We provide evidence of a relationship between the left hippocampus and attentional bias to threat, a risk marker for affective disorders such as anxiety. Future studies examining the specific functional role of the hippocampus in attentional bias to threat in the case of childhood SED and the relationship between structure and function in this context are needed.

Our hippocampal findings are consistent with several studies that have reported positive associations between family income and gray matter volume (Johnson et al., 2016). We extended the existing literature by finding the associations in middle childhood. Studies across the lifespan reported a relationship between family income and gray matter volume with these effects present in samples as young as infancy (Hair et al., 2015), to middle childhood to early adulthood (Hanson et al., 2015; Jednoróg et al., 2012), into middle (Butterworth, Cherbuin, Sachdev, & Anstey, 2011) and late adulthood (Staff et al., 2012). While the association was stronger in the left hippocampus for our study, we find support for a relationship between family income and bilateral hippocampal volume.

We extend the understanding of the association between family income and hippocampal development by examining a behavioral risk marker for psychopathology. Our study suggests a positive relationship between income-to-needs ratio and left hippocampal volume and a negative relationship between left hippocampal volume and attentional bias to threat. Additionally, we found that lower INR was associated with greater attentional bias to threat; however, this finding should be interpreted with caution as it did not survive the multiple comparisons threshold. The finding is consistent with other studies that have found childhood adversity in the form of physical abuse to be related to increased attentional bias to threatening faces (Pollak & Tolley-Schell, 2003). However, a recent study found that children from families experiencing low income were more likely to have an attentional bias away from, not toward, threat (Raver et al., 2017). In this study, negative images included various threatening information including animals, objects, and situations. Additionally, stimuli were presented for 250 ms whereas the paradigm of the current study includes only threatening faces which were presented for 500 ms. The differences in the study designs may contribute to the inconsistent findings as attentional bias toward or away from threats is highly sensitive to these design factors (Cisler, Bacon, & Williams, 2009; Koster, Crombez, Verschuere, & De Houwer, 2004; Matlow, Gard, & Berg, 2012). It has been demonstrated that both anxious and non-anxious children have significant attentional bias to threat; however, non-anxious children learn to inhibit this bias with age and anxious children do not (Kindt, Brosschot, & Everaerd, 1997; Kindt & Van Den Hout, 2001). These findings could explain the individual differences observed in our sample such that children experiencing low family income with large attentional biases to threat may have difficulties inhibiting attentional allocation to threat while some children experiencing low family income may have no attentional bias to threat or attentional bias away from threat. Thus, attentional bias to threat is often more clearly observed in children at risk for developing psychopathology. For example, behaviorally inhibited (BI) children showed increased attentional bias to threat and social withdrawal compared to non-BI children in adolescence (Pérez-Edgar, Bar-Haim, et al., 2010). It is noteworthy that the mean reaction times between threat and happy trials were similar. However, there was no significant association between the hippocampus, amygdala, or vlPFC for the happy trials (ps > 0.05). This supports the specificity of the results to attentional bias to threat as opposed to the degree of which the faces had affective expressions (happy or threat). Thus, the regression analyses may suggest the specific associations among INR, hippocampal volume, and attentional bias to threat.

While the amygdala is more traditionally thought to play a role in attentional bias to threat, our results suggest the morphometry of the hippocampus may also be involved in threat bias. Activity in the hippocampus has been associated with attentional bias to threat; specifically, it has been associated with deficits in disengagement from threat towards neutral stimuli (Price et al., 2014b). It has been hypothesized that the hippocampus, in a network of regions including the amygdala and prefrontal cortex, is involved in distinguishing new information regarding threat from previously learned information (Milad & Quirk, 2012). Further studies will be needed to examine the structure-function relationship of the hippocampus in attentional bias to threat. However, we add further evidence to the body of literature that also suggest a direct link between the structure of the hippocampus and attentional bias to threat (Cha et al., 2016; Fani et al., 2013; Grupe et al., 2016).

Like the hippocampal volumes, for the right amygdala in our sample, lower INR was associated with lower, not greater, gray matter volume. This finding replicates the positive relationship found between income-to-needs ratio and amygdala volume as similar results were observed at age 10 (Barch et al., 2016). The finding is also consistent with a study that found that children experiencing low SES (ages 9–15) had smaller left amygdala volumes than comparison children (Hanson et al., 2015). While INR was only associated with the right amygdala, any interpretations concerning the laterality of the finding will need larger sample sizes. However, in contrast to the hippocampus, we did not find a relationship between amygdala volume and threat bias. While abnormal functional activity patterns in the amygdala have strongly been associated with threat bias across several age groups and disorders (Bishop, 2008; Monk et al., 2008; Price et al., 2014a; Roy et al., 2008), little is known about the specific link between amygdala structure and threat bias. In a study examining the structure of the amygdala, consistent with our findings, attentional bias to threat using a dot probe paradigm was not associated with amygdala gray matter volume (Carlson, Reinke, & Habib, 2009). To address the questions of the relationship between amygdala structure and function in threat bias, future studies are needed to examine the relationship among family income, amygdala structure, and threat bias including both the neural measures of the morphometry and task-related functional activation.

Relationships between prefrontal cortex volume and family income have been previously reported (Hair et al., 2015; Hanson et al., 2013; Holz et al., 2015), however, we did not find a significant relationship between INR and vlPFC gray matter volume. This may be due to the relatively small parcellation of the prefrontal cortex we used as we had an a priori hypothesis of the vlPFC based upon previous attentional bias to threat fMRI studies. While the functional activity of the vlPFC has been demonstrated in several studies of attentional bias to threat (Bishop, 2008; Monk et al., 2006; Monk et al., 2008), we also did not find evidence of a relationship between the gray matter volume of the vlPFC and attentional bias to threat. The null findings may also be in part explained by the different developmental trajectories of subcortical and cortical development and a relationship between INR, the vlPFC gray matter, and threat bias may be only present when the prefrontal cortex reaches maturity in early adulthood (Giedd et al., 1999). Future studies will be needed to examine the specific regions of the prefrontal cortex associated with INR in middle childhood as well as the contribution of function versus structure of the vlPFC in attentional bias to threat. We also note that all our findings should be interpreted with caution, as we should not conflate structure and function of the brain.

Studies of SED typically chose to focus on one indicator of socioeconomic status such as family income or maternal education. However, this study chose to include both indicators. We acknowledge that socioeconomic status is a multi-dimensional construct in which INR and maternal education may be missing some of the complexity. For example, in some cases maternal education may play a moderating role between SED and child developmental outcomes (Augustine & Crosnoe, 2010; Hoff, 2003; Miller et al., 2011). Unfortunately, due to our small sample size we could not conduct the moderation analysis. However, due to the mixed evidence reported concerning the direct effects of maternal education and brain morphometry (Hanson et al., 2011; Luby et al., 2013a; Noble, Grieve, et al., 2012), we also examined its direct effect. We contribute to evidence suggesting a nonsignificant relationship between maternal education and gray matter volume in the hippocampus, amygdala, and prefrontal cortex (Hanson et al., 2011; Luby et al., 2013a). Further, we tested if the correlation between INR and brain structure and maternal education and brain structure was significantly different. For the left hippocampus, we provide evidence of a significant difference between the correlations. This finding supports the hypothesis that INR and maternal education capture unique aspects of a child’s environment and therefore may have different relationships with brain structure (Brito & Noble, 2014).

We further tested the specificity of our results in several different ways. First, we examined if the association between INR and hippocampal/amygdala volume was specific to these regions by also examining the relationship in brain regions that we did not hypothesize to be related to INR. We hypothesized INR would not be related to the volume of the precentral gyrus, basal ganglia, and lateral occipital gyrus. There was no significant association between INR and precentral gyrus or lateral occipital gyrus volume. This suggests the associations between INR and brain morphometry may be specific to certain regions (the hippocampus or amygdala) and may not be associated with brain structure globally. We did not find a significant correlation between INR and basal ganglia volume. Second, we provided post hoc regression analyses to indicate that the results were not driven by the children’s IQ, anxiety symptoms as measured by the SCARED, or pubertal status.

While smaller hippocampal and amygdala volumes and increased attentional bias to threat are considered as negative outcomes, we would like to discuss alternative explanations. First, from infancy to adolescence, hippocampal gray volume increases non-linearly with its volume peaking between 9–11 years old (Raz et al., 2005; Uematsu et al., 2012; Voineskos et al., 2015). A similar pattern is observed for the amygdala with its volume peaking within the same age range (Payne, Machado, Bliwise, & Bachevalier, 2010; Uematsu et al., 2012). In more extreme cases of early adversity, such as maternal deprivation, amygdala functional connectivity to the prefrontal cortex has been shown to reach more mature, adult-like patterns earlier compared to children who had not experienced maternal deprivation (Gee et al., 2013). Therefore, our results for the smaller hippocampal and amygdala volumes in children experiencing low family income may reflect accelerated structural maturation (such as increased synaptic pruning) associated with exposure to early adversity. While the smaller volume may reflect increased synaptic pruning, it may not be adaptive regarding attentional bias to threat as smaller volumes were associated with greater threat bias scores. Animal studies suggest that short-term neuronal remodeling in the context of chronic stress is an adaptive function to protect the brain structure from excitotoxicity (McEwen, Eiland, Hunter, & Miller, 2012). However, this response to stress (shrinkage of dendrites in the CA3 region, and loss of spines in CA1 neurons) may have long-term consequences and promote maladaptive behaviors in response to stress in the future (Davidson & McEwen, 2012). However, we highlight that evidence from animal models suggests that dendritic remodeling associated chronic stress is not fixed and has shown to be reversible (McEwen, 2016) as well as amenable to interventions (Davidson & McEwen, 2012). It is important to note, however, animal models cannot capture all aspects of the experience of SED (Hackman, Farah, & Meaney, 2010; Thapar & Rutter, 2015) and should always be interpreted with caution. However, currently they are useful to inform human studies about cause and effect relationships between SED and the brain (Perry et al., 2018).

Similarly, for children experiencing low family income, attention bias to threat to some degree may be adaptive to efficiently respond to possible dangerous situations in the environment (Sussman, Winkler, & Schroger, 2003). Therefore, it is critical to examine situations in which attentional bias to threat may be towards the extreme end, and therefore a potential socioemotional risk marker for psychopathology. As suggested by biological embedding, differences in the quality of an individual’s early environment may ‘sculpt’ the development of the central nervous system which then may impact cognitive, social, and behavioral development (Hertzman, 1999). For some children experiencing low income, the exposure to increased levels of family turmoil, violence in the home, and violent crime in the neighborhood may increase the allocation of attentional resources towards potential threats in the environment to respond quickly. According to the theory of experiential canalization, living in a low-resource environment involves adaptations that have short-term benefits that may eventually lead to long-term costs (Blair & Raver, 2012). Neurobiological and psychological mechanisms (such as lower hippocampal volume and attentional bias to threat) may be protective by promoting short-term adaptations, but can contribute to long-term dysregulation of stress mechanisms which can have deleterious effects on resiliency to stress and outcomes such as mental health (McEwen & Gianaros, 2010).

The findings should be considered bearing in mind the following limitations. First, the sample size of the current study was modest and will need replication in a larger sample size. We replicated findings from studies analyzing the relationship between INR and the hippocampus and amygdala; however, we will need to replicate the associations with attentional bias to threat in a large sample of children. Second, INR may influence the brain and emotional development through lack of cognitive stimulation in the home environment (Barry, Vervliet, & Hermans, 2015; Conger, Conger, & Martin, 2010; Lipina et al., 2013), toxins from the environment (Gray, Edwards, & Miranda, 2013; Liu & Lewis, 2014), or nutritional deprivation (Kant & Graubard, 2012). Thus, it is important for future studies to examine the role of these risk factors to further understand environmental and biological pathways of the neural embedding of INR. Additionally, the study did not focus on paternal psychopathology, this aspect should be explored in the future as a potential confound or moderator of the relationship between SED and brain morphometry (Brown & Moran, 1997; Hackman et al., 2010; Spence, Najman, Bor, O’callaghan, & Williams, 2002). Further, SED can co-vary with maltreatment, abuse, and neglect as well as parental substance abuse (Conger, Conger, & Martin, 2010; Herrenkohl & Herrenkohl, 2007; Trickett, Aber, Carlson, & Cicchetti, 1991). While the current study was limited in its ability to address the potential relations between the findings and these factors, future studies should investigate the unique contributions of the different risk factors for brain structural development. Third, we assessed the relationship between INR and brain structure based upon the INR that was calculated for the last twelve months of the participant’s life. While this is one of the most objective and commonly used methods to assess socioeconomic status, this measurement does not assess the chronicity of the exposure. Thus, it is unclear whether the variations in brain structure in the current study are associated with family income during the past year or family income since birth. To further examine the timing versus the chronicity of exposure, future studies are needed to assess the amount of time spent in low-income environments and its relationship to neural morphometry and attentional bias to threat.

Fourth, due to the cross-sectional nature of our data we could not test whether left hippocampal volumes mediated the relationship between family income and attentional bias to threat. While substantial biases in the parameters have been shown for cross-sectional mediation (Maxwell & Cole, 2007; Maxwell, Cole, & Mitchell, 2011), we provide evidence of a potential mediational model that will need to be followed up with a longitudinal study in which mediation can be accurately examined. Fifth, the stability of the dot probe paradigm has recently come into debate as some studies have demonstrated poor internal reliability and test-retest reliability (Kappenman, Farrens, Luck, & Proudfit, 2014; R. B. Price et al., 2015; Schmukle, 2005). As we only measured the dot probe at one-time period, we could not assess the reliability of our estimates. There also are alternative methods for assessing attentional bias to threat scores using the dot probe (Price et al., 2015). These approaches will help to determine if threat bias reflects an enhance orientation to non-neutral stimuli or a difficulty in disengagement from non-neutral stimuli. Further, replication of these findings could also include eye-tracking to rule out possible location effects during the dot probe (a correspondence between the spatial location of the probe and the location of the participant’s attention). Thus, it will be important that future studies will include multiple sessions of the dot probe to examine the reliability of the threat bias measure. Lastly, our analysis was limited by the atlas used by Freesurfer for the segmentation. We chose structures that we hypothesized would be associated with both SED and attentional bias to threat. Regarding attentional bias to threat, we could not capture all the structures that have been implicated in this task. For example, the dot probe has been shown to involve activity in multiple subcortical regions in the ascending reticular activity system (Pine, 2003; Price et al., 2013; Sara & Bouret, 2012). However, we could not examine these regions as they were not adequately defined in the atlas used for segmentation. Future studies using manual tracing or automatic segmentation with an atlas that includes these regions will be critical for assessing the potential relationship between these structures, INR, and threat bias. Additionally, while there have been studies of the relationship between family income and cortical thickness (Lawson, Duda, Avants, Wu, & Farah, 2013; Noble et al., 2015), we focused on gray matter volume as we examined subcortical morphometry and thickness measurements are not provided for these regions. We acknowledge the limitations of the correlational relationship between INR, gray matter volume, and threat bias. Future studies using an intervention or longitudinal approach will help to understand a mechanism of associations among socioeconomic status, brain structure, and socioemotional behavior in children. Future directions include examination of cortical thickness (focusing on the prefrontal cortex), functional activity (e.g. affective dot-probe fMRI), and structural connectivity (e.g. white matter organization) in and between these regions and its relationship to family income and attentional bias to threat. It will also be critical to examine the functional connectivity between the amygdala and hippocampus as well as their connectivity with other brain regions as altered connectivity patterns have been observed across several types of early adversity (Cisler et al., 2013; Gee et al., 2013; Sripada, Swain, Evans, Welsh, & Liberzon, 2014).

5. Conclusions

In the current study, we found a positive relationship between income-to-needs ratio and gray matter volume during middle childhood specifically in the bilateral hippocampus and right amygdala. Additionally, we observed an association between lower income-to-needs ratio and attentional biased responses to threatening faces on an affective dot probe; although this finding did not survive multiple comparison correction. Lastly, lower left hippocampal volumes were associated with greater attentional bias to threat. The main contribution of these findings is extending the current knowledge about the associations between family income-related variations in brain structure and neural and behavioral risk for socioemotional outcomes. Previous studies focus more on the associations between childhood SED, brain structure, and cognitive outcomes such as executive function and academic performance. However, SED has been associated with elevated risks for negative socioemotional outcomes. Thus, the current study addresses the gap in the literature by demonstrating an environment - brain morphometry - behavior relationship that may play a role in the relationship between childhood SED and socioemotional difficulties.

Our findings may provide evidence of possible early socioemotional and neural risk markers for socio-emotional difficulties for children living in families experiencing low income. Middle childhood is a developmental period in which symptoms of psychopathology arise, specifically anxiety and mood disorders (Feng, Shaw, & Silk, 2008; Shanahan, Calkins, Keane, Kelleher, & Suffness, 2014). Thus, it is critical to detect the relationship among environmental risks, brain morphology, and behavioral markers that have been associated with pediatric anxiety (Puliafico & Kendall, 2006) and internalizing symptoms (Hardee et al., 2013). While the data provide preliminary evidence for an association between INR, left hippocampal volume, and attentional bias to threat, future studies will be needed to examine this relationship in greater depth.

We acknowledge the sample of children in this study did not have a current or past diagnosis of psychopathology. This was to reduce a confounding effect of psychopathology on the associations between SED and outcomes. This is likely to explain why the main findings of the study was independent of current anxiety symptoms. Attentional bias to threat has been well-researched in terms of predictive ability of anxious behavior later in development (Pérez-Edgar, Bar-Haim, et al., 2010; Pérez-Edgar, McDermott, et al., 2010; Pérez-Edgar et al., 2011). Thus, caution is required for interpreting the results of the current study. The variation in the gray matter volume and attentional bias to threat suggest only potential risks for future psychopathology or subclinical symptoms. Our findings may also help to support initiatives for preventative efforts to start earlier, such as middle childhood when the amygdala and hippocampal structures may have increased vulnerability. Our results may also elucidate a mediational pathway in which the structure of the left hippocampus mediates the relationship between INR and attentional bias to threat, which will need to be established using longitudinal data.

Supplementary Material

13415_2018_670_MOESM1_ESM
Sup_Fig1
Sup_Fig2

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development [R21HD078797; R01 HD090068]; the Professional Research Opportunity for Faculty (PROF) and Faculty Research Fund (FRF), University of Denver; and the Victoria S. Levin Award For Early Career Success in Young Children’s Mental Health Research, Society for Research in Child Development (SRCD). Special thanks to Daniel Mason, Christian Capistrano, Laura Jeske, Daniel Bartholomew, Naomi Wallace, Nanxi Xu, and Christina Congleton for their help in recruitment, data collection, and data entry. We thank Christopher Madan for his guide on visualization of the MRI data (https://f1000research.com/articles/4–466/v1). We also thank Dr. Sarah Watamura for her insightful comments on the manuscript. We thank Dr. Cathy Durso, Dr. Kimberly Chiew, and Dr. Peter Sokol-Hessner for their comments on the statistical analyses. We thank Rebekah Tribble for her editorial comments. The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest in the research.

References

  1. Abend R, Pine D, & Bar-Haim Y. (2014). The TAU-NIMH Attention Bias Measurement Toolbox. [Google Scholar]
  2. Alvarez RP, Biggs A, Chen G, Pine DS, & Grillon C. (2008). Contextual fear conditioning in humans: cortical-hippocampal and amygdala contributions. Journal of Neuroscience, 28(24), 6211–6219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Augustine JM, & Crosnoe R. (2010). Mothers’ depression and educational attainment and their children’s academic trajectories. Journal of health and social behavior, 51(3), 274–290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bar-Haim Y, Lamy D, Pergamin L, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, & Van Ijzendoorn MH (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychological bulletin, 133(1), 1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barch D, Pagliaccio D, Belden A, Harms MP, Gaffrey M, Sylvester CM, … Luby J. (2016). Effect of Hippocampal and Amygdala Connectivity on the Relationship Between Preschool Poverty and School-Age Depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 173(6), 625–634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Barry TJ, Vervliet B, & Hermans D. (2015). An integrative review of attention biases and their contribution to treatment for anxiety disorders. Front Psychol, 6, 968. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00968 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Beck AT (2008). The evolution of the cognitive model of depression and its neurobiological correlates. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(8), 969–977. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Benjamini Y, & Hochberg Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the royal statistical society. Series B (Methodological), 289–300. [Google Scholar]
  9. Birmaher B, Khetarpal S, Brent D, Cully M, Balach L, Kaufman J, & Neer SM (1997). The screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders (SCARED): Scale construction and psychometric characteristics. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(4), 545–553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bishop. (2008). Neural mechanisms underlying selective attention to threat. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1129(1), 141–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bishop Duncan, J., Brett M, & Lawrence AD (2004). Prefrontal cortical function and anxiety: controlling attention to threat-related stimuli. Nature neuroscience, 7(2), 184–188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Blair. (2006). How similar are fluid cognition and general intelligence? A developmental neuroscience perspective on fluid cognition as an aspect of human cognitive ability. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(2), 109–125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Blair, & Raver. (2012). Child development in the context of adversity: experiential canalization of brain and behavior. American Psychologist, 67(4), 309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Blakemore Burnett, & Dahl. (2010). The role of puberty in the developing adolescent brain. Human brain mapping, 31(6), 926–933. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bloss EB, Janssen WG, McEwen BS, & Morrison JH (2010). Interactive effects of stress and aging on structural plasticity in the prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(19), 6726–6731. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Brito NH, & Noble KG (2014). Socioeconomic status and structural brain development. Frontiers in neuroscience, 8, 276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Brooks-Gunn J, & Duncan GJ (1997). The effects of poverty on children. The future of children, 55–71. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Brown GW, & Moran PM (1997). Single mothers, poverty and depression. Psychological medicine, 27(1), 21–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Butterworth P, Cherbuin N, Sachdev P, & Anstey KJ (2011). The association between financial hardship and amygdala and hippocampal volumes: results from the PATH through life project. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 7(5), 548–556. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Carlson JM, Reinke KS, & Habib R. (2009). A left amygdala mediated network for rapid orienting to masked fearful faces. Neuropsychologia, 47(5), 1386–1389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cha J, Greenberg T, Song I, Blair Simpson H, Posner J, & Mujica-Parodi LR (2016). Abnormal hippocampal structure and function in clinical anxiety and comorbid depression. Hippocampus, 26(5), 545–553. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Cisler JM, Bacon AK, & Williams NL (2009). Phenomenological characteristics of attentional biases towards threat: A critical review. Cognitive therapy and research, 33(2), 221–234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Cisler JM, & Koster EH (2010). Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: An integrative review. Clinical psychology review, 30(2), 203–216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Conger RD, Conger KJ, & Martin MJ (2010). Socioeconomic status, family processes, and individual development. Journal of Marriage and Family, 72(3), 685–704. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Dalgleish T, & Watts FN (1990). Biases of attention and memory in disorders of anxiety and depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 10(5), 589–604. [Google Scholar]
  26. Dannlowski U, Stuhrmann A, Beutelmann V, Zwanzger P, Lenzen T, Grotegerd D, … Bauer J 2012. Limbic scars: long-term consequences of childhood maltreatment revealed by functional and structural magnetic resonance imaging. Biological psychiatry, 71(4), 286–293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Davidson RJ, & McEwen BS (2012). Social influences on neuroplasticity: stress and interventions to promote well-being. Nature neuroscience, 15(5), 689. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. De Bellis MD, Casey B, Dahl RE, Birmaher B, Williamson DE, Thomas KM, … Hall J (2000). A pilot study of amygdala volumes in pediatric generalized anxiety disorder. Biological psychiatry, 48(1), 51–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. De Voogd E, Wiers R, Prins P, de Jong P, Boendermaker W, Zwitser R, & Salemink E (2016). Online attentional bias modification training targeting anxiety and depression in unselected adolescents: Short-and long-term effects of a randomized controlled trial. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 87, 11–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Dia DA, & Bradshaw W (2008). Cognitive risk factors to the development of anxiety and depressive disorders in adolescents. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal, 25(6), 469–481. [Google Scholar]
  31. Ding Y, Lawrence N, Olie E, Cyprien F, Le Bars E, Bonafe A, … Jollant F (2015). Prefrontal cortex markers of suicidal vulnerability in mood disorders: a model-based structural neuroimaging study with a translational perspective. Translational psychiatry, 5(2), e516. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Evans GW (2004). The environment of childhood poverty. American Psychologist, 59(2), 77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Evans GW, & English K (2002). The environment of poverty: Multiple stressor exposure, psychophysiological stress, and socioemotional adjustment. Child development, 73(4), 1238–1248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Evans GW, & Kim P (2013). Childhood poverty, chronic stress, self-regulation, and coping. Child Development Perspectives, 7(1), 43–48. [Google Scholar]
  35. Fani N, Gutman D, Tone EB, Almli L, Mercer KB, Davis J, … Dinov ID (2013). FKBP5 and attention bias for threat: associations with hippocampal function and shape. JAMA psychiatry, 70(4), 392–400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Feng X, Shaw DS, & Silk JS (2008). Developmental trajectories of anxiety symptoms among boys across early and middle childhood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117(1), 32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, Albert M, Dieterich M, Haselgrove C, … Klaveness S (2002). Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron, 33(3), 341–355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Fitzgerald A, Rawdon C, & Dooley B (2016). A randomized controlled trial of attention bias modification training for socially anxious adolescents. Behaviour research and therapy, 84, 1–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Fu X, Taber-Thomas BC, & Pérez-Edgar K (2015). Frontolimbic functioning during threat-related attention: relations to early behavioral inhibition and anxiety in children. Biological psychology. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Gee DG, Gabard-Durnam LJ, Flannery J, Goff B, Humphreys KL, Telzer EH, … Tottenham N (2013). Early developmental emergence of human amygdala-prefrontal connectivity after maternal deprivation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(39), 15638–15643. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Jeffries NO, Castellanos FX, Liu H, Zijdenbos A, … Rapoport JL (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nature neuroscience, 2(10), 861–863. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Goldman KJ, Shulman C, Bar-Haim Y, Abend R, & Burack JA (2016). Attention allocation to facial expressions of emotion among persons with Williams and Down syndromes. Development and Psychopathology, 1–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Gray SC, Edwards SE, & Miranda ML (2013). Race, socioeconomic status, and air pollution exposure in North Carolina. Environmental research, 126, 152–158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Grupe DW, Wielgosz J, Davidson RJ, & Nitschke JB (2016). Neurobiological correlates of distinct post-traumatic stress disorder symptom profiles during threat anticipation in combat veterans. Psychological medicine, 46(9), 1885–1895. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Guadalupe T, Zwiers MP, Teumer A, Wittfeld K, Vasquez AA, Hoogman M, … Hegenscheid K (2014). Measurement and genetics of human subcortical and hippocampal asymmetries in large datasets. Human brain mapping, 35(7), 3277–3289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Gur RC, Turetsky BI, Matsui M, Yan M, Bilker W, Hughett P, & Gur RE (1999). Sex differences in brain gray and white matter in healthy young adults: correlations with cognitive performance. The Journal of Neuroscience, 19(10), 4065–4072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Hackman DA, Farah MJ, & Meaney MJ (2010). Socioeconomic status and the brain: mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nature reviews neuroscience, 11(9), 651. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hair NL, Hanson JL, Wolfe BL, & Pollak SD (2015). Association of child poverty, brain development, and academic achievement. JAMA pediatrics, 169(9), 822–829. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hall BS, Moda RN, & Liston C (2015). Glucocorticoid mechanisms of functional connectivity changes in stress-related neuropsychiatric disorders. Neurobiology of stress, 1, 174–183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hankin BL, Snyder HR, & Gulley. (2016). Cognitive risks in developmental psychopathology. Developmental Psychopathology, Maladaptation and Psychopathology, 3, 312. [Google Scholar]
  51. Hanson Chandra, Wolfe, & Pollak. (2011). Association between income and the hippocampus. PloS one, 6(5), e18712. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Hanson Chung, M. K., Avants BB, Shirtcliff EA, Gee JC, Davidson RJ, & Pollak SD (2010). Early stress is associated with alterations in the orbitofrontal cortex: a tensor-based morphometry investigation of brain structure and behavioral risk. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(22), 7466–7472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Hanson Hair, Shen DG, Shi F, Gilmore JH, Wolfe BL, & Pollak SD (2013). Family poverty affects the rate of human infant brain growth. PloS one, 8(12), e80954. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Hanson Nacewicz, Sutterer Cayo, A. A., Schaefer SM, Rudolph KD, … Davidson RJ (2015). Behavioral problems after early life stress: contributions of the hippocampus and amygdala. Biological psychiatry, 77(4), 314–323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Hardee JE, Benson BE, Bar-Haim Y, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Chen G, … Pine DS (2013). Patterns of neural connectivity during an attention bias task moderate associations between early childhood temperament and internalizing symptoms in young adulthood. Biological psychiatry, 74(4), 273–279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Harrell FE (2001). Regression modeling strategies, with applications to linear models, survival analysis and logistic regression. GET ADDRESS: Springer. [Google Scholar]
  57. Hertzman C (1999). The biological embedding of early experience and its effects on health in adulthood. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 896(1), 85–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Hoff-Ginsberg E, & Tardif T (1995). Socioeconomic status and parenting. [Google Scholar]
  59. Hoff E (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child development, 74(5), 1368–1378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Holland PC, & Gallagher M (1999). Amygdala circuitry in attentional and representational processes. Trends in cognitive sciences, 3(2), 65–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Holz NE, Boecker R, Hohm E, Zohsel K, Buchmann AF, Blomeyer D, … Wolf I (2015). The long-term impact of early life poverty on orbitofrontal cortex volume in adulthood: results from a prospective study over 25 years. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40(4), 996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Hutcheson NL, Clark DG, Bolding MS, White DM, & Lahti AC (2014). Basal ganglia volume in unmedicated patients with schizophrenia is associated with treatment response to antipsychotic medication. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 221(1), 6–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Jednorog Altarelli, Monzalvo Fluss, Dubois J, Billard C, … Ramus F (2012). The influence of socioeconomic status on children’s brain structure. PloS one, 7(8), e42486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Johnson Riis, & Noble. (2016). State of the art review: poverty and the developing brain. Pediatrics, 137(4), peds. 2015–3075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Kant AK, & Graubard BI (2012). Race-ethnic, family income, and education differentials in nutritional and lipid biomarkers in US children and adolescents: NHANES 2003–2006. The American journal of clinical nutrition, ajcn. 035535. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Kappenman ES, Farrens JL, Luck SJ, & Proudfit GH (2014). Behavioral and ERP measures of attentional bias to threat in the dot-probe task: poor reliability and lack of correlation with anxiety. Front Psychol, 5, 1368. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01368 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Kindt M, Brosschot JF, & Everaerd W (1997). Cognitive processing bias of children in a real life stress situation and a neutral situation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 64(1), 79–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Kindt M, & Van Den Hout M (2001). Selective attention and anxiety: A perspective on developmental issues and the causal status. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 23(3), 193–202. [Google Scholar]
  69. Koster EH, Crombez G, Verschuere B, & De Houwer J (2004). Selective attention to threat in the dot probe paradigm: Differentiating vigilance and difficulty to disengage. Behaviour research and therapy, 42(10), 1183–1192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Lawson GM, Duda JT, Avants BB, Wu J, & Farah MJ (2013). Associations between children’s socioeconomic status and prefrontal cortical thickness. Developmental science, 16(5), 641–652. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Lipina S, Segretin S, Hermida J, Prats L, Fracchia C, Camelo JL, & Colombo J (2013). Linking childhood poverty and cognition: environmental mediators of non-verbal executive control in an Argentine sample. Developmental science, 16(5), 697–707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Liston C, & Gan W-B (2011). Glucocorticoids are critical regulators of dendritic spine development and plasticity in vivo. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(38), 16074–16079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Liu J, & Lewis G (2014). Environmental toxicity and poor cognitive outcomes in children and adults. Journal of environmental health, 76(6), 130. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Luby Belden, Botteron Marrus, Harms MP, Babb C, … Barch D (2013a). The effects of poverty on childhood brain development: the mediating effect of caregiving and stressful life events. JAMA pediatrics, 167(12), 1135–1142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Luby Belden, Botteron Marrus, N., Harms MP, Babb C, … Barch D (2013b). The effects of poverty on childhood brain development: the mediating effect of caregiving and stressful life events. JAMA pediatrics, 167(12), 1135–1142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. MacDonald AW, Cohen JD, Stenger VA, & Carter CS (2000). Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science, 288(5472), 1835–1838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Maren S, Phan KL, & Liberzon I (2013). The contextual brain: implications for fear conditioning, extinction and psychopathology. Nature reviews neuroscience, 14(6), 417. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Mathews A, & MacLeod C (2005). Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol, 1, 167–195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Matlow R, Gard D, & Berg D (2012). Difficulty disengaging from threat in anxiety: preliminary evidence for delayed response execution. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 3(3), 455–469. [Google Scholar]
  80. Maxwell SE, & Cole DA (2007). Bias in cross-sectional analyses of longitudinal mediation. Psychological methods, 12(1), 23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Maxwell SE, Cole DA, & Mitchell MA (2011). Bias in cross-sectional analyses of longitudinal mediation: Partial and complete mediation under an autoregressive model. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 46(5), 816–841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. McEwen BS (1998). Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. New England journal of medicine, 338(3), 171–179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. McEwen BS (2003). Mood disorders and allostatic load. Biological psychiatry, 54(3), 200–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. McEwen BS (2007). Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiological reviews, 87(3), 873–904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. McEwen BS (2016). Stress-induced remodeling of hippocampal CA3 pyramidal neurons. Brain research, 1645, 50–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. McEwen BS, Eiland L, Hunter RG, & Miller MM (2012). Stress and anxiety: structural plasticity and epigenetic regulation as a consequence of stress. Neuropharmacology, 62(1), 3–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. McEwen BS, & Gianaros PJ (2010). Central role of the brain in stress and adaptation: links to socioeconomic status, health, and disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1186(1), 190–222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Milad MR, & Quirk GJ (2012). Fear extinction as a model for translational neuroscience: ten years of progress. Annual review of psychology, 63, 129–151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Milham MP, Nugent AC, Drevets WC, Dickstein DS, Leibenluft E, Ernst M, … Pine DS (2005). Selective reduction in amygdala volume in pediatric anxiety disorders: a voxel-based morphometry investigation. Biological psychiatry, 57(9), 961–966. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Miller GE, Lachman ME, Chen E, Gruenewald TL, Karlamangla AS, & Seeman TE (2011). Pathways to resilience: Maternal nurturance as a buffer against the effects of childhood poverty on metabolic syndrome at midlife. Psychological science, 22(12), 1591–1599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Mogg Bradley, & Williams. (1995). Attentional bias in anxiety and depression: The role of awareness. British journal of clinical psychology, 34(1), 17–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Monk Nelson, McClure Mogg, K., Bradley BP, Leibenluft E, … Ernst M (2006). Ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activation and attentional bias in response to angry faces in adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(6), 1091–1097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Monk Telzer, Mogg Bradley, B. P., Mai X, Louro HM, … Pine DS (2008). Amygdala and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activation to masked angry faces in children and adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder. Archives of general psychiatry, 65(5), 568–576. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Najman JM, Hayatbakhsh MR, Clavarino A, Bor W, O’Callaghan MJ, & Williams GM (2010). Family poverty over the early life course and recurrent adolescent and young adult anxiety and depression: a longitudinal study. American Journal of Public Health, 100(9), 1719–1723. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Noble Grieve S. M., Korgaonkar MS, Engelhardt LE, Griffith EY, Williams LM, & Brickman AM (2012). Hippocampal volume varies with educational attainment across the life-span. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 6, 307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Noble Houston, Brito Bartsch, Kan Kuperman, … Libiger. (2015). Family income, parental education and brain structure in children and adolescents. Nature neuroscience, 18(5), 773. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Noble Houston, Kan, & Sowell. (2012). Neural correlates of socioeconomic status in the developing human brain. Developmental science, 15(4), 516–527. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Noble Wolmetz, M. E., Ochs LG, Farah MJ, & McCandliss BD (2006). Brain-behavior relationships in reading acquisition are modulated by socioeconomic factors. Developmental science, 9(6), 642–654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Oitzl MS, Champagne DL, van der Veen R, & de Kloet ER (2010). Brain development under stress: hypotheses of glucocorticoid actions revisited. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(6), 853–866. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Pachter LM, Auinger P, Palmer R, & Weitzman M (2006). Do parenting and the home environment, maternal depression, neighborhood, and chronic poverty affect child behavioral problems differently in different racial-ethnic groups? Pediatrics, 117(4), 1329–1338. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Payne C, Machado CJ, Bliwise NG, & Bachevalier J (2010). Maturation of the hippocampal formation and amygdala in Macaca mulatta: a volumetric magnetic resonance imaging study. Hippocampus, 20(8), 922–935. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Peper Pol, Crone, & Van Honk, J. (2011). Sex steroids and brain structure in pubertal boys and girls: a mini-review of neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience, 191, 28–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Pérez-Edgar K, Bar-Haim Y, McDermott JM, Chronis-Tuscano A, Pine DS, & Fox NA (2010). Attention biases to threat and behavioral inhibition in early childhood shape adolescent social withdrawal. Emotion, 10(3), 349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Pérez-Edgar K, McDermott JNM, Korelitz K, Degnan KA, Curby TW, Pine DS, & Fox NA (2010). Patterns of sustained attention in infancy shape the developmental trajectory of social behavior from toddlerhood through adolescence. Developmental psychology, 46(6), 1723. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Pérez-Edgar K, Reeb-Sutherland BC, McDermott JM, White LK, Henderson HA, Degnan KA, … Fox NA (2011). Attention biases to threat link behavioral inhibition to social withdrawal over time in very young children. Journal of abnormal child psychology, 39(6), 885–895. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Perry RE, Finegood ED, Braren SH, Dejoseph ML, Putrino DF, Wilson DA, … Investigators, F. L. P. K. (2018). Developing a neurobehavioral animal model of poverty: Drawing cross-species connections between environments of scarcity-adversity, parenting quality, and infant outcome. Development and psychopathology, 1–20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Pessoa L (2008). On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nature reviews neuroscience, 9(2), 148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Pessoa L (2013). The cognitive-emotional brain: From interactions to integration: MIT press. [Google Scholar]
  109. Petersen AC, Crockett L, Richards M, & Boxer A (1988). A self-report measure of pubertal status: Reliability, validity, and initial norms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 17(2), 117–133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Pine. (2003). Developmental psychobiology and response to threats: relevance to trauma in children and adolescents. Biological Psychiatry, 53(9), 796–808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Pine. (2007). Research review: a neuroscience framework for pediatric anxiety disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(7), 631–648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Pine Mogg, K., Bradley BP, Montgomery L, Monk CS, McClure E, … Kaufman J (2005). Attention bias to threat in maltreated children: Implications for vulnerability to stress-related psychopathology. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(2), 291–296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Pollak, & Tolley-Schell. (2003). Selective attention to facial emotion in physically abused children. Journal of abnormal psychology, 112(3), 323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Price Kuckertz, Siegle Ladouceur, C. D., Silk JS, Ryan ND, … Amir N (2015). Empirical recommendations for improving the stability of the dot-probe task in clinical research. Psychological Assessment, 27(2), 365. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Price Siegle, Silk Ladouceur C. D., McFarland A, Dahl RE, & Ryan ND (2014a). Looking under the hood of the dot-probe task: An fMRI study in anxious youth. Depression and anxiety, 31(3), 178–187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Price Siegle, G. J., Silk JS, Ladouceur C, McFarland A, Dahl RE, & Ryan ND (2013). Sustained neural alterations in anxious youth performing an attentional bias task: A pupilometry study. Depression and anxiety, 30(1), 22–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Price Siegle G. J., Silk JS, Ladouceur CD, McFarland A, Dahl RE, & Ryan ND (2014b). Looking under the hood of the dot-probe task: An fMRI study in anxious youth. Depression and anxiety, 31(3), 178–187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Price RB, Kuckertz JM, Siegle GJ, Ladouceur CD, Silk JS, Ryan ND, … Amir N (2015). Empirical recommendations for improving the stability of the dot-probe task in clinical research. Psychol Assess, 27(2), 365–376. doi: 10.1037/pas0000036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Puliafico AC, & Kendall PC (2006). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious youth: A review. Clinical child and family psychology review, 3(3–4), 162–180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Qiu A, Crocetti D, Adler M, Mahone EM, Denckla MB, Miller MI, & Mostofsky SH (2009). Basal ganglia volume and shape in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166(1), 74–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Raver CC, Blair C, & Garrett-Peters P (2015). Poverty, household chaos, and interparental aggression predict children’s ability to recognize and modulate negative emotions. Development and psychopathology, 27(03), 695–708. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Raver CC, Roy AL, Pressler E, Ursache AM, & Charles McCoy D (2016). Poverty-Related Adversity and Emotion Regulation Predict Internalizing Behavior Problems among Low-Income Children Ages 8–11. Behavioral Sciences, 7(1), 2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Raz N, Lindenberger U, Rodrigue KM, Kennedy KM, Head D, Williamson A, … Acker JD (2005). Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: general trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cerebral cortex, 15(11), 1676–1689. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Reiss AL, Abrams MT, Singer HS, Ross JL, & Denckla MB (1996). Brain development, gender and IQ in children: a volumetric imaging study. Brain, 119(5), 1763–1774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Repetti RL, Taylor SE, & Seeman TE (2002). Risky families: family social environments and the mental and physical health of offspring. Psychological bulletin, 128(2), 330. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Revelle W (2014). psych: Procedures for psychological, psychometric, and personality research. Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois, 165. [Google Scholar]
  127. Roy Vasa, Bruck M, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Sweeney M, … Pine DS (2008). Attention bias toward threat in pediatric anxiety disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(10), 1189–1196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Salemink E, van den Hout MA, & Kindt M (2007). Selective attention and threat: quick orienting versus slow disengagement and two versions of the dot probe task. Behav Res Ther, 45(3), 607–615. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.04.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Sampson RJ, Raudenbush SW, & Earls F (1997). Neighborhoods and violent crime: A multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science, 277(5328), 918–924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Sara, & Bouret S (2012). Orienting and reorienting: the locus coeruleus mediates cognition through arousal. Neuron, 76(1), 130–141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Schmukle SC (2005). Unreliability of the dot probe task. European Journal of Personality, 19(7), 595–605. doi: 10.1002/per.554 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  132. Schneider E, & Zuccoloto A (2007). E-prime 2.0 [Computer software]: Pittsburg. PA: Psychological Software Tools. [Google Scholar]
  133. Shanahan L, Calkins SD, Keane SP, Kelleher R, & Suffness R (2014). Trajectories of internalizing symptoms across childhood: the roles of biological self-regulation and maternal psychopathology. Development and psychopathology, 26(4pt2), 1353–1368. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Spence SH, Najman JM, Bor W, O’callaghan MJ, & Williams GM (2002). Maternal anxiety and depression, poverty and marital relationship factors during early childhood as predictors of anxiety and depressive symptoms in adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(4), 457–469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Staff RT, Murray AD, Ahearn TS, Mustafa N, Fox HC, & Whalley LJ (2012). Childhood socioeconomic status and adult brain size: childhood socioeconomic status influences adult hippocampal size. Annals of neurology, 71(5), 653–660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Stein JL, Medland SE, Vasquez AA, Hibar DP, Senstad RE, Winkler AM, … Bergmann Ø (2012). Identification of common variants associated with human hippocampal and intracranial volumes. Nature genetics, 44(5), 552–561. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Sussman E, Winkler I, & Schroger E (2003). Top-down control over involuntary attention switching in the auditory modality. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 10(3), 630–637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Taki Y, Thyreau B, Kinomura S, Sato K, Goto R, Kawashima R, & Fukuda H (2011). Correlations among brain gray matter volumes, age, gender, and hemisphere in healthy individuals. PLoS One, 6(7), e22734. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Thai N, Taber-Thomas BC, & Pérez-Edgar KE (2016). Neural correlates of attention biases, behavioral inhibition, and social anxiety in children: An ERP study. Developmental cognitive neuroscience, 19, 200–210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Thapar A, & Rutter M (2015). Using natural experiments and animal models to study causal hypotheses in relation to child mental health problems Rutter’s child and adolescent psychiatry: John Wiley and Sons Limited, Oxford. [Google Scholar]
  141. Tottenham Tanaka, J. W., Leon AC, McCarry T, Nurse M, Hare TA, … Nelson C (2009). The NimStim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Res, 168(3), 242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Troller-Renfree S, McDermott JM, Nelson CA, Zeanah CH, & Fox NA (2015). The effects of early foster care intervention on attention biases in previously institutionalized children in Romania. Developmental science, 18(5), 713–722. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Uematsu A, Matsui M, Tanaka C, Takahashi T, Noguchi K, Suzuki M, & Nishijo H (2012). Developmental trajectories of amygdala and hippocampus from infancy to early adulthood in healthy individuals. PloS one, 7(10), e46970. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Vijayakumar N, Whittle S, Yücel M, Dennison M, Simmons J, & Allen NB (2014). Thinning of the lateral prefrontal cortex during adolescence predicts emotion regulation in females. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 3(11), 1845–1854. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Voineskos AN, Winterburn JL, Felsky D, Pipitone J, Rajji TK, Mulsant BH, & Chakravarty MM (2015). Hippocampal (subfield) volume and shape in relation to cognitive performance across the adult lifespan. Human brain mapping, 36(8), 3020–3037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Wang Braskie, M. N., Hafzalla GW, Faskowitz J, McMahon KL, de Zubicaray GI, … Thompson PM (2017). Relationship of a common OXTR gene variant to brain structure and default mode network function in healthy humans. NeuroImage, 147, 500–506. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Wechsler D (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence: Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
  148. Wechsler D (2014). Wechsler adult intelligence scale-Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV). San Antonio, Texas: Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
  149. Welberg LA, & Seckl JR (2001). Prenatal stress, glucocorticoids and the programming of the brain. Journal of neuroendocrinology, 13(2), 113–128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Wilcox RR, & Tian T (2008). Comparing dependent correlations. The Journal of general psychology, 135(1), 105–112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Wyciszkiewicz A, & Pawlak MA (2014). Basal ganglia volumes: MR-derived reference ranges and lateralization indices for children and young adults. The neuroradiology journal, 27(5), 595–612. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

13415_2018_670_MOESM1_ESM
Sup_Fig1
Sup_Fig2

RESOURCES