Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: Appetite. 2019 Nov 18;146:104521. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.104521

Naturalistic, multimethod exploratory study of sleep duration and quality as predictors of dysregulated eating in youth with overweight and obesity

Andrea B Goldschmidt a,*, E Whitney Evans a,*, Jared M Saletin a, Katie O’Sullivan b, Dorit Koren c, Scott G Engel d, Alissa Haedt-Matt e
PMCID: PMC6954978  NIHMSID: NIHMS1544550  PMID: 31751632

Abstract

Although poor sleep has been found to adversely impact eating and weight regulation in youth, past research is limited by retrospective reporting and/or non-naturalistic designs. We investigated the feasibility of combining three momentary, ecologically valid approaches to assessing sleep and eating behavior, and associations between these constructs, among youth (aged 8-14y) with overweight/obesity (n=40). Participants completed 14 overlapping days of actigraphy assessment and smartphone-based ecological momentary assessment (EMA) of eating behavior, of which 3 days also included computerized, self-guided 24-hour dietary recall. Feasibility of completing measures concurrently was evaluated by generating frequencies of compliance. Associations between sleep indices and next-day eating behavior were examined via generalized estimating equations. Of 29 participants who provided EMA and 24-hour recall data that aligned with previous night actigraphy data, both EMA and sleep data were available on an average of 8.6 out of 14 possible days, and both 24-hour recall and sleep data on an average of 2.7 out of 3 possible days. Each additional hour of sleep was associated with consuming fewer calories from solid fats, alcohol, and added sugars (b=0.70; p=.04). Combining naturalistic, momentary assessments of sleep and eating behavior appears to be acceptable in youth. Larger experimental studies are needed to further understand associations between sleep parameters and eating behavior.

Keywords: Eating behavior, actigraphy, ecological momentary assessment, dietary recall, overweight, obesity

1. Introduction

Although causes of overweight/obesity are multifactorial, dysregulated eating behaviors leading to excess energy intake are a major factor in their onset and maintenance (French, Epstein, Jeffery, Blundell, & Wardle, 2012). One such behavior that is prevalent among youth with overweight/obesity is loss of control (LOC) eating, involving a sense that one cannot control what or how much one is eating irrespective of the amount of food consumed (Goldschmidt, 2017). In addition to cross-sectional associations with overweight/obesity, LOC eating is prospectively associated with weight gain/adiposity (Sonneville et al., 2013; Tanofsky-Kraff et al., 2006; Tanofsky-Kraff et al., 2009) and suboptimal psychosocial functioning (Goldschmidt et al., 2008; Schluter, Schmidt, Kittel, Tetzlaff, & Hilbert, 2016; Tanofsky-Kraff et al., 2011; Tanofsky-Kraff et al., 2004), and may undermine weight control treatment (Goldschmidt, Khoury, et al., 2018; Sysko et al., 2012; Wildes et al., 2010). Therefore, understanding factors that contribute to occurrence of LOC eating episodes may inform treatments targeting excess weight gain in children.

Poor sleep quality and/or quantity (e.g., difficulties initiating sleep, sleep fragmentation/nighttime awakenings, short sleep duration, and other constructs) have been associated with an increased risk for pediatric overweight and obesity in multiple cross-sectional and prospective studies (Fatima, Doi, & Mamun, 2016; Miller, Kruisbrink, Wallace, Ji, & Cappuccio, 2018), indicating that improving sleep hygiene may be a viable target for interventions addressing these conditions (Hart, Hawley, & Wing, 2016). Sleep is postulated to impact body weight via a number of neuroendocrine, metabolic, cognitive, and behavioral pathways (St-Onge, 2017). Research conducted primarily in non-obese samples suggests that at both the between- and within-subjects levels, sleep restriction enhances hunger, potentially via appetite-regulating hormones such as the orexigenic hormone ghrelin and the anorexigenic hormone leptin (Boeke, Storfer-Isser, Redline, & Taveras, 2014; Leproult & Van Cauter, 2010; Spiegel, Tasali, Penev, & Van Cauter, 2004; St-Onge, O’Keeffe, Roberts, RoyChoudhury, & Laferrere, 2012), and increases neural reward sensitivity to food cues (Benedict et al., 2012; St-Onge, McReynolds, et al., 2012; St-Onge, Wolfe, Sy, Shechter, & Hirsch, 2014), all of which could lead to excess energy intake and subsequent increases in body weight. Alternatively, poor quality sleep is associated with decreased executive functioning at the between-subjects level (Sadeh, Gruber, & Raviv, 2002), which could inhibit regulation of eating behavior and lead to subsequent feelings of LOC. Indeed, increased time to sleep onset, poor sleep continuity, and shortened sleep duration have been associated with overeating and LOC eating behaviors in adults at the between-subjects level (Trace et al., 2012; Tzischinsky, Latzer, Epstein, & Tov, 2000; Vardar, Caliyurt, Arikan, & Tuglu, 2004; Yeh & Brown, 2014), although research characterizing associations between sleep and disordered eating behavior has been relatively limited in pediatric samples.

Two recent naturalistic studies of youth across the weight spectrum reported that experimental sleep restriction was associated with higher energy intake (Hart et al., 2013) and greater intake of energy-dense desserts and sweets (Beebe et al., 2013), but neither study specifically examined the experience of LOC. By contrast, Tzischinsky and Latzer (2006) found that children with overweight/obesity and concurrent binge eating (i.e., objectively large LOC episodes) had lower sleep efficiency and increased wakefulness (both measured via actigraphy) than control children with overweight/obesity. Yet, a major limitation to this body of research is that eating behavior was retrospectively reported, which may be particularly problematic in youth given developmental factors that impede recall and understanding of complex constructs (Fan et al., 2006; Matthews, Hall, & Dahl, 2014). Therefore, combining objective and subjective methods of real-time, naturalistic data collection may advance the field’s understanding of the relation between sleep and eating behavior in youth.

The aims of this exploratory pilot study were to: 1) assess the feasibility of combining three methods of naturalistic assessment of sleep and eating behavior, namely actigraphy, an objective method for measuring sleep patterns in the natural environment (Ancoli-Israel et al., 2003); ecological momentary assessment (EMA), an experience sampling technique in which events and associated behaviors, emotions, and cognitions are self-reported in near real-time in the natural environment (Stone & Shiffman, 1994); and 24-hour dietary recall involving detailed self-reports of the quantity of all foods and beverages consumed within the prior 24 hours; and 2) investigate whether sleep quantity and/or quality is related to alterations in eating behavior on the following day. Given that actigraphy is a non-intrusive technology, whereas EMA and dietary recalls are more labor-intensive, we expected that compliance would be higher for the actigraph protocol relative to the EMA and dietary recall protocols. We further hypothesized that shorter sleep duration (as reflected in total sleep time; TST) and poorer sleep quality [as reflected in increased nighttime awakenings, higher wake after sleep onset (WASO) scores, and lower sleep efficiency] would be associated with greater energy intake, poorer dietary quality, and elevated risk of LOC eating on the following day.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants and procedures

Participants were children and adolescents with overweight or obesity who were involved in a larger pilot study investigating a broad array of contextual factors associated with eating behavior in the natural environment (Goldschmidt, Smith, et al., 2018). Primary aims of this larger study were to identify physiological, emotional, interpersonal, and environmental antecedents and consequences of LOC eating, and to evaluate the prevalence of LOC eating in the natural environment among a heterogeneous sample of young people. The present study represents the first investigation of associations between sleep and eating behavior from this sample.

Participants were recruited from two academic institutions in Chicago, IL (The University of Chicago Medicine and Illinois Institute of Technology) via community flyers, direct pediatrician referrals, and phone logs from previous studies. To be included, youth had to have a body mass index (BMI; kg/m2) ≥ 85th age- and sex-adjusted percentile and be between the ages of 8-14 years. Exclusion criteria were: medical conditions or medications known to influence sleep, weight, or appetite (e.g., sleep apnea); eating disorders other than binge eating disorder (as assessed by diagnostic items from the Child Eating Disorder Examination; Bryant-Waugh, Cooper, Taylor, & Lask, 1996); inability to read and understand English fluently; and concurrent treatment for overweight/obesity. Caregivers of interested individuals completed a phone screen to assess basic study entry criteria, and eligible participants and their caregiver(s) attended a baseline study visit. In total, 92 youth were screened via phone, 44 of whom presented to the research sites for a baseline evaluation, and 40 of whom provided adequate EMA data (e.g., at least 7 days of EMA recording) to be considered in the current analyses. At the baseline assessment, participants provided written informed assent/consent, had their height and weight measured, and completed interviews and questionnaires assessing sleep, eating patterns, and psychosocial functioning. Participants and their caregivers were then trained to complete the EMA, actigraphy, and dietary recall protocols. Training included both parents and children, lasted approximately 30-60 minutes, and was led by the research assistant, who 1) visually proceeded through each EMA screen with instructions on rating oneself in each domain; 2) demonstrated proper watch placement and filled out a sample sleep diary with the family; and 3) completed an abbreviated sample dietary recall online with the family, highlighting the level of detail necessary for accurate reporting.

Participants were asked to complete EMA recordings after each eating occasion (event-contingent); before bedtime (interval-contingent); and at 3-5 semi-random times throughout the day (signal-contingent; Wheeler & Reis, 1991). EMA recordings were not collected during the school day, such that signaled prompts occurred between 7:00-8:00am, 3:00-4:00pm, and 6:00-7:00pm on weekdays and every 2-3 hours between 8:00am-9:00pm on the weekends. During all recordings, participants were instructed to report on characteristics of any recent eating episode that had not been previously recorded. This combination of signal-, event-, and interval-contingent recordings has been successfully implemented in previous EMA studies of youth with overweight/obesity (Hilbert, Rief, Tuschen-Caffier, de Zwaan, & Czaja, 2009; Ranzenhofer et al., 2014). A 1-day practice period during which adherence was ≥70% of EMA ratings qualified children to initiate the 14-day study period; these data were not used in statistical analyses to reduce concerns about the effect of immediate reactivity to self-monitoring. Participants and their caregivers were contacted by phone by a member of the study team after the first day of EMA recording, and every 2-3 days thereafter, to receive feedback regarding compliance rates and address questions or concerns regarding assessment procedures. These phone calls were unscripted and typically involved informing participants about completion rates and incentives earned, problem-solving technical issues (e.g., poor Internet connection), and trouble-shooting any reasons for suboptimal compliance (e.g., keeping smartphone volume at an adequate setting, leaving the actigraph watch in plain sight to enable re-placing it on the wrist as soon as possible after any removals).

Participants were instructed wear wrist actigraphy monitors (Actiwatch 2, Respironics/Phillips, Bend, OR) continuously (except during contact sports or swimming) on the non-dominant wrist, unobstructed by clothing, throughout the 14-day protocol. For each night of actigraphy data collection, participants were instructed to mark the time they began falling asleep and the time they awakened by pressing an event marker button on the actigraphy monitor, which is a standard procedure. This was supplemented by daily sleep diaries (see “Measures” section for more details). Dietary intake was assessed via three, non-consecutive 24-hour dietary recalls collected using the self-guided Automated Self-Administered 24-hour Dietary Assessment Tool for Children (ASA24-Kids), version 2016, developed by the National Cancer Institute, Bethesda, MD. The ASA24-Kids is a Web-based data collection tool with complementary nutrient analysis of all foods and beverages consumed during the recall timeframe. Modifications for children include use of an animated avatar to guide children through response options while maintaining their interest; simplification of wording for food and beverage items; and inclusion of common misspellings to minimize food-finding difficulties. ASA24-Kids recordings were completed on two randomly selected weekdays and one randomly selected weekend day during the 14-day protocol. Participants and their caregivers were contacted via phone by research staff to indicate that a recall day had been selected, and they were then instructed to log on to the ASA24 website and report everything they ate or drank from 0:00h-23:59h on the previous day. Caregivers and children were trained in completing the ASA24-Kids together during the baseline evaluation, as children often require caregiver input to report on aspects of dietary intake (e.g., brand names, food preparation techniques, portion size amounts). Research staff also guided children and caregivers together through ASA24-Kids data collection procedures by phone throughout the 14-day protocol, which has been suggested as a means to improve recall accuracy and compliance (Diep et al., 2015).

Participants received $50 for the intake assessment; $50 for completion of the 14-day protocol; and up to $50 for daily assessments prorated according to degree of response to random signals ($1 for each response to a total of 50 semi-random signals over the course of the 14-day protocol). Study procedures were approved by The University of Chicago and Illinois Institute of Technology Institutional Review Boards.

2.1. Measures

2.2.1. Baseline measures.

Height and weight were measured in light indoor clothing by a trained research assistant via stadiometer and calibrated digital scale, respectively. Child BMI z-score was calculated using Centers for Disease Control and Prevention growth charts and accompanying procedures (Kuczmarski et al., 2000). Demographic data were reported by children and caregivers, and included children’s age, gender, race/ethnicity, current medications, and medical problems. Children’s behavior problems were assessed via the Achenbach Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach, 1991), a parent-report measure of child psychosocial functioning and competency that has good reliability and validity (Achenbach, 1991; Achenbach & Elderbrock, 1991). Sleep items were excluded from the total score calculations to avoid confounding effects on psychosocial functioning.

2.2.2. EMA.

At eating episode recordings, participants reported on the type of eating episode they experienced (meal, snack, or binge). Ratings for LOC (“While you were eating, did you feel a sense of loss of control?”; “While you were eating, did you feel that you could not stop eating once you had started?”; “While you were eating, did you feel like you could not resist eating?”; “While you were eating, did you feel like a car without brakes, you just kept eating and eating?”) were made on a 1- to 5-point Likert-type scale (1-“no, not at all,” and 5-“yes, extremely”). These items were modeled after conceptualizations of LOC in previous EMA studies (Goldschmidt et al., 2014; Hilbert et al., 2009; Ranzenhofer et al., 2014). The four items assessing LOC were summed to form a total score (range=4-20) based on their high internal consistency (α=.91). LOC days were characterized as those on which there was a score of above 4 during at least one eating episode, indicating that some degree of LOC was experienced during at least one eating occasion.

2.2.3. Actigraphy.

Actigraphy is a well-established measure of sleep in the natural environment. It has been validated against the “gold standard” polysomnography, with agreement rates for minute-by-minute sleep—wake identification exceeding 90% (Ancoli-Israel et al., 2003; Sadeh et al., 2002). Actigraphy data were collected continuously and stored in 30-second epochs. In-bed time began with the first minute of a 10-minute period in which the activity counts were all zero and wake-up time was the first minute of a 10-minute period in which the activity counts were all greater than zero (Sadeh, Sharkey, & Carskadon, 1994). Data were analyzed based on the “medium” threshold (meaning that the number of activity counts required to be considered “awake” was 40, as compared to 20 for the “low” threshold and 80 for the “high” threshold) for sleep-wake detection in Philips Actiware v 6.0.7 software, which is the default threshold. Data collected are described in Table 1, and included time in bed (TIB), sleep duration (total sleep time, or TST), wake time, bed time, sleep efficiency (sleep duration divided by the number of minutes in the rest interval), awakenings, and WASO (number of minutes of wake time detected after the participant initially fell asleep). These measures were constrained to nighttime sleep. Sleep intervals were defined as the period between bed time and wake time. The amount of time between bed time and sleep onset is defined as sleep latency. Sleep diaries developed by the American Academy of Sleep Medicine were used to inform coding of sleep and wake times during preliminary actigraphy analyses, such that diary data informed the identification of boundaries for when sleep could have started and ended based on the time the participant got into and out of bed each night. Participants (with caregiver assistance/corroboration when necessary) were instructed to indicate the start and end time of all sleep episodes, including nighttime sleep/awakenings and naps. Actigraphy and sleep diary data were used in conjunction to determine TST, number of nighttime awakenings, WASO, and sleep efficiency (i.e., bedtimes and wake times noted on the sleep diaries were used to verify the boundaries noted by actigraphy, within which to identify sleep and wake periods in the actigraphy data). WASO is a measure of sleep fragmentation (Shrivastava, Jung, Saadat, Sirohi, & Crewson, 2014), and sleep efficiency is the percentage of time spent in bed actually spent asleep. No restrictions were placed on sleep as the study was designed to assess naturalistic associations between sleep and eating behavior.

Table 1.

Detailed description of sleep parameters

Parameter Definition Source of data
Bed time Clock time representing time in bed Sleep diary, actigraphy
Wake time Clock time representing time of awakening Sleep diary, actigraphy
Time in bed (min) Time from resting in bed to getting up Actigraphy
Sleep onset latency (min) Time from resting in bed to sleep onset Actigraphy
Sleep onset First minute of a 10-minute period after bed time in which activity counts were zero Actigraphy
Wake after sleep onset min) Time spent awake after sleep onset Actigraphy
Sleep efficiency, % Proportion of time in bed spent asleep Actigraphy
Total sleep time (min) Time from sleep onset to awakening Actigraphy
Nighttime awakenings Number of awakening after sleep onset Actigraphy
Sleep interval (min) Minutes between bed time and wake time Actigraphy

2.2.4. Dietary recall.

Dietary data from the ASA24-Kids provided estimates for total energy intake at both the eating occasion and day levels, as well as diet quality, measured by the Healthy Eating Index 2010 (HEI-2010; Guenther et al., 2013), for each recall day. The ASA24-Kids uses a modified version of the United States Department of Agriculture’s Automated Multiple Pass Method to reduce bias by reviewing the previous day’s diet with participants up to six times (Park et al., 2018). Participants are instructed to provide information on quantity of all food and beverages consumed as well as the time, eating occasion and location at which they were consumed. The ASA24 has been validated against interviewer-administered 24-hour recalls and biomarkers and may provide more accurate portion size estimations (Kirkpatrick et al., 2016; Kirkpatrick et al., 2014; Park et al., 2018; Thompson et al., 2015). Whereas collecting three, non-consecutive 24-hour dietary recalls is the most accurate and reliable method for assessing dietary intake in children (Burrows, Martin, & Collins, 2010), under-reporting is common, particularly in older children and those with overweight or obesity (Lioret et al., 2011). Accordingly, recalls for which participants reported <500 calorie intake (n=6) were not used in data analyses due to implausibility of reported intake.

2.3. Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were conducted using SAS 9.4 (2014; SAS Institute, Inc., Cary, NC). Descriptive statistics were generated for demographics and anthropometrics at baseline. The primary aim, to assess the feasibility of children and adolescents completing concurrent measures of eating-related constructs, sleep, and diet in real-time for 14 days, was evaluated by determining overlapping adherence for all three measures (i.e., number of days participants completed EMA, ASA24-Kids, and wore the Actiwatch). We also compared characteristics of children who provided matching EMA and 24-hour recall data that aligned with actigraphy data on their prior night’s sleep (“completers”) to those that did not (“non-completers”); however, these comparisons are descriptive only due to small sample sizes.

To assess the secondary aim to examine the relationships of sleep with eating behaviors and dietary intake, actigraphy data measuring sleep-related constructs were date-matched to the EMA- and ASA24-Kids-measured eating behavior data separately. As this was an exploratory study, and research on sleep and eating behavior in youth is emerging, we explored a range of sleep-related variables, including TST, number of nighttime awakenings, WASO, and sleep efficiency. Given that participants contributed multiple days of data for each measure, observations were correlated. Thus, a generalized estimating equation accounting for both a binomial distribution (with LOC dichotomized as a yes/no variable) and repeated measures was used to determine the odds of experiencing LOC eating based on the prior night’s sleep. A similar model assuming a normal distribution was used to estimate the relationships of the prior night’s sleep with energy intake and diet quality as assessed via ASA24-Kids. All analyses reflect between-subjects effects. Given the continuous nature and normal distribution of total energy intake and diet quality, this second model assumed a normal distribution. Results are reported both unadjusted, and adjusting for weekend vs. weekday (given evidence that pediatric sleep and eating behavior vary on weekends vs. weekdays; Hansen, Janssen, Schiff, Zee, & Dubocovich, 2005; Hart, Raynor, Osterholt, Jelalian, & Wing, 2011), age, sex, BMI and CBCL total scores excluding sleep-related items (given prior research linking internalizing and externalizing symptoms to both sleep and LOC eating in children; Baddam, Canapari, van Noordt, & Crowley, 2018; Byrne, LeMay-Russell, & Tanofsky-Kraff, 2019).

Because this was an exploratory study designed to generate effect sizes and assess for the presence of any associations of interest between sleep and eating behavior that should be further evaluated in future studies, power analyses are not presented and it was concluded that adjusting for multiple comparisons would limit discoveries from these exploratory data. Therefore, alpha was set at .05 for all analyses.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive characteristics

To assess our primary aim related to feasibility, we found that of 40 participants who completed this study, 29 provided matching EMA and 24-hour recall data that aligned with actigraphy data on their prior night’s sleep. On average, these 29 participants provided 13.1 days of EMA data and 11.2 days of sleep data (out of a possible 14 days). These data overlapped on 8.6 days, on average, and the EMA, sleep and ASA24-Kids data overlapped on an average of 2.7 days (out of a possible 3 days). As shown in Table 2, participants with complete data who were included in subsequent statistical analyses were 11.6±1.9 years of age, on average, and 56.7% were female. Nearly 80% of participants were from a racial/ethnic minority group, and more than 60% had mothers who pursued education beyond a college degree. The average BMI z-score was 2.1 units, which is consistent with this sample of youth with overweight or obesity. Those without complete data (i.e., participants who did not have EMA, ASA24-Kids and actigraphy data) were older and were more likely to be male, to have a racial/ethnic minority background, and to have a mother with less education (statistical comparisons between completers and noncompleters could not be performed due to small sample sizes). Actigraphy data suggest that participants slept 7h 17m (range=1h 4m-14h 36m) each night and, on average, experienced 31.8 awakenings each night. Participants reported consuming 1,684.1±772 calories per day, on average, (range=541.5 to 4,581.0 calories/day), and their mean diet quality, as measured by the total HEI-2010 score, was 43.5±11.9 out of a possible score of 100 (range=5.04 to 82.6).

Table 2.

Participant characteristics

Full sample (N=40) Participants with incomplete data (n=11) Participants with complete data (n=29)
Age, y 11.8±1.9 12.4±2.3 11.6±1.9
% female 55.0% 45.5% 56.7%
Race/Ethnicity
 Non-Hispanic, White 15.4% 0% 20.7%
 Non-Hispanic, Black 64.1% 80% 58.6%
 Non-Hispanic, Other 2.5% 0% 3.5%
 Hispanic (all races) 18.0% 20% 17.2%
Maternal education
 High school degree or less 18.9% 10% 17.9%
 Some college 10.8% 40% 10.7%
 College degree 18.9% 30% 10.7%
 More than college 51.4% 20% 60.7%
BMI z-score 2.1±0.5 2.0±0.6 2.1±0.5

3.2. Associations between sleep and next-day eating behavior

In the analysis of LOC eating (as measured by EMA) and sleep, participants contributed a total of 257 observations (days), 44 (19.4%) of which included an eating occasion at which LOC was reported. On days on which LOC eating occurred, participants consumed significantly more calories as compared to non-LOC days (2,436.7±742 calories vs. 1,675.7±686 calories, respectively; p=.008). Table 3 includes descriptive data for sleep characteristics on LOC vs. non-LOC days; when we statistically examined whether prior night’s sleep (TST, number of night time awakenings, WASO, and sleep efficiency, all modeled separately) in relation to odds of having an LOC eating episode the following day, none of these relationships were significant after adjusting for age, sex, BMI z-score and CBCL total score (excluding sleep-related items; Table 4).

Table 3.

Sleep patterns across study days

All days Loss of control eating days (n=44) Non-loss of control eating days (n=213)

Range M±SD M±SD M±SD
Total sleep time 1.1-14.6 7.2±1.9 7.2±1.9 7.1±2.0
Nighttime awakenings 0.0-71.0 31.8±13.5 31.7±27.5 31.9±13.5
Wake after sleep onset 0.0–219.0 44.7±32.3 45.4±29.6 44.5±32.2
Sleep efficiency, % 21.5-99.9 84.0±11.3 82.7±12.6 84.2±11.0

Note: Data included in this table are for illustrative purposes only; results of statistical analyses assessing associations between sleep and loss of control eating are report in Table 4.

Table 4.

Odds of experiencing loss of control eating, as measured by ecological momentary assessment, based on measures of sleep

Odds ratio 95% Confidence Interval
Total hours of sleep

 Unadjusted model 0.84 (0.67 to 1.05)
 Fully adjusted model 0.86 (0.63 to 1.16)

Number of nighttime awakenings

 Unadjusted model 0.98 (0.94 to 1.02)
 Fully adjusted model 0.99 (0.95 to 1.03)

Wake after sleep onset

 Unadjusted model 0.99 (0.98 to 1.02)
 Fully adjusted model 1.00 (0.98 to 1.01)

Sleep efficiency

 Unadjusted model 0.98 (0.96 to 1.02)
 Fully adjusted model 0.97 (0.94 to 1.00)

Note: Fully adjusted models include week vs. weekend day, age, sex, BMI z-score and CBCL total score (minus sleep-related items) as covariates.

Table 5 shows the associations of measures of sleep (TST, number of nighttime awakenings, WASO, and sleep efficiency) with total energy intake and diet quality as assessed by HEI-2010. TST was positively associated with reported energy intake, although findings were no longer significant after adjusting for weekend vs. weekday, age, sex, BMI z-score, and CBCL total score. In further exploring these findings, after adjusting for weekend vs. weekday, age, sex, z-BMI, and CBCL total score, participants had 0.76 the odds of skipping breakfast with each additional hour of sleep, which was marginally significant (p=.09). Finally, while the relationships of sleep measures and overall diet quality (i.e., total HEI-2010 score) were not significant, we did find that each additional hour of sleep was associated with consuming fewer calories from solid fats, alcohol, and added sugars (b=0.70; p=.04) after adjusting for weekend vs. weekday, age, sex, BMI z-score, and total CBCL score. Given the recommendation to limit calories from solid fats, alcohol, and added sugars to <20% of total energy intake, a higher score for this component reflects a lower intake.

Table 5.

Relationships of measure of sleep with dietary intake, as measured by ASA24-Kids dietary recalls

Effect estimate p-value
Total energy intake

Total hours of sleep

 Unadjusted model 78.1 calories .05
 Fully adjusted model 29.2 calories .43

Number of nighttime awakenings

 Unadjusted model 9.1 calories .21
 Fully adjusted model −5.3 calories .55

Wake after sleep onset

 Unadjusted model  0.4 calories .93
 Fully adjusted model −4.5 calories .15

Sleep efficiency

 Unadjusted model −1.5 calories .84
 Fully adjusted model 1.3 calories .82

Diet quality (total Healthy Eating Index score)

Total hours of sleep

 Unadjusted model 0.31 .61
 Fully adjusted model 0.39 .49

Number of nighttime awakenings

 Unadjusted model 0.11 .50
 Fully adjusted model 0.28 .06

Wake after sleep onset

 Unadjusted model 0.05 .55
 Fully adjusted model 0.10 .09

Sleep efficiency

 Unadjusted model −0.15 .27
 Fully adjusted model −0.17 .27

Note: Adjusted models include week vs. weekend day, age, sex, BMI z-score and CBCL total score (minus sleep-related items) as covariates. The Healthy Eating Index, 2010 measures compliance with the Dietary Guidelines for Americans, 2010 and the total score is out of 100 points.

4. Discussion

The primary aim of this exploratory pilot study was to evaluate the feasibility of assessing sleep duration/quality and eating behavior in children and young adolescents using multiple naturalistic, momentary methodologies. We found that nearly 75% of participants provided adequate sleep and eating behavior data to be included in analyses. Participants contributed an average of 8.06 EMA recordings per day that were linked to actigraph sleep data from the previous night (from a maximum of 14 days), and 2.7 ASA24-Kids recordings linked to actigraph sleep data from the previous night (from a maximum of 3 days) over the 14-day study, indicating that the protocol, although potentially burdensome, was acceptable and resulted in moderate to high compliance in a sample of children and young adolescents. It should be noted, however, that participants were compensated for participation, which may have introduced selection bias and potentially skewed compliance data. Additionally, older participants, males, youth from racial/ethnic minority backgrounds, and those with mothers who had received less education were less likely to have complete data across all three assessment methodologies (although it is unclear if these differences were statistically significant). This is consistent with the previous EMA compliance literature (Wen, Schneider, Stone, & Spruijt-Metz, 2017) and suggests that special efforts may be needed to optimally engage these youth (e.g., providing incentives that are more appealing to these demographic characteristics).

A secondary aim was to assess whether sleep parameters were related to next day eating behavior. Despite the small sample size, several findings of statistical or marginal significance emerged (although as the study was observational in nature, the findings should be interpreted with caution, particularly regarding directions of causation). First, contrary to expectation, greater TST was associated with slightly greater next day energy intake. It is noteworthy that this association was attenuated and was no longer significant when covariates were included. Moreover, given the finding that each additional hour of sleep was associated with lower odds of skipping breakfast, it is possible that this association may be accounted for, at least in part, by the fact that well rested children and young adolescents are more likely to consume breakfast. Although investigation of physical activity was outside the scope of this study, it is also possible that youth who were better rested engaged in more activity throughout the day, which in turn promoted increased hunger and energy intake. This possibility should be explored in future research. More consistent with previous findings (Beebe et al., 2013), each additional hour of sleep was associated with consuming fewer calories from solid fats, alcohol, and added sugars (b=0.70; p=.04) after adjusting for covariates. Given the non-experimental design, it is equally plausible that longer sleep time is associated with improved dietary choices the next day, or that improved dietary choices lead to longer subsequent sleep time (or that some unmeasured variable affects both dietary choice and sleep time). Future research should seek to explicate the direction of these associations so as to guide intervention efforts.

Contrary to expectation, sleep was not related to EMA-reported LOC severity on the following day. This is likely due to the low base rates of self-reported LOC in this sample, which was recruited solely on the basis of having overweight/obesity, irrespective of baseline eating pathology. However, of note, there was a marked difference in variance size for nighttime awakenings on days on which LOC eating occurred relative to days on which it did not (see Table 2), which may reflect a disruption in sleep homeostasis on nights preceding LOC versus non-LOC days. Future research—including data collected in a laboratory capable of assessing sleep homeostatic physiology—should explore associations between LOC eating and sleep (particularly focusing on variance in sleep metrics) in samples enriched for maladaptive eating.

This study was strengthened by the use of multiple forms of technology-based methodologies (both objective and subjective) to characterize sleep and eating behavior in the natural environment and at the momentary level. Other strengths include the generalizable sample of youth with overweight/obesity who were recruited from the community, and the assessment of different facets of sleep and eating behavior (e.g., quality and quantity). Given that this was an observational pilot study that focused on feasibility, the primary limitation was the small sample size. However, the relatively good compliance suggests that these assessment techniques could be undertaken in larger samples to characterize sleep and eating behavior in children and young adolescents, and the current study provides valuable information about effect sizes that could inform power calculations for larger studies. All eating-related constructs were assessed via self-report and analyzed at the between-subjects level, which could have biased the results. Future research may benefit from integration of sensor-based or laboratory assessment of eating behavior, and exploration of within-person findings to inform intervention development. Additionally, there was no non-overweight control group, and thus it cannot be assumed that results are specific to youth with overweight/obesity. Although actigraphy is a validated method for assessing sleep in the natural environment, the Actiware algorithm, which was set to a “medium” wake threshold in the current study, may be overly sensitive to motion and, as a result, tends to overestimate the number of awakenings relative to polysomnography (Paquet, Kawinska, & Carrier, 2007). This could explain the high average number of awakenings per night in the current sample, although it should be noted that the setting was the same for every participant, and thus nighttime awakenings nevertheless reflect within-study patterns. Future research should explore whether established thresholds are appropriate for youth with overweight/obesity. In addition, participants were required to indicate the start and end of the overall nighttime sleep period via pressing a button on the actigraph watch, which could have affected compliance and/or sleep onset. Finally, participants did not undergo baseline overnight polysomnography, so undiagnosed sleep apnea may be a confounding variable that was not accounted for.

5. Conclusions

Given that sleep deficiency poses a vulnerability to multiple risky health-related behaviors (Pasch, Laska, Lytle, & Moe, 2010), yet is modifiable via intervention (Blake, Sheeber, Youssef, Raniti, & Allen, 2017), improving sleep hygiene may be a promising avenue for enhancing regulation of eating and weight in young people (Hart et al., 2016). Future research should replicate and extend our findings in larger samples to better characterize prospective associations between sleep and eating behavior, and identify potential mechanisms explaining these associations.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to all the families who took part in this study, and to study assessors and referral sources.

Role of the funding sources: This research was funded by grants from the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences (UL1-TR000430) and the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases (K23-DK105234). The funding sources had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; or in the decision to submit the article for publication.

Abbreviations:

LOC

loss of control

EMA

ecological momentary assessment

TST

total sleep time

WASO

wake after sleep onset

BMI

body mass index

ASA24-Kids

Automated Self-Administered 24-hour Dietary Assessment Tool for Children

CBCL

Achenbach Child Behavior Checklist

TIB

time in bed

HEI-2010

Healthy Eating Index 2010

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Declarations of interest: None

References

  1. Achenbach TM (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 and 1991 profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry. [Google Scholar]
  2. Achenbach TM, & Elderbrock C (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist and Revised Child Behavior Profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry. [Google Scholar]
  3. Ancoli-Israel S, Cole R, Alessi C, Chambers M, Moorcroft W, & Pollak CP (2003). The role of actigraphy in the study of sleep and circadian rhythms. Sleep, 26(3), 342–392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baddam SKR, Canapari CA, van Noordt SJR, & Crowley MJ (2018). Sleep Disturbances in Child and Adolescent Mental Health Disorders: A Review of the Variability of Objective Sleep Markers. Medical sciences (Basel, Switzerland), 6(2) doi: 10.3390/medsci6020046 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Beebe DW, Simon S, Summer S, Hemmer S, Strotman D, & Dolan LM (2013). Dietary intake following experimentally restricted sleep in adolescents. Sleep, 36(6), 827–834. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2704 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Benedict C, Brooks SJ, O’Daly OG, Almen MS, Morell A, Aberg K, … Schioth HB. (2012). Acute sleep deprivation enhances the brain’s response to hedonic food stimuli: an fMRI study. The Journal of clinical endocrinology and metabolism, 97(3), E443–447. doi: 10.1210/jc.2011-2759 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Blake MJ, Sheeber LB, Youssef GJ, Raniti MB, & Allen NB (2017). Systematic review and meta-analysis of adolescent cognitive-behavioral sleep interventions. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev, 20(3), 227–249. doi: 10.1007/s10567-017-0234-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Boeke CE, Storfer-Isser A, Redline S, & Taveras EM (2014). Childhood sleep duration and quality in relation to leptin concentration in two cohort studies. Sleep, 37(3), 613–620. doi: 10.5665/sleep.3510 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bryant-Waugh RJ, Cooper PJ, Taylor CL, & Lask BD (1996). The use of the Eating Disorder Examination with children: A pilot study. Int J Eat Disord, 19(4), 391–397. doi: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Burrows TL, Martin RJ, & Collins CE (2010). A systematic review of the validity of dietary assessment methods in children when compared with the method of doubly labeled water. J Am Diet Assoc, 110(10), 1501–1510. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2010.07.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Byrne ME, LeMay-Russell S, & Tanofsky-Kraff M (2019). Loss-of-Control Eating and Obesity Among Children and Adolescents. Curr Obes Rep doi: 10.1007/s13679-019-0327-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Diep CS, Hingle M, Chen T-A, Dadabhoy HR, Beltran A, Baranowski J, … Baranowski T. (2015). The Automated Self-Administered 24-Hour Dietary Recall for Children, 2012 Version, for Youth Aged 9 to 11 Years: A Validation Study. J Acad Nutr Diet, 115(10), 1591–1598. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2015.02.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Fan X, Miller BC, Park K-E, Winward BW, Christensen M, Grotevant HD, & Tai RH (2006). An exploratory study about inaccuracy and invalidity in adolescent self-report surveys. Field Methods, 18(3), 223–244. doi: 10.1177/152822x06289161 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  14. Fatima Y, Doi SA, & Mamun AA (2016). Sleep quality and obesity in young subjects: a meta-analysis. Obes Rev, 17(11), 1154–1166. doi: 10.1111/obr.12444 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. French SA, Epstein LH, Jeffery RW, Blundell JE, & Wardle J (2012). Eating behavior dimensions. Associations with energy intake and body weight. A review. Appetite, 59, 541–549. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.07.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Goldschmidt AB (2017). Are loss of control while eating and overeating valid constructs? A critical review of the literature. Obes Rev, 18(4), 412–449. doi: 10.1111/obr.12491 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Goldschmidt AB, Crosby R, Cao L, Engel SG, Durkin N, Beach HM, … Peterson CB. (2014). Ecological momentary assessment of eating episodes in obese adults. Psychosom Med, 76(9), 747–752. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Goldschmidt AB, Jones M, Manwaring JL, Luce KH, Osborne MI, Cunning D, … Taylor CB. (2008). The clinical significance of loss of control over eating in overweight adolescents. Int J Eat Disord, 41(2), 153–158. doi: 10.1002/eat.20481 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Goldschmidt AB, Khoury J, Jenkins TM, Bond DS, Thomas JG, Utzinger LM, … Mitchell JE. (2018). Adolescent Loss-of-Control Eating and Weight Loss Maintenance After Bariatric Surgery. Pediatrics, 141(1) doi: 10.1542/peds.2017-1659 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Goldschmidt AB, Smith KE, Crosby RD, Boyd HK, Dougherty E, Engel SG, & Haedt-Matt A (2018). Ecological momentary assessment of maladaptive eating in children and adolescents with overweight or obesity. Int J Eat Disord, 51(6), 549–557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Guenther PM, Casavale KO, Reedy J, Kirkpatrick SI, Hiza HAB, Kuczynski KJ, … Krebs-Smith SM. (2013). Update of the Healthy Eating Index: HEI-2010. J Acad Nutr Diet, 113(4), 569–580. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2012.12.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hansen M, Janssen I, Schiff A, Zee PC, & Dubocovich ML (2005). The impact of school daily schedule on adolescent sleep. Pediatrics, 115(6), 1555–1561. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-1649 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hart CN, Carskadon MA, Considine RV, Fava JL, Lawton J, Raynor HA, … Wing R. (2013). Changes in children’s sleep duration on food intake, weight, and leptin. Pediatrics, 132(6), e1473–1480. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-1274 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hart CN, Hawley NL, & Wing RR (2016). Development of a behavioral sleep intervention as a novel approach for pediatric obesity in school-aged children. Pediatr Clin North Am, 63(3), 511–523. doi: 10.1016/j.pcl.2016.02.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hart CN, Raynor HA, Osterholt KM, Jelalian E, & Wing RR (2011). Eating and activity habits of overweight children on weekdays and weekends. International Journal of Pediatric Obesity 6(5-6), 467–472. doi: 10.3109/17477166.2011.590204 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hilbert A, Rief W, Tuschen-Caffier B, de Zwaan M, & Czaja J (2009). Loss of control eating and psychological maintenance in children: An ecological momentary assessment study. Behav Res Ther, 47(1), 26–33. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.10.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kirkpatrick SI, Potischman N, Dodd KW, Douglass D, Zimmerman TP, Kahle LL, … Subar AF. (2016). The use of digital images in 24-hour recalls may lead to less misestimation of portion size compared with traditional interviewer-administered recalls. J Nutr, 146(12), 2567–2573. doi: 10.3945/jn.116.237271 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kirkpatrick SI, Subar AF, Douglass D, Zimmerman TP, Thompson FE, Kahle LL, … Potischman N. (2014). Performance of the Automated Self-Administered 24-hour Recall relative to a measure of true intakes and to an interviewer-administered 24-h recall. Am J Clin Nutr, 100(1), 233–240. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.114.083238 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kuczmarski RJ, Ogden CL, Grummer-Strawn LM, Flegal KM, Guo SS, Wei R, … Johnson CL. (2000). CDC growth charts: United States. Adv Data, 314, 1–27. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Leproult R, & Van Cauter E (2010). Role of sleep and sleep loss in hormonal release and metabolism. Endocr Dev, 17, 11–21. doi: 10.1159/000262524 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lioret S, Touvier M, Balin M, Huybrechts I, Dubuisson C, Dufour A, … Lafay L. (2011). Characteristics of energy under-reporting in children and adolescents. Br J Nutr, 105(11), 1671–1680. doi: 10.1017/s0007114510005465 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Matthews KA, Hall M, & Dahl RE (2014). Sleep in Healthy Black and White Adolescents. Pediatrics, 133(5), e1189–e1196. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-2399 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Miller MA, Kruisbrink M, Wallace J, Ji C, & Cappuccio FP (2018). Sleep duration and incidence of obesity in infants, children, and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies. Sleep, 41(4) doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsy018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Paquet J, Kawinska A, & Carrier J (2007). Wake detection capacity of actigraphy during sleep. Sleep, 30(1), 1362–1369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Park Y, Dodd KW, Kipnis V, Thompson FE, Potischman N, Schoeller DA, … Subar AF. (2018). Comparison of self-reported dietary intakes from the Automated Self-Administered 24-h recall, 4-d food records, and food-frequency questionnaires against recovery biomarkers. Am J Clin Nutr, 107(1), 80–93. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqx002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Pasch KE, Laska MN, Lytle LA, & Moe SG (2010). Adolescent sleep, risk behaviors, and depressive symptoms: Are they linked? Am J Health Behav, 34(2), 237–248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Ranzenhofer LM, Engel SG, Crosby RD, Anderson M, Vannucci A, Cohen LA, … Tanofsky-Kraff M. (2014). Using ecological momentary assessment to examine interpersonal and affective predictors of loss of control eating in adolescent girls. Int J Eat Disord, 47(7), 748–757. doi: 10.1002/eat.22333 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Sadeh A, Gruber R, & Raviv A (2002). Sleep, neurobehavioral functioning, and behavior problems in school-age children. Child Dev, 73(2), 405–417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Schluter N, Schmidt R, Kittel R, Tetzlaff A, & Hilbert A (2016). Loss of control eating in adolescents from the community. Int J Eat Disord, 49(4), 413–420. doi: 10.1002/eat.22488 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Shrivastava D, Jung S, Saadat M, Sirohi R, & Crewson K (2014). How to interpret the results of a sleep study. Journal of community hospital internal medicine perspectives, 4(5), 24983. doi: 10.3402/jchimp.v4.24983 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Sonneville KR, Horton NJ, Micali N, Crosby RD, Swanson SA, Solmi F, & Field AE (2013). Longitudinal associations between binge eating and overeating and adverse outcomes among adolescents and young adults: Does loss of control matter? JAMA Pediatr, 167(2), 149–155. doi: 10.1001/2013.jamapediatrics.12 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Spiegel K, Tasali E, Penev P, & Van Cauter E (2004). Brief communication: Sleep curtailment in healthy young men is associated with decreased leptin levels, elevated ghrelin levels, and increased hunger and appetite. Ann Intern Med, 141(11), 846–850. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. St-Onge MP (2017). Sleep—obesity relation: underlying mechanisms and consequences for treatment. Obesity reviews : an official journal of the International Association for the Study of Obesity, 18, 34–39. doi: 10.1111/obr.12499 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. St-Onge MP, McReynolds A, Trivedi ZB, Roberts AL, Sy M, & Hirsch J (2012). Sleep restriction leads to increased activation of brain regions sensitive to food stimuli. Am J Clin Nutr, 95(4), 818–824. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.111.027383 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. St-Onge MP, O’Keeffe M, Roberts AL, RoyChoudhury A, & Laferrere B (2012). Short sleep duration, glucose dysregulation and hormonal regulation of appetite in men and women. Sleep, 35(11), 1503–1510. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2198 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. St-Onge MP, Wolfe S, Sy M, Shechter A, & Hirsch J (2014). Sleep restriction increases the neuronal response to unhealthy food in normal-weight individuals. Int J Obes, 38(3), 411–416. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2013.114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Stone AA, & Shiffman S (1994). Ecological momentary assessment in behavioral medicine. Ann Behav Med, 16, 199–202. [Google Scholar]
  48. Sysko R, Devlin MJ, Hildebrandt TB, Brewer SK, Zitsman JL, & Walsh BT (2012). Psychological outcomes and predictors of initial weight loss outcomes among severely obese adolescents receiving laparoscopic adjustable gastric banding. J Clin Psychiatry, 73(10), 1351–1357. doi: 10.4088/JCP.12m07690 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Tanofsky-Kraff M, Cohen ML, Yanovski SZ, Cox C, Theim KR, Keil M, … Yanovski JA. (2006). A prospective study of psychological predictors of body fat gain among children at high risk for adult obesity. Pediatrics, 117(4), 1203–1209. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-1329 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Tanofsky-Kraff M, Shomaker LB, Olsen C, Roza CA, Wolkoff LE, Columbo KM, … Yanovski JA. (2011). A prospective study of pediatric loss of control eating and psychological outcomes. J Abnorm Psychol, 120(1), 108–118. doi: 10.1037/a0021406 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Tanofsky-Kraff M, Yanovski SZ, Schvey NA, Olsen CH, Gustafson J, & Yanovski JA (2009). A prospective study of loss of control eating for body weight gain in children at high risk for adult obesity. Int J Eat Disord, 42(1), 26–30. doi: 10.1002/eat.20580 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Tanofsky-Kraff M, Yanovski SZ, Wilfley DE, Marmarosh C, Morgan CM, & Yanovski JA (2004). Eating-disordered behaviors, body fat, and psychopathology in overweight and normal-weight children. J Consult Clin Psychol, 72(1), 53–61. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.1.53 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Thompson FE, Dixit-Joshi S, Potischman N, Dodd KW, Kirkpatrick SI, Kushi LH, … Subar AF. (2015). Comparison of interviewer-administered and Automated Self-Administered 24-Hour Dietary Recalls in 3 diverse integrated health systems. Am J Epidemiol, 181(12), 970–978. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwu467 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Trace SE, Thornton LM, Runfola CD, Lichtenstein P, Pedersen NL, & Bulik CM (2012). Sleep problems are associated with binge eating in women. Int J Eat Disord, 45(5), 695–703. doi: 10.1002/eat.22003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Tzischinsky O, & Latzer Y (2006). Sleep-wake cycles in obese children with and without binge-eating episodes. J Paediatr Child Health, 42(11), 688–693. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1754.2006.00952.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Tzischinsky O, Latzer Y, Epstein R, & Tov N (2000). Sleep-wake cycles in women with binge eating disorder. Int J Eat Disord, 27(1), 43–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Vardar E, Caliyurt O, Arikan E, & Tuglu C (2004). Sleep quality and psychopathological features in obese binge eaters. Stress and Health, 20(1), 35–41. doi: 10.1002/smi.992 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  58. Wen CKF, Schneider S, Stone AA, & Spruijt-Metz D (2017). Compliance with mobile ecological momentary assessment protocols in children and adolescents: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 19(4), e132. doi: 10.2196/jmir.6641 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Wheeler L, & Reis HT (1991). Self-recording of everyday life events: Origins, types, and uses. J Pers, 59(3), 339–354. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1991.tb00252.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  60. Wildes JE, Marcus MD, Kalarchian MA, Levine MD, Houck PR, & Cheng Y (2010). Self-reported binge eating in severe pediatric obesity: Impact on weight change in a randomized controlled trial of family-based treatment. Int J Obes, 34(7), 1143–1148. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2010.35 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Yeh SS, & Brown RF (2014). Disordered eating partly mediates the relationship between poor sleep quality and high body mass index. Eat Behav, 15(2), 291–297. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2014.03.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES