Skip to main content
NeuroImage : Clinical logoLink to NeuroImage : Clinical
. 2019 Dec 30;25:102159. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.102159

Brain age prediction in stroke patients: Highly reliable but limited sensitivity to cognitive performance and response to cognitive training

Geneviève Richard a,b,c,, Knut Kolskår a,b,c, Kristine M Ulrichsen a,b,c, Tobias Kaufmann a, Dag Alnæs a, Anne-Marthe Sanders a,b,c, Erlend S Dørum a,b,c, Jennifer Monereo Sánchez d,e, Anders Petersen f, Hege Ihle-Hansen g, Jan Egil Nordvik h, Lars T Westlye a,b,
PMCID: PMC6953960  PMID: 31927499

Highlights

  • Longitudinal brain age prediction is feasible and reliable in stroke patients.

  • Minor brain lesions have minimal impact on brain age estimations.

  • Brain age was not significantly associated with cognitive performance.

  • Brain age was not significantly associated with response to cognitive training.

Keywords: Computerized cognitive training; Transcranial direct current stimulation; Magnetic resonance imaging; Brain age prediction; Cerebral stroke, T1

Abstract

Cognitive deficits are important predictors for outcome, independence and quality of life after stroke, but often remain unnoticed and unattended because other impairments are more evident. Computerized cognitive training (CCT) is among the candidate interventions that may alleviate cognitive difficulties, but the evidence supporting its feasibility and effectiveness is scarce, partly due to the lack of tools for outcome prediction and monitoring. Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) provides candidate markers for disease monitoring and outcome prediction. By integrating information not only about lesion extent and localization, but also regarding the integrity of the unaffected parts of the brain, advanced MRI provides relevant information for developing better prediction models in order to tailor cognitive intervention for patients, especially in a chronic phase.

Using brain age prediction based on MRI based brain morphometry and machine learning, we tested the hypotheses that stroke patients with a younger-appearing brain relative to their chronological age perform better on cognitive tests and benefit more from cognitive training compared to patients with an older-appearing brain. In this randomized double-blind study, 54 patients who suffered mild stroke (>6 months since hospital admission, NIHSS≤7 at hospital discharge) underwent 3-weeks CCT and MRI before and after the intervention. In addition, patients were randomized to one of two groups receiving either active or sham transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). We tested for main effects of brain age gap (estimated age – chronological age) on cognitive performance, and associations between brain age gap and task improvement. Finally, we tested if longitudinal changes in brain age gap during the intervention were sensitive to treatment response. Briefly, our results suggest that longitudinal brain age prediction based on automated brain morphometry is feasible and reliable in stroke patients. However, no significant association between brain age and both performance and response to cognitive training were found.

1. Introduction

Stroke is among the most common causes of acquired cognitive disabilities during adulthood, with a projected increase in prevalence over the next decades due to the aging population (Feigin et al., 2014; Feigin et al., 2017). Despite recent reductions in stroke-related mortalities, largely due to major improvements in acute health care and treatment (Zhang et al., 2012) many stroke survivors suffer from long-term and pervasive cognitive deficits (Barbay et al., 2018; Barker-Collo et al. 2010; Cumming et al., 2014; Haacke et al., 2006; Nakling et al., 2017; Patel et al., 2002) that often remain unnoticed by the health care system due to its typically delayed manifestation (Jacova et al., 2012; Kalaria et al., 2016).

Previous studies and treatment programs have largely targeted patients in the acute and sub-acute phase, as it has been assumed that recovery and cognitive rehabilitation are more likely to be successful during a limited time window following the insult (Zucchella et al., 2014). Whereas the temporal aspects of cognitive interventions following stroke is important, evidence suggests that recovery can also occur in chronic stages, i.e. years after the insult (Berthier et al., 2011; Moss and Nicholas 2006). As a result, there is an increasing need for developing and validating tools that can be used to predict long-term outcome and for monitoring of the effects of cognitive rehabilitation after stroke (Hope et al., 2013).

Advanced neuroimaging techniques based on magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) offer a range of candidate markers for disease monitoring and outcome prediction. In addition to providing detailed information about the localization and extent of the lesion, which represent key clinical information in the acute phase, imaging techniques allow for a characterization of the structural and functional integrity of the whole brain, including areas not directly damaged by the stroke (Kalaria et al., 2016; Werden et al., 2017). This information is highly relevant in a cognitive rehabilitation context, where the potential for improvement and recovery are not only defined by the lesion itself, but by the integrity and efficiency of the unaffected brain regions (Ihle-Hansen et al., 2014). Further, it is widely acknowledged that the brain systems supporting cognitive functions are broadly distributed, supporting a network-based conceptualization of the functional neuroanatomy of cognitive functions. Hence, lesions in widely different parts of the brain may result in overlapping cognitive symptoms, depending on the brain networks involved (Guggisberg et al., 2019). A direct implication of this is that both the degree of cognitive impairment and the individual potential for improvement in response to intervention may be less dependent on the exact characteristics of the lesion than the structural integrity of the unaffected brain networks.

Here, we test this concept by utilizing multivariate brain age prediction using machine learning and sensitive measures of brain morphometry. Briefly, combining a wide array of informative brain imaging features in a prediction model allows for an accurate prediction of the age of an unseen individual (Franke et al., 2012; Franke et al., 2010). The degree to which the model under- or over-estimate the individual's age has been shown to be sensitive to a variety of health- related characteristics, including cognitive function and mortality (Boyle et al., 2019; Cole and Franke 2017; Cole et al., 2018; Richard et al., 2018), and brain age prediction using MRI data has recently been shown to be sensitive both to the clinical manifestation and polygenic risk of various brain disorders (Hogestol et al., 2019; Kaufmann et al., 2019).

Based on the notion that brain age prediction offers a sensitive summary measure of brain integrity, we first tested whether brain age is sensitive to cognitive function in chronic stroke patients. Next, to assess the predictive value of brain age prediction in a cognitive rehabilitation context, we tested if brain age prior to the intervention is associated with response to an intensive computerized cognitive training (CCT) program. As a follow-up analysis to a previous study (Kolskår et al., 2019) reporting no robust beneficial effects of transcranial direct brain stimulation (tDCS) on cognitive improvement, we assessed if any beneficial effects of tDCS (active vs sham) would be dependent on brain age. Finally, we tested to which degree longitudinal changes in brain age during the course of the intervention are sensitive to treatment response. We hypothesized that (1) brain age prediction would constitute a reliable and sensitive method for characterizing individual level brain health. We further anticipated that (2) patients with a relatively young-appearing brain age (which may imply higher cognitive or brain reserve) would show better cognitive function at baseline, and (3) would show larger improvements in task performance. Lastly, to the extent that intensive cognitive training shows beneficial effects on cognitive performance and the brain (Engvig et al., 2010), we hypothesized that (4) cognitive gains would be reflected in longitudinal changes in brain age during the course of the intervention.

We tested these hypotheses in a group of 54 chronic patients who suffered mild stroke (> 6 months since hospital admission, NIHSS ≤ 7 at hospital discharge) invited to take part in a randomized, double blind study aimed to test the utility of tDCS in combination with CCT to improve cognitive performance following stroke (Kolskår et al., 2019; Ulrichsen et al., 2019). For unbiased brain age prediction, we utilized a large independent training set, and employed stringent procedures for multiple comparison correction to increase the robustness of the results.

2. Materials and methods

Table 1 summarizes key clinical and demographic information for the patient group. Fig. 1 depicts the timeline of the intervention, including histograms depicting the time range between MRI assessments. Patients were recruited with the main aim of testing the clinical feasibility of combining CCT and tDCS to improve cognitive function in chronic stroke patients. Description of the extent and localization of individual patient lesions, as well as recruitment procedures are detailed in (Kolskår et al., 2019). Supplementary Figure 1 depicts a heatmap of the lesion location across 68 patients a heatmap where the voxel value reflects the number of participants having a lesion in that voxel. Briefly, patients admitted to the Stroke Unit at Oslo University Hospital and at Diakonhjemmet Hospital, Oslo, Norway during 2013–2016 were invited to participate through letters. Stroke was defined as any form of strokes of either ischemic or hemorrhagic etiology; transient ischemic attacks (TIA) were excluded. Additional exclusion criteria included MRI contraindications and other neurological diseases diagnosed prior to the stroke.

Table 1.

Demographics and sample characteristics. 1MMSE and MoCA scores at inclusion. 2NIHSS score at hospital discharge. 3Only one patient suffered from intracerebral hemorrhage (Kolskår et al., 2019; Ulrichsen et al., 2019).

Training sample Cam-CAN
Test sample StrokeMRI Baseline
Longitudinal
Healthy controls Stroke patients Stroke patients
Mean (SD) Range Mean (SD) Range Mean (SD) Range
Total N (% females) 628 (51.6%) 68 (27.9%) 54 (25.9%)
Age 54.2 (18.3) 18–87 67.98 (10.24) 24.3–81.8 69.72 (7.46) 47.8–82.0
Education (in years) 14.32 (3.78) 7–30 14.38 (3.75) 9–30
MMSE1 27.91 (1.97) 22–30 28.00 (1.87) 22–30
MoCA1 25.70 (3.15) 14–30 25.92 (2.77) 17–30
Months since stroke 26.67 (9.13) 6–45 25.74 (9.17) 6–45
NIHSS2 1.31 (1.52) 0–7 1.33 (1.53) 0–7
TOAST classification for ischemic stroke3 Large artery artherosclerosis (23) Large artery artherosclerosis (20)
Cardioembolism (7) Cardioembolism (6)
Small vessel occlusion (21) Small vessel occlusion (18)
Other (17) Other (10)
Stroke location Right (30) Right (23)
Left (22) Left (18)
Brain stem / Cerebellum (9) Brain stem / Cerebellum (8)
Bilateral (7) Bilateral (5)

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Timeline of the intervention.

Approximately 250 patients responded to the letter to either decline the invitation or express their interest, of which 72 completed the first assessment and 54 patients completed the full protocol (Kolskår et al., 2019; Ulrichsen et al., 2019); including three MRI brain scan sessions, three sessions with cognitive assessments, one EEG assessment, and seven CCT sessions in addition to 10 CCT sessions performed at home.

Four patients were excluded from the analysis in the current study. Two were excluded based on poor quality or incomplete MRI data, one based on incomplete cognitive assessment at baseline and one due to lack of confirmed stroke. The remaining 68 patients were included in the brain age estimation and associations with baseline cognitive performance (age = 24.3–81.8, mean = 67.98, SD = 10.24, 19 females). All 54 patients who completed the training sessions were included in the remaining analyses (age = 47.8–82.0, mean = 69.72, SD = 7.46, 14 females).

Training set for brain age prediction

The healthy controls used as training set for the age prediction model were obtained from the Cambridge centre for Ageing and Neuroscience (Cam-CAN) sample (http://www.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/datasets/camcan/; (Shafto et al., 2014; Taylor et al., 2017)). Briefly, volunteers were recruited to Cam-CAN through a large-scale collaborative research project funded by the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (BBSRC, grant number BB/H008217/1), the UK Medical Research Council and University of Cambridge. For more information, see http://www.cam-can.org. Data from 628 individuals (age = 18–87, mean = 54.2, SD = 18.3, 324 females) were included in the training set (Richard et al., 2018).

Cognitive assessment at baseline

Similar to our recent study (Richard et al., 2018), cognitive performance at baseline was assessed with a set of neuropsychological and computerized tests assumed to be sensitive to cognitive aging, including the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA; Nasreddine et al., 2005), the vocabulary and matrix subtests of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI; Wechsler 1999), the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT-II; Delis et al., 2000), and the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS) color word interference test (Stroop; Delis et al., 2001). We included several computerized tests from the Cognitive Assessment at Bedside for iPAD (CABPad; Willer et al., 2016), including motor speed, verbal fluency (phonological and semantic), working memory (forward and backward memory span), spatial Stroop (executive control of attention), spatial attention span, and symbol digit coding tests. Further, a computerized test based on the Theory of Visual Attention (TVA; Bundesen 1990; Bundesen and Habekost 2008; Dyrholm et al., 2011) provided measures of visual short-term memory capacity (K), processing speed (C), and perceptual threshold (t0). Several variables were highly correlated, and we used the clustering solution from Richard et al. (2018), which included seven broad cognitive domains. Cluster 1 reflected memory and learning (CVLT, attention span, MoCA), cluster 2 visual processing speed (TVA-parameters C and t0), cluster 3 verbal skills (phonological and semantic flow), cluster 4 attentional control and speed (spatial Stroop), cluster 5 executive control and speed (color-word Stroop), cluster 6 reasoning and psychomotor speed (matrix, symbol coding and motor speed, visual short-term memory capacity (TVA-parameter K)), and cluster 7 working memory (forward and backward memory span). Briefly, the clusters were computed using normalized sum scores of highly correlated test scores. Prior to calculating summary scores based on the seven clusters mentioned above, we used outlierTest from the car package (Fox and Weisberg 2011) to identify the most extreme observations based on a linear model, including age and sex. 17 observations were identified as outliers based on a Bonferroni corrected p < 0.05 and treated as missing values, we then replaced these extremes and imputed the 75 missing values (2.63% of the scores were missing/incomplete) using predictive mean matching (pmm) method from the mice package in R (multivariate imputation by chained equations; Buuren & Groothuis-Oudshoorn 2011).

CCT protocol

All patients completed a computerized working memory training program consisting of 25 online training sessions (Cogmed Systems AB, Stockholm, Sweden). Similar to our recent study (Kolskår et al., 2019), we used data from 17 of the 25 training sessions over a period of three to four weeks, corresponding to approximately five weekly training sessions. Seven sessions were carried out at the hospital, of which six were in combination with tDCS (either sham or active stimulation). On average, patients received two training sessions with tDCS per week with a minimum of one day between each session. The remaining 10 training sessions were home-training. Each training session took approximately 45 min in which the participant completed eight different exercises. In total, 10 different tasks targeting verbal and visuospatial working memory were used, i.e. Grid, Hidden, Cube, Sort, Digits, 3D Cube, Twist, Assembly, Rotating and Chaos. The difficulty level of each task is adapted to the participant's performance, and in general, for each task, it takes approximately two sessions for the difficulty level to be appropriately adjusted to the individual level of performance. Thus, we discarded the two first training sessions of each task from our analysis. In addition, we included only tasks with a minimum of three training sessions after exclusion of the two first sessions, discarding Assembly and Chaos from further analysis.

tDCS protocol

The tDCS protocol has been described in details in a prior publication (Kolskår et al., 2019). Participants were randomly assigned to an active or a sham condition, using an in-house Matlab script to randomly generate a code for each participant while ensuring that each block of 20 participants was balanced across conditions. Both the participant and the experimenter remained blinded throughout the experiment. Stimulation was delivered during the first 20 min of the CCT sessions using a battery-driven direct current stimulator (Neuroconn DC-STIMULATOR PLUS, neuroConn GmbH, Illmenau, Germany), through 5 × 7 cm rubber pads using the following parameters: DC current = 1 mA, total duration = 20 min, ramp-up = 120 s, fade-out = 30 s, and current density = 28.57 μA/cm2. The sham stimulation followed the factory settings which include a ramp-up and a fade-out period. Based on previous literature (Au et al., 2016; Pope and Miall 2012), we used the 10–20 system for the electrode location, with the anodal electrode covering F3 and the cathodal electrode placed over O2, and fixated with rubber bands. The pads were covered with high-conductive gel (Abralyt HiCl, Falk Minow Services Herrsching, Germany) to keep the impedance threshold under < 20 kΩ. For security reason, the device has an absolute impedance threshold of 40 kΩ. Following each stimulation period, participants were asked to fill in a side-effect form. In addition, after the last stimulation session, they were asked to make a guess whether they thought they received active stimulation or sham stimulation and the reason for their guess.

MRI acquisition

Patients were scanned on a 3T GE 750 Discovery MRI scanner with a 32-channel head coil at Oslo University Hospital. Paddings were used to reduce head motion. T1-weighted data was acquired using a 3D IR-prepared FSPGR (BRAVO) with the following parameters: repetition time (TR): 8.16 ms, echo time (TE): 3.18 ms, inversion time (TI): 450 ms, flip angle (FA): 12º, voxel size: 1 × 1 × 1 mm, field of view (FOV): 256×256 mm, 188 sagittal slices, scan time: 4:43 min.

Cam-CAN participants were scanned on a 3T Siemens TIM Trio scanner with a 32-channel head-coil at Medical Research Council (UK) Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit (MRC—CBSU) in Cambridge, UK. High-resolution 3D T1-weighted data was acquired using a magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo (MPRAGE) sequence with the following parameters: TR: 2250 ms, TE: 2.99 ms, TI: 900 ms, FA: 9°, FOV of 256×240×192 mm; voxel size =1 mm3 isotropic, GRAPPA acceleration factor of 2, scan time 4:32 min (Shafto et al., 2014).

MRI processing

All T1-weighted images were processed using FreeSurfer 5.3 (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu; (Dale et al., 1999)) including brain extraction, intensity normalization, automated tissue segmentation, generation of white and pial surfaces (Dale et al., 1999). All reconstructions were visually assessed and corrected as appropriate, and data with excessive motion or other major artefacts were discarded.

For StrokeMRI, images were processed with the longitudinal Freesurfer pipeline (Reuter and Fischl 2011; Reuter et al., 2012), which substantially increases reliability and power (Reuter et al., 2012). For each individual dataset, we extracted mean cortical thickness, area and volumes from 180 regions of interests (ROIs) per hemisphere based on a surface-based atlas (Glasser et al., 2016), yielding 1080 structural brain features per individual.

Age prediction

Based on a recent implementation (Kaufmann et al., 2019), brain age estimation was performed both using global and regional features as input. The regional brain age estimations were based on lobesStrict segmentation (occipital, frontal, temporal, parietal, cingulate and insulate) from Freesurfer (Dale et al., 1999). Overall, one global and 12 hemisphere specific lobe-based models were trained to estimate age in 628 healthy controls from the Cam-CAN cohort, using the same pipeline as previously described (Richard et al., 2018). We used xgboost package (extreme gradient boosting) (Chen and Guestrin 2016; Chen et al., 2017) in R with the following parameters: learning rate (eta) = 0.1, nround = 1500, gamma = 1, max_depth = 6, subsample=0.5, to build the prediction models. For each model, the performance was estimated using a 10-fold cross-validation procedure within the training set.

Next, we tested the performance of our trained models by predicting age in unseen subjects in the test sample. More specifically, we calculated the Pearson correlation between the predicted and the chronological age, as well as the mean absolute error (MAE) in years. For each individual and for each model, we calculated the brain age gap (BAG), i.e. the difference between the estimated and chronological age, yielding 13 BAGs per individuals. Next, in order to account for age-related bias in the age prediction (Le et al., 2018), we used linear modeling to regress out the main effect of age, age2 and sex from each BAG, resulting in 13 residualized BAG (BAGR) used in the calculation of MAE and further analyses.

In some instances, the stroke lesions directly interfered with the cortical reconstruction process in Freesurfer, which inevitably influences the estimated morphometric parameters in the relevant part of the brain. In order to assess the influence of the stroke lesion on the brain age estimates, we used outlierTest from the car package (Fox and Weisberg, 2011) to identify the most extreme morphometric estimations based on a linear model, including age, age2 and sex. We identified 479 observations (0.24% of all observations) as extreme and replaced them using predictive mean matching (pmm) method from the mice package in R (multivariate imputation by chained equations; Buuren and Groothuis-Oudshoorn 2011). Next, we estimated brain age using the resulting data frame containing imputed estimations and compared it with the original estimations. Subsequent analyses were performed both with and without the outliers included. Briefly, the estimated brain age based on the original Freesurfer estimations and the estimations after imputing realistic values to replace outliers resulted in nearly identical outcomes. (See supplemental results for the analyses performed after removing the outliers.)

In addition to the global model including all T1 features, we calculated a robust brain age based on the median of the 12 regional brain ages.

Processing of Cogmed data

For each participant and for each included task, we used linear modeling to quantify the changes in performance across the training period, i.e. the cognitive improvement, using performance as dependent variable and session number as independent variable (Kolskår et al., 2019). In addition, we used the generic function predict in R (Chambers and Hastie 1992) to estimate the baseline score and the final score using the resulting individual linear models for each trained task. To derive a common score across the trained tasks, we performed a principal component analysis (PCA) on the performance improvement scores and we used the first component as the individual's performance improvement (Kolskår et al., 2019). All test scores were zero-centered and standardized prior to running the PCA.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using R version 3.3.3 (2017–03–06) (R Core Team 2017). We assessed the reliability of the age estimations using intra-class coefficient (ICC) using ICCest function from the ICC R package (Wolak et al., 2012) across the two baseline MRI and across all three MRI sessions.

To test if patients with relatively younger-appearing brain age show better cognitive performance at baseline, we employed linear models with the seven summary scores based on the clustering solution from (Richard et al., 2018) as independent variable and each BAGR as the dependent variable, including age and sex as covariates. To test if patients with relatively younger-appearing brain age would show larger improvement in task performance, we employed linear models with Cogmed performance gain score derived from the PCA as independent variable and each BAGR as dependent variable, including age and sex as covariates. For transparency, we report both uncorrected p-values and p-values adjusted using false discovery rate (FDR; Benjamini and Hochberg 1995) from the p.adjust function from the stats R package (R Core Team 2017). We have previously reported no significant beneficial effects of tDCS on cognitive improvement in response to the intervention (Kolskår et al., 2019). Here, as a follow-up analysis, we added tDCS group (sham vs experimental) as an additional variable and tested for interactions between tDCS and BAGR on training gain to assess if any beneficial effects of tDCS would be dependent on BAGR.

Lastly, in order to assess if cognitive improvements in response to intensive cognitive training is associated with reduced brain age during the course of the intervention, we tested for associations between cognitive performance and BAGR by time interaction in a longitudinal context using linear mixed effects models (LME). For each trained task, we used the estimated baseline and final scores from the individual linear models, and we used BAGR from scan number 2 and 3 as timepoint one and two, respectively. Estimated task performance was entered as dependent variable, with BAGR, time, BAGR by time interaction, age and sex as fixed factors, and participant as random factor.

3. Results

Brain age predictions

Ten-fold cross-validation on the training sample (Cam-CAN) revealed relatively high correlations between chronological and predicted age for each of the 13 models, confirming reasonable model performance. Supplementary Fig. 2 shows Pearson correlation with confidence intervals between estimated brain age and chronological age within the training sample for each of the 13 trained models ranging from r = 0.84 (CI = 0.81–0.86) for the full model to r = 0.61 (CI = 0.56–0.66) for the model based on right cingulate features.

Table 2 shows Pearson correlation between estimated brain age and chronological age with their 95% confidence intervals on the test sample at baseline (stroke patients) for each model, in addition to the MAE calculated from BAGR. The correlations ranged from r = 0.58 (CI = 0.40–0.72, MAE = 4.27) for the most comprehensive model based on the median of the 12 regional models to r = 0.09 (CI = −0.16–0.32 MAE = 7.29) for the left cingulate model. See Suppl. Table 1 for the model performance after replacing outliers by imputed values.

Table 2.

Pearson correlation between estimated brain age and chronological age with their 95% confidence intervals on the test sample at baseline (stroke patients from StrokeMRI sample) for each model, and the MAE calculated from BAGR.

Models r LowerCI UpperCI MAE
Global
all_T1 0.550 0.358 0.697 5.506
median 0.579 0.395 0.718 4.271
Regional
left_frontal 0.448 0.234 0.620 5.799
right_frontal 0.403 0.182 0.585 5.663
left_parietal 0.413 0.194 0.593 6.287
right_parietal 0.460 0.248 0.629 6.086
left_occipital 0.221 −0.019 0.436 7.642
right_occipital 0.206 −0.034 0.423 6.953
left_temporal 0.416 0.198 0.596 7.588
right_temporal 0.455 0.242 0.625 5.733
left_cingulate 0.086 −0.156 0.317 6.953
right_cingulate 0.439 0.224 0.613 8.804
left_insula 0.309 0.076 0.510 7.732
right_insula 0.366 0.140 0.556 6.277

Table 3 shows ICC with their confidence intervals for each model for the two baselines and for the three timepoints ranging from 0.89 (CI = 0.82–0.94) for the right parietal model to 0.68 (CI = 0.50–0.80) for the left cingulate model across the two baseline assessments, and ranging from 0.86 (CI = 0.79–0.91) for the right parietal model to 0.70 (CI = 0.57–0.80) for the left cingulate model across the three timepoints. See Suppl. Table 2 for the estimation after replacing outliers by imputed values.

Table 3.

Intra-class correlation (ICC) with their confidence interval of the estimated brain age for the two baseline scans (scan one and two), and for the three timepoints (scan one, two and three).

Baseline (scan 1 and 2)
All time points (scan 1 to 3)
Models ICC LowerCI UpperCI ICC LowerCI UpperCI
Global
BAGR_all_T1 0.788 0.661 0.871 0.783 0.686 0.859
BAGR_median 0.887 0.813 0.932 0.831 0.750 0.891
Regional
BAGR_left_frontal 0.790 0.664 0.872 0.812 0.724 0.879
BAGR_right_frontal 0.761 0.622 0.854 0.74 0.628 0.829
BAGR_left_parietal 0.803 0.684 0.880 0.805 0.714 0.874
BAGR_right_parietal 0.891 0.819 0.935 0.856 0.786 0.908
BAGR_left_occipital 0.823 0.715 0.893 0.815 0.728 0.881
BAGR_right_occipital 0.786 0.658 0.870 0.811 0.723 0.878
BAGR_left_temporal 0.800 0.679 0.878 0.807 0.718 0.876
BAGR_right_temporal 0.726 0.572 0.831 0.754 0.646 0.839
BAGR_left_cingulate 0.677 0.503 0.799 0.700 0.577 0.801
BAGR_right_cingulate 0.814 0.700 0.887 0.802 0.711 0.872
BAGR_left_insula 0.809 0.693 0.884 0.802 0.711 0.872
BAGR_right_insula 0.791 0.666 0.873 0.789 0.693 0.863

Table 4 and Table 5 show summary statistics from the linear models testing for associations between cognitive performance at baseline and Cogmed performance gain, respectively, and BAGR, including age and sex in the models. As expected, we found a main effect of age on cognitive performance at baseline. However, the analyses revealed no significant associations between cognitive performance at baseline and BAGR after FDR correction for multiple comparisons. Amongst the non-significant findings, the strongest associations were found between cluster 5 (executive control and speed) and the right cingulate, cluster 7 (working memory) and the right cingulate, and cluster 4 (attentional control and speed) and the right temporal BAGR. Further, we did not find any significant associations between performance improvement score and BAGR, nor main effect of age, nor sex on the performance improvement score after FDR corrections. Amongst the non-significant findings, the strongest associations with Cogmed performance gain were found for the left frontal and left parietal models, indicating higher cognitive gain for participants with lower BAGR (younger-appearing brains relative to their chronological age).

Table 4.

Summary statistics of the associations between cognitive performance at baseline and BAGR, including age and sex using linear models. Cluster 1: memory and learning. Cluster 2: visual processing speed. Cluster 3: verbal skills. Cluster 4: attentional control and speed. Cluster 5: executive control and speed. Cluster 6: reasoning and psychomotor speed. Cluster 7: working memory. The reported p-values are uncorrected values, and no main effect of BAGR remained significant after FDR correction.

Clusters Models Bagr (t (p)) Age (t (p)) Sex (t (p))
Cluster1 all_T1 0.152 (0.880) −4.884 (<0.001) −2.200 (0.032)
Cluster1 median −0.095 (0.925) −4.886 (<0.001) −2.220 (0.030)
Cluster1 left_frontal −0.032 (0.975) −4.886 (<0.001) −2.201 (0.031)
Cluster1 right_frontal −0.795 (0.43) −4.933 (<0.001) −2.169 (0.034)
Cluster1 left_parietal −0.356 (0.723) −4.879 (<0.001) −2.227 (0.030)
Cluster1 right_parietal 0.466 (0.643) −4.902 (<0.001) −2.187 (0.032)
Cluster1 left_occipital −0.797 (0.429) −4.957 (<0.001) −2.281 (0.026)
Cluster1 right_occipital 0.478 (0.634) −4.846 (<0.001) −2.182 (0.033)
Cluster1 left_temporal −0.283 (0.778) −4.893 (<0.001) −2.232 (0.029)
Cluster1 right_temporal −0.495 (0.622) −4.906 (<0.001) −2.246 (0.028)
Cluster1 left_cingulate 1.018 (0.313) −4.910 (<0.001) −2.146 (0.036)
Cluster1 right_cingulate −0.330 (0.742) −4.843 (<0.001) −2.230 (0.029)
Cluster1 left_insula −0.898 (0.373) −4.921 (<0.001) −2.322 (0.024)
Cluster1 right_insula −0.651 (0.518) −4.922 (<0.001) −2.176 (0.033)
Cluster2 all_T1 0.277 (0.783) −3.716 (<0.001) 1.853 (0.069)
Cluster2 median 0.263 (0.793) −3.723 (<0.001) 1.848 (0.069)
Cluster2 left_frontal 0.120 (0.905) −3.721 (<0.001) 1.821 (0.073)
Cluster2 right_frontal −0.754 (0.454) −3.753 (<0.001) 1.884 (0.064)
Cluster2 left_parietal 0.304 (0.762) −3.729 (<0.001) 1.840 (0.070)
Cluster2 right_parietal −0.012 (0.991) −3.718 (<0.001) 1.830 (0.072)
Cluster2 left_occipital 0.122 (0.903) −3.700 (<0.001) 1.838 (0.071)
Cluster2 right_occipital −0.931 (0.355) −3.806 (<0.001) 1.782 (0.08)
Cluster2 left_temporal 1.283 (0.204) −3.752 (<0.001) 1.927 (0.058)
Cluster2 right_temporal 0.345 (0.731) −3.713 (<0.001) 1.851 (0.069)
Cluster2 left_cingulate 0.498 (0.62) −3.716 (<0.001) 1.870 (0.066)
Cluster2 right_cingulate −0.431 (0.668) −3.679 (<0.001) 1.833 (0.072)
Cluster2 left_insula −0.482 (0.632) −3.728 (<0.001) 1.773 (0.081)
Cluster2 right_insula −0.658 (0.513) −3.750 (<0.001) 1.880 (0.065)
Cluster3 all_T1 −0.497 (0.621) −2.96 (0.004) −3.072 (0.003)
Cluster3 median −0.529 (0.599) −2.947 (0.004) −3.068 (0.003)
Cluster3 left_frontal 0.986 (0.328) −2.996 (0.004) −3.119 (0.003)
Cluster3 right_frontal −0.744 (0.460) −2.977 (0.004) −2.997 (0.004)
Cluster3 left_parietal −0.808 (0.422) −2.935 (0.005) −3.063 (0.003)
Cluster3 right_parietal 0.796 (0.429) −2.971 (0.004) −2.995 (0.004)
Cluster3 left_occipital −0.608 (0.545) −2.992 (0.004) −3.079 (0.003)
Cluster3 right_occipital 0.243 (0.809) −2.919 (0.005) −3.013 (0.004)
Cluster3 left_temporal 0.264 (0.793) −2.945 (0.004) −3.016 (0.004)
Cluster3 right_temporal −0.901 (0.371) −2.985 (0.004) −3.096 (0.003)
Cluster3 left_cingulate −0.462 (0.646) −2.959 (0.004) −3.066 (0.003)
Cluster3 right_cingulate −1.708 (0.092) −2.87 (0.006) −3.118 (0.003)
Cluster3 left_insula −1.207 (0.232) −2.987 (0.004) −3.183 (0.002)
Cluster3 right_insula −1.909 (0.061) −3.086 (0.003) −2.983 (0.004)
Cluster4 all_T1 −1.687 (0.096) −4.182 (<0.001) −0.170 (0.866)
Cluster4 median −1.990 (0.051) −4.166 (<0.001) −0.142 (0.887)
Cluster4 left_frontal −0.833 (0.408) −4.060 (<0.001) 0.043 (0.966)
Cluster4 right_frontal −0.838 (0.405) −4.106 (<0.001) 0.034 (0.973)
Cluster4 left_parietal −1.325 (0.190) −4.076 (<0.001) −0.040 (0.968)
Cluster4 right_parietal −1.471 (0.146) −4.112 (<0.001) −0.106 (0.916)
Cluster4 left_occipital −1.884 (0.064) −4.303 (<0.001) −0.155 (0.878)
Cluster4 right_occipital −1.334 (0.187) −4.213 (<0.001) −0.103 (0.918)
Cluster4 left_temporal −0.280 (0.780) −4.069 (<0.001) −0.032 (0.974)
Cluster4 right_temporal −2.063 (0.043) −4.243 (<0.001) −0.125 (0.901)
Cluster4 left_cingulate 0.023 (0.982) −4.063 (<0.001) −0.015 (0.988)
Cluster4 right_cingulate −1.316 (0.193) −4.003 (<0.001) −0.029 (0.977)
Cluster4 left_insula −0.042 (0.967) −4.064 (<0.001) −0.021 (0.983)
Cluster4 right_insula −0.637 (0.527) −4.094 (<0.001) 0.026 (0.979)
Cluster5 all_T1 −1.318 (0.192) −4.442 (<0.001) −2.098 (0.040)
Cluster5 median −1.427 (0.158) −4.415 (<0.001) −2.077 (0.042)
Cluster5 left_frontal 0.570 (0.571) −4.388 (<0.001) −2.000 (0.050)
Cluster5 right_frontal −0.82 (0.415) −4.404 (<0.001) −1.917 (0.060)
Cluster5 left_parietal 0.464 (0.644) −4.381 (<0.001) −1.955 (0.055)
Cluster5 right_parietal −0.449 (0.655) −4.362 (<0.001) −1.986 (0.051)
Cluster5 left_occipital −0.417 (0.678) −4.386 (<0.001) −1.988 (0.051)
Cluster5 right_occipital −1.995 (0.050) −4.639 (<0.001) −2.145 (0.036)
Cluster5 left_temporal −1.263 (0.211) −4.430 (<0.001) −2.052 (0.044)
Cluster5 right_temporal −1.827 (0.072) −4.515 (<0.001) −2.103 (0.039)
Cluster5 left_cingulate 1.429 (0.158) −4.405 (<0.001) −1.874 (0.066)
Cluster5 right_cingulate −2.387 (0.020) −4.351 (<0.001) −2.067 (0.043)
Cluster5 left_insula −1.840 (0.070) −4.487 (<0.001) −2.202 (0.031)
Cluster5 right_insula 0.655 (0.515) −4.354 (<0.001) −2.006 (0.049)
Cluster6 all_T1 −0.008 (0.993) −5.334 (<0.001) −2.252 (0.028)
Cluster6 median −0.278 (0.782) −5.334 (<0.001) −2.275 (0.026)
Cluster6 left_frontal 0.176 (0.860) −5.339 (<0.001) −2.268 (0.027)
Cluster6 right_frontal −1.132 (0.262) −5.415 (<0.001) −2.211 (0.031)
Cluster6 left_parietal −0.128 (0.898) −5.329 (<0.001) −2.263 (0.027)
Cluster6 right_parietal 0.309 (0.758) −5.342 (<0.001) −2.239 (0.029)
Cluster6 left_occipital −1.046 (0.300) −5.443 (<0.001) −2.350 (0.022)
Cluster6 right_occipital −0.179 (0.858) −5.333 (<0.001) −2.268 (0.027)
Cluster6 left_temporal 0.673 (0.503) −5.346 (<0.001) −2.227 (0.030)
Cluster6 right_temporal −0.280 (0.781) −5.343 (<0.001) −2.273 (0.026)
Cluster6 left_cingulate 0.825 (0.412) −5.347 (<0.001) −2.202 (0.031)
Cluster6 right_cingulate −1.443 (0.154) −5.291 (<0.001) −2.310 (0.024)
Cluster6 left_insula −1.027 (0.308) −5.385 (<0.001) −2.378 (0.020)
Cluster6 right_insula 0.013 (0.990) −5.332 (<0.001) −2.256 (0.028)
Cluster7 all_T1 0.206 (0.838) −5.298 (<0.001) −1.473 (0.146)
Cluster7 median −0.431 (0.668) −5.303 (<0.001) −1.524 (0.132)
Cluster7 left_frontal 0.636 (0.527) −5.333 (<0.001) −1.544 (0.128)
Cluster7 right_frontal 0.003 (0.998) −5.298 (<0.001) −1.495 (0.140)
Cluster7 left_parietal −0.378 (0.707) −5.292 (<0.001) −1.506 (0.137)
Cluster7 right_parietal 0.151 (0.881) −5.303 (<0.001) −1.486 (0.142)
Cluster7 left_occipital −0.595 (0.554) −5.344 (<0.001) −1.541 (0.128)
Cluster7 right_occipital −1.219 (0.227) −5.441 (<0.001) −1.591 (0.117)
Cluster7 left_temporal 0.799 (0.427) −5.317 (<0.001) −1.458 (0.150)
Cluster7 right_temporal −0.336 (0.738) −5.311 (<0.001) −1.515 (0.135)
Cluster7 left_cingulate −0.572 (0.569) −5.323 (<0.001) −1.541 (0.128)
Cluster7 right_cingulate −2.241 (0.028) −5.312 (<0.001) −1.576 (0.120)
Cluster7 left_insula 0.989 (0.326) −5.335 (<0.001) −1.391 (0.169)
Cluster7 right_insula −0.234 (0.816) −5.307 (<0.001) −1.479 (0.144)

Table 5.

Summary statistics of the associations between Cogmed performance gain and BAGR, including age and sex using linear models. The reported p-values are uncorrected values, and no main effect of BAGR remained significant after FDR correction.

Models Bagr (t (p)) Age (t (p)) Sex (t (p))
Global
BAGR_all_T1 −0.383 (0.703) 0.497 (0.622) −0.900 (0.373)
BAGR_median 0.382 (0.704) 0.453 (0.652) −0.902 (0.371)
Regional
BAGR_left_frontal 2.399 (0.020) 0.285 (0.777) −1.146 (0.257)
BAGR_right_frontal 0.241 (0.810) 0.479 (0.634) −0.913 (0.366)
BAGR_left_parietal 2.037 (0.047) 0.425 (0.673) −0.876 (0.385)
BAGR_right_parietal 0.264 (0.793) 0.493 (0.625) −0.899 (0.373)
BAGR_left_occipital −0.083 (0.934) 0.489 (0.627) −0.899 (0.373)
BAGR_right_occipital 0.392 (0.697) 0.502 (0.618) −0.909 (0.368)
BAGR_left_temporal 0.159 (0.875) 0.484 (0.631) −0.900 (0.372)
BAGR_right_temporal 0.963 (0.340) 0.452 (0.653) −0.925 (0.359)
BAGR_left_cingulate 0.750 (0.457) 0.438 (0.663) −0.883 (0.381)
BAGR_right_cingulate 1.150 (0.256) 0.436 (0.664) −1.053 (0.297)
BAGR_left_insula 0.458 (0.649) 0.487 (0.629) −0.882 (0.382)
BAGR_right_insula 0.693 (0.491) 0.452 (0.653) −0.952 (0.345)

Table 6 shows summary statistics from the linear mixed effects models testing for longitudinal associations between cognitive performance and BAGR, including age and sex in the models. The analyses revealed robust main effects of session and age, indicating increasing performance during the course of the intervention, and generally lower performance with increasing age. Beyond this, we did not find any significant associations between performance and BAGR, nor BAGR by time interaction after FDR correction for multiple comparisons. Amongst the non-significant findings, the five strongest associations were found between the left occipital and Digits, the left frontal and Cube and 3D Cube, the left parietal and Hidden, and the right insula and Twist and the eight strongest BAGR by time interactions were found between the left occipital and Digits, the left frontal and Cube and 3D Cube, the left parietal and Hidden, the right insula and Twist and Rotating, and the left temporal and Cube and 3D Cube.

Table 6.

Summary statistics from the linear mixed effects models testing for associations between cognitive performance and BAGR by time interaction, including age and sex in the models. The reported p-values are uncorrected values, and no main effect of BAGR nor BAGR by time interaction remained significant after FDR correction.

Test Models Bagr (t (p)) Session (t (p)) Age (t (p)) Sex (t (p)) Bagr:session (t (p))
Grid BAGR_all_T1 −0.478 (0.635) 10.704 (<0.001) −3.538 (0.001) −1.064 (0.292) 0.338 (0.737)
Sort BAGR_all_T1 −1.409 (0.165) 10.097 (<0.001) −2.642 (0.011) −0.573 (0.569) 1.157 (0.253)
Digits BAGR_all_T1 0.739 (0.463) 10.198 (<0.001) −6.649 (<0.001) −2.507 (0.015) −0.685 (0.496)
Cube BAGR_all_T1 0.053 (0.958) 15.279 (<0.001) −3.047 (0.004) −1.384 (0.172) −0.211 (0.833)
Hidden BAGR_all_T1 −0.213 (0.832) 9.487 (<0.001) −5.612 (<0.001) −2.764 (0.008) 0.929 (0.357)
Twist BAGR_all_T1 −0.230 (0.819) 6.202 (<0.001) −3.813 (<0.001) −1.453 (0.152) 0.088 (0.930)
3D Cube BAGR_all_T1 1.444 (0.155) 6.369 (<0.001) −4.353 (<0.001) −1.574 (0.122) −1.631 (0.109)
Rotating BAGR_all_T1 0.198 (0.844) 4.307 (<0.001) −3.984 (<0.001) −1.508 (0.138) −0.349 (0.729)
Grid BAGR_median −0.507 (0.615) 10.840 (<0.001) −3.521 (0.001) −1.050 (0.299) 0.116 (0.908)
Sort BAGR_median −1.288 (0.204) 10.063 (<0.001) −2.648 (0.011) −0.610 (0.544) 0.968 (0.338)
Digits BAGR_median 1.091 (0.280) 10.377 (<0.001) −6.590 (<0.001) −2.472 (0.017) −1.165 (0.250)
Cube BAGR_median 0.157 (0.876) 15.422 (<0.001) −3.043 (0.004) −1.376 (0.175) −0.533 (0.596)
Hidden BAGR_median 0.357 (0.722) 9.768 (<0.001) −5.420 (<0.001) −2.665 (0.010) 0.286 (0.776)
Twist BAGR_median 0.520 (0.605) 6.203 (<0.001) −3.816 (<0.001) −1.505 (0.138) −0.614 (0.542)
3D Cube BAGR_median 1.345 (0.185) 6.454 (<0.001) −4.335 (<0.001) −1.522 (0.134) −1.808 (0.077)
Rotating BAGR_median 0.894 (0.376) 4.327 (<0.001) −3.982 (<0.001) −1.538 (0.130) −1.072 (0.289)
Grid BAGR_left_frontal 0.298 (0.767) 10.94 (<0.001) −3.574 (0.001) −1.165 (0.249) −0.981 (0.331)
Sort BAGR_left_frontal −0.020 (0.984) 10.125 (<0.001) −2.661 (0.011) −0.749 (0.457) −0.624 (0.536)
Digits BAGR_left_frontal 1.800 (0.078) 10.527 (<0.001) −6.643 (<0.001) −2.450 (0.018) −1.942 (0.058)
Cube BAGR_left_frontal 2.423 (0.019) 16.704 (<0.001) −3.086 (0.003) −1.439 (0.156) −3.187 (0.003)
Hidden BAGR_left_frontal 1.343 (0.186) 9.835 (<0.001) −5.277 (<0.001) −2.502 (0.016) −1.278 (0.207)
Twist BAGR_left_frontal 1.333 (0.189) 6.316 (<0.001) −3.850 (<0.001) −1.524 (0.134) −1.667 (0.102)
3D Cube BAGR_left_frontal 2.274 (0.027) 6.407 (<0.001) −4.419 (<0.001) −1.537 (0.130) −2.380 (0.021)
Rotating BAGR_left_frontal 1.496 (0.141) 4.297 (<0.001) −4.025 (<0.001) −1.529 (0.132) −1.526 (0.133)
Grid BAGR_right_frontal 0.024 (0.981) 10.660 (<0.001) −3.554 (0.001) −1.117 (0.269) −0.019 (0.985)
Sort BAGR_right_frontal 0.417 (0.679) 10.034 (<0.001) −2.631 (0.011) −0.681 (0.499) −0.187 (0.853)
Digits BAGR_right_frontal −0.428 (0.671) 10.187 (<0.001) −6.666 (<0.001) −2.475 (0.017) 0.324 (0.747)
Cube BAGR_right_frontal 1.340 (0.186) 15.643 (<0.001) −3.027 (0.004) −1.387 (0.171) −1.412 (0.164)
Hidden BAGR_right_frontal −0.501 (0.618) 9.721 (<0.001) −5.308 (<0.001) −2.535 (0.014) 0.592 (0.556)
Twist BAGR_right_frontal 0.012 (0.990) 6.264 (<0.001) −3.789 (<0.001) −1.503 (0.139) −0.320 (0.750)
3D Cube BAGR_right_frontal 1.279 (0.207) 6.294 (<0.001) −4.351 (<0.001) −1.513 (0.136) −1.269 (0.210)
Rotating BAGR_right_frontal 0.330 (0.743) 4.298 (<0.001) −3.980 (<0.001) −1.526 (0.133) −0.464 (0.645)
Grid BAGR_left_parietal 0.879 (0.384) 10.990 (<0.001) −3.536 (0.001) −1.094 (0.279) −1.331 (0.189)
Sort BAGR_left_parietal −0.197 (0.845) 10.109 (<0.001) −2.665 (0.010) −0.661 (0.511) −0.344 (0.733)
Digits BAGR_left_parietal 0.968 (0.338) 10.668 (<0.001) −6.620 (<0.001) −2.417 (0.019) −1.482 (0.145)
Cube BAGR_left_parietal 0.075 (0.941) 15.499 (<0.001) −3.044 (0.004) −1.377 (0.174) −0.514 (0.610)
Hidden BAGR_left_parietal 2.051 (0.046) 10.062 (<0.001) −5.262 (<0.001) −2.544 (0.014) −2.060 (0.045)
Twist BAGR_left_parietal 1.535 (0.131) 6.406 (<0.001) −3.804 (<0.001) −1.496 (0.141) −1.775 (0.082)
3D Cube BAGR_left_parietal 0.291 (0.772) 6.561 (<0.001) −4.398 (<0.001) −1.493 (0.141) −1.054 (0.297)
Rotating BAGR_left_parietal 1.225 (0.227) 4.423 (<0.001) −3.986 (<0.001) −1.521 (0.134) −1.515 (0.136)
Grid BAGR_right_parietal −0.990 (0.327) 10.795 (<0.001) −3.558 (0.001) −1.072 (0.289) 0.661 (0.512)
Sort BAGR_right_parietal −0.489 (0.627) 9.883 (<0.001) −2.661 (0.010) −0.673 (0.504) 0.248 (0.805)
Digits BAGR_right_parietal 0.202 (0.841) 10.461 (<0.001) −6.620 (<0.001) −2.432 (0.019) −0.712 (0.480)
Cube BAGR_right_parietal −0.327 (0.745) 15.394 (<0.001) −3.058 (0.004) −1.385 (0.172) −0.116 (0.908)
Hidden BAGR_right_parietal −0.450 (0.655) 9.734 (<0.001) −5.344 (<0.001) −2.544 (0.014) 0.817 (0.418)
Twist BAGR_right_parietal −0.226 (0.822) 6.180 (<0.001) −3.832 (<0.001) −1.487 (0.143) 0.086 (0.932)
3D Cube BAGR_right_parietal 0.994 (0.325) 6.366 (<0.001) −4.386 (<0.001) −1.554 (0.126) −1.489 (0.143)
Rotating BAGR_right_parietal 0.625 (0.535) 4.309 (<0.001) −3.984 (<0.001) −1.543 (0.129) −0.716 (0.477)
Grid BAGR_left_occipital −1.717 (0.092) 11.041 (<0.001) −3.487 (0.001) −1.095 (0.279) 1.794 (0.079)
Sort BAGR_left_occipital −0.489 (0.627) 10.050 (<0.001) −2.561 (0.014) −0.664 (0.510) 0.282 (0.779)
Digits BAGR_left_occipital 2.399 (0.020) 10.716 (<0.001) −6.684 (<0.001) −2.480 (0.016) −2.383 (0.021)
Cube BAGR_left_occipital −0.202 (0.840) 15.254 (<0.001) −3.083 (0.003) −1.423 (0.161) 0.473 (0.638)
Hidden BAGR_left_occipital 0.028 (0.978) 9.630 (<0.001) −5.413 (<0.001) −2.599 (0.012) 0.407 (0.685)
Twist BAGR_left_occipital −0.265 (0.792) 6.197 (<0.001) −3.782 (<0.001) −1.483 (0.144) 0.144 (0.886)
3D Cube BAGR_left_occipital 0.340 (0.735) 6.246 (<0.001) −4.33 (<0.001) −1.515 (0.136) −0.510 (0.612)
Rotating BAGR_left_occipital −0.718 (0.476) 4.271 (<0.001) −4.005 (<0.001) −1.54 (0.130) 0.899 (0.373)
Grid BAGR_right_occipital −0.632 (0.530) 10.557 (<0.001) −3.532 (0.001) −1.061 (0.294) 0.236 (0.814)
Sort BAGR_right_occipital −1.314 (0.195) 9.815 (<0.001) −2.613 (0.012) −0.621 (0.537) 1.026 (0.310)
Digits BAGR_right_occipital 0.595 (0.555) 10.645 (<0.001) −6.638 (<0.001) −2.400 (0.020) −1.355 (0.182)
Cube BAGR_right_occipital −0.982 (0.331) 15.919 (<0.001) −2.993 (0.004) −1.297 (0.200) −0.201 (0.842)
Hidden BAGR_right_occipital −0.265 (0.792) 9.655 (<0.001) −5.254 (<0.001) −2.478 (0.016) −0.063 (0.950)
Twist BAGR_right_occipital −0.578 (0.566) 6.114 (<0.001) −3.808 (<0.001) −1.433 (0.158) 0.065 (0.948)
3D Cube BAGR_right_occipital 0.311 (0.757) 6.414 (<0.001) −4.349 (<0.001) −1.470 (0.148) −1.135 (0.262)
Rotating BAGR_right_occipital −0.448 (0.656) 4.202 (<0.001) −4.007 (<0.001) −1.480 (0.145) −0.052 (0.959)
Grid BAGR_left_temporal 0.192 (0.849) 10.683 (<0.001) −3.572 (0.001) −1.073 (0.288) −0.517 (0.608)
Sort BAGR_left_temporal −0.300 (0.766) 9.878 (<0.001) −2.656 (0.011) −0.687 (0.495) 0.189 (0.851)
Digits BAGR_left_temporal 0.733 (0.467) 10.26 (<0.001) −6.645 (<0.001) −2.469 (0.017) −0.552 (0.583)
Cube BAGR_left_temporal 1.224 (0.227) 16.324 (<0.001) −3.078 (0.003) −1.293 (0.202) −2.241 (0.030)
Hidden BAGR_left_temporal 0.367 (0.715) 9.897 (<0.001) −5.359 (<0.001) −2.665 (0.010) 0.33 (0.743)
Twist BAGR_left_temporal 0.160 (0.874) 6.176 (<0.001) −3.829 (<0.001) −1.492 (0.142) −0.18 (0.858)
3D Cube BAGR_left_temporal 1.516 (0.136) 6.551 (<0.001) −4.407 (<0.001) −1.412 (0.164) −2.271 (0.028)
Rotating BAGR_left_temporal 0.480 (0.633) 4.297 (<0.001) −3.988 (<0.001) −1.527 (0.133) −0.403 (0.689)
Grid BAGR_right_temporal 0.116 (0.908) 10.638 (<0.001) −3.552 (0.001) −1.120 (0.268) −0.128 (0.899)
Sort BAGR_right_temporal −0.530 (0.599) 9.997 (<0.001) −2.660 (0.011) −0.679 (0.500) 0.684 (0.497)
Digits BAGR_right_temporal 1.236 (0.222) 10.329 (<0.001) −6.602 (<0.001) −2.513 (0.015) −1.445 (0.155)
Cube BAGR_right_temporal 0.791 (0.433) 15.444 (<0.001) −3.033 (0.004) −1.417 (0.162) −1.110 (0.272)
Hidden BAGR_right_temporal 1.014 (0.315) 9.635 (<0.001) −5.331 (<0.001) −2.620 (0.011) −0.902 (0.372)
Twist BAGR_right_temporal −0.183 (0.856) 6.233 (<0.001) −3.789 (<0.001) −1.456 (0.151) −0.011 (0.991)
3D Cube BAGR_right_temporal 1.417 (0.163) 6.186 (<0.001) −4.397 (<0.001) −1.630 (0.109) −1.332 (0.189)
Rotating BAGR_right_temporal −0.168 (0.867) 4.288 (<0.001) −3.991 (<0.001) −1.506 (0.138) 0.077 (0.939)
Grid BAGR_left_cingulate 0.182 (0.856) 10.635 (<0.001) −3.531 (0.001) −1.076 (0.287) −0.369 (0.714)
Sort BAGR_left_cingulate 0.667 (0.508) 9.662 (<0.001) −2.683 (0.010) −0.746 (0.459) −0.481 (0.633)
Digits BAGR_left_cingulate 0.318 (0.752) 10.09 (<0.001) −6.641 (<0.001) −2.440 (0.018) −0.345 (0.731)
Cube BAGR_left_cingulate −1.008 (0.318) 15.291 (<0.001) −3.076 (0.003) −1.441 (0.156) 1.259 (0.214)
Hidden BAGR_left_cingulate −0.726 (0.472) 9.718 (<0.001) −5.376 (<0.001) −2.661 (0.010) 1.235 (0.223)
Twist BAGR_left_cingulate −0.550 (0.585) 6.115 (<0.001) −3.860 (<0.001) −1.546 (0.128) 0.737 (0.465)
3D Cube BAGR_left_cingulate 0.251 (0.803) 6.225 (<0.001) −4.348 (<0.001) −1.474 (0.146) −0.504 (0.617)
Rotating BAGR_left_cingulate 0.524 (0.603) 4.161 (<0.001) −4.013 (<0.001) −1.546 (0.128) −0.453 (0.652)
Grid BAGR_right_cingulate −0.486 (0.629) 10.575 (<0.001) −3.564 (0.001) −1.112 (0.271) 0.353 (0.725)
Sort BAGR_right_cingulate 0.034 (0.973) 9.922 (<0.001) −2.625 (0.012) −0.706 (0.484) 0.099 (0.921)
Digits BAGR_right_cingulate 0.266 (0.792) 10.123 (<0.001) −6.658 (<0.001) −2.447 (0.018) −0.333 (0.741)
Cube BAGR_right_cingulate −0.263 (0.794) 15.110 (<0.001) −3.116 (0.003) −1.39 (0.171) −0.248 (0.805)
Hidden BAGR_right_cingulate 0.390 (0.698) 9.694 (<0.001) −5.264 (<0.001) −2.506 (0.015) −0.424 (0.674)
Twist BAGR_right_cingulate 0.684 (0.497) 6.138 (<0.001) −3.846 (<0.001) −1.493 (0.142) −0.737 (0.465)
3D Cube BAGR_right_cingulate 1.072 (0.289) 6.119 (<0.001) −4.48 (<0.001) −1.511 (0.137) −1.521 (0.135)
Rotating BAGR_right_cingulate 0.262 (0.794) 4.352 (<0.001) −3.955 (<0.001) −1.533 (0.131) −0.056 (0.956)
Grid BAGR_left_insula 0.704 (0.485) 10.678 (<0.001) −3.568 (0.001) −1.153 (0.254) −0.543 (0.590)
Sort BAGR_left_insula −1.289 (0.204) 10.083 (<0.001) −2.639 (0.011) −0.663 (0.510) 1.223 (0.227)
Digits BAGR_left_insula 1.611 (0.114) 10.372 (<0.001) −6.716 (<0.001) −2.564 (0.013) −1.274 (0.209)
Cube BAGR_left_insula 0.282 (0.779) 15.283 (<0.001) −3.064 (0.004) −1.422 (0.161) −0.174 (0.863)
Hidden BAGR_left_insula 0.406 (0.686) 9.749 (<0.001) −5.526 (<0.001) −2.753 (0.008) 0.280 (0.781)
Twist BAGR_left_insula 0.570 (0.571) 6.193 (<0.001) −3.848 (<0.001) −1.555 (0.126) −0.323 (0.748)
3D Cube BAGR_left_insula 1.625 (0.111) 6.310 (<0.001) −4.350 (<0.001) −1.510 (0.137) −1.862 (0.069)
Rotating BAGR_left_insula 1.102 (0.276) 4.277 (<0.001) −3.982 (<0.001) −1.530 (0.132) −1.143 (0.258)
Grid BAGR_right_insula −0.205 (0.838) 10.677 (<0.001) −3.546 (0.001) −1.107 (0.273) 0.138 (0.890)
Sort BAGR_right_insula −0.291 (0.772) 9.912 (<0.001) −2.637 (0.011) −0.687 (0.495) 0.156 (0.877)
Digits BAGR_right_insula −0.749 (0.457) 10.191 (<0.001) −6.797 (<0.001) −2.417 (0.019) 1.071 (0.289)
Cube BAGR_right_insula −0.928 (0.358) 15.415 (<0.001) −3.053 (0.004) −1.372 (0.176) 0.779 (0.440)
Hidden BAGR_right_insula 0.486 (0.629) 9.671 (<0.001) −5.345 (<0.001) −2.560 (0.013) −0.252 (0.802)
Twist BAGR_right_insula 2.631 (0.011) 6.561 (<0.001) −3.843 (<0.001) −1.656 (0.104) −2.528 (0.015)
3D Cube BAGR_right_insula −0.859 (0.394) 6.227 (<0.001) −4.392 (<0.001) −1.486 (0.143) 0.889 (0.378)
Rotating BAGR_right_insula 1.919 (0.061) 4.470 (<0.001) −3.970 (<0.001) −1.635 (0.108) −2.041 (0.047)

4. Discussion

Cognitive deficits are important predictors for outcome, independence and quality of life in stroke survivors, and computerized cognitive training has been suggested among the candidate interventions that may alleviate them. However, the lack of widely adapted tools for stratification, outcome prediction and treatment monitoring prevent an adequate assessment of the effectiveness of such training. Advanced brain MRI provides various candidate markers for disease monitoring and outcome prediction, integrating lesion specific information and characterization of the integrity of the unaffected parts of the brain, which is highly relevant for cognitive functions and long-term outcome. Here, we used brain age prediction based on brain morphometry and machine learning to test the hypotheses that patients with younger-appearing brains relative to their chronological age would show preserved cognitive function and show more beneficial treatment response compared to patients with relatively older-appearing brains.

Based on the notion that brain age prediction offers a sensitive summary measure of brain integrity, we first tested the prediction accuracy and then the reliability in a longitudinal context. The estimated performance of the 13 trained models within the training set using a 10-fold cross-validation procedure suggested a relatively good model fit, with correlations ranging from 0.84 to 0.61 for the model based on all T1 features and for the right cingulate model respectively. Further, the models estimated brain age on the test sample also suggested an acceptable model fit with some regional differences in performance with MAE ranging from 5.50 to 8.80 for the most comprehensive model based on all T1 features and for the model based on right cingulate features respectively. In addition, the age estimation based on the median of the 12 regional models achieved the highest performance with a Pearson correlation of r = 0.58 (CI = 0.40–0.72, MAE=4.27).

In line with a recent implementation in patients with MS (Hogestol et al., 2019), our results demonstrated high reliability across all timepoints for the global and regional models with ICC ranging from 0.70 to 0.86 for the right occipital and the right parietal models respectively. The brain age estimation based on the median of the 12 regional models was amongst the most reliable models with an ICC of 0.89. across the two baselines and 0.83 across all timepoints, outperforming the estimation based on all T1 features.

A particular challenge in clinical neuroimaging is that lesions may directly interfere with automated processing and brain segmentations at the site of the lesions. Here, to test for the influence of brain lesions on BAGR estimation and reliability, we used outlier detection on the individual features level to identify extreme observations and replace those extremes by more realistic or probable values by means of imputation using predictive mean matching. Comparisons between brain age from the raw and imputed feature sets revealed only minimal influence of outliers on both brain age estimates and reliability. Importantly, this suggests that our model predictions are robust to gross segmentation errors caused by the lesions, which supports the feasibility of automated brain age prediction in patient groups with brain disorders and lesions.

To test the hypotheses that patients with low BAGR at baseline show better cognitive function and a more positive treatment response, we used seven summary scores derived from a set of neuropsychological and computerized tests at baseline and the performance gain during the course of the intensive training period. After corrections for multiple comparisons, linear models revealed no significant associations between BAGR and summary scores from the baseline assessment or performance gain. In line with a few previous studies (Boyle et al., 2019; Høgestøl et al., 2019), region specific models revealed putative associations between summary scores for executive control and speed, working memory and the right cingulate BAGR, as well as, attentional control and speed and the right temporal BAGR, suggesting better cognitive performance with lower BAGR. In addition, regional analysis revealed non-significant putative associations between performance gain and left frontal and left parietal BAGR, indicating more positive treatment response for patients with lower BAGR.

In order to assess if cognitive gains in response to intensive cognitive training are reflected in longitudinal changes in brain age during the course of the intervention, we tested for interactions between BAGR and session on cognitive performance using linear mixed effects models. Our analyses revealed a few putative associations between BAGR and Cogmed performance, and also session by BAGR interactions on cognitive performance in a longitudinal context. However, none of these associations and interactions remained significant after correction for multiple comparisons, and the direction of the associations, if any, did not seem to converge. Hence, our results did not provide support for our hypothesis that cognitive gains would be reflected in longitudinal changes in brain age during the course of the intervention.

While this study does not provide strong support for the utility of brain age estimation as a sensitive measure for cognitive reserve and potential predictor for training outcomes in stroke patients, our results suggest that region specific brain age estimations are reliable and might provide more information than global brain age as a measure of brain integrity. Future studies are needed to confirm the putative associations between summary scores for executive control and speed (cluster 5) and working memory (cluster 7) and the right cingulate BAGR, as well as summary scores for attentional control and speed (cluster 4) and the right temporal BAGR; in addition to the weak associations found between the left frontal and left parietal brain age with the rate of cognitive improvement. In general, these findings are in line with our recent study suggesting that, by capturing distinct measures of brain aging, tissue specific age prediction models might better inform us about the individual determinants and heterogeneity of the aging brain compared to models collapsing several brain compartments (Richard et al., 2018).

The following methodological considerations should be taken into account while interpreting the current results. Although our patient sample was highly heterogenous in terms of localization, extent of the lesions and stroke etiologies, most of them had small lesions. Further studies are needed to confirm that the current approach for brain age prediction based on automated brain morphometry is also feasible for patients with larger lesions, and how stroke etiologies can impact BAGR and cognitive improvement, for instance progressive vascular disease (small vessel and large vessel disease) may affect cognition prior to the stroke (Ihle-Hansen et al., 2014). In addition, the patients included in this study suffered from mild to moderate stroke (NIHSS ≤ 7 at hospital discharge), representing a high functioning group with mild cognitive deficits and better overall prognosis, limiting the generalizability of our findings to more severe populations. Although previous studies have reported links between cognitive function and brain age in healthy controls, it is conceivable that the current associations would be stronger had we sampled from a wider distribution in terms of stroke severity and cognitive symptoms. The lack of control group prevents us from distinguishing between the time-related changes and training-related changes (Kolskår et al., 2019), and, although tailored to the current cognitive intervention regime, a longer interval between time points might have increased sensitivity to detect relevant associations between cognitive changes and changes in brain age.

In conclusion, reliable and non-invasive markers of brain health and cognitive function are needed to help improving the current treatment programs targeting cognitive deficits following stroke and other brain disorders. Although the results do not support an immediate clinical utility of brain age prediction in highly functioning stroke patients, we speculate that using region specific brain age model as opposed to reducing the whole brain to one summary score might be as reliable and more informative potential biomarkers of brain integrity and health. Importantly, our study supports the feasibility of automated brain age prediction in patient groups with brain disorders and lesions by highlighting the high reliability and minimal impact of lesions on brain age estimations.

Acknowledgements and funding

This study was supported by the Norwegian Extra Foundation for Health and Rehabilitation (2015/FO5146), the Research Council of Norway (249795, 248238), the South-Eastern Norway Regional Health Authority (2014097, 2015044, 2015073, 2018076), Sunnaas Rehabilitation Hospital, the Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, and the European Research Council under the European Union's Horizon 2020 research and Innovation program (ERC StG, Grant 802998).

Declaration of Competing Interest

None

Footnotes

Supplementary material associated with this article can be found, in the online version, at doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2019.102159.

Contributor Information

Geneviève Richard, Email: genevieve.richard@medisin.uio.no.

Lars T. Westlye, Email: l.t.westlye@psykologi.uio.no.

Appendix. Supplementary materials

mmc1.docx (11.9MB, docx)

References

  1. Au J., Katz B., Buschkuehl M., Bunarjo K., Senger T., Zabel C., Jaeggi S.M., Jonides J. Enhancing working memory training with transcranial direct current stimulation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2016;28:1419–1432. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00979. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Barbay M., Diouf M., Roussel M., Godefroy O., group Gs Systematic review and meta-analysis of prevalence in post-stroke neurocognitive disorders in hospital-based studies. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 2018;46:322–334. doi: 10.1159/000492920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Barker Collo S., Feigin V.L., Parag V., Lawes C.M., Senior H. Auckland stroke outcomes study. Part 2: cognition and functional outcomes 5 years poststroke. Neurology. 2010;75:1608–1616. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181fb44c8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Benjamini Y., Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate - a Practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B-Methodol. 1995;57:289–300. [Google Scholar]
  5. Berthier M.L., Garcia-Casares N., Walsh S.F., Nabrozidis A., Ruiz de Mier R.J., Green C., Davila G., Gutierrez A., Pulvermuller F. Recovery from post-stroke aphasia: lessons from brain imaging and implications for rehabilitation and biological treatments. Discov. Med. 2011;12:275–289. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Boyle R., Jollans L., Rueda-Delgado L.M., Rizzo R., Yener G.G., McMorrow J.P., Knight S.P., Carey D., Robertson I.H., Emek-Savaş D.D., Stern Y., Kenny R.A., Whelan R. Brain-predicted age difference score is related to specific cognitive functions: a multi-site replication analysis. bioRxiv. 2019 doi: 10.1007/s11682-020-00260-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bundesen C. A theory of visual attention. Psychol. Rev. 1990;97:523–547. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.97.4.523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bundesen C., Habekost T. Oxford University Press; 2008. Principles of Visual Attention: Linking Mind and Brain. [Google Scholar]
  9. Buuren S., Groothuis-Oudshoorn K. mice: multivariate imputation by chained equations inR. J. Stat. Softw. 2011;45 [Google Scholar]
  10. Chambers J.M., Hastie T.J. In: Statistical Models in S. Wadsworth & Brooks/Cole; Pacific Grove, California: 1992. [Google Scholar]
  11. Chen T., Guestrin C. 2016. XGBoost: a scalable tree boosting system. [Google Scholar]; arXiv:1603.02754v3.
  12. Chen T., He T., Benesty M., Khotilovich V., Tang Y. 2017. xgboost: extreme gradient boosting. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=xgboost. R package version 0.6-4.
  13. Cole J.H., Franke K. Predicting age using neuroimaging: innovative brain ageing biomarkers. Trends. Neurosci. 2017 doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2017.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cole J.H., Marioni R.E., Harris S.E., Deary I.J. Brain age and other bodily ‘ages’: implications for neuropsychiatry. Mol. Psychiatry. 2018 doi: 10.1038/s41380-018-0098-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cumming T.B., Brodtmann A., Darby D., Bernhardt J. The importance of cognition to quality of life after stroke. J. Psychosom. Res. 2014;77:374–379. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2014.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dale A.M., Fischl B., Sereno M.I. Cortical surface-based analysis - I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage. 1999;9:179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Delis D.C., Kaplan E., Kramer J.H. Harcourt Assessment Company; San Antonio, TX: 2001. Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System: Technical Manual. [Google Scholar]
  18. Delis D.C., Kramer J.H., Kaplan E., Ober B.A. Edition (CVLT-II) Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 2000. California Verbal Learning Test-Second. [Google Scholar]
  19. Dyrholm M., Kyllingsbæk S., Espeseth T., Bundesen C. Generalizing parametric models by introducing trial-by-trial parameter variability: the case of tva. J. Math. Psychol. 2011;55:416–429. [Google Scholar]
  20. Engvig A., Fjell A.M., Westlye L.T., Moberget T., Sundseth O., Larsen V.A., Walhovd K.B. Effects of memory training on cortical thickness in the elderly. Neuroimage. 2010;52:1667–1676. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Feigin V.L., Forouzanfar M.H., Krishnamurthi R., Mensah G.A., Connor M., Bennett D.A., Moran A.E., Sacco R.L., Anderson L., Truelsen T., O'Donnell M., Venketasubramanian N., Barker-Collo S., Lawes C.M.M., Wang W., Shinohara Y., Witt E., Ezzati M., Naghavi M., Murray C. Global and regional burden of stroke during 1990–2010: findings from the global burden of disease study 2010. The Lancet. 2014;383:245–255. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(13)61953-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Feigin V.L., Norrving B., Mensah G.A. Global burden of stroke. Circ. Res. 2017;120:439–448. doi: 10.1161/CIRCRESAHA.116.308413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Fox J., Weisberg S. 2nd. Sage; Thousand Oaks, CA: 2011. An {R} Companion to Applied Regression. editor. [Google Scholar]
  24. Franke K., Luders E., May A., Wilke M., Gaser C. Brain maturation: predicting individual Brainage in children and adolescents using structural MRI. Neuroimage. 2012;63:1305–1312. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Franke K., Ziegler G., Kloppel S., Gaser C., Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging I. Estimating the age of healthy subjects from T1-weighted MRI scans using kernel methods: exploring the influence of various parameters. Neuroimage. 2010;50:883–892. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Glasser M.F., Coalson T.S., Robinson E.C., Hacker C.D., Harwell J., Yacoub E., Ugurbil K., Andersson J., Beckmann C.F., Jenkinson M., Smith S.M., Van Essen D.C. A multi-modal parcellation of human cerebral cortex. Nature. 2016;536:171–178. doi: 10.1038/nature18933. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Guggisberg A.G., Koch P.J., Hummel F.C., Buetefisch C. Brain networks and their relevance for stroke rehabilitation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2019 doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Haacke C., Althaus A., Spottke A., Siebert U., Back T., Dodel R. Long-term outcome after stroke: evaluating health-related quality of life using utility measurements. Stroke. 2006;37:193–198. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000196990.69412.fb. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hogestol E.A., Kaufmann T., Nygaard G.O., Beyer M.K., Sowa P., Nordvik J.E., Kolskar K., Richard G., Andreassen O.A., Harbo H.F., Westlye L.T. Cross-Sectional and longitudinal MRI brain scans reveal accelerated brain aging in multiple sclerosis. Front. Neurol. 2019;10:450. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.00450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Høgestøl E.A., Nygaard G.O., Emhjellen P.E., Kaufmann T., Beyer M.K., Sowa P., Andreassen O.A., Celius E.G., Landrø N.I., Westlye L.T., Harbo H.F. 2019. Brain age estimation is a sensitive marker of processing speed in the early course of multiple sclerosis. bioRxiv. 10.1101/651521.
  31. Hope T.M.H., Seghier M.L., Leff A.P., Price C.J. Predicting outcome and recovery after stroke with lesions extracted from MRI images. Neuroimage-Clin. 2013;2:424–433. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Ihle-Hansen H., Thommessen B., Fagerland M.W., Oksengard A.R., Wyller T.B., Engedal K., Fure B. Multifactorial vascular risk factor intervention to prevent cognitive impairment after stroke and TIA: a 12-month randomized controlled trial. Int. J. Stroke. 2014;9:932–938. doi: 10.1111/j.1747-4949.2012.00928.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Jacova C., Pearce L.A., Costello R., McClure L.A., Holliday S.L., Hart R.G., Benavente O.R. Cognitive impairment in lacunar strokes: the SPS3 trial. Ann. Neurol. 2012;72:351–362. doi: 10.1002/ana.23733. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kalaria R.N., Akinyemi R., Ihara M. Stroke injury, cognitive impairment and vascular dementia. Biochim. Biophys. Acta. 2016;1862:915–925. doi: 10.1016/j.bbadis.2016.01.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kaufmann T., van der Meer D., Doan N.T., Schwarz E., Lund M.J., Agartz I., Alnaes D., Barch D.M., Baur-Streubel R., Bertolino A., Bettella F., Beyer M.K., Boen E., Borgwardt S., Brandt C.L., Buitelaar J., Celius E.G., Cervenka S., Conzelmann A., Cordova-Palomera A., Dale A.M., de Quervain D.J.F., Carlo P., Djurovic S., Dorum E.S., Eisenacher S., Elvsashagen T., Espeseth T., Fatouros-Bergman H., Flyckt L., Franke B., Frei O., Haatveit B., Haberg A.K., Harbo H.F., Hartman C.A., Heslenfeld D., Hoekstra P.J., Hogestol E.A., Jernigan T.L., Jonassen R., Jonsson E.G., Karolinska Schizophrenia P., Kirsch P., Kloszewska I., Kolskar K.K., Landro N.I., Hellard S., Lesch K.P., Lovestone S., Lundervold A., Lundervold A.J., Maglanoc L.A., Malt U.F., Mecocci P., Melle I., Meyer-Lindenberg A., Moberget T., Norbom L.B., Nordvik J.E., Nyberg L., Oosterlaan J., Papalino M., Papassotiropoulos A., Pauli P., Pergola G., Persson K., Richard G., Rokicki J., Sanders A.M., Selbaek G., Shadrin A.A., Smeland O.B., Soininen H., Sowa P., Steen V.M., Tsolaki M., Ulrichsen K.M., Vellas B., Wang L., Westman E., Ziegler G.C., Zink M., Andreassen O.A., Westlye L.T. Common brain disorders are associated with heritable patterns of apparent aging of the brain. Nat. Neurosci. 2019;22:1617–1623. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0471-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kolskår K.K., Richard G., Alnæs D., Dørum E.S., Sanders A.-.M., Ulrichsen K.M., Sánchez J.M., Ihle-Hansen H., Nordvik J.E., Westlye L.T. 2019. A randomized, double blind study of computerized cognitive training in combination with transcranial direct currentstimulation (tDCS) in stroke patients–utility of fMRI as marker for training outcome. 10.1101/603985.
  37. Le T.T., Kuplicki R.T., McKinney B.A., Yeh H.W., Thompson W.K., Paulus M.P., Tulsa I. A nonlinear simulation framework supports adjusting for age when analyzing Brainage. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2018;10:317. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Moss A., Nicholas M. Language rehabilitation in chronic aphasia and time postonset: a review of single-subject data. Stroke. 2006;37:3043–3051. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000249427.74970.15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Nakling A.E., Aarsland D., Naess H., Wollschlaeger D., Fladby T., Hofstad H., Wehling E. Cognitive deficits in chronic stroke patients: neuropsychological assessment, depression, and self-reports. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Dis. Extra. 2017;7:283–296. doi: 10.1159/000478851. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Nasreddine Z.S., Phillips N.A., Bedirian V., Charbonneau S., Whitehead V., Collin I., Cummings J.L., Chertkow H. The montreal cognitive assessment, MOCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2005;53:695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Patel M.D., Coshall C., Rudd A.G., Wolfe C.D.A. Cognitive impairment after stroke: clinical determinants and its associations with long-term stroke outcomes. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2002;50:700–706. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50165.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Pope P.A., Miall R.C. Task-specific facilitation of cognition by cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation of the cerebellum. Brain Stimul. 2012;5:84–94. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.03.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. R Core Team . R Foundation for Statistical Computing; Vienna, Austria: 2017. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. [Google Scholar]
  44. Reuter M., Fischl B. Avoiding asymmetry-induced bias in longitudinal image processing. Neuroimage. 2011;57:19–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Reuter M., Schmansky N.J., Rosas H.D., Fischl B. Within-subject template estimation for unbiased longitudinal image analysis. Neuroimage. 2012;61:1402–1418. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Richard G., Kolskar K., Sanders A.M., Kaufmann T., Petersen A., Doan N.T., Monereo Sanchez J., Alnaes D., Ulrichsen K.M., Dorum E.S., Andreassen O.A., Nordvik J.E., Westlye L.T. Assessing distinct patterns of cognitive aging using tissue-specific brain age prediction based on diffusion tensor imaging and brain morphometry. PeerJ. 2018;6:e5908. doi: 10.7717/peerj.5908. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Shafto M.A., Tyler L.K., Dixon M., Taylor J.R., Rowe J.B., Cusack R., Calder A.J., Marslen-Wilson W.D., Duncan J., Dalgleish T., Henson R.N., Brayne C., Matthews F.E., Cam C.A.N. The Cambridge centre for ageing and neuroscience (Cam-CAN) study protocol: a cross-sectional, lifespan, multidisciplinary examination of healthy cognitive ageing. BMC Neurol. 2014;14:204. doi: 10.1186/s12883-014-0204-1. ARTN. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Taylor J.R., Williams N., Cusack R., Auer T., Shafto M.A., Dixon M., Tyler L.K., Cam C., Henson R.N. The Cambridge centre for ageing and neuroscience (Cam-CAN) data repository: structural and functional MRI, MEG, and cognitive data from a cross-sectional adult lifespan sample. Neuroimage. 2017;144:262–269. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.09.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Ulrichsen K.M., Alnæs D., Kolskår K.K., Richard G., Sanders A.-.M., Dørum E.S., Ihle-Hansen H., Pedersen M.L., Tornås S., Nordvik J.E., Westlye L.T. 2019. Dissecting the cognitive phenotype of post-stroke fatigue using drift diffusion modeling of sustained attention. 10.1101/582502. [DOI] [PubMed]
  50. Wechsler D. Psychological Corporation; 1999. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) [Google Scholar]
  51. Werden E., Cumming T., Li Q., Bird L., Veldsman M., Pardoe H.R., Jackson G., Donnan G.A., Brodtmann A. Structural MRI markers of brain aging early after ischemic stroke. Neurology. 2017;89:116–124. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Willer L., Pedersen P.M., Forchhammer H.B., Christensen H.K. Cognitive assessment at bedside for iPad: a preliminary validation of a novel cognitive test for stroke patients. Eur. Stroke J. 2016;1:294–301. doi: 10.1177/2396987316665233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Wolak M.E., Fairbairn D.J., Paulse Y.R. Guidelines for estimating repeatability. Method. Ecol. Evolut. 2012;3:129–137. [Google Scholar]
  54. Zhang Y., Chapman A.M., Plested M., Jackson D., Purroy F. The incidence, prevalence, and mortality of stroke in France, Germany, Italy, Spain, the UK, and the US: a literature review. Stroke Res. Treat. 2012;2012 doi: 10.1155/2012/436125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Zucchella C., Capone A., Codella V., Vecchione C., Buccino G., Sandrini G., Pierelli F., Bartolo M. Assessing and restoring cognitive functions early after stroke. Funct. Neurol. 2014;29:255–262. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

mmc1.docx (11.9MB, docx)

Articles from NeuroImage : Clinical are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES