Skip to main content
Elsevier Sponsored Documents logoLink to Elsevier Sponsored Documents
. 2020 Jan 8;105(1):106–121.e10. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.10.001

Natural Variation in a Dendritic Scaffold Protein Remodels Experience-Dependent Plasticity by Altering Neuropeptide Expression

Isabel Beets 1, Gaotian Zhang 2, Lorenz A Fenk 1, Changchun Chen 1, Geoffrey M Nelson 1, Marie-Anne Félix 2,, Mario de Bono 1,3,4,∗∗
PMCID: PMC6953435  PMID: 31757604

Summary

The extent to which behavior is shaped by experience varies between individuals. Genetic differences contribute to this variation, but the neural mechanisms are not understood. Here, we dissect natural variation in the behavioral flexibility of two Caenorhabditis elegans wild strains. In one strain, a memory of exposure to 21% O2 suppresses CO2-evoked locomotory arousal; in the other, CO2 evokes arousal regardless of previous O2 experience. We map that variation to a polymorphic dendritic scaffold protein, ARCP-1, expressed in sensory neurons. ARCP-1 binds the Ca2+-dependent phosphodiesterase PDE-1 and co-localizes PDE-1 with molecular sensors for CO2 at dendritic ends. Reducing ARCP-1 or PDE-1 activity promotes CO2 escape by altering neuropeptide expression in the BAG CO2 sensors. Variation in ARCP-1 alters behavioral plasticity in multiple paradigms. Our findings are reminiscent of genetic accommodation, an evolutionary process by which phenotypic flexibility in response to environmental variation is reset by genetic change.

Keywords: experience-dependent plasticity; natural variation; neuropeptide; genetic accommodation; Caenorhabditis elegans, carbon dioxide sensing; oxygen sensing

Highlights

  • Behavioral flexibility varies across Caenorhabditis and C. elegans wild isolates

  • A natural polymorphism in ARCP-1 underpins inter-individual variation in plasticity

  • ARCP-1 is a dendritic scaffold protein localizing cGMP signaling machinery to cilia

  • Disrupting ARCP-1 alters behavioral plasticity by changing neuropeptide expression


Individuals can vary in their capacity to adapt their behavior to changes in the environment. Beets et al. identify a natural genetic polymorphism that modifies behavioral flexibility by altering the neuromodulatory output of primary sensory neurons.

Introduction

Animals reconfigure their behavior and physiology in response to experience, and many studies highlight mechanisms underlying such plasticity (Bargmann, 2012, Owen and Brenner, 2012). While plasticity is presumed crucial for evolutionary success, it has costs and often varies across species and between individuals (Coppens et al., 2010, Dewitt et al., 1998, Mery, 2013, Niemelä et al., 2013). Variation in behavioral flexibility is thought to underlie inter-individual differences in cognitive ability and capacity to cope with environmental challenges (Coppens et al., 2010, Niemelä et al., 2013). The genetic and cellular basis of inter-individual variation in experience-dependent plasticity is, however, poorly understood.

Genetic accommodation and assimilation are concepts used to describe variation in plasticity on an evolutionary timescale. Waddington and Schmalhausen suggested genetic assimilation occurs when a phenotype initially responsive to the environment becomes fixed in a specific state (Renn and Schumer, 2013, Schmalhausen, 1949, Waddington, 1942, Waddington, 1953). This loss of plasticity may reflect genetic drift or selection against the costs of expressing adaptive behaviors (Niemelä et al., 2013). Studies of genetic assimilation led to the broader concept of genetic accommodation, referring to evolutionary genetic variation leading to any change in the environmental regulation of a phenotype (Crispo, 2007, West-Eberhard, 2005). Many studies in insects, fish, rodents, and primates highlight inter-individual variation in behavioral plasticity; in some cases this has been shown to be heritable (Dingemanse and Wolf, 2013, Izquierdo et al., 2007, Mery et al., 2007), but the mechanisms responsible for these differences remain enigmatic.

Many animals use gradients of respiratory gases to help locate prey, mates, or predators and have evolved sophisticated behavioral responses to environmental changes in oxygen (O2) and carbon dioxide (CO2) levels (Carrillo and Hallem, 2015, Cummins et al., 2014, Guerenstein and Hildebrand, 2008, Prabhakar and Semenza, 2015). Where studied, behavioral responses to CO2 have been shown to depend on environmental context, past experience, and life stage (Carrillo et al., 2013, Fenk and de Bono, 2017, Guillermin et al., 2017, Hallem and Sternberg, 2008, Sachse et al., 2007, Vulesevic et al., 2006). This flexibility makes CO2-sensing an attractive paradigm to study natural variation in behavioral plasticity.

CO2 responses in Caenorhabditis elegans are sculpted by previous O2 experience (Carrillo et al., 2013, Fenk and de Bono, 2017, Kodama-Namba et al., 2013). Acclimation to surface O2 levels (i.e., 21%) generates a memory that suppresses aversion of high CO2. The O2 memory is written over hours by O2 sensors, called URX, AQR, and PQR, whose activity is tonically stimulated by 21% O2 (Busch et al., 2012, Fenk and de Bono, 2017). 21% O2 is itself aversive to C. elegans, most likely because it signals surface exposure (Gray et al., 2004, Persson et al., 2009). By suppressing CO2 aversiveness, C. elegans acclimated to 21% O2 may increase their chance of escaping the surface into buried environments with elevated CO2 (Fenk and de Bono, 2017).

Here, we show that the impact of O2 experience on CO2 aversion varies across Caenorhabditis species and between wild C. elegans isolates. By characterizing differences between C. elegans isolates, we identify a polymorphism in a dendritic ankyrin-repeat scaffold protein, ARCP-1, that alters plasticity in one strain. ARCP-1 biochemically interacts with the conserved cyclic nucleotide phosphodiesterase PDE-1 and localizes it with molecular sensors for CO2 to the dendritic ends of BAG sensory neurons. Disrupting ARCP-1 resets CO2 sensitivity and experience-dependent plasticity of CO2 escape, in part by altering neuropeptide expression and conferring strong aversion to CO2.

Results

Natural Variation in Experience-Dependent Plasticity in Caenorhabditis

In C. elegans, a memory of recent O2 levels reprograms aversive responses to CO2 (Fenk and de Bono, 2017). We hypothesized this experience-dependent plasticity is evolutionarily variable. To investigate this, we compared the CO2 responses of different Caenorhabditis species grown at 21% or 7% O2 (Figure S1A). Animals were transferred to a thin bacterial lawn in a microfluidic chamber kept at 7% O2, stimulated with 3% CO2, and their behavioral responses quantified. We used a background level of 7% O2 in all assays because C. elegans dwell locally at this O2 concentration, making locomotory arousal by CO2 prominent. By contrast, 21% O2 evokes sustained rapid movement, making CO2 responses above this high baseline proportionally small. As a representative C. elegans strain, we used LSJ1, a wild-type (N2-like) laboratory strain bearing natural alleles of the neuropeptide receptor npr-1(215F) and the neuroglobin glb-5(Haw). We did not use the standard N2 strain, because it has acquired mutations in npr-1 and glb-5 that confer gas-sensing defects (McGrath et al., 2009, Persson et al., 2009). As expected, C. elegans was aroused more strongly by CO2 when acclimated to 7% O2 (Figure S1B). By contrast, O2 experience did not alter the absolute speed of representative strains of C. latens and C. angaria at 3% CO2 (Figure S1B). Because C. angaria was not aroused by 3% CO2, we tested its response to 5% and 10% CO2. These levels evoked locomotory arousal that, as in C. elegans, was stronger in animals acclimated to 7% O2 (Figure S1C). Thus, C. angaria is less sensitive to CO2 than C. elegans, but its arousal by CO2 remains dependent on O2 experience. By contrast, CO2 responses of C. latens were unaffected by previous O2 experience at any concentration tested (Figure S1D). Unexpectedly, acclimation to 7% O2 suppressed rather than enhanced the locomotory response of C. nigoni to CO2 (Figure S1B). Thus, the effect of O2 memory on CO2-evoked behavioral responses is evolutionarily variable.

Effect of O2 Memory on CO2 Responses Varies between C. elegans Wild Isolates

Our findings prompted us to seek intra-species variation in how O2 experience influences CO2 responses, by studying a genetically diverse collection of wild C. elegans isolates (Figure S2A). Most strains responded like the reference strain (Figures 1A–1C and S2A). However, two isolates, the French JU1249 and German MY16 strains, responded more strongly than other isolates to a rise in CO2 regardless of O2 experience (Figures 1B and 1D). For MY16 CO2 aversion was stronger when animals were acclimated to 7% O2, recapitulating the cross-modulation of CO2 responses observed in most strains (Figures 1C and 1E). By contrast, JU1249 animals acclimated to 21% O2 further enhanced rather than suppressed CO2 escape (Figures 1C and 1F). To probe further if the O2-dependent plasticity of CO2 escape had changed in JU1249, we quantified escape responses at different CO2 concentrations. npr-1; glb-5 control animals always responded more strongly to CO2 when acclimated to 7% O2, but this was not the case for JU1249 at any CO2 concentration tested (Figure 1G). CO-evoked arousal was stronger in JU1249 animals acclimated to 21% O2 than in those acclimated to 7% O2 (Figure 1H), suggesting that JU1249 fails to suppress CO2 escape at 21% O2.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Natural Variation in the Regulation of CO2 Escape by Previous O2 Experience

(A) A C. elegans reference strain is more strongly aroused by CO2 when acclimated to 7% rather than 21% O2. Two-way ANOVA with Šidák test; n = 6 assays. In this, and all subsequent figures, the background O2 level in the assay is 7%.

(B) Natural variation in the CO2 response of C. elegans wild isolates acclimated to 21% O2. Bars represent average increase in speed ± SEM when CO2 rises from 0% to 3%. The CO2-evoked speed increase is significantly different (p < 0.05) between isolates labeled with different letters (a–d). One-way ANOVA with Tukey test; n = 6 assays.

(C) The effect of O2 memory on CO2 responses in wild C. elegans isolates. Bars show mean ± SEM for time intervals indicated in (A) and Figure S2A. Two-way ANOVA with Šidák test; n = 6 assays.

(D) JU1249 and MY16 are more strongly aroused by CO2, regardless of previous O2 experience. Bars plot mean ± SEM. Two-way ANOVA with Tukey test; n = 6 assays.

(E and F) CO2 responses of MY16 (E) and JU1249 (F) animals acclimated to 21% or 7% O2. Two-way ANOVA with Šidák test; n = 6 assays.

(G) Acclimation to 21% O2 in JU1249, unlike the reference strain LSJ1, enhances rather than suppresses locomotory arousal at different CO2 concentrations. n = 30–61 animals for npr-1; glb-5, n = 59–66 animals for JU1249. Mann-Whitney U test.

(H) CO2 arousal is increased more strongly in JU1249 animals acclimated to 21% rather than 7% O2. Bars plot mean ± SEM for time intervals indicated in (G). Two-way ANOVA with Šidák test. n = 4 assays.

For (A), (E), and (F), solid lines plot mean and shaded areas show SEM. Black bars indicate time intervals used for statistical comparisons. For (A)–(H), 20–30 animals were assayed in at least 4 trials for each condition. p < 0.05; ∗∗p < 0.01; ∗∗∗p < 0.001; ∗∗∗∗p < 0.0001; ns, not significant.

See also Figures S1 and S2.

The increased locomotory arousal of JU1249 and MY16 in response to CO2 could reflect reduced inhibitory input from the neural circuit signaling 21% O2. To probe this, we asked if these isolates show altered behavioral responses to 21% O2. All isolates we tested responded similarly when we switched O2 from 7% to 21% (Figure S2B), suggesting they retained a functional O2-sensing circuit.

In our assays, we exposed animals acclimated to 21% O2 to a downshift to 7% O2 3 min before the CO2 stimulus. To ask if this drop in O2, rather than O2 experience, altered the CO2 response in JU1249, we extended the time animals spent at 7% O2 prior to receiving the CO2 stimulus to 24 min. JU1249 was still more strongly aroused by CO2 when acclimated to 21% rather than 7% O2; as expected, O2 experience had the opposite effect on plasticity in npr-1; glb-5 controls (Figure S2C). We also compared the behavioral responses of JU1249 and npr-1; glb-5 animals to a 21% to 7% O2 stimulus and found no significant differences (Figure S2D). Thus, the ability of an O2 memory to modify CO2 escape appears to be altered in JU1249, recapitulating the phenotype observed in C. nigoni.

Natural Variation in the Ankyrin Repeat Protein ARCP-1 Alters Plasticity of CO2 Responses

We sought the genetic changes conferring altered plasticity of CO2 responses in JU1249. Besides altering this phenotype, JU1249 exhibited reduced aggregation and bordering behavior on an E. coli food lawn compared to other C. elegans wild isolates (Figures 2A and 2B). We speculated JU1249 aggregated poorly because increased avoidance of CO2 shifted the balance between attraction and repulsion as aerobic animals come together. In this model, the aggregation phenotype, which is easy to score, is linked to altered JU1249 CO2 responses.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Natural Variation in ARCP-1 Alters CO2 Responses

(A and B) Individuals of JU2825, like most C. elegans wild isolates, aggregate at the border of an E. coli lawn (A). By contrast, JU1249 animals disperse across the lawn (B).

(C) Selection-based QTL mapping approach to establish the genetic basis of solitary behavior in JU1249.

(D) Line plots showing differences in JU1249 allele frequencies between treatment A and B for each replicate pair, using a sliding window 5 single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) wide and a step size of one SNP. Replicates are indicated by different colors. Chromosome I shows little consistent deviations from equal frequencies in the two treatments, whereas chromosome III shows a strong enrichment at the 3–4 Mb interval.

(E) Read-count frequency differences between treatment A and B analyzed for consistency across eight replicates using the Cochran-Mantel-Haenszel test. Only chromosome III is shown. p values are shown as –log10 (p value) adjusted by the Bonferroni correction.

(F) Gene structure of arcp-1 (F34D10.6). Boxes represent exons and lines indicate introns. The wild isolate JU1249 has an 8 bp deletion that introduces a frameshift. The db1082 allele, isolated in a genetic screen for aggregation-defective mutants, replaces a Gln codon with a premature stop codon.

(G) Wild-type arcp-1b rescues bordering and aggregation phenotypes of JU1249 and db1082 animals. For each assay, 50–60 animals were transferred to a bacterial lawn and behaviors were scored after 6 h. One-way ANOVA with Tukey test. n ≥ 6 assays.

(H) arcp-1(db1082) animals, like JU1249, fail to suppress CO2 responses when acclimated to 21% O2. n = 5–6 assays. Two-way ANOVA with Šidák test.

(I) Expressing wild-type arcp-1 restores the O2-dependent modulation of CO2 responses in arcp-1; npr-1 mutants. n = 67–105 animals. Mann-Whitney U test.

(J) An arcp-1b transgene, but not arcp-1a, rescues the enhanced locomotory arousal evoked by CO2 in arcp-1; npr-1 animals acclimated to 21% O2. n ≥ 4 assays for all genotypes. One-way ANOVA with Tukey test.

(K) An arcp-1b transgene rescues the enhanced CO2 response of JU1249 animals acclimated to 21% O2. n = 6 assays. One-way ANOVA with Tukey test.

For (H)–(K), each genotype was tested in at least 4 assays with 20–30 animals per trial. Solid lines plot mean; shaded areas show SEM; horizontal black bars indicate time intervals for statistical comparisons; vertical bars plot mean ± SEM. ∗∗p < 0.01; ∗∗∗p < 0.001; ∗∗∗∗p < 0.0001; ns, not significant.

See also Figures S3, S4, and S5 and Data S1 and S2.

Before testing this hypothesis, we ruled out the possibility that JU1249 is genetically contaminated by the non-aggregating N2 lab strain, by genotyping the npr-1, glb-5, and nath-10 loci, which have acquired polymorphisms during N2 domestication (Duveau and Félix, 2012, McGrath et al., 2009, Persson et al., 2009, Weber et al., 2010). JU1249 exhibited the natural alleles found in other wild isolates at all three loci (Figure S3).

To map the JU1249 aggregation defect, we used a selection-based quantitative trait locus (QTL) mapping approach in which we crossed JU1249 to the aggregating C. elegans wild isolate JU2825 (Figure 2A). To find conditions for selection-based QTL mapping, we first defined two treatments that differentially selected for aggregating and solitary animals and performed competition tests between JU1249 and JU2825 under these treatments. Starting with a 50:50% mix of each strain, JU1249 (solitary) outcompeted JU2825 (aggregating) when the populations were transferred by liquid harvest and aliquot (Figure S4A, treatment A), indicating that JU1249 has higher fitness in these conditions than JU2825. When cultivated by transferring an agar chunk from the border of the food lawn, where aggregating animals accumulate (Figure S4A, treatment B), JU2825 outcompeted JU1249, which indicates the aggregation trait in C. elegans is selectable. We used treatments A and B as selection regimes on populations of cross-progenies of JU1249 and JU2825 (Figure 2C), sequenced their genomes, and compared allele frequencies of paired replicate populations under the two treatments (Data S1; STAR Methods). Populations selected for aggregation (treatment B) were expected to have higher frequencies of JU2825 alleles at the QTL that affect the variation in aggregation behavior compared to the paired populations (treatment A). Our analysis showed large variation in allele frequencies among replicates, suggesting founder effects due to the moderate population sizes in the first crosses (Figures 2D and S4B). We used two criteria to identify candidate QTL regions associated with the aggregation phenotype. First, we identified regions that show consistent differences in allele frequencies among all replicate pairs for the two treatments (Figures S4B and S4C). Second, we narrowed down these regions by examining replicates for the position of the closest recombination event that was selected (Figure S4D). Based on these criteria, we identified a genomic interval on chromosome III (3361869–4086899 bp) as a candidate region, showing a highly significant difference in allele frequencies among the eight population pairs (Figures 2E and S4C).

The 725 kb QTL region in JU1249 contained 3 polymorphisms in protein-coding genes compared to N2 and JU2825 (Data S1J). An 8 bp deletion (mfP22) in the open reading frame of the gene F34D10.6, which we named arcp-1 (for ankyrin repeat containing protein, see below), stood out as a promising candidate for two reasons. First, mfP22 is the only polymorphism predicted to abolish protein function (Data S1J and S1K), introducing a frameshift and premature stop codon in both transcripts of the arcp-1 gene (Figures 2F and S5A). Second, we independently found several alleles of arcp-1 in a collection of sequenced mutants that suppress aggregation behavior of npr-1(null) animals, including two that introduced premature stop codons. The number and kind of these alleles made it likely that disrupting arcp-1 caused an aggregation defect. Consistent with this hypothesis, the aggregation defect of one strain (db1082 allele) mapped to a 1 Mb interval on chromosome III, centered on arcp-1 (Figures 2F and S5A). Two mutants from the million mutation project (Thompson et al., 2013), harboring arcp-1 alleles (gk856856 and gk863317) that introduce premature stop codons, were also defective in aggregation and bordering (Figures S5B and S5C). To show conclusively that mutations in arcp-1 disrupt aggregation, we performed transgenic rescue experiments. Expressing wild-type arcp-1 in JU1249 or in arcp-1(db1082); npr-1(null) mutants restored aggregation and bordering behavior (Figure 2G).

To gain insight into the distribution of the arcp-1(mfP22) polymorphism in C. elegans, we examined other wild isolates. Our analysis suggests mfP22 is a rare allele, because we did not find it in a set of 151 worldwide C. elegans isolates, including MY16 (Data S2).

Does disrupting arcp-1 alter responses to CO2? arcp-1(db1082); npr-1(null) animals behaved like JU1249: they showed no overt defect in their response to a 21%-to-7% O2 downshift (Figure S5D) but failed to suppress escape from different CO2 concentrations when acclimated to 21% O2 (Figures 2H, S5E, and S5F). A wild-type arcp-1 transgene rescued this CO2 plasticity defect (Figure 2I). arcp-1 is thus required for animals acclimated to 21% O2 to suppress escape from high CO2 environments.

Gene predictions and cDNA cloning revealed arcp-1a and arcp-1b transcripts that overlap at their 3′ end (Figure 2F; Wormbase WS265). The db1082 and mfP22 alleles affect both arcp-1 transcripts (Figure 2F). Expressing arcp-1b fully rescued the heightened CO2 response of these animals, whereas a transgene for the longer arcp-1a transcript did not (Figures 2J and 2K). A mutation that only disrupted arcp-1a also failed to recapitulate the enhanced CO2 response and aggregation phenotype of mutants defective in both arcp-1 transcripts (Figures S5C and S5G). Thus arcp-1, and more specifically the product of its b transcript, is required for animals to suppress CO2 escape following acclimation to 21% O2.

ARCP-1 Acts in BAG Sensory Neurons to Suppress CO2 Escape Behavior

arcp-1 encodes an ankyrin repeat protein (Figure 3A) homologous to C. elegans ankyrin UNC-44 and vertebrate ankyrins (Otsuka et al., 1995). These proteins are important for the subcellular localization of neural signaling complexes (e.g., anchoring components of the axon initial segment and allowing cyclic nucleotide-gated channels to accumulate in photoreceptor cilia) (Kizhatil et al., 2009, Leterrier et al., 2017, Maniar et al., 2011). Besides ankyrin repeats, ARCP-1 contains a DPY-30 domain (Figure 3A). Both domains are common protein interaction motifs that regulate the function and spatial organization of diverse signaling complexes (Gopal et al., 2012, Jones and Svitkina, 2016, Monteiro and Feng, 2017, Sivadas et al., 2012). ARCP-1’s domain structure suggests it serves a similar role trafficking or localizing signaling proteins in the nervous system.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

ARCP-1B Acts in BAG Sensors to Suppress CO2 Escape Behavior

(A) Protein domain architecture of ARCP-1B.

(B) Schematic model of the core neural circuits for O2 and CO2 responses in C. elegans (Fenk and de Bono, 2017, Guillermin et al., 2017, Laurent et al., 2015). O2-sensing neurons URX, AQR, and PQR tonically signal 21% O2. CO2 stimuli and O2 downshifts are detected by BAG and other neurons. The O2 sensors cross-modulate the neural circuit underlying CO2 escape. The role of RIA, RIG, AIA, and AIZ in the CO2 circuit is hypothesized based on their function in CO2 aerotaxis (Guillermin et al., 2017).

(C) A fosmid reporter transgene for arcp-1 is expressed in all major O2 and CO2 sensors, and other sensory neurons. Scale bar, 10 μm; A, anterior; V, ventral.

(D) Cell-specific expression of arcp-1b in BAG, using the flp-17 promoter (BAGp), rescues locomotory arousal by CO2, whereas expression in URX, AQR, and PQR, using the gcy-32 promoter (URX-AQR-PQRp), does not. One-way ANOVA with Tukey test. n ≥ 5 assays with 20–30 animals per trial. p < 0.05; ∗∗∗p < 0.001; ∗∗∗∗p < 0.0001; ns, not significant.

See also Figures S5 and S6.

A fosmid-based bicistronic transgene that co-expressed arcp-1 and free GFP was expressed in the main CO2 and O2 sensors: the URX, AQR, PQR, and BAG neurons (Figures 3B and 3C). We also observed expression in a subset of other sensory neurons (i.e., AFD, ASE, AWC, and AWB) (Figure 3C). This raised the possibility that disrupting arcp-1 modifies plasticity in multiple paradigms. To test this, we assayed arcp-1 mutants in a salt-based associative learning paradigm (Figure S6A; Beets et al., 2012, Hukema et al., 2008). arcp-1 mutants were defective in gustatory plasticity: although mock-conditioned animals showed normal attraction to NaCl, upon salt conditioning they failed to downregulate salt chemotaxis behavior (Figure S6B).

To gain insight into arcp-1 function, we focused on the failure of arcp-1 mutants to suppress CO2 escape when acclimated to 21% O2. Because arcp-1 is expressed in the BAG CO2 sensors, we asked if it acts in these neurons to suppress CO2 escape. Cell-specific expression of wild-type arcp-1 in BAG using the flp-17 promoter (Kim and Li, 2004) rescued the increased locomotory activity of arcp-1 mutants at 3% CO2 (Figure 3D). We also tested if arcp-1 can act in URX, AQR, and PQR neurons, which sense 21% O2, to suppress CO2 escape. Expressing arcp-1 in these neurons, using the gcy-32 promoter (Yu et al., 1997), did not rescue the CO2 phenotype of arcp-1 mutants (Figure 3D). By contrast, the arcp-1 aggregation defect could be rescued by expressing arcp-1 either in BAG or in URX, AQR, and PQR (Figures S5H and S5I). Together, these data show that arcp-1 functions in gas-sensing neurons and cell-autonomously suppresses CO2 escape in the BAG CO2 sensors.

BAG Responses to CO2 Are Tuned by ARCP-1

We investigated if the increased behavioral response of arcp-1 animals to CO2 was associated with increased CO2-evoked Ca2+ responses in BAG neurons. Using the ratiometric sensor YC3.60, we quantified fluorescence changes at the cell body of BAG in response to CO2. Animals acclimated to 21% O2 were transferred to a microfluidic chamber kept at 7% O2 and stimulated with different CO2 concentrations. BAG Ca2+ responses evoked by 1% and 3% CO2 were significantly higher in arcp-1 mutants compared to controls (Figure 4A). Unlike for CO2 escape, expressing arcp-1 either in BAG or in URX, AQR, and PQR rescued the CO2 Ca2+ phenotype in BAG (Figure 4B). At 3% CO2, animals with arcp-1 rescued in URX, AQR, and PQR even showed a smaller increase in Ca2+ activity compared to npr-1 controls, which may be due to an overexpression effect of the gcy-32p::arcp-1b transgene (Figure 4B). Because BAG neurons exhibit a phasic-tonic response to CO2, we also measured Ca2+ responses during prolonged CO2 stimulation. BAG tonic responses to 3% CO2 were reduced in arcp-1 mutants, although the effect was small (Figure S6C).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

ARCP-1 Suppresses BAG Responses to CO2

(A and B) Mean traces of BAG Ca2+ activity in npr-1 and arcp-1; npr-1 animals in response to different CO2 concentrations. Mutants for arcp-1 show increased Ca2+ activity at 1% and 3% CO2 (A), which is rescued by expressing arcp-1 either in BAG (flp-17p) or URX, AQR, and PQR (gcy-32p) (B). n = number of animals. Two-way ANOVA with Šidák test in (A). One-way ANOVA with Holm-Šidák test in (B).

(C–E) CO2-evoked turning behavior. (C) Rising CO2 levels stimulate stronger turning behavior in JU1249 (n = 85 animals) than in npr-1(215F) animals (n = 81). Mann-Whitney U test. (D) CO2-evoked turning is also increased in arcp-1(db1082); npr-1(ad609) animals. BAG-specific expression of a flp-17p::arcp-1b transgene rescues this phenotype, whereas expression of arcp-1b in URX, AQR, and PQR (gcy-32p) does not. One-way ANOVA with Tukey test. n ≥ 5 assays with 20–30 animals per trial. (E) The increased turning of arcp-1; npr-1 animals in response to CO2 requires the GCY-9 CO2 receptor. One-way ANOVA with Tukey test. n = 9 assays with 20–30 animals per trial.

For (A)–(E), solid lines plot mean; shaded areas show SEM; black bars indicate time intervals for statistical comparisons; bar graphs plot mean ± SEM for these intervals. p < 0.05; ∗∗p < 0.01; ∗∗∗p < 0.001; ∗∗∗∗p < 0.0001; ns, not significant.

See also Figure S6.

BAG neurons respond not only to a rise in CO2, but also to a fall in O2 (Zimmer et al., 2009). We asked if the CO2 phenotypes of arcp-1 animals could be indirectly linked to changes in BAG’s ability to respond to O2. BAG Ca2+ activity at 7% O2, measured by YC2.60, was similar for arcp-1 mutants and npr-1 controls, although arcp-1 animals displayed higher Ca2+ at 21% O2 (Figure S6D). Ca2+ responses in URX to a 7% to 21% O2 stimulus were unaffected in arcp-1 animals (Figure S6E).

BAG O2 responses are mediated by the guanylate cyclases GCY-31/GCY-33 and are abolished in mutants of these genes (Zimmer et al., 2009). Animals lacking gcy-33 and gcy-31, like arcp-1 mutants, were aroused more strongly by CO2, but the effects on CO2 escape were additive in an arcp-1; gcy-33; gcy-31; npr-1 quadruple mutant (Figure S6F). Moreover, in gcy-33; gcy-31 mutants, CO2 arousal was suppressed when animals were acclimated to 21% O2—unlike in arcp-1 animals (Figure S6G). These results indicate that arcp-1 can act in a separate genetic pathway from gcy-33 and gcy-31 to regulate CO2 escape. Together with our rescue and Ca2+ imaging data, these findings are consistent with arcp-1 suppressing CO2 escape by inhibiting BAG responses to CO2.

ARCP-1 Inhibits BAG-Mediated Turning Downstream of the CO2 Receptor GCY-9

C. elegans respond to a rise in CO2 not only by becoming aroused and moving faster but also by re-orienting their direction of travel and increasing the frequency of sharp (omega) turns. This behavior is also mediated by BAG (Fenk and de Bono, 2015, Hallem and Sternberg, 2008). Because ARCP-1 acts in BAG to suppress CO2-evoked Ca2+ responses and locomotory arousal, we asked if it also inhibits CO2-evoked turning. Both arcp-1 mutants and JU1249 showed increased turning in response to a rise in CO2 compared to controls (Figures 4C and 4D). This phenotype was rescued by expressing arcp-1 in BAG, but not by expressing it in URX, AQR, and PQR (Figure 4D).

To gain insight into the molecular functions of arcp-1, we examined its effect on CO2-evoked turns further. This part of the locomotory response to CO2 is driven by cGMP signaling from the guanylyl cyclase receptor GCY-9 in BAG neurons (Fenk and de Bono, 2015, Hallem et al., 2011). GCY-9 is a molecular receptor for CO2 and appears to be specifically expressed in BAG (Hallem et al., 2011, Smith et al., 2013). To examine if arcp-1 regulates turning downstream of GCY-9, we measured CO2-evoked turns in a gcy-9; arcp-1 mutant. Disrupting gcy-9 abolished turning evoked by 3% CO2 in both npr-1 and arcp-1; npr-1 animals (Figure 4E), which implies that the mutant’s turning phenotype depends on GCY-9, and ARCP-1 antagonizes GCY-9 signaling in BAG.

ARCP-1 Localizes Phosphodiesterase PDE-1 to BAG Cilia

The ankyrin repeats and DPY-30 motif of ARCP-1 suggest it serves as an interaction partner or scaffold for other proteins. To identify its molecular partners, we took a biochemical approach (Figure 5A). We first made a transgenic strain that expressed GFP-ARCP-1B and showed that it rescued the enhanced CO2 response of the arcp-1 mutant (Figure S7A). We then used anti-GFP nanobodies to pull down GFP-ARCP-1B fusion proteins from C. elegans lysates and identified putative interacting proteins by mass spectrometry (Figure 5A; Data S3). As a negative control, we immunoprecipitated other GFP-tagged cytoplasmic proteins in parallel. Across two independent experiments, we identified phosphodiesterase 1 (PDE-1) as the top specific hit (i.e., the protein having the highest number of spectral counts in ARCP-1B immunoprecipitates [IPs] while having none in control IPs) (Figure 5B; Data S3).

Figure 5.

Figure 5

ARCP-1 Is a Scaffolding Protein that Localizes Phosphodiesterase PDE-1 to Dendritic Endings

(A) Schematic of coimmunoprecipitation (coIP) approach to identify ARCP-1B interactors, by pull-down of an N-terminal GFP tag.

(B) Top ten specific putative interactors of GFP-ARCP-1B identified in two independent coIPs. IPs of other cytoplasmic GFP-tagged proteins provide negative controls.

(C and D) GFP-tagged ARCP-1B and PDE-1B proteins are both enriched at the sensory endings of BAG. Scale bar, 10 μm; A, anterior; V, ventral.

(E) Disrupting arcp-1 reduces enrichment of PDE-1, expressed from the flp-17p, at BAG cilia. Bars plot mean ± SEM n (in bars) = number of animals. Mann-Whitney U test.

(F) pde-1 mutants phenocopy the increased turning frequency of arcp-1 mutants in response to CO2. pde-1; arcp-1 double mutants do not show an additive phenotype. Solid lines plot mean; shaded areas show SEM; black bars indicate time intervals for statistical comparisons; bar graphs plot mean ± SEM for these intervals. One-way ANOVA with Tukey test. n ≥ 8 assays with 20–30 animals per trial. p < 0.05; ∗∗p < 0.01; ∗∗∗∗p < 0.0001; ns, not significant.

See also Figure S7 and Data S3.

PDE-1 is a Ca2+-activated cyclic guanosine monophosphate (cGMP)/cyclic AMP (cAMP) phosphodiesterase orthologous to mammalian Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent PDE1 and is expressed in many neurons, including BAG (Couto et al., 2013, Hallem et al., 2011). As expected from our biochemical data, PDE-1 and ARCP-1 localize to similar compartments in BAG. GFP-tagged ARCP-1B was enriched at sensory endings (Figure 5C), similar to what we observe and what has been reported for PDE-1 (Martínez-Velázquez and Ringstad, 2018; Figure 5D).

The biochemical interaction of ARCP-1 and PDE-1, and their co-localization at dendritic endings, led us to hypothesize that ARCP-1 regulates PDE-1 localization. To test this, we compared enrichment of PDE-1 at BAG cilia in arcp-1 and control animals. Overall, PDE-1 expression was slightly higher in arcp-1 mutants, but enrichment of PDE-1 at the cilia was reduced by more than half in these animals (Figure 5E). To extend this observation, we investigated the subcellular localization of other signaling components of the gas-sensing neurons in arcp-1 mutants. We observed a reduction of GCY-9 levels in BAG cilia, as well as reduced levels of the O2-sensing guanylate cyclase GCY-35 at the sensory endings of URX (Figures S7B and S7C). These phenotypes were not due to a general defect in dendritic localization, because arcp-1 mutants showed normal levels of the cGMP-gated channel subunit TAX-4 and the O2-sensing guanylate cyclase GCY-33 in BAG cilia (Figures S7D and S7E). arcp-1 mutants did not exhibit overt defects in dendritic morphology, based on expression of a flp-17p::gfp transgene and DiI filling of amphid sensory neurons (Figures S7F and S7G). Together, our data suggest that ARCP-1 acts as a scaffold that helps co-localize signal transduction components at sensory endings of some neurons.

Our behavioral, Ca2+ imaging and cell biological results led us to speculate that ARCP-1 promotes a Ca2+-dependent feedback mechanism mediated by PDE-1, which keeps BAG CO2 responses in check by degrading cGMP following activation of the CO2 receptor GCY-9. If this is correct, disrupting pde-1 should phenocopy arcp-1 and increase the frequency of CO2-evoked turns. Moreover, the arcp-1 and pde-1 phenotypes should not be additive. As predicted, pde-1 mutants turned more in response to 3% CO2 than controls and even arcp-1 mutants, likely because pde-1 is more widely expressed and serves broader functions than arcp-1. The turning phenotype was comparable for pde-1, arcp-1, and pde-1; arcp-1 mutants (Figure 5F). These results are consistent with pde-1 and arcp-1 acting in the same genetic pathway to keep CO2 responses in check.

PDE-1 and ARCP-1 Inhibit Expression of FLP-19 Neuropeptides

To investigate further how disrupting arcp-1 alters BAG function, we specifically labeled these neurons with GFP, used fluorescence-activated cell sorting (FACS) to isolate the fluorescent cells from acutely dissociated arcp-1;npr-1 and npr-1 control animals, and profiled their gene expression using RNA sequencing (RNA-seq) (see STAR Methods). Genes that are hallmarks of BAG, such as those involved in CO2 signaling (gcy-9, pde-1, flp-17) and BAG cell fate determination (ets-5) (Guillermin et al., 2011, Hallem et al., 2011), were among the top enriched genes in our dataset (Data S4). arcp-1 itself was among the 100 most highly expressed genes in BAG. The BAG profiles highlighted significant gene expression differences between arcp-1 mutants and controls, notably changes in the abundance of mRNAs encoding neuropeptides, genes involved in ciliary intraflagellar transport, ion channels, and gap junction subunits (see Discussion; Data S4D). These data suggest that loss of ARCP-1 leads to altered gene expression.

One of the most abundant transcripts expressed in BAG whose expression was significantly altered by defects in arcp-1 was the neuropeptide flp-19. flp-19 expression was upregulated 2.4-fold in arcp-1 animals, which would be consistent with increased BAG signaling. Previous work has shown that GCY-9, PDE-1, and the cGMP-gated Ca2+ channel TAX-4 control flp-19 expression in BAG (Romanos et al., 2017), making it an interesting candidate for altering CO2 responses in the arcp-1 mutant. To confirm that defects in arcp-1 increased expression of flp-19, we introduced a flp-19p::gfp reporter transgene (Kim and Li, 2004) into arcp-1 mutants and quantified fluorescence in BAG neurons. Disrupting arcp-1 significantly increased BAG expression of the neuropeptide reporter (Figure 6A). This phenotype was rescued by expressing wild-type arcp-1 in BAG, but not in the O2 sensors URX, AQR, and PQR (Figure 6A). Thus, arcp-1 controls flp-19 expression cell-autonomously in BAG. We observed a similar increase in expression of the flp-19 reporter when pde-1 was mutated (Figure 6A). BAG expression of flp-19 in mutants lacking both arcp-1 and pde-1 was similar to that of the single mutants (Figure S7H). These data suggest that ARCP-1 and PDE-1 together reduce BAG signaling by lowering the expression of some neuropeptides. However, disrupting arcp-1 does not generally increase BAG neuropeptide expression as judged from our BAG profiling experiments (Data S4) and analysis of a flp-17 neuropeptide reporter in BAG (Figure S7I).

Figure 6.

Figure 6

PDE-1 and ARCP-1 Inhibit BAG Expression of FLP-19 Neuropeptides that Potentiate Behavioral Responses to CO2

(A) Mean fluorescence ± SEM of a flp-19 neuropeptide reporter (flp-19p::gfp) in BAG, indicating that PDE-1 and ARCP-1 inhibit flp-19 expression. BAG-specific expression of arcp-1b, using the flp-17 promoter (BAGp), rescues this phenotype, whereas expression in URX, AQR and PQR, using the gcy-32 promoter (URX-AQR-PQRp), does not. n (in bars) = number of animals. One-way ANOVA with Tukey test.

(B) Mean fluorescence ± SEM of flp-19 neuropeptide reporter in BAG neurons of JU1249 and JU2825. Increased expression of flp-19 in JU1249 is rescued by expressing arcp-1b from the BAG-specific flp-17 promoter (BAGp). n (in bars) = number of animals. Kruskal-Wallis with Dunn test.

(C) Disrupting flp-19 suppresses the potentiated turning phenotype of arcp-1; npr-1 animals in response to 3% CO2. One-way ANOVA with Holm Šidák test. n = 9 assays.

(D) CO2-evoked turning of arcp-1; npr-1 mutants following cell-specific knock down of flp-19 expression in BAG. Knock down of flp-19 in the mutant background suppresses turning at 3% CO2, whereas knock down of gfp does not. One-way ANOVA with Dunnett’s test. n ≥ 7 assays.

(E) Knock down of flp-19 expression in BAG partially rescues the increased arousal phenotype of arcp-1; npr-1 animals at 3% CO2. One-way ANOVA with Dunnett’s test. n ≥ 7 assays with 20–30 animals per trial.

(F) BAG-specific knock down of flp-19 in npr-1 animals does not affect the plasticity of CO2 escape in response to previous O2 experience. Two-way ANOVA with Šidák test. n = 7–8 assays.

(G) Animals overexpressing flp-19 in BAG move significantly faster at 3% CO2 compared to npr-1 controls, although their response is still lower than npr-1 animals grown at 7% O2 and arcp-1 mutants. n ≥ 3 assays. One-way ANOVA with Tukey test.

For (C)–(G), 20–30 animals were tested per assay. Solid lines plot mean; shaded areas show SEM; black bars indicate time intervals for statistical comparisons; bars plot mean ± SEM for these intervals. p < 0.05; ∗∗∗p < 0.001; ∗∗∗∗p < 0.0001; ns, not significant.

See also Figure S7 and Data S4.

To ask if flp-19 expression was elevated in JU1249, we backcrossed the flp-19p::gfp transgene ten times to this isolate. We did the same for JU2825 that, unlike JU1249, suppressed CO2 escape when acclimated to 21% O2 (Figure S2A). BAG expression of flp-19 was low in JU2825 and high in JU1249 (Figure 6B). Restoring arcp-1 in BAG significantly reduced flp-19 expression (Figure 6B). Thus, disrupting arcp-1 also increases flp-19 expression in JU1249.

FLP-19 Neuropeptide Signaling from BAG Potentiates Behavioral Responses to CO2

Does increased BAG expression of flp-19 in arcp-1 mutants enhance the behavioral responses of these animals to CO2? If increased flp-19 expression heightened aversion to CO2 in arcp-1 animals, then disrupting flp-19 should reverse this phenotype. Consistent with this hypothesis, deleting flp-19 restored turning at 3% CO2 in the arcp-1 mutant, while it had no effect on this behavior in npr-1 animals (Figure 6C).

To confirm that FLP-19 release from BAG potentiates CO2 responses, we knocked down flp-19 expression specifically in these neurons by expressing RNAi sense and antisense sequences of flp-19 from a BAG-specific gcy-33 promoter (Hallem et al., 2011, Yu et al., 1997). As a negative control, we expressed sense and antisense sequences for gfp under the same promoter and found it had no effect on CO2 responses (Figures 6D and 6E). By contrast, BAG-specific knockdown of flp-19 in arcp-1 mutants restored the frequency of CO2-evoked turns (Figure 6D) and reduced CO2-evoked locomotory arousal in animals acclimated to 21% O2 (Figure 6E). These data suggest increased flp-19 expression in BAG contributes to the enhanced behavioral responses to CO2 in arcp-1 mutants.

The neuropeptide gene flp-19 is also expressed in URX. However, knock down of flp-19 in these neurons, using the gcy-32 promoter, enhanced rather than reduced locomotory arousal at 3% CO2 in arcp-1 animals and increased baseline locomotion in the absence of CO2 (Figure S7K). This result is consistent with previous reports (Carrillo et al., 2013) and suggests that the RNAi effect in BAG is specific to these neurons. We wondered if altered expression of flp-19 from URX contributes to the enhanced CO2 aversion in arcp-1 animals as well. If this is the case, flp-19 expression in URX should be reduced in arcp-1 mutants. Indeed, disrupting arcp-1 decreased expression of the flp-19 reporter in URX. This phenotype was rescued by expressing arcp-1 either in URX or BAG neurons (Figure S7L), suggesting that BAG signaling indirectly influences flp-19 expression in URX.

Does FLP-19 release from BAG promote escape from CO2 in animals that retain functional arcp-1? In npr-1 animals, BAG-specific knock down of flp-19 did not compromise CO2 escape in animals acclimated to 21% O2 or 7% O2 (Figure 6F). Thus, flp-19 is not required for the O2-dependent modulation of CO2 responses. Consistent with this finding, we observed similar expression of the flp-19 reporter in npr-1 animals acclimated to 21% or 7% O2, suggesting that flp-19 expression is not regulated by O2 experience (Figure S7J).

We next asked if increased flp-19 expression in BAG is sufficient to boost C. elegans’ locomotory arousal by CO2. To test this, we overexpressed flp-19 specifically in the BAG neurons of npr-1 animals, acclimated these transgenic animals to 21% O2, and quantified their speed at 3% CO2. Animals overexpressing flp-19 in BAG moved significantly faster at 3% CO2 than npr-1 controls, although their locomotory arousal was weaker than that of arcp-1 animals or of npr-1 animals grown at 7% O2 (Figure 6G). Thus, acclimation to 7% O2 or disrupting arcp-1 alters other signals besides flp-19 to heighten CO2 responses. However, in both scenarios—disruption of arcp-1 or O2 acclimation—increasing flp-19 expression in BAG can potentiate behavioral responses to CO2, leading to increased CO2 aversion.

Discussion

Individuals differ in how they respond to altered circumstances in their environment. This is generally ascribed to a combination of genetic variation and different life experiences. How neural circuits encoding behavioral plasticity vary across individuals is, however, poorly understood. Here, we show that Caenorhabditis species and wild isolates of C. elegans can differ in how past O2 experience influences CO2 escape behavior. We uncover a genetic variant and neuronal mechanism responsible for this variation in behavioral flexibility in one natural C. elegans isolate.

The behavioral phenotypes that we observe are reminiscent of genetic accommodation, when the reaction norm of a flexible phenotype responsive to the environment is altered by genetic change (Figure 7A). Underlying this behavioral change, we find that disrupting ARCP-1 both increases CO2 sensitivity and alters the effect of previous O2 experience on CO2 escape. Animals lacking this dendritic scaffold protein become strongly aroused by CO2 regardless of previous O2 experience, and acclimation to 21% O2 further enhances, rather than suppresses, escape from this aversive cue. We show that loss of arcp-1 mediates these phenotypes by directly altering CO2 responses, rather than by affecting the ability to respond to O2.

Figure 7.

Figure 7

A Model for How Genetic Variation in arcp-1 Affects CO2 Escape Behavior

(A) Effect of the natural arcp-1 allele on experience-dependent plasticity, shown as behavioral reaction norms. C. elegans wild isolates acclimated to a high (21%) O2 environment suppress their aversion to CO2 (left panel). A shift to a low (7%) O2 environment results in a heightened CO2 response. A mutation in arcp-1 alters experience-dependent plasticity and genetically fixes a strong aversive response to CO2 in part by increasing flp-19 neuropeptide expression in BAG CO2 sensors (right panel).

(B) CO2 is detected by the receptor guanylate cyclase GCY-9, expressed in BAG cilia. The ankyrin-repeat scaffold protein ARCP-1 is also enriched at dendritic sensory endings, interacts with PDE-1, and localizes this phosphodiesterase to the cilia of BAG CO2-sensory neurons. PDE-1 and ARCP-1 inhibit CO2-evoked Ca2+ activity and expression of FLP-19 neuropeptide messengers in BAG. In the absence of ARCP-1, less GCY-9 and PDE-1 localize to BAG cilia, and flp-19 is more strongly expressed. Increased FLP-19 expression in BAG contributes to resetting a strong aversive response to CO2 in arcp-1; npr-1 animals regardless of previous O2 experience.

We identify the BAG CO2 sensors as the main site where ARCP-1 suppresses CO2 escape in animals acclimated to 21% O2. Together with previous work (Couto et al., 2013, Romanos et al., 2017), our results suggest a model (Figure 7B) in which ARCP-1 binds and co-localizes the Ca2+-activated phosphodiesterase PDE-1 with guanylyl cyclase receptors for CO2 at the BAG cilia. ARCP-1 and PDE-1 keep signaling from these neurons in check by suppressing CO2-evoked Ca2+ responses and neuropeptide expression. Natural genetic variation has been found to directly alter sensory systems in other animals (McGrath, 2013, Prieto-Godino et al., 2017). We identify BAG as a major cellular focus for variation in CO2 responses, but the possibility remains that loss of arcp-1 disrupts plasticity in other sensory circuits, which may indirectly promote CO2 aversion as well. Some evidence points to changes in URX, but these are not sufficient to explain the heightened CO2 escape behavior in arcp-1 mutants.

Mounting evidence suggests that natural variation in behavioral flexibility is genetically determined (Izquierdo et al., 2007, Mery, 2013, Mery et al., 2007). One well-established example is the natural variation seen at the foraging gene in Drosophila melanogaster. This polymorphism causes individual variation in learning and memory, among other phenotypes, by altering the activity of cGMP-dependent protein kinase G (Mery et al., 2007). It is notable that, both in flies and worms, genetic variation affecting cGMP signaling underlies inter-individual variation in experience-dependent plasticity. Besides gas sensors, ARCP-1 is expressed in olfactory, gustatory, and thermosensory neurons that all signal using cGMP, and arcp-1 mutants show reduced plasticity in a gustatory paradigm. Correlated differences in the plasticity of different sensory modalities have been described as coping styles or behavioral syndromes in other animal models (Coppens et al., 2010) and may also reflect a common genetic or molecular basis. Identifying how loss of arcp-1 compromises plasticity in other sensory circuits should provide a better understanding of such correlated changes in behavioral flexibility.

We have shown that the absence of ARCP-1 alters expression of a range of genes in BAG. One way this influences CO2 aversion is by altering the expression of neuropeptide messengers. Neuropeptides are a diverse group of neuromodulators that, both in vertebrates and invertebrates, are involved in circuit plasticity (Jékely et al., 2018, Taghert and Nitabach, 2012). Natural genetic variation in neuropeptide pathways has been linked to individual differences in aging and social behaviors (Donaldson and Young, 2008, Yin et al., 2017). Our results suggest that they also contribute to heritable differences in behavioral plasticity between individuals. In humans and other primates, natural polymorphisms in serotonergic and dopaminergic systems have been associated with individual differences in memory and cognitive ability (Izquierdo et al., 2007, Zhang et al., 2007). Changing the neuromodulatory tone of circuits likely represents a general mechanism by which genetic variation sculpts individual behavioral plasticity.

Disrupting ARCP-1 increases expression of FLP-19 neuropeptides in BAG. This potentiates or disinhibits both CO2-evoked turning and locomotory arousal in animals acclimated to 21% O2. A FLP-19 receptor is currently unknown; the C. elegans genome encodes ∼150 predicted neuropeptide receptors but none have been reported to bind FLP-19 (Peymen et al., 2014). FLP-19 neuropeptides belong to the ancient and conserved family of RFamide neuropeptides (Peymen et al., 2014). Previous work suggested that CO2-evoked cGMP and Ca2+ signaling promote flp-19 expression in BAG, and this effect is counterbalanced by PDE-1 (Romanos et al., 2017). In arcp-1 mutants, the GCY-9 CO2 receptor and PDE-1 are less enriched at BAG cilia. Although gcy-9 expression is slightly reduced, disrupting ARCP-1 increases BAG Ca2+ activity in response to CO2. This is consistent with proper ciliary localization of PDE-1 keeping BAG Ca2+ signaling in check and could explain the increased flp-19 expression. ARCP-1 and PDE-1 may also orchestrate microdomains of cGMP that can regulate gene expression (Arora et al., 2013, O’Halloran et al., 2012). In vertebrate neurons, nanodomains of the ankyrin G protein, a homolog of ARCP-1, localize to the dendritic spines and the axon initial segment and contribute to neural plasticity (Grubb and Burrone, 2010, Smith et al., 2014). Likewise, mammalian PDE1 has been implicated in the experience-dependent adaptation of sensory responses. In mouse olfactory neurons, PDE1 is specifically enriched at the cilia, although a molecular anchor that localizes the protein to this compartment has not yet been identified (Cygnar and Zhao, 2009).

The molecular mechanism by which ARCP-1 controls flp-19 expression, and whether this relates to its ciliary function, remains to be understood. Interestingly, our transcriptional profiling of BAG neurons revealed a suite of genes involved in intraflagellar transport, including the axonemal dynein che-3, that show ∼2-fold increased expression in arcp-1 animals, although we did not observe obvious defects in cilia morphology. This suggests a feedback mechanism exists by which signaling at the cilium regulates expression of genes involved in ciliary transport. Identifying the molecular factors involved is the next step forward toward understanding this transcriptional regulation.

The mechanisms through which natural genetic variation in arcp-1, acting on an evolutionary timescale, and O2 experience, acting over an animal’s lifetime, sculpt CO2 responsiveness seem to be at least partly distinct. However, in both scenarios—disruption of arcp-1 or acclimation to different O2 environments—release of FLP-19 neuropeptides from BAG can boost the animal’s response to this aversive cue, and through alterations in neuropeptide expression, a strong aversive response may become fixed.

CO2 responses vary between Caenorhabditis species (Carrillo and Hallem, 2015, Pline and Dusenbery, 1987, Viglierchio, 1990). Our results show this variation is at least in part due to differences in O2-dependent modulation, suggesting it is an adaptive trait. We speculate that the influence of O2 memory on other sensory responses enables animals to reconfigure their behavioral priorities according to past experience (Fenk and de Bono, 2017). Animals at the surface may prioritize escape from 21% O2 and gradually suppress their CO2 aversion to facilitate migration to buried environments with less aeration and elevated CO2 levels. Natural variation in the O2-dependent modulation of CO2 escape may result in animals occupying different ecological niches. Alternatively, there could be selection against the costs to maintain sensory systems for behavioral plasticity (Dewitt et al., 1998), which may account for the reduced plasticity of CO2 responses in some nematode species. We do not know the O2 and CO2 conditions in which the arcp-1 deletion may have been selected and can therefore only speculate about its potential fitness benefits. The arcp-1 mutation was not found in any other wild isolate so is likely recent. However, our data indicate substantial variation among both Caenorhabditis species and C. elegans isolates in the response to CO2 (Figures 1 and S2). These findings are consistent with what has been found for other traits (Frézal et al., 2018), where the phenotypic variation for the strain chosen for study is caused by a rare allele found only in that strain, yet phenotypic variation itself is not restricted to that strain. Understanding evolutionary mechanisms that might select for altered plasticity requires more in-depth knowledge on the ecology of these species. The behavioral phenotypes that we observe are consistent with genetic accommodation for a cross-modal gene-environment interaction (Pigliucci et al., 2006). In summary, our study illustrates how natural genetic variation, by altering the neuromodulatory control of aversive behavior, contributes to individual differences in behavioral flexibility.

STAR★Methods

Key Resources Table

REAGENT or RESOURCE SOURCE IDENTIFIER
Antibodies

GFP-Trap Agarose ChromoTek Cat#gta-20; RRID: AB_2631357

Bacterial and Virus Strains

E. coli: Strain OP50 Caenorhabditis Genetics Center Wormbase: OP50; RRID: WB-STRAIN:OP50

Chemicals, Peptides, and Recombinant Proteins

Dermabond tissue adhesive for worm glueing Ethicon Cat#AHV12

Critical Commercial Assays

SuperScript II reverse transcriptase Invitrogen Cat#18064-014
KAPA Hifi HotStart kit KAPA Biosystems Cat#KK2601
Ampure XP beads Beckman Coulter Cat#A63881
Nextera XT DNA sample preparation kit Illumina Cat#FC-131-1096

Deposited Data

Genome sequence data of JU1249 and JU2825 This paper NCBI: PRJNA514933
Genome sequence data of replicate populations for QTL mapping This paper NCBI: PRJNA515248
RNA-Seq data of sorted BAG neurons This paper GEO: GSE135687

Experimental Models: Organisms/Strains

C. elegans: Strain AX1796: glb-5(Haw) V; npr-1(g320) X de Bono lab; Persson et al., 2009 AX1796
C. elegans: Strain LSJ1: Bristol strain Caenorhabditis Genetics Center RRID: WB-STRAIN:LSJ1
C. angaria: Strain RGD1: Caenorhabditis angaria wild isolate Caenorhabditis Genetics Center RRID: WB-STRAIN:RGD1
C. latens: Strain VX80: Caenorhabditis latens wild isolate Caenorhabditis Genetics Center RRID: WB-STRAIN:VX80
C. japonica: Strain DF5081: Caenorhabditis japonica wild isolate Caenorhabditis Genetics Center RRID: WB-STRAIN:DF5081
C. wallacei: Strain JU1904: Caenorhabditis wallacei wild isolate Caenorhabditis Genetics Center RRID: WB-STRAIN:JU1904
C. tropicalis: Strain JU1373: Caenorhabditis tropicalis wild isolate Caenorhabditis Genetics Center RRID: WB-STRAIN:JU1373
C. briggsae: Strain HK105: Caenorhabditis briggsae wild isolate Caenorhabditis Genetics Center RRID: WB-STRAIN:HK105
C. nigoni: Strain JU1422: Caenorhabditis nigoni wild isolate Caenorhabditis Genetics Center RRID: WB-STRAIN:JU1422
C. sinica: Strain JU800: Caenorhabditis sinica wild isolate Caenorhabditis Genetics Center RRID: WB-STRAIN:JU800
C. elegans: Strain ED3011: C. elegans wild isolate Caenorhabditis Genetics Center RRID: WB-STRAIN:ED3011
C. elegans: Strain ED3073: C. elegans wild isolate Caenorhabditis Genetics Center RRID: WB-STRAIN:ED3073
C. elegans: Strain EG4946: C. elegans wild isolate Caenorhabditis Genetics Center RRID: WB-STRAIN:EG4946
C. elegans: Strain JU258: C. elegans wild isolate Caenorhabditis Genetics Center RRID: WB-STRAIN:JU258
C. elegans: Strain JU561: C. elegans wild isolate Caenorhabditis Genetics Center RRID: WB-STRAIN:JU561
C. elegans: Strain JU1088: C. elegans wild isolate Caenorhabditis Genetics Center RRID: WB-STRAIN:JU1088
C. elegans: Strain JU1248: C. elegans wild isolate M.-A. Félix; Troemel et al., 2008 RRID: WB-STRAIN:JU1248
C. elegans: Strain JU1543: C. elegans wild isolate M.-A. Félix RRID: WB-STRAIN:JU1543
C. elegans: Strain JU2825: C. elegans wild isolate M.-A. Félix JU2825
C. elegans: Strain MY16: C. elegans wild isolate Caenorhabditis Genetics Center RRID: WB-STRAIN:MY16
C. elegans: Strain JU1249: C. elegans wild isolate M.-A. Félix; Zhang et al., 2016 RRID: WB-STRAIN:JU1249
C. elegans: Strain AX613: npr-1(g320) X de Bono lab; Persson et al., 2009 AX613
C. elegans: Strain JU3221: arcp-1(mfP22) III; npr-1(215F) X; mfEx94 [arcp-1p::arcp-1b::sl2::gfp; myo-2p::dsRed2] in JU1249 background This paper JU3221
C. elegans: Strain AX204: npr-1(ad609) X de Bono lab; Persson et al., 2009 AX204
C. elegans: Strain AX6574: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X 4x outcrossed This paper AX6574
C. elegans: Strain AX7324: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X 5x outcrossed This paper AX7324
C. elegans: Strain AX6723: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx975 [arcp-1p::arcp-1b::sl2::gfp; unc-122p::rfp] This paper AX6723
C. elegans: Strain AX7094: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx1050 [arcp-1p::arcp-1b::sl2::mKate; lin-44p::gfp] This paper AX7094
C. elegans: Strain AX6720: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx974 [arcp-1p::arcp-1a::sl2::gfp; unc-122p::rfp] This paper AX6720
C. elegans: Strain N2: C. elegans Bristol strain Caenorhabditis Genetics Center RRID: WB-STRAIN:N2_(ancestral)
C. elegans: Strain MY10: C. elegans wild isolate Caenorhabditis Genetics Center RRID: WB-STRAIN:MY10
C. elegans: Strain JU1247: C. elegans wild isolate M.-A. Félix, (Troemel et al., 2008) RRID: WB-STRAIN:JU1247
C. elegans: Strain AX6901: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx1002 [fosmid arcp-1p::arcp-1::sl2::gfp; unc-122p::rfp] This paper AX6901
C. elegans: Strain AX6766: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx984 [gcy-32p::arcp-1b::sl2::gfp; unc-122p::rfp] This paper AX6766
C. elegans: Strain AX6805: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx990 [flp-17p::arcp-1b::sl2::gfp; unc-122p::rfp] This paper AX6805
C. elegans: Strain AX6931: arcp-1(gk856856) III; npr-1(ad609) X This paper AX6931
C. elegans: Strain AX6929: arcp-1(gk863317) III; npr-1(ad609) X This paper AX6929
C. elegans: Strain AX6927: arcp-1(gk852871) III; npr-1(ad609) X This paper AX6927
C. elegans: Strain AX7023: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx1035 [gcy-32p::arcp-1b::sl2::mKate; lin-44p::gfp] This paper AX7023
C. elegans: Strain AX7095: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx990 [flp-17p::arcp-1b::sl2::gfp; unc-122p::rfp]; dbEx1035 [gcy-32p::arcp-1b::sl2::mKate; lin-44p::gfp] This paper AX7095
C. elegans: Strain AX7179: gcy-9(n4470) npr-1(ad609) X This paper AX7179
C. elegans: Strain AX7238: arcp-1(db1082) III; gcy-9(n4470) npr-1(ad609) X This paper AX7238
C. elegans: Strain AX7116: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; dbIs20 [arcp-1p::gfp::arcp-1b; unc-122p::rfp] This paper AX7116
C. elegans: Strain AX6969: malt-1(db1194) II; npr-1(ad609) X; dbIs16 [rab-3p::malt-1::gfp; unc-122p::rfp] This paper AX6969
C. elegans: Strain AX7082: eif-3.L(db1015) II; npr-1(ad609) X; dbIs19 [rab-3p::eif-3.L::gfp; unc-122p::rfp] This paper AX7082
C. elegans: Strain AX7419: npr-1(ad609) X dbEx1075 [flp-17p::pde-1b::gfp; unc-122p::rfp] This paper AX7419
C. elegans: Strain AX7422: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx1075 [flp-17p::pde-1b::gfp; unc-122p::rfp] This paper AX7422
C. elegans: Strain AX2272: pde-1(ok2924) I; npr-1(ad609) X de Bono lab; Couto et al., 2013 AX2272
C. elegans: Strain AX7453: pde-1(ok2924) I; arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X This paper AX7453
C. elegans: Strain AX6881: npr-1(ad609) X dbEx [flp-17p::YC3.60] This paper AX6881
C. elegans: Strain AX6893: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx [flp-17p::YC3.60] This paper AX6893
C. elegans: Strain AX7842: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx [flp-17p::YC3.60]; dbEx1035 [gcy-32p::arcp-1b::sl2::mKate; lin-44::gfp] This paper AX7842
C. elegans: Strain AX7845: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx [flp-17p::YC3.60] dbEx1172 [gcy-33p::arcp-1b::sl2::mKate; unc-122p::gfp] This paper AX7845
C. elegans: Strain AX3516: npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx614 [gcy-37p::YC2.60; unc-122p::rfp] de Bono lab; Fenk and de Bono, 2017 AX3516
C. elegans: Strain AX6877: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx614 [gcy-37p::YC2.60; unc-122p::rfp] This paper AX6877
C. elegans: Strain AX3432: npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx623 [flp-17p::YC2.60; F15E11.1::mCherry] de Bono lab; Gross et al., 2014 AX3432
C. elegans: Strain AX7182: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx623 [flp-17p::YC2.60; F15E11.1::mCherry] This paper AX7182
C. elegans: Strain AX7656: gcy-33(ok232) V; gcy-31(ok296) npr-1(ad609) X This paper AX7656
C. elegans: Strain AX7657: arcp-1(db1082) III; gcy-33(ok232) V; gcy-31(ok296) npr-1(ad609) X This paper AX7657
C. elegans: Strain AX7362: npr-1(ad609) X; wzIs132 [gcy-9p::gcy-9::dsRed] This paper AX7362
C. elegans: Strain AX7361: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; wzIs132 [gcy-9p::gcy-9::dsRed] This paper AX7361
C. elegans: Strain AX7366: npr-1(ad609) X; wzEx156 [gcy-9p::tax-4::gfp] This paper AX7366
C. elegans: Strain AX7365: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; wzEx156 [gcy-9p::tax-4::gfp] This paper AX7365
C. elegans: Strain AX2997: gcy-33(ok232) V; npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx [flp-17p::gcy-33::gfp; unc-122p::rfp] de Bono lab; Gross et al., 2014 AX2997
C. elegans: Strain AX7315: arcp-1(db1082) III; gcy-33(ok232) V; npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx [flp-17p::gcy-33::gfp; unc-122p::rfp] This paper AX7315
C. elegans: Strain AX6516: npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx1053 [gcy-37p::gcy-35::HA::gfp::sl2::mCherry] This paper AX6516
C. elegans: Strain AX7278: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx1053 [gcy-37p::gcy-35::HA::gfp::sl2::mCherry] This paper AX7278
C. elegans: Strain AX7019: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx1033 [flp-17p::gfp; unc-122p::rfp] This paper AX7019
C. elegans: Strain AX7021: npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx1033 [flp-17p::gfp; unc-122p::rfp] This paper AX7021
C. elegans: Strain AX7268: npr-1(ad609) X; ynIs34 [flp-19p::gfp] This paper AX7268
C. elegans: Strain AX7271: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; ynIs34 [flp-19p::gfp] This paper AX7271
C. elegans: Strain AX7279: pde-1(ok2924) I; npr-1(ad609) X; ynIs34 [flp-19p::gfp] This paper AX7279
C. elegans: Strain AX7272: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; ynIs34 [flp-19p::gfp]; dbEx1063 [flp-17p::arcp-1b::sl2::mKate; unc-122p::gfp] This paper AX7272
C. elegans: Strain AX7273: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; ynIs34 [flp-19p::gfp] dbEx1035 [gcy-32p::arcp-1b::sl2::mKate; lin-44::gfp] This paper AX7273
C. elegans: Strain AX7550: pde-1(ok2924) I; arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; ynIs34 [flp-19p::gfp] This paper AX7550
C. elegans: Strain AX7722: ynIs34 [flp-19p::gfp] backcrossed 10x in JU2825 background This paper AX7722
C. elegans: Strain AX7724: ynIs34 [flp-19p::gfp] backcrossed 10x in JU1249 background This paper AX7724
C. elegans: Strain AX7726: dbEx1063 [flp-17p::arcp-1b::sl2::mKate; unc-122p::gfp]; ynIs34 [flp-19p::gfp] backcrossed 10x in JU1249 background This paper AX7726
C. elegans: Strain AX7210: npr-1(ad609) X; ynIs64 [flp-17p::gfp] This paper AX7210
C. elegans: Strain AX7208: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; ynIs64 [flp-17p::gfp] This paper AX7208
C. elegans: Strain AX7321: flp-19(ok2460) npr-1(ad609) X This paper AX7321
C. elegans: Strain AX7322: arcp-1(db1082) III; flp-19(ok2460) npr-1(ad609) X This paper AX7322
C. elegans: Strain AX7754: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx1171 [gcy-33p::gfp (sas); unc-122p::rfp] This paper AX7754
C. elegans: Strain AX7760: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx1173 [gcy-33p::flp-19 (sas); unc-122p::rfp] This paper AX7760
C. elegans: Strain AX7788: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx1178 [gcy-32p::gfp (sas); unc-122p::rfp] This paper AX7788
C. elegans: Strain AX7678: arcp-1(db1082) III; npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx1153 [gcy-32p::flp-19 (sas); unc-122p::gfp] This paper AX7678
C. elegans: Strain AX7793: npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx1173 [gcy-33p::flp-19 (sas); unc-122p::rfp] This paper AX7793
C. elegans: Strain AX7437: npr-1(ad609) X; dbEx1077 [flp-17p::flp-19::sl2::mKate; unc-122p::gfp] This paper AX7437

Oligonucleotides

Primers used in this study This paper Table S2

Software and Algorithms

FlyCapture Point Grey Research https://www.flir.com/products/flycapture-sdk
Zentracker Laurent et al., 2015 https://github.com/wormtracker/zentracker
Neuron Analyzer Laurent et al., 2015 https://github.com/neuronanalyser/neuronanalyser
RStudio 0.99.903 R Development Core Team, 2015 https://www.R-project.org
Pindel Ye et al., 2009 http://gmt.genome.wustl.edu/packages/pindel/
Variant Effect Predictor (VEP) McLaren et al., 2016 www.ensembl.org/info/docs/tools/vep/index.html
Tablet 1.16.09.06 Milne et al., 2013 https://ics.hutton.ac.uk/tablet/
BWA 0.7.8-R455 Li and Durbin, 2009 http://bio-bwa.sourceforge.net/bwa.shtml
Samtools 1.2 Li et al., 2009 http://samtools.sourceforge.net/
Picard 1.114 Broad Institute http://broadinstitute.github.io/picard/
GATK 3.2-2 Van der Auwera et al., 2013 https://software.broadinstitute.org/gatk/
Bowtie2 0.11.0 Langmead and Salzberg, 2012 http://bowtie-bio.sourceforge.net/bowtie2/index.shtml
rRNA remover code This paper https://github.com/lmb-seq/RNA-Seq_utilities
Code for concatenating FASTQ files This paper https://github.com/lmb-seq/RNA-Seq_utilities
PRAGUI RNA-Seq analysis pipeline This paper https://github.com/lmb-seq/PRAGUI
Mascot Matrix Science http://www.matrixscience.com/
Scaffold Proteome Software Inc http://www.proteomesoftware.com/products/scaffold/
Prism 7.0 GraphPad Software https://www.graphpad.com
MATLAB R2014b 8.4 Mathworks https://www.mathworks.com/products/matlab.html
Metamorph Molecular Devices https://www.moleculardevices.com/products/cellular-imaging-systems/acquisition-and-analysis-software/metamorph-microscopy#gref
Fiji (ImageJ) Schindelin et al., 2012 https://imagej.net/Fiji
Imaris Bitplane https://imaris.oxinst.com/

Other

Certified gas mixes BOC N/A

Lead Contact and Materials Availability

Further information and requests for resources and reagents should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the Lead Contact, Mario de Bono (debono@mrc-lmb.cam.ac.uk, mdebono@ist.ac.at).

Experimental Model and Subject Details

Animals

C. elegans and other Caenorhabditis species were maintained under standard conditions (Stiernagle, 2006) on nematode growth medium (NGM) plates seeded with E. coli OP50. Young adult hermaphrodites were used in all experiments. For gonochoristic Caenorhabditis species, young adult females were used. For a list of strains and transgene details, see Table S1 and the Key Resources Table.

The mutations in arcp-1 alleles obtained by forward genetics, and in the JU1249 wild isolate, are shown in Figure S5A. The C. elegans strain JU1249 was isolated from a rotten apple collected in 2007 in Santeuil, France (Zhang et al., 2016). A detailed description of the forward genetic screen that isolated the db1082 allele will be described elsewhere. Causal variants in aggregation-defective mutants from this screen were identified by SNP-based mapping in combination with WGS (Minevich et al., 2012).

Microbe strains

The Escherichia coli OP50 strain was used as a food source for C. elegans and other Caenorhabditis species.

Method Details

Molecular biology

Transgenes were cloned using the Multisite Gateway Three-Fragment cloning system (12537-023, Invitrogen) into pDESTR4R3 II. For transgenic lines, the promoter lengths were: arcp-1p (1.2 kb for arcp-1a and 2 kb for arcp-1b), flp-17p (3.3 kb), gcy-32p (0.6 kb), and gcy-33p (1.0 kb). For rescue experiments, cDNA of arcp-1 isoforms was amplified and cloned into pDONR221, using primers listed in Table S2.

For immunoprecipitation and subcellular localization of ARCP-1, a functional arcp-1p::gfp::arcp-1b transgene was made by fusing GFP coding sequences upstream of the arcp-1b cDNA sequence. To investigate the subcellular localization of PDE-1 in BAG neurons, the pde-1b cDNA sequence was cloned into pDONR221 using primers listed in Table S2. This plasmid was used to generate a flp-17p::pde-1b::gfp transgene, by cloning the GFP reporter sequence in frame and downstream of the pde-1b cDNA sequence. Details of strains and transgenes used to study the subcellular localization of gcy-9, tax-4, gcy-33 and gcy-35 are provided in Table S1. The gcy-9p::gcy-9::mCherry and gcy-9p::tax-4::gfp strains were a kind gift from Dr. Niels Ringstad (New York University School of Medicine, USA).

For flp-19 RNAi, 469 bp of flp-19 cDNA starting from the sequence GCTTTTCCTGTTAA was cloned in both the sense and antisense orientations. For cell-specific RNAi experiments, we expressed these fragments in BAG using the gcy-33p (1.0 kb) and in URX neurons using gcy-32p (0.6 kb). To overexpress flp-19 in BAG, we amplified flp-19 cDNA using primers listed in Table S2, and fused this sequence to the flp-17 (3.3 kb) promoter.

To characterize the expression pattern of arcp-1, we made a fluorescent reporter transgene by fosmid recombineering. pBALU9 was used to amplify a reporter cassette, containing the gpd-2 intergenic SL2 sequence and a GFP coding sequence, which was inserted downstream of the arcp-1 stop codon in the WRM0633bA06 fosmid as described (Tursun et al., 2009). The reporter strain for flp-19 neuropeptide expression (flp-19p::gfp) was a kind gift from Dr. Roger Pocock (Monash University, Australia).

Genotyping of natural polymorphisms

Polymorphisms of npr-1, glb-5, nath-10 and arcp-1 genes in C. elegans wild isolates were genotyped by PCR. Primers used are listed in Table S2.

Behavioral assays

All experiments used young adult hermaphrodite animals, therefore sample stratification was not required within each genotype/condition. For most experiments, measurements were scored using an automated algorithm so blind scoring was not undertaken: see each subsection for details. For details of statistical tests, see the relevant Figure legend for each experiment and also the subsection ‘‘Quantification and Statistical Analysis.’’ All recordings that passed the automated analysis pipeline were included in the final dataset. For rescue and RNAi experiments, behavioral responses and phenotypes were confirmed by testing at least two independent transgenic strains.

Locomotory responses to CO2 and O2

Behavioral responses to gas stimuli were assayed as described (Fenk and de Bono, 2017, Laurent et al., 2015). Animals were acclimated to different O2 levels by growing them for one generation at 21% O2 (room air) or in a gas-controlled incubator kept at 7% O2. For each assay, 20-30 young adult hermaphrodites were transferred onto NGM plates seeded 16–20 h earlier with 20 μL of E. coli OP50. To control gas levels experienced by C. elegans, animals were placed under a 200 μm deep square polydimethylsiloxane (PDMS) chamber with inlets connected to a PHD 2000 Infusion syringe pump (Harvard apparatus). Humidified gas mixtures were delivered at a flow rate of 3.0 ml/min. Behavioral responses to changes in O2 levels were measured by exposing animals to a stimulus train of 7% O2 - 21% O2 - 7% O2 (upshift) or 21% O2 - 7% O2 - 21% O2 (downshift), in which each stimulus comprised a 3 min time interval. Locomotory responses to CO2 were measured by exposing animals to a series of 0% CO2 (3 min) - X% CO2 (3 min) - 0% CO2 (3 min), with X corresponding to 1%, 3%, 5% or 10% CO2 depending on the experiment. In all CO2 assays, a background level of 7% O2 was used. Movies were recorded during the stimulus train using FlyCapture (Point Grey Research) on a Leica MZ6 dissecting microscope with a Point Grey Grasshopper camera running at 2 frames/s. Video recording was started 2 min after animals were placed under the PDMS chamber to ensure that the initial environment was in a steady state. In assays where we prolonged the exposure to 7% O2 before CO2 stimulation, video recording was started 21 min after animals were placed under the PDMS chamber kept at 7% O2, and animals were stimulated with 3% CO2 at t = 24 min. Videos were analyzed in Zentracker, a custom-written MATLAB software (https://github.com/wormtracker/zentracker). All worms in the field of view were analyzed except those in contact with other animals. Speed was calculated as instantaneous centroid displacement between successive frames. Omega turns were identified as described (Laurent et al., 2015). In total 2-4 assay plates with 20-30 animals per plate were tested per day, and each genotype or condition was assayed in at least two independent experiments. As locomotion measurements were conducted using an automated algorithm, genotypes were not blinded prior to analysis.

Aggregation and bordering behavior

L4 animals were picked to a fresh plate 24 h before the assay. Sixty animals were then repicked to the assay plate (an NGM plate seeded 2 days earlier with 100 μL of E. coli OP50), and bordering and aggregation was scored 2 and 6 h later. The scorer was blind to genotype. Behavior was always scored on 2-4 assay plates (each containing 60 animals) per day and tested in at least two independent experiments.

Salt-based associative learning

Gustatory plasticity was tested as described (Beets et al., 2012, Hukema et al., 2008), in a climate-controlled room set at 20°C and 40% relative humidity. Synchronized young adult hermaphrodites were grown at 25°C on culture plates seeded with E. coli OP50. Animals were collected and washed three times over a period of 15 min with chemotaxis buffer (CTX, 5 mM KH2PO4/K2HPO4 pH 6.6, 1 mM MgSO4, and 1 mM CaCl2). Mock-conditioned animals were washed in CTX buffer without NaCl, whereas NaCl-conditioned animals were washed in CTX containing 100 mM NaCl for salt conditioning. Salt chemotaxis behavior of mock- and NaCl-conditioned animals was then tested on four-quadrant plates (Falcon X plate, Becton Dickinson Labware) filled with buffered agar (2% agar, 5 mM KH2PO4/K2HPO4 pH 6.6, 1 mM MgSO4, and 1 mM CaCl2) of which two opposing pairs have been supplemented with 25 mM NaCl. Assay plates were always prepared fresh and left open to solidify and dry for 60 min. Plates were then closed and used on the same day. After the washes, 50 - 150 animals were pipetted on the intersection of the four quadrants and allowed to crawl for 10 min on the quadrant plate. A chemotaxis index was calculated as (n(A) – n(C)) / (n(A) + n(C)) where n(A) is the number of worms within the quadrants containing NaCl and n(C) is the number of worms within the control quadrants without NaCl. The scorer was blind to genotype.

Selection-based QTL mapping

Competition assays

The C. elegans strains JU1249 and JU2825 were competed for several generations using different transfer methods. At the start of the assay, ten JU1249 and JU2825 L4 larvae were put together on a 10 cm NGM plate seeded with E. coli OP50. Five biological replicates were maintained at 23°C. Before the cultures starved, a small fraction of the population (200 to 400 animals) was used to seed a fresh culture plate. In Treatment A, the worms were harvested with M9 buffer and 2 μL of worm pellet was transferred to the next plate. In Treatment B, an agar cube (chunk) was cut at the edge of the bacterial lawn and deposited onto the next plate. After each transfer, the remaining animals were stored in M9 buffer at −80°C to quantify the relative proportions of JU1249 and JU2825 alleles.

The genomes of JU1249 and JU2825 were sequenced on an Illumina Hiseq4000 at 20x coverage with paired-end 150 bp reads. For each genome, the raw data were aligned to the reference genome (C. elegans WS243 masked from http://wormbase.org) and analyzed using BWA, SAMtools, Picard and Genome Analysis Toolkit (GATK) (Li and Durbin, 2009, Li et al., 2009, Van der Auwera et al., 2013). The accession number for the genomic sequence data of JU1249 and JU2825 is NCBI: PRJNA514933 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/genome/?term=PRJNA514933).

From the output BAM files, homozygous SNPs between the two strains were called and filtered with a raw read depth threshold of 10-300. Allele quantification for the III_663310 polymorphism was performed using pyrosequencing as previously described (Duveau and Félix, 2012). Primers for pyrosequencing are listed in Table S2. In brief, C. elegans samples harvested after each transfer were centrifuged at 3,000 rpm for 2 min. Lysates of 2 μL of the worm pellets were used as PCR templates and allele frequencies were quantified with a pyrosequencer (PyroMark Q96 ID; Biotage). The accuracy of this quantification method was estimated by measuring the allele frequencies of PCR products that were amplified using C. elegans lysates of known proportions of JU1249 and JU2825 individual L4 larvae. On average, a 2% difference was measured between expected and observed allele frequencies.

Selection-based QTL mapping experiment

Segregating populations were generated by crossing the parental JU1249 and JU2825 C. elegans wild isolates in both directions, using ten males and two self-sperm exhausted hermaphrodites in each cross. From the F1 progeny, eight biological replicates were set up to generate F2 by crossing again ten males and two self-sperm exhausted hermaphrodites. From each F2 replicate, six males and two L4 stage hermaphrodites were crossed to have plenty of F3 progeny. In the F3 generation, two paired founding populations of 200 L4 larvae (100 from each initial cross direction) were set up per replicate and submitted to contrasted selection regimes. Treatment A transferred worms through liquid harvest and Treatment B by chunking, as described for the competition assay above. In both Treatments, 200-400 animals were transferred before starvation. Males were maintained in the population during each of the first five transfers by picking 50 males. In total 19-20 transfers were done for populations under Treatment A and 17-19 transfers for populations with Treatment B. Genomic DNA of each population (about 105 individuals) was extracted as a pool and sequenced as described above.

The reads of each pool were aligned to the N2 reference genome as described above. The BAM files were filtered for allele information on the positions of homozygous SNPs between the two parents. Allele frequencies were analyzed in each pool. A Cochran-Mantel-Haenszel (CMH) test was used to analyze the consistency of the allele frequency difference between populations with different treatments among the eight replicates, except in the genomic positions 4396879-16406352 on Chromosome IV, where replicate 3 was excluded because one parental genome was fixed in this region in both treatments (McDonald, 2009). The null hypothesis for this CMH test is an equal distribution of sequence reads between the two treatments, and does not consider noise due to allelic drift in the populations, thus inflating the -log(p value). Drift could not be simulated because, for experimental simplicity, population sizes and generations were not controlled during the transfers. We note that, although population size in the experiment was low, the mapping had a relatively good resolution due to the number of populations (16 in total), which yielded independent recombination events.

RStudio (v 0.99.903) and packages (ggplot2, plyr, evobiR) were used for statistical analysis, plots of allele frequencies and CMH tests. Pindel (Ye et al., 2009) was used to detect homozygous indels in the candidate region between JU1249 and JU2825, but no additional polymorphism was found. High quality homozygous variants of the parental strains in the candidate region were annotated using VEP (http://www.ensembl.org//useast.ensembl.org/info/docs/tools/vep/index.html?redirectsrc=//www.ensembl.org%2Finfo%2Fdocs%2Ftools%2Fvep%2Findex.html) (McLaren et al., 2016). The mfP22 deletion was verified by PCR and Sanger sequencing, using primers listed in Table S2. The accession number for the genomic sequence data of the replicate populations is NCBI: PRJNA515248 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/genome/?term=PRJNA515248).

Distribution of mfP22 allele in wild isolates

To examine the distribution of the mfP22 deletion in C. elegans wild isolates, we monitored the presence of the deletion visually, using Tablet 1.16.09.06 (Milne et al., 2013), for 151 isotypes with whole genome sequences in the CeNDR database (Cook et al., 2016). The mfP22 allele was only found in JU1249 (Data S2).

Confocal microscopy and image analysis

Confocal images were acquired using a Zeiss LSM 710 microscope or a Nikon Eclipse Ti inverted setup coupled to an Andor Ixon EMCCD camera and a spinning disk confocal unit. Projections of z stacks were generated using Fiji (ImageJ).

Expression of arcp-1 in URX, AQR and PQR was confirmed by co-expression with a gcy-32p::mCherry transgene. Expression in BAG and AWB neurons was verified by crossing arcp-1 reporter strains with flp-17p::mCherry and str-1p::mCherry marker strains, respectively. Expression in AWC and ASE was confirmed by co-expression with ceh-36p::RFP and odr-1p::RFP transgenes. For DiI staining, animals were incubated in DiI solution (0.01 mg/ml) for 3 h and washed with M9 buffer before mounting for confocal microscopy.

To quantify the fluorescence of reporter-tagged proteins in cilia and neuron cell bodies, z stack images were taken on a spinning disk confocal microscope using a 60x lens and 100 ms exposure time. Z-projections of image stacks were generated with Fiji (ImageJ). Regions of interest (ROIs) were selected by centering a 50-pixel by 50-pixel square region over the distal dendrite or soma of the BAG neurons, respectively. All measurements were background-corrected by subtracting the mean values of a 50-pixel by 50-pixel square region drawn outside of the neuron.

To quantify the expression of neuropeptide reporters in BAG soma, z stack images were taken on a spinning disk confocal microscope using a 60x lens and 100 ms exposure time. 3D images were reconstituted using the IMARIS software package (Bitplane). GFP pixel intensities brighter than a threshold value (1000 for flp-19 and 3000 for flp-17 reporters) were cropped by creating a surface with 0.25 mm details. The mean pixel intensities inside the surface were calculated after background subtraction.

Calcium imaging

L4 animals expressing a ratiometric yellow cameleon sensor were picked 24 h before imaging. Animals were glued to agarose pads (2% agarose in M9 buffer, 1 mM CaCl2) using Dermabond tissue adhesive (Ethicon) with the nose immersed in a mix of bacterial food (E. coli OP50) and M9 buffer. To deliver gas stimuli, glued animals were placed under a microfluidic chamber with inlets connected to a PHD 2000 Infusion syringe pump (Harvard Apparatus) running at a flow rate of 2.5 ml/min. An electronic valve system placed between the syringes and the microfluidic chamber allowed switching between two different gas mixtures in a controlled manner at pre-specified time intervals. Imaging data were analyzed using Neuron Analyzer, a customwritten MATLAB program (code available at https://github.com/neuronanalyser/neuronanalyser). As measurements were conducted using an automated algorithm, genotypes were not blinded prior to analysis.

CO2-evoked Ca2+ activity

Animals expressing a flp-17p::YC3.60 (yellow cameleon 3.60) transgene were used for ratiometric imaging of relative calcium concentration in BAG cell bodies (Bretscher et al., 2011). After immobilization, animals were placed under a microfluidic PDMS chamber and exposed to a 0% CO2 (3 min) - X% CO2 (3 min) - 0% CO2 (3 min) stimulus train, with X corresponding to 1%, 3% or 5% CO2 depending on the experiment. To measure CO2-evoked tonic Ca2+ activity in BAG, the time interval for CO2 stimulation was prolonged from 3 min to 18 min. In all experiments, the background O2 level was 7% O2. Calcium imaging was done at 2 frames/s on an AZ100 microscope (Nikon) bearing a TwinCam adaptor (Cairn Research) mounted with two ORCAFlash4.0 V2 digital cameras (Hamamatsu) using an AZ Plan Fluor 2x lens with 2x zoom and an exposure time of 500 ms.

O2-evoked Ca2+ activity

We used animals expressing a gcy-37p::YC2.60 transgene to measure Ca2+ activity of URX neurons in response to O2 stimuli (Fenk and de Bono, 2017). To measure O2 responses in BAG, we used animals expressing a flp-17p::YC2.60 transgene (Gross et al., 2014). After immobilization, animals were placed under a Y-shaped microfluidic chamber and exposed to an O2 upshift (7% - 21% - 7% O2) in case of URX imaging, or an O2 downshift (21% - 7% - 21% O2) for BAG. Each stimulus comprised a 2 min time window. Images were recorded at 2 frames/s with an exposure time of 100 ms for a total of 6 min, on a Zeiss Axiovert inverted microscope with an EMCCD Evolve 512 Deltacamera (Photometrics) and a 40x C-Apochromat lens, using MetaMorph acquisition software (Molecular Devices). To reduce photobleaching an optical density filter 2.0 or 1.5 was used. Excitation light was passed through an excitation filter for CFP (438/24-25, Semrock) and a dichroic filter for YFP (DiO2-25x36, Semrock). A beam splitter (Optical Insights) was used to separate the cyan and yellow emission light using a dichroic filter for 483/32-25 nm (CFP) and 542/27-25 nm (YFP) (Semrock).

Immunoprecipitation from C. elegans

Two independent coIP experiments were performed to identify putative interactors of ARCP-1B. Samples for GFP-ARCP-1B were always processed in parallel with control samples of other cytoplasmic GFP-tagged proteins (MALT-1-GFP and EIF-3.L-GFP), providing negative controls. For coIP experiments, lysis buffer was prepared with 50 mM HEPES (pH 7.4), 1 mM EGTA, 1 mM MgCl2, 100 mM KCl, 10% glycerol, 0.05% Tergitol type-NP40 (Sigma-Aldrich), 1mM DTT, 0.1M PMSF with 1 complete EDTA-free proteinase inhibitor cocktail tablet (Roche Applied Science) per 12 ml. Worms were washed twice in ice-cold M9 and once in ice-cold lysis buffer, and then snap-frozen in liquid nitrogen. Frozen worm pellets (∼10 g) were pulverized using a Freezer/Mill (SPEX SamplePrep). Crude extract was clarified at 4°C for 10 min at 20,000 g, and again for 20 min at 100,000 g with a TLA-100 rotor (Beckman Coulter). For immunoprecipitation, samples were incubated with GFP-Trap (ChromoTek) for 4 h at 4°C, then washed 3 times with 50 mM HEPES, 100 mM KCl. Purified complexes were eluted in SDS-sample buffer at 95°C and further fractionated by SDS-PAGE prior to mass spectrometry analysis.

Proteins were identified by Orbitrap-mass spectrometry and MASCOT database searching. Gel samples were destained with 50% v/v acetonitrile and 50 mM ammonium bicarbonate, reduced with 10 mM DTT, and alkylated with 55 mM iodoacetamide. Digestion was with 6 ng/μl trypsin (Promega) overnight at 37°C, and peptides extracted in 2% v/v formic acid 2% v/v acetonitrile, and analyzed by nano-scale capillary LC-MS/MS (Ultimate U3000 HPLC, Thermo Scientific Dionex) at a flow of ∼300 nL/min. A C18 Acclaim PepMap100 5 μm, 100 μm x 20 mm nanoViper (Thermo Scientific Dionex), trapped the peptides prior to separation on a C18 Acclaim PepMap100 3 μm, 75 μm x 250 mm nanoViper. Peptides were eluted with an acetonitrile gradient. The analytical column outlet was interfaced via a nano-flow electrospray ionisation source with a linear ion trap mass spectrometer (Orbitrap Velos, Thermo Scientific). Data dependent analysis was performed using a resolution of 30,000 for the full MS spectrum, followed by ten MS/MS spectra in the linear ion trap. MS spectra were collected over a m/z range of 300–2000. MS/MS scans were collected using a threshold energy of 35 for collision-induced dissociation. LC-MS/MS data were searched against the UniProt KB database using Mascot (Matrix Science), with a precursor tolerance of 10 ppm and a fragment ion mass tolerance of 0.8 Da. Two missed enzyme cleavages and variable modifications for oxidised methionine, carbamidomethyl cysteine, pyroglutamic acid, phosphorylated serine, threonine and tyrosine were included. MS/MS data were validated using the Scaffold program (Proteome Software Inc).

RNA-seq of sorted BAG neurons

Adult cell isolation

Synchronized young adult hermaphrodites with GFP-labeled BAG neurons (expressing a flp-17p::gfp transgene) were acutely dissociated as described (Kaletsky et al., 2016). Synchronized adult worms were washed with M9 buffer to remove excess bacteria. The pellet (∼250 μl) was washed with 500 μl lysis buffer (200 mM DTT, 0.25% SDS, 20 mM HEPES pH 8.0, 3% sucrose) and resuspended in 750 μl lysis buffer. Worms were incubated in lysis buffer for 6.5 min at room temperature. The pellet was washed 5 times with M9 and resuspended in 20 mg/ml pronase from Streptomyces griseus (Roche). Worms were pipetted up and down for 12 min at room temperature; then ice-cold PBS buffer containing 2% fetal bovine serum (GIBCO) was added. Cell suspensions were passed over a 5 μm syringe filter (Millipore). The filtered cells were diluted in PBS and sorted using a Sony Biotechnology Synergy High Speed Cell Sorter. Gates for detection were set by comparison to npr-1 cell suspensions prepared on the same day alongside the experimental samples. Positive fluorescent events were sorted directly into Eppendorf tubes containing 10 μL of 0.2% (vol/vol) Triton X-100 and 2 U μl-1 RNase inhibitor. Six biological replicates were prepared for each genotype, i.e., npr-1(ad609) and arcp-1(db1082); npr-1(ad609) animals. For each replicate sample, approximately 4,000 GFP positive events were collected.

RNA amplification and library preparation

RNA-seq was done using a Smart-seq2 protocol as described (Picelli et al., 2014). After neuron isolation by FACS, cDNA was prepared from each sample by reverse transcription using SuperScript II reverse transcriptase (18064-014, Invitrogen), Oligo-dT30 and Template-Switching Oligonucleotide (TSO) primers listed in Table S2. After the first strand reaction, the cDNA was amplified with the KAPA Hifi HotStart kit (KK2601, KAPA Biosystems) and IS PCR primers listed in Table S2. cDNA was then purified using Ampure XP beads (A 63881, Beckman Coulter), tagmented and 1 μg was used for preparing libraries with the Illumina Nextera XT DNA sample preparation kit (FC-131-1096, Illumina), as per manufacturer suggested practices. Sequencing libraries were then submitted for sequencing on the Illumina HiSeq 4000 platform.

RNA-seq data analysis

Prior to analysis the raw files were pre-processed using Bowtie2 version 0.11.0 to remove ribosomal RNA that mapped to a ribosomal RNA sequence library (Wormbase, WS255). Additionally, FASTQ files relating to the same sample but sequenced over multiple flow cell lanes were concatenated to give a single file. Custom rRNA_remover and rna_seq_lane_merger scripts were used (available on GitHub: https://github.com/lmb-seq/RNA-Seq_utilities). The files were then processed by PRAGUI - a Python3 pipeline for RNA-seq data analysis. PRAGUI automates analysis by incorporating widely used RNA-seq processing packages including: Trim Galore, FastQC, STAR, DESeq2, HTSeq, Cufflinks and MultiQC. PRAGUI can be found at: https://github.com/lmb-seq/PRAGUI. The following parameters were used with PRAGUI: DESeq2 analysis (labeled as “DESeq”), unstranded paired-end library, worm organism with C. elegans genome fasta file and canonical gene set gtf file (Wormbase, WS255), STAR arguments set to “–outSAMstrandFieldDESeq intronMotif–readFilesCommand zcat -c–outSAMtype BAM SortedByCoordinate,” mapq set to 20. All other PRAGUI parameters were kept default. 5.5 − 17 million reads were obtained per sample and mapped to the C. elegans genome. Sequences are deposited at GEO (GSE135687).

Quantification and Statistical Analysis

The number of animals and replicates used per experiment is described in detail in the ‘‘Methods Details’’ subsection for each assay and in the relevant Figure legends. Specifically, for the main behavioral assays: locomotory responses to CO2 and O2 were measured in >4 trials per condition with 20-30 animals each; aggregation and bordering assays were conducted with >4 trials per genotype of 50 - 60 animals each.

Statistical analyses used GraphPad Prism 7.0 and Mathworks MATLAB R2014b (8.4). Exact tests used are indicated in figure legends. In general, where more than two groups tested with a single condition were compared, a one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s or Šidák’s multiple comparisons test was used. Where multiple groups tested with multiple conditions were compared, a two-way ANOVA with Tukey’s or Šidák’s post hoc test was used. Where appropriate, a D’Agostino & Pearson or Shapiro-Wilk normality test was conducted to assess if the data fit a normal distribution. For locomotory assays where two groups were compared over one time interval, we chose time intervals where we expected the locomotory changes to have plateaued and used a Mann-Whitney u test for statistical comparisons as described (Laurent et al., 2015). For the intervals of interest, we determined independent per-subject means derived from individuals flagged as continuously valid for at least 10 s during the interval. We considered all individuals in the field of view as valid except those in contact with other animals and those that were off the food lawn or less than half a body-length from the border. Following these criteria, each individual was sampled at most once per interval.

Data and Software Availability

Datasets

The genome sequencing data of JU1249 and JU2825 is available on NCBI: PRJNA514933 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/genome/?term=PRJNA514933). The genomic sequence data of the replicate populations for QTL mapping is available on NCBI: PRJNA515248 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/genome/?term=PRJNA515248). Sequence data from the RNA-Seq analysis of sorted BAG neurons is deposited on GEO: GSE135687 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/geo/query/acc.cgi?acc=GSE135687).

Codes

Locomotory assays

Videos of locomotory assays were analyzed in Zentracker, a custom-written MATLAB software available on https://github.com/wormtracker/zentracker.

Calcium imaging

Recordings were analyzed using Neuron Analyzer, a customwritten MATLAB program available at https://github.com/neuronanalyser/neuronanalyser.

RNA-seq analysis

Codes for removing rRNA sequences from datasets and for concatenating FASTQ files relating to the same sample but sequenced over multiple flow cell lanes, are available on GitHub: https://github.com/lmb-seq/RNA-Seq_utilities. The git repository for PRAGUI can be found at: https://github.com/lmb-seq/PRAGUI.

Acknowledgments

We thank Roger Pocock (Monash University, Australia), Niels Ringstad (New York University School of Medicine, USA), and the Caenorhabditis Genetics Center, funded by NIH Office of Research Infrastructure Programs (P40 OD010440), for strains. We gratefully acknowledge Tim Stevens, Gurpreet Ghattaoraya, and members of the de Bono and Schafer labs for advice and comments. This work was funded by an Advanced Grant from the European Research Council to M.d.B (ACMO 269058) and by the Medical Research Council (United Kingdom). I.B. received an EMBO Long-term Fellowship (ALTF 387-2015) and EU Marie-Curie Individual Fellowship (MSCA-IF 704383) and is a fellow of the Research Foundation—Flanders (FWO). Work in the Félix lab was funded by a grant from the Fondation pour la Recherche Médicale (DEQ20150331704). G.Z. was funded in part by a fellowship from the China Scholarship Council.

Author Contributions

I.B., G.Z., M.-A.F., and M.d.B. designed experiments. I.B., G.Z., L.A.F., and C.C. performed experiments. I.B., G.Z., M.-A.F., and M.d.B. analyzed the data. G.Z. and G.N. did the genome sequence data analyses. I.B., M.-A.F., and M.d.B. wrote the manuscript.

Declaration of Interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Published: November 19, 2019

Footnotes

Supplemental Information can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2019.10.001.

Contributor Information

Marie-Anne Félix, Email: felix@biologie.ens.fr.

Mario de Bono, Email: mdebono@ist.ac.at.

Supporting Citations

The following references appear in the Supplemental Information: de Bono and Bargmann (1998); Félix and Duveau, 2012, Kiontke et al., 2011; Richaud et al. (2018).

Supplemental Information

Document S1. Figures S1–S7 and Tables S1 and S2
mmc1.pdf (39.3MB, pdf)
Data S1. Data Used for Selection-Based QTL Mapping, Related to Figure 2

A) List of single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) between JU1249 and JU2825. B) Number of reads in the eight replicates of treatment A, by SNP. C) Number of reads in the eight replicates of treatment B, by SNP. D) Allele frequencies in the eight replicates of treatment A, by SNP. Data in red were used to generate Figure S4C. E) Allele frequencies in the eight replicates of treatment B, by SNP. Data in red were used to generate Figure S4C. F) Allele frequencies in the eight replicates of treatment A, displayed using a sliding window 10 SNPs wide and a step size of one SNP for all chromosomes except chromosome IV, where we used a sliding window 100 SNPs wide and a step size of one SNP. G) Allele frequencies in the eight replicates of treatment B, displayed using a sliding window 10 SNPs wide and a step size of one SNP for all chromosomes except chromosome IV, where we used a sliding window 100 SNPs wide and a step size of one SNP. H) Differences in allele frequencies between treatment A and treatment B in each of the eight replicates, displayed using a sliding window 5 SNPs wide and a step size of one SNP for all chromosomes, except a sliding window 100 SNPs wide and a step size of one SNP for chromosome IV. In each pair of replicates, the same genomic positions were first selected between treatment A and treatment B, before subtracting allele frequencies between treatments. I) Read statistics used for the CMH analysis. The same genomic positions were first selected for both treatments among all the replicates, before the CMH analysis. The part in gray on chromosome IV for replicate 3 was not used in the test as one parental allele was fixed in both treatments. J) Annotation of variants detected in JU1249 compared to the reference N2, using the VEP algorithm. The F34D10.6 deletion (in red) appears as the only high impact variation. K) Annotation of variants detected in JU2825 compared to the reference N2.

mmc2.xlsx (36.8MB, xlsx)
Data S2. Distribution of the mfP22 Deletion, Related to Figure 2

List of wild isolates where the mfP22 deletion is absent, based on mapped sequence reads available at the Caenorhabditis elegans Natural Diversity Resource, http://elegansvariation.org (Cook et al., 2016).

mmc3.xlsx (10.6KB, xlsx)
Data S3. Protein Identifications from CoIP of GFP-ARCP-1B, Related to Figure 5

Proteins identified by mass spectrometry in two independent coIP experiments for interactors of GFP-ARCP-1B. IP of GFP-tagged cytoplasmic proteins (MALT-1-GFP and EIF-3.L-GFP) provided a negative control. Total spectrum counts in GFP-ARCP-1B and control samples are listed for proteins that were at least 3-fold enriched in the GFP-ARCP-1B sample in both experiments.

mmc4.xlsx (41.6KB, xlsx)
Data S4. Expression Profiling of BAG Neurons in npr-1 and arcp-1; npr-1 Animals Using RNA-Seq, Related to Figure 6

A-D) Genes expressed in BAG neurons, which were isolated by FACS from adult npr-1 and arcp-1; npr-1 animals, with six biological replicates per genotype. A-B) Values indicate transcripts per kilobase million (TPM). C-D) Values show fragments per kilobase million (FPKM). Genes are listed based upon an expression detection threshold of 1 count per million reads per gene in at least 6 samples. E) Genes differentially expressed in BAG neurons of npr-1 and arcp-1; npr-1 animals.

mmc5.xlsx (4.6MB, xlsx)
Document S2. Article plus Supplemental Information
mmc6.pdf (43.2MB, pdf)

References

  1. Arora K., Sinha C., Zhang W., Ren A., Moon C.S., Yarlagadda S., Naren A.P. Compartmentalization of cyclic nucleotide signaling: a question of when, where, and why? Pflugers Arch. 2013;465:1397–1407. doi: 10.1007/s00424-013-1280-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bargmann C.I. Beyond the connectome: how neuromodulators shape neural circuits. BioEssays. 2012;34:458–465. doi: 10.1002/bies.201100185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Beets I., Janssen T., Meelkop E., Temmerman L., Suetens N., Rademakers S., Jansen G., Schoofs L. Vasopressin/oxytocin-related signaling regulates gustatory associative learning in C. elegans. Science. 2012;338:543–545. doi: 10.1126/science.1226860. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bretscher A.J., Kodama-Namba E., Busch K.E., Murphy R.J., Soltesz Z., Laurent P., de Bono M. Temperature, oxygen, and salt-sensing neurons in C. elegans are carbon dioxide sensors that control avoidance behavior. Neuron. 2011;69:1099–1113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Busch K.E., Laurent P., Soltesz Z., Murphy R.J., Faivre O., Hedwig B., Thomas M., Smith H.L., de Bono M. Tonic signaling from O2 sensors sets neural circuit activity and behavioral state. Nat. Neurosci. 2012;15:581–591. doi: 10.1038/nn.3061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Carrillo M.A., Hallem E.A. Gas sensing in nematodes. Mol. Neurobiol. 2015;51:919–931. doi: 10.1007/s12035-014-8748-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Carrillo M.A., Guillermin M.L., Rengarajan S., Okubo R.P., Hallem E.A. O2-sensing neurons control CO2 response in C. elegans. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:9675–9683. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4541-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cook D.E., Zdraljevic S., Roberts J.P., Andersen E.C. CeNDR, the Caenorhabditis elegans natural diversity resource. Nucleic Acids Res. 2016;45(D1):D650–D657. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkw893. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Coppens C.M., de Boer S.F., Koolhaas J.M. Coping styles and behavioural flexibility: towards underlying mechanisms. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2010;365:4021–4028. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Couto A., Oda S., Nikolaev V.O., Soltesz Z., de Bono M. In vivo genetic dissection of O2-evoked cGMP dynamics in a Caenorhabditis elegans gas sensor. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2013;110:E3301–E3310. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1217428110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Crispo E. The Baldwin effect and genetic assimilation: revisiting two mechanisms of evolutionary change mediated by phenotypic plasticity. Evolution. 2007;61:2469–2479. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00203.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cummins E.P., Selfridge A.C., Sporn P.H., Sznajder J.I., Taylor C.T. Carbon dioxide-sensing in organisms and its implications for human disease. Cell. Mol. Life Sci. 2014;71:831–845. doi: 10.1007/s00018-013-1470-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cygnar K.D., Zhao H. Phosphodiesterase 1C is dispensable for rapid response termination of olfactory sensory neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 2009;12:454–462. doi: 10.1038/nn.2289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. de Bono M., Bargmann C.I. Natural variation in a neuropeptide Y receptor homolog modifies social behavior and food response in C. elegans. Cell. 1998;94:679–689. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)81609-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Dewitt T.J., Sih A., Wilson D.S. Costs and limits of phenotypic plasticity. Trends Ecol. Evol. 1998;13:77–81. doi: 10.1016/s0169-5347(97)01274-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dingemanse N.J., Wolf M. Between-individual differences in behavioural plasticity within populations: causes and consequences. Anim. Behav. 2013;85:1031–1039. [Google Scholar]
  17. Donaldson Z.R., Young L.J. Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the neurogenetics of sociality. Science. 2008;322:900–904. doi: 10.1126/science.1158668. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Duveau F., Félix M.A. Role of pleiotropy in the evolution of a cryptic developmental variation in Caenorhabditis elegans. PLoS Biol. 2012;10:e1001230. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001230. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Félix M.A., Duveau F. Population dynamics and habitat sharing of natural populations of Caenorhabditis elegans and C. briggsae. BMC Biol. 2012;10:59. doi: 10.1186/1741-7007-10-59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fenk L.A., de Bono M. Environmental CO2 inhibits Caenorhabditis elegans egg-laying by modulating olfactory neurons and evokes widespread changes in neural activity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2015;112:E3525–E3534. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1423808112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fenk L.A., de Bono M. Memory of recent oxygen experience switches pheromone valence in Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2017;114:4195–4200. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1618934114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Frézal L., Demoinet E., Braendle C., Miska E., Félix M.A. Natural genetic variation in a multigenerational phenotype in C. elegans. Curr. Biol. 2018;28:2588–2596. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.05.091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gopal R., Foster K.W., Yang P. The DPY-30 domain and its flanking sequence mediate the assembly and modulation of flagellar radial spoke complexes. Mol. Cell. Biol. 2012;32:4012–4024. doi: 10.1128/MCB.06602-11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gray J.M., Karow D.S., Lu H., Chang A.J., Chang J.S., Ellis R.E., Marletta M.A., Bargmann C.I. Oxygen sensation and social feeding mediated by a C. elegans guanylate cyclase homologue. Nature. 2004;430:317–322. doi: 10.1038/nature02714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gross E., Soltesz Z., Oda S., Zelmanovich V., Abergel Z., de Bono M. GLOBIN-5-dependent O2 responses are regulated by PDL-1/PrBP that targets prenylated soluble guanylate cyclases to dendritic endings. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:16726–16738. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5368-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Grubb M.S., Burrone J. Activity-dependent relocation of the axon initial segment fine-tunes neuronal excitability. Nature. 2010;465:1070–1074. doi: 10.1038/nature09160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Guerenstein P.G., Hildebrand J.G. Roles and effects of environmental carbon dioxide in insect life. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 2008;53:161–178. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ento.53.103106.093402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Guillermin M.L., Castelletto M.L., Hallem E.A. Differentiation of carbon dioxide-sensing neurons in Caenorhabditis elegans requires the ETS-5 transcription factor. Genetics. 2011;189:1327–1339. doi: 10.1534/genetics.111.133835. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Guillermin M.L., Carrillo M.A., Hallem E.A. A single set of interneurons drives opposite behaviors in C. elegans. Curr. Biol. 2017;27:2630–2639. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.07.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hallem E.A., Sternberg P.W. Acute carbon dioxide avoidance in Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2008;105:8038–8043. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0707469105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hallem E.A., Spencer W.C., McWhirter R.D., Zeller G., Henz S.R., Rätsch G., Miller D.M., 3rd, Horvitz H.R., Sternberg P.W., Ringstad N. Receptor-type guanylate cyclase is required for carbon dioxide sensation by Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2011;108:254–259. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1017354108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hukema R.K., Rademakers S., Jansen G. Gustatory plasticity in C. elegans involves integration of negative cues and NaCl taste mediated by serotonin, dopamine, and glutamate. Learn. Mem. 2008;15:829–836. doi: 10.1101/lm.994408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Izquierdo A., Newman T.K., Higley J.D., Murray E.A. Genetic modulation of cognitive flexibility and socioemotional behavior in rhesus monkeys. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2007;104:14128–14133. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0706583104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Jékely G., Melzer S., Beets I., Kadow I.C.G., Koene J., Haddad S., Holden-Dye L. The long and the short of it - a perspective on peptidergic regulation of circuits and behaviour. J. Exp. Biol. 2018;221:jeb166710–jeb166714. doi: 10.1242/jeb.166710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Jones S.L., Svitkina T.M. Axon initial segment cytoskeleton: architecture, development, and role in neuron polarity. Neural Plast. 2016;2016:6808293. doi: 10.1155/2016/6808293. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kaletsky R., Lakhina V., Arey R., Williams A., Landis J., Ashraf J., Murphy C.T. The C. elegans adult neuronal IIS/FOXO transcriptome reveals adult phenotype regulators. Nature. 2016;529:92–96. doi: 10.1038/nature16483. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kim K., Li C. Expression and regulation of an FMRFamide-related neuropeptide gene family in Caenorhabditis elegans. J. Comp. Neurol. 2004;475:540–550. doi: 10.1002/cne.20189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kiontke K.C., Félix M.A., Ailion M., Rockman M.V., Braendle C., Pénigault J.B., Fitch D.H.A. A phylogeny and molecular barcodes for Caenorhabditis, with numerous new species from rotting fruits. BMC Evol. Biol. 2011;11:339. doi: 10.1186/1471-2148-11-339. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kizhatil K., Baker S.A., Arshavsky V.Y., Bennett V. Ankyrin-G promotes cyclic nucleotide-gated channel transport to rod photoreceptor sensory cilia. Science. 2009;323:1614–1617. doi: 10.1126/science.1169789. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kodama-Namba E., Fenk L.A., Bretscher A.J., Gross E., Busch K.E., de Bono M. Cross-modulation of homeostatic responses to temperature, oxygen and carbon dioxide in C. elegans. PLoS Genet. 2013;9:e1004011. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1004011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Langmead B., Salzberg S.L. Fast gapped-read alignment with Bowtie 2. Nat. Methods. 2012;9:357–359. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1923. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Laurent P., Soltesz Z., Nelson G.M., Chen C., Arellano-Carbajal F., Levy E., de Bono M. Decoding a neural circuit controlling global animal state in C. elegans. eLife. 2015;4 doi: 10.7554/eLife.04241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Leterrier C., Clerc N., Rueda-Boroni F., Montersino A., Dargent B., Castets F. Ankyrin G membrane partners drive the establishment and maintenance of the axon initial segment. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 2017;11:6. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2017.00006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Li H., Durbin R. Fast and accurate short read alignment with Burrows-Wheeler transform. Bioinformatics. 2009;25:1754–1760. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btp324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Li H., Handsaker B., Wysoker A., Fennell T., Ruan J., Homer N., Marth G., Abecasis G., Durbin R., 1000 Genome Project Data Processing Subgroup The sequence alignment/map format and SAMtools. Bioinformatics. 2009;25:2078–2079. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btp352. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Maniar T.A., Kaplan M., Wang G.J., Shen K., Wei L., Shaw J.E., Koushika S.P., Bargmann C.I. UNC-33 (CRMP) and ankyrin organize microtubules and localize kinesin to polarize axon-dendrite sorting. Nat. Neurosci. 2011;15:48–56. doi: 10.1038/nn.2970. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Martínez-Velázquez L.A., Ringstad N. Antagonistic regulation of trafficking to Caenorhabditis elegans sensory cilia by a Retinal Degeneration 3 homolog and retromer. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2018;115:E438–E447. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1712302115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. McDonald J.H. Sparky House Publishing; 2009. Handbook of biological statistics. [Google Scholar]
  49. McGrath P.T. Varieties of behavioral natural variation. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2013;23:24–28. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. McGrath P.T., Rockman M.V., Zimmer M., Jang H., Macosko E.Z., Kruglyak L., Bargmann C.I. Quantitative mapping of a digenic behavioral trait implicates globin variation in C. elegans sensory behaviors. Neuron. 2009;61:692–699. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.02.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. McLaren W., Gil L., Hunt S.E., Riat H.S., Ritchie G.R.S., Thormann A., Flicek P., Cunningham F. The Ensembl variant effect predictor. Genome Biol. 2016;17:122. doi: 10.1186/s13059-016-0974-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Mery F. Natural variation in learning and memory. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2013;23:52–56. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Mery F., Belay A.T., So A.K.C., Sokolowski M.B., Kawecki T.J. Natural polymorphism affecting learning and memory in Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2007;104:13051–13055. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0702923104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Milne I., Stephen G., Bayer M., Cock P.J.A., Pritchard L., Cardle L., Shaw P.D., Marshall D. Using Tablet for visual exploration of second-generation sequencing data. Brief. Bioinform. 2013;14:193–202. doi: 10.1093/bib/bbs012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Minevich G., Park D.S., Blankenberg D., Poole R.J., Hobert O. CloudMap: a cloud-based pipeline for analysis of mutant genome sequences. Genetics. 2012;192:1249–1269. doi: 10.1534/genetics.112.144204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Monteiro P., Feng G. SHANK proteins: roles at the synapse and in autism spectrum disorder. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2017;18:147–157. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Niemelä P.T., Vainikka A., Forsman J.T., Loukola O.J., Kortet R. How does variation in the environment and individual cognition explain the existence of consistent behavioral differences? Ecol. Evol. 2013;3:457–464. doi: 10.1002/ece3.451. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. O’Halloran D.M., Hamilton O.S., Lee J.I., Gallegos M., L’Etoile N.D. Changes in cGMP levels affect the localization of EGL-4 in AWC in Caenorhabditis elegans. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e31614. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0031614. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Otsuka A.J., Franco R., Yang B., Shim K.H., Tang L.Z., Zhang Y.Y., Boontrakulpoontawee P., Jeyaprakash A., Hedgecock E., Wheaton V.I. An ankyrin-related gene (unc-44) is necessary for proper axonal guidance in Caenorhabditis elegans. J. Cell Biol. 1995;129:1081–1092. doi: 10.1083/jcb.129.4.1081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Owen G.R., Brenner E.A. Mapping molecular memory: navigating the cellular pathways of learning. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 2012;32:919–941. doi: 10.1007/s10571-012-9836-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Persson A., Gross E., Laurent P., Busch K.E., Bretes H., de Bono M. Natural variation in a neural globin tunes oxygen sensing in wild Caenorhabditis elegans. Nature. 2009;458:1030–1033. doi: 10.1038/nature07820. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Peymen K., Watteyne J., Frooninckx L., Schoofs L., Beets I. The FMRFamide-like peptide family in nematodes. Front. Endocrinol. (Lausanne) 2014;5:90. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2014.00090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Picelli S., Faridani O.R., Björklund A.K., Winberg G., Sagasser S., Sandberg R. Full-length RNA-seq from single cells using Smart-seq2. Nat. Protoc. 2014;9:171–181. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2014.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Pigliucci M., Murren C.J., Schlichting C.D. Phenotypic plasticity and evolution by genetic assimilation. J. Exp. Biol. 2006;209:2362–2367. doi: 10.1242/jeb.02070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Pline M., Dusenbery D.B. Responses of plant-parasitic nematodeMeloidogyne incognita to carbon dioxide determined by video camera-computer tracking. J. Chem. Ecol. 1987;13:873–888. doi: 10.1007/BF01020167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Prabhakar N.R., Semenza G.L. Oxygen sensing and homeostasis. Physiology (Bethesda) 2015;30:340–348. doi: 10.1152/physiol.00022.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Prieto-Godino L.L., Rytz R., Cruchet S., Bargeton B., Abuin L., Silbering A.F., Ruta V., Dal Peraro M., Benton R. Evolution of acid-sensing olfactory circuits in Drosophilids. Neuron. 2017;93:661–676. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.12.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. R Development Core Team . R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2015. R: a language and environment for Statistical Computing.https://www.r-project.org/ [Google Scholar]
  69. Renn S.C.P., Schumer M.E. Genetic accommodation and behavioural evolution: insights from genomic studies. Anim. Behav. 2013;85:1012–1022. [Google Scholar]
  70. Richaud A., Zhang G., Lee D., Lee J., Félix M.A. The local coexistence pattern of selfing genotypes in Caenorhabditis elegans natural metapopulations. Genetics. 2018;208:807–821. doi: 10.1534/genetics.117.300564. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Romanos T.R., Petersen J.G., Pocock R. Control of neuropeptide expression by parallel activity-dependent pathways in Caenorhabditis elegans. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:38734. doi: 10.1038/srep38734. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Sachse S., Rueckert E., Keller A., Okada R., Tanaka N.K., Ito K., Vosshall L.B. Activity-dependent plasticity in an olfactory circuit. Neuron. 2007;56:838–850. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Schindelin J., Arganda-Carreras I., Frise E., Kaynig V., Longair M., Pietzsch T., Preibisch S., Rueden C., Saalfeld S., Schmid B. Fiji: an open-source platform for biological-image analysis. Nat. Methods. 2012;9:676–682. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Schmalhausen I.I. Blakiston; 1949. Factors of Evolution. [Google Scholar]
  75. Sivadas P., Dienes J.M., St Maurice M., Meek W.D., Yang P. A flagellar A-kinase anchoring protein with two amphipathic helices forms a structural scaffold in the radial spoke complex. J. Cell Biol. 2012;199:639–651. doi: 10.1083/jcb.201111042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Smith E.S., Martinez-Velazquez L., Ringstad N. A chemoreceptor that detects molecular carbon dioxide. J. Biol. Chem. 2013;288:37071–37081. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M113.517367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Smith K.R., Kopeikina K.J., Fawcett-Patel J.M., Leaderbrand K., Gao R., Schürmann B., Myczek K., Radulovic J., Swanson G.T., Penzes P. Psychiatric risk factor ANK3/ankyrin-G nanodomains regulate the structure and function of glutamatergic synapses. Neuron. 2014;84:399–415. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.10.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Stiernagle T. WormBook; 2006. Maintenance of C. elegans.http://wormbase.org [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Taghert P.H., Nitabach M.N. Peptide neuromodulation in invertebrate model systems. Neuron. 2012;76:82–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Thompson O., Edgley M., Strasbourger P., Flibotte S., Ewing B., Adair R., Au V., Chaudhry I., Fernando L., Hutter H. The million mutation project: a new approach to genetics in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genome Res. 2013;23:1749–1762. doi: 10.1101/gr.157651.113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Troemel E.R., Félix M.A., Whiteman N.K., Barrière A., Ausubel F.M. Microsporidia are natural intracellular parasites of the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. PLoS Biol. 2008;6:2736–2752. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Tursun B., Cochella L., Carrera I., Hobert O. A toolkit and robust pipeline for the generation of fosmid-based reporter genes in C. elegans. PLoS ONE. 2009;4:e4625. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004625. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Van der Auwera G.A., Carneiro M.O., Hartl C., Poplin R., Del Angel G., Levy-Moonshine A., Jordan T., Shakir K., Roazen D., Thibault J. From FastQ data to high confidence variant calls: the Genome Analysis Toolkit best practices pipeline. Curr. Protoc. Bioinformatics. 2013;43:11.10.1-33. doi: 10.1002/0471250953.bi1110s43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Viglierchio D.R. Carbon dioxide sensing by Panagrellus silusiae and Ditylenchus dipsaci. Revue De Nematologie. 1990;13:425–432. [Google Scholar]
  85. Vulesevic B., McNeill B., Perry S.F. Chemoreceptor plasticity and respiratory acclimation in the zebrafish Danio rerio. J. Exp. Biol. 2006;209:1261–1273. doi: 10.1242/jeb.02058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Waddington C.H. Canalization of development and the inheritance of acquired characters. Nature. 1942;150:563–565. [Google Scholar]
  87. Waddington C.H. Genetic assimilation of an acquired character. Evolution. 1953;7:118–126. [Google Scholar]
  88. Weber K.P., De S., Kozarewa I., Turner D.J., Babu M.M., de Bono M. Whole genome sequencing highlights genetic changes associated with laboratory domestication of C. elegans. PLoS ONE. 2010;5:e13922. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013922. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. West-Eberhard M.J. Developmental plasticity and the origin of species differences. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2005;102(Suppl 1):6543–6549. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0501844102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Ye K., Schulz M.H., Long Q., Apweiler R., Ning Z. Pindel: a pattern growth approach to detect break points of large deletions and medium sized insertions from paired-end short reads. Bioinformatics. 2009;25:2865–2871. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btp394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Yin J.A., Gao G., Liu X.J., Hao Z.Q., Li K., Kang X.L., Li H., Shan Y.H., Hu W.L., Li H.P., Cai S.Q. Genetic variation in glia-neuron signalling modulates ageing rate. Nature. 2017;551:198–203. doi: 10.1038/nature24463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Yu S., Avery L., Baude E., Garbers D.L. Guanylyl cyclase expression in specific sensory neurons: a new family of chemosensory receptors. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1997;94:3384–3387. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.7.3384. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Zhang Y., Bertolino A., Fazio L., Blasi G., Rampino A., Romano R., Lee M.L.T., Xiao T., Papp A., Wang D., Sadée W. Polymorphisms in human dopamine D2 receptor gene affect gene expression, splicing, and neuronal activity during working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2007;104:20552–20557. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0707106104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Zhang G., Sachse M., Prevost M.C., Luallen R.J., Troemel E.R., Félix M.A. A Large Collection of Novel Nematode-Infecting Microsporidia and Their Diverse Interactions with Caenorhabditis elegans and Other Related Nematodes. PLoS Pathog. 2016;12:e1006093. doi: 10.1371/journal.ppat.1006093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Zimmer M., Gray J.M., Pokala N., Chang A.J., Karow D.S., Marletta M.A., Hudson M.L., Morton D.B., Chronis N., Bargmann C.I. Neurons detect increases and decreases in oxygen levels using distinct guanylate cyclases. Neuron. 2009;61:865–879. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.02.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Document S1. Figures S1–S7 and Tables S1 and S2
mmc1.pdf (39.3MB, pdf)
Data S1. Data Used for Selection-Based QTL Mapping, Related to Figure 2

A) List of single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) between JU1249 and JU2825. B) Number of reads in the eight replicates of treatment A, by SNP. C) Number of reads in the eight replicates of treatment B, by SNP. D) Allele frequencies in the eight replicates of treatment A, by SNP. Data in red were used to generate Figure S4C. E) Allele frequencies in the eight replicates of treatment B, by SNP. Data in red were used to generate Figure S4C. F) Allele frequencies in the eight replicates of treatment A, displayed using a sliding window 10 SNPs wide and a step size of one SNP for all chromosomes except chromosome IV, where we used a sliding window 100 SNPs wide and a step size of one SNP. G) Allele frequencies in the eight replicates of treatment B, displayed using a sliding window 10 SNPs wide and a step size of one SNP for all chromosomes except chromosome IV, where we used a sliding window 100 SNPs wide and a step size of one SNP. H) Differences in allele frequencies between treatment A and treatment B in each of the eight replicates, displayed using a sliding window 5 SNPs wide and a step size of one SNP for all chromosomes, except a sliding window 100 SNPs wide and a step size of one SNP for chromosome IV. In each pair of replicates, the same genomic positions were first selected between treatment A and treatment B, before subtracting allele frequencies between treatments. I) Read statistics used for the CMH analysis. The same genomic positions were first selected for both treatments among all the replicates, before the CMH analysis. The part in gray on chromosome IV for replicate 3 was not used in the test as one parental allele was fixed in both treatments. J) Annotation of variants detected in JU1249 compared to the reference N2, using the VEP algorithm. The F34D10.6 deletion (in red) appears as the only high impact variation. K) Annotation of variants detected in JU2825 compared to the reference N2.

mmc2.xlsx (36.8MB, xlsx)
Data S2. Distribution of the mfP22 Deletion, Related to Figure 2

List of wild isolates where the mfP22 deletion is absent, based on mapped sequence reads available at the Caenorhabditis elegans Natural Diversity Resource, http://elegansvariation.org (Cook et al., 2016).

mmc3.xlsx (10.6KB, xlsx)
Data S3. Protein Identifications from CoIP of GFP-ARCP-1B, Related to Figure 5

Proteins identified by mass spectrometry in two independent coIP experiments for interactors of GFP-ARCP-1B. IP of GFP-tagged cytoplasmic proteins (MALT-1-GFP and EIF-3.L-GFP) provided a negative control. Total spectrum counts in GFP-ARCP-1B and control samples are listed for proteins that were at least 3-fold enriched in the GFP-ARCP-1B sample in both experiments.

mmc4.xlsx (41.6KB, xlsx)
Data S4. Expression Profiling of BAG Neurons in npr-1 and arcp-1; npr-1 Animals Using RNA-Seq, Related to Figure 6

A-D) Genes expressed in BAG neurons, which were isolated by FACS from adult npr-1 and arcp-1; npr-1 animals, with six biological replicates per genotype. A-B) Values indicate transcripts per kilobase million (TPM). C-D) Values show fragments per kilobase million (FPKM). Genes are listed based upon an expression detection threshold of 1 count per million reads per gene in at least 6 samples. E) Genes differentially expressed in BAG neurons of npr-1 and arcp-1; npr-1 animals.

mmc5.xlsx (4.6MB, xlsx)
Document S2. Article plus Supplemental Information
mmc6.pdf (43.2MB, pdf)

Data Availability Statement

Datasets

The genome sequencing data of JU1249 and JU2825 is available on NCBI: PRJNA514933 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/genome/?term=PRJNA514933). The genomic sequence data of the replicate populations for QTL mapping is available on NCBI: PRJNA515248 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/genome/?term=PRJNA515248). Sequence data from the RNA-Seq analysis of sorted BAG neurons is deposited on GEO: GSE135687 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/geo/query/acc.cgi?acc=GSE135687).

Codes

Locomotory assays

Videos of locomotory assays were analyzed in Zentracker, a custom-written MATLAB software available on https://github.com/wormtracker/zentracker.

Calcium imaging

Recordings were analyzed using Neuron Analyzer, a customwritten MATLAB program available at https://github.com/neuronanalyser/neuronanalyser.

RNA-seq analysis

Codes for removing rRNA sequences from datasets and for concatenating FASTQ files relating to the same sample but sequenced over multiple flow cell lanes, are available on GitHub: https://github.com/lmb-seq/RNA-Seq_utilities. The git repository for PRAGUI can be found at: https://github.com/lmb-seq/PRAGUI.

RESOURCES