Abstract
OBJECTIVE:
Enhanced odor sensitivity is a phenomenon that potentially underlies conditions such as multiple chemical sensitivity (MCS). Currently, there are no treatments that have been shown to effectively decrease odor sensitivity. Given similarities of odor hypersensitivity/MCS to pain sensitization disorders such as fibromyalgia, there may be a potential for interventions that improve pain tolerance to modulate odor sensitivity.
METHODS:
This exploratory study randomized 72 healthy community adult volunteers to receive one of six treatments in between two assessments of thermal pain tolerance and odor threshold. Participants were randomized to receive either cathodal, anodal, or sham transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) aimed at dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Additionally, participants were provided a brief cognitive behavioral intervention (CBI) for pain consisting of task framing, cognitive restructuring, and distraction technique training, or a control intervention consisting of information about pain.
RESULTS:
Persons who received brief CBI showed significantly increased odor thresholds (reduced sensitivity) over the course of intervention (F[1, 62] = 7.29, p = .009, ηp2 = .11), whereas the control intervention was not associated with altered odor thresholds. Moreover, in those that received brief CBI, more severe anxiety associated with larger reductions in odor sensitivity (ρ = .364, p = .035). There was no effect of tDCS (F[2, 62] = .11, p = .90), nor interaction between tDCS and CBI (F[2, 62] = .32, p = .73).
CONCLUSIONS:
Given the connection between anxiety and MCS, results suggest that CBT techniques for somatic processes may show promise in treating conditions characterized by increased sensitivity to odors (e.g. MCS).
Keywords: Odor Sensitivity, Multiple Chemical Sensitivity, Central Sensitization, Anxiety Sensitivity, Pain, Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, Brain Stimulation
INTRODUCTION
Heightened sensory sensitivity is increasingly being investigated as an important biological factor underlying a spectrum of psychiatric conditions, including autism spectrum, anxiety, trauma-related, and obsessive-compulsive related disorders (1–4). Individuals who endorse heightened sensory sensitivity report being over-stimulated by sensory stimuli (i.e. odors, lights, sounds, touch) to a degree that is distressing and/or painful (5). Specifically, one such clinical phenomenon is multiple chemical sensitivity (MCS) (6–8). Affected individuals report the ability to detect very low concentrations of common odorants, particularly harsh chemicals and environmental pollutants (at much lower than harmful levels), and that the presence of such odorants causes intense irritation and symptoms akin to panic attacks (e.g., lightheadedness, increased heart rate, difficulty breathing, headaches, concentration difficulties) (9–11). Although little research has been conducted on the prevalence of MCS, existing data finds it common, with 11-33% of the population considering themselves “chemically sensitive” (7, 8) and 1-6% of the population being impaired by the condition (6, 12).
The diagnosis of MCS remains a controversial topic, in large part due to its unknown etiology. While numerous biological (19–24) and psychological (25–32) contributing factors have been considered, more recent work describes MCS as a disorder of central sensitization (13), a state in which sensory input becomes amplified by the central nervous system. Wherein past exposure of certain odors were benign, repeated exposures lead to a myriad of adverse reactions. Chronic pain conditions such as fibromyalgia are also believed to involve a sensitized or hyperactive central nervous system, leading to an increased gain on pain and sensory processing (14). These sensitization processes also appear to play a role at the behavioral level, as studies have linked sensitivity to negative body sensations (e.g., anxiety sensitivity) with both heightened odor sensitivity (1) and chronic pain (15). Interestingly, MCS and pain regularly co-exist, as increased sensitivity to laboratory pain is reported in MCS (16) and MCS is often comorbid with fibromyalgia and other chronic pain conditions (17, 18).
Despite its prevalence and association with pain and significant functional impairment, MCS has no well-established treatments. Several studies have reported limited benefit (e.g. increased quality of life, sense of personal control) after a behavioral intervention (mindfulness-based cognitive therapy/stress reduction) but no symptom reduction (33–35). However, case reports have indicated potential for cognitive-behavioral approaches (36–38). Indeed, cognitive-behavior therapy (CBT) has shown positive evidence for reducing sensory sensitivities across other modalities including “misophonia”, a syndrome characterized by selective sound sensitivity (39–44), and pain/chronic pain conditions (45–48). In addition, numerous studies have reported efficacy in using CBT for fibromyalgia (49–57). CBT is believed to be effective for psychosomatic pain and other sensory sensitivities because affected individuals demonstrate poor emotional associations and maladaptive coping behaviors in response to aversive sensory experiences (58). Given that people with MCS experience negative emotional responses to common odorants (59, 60), CBT aimed to correct maladaptive thoughts and emotions, as well as provide better ways to cope could be an effective treatment.
Another approach at influencing sensory processing may be to modulate emotion-related neural activity in the limbic cortex. Studies have found that minimally invasive brain stimulation targeting the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) improves acute pain tolerance as well as chronic pain (61–68). Activation of DLPFC is thought to improve cognitive control over pain by modulating downstream limbic activity and emotional aspects of pain (63, 69). A previous study (from which the data for the current study were derived) found that cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) aimed at DLPCF, when combined with a brief cognitive behavioral intervention (CBI), was associated with a significant increase in pain tolerance in healthy adults as compared to these interventions by themselves, anodal tDCS, sham tDCS, or pain education (70). However, a lack of data exist regarding the effects of brain stimulation on olfactory sensitivity and MCS. While clinical trials for transcranial pulsed electromagnetic fields, a less intense form of brain stimulation than tDCS that does not target specific neural regions, are currently being conducted for MCS (71), available published data has focused on brain stimulation methods to improve smell acuity in persons with smell loss (72–75).
Due to the associations between MSC and pain, including neuroimaging evidence that olfactory sensitization utilizes the same brain networks responsible for pain processing (76) and the significant role that emotion plays in both pain and olfaction (77), it stands to reason that the therapeutic approaches that alter pain sensitivity may be similarly effective at modulating odor sensitivity. Thus, a subset of the healthy adults who took part in the randomized controlled trial of a brief cognitive behavioral intervention (CBI) and tDCS for acute pain tolerance (70) were recruited for this proof of concept study. The purpose of this exploratory study was to create hypotheses for future, large-scale randomized trials. However, due to our prior findings for pain, we made preliminary hypotheses that both CBI and tDCS would be associated with reduced odor sensitivity (higher threshold), and that their combination would produce the greatest effect. Also, given the purported limbic-emotional moderating effects of both CBT and tDCS, as well as evidence showing an association between odor sensitivity and stress and anxiety (1, 78), we explored the relationship between anxiety sensitivity and odor thresholds at baseline and follow-up, including whether anxiety sensitivity predicted a post-intervention change in odor detection.
METHODS
Participants
A sample of seventy-two healthy adults were recruited over a 4-month time period as part of a pilot trial of brief CBI and tDCS for acute pain tolerance (70). The sample was primarily middle-aged (M = 30.86, SD = 11.47, Age Range = 21-66), female (76.4%), and white (87.5%). Exclusionary criteria consisted of factors that would interfere with tDCS or the validity of the pain study: (a) pregnancy, (b) history of chronic pain conditions, (c) seizures or family history of seizure disorders, (d) active suicidality, (e) implanted metal devices (e.g., pacemakers, metal plates, wires), (f) history of brain surgery or loss of consciousness for > 15 minutes, (g) prescribed medication associated with lowered seizure threshold, and (h) latex allergy. Additional exclusionary criteria for participating in the olfactory component of the study included current smokers or any problem with sense of smell (e.g. current/recent upper respiratory infection, chronic rhinosinusitis, polyps, etc.). All participants signed a written informed consent approved by the institutional review board at the Medical University of South Carolina.
Measures
Odor sensitivity was determined via the Smell Threshold Test™ (STT™ [79]). The STT™ required the systematic presentation of a set of squeeze bottles containing a serial dilution of phenyl-ethyl alcohol (PEA), a “rose-like” scent. In a single staircase method with forced choice regarding which bottle smelled stronger, a bottle containing a given concentration of PEA was presented under the nose in rapid succession with an odorless bottle. Propylene glycol served as the odorless control. Subsequent presentation of a higher or lower concentration of odorant was dependent on a correct or incorrect response from the previous trial. This method was repeated until 7 reversals (up and down the staircase) were made. Odor detection threshold score was determined by the average of the last 4 reversals. The STT™ is based upon over three decades of research, with norms based on hundreds of subjects, and 75%, 95%, and 99% confidence intervals provided for each decade of age (80, 81).
Pain testing involved the application of a thermal stimulus to the left volar forearm that increased in temperature from room temperature (32° C) at a rate of 0.25° C per second until participants could no longer tolerate the pain (see Powers et al. [70]). Participants were able to stop the thermode at peak tolerance using a handheld button, and the thermode was programmed to stop heating at 52° C to avoid the possibility of tissue damage.
Anxiety sensitivity, or the fear of experiencing anxiety and its related cognitive, physiological, and social consequences, was measured with the 18-item Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3 (ASI; 82). Using a 5-point rating scale that ranged from 0 (very little) to 4 (very much), items were rated according to how much the respondent agreed to each statement. The cognitive concerns items related to fear of the mental consequences of anxiety such as worry of “going crazy” or being “mentally ill”. The physical concerns items related to fear of anxiety-related physiological arousal including worry of a “heart attack” or “choking to death”. And finally, the social concerns items related to fear of the social aspects of anxiety such as worry of being evaluated negatively by others for blushing, sweating, or fainting. Total ASI scores were calculated by the sum of all items and could range from 0 to 72, with higher scores indicating greater anxiety sensitivity. The psychometric properties of the ASI are adequate to good on indices of reliability and validity (82, 83). In the current sample, internal consistency was good as evidenced by a Cronbach’s alpha of .88.
Procedure
The brief CBI (developed by Powers et al. [70]) was delivered via a 3-minute audio recording consisting of some key components of CBT, delivered to participants via audio recording on headphones. The first component of the intervention involved cognitive framing, which included information about the nature of the thermal pain tolerance task, information about the minimal risk of thermal stimulation to do harm, and information about the participant’s capacity to control/stop the stimulation at any time. The second component consisted of cognitive restructuring, which included information and examples of negative or pain-catastrophizing thoughts, how to recognize negative thoughts, and how to change negative thoughts into positive or self-supportive thoughts regarding pain tolerance. Finally, participants were given a distraction technique, which involved encouragement to engage in tasks like slowly counting backwards from 10 to distract from the pain and to challenge impulses to stop the painful stimulation early. In contrast, those who received the control condition listened to a different 3-minute audio recording through headphones describing pain physiology (e.g., function and purpose of pain, information about transduction, transmission and modulation), gate theory of pain (e.g., information about spinal and brain stem processes involved in limiting pain signals reaching the level of perception), and central pain processing (e.g., sensory, affective, and cognitive dimensions of pain experience, information about pain processing in the reticular system, somatosensory cortex and the limbic system). The audio recordings were balanced for length and number of words, were written at an eighth grade comprehension level, and the same person’s voice was used for both recordings. Experimenters were blind to which recording the participants received. A manipulation check was performed to ensure validity of the interventions, which revealed that participants were engaged and learned material unique to their condition (see Powers et al. [70]).
The tDCS session was 20-minutes long and conducted with the Phoresor-II Auto (Model PM850, lomed, Salt Lake City, Utah, USA; this product is not labeled for the use under discussion) using 2.0mA current. We used 4×4 cm sponge electrodes soaked in sterile saline. The first electrode was placed over the left DLPFC (F3 from the EEQ 10-20 system) located via the Beam F3 measurement system (84). In order to isolate the effects of directional current flow on the DLPFC, and to minimize confounding effects of potential distal brain changes associated with placement of the reference electrode over cortical areas that might impact pain perception, sensory processing, and/or cognitive processing (85), the second electrode was attached to the right shoulder. Sham tDCS consisted of turning the device on for 30 seconds to temporarily mimic tingling and skin sensations of verum tDCS and then ramped-down to 0mA for the remainder of the 20-minute session (86, 87). An attached laptop computer conducted custom-developed software, which interfaced with an ONTRAK ADU218 device that randomized participants, keeping both the participant and device operator masked to condition. The system delivered anodal, cathodal, or sham stimulation. The current density and total charge delivered according to the above parameters is consistent with those used safely in other studies (88). Participants were given vitamin-E cream to apply to the scalp following tDCS to reduce possible skin irritation. At the end of the study, participants were asked to guess whether they received active or sham tDCS. Guesses were no better than chance for all groups (see Powers et al. [70]).
After providing consent, participants completed the ASI-3, followed by a baseline assessment of their odor threshold via the STT, and then baseline pain testing. Participants were then randomized to receive either cathodal, anodal, or sham tDCS, and either CBI or a control intervention. Thus, participants were randomized to one of six conditions (groups) in a 3×2×2 design (cathodal/anodal/sham tDCS x CBI/control x time). This design could be seen as too complex for traditional treatment outcome research (89), but the purpose of the current exploratory study was to generate preliminary data for future hypothesis-driven clinical trials. The number of participants assigned to each condition, and their demographic characteristics, are shown in Table 1. After receiving their experimental intervention, participants completed follow-up pain and STT™ testing. The experiment took approximately 1 hour and participants were compensated $50 for time and effort.
Table 1.
Group demographic characteristics
Sham-Control | Sham-CBI | Anodal-Control | Anodal-CBI | Cathodal-Control | Cathodal-CBI | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Group | (n = 12) | (n = 11) | (n = 14) | (n = 13) | (n = 11) | (n = 11) | p-value |
Age: M(SD) | 31.33 (12.07) | 26.18 (4.85) | 31.79 (12.12) | 31.00 (14.36) | 33.82 (13.53) | 30.73 (9.65) | .76 |
Sex (% female) | 8 (66.7%) | 7 (63.6%) | 11 (78.6%) | 12 (92.3%) | 10 (90.9%) | 7 (63.6%) | .34 |
Race (% white) | 12 (100%) | 10 (90.9%) | 11 (78.6%) | 11 (84.6%) | 10 (90.9%) | 9 (81.8%) | .76 |
CBI = brief cognitive behavioral intervention
Statistical Analyses
Because the current data were collected as an exploratory study within an ongoing investigation, effect size and, in turn, sample size was determined by the main study hypotheses, namely the effects of CBI and tDCS on pain tolerance. Thus, we conducted a post-hoc power analysis to determine whether the current sample was powered adequately to detect significant group differences in odor threshold, i.e. detect medium effect sizes (Cohen’s f = .25) given the current sample size, number of groups and measurements, and alpha level at .05. Data were also checked to ensure they met the assumptions of parametric tests. Independent-samples t-tests were used to rule out whether demographic variables influenced baseline or follow-up odor threshold scores, while factorial ANOVA determined any baseline differences in odor threshold across groups. However, even in the case where no relationships exist between demographic variables and odor sensitivity, they could still influence change over time and were thus included as covariates in the primary analytic model. In addition, a repeated-measures analysis (versus ANCOVA) was chosen to control for dependencies between measures within subjects (90) and to ensure that the analysis reflected differences in the change in odor sensitivity, rather than differences in post-intervention odor sensitivity per se (91). Thus, a 3 (sham, anodal or cathodal tDCS) x 2 (CBI or control) x time (pre- and post-intervention) repeated measures ANCOVA (age and sex as covariates) was used to examined the effects of the interventions on the change in odor threshold score. To better clarify the nature of any intervention by time interactions, group differences in odor threshold using percent change from baseline were explored with univariate ANOVA and post-hoc tests (uncorrected). And finally, associations between anxiety sensitivity and odor threshold at baseline, follow-up, and in the percent change from baseline were examined using correlation with a two-tailed test of significance. Critical alpha level was set at .05.
RESULTS
The post-hoc power analysis revealed that we achieved adequate power (ß = .90). Thus, we determined that the current dataset was suitable to test the proposed hypotheses. Kolmogorov-Smirnov and Shapiro-Wilks tests revealed that odor threshold data at baseline and post-treatment, as well as ASI scores, were not normally distributed. While the odor threshold data were only non-normal in some conditions, the ASI data were consistently positively skewed and platykurtic. Given that ANOVA is typically robust to violations of normality (92) and that the assumption of sphericity was not violated (Χ2 = .00, p > .999), the data were deemed suitable for parametric testing in the main analysis.
In the overall sample of 72 adults, mean odor threshold was −4.69 (±1.24) at baseline and −4.56 (±1.06) at follow-up. There were two participants (one from the Anodal-Control group and the other from the Cathodal-CBI group) whom had baseline odor threshold scores of −2, indicative of a significant olfactory deficit, and thus removed from subsequent analyses. Sex had no impact on odor threshold at baseline (Mmale = −4.89, SDmale = 1.29; Mfemale = −4.73, SDfemale = 1.14; p = .63), at post intervention follow-up (Mmale = −4.70, SDmale = 0.92; Mfemale = −4.56, SDfemale = 1.10; p = .63), or in percent change from baseline (Mmale = 0.35, SDmale = 21.28; Mfemale = 1.20, SDfemale = 22.91; p = .89). There were also no associations between age and odor threshold at baseline, follow-up, or in the percent change from baseline (all p-values > .22). Table 2 shows pre- and post-intervention odor thresholds for each group. There were no differences between the six groups on odor detection threshold at baseline (F[5, 64] = .45, p = .81), or post-intervention (F[5, 64] = .35, p = .88).
Table 2.
Mean (SD) odor threshold scores before and after intervention across groups.
Odor Threshold | ||
---|---|---|
Group | Baseline | Post-Intervention |
Sham-CBI | −4.92 (1.78) | −4.40 (1.18) |
Anodal-CBI | −4.95 (0.90) | −4.34 (1.12) |
Cathodal-CBI | −5.06 (0.43) | −4.65 (0.80) |
Sham-Control | −4.48 (1.23) | −4.74 (1.08) |
Anodal-Control | −4.58 (1.35) | −4.78 (1.14) |
Cathodal-Control | −4.64 (0.96) | −4.65 (1.09) |
CBI = brief cognitive behavioral intervention
Results of the RM ANCOVA revealed that the CBI, but not the tDCS, intervention was associated with a change in odor threshold. Specifically, there was a significant interaction effect of CBI and time (F[1, 62] = 7.20, p = .009, ηp2 = .10), demonstrating that odor threshold scores were significantly increased (odor sensitivity was reduced) in participants who underwent CBI (i.e. cognitive framing, cognitive restructuring, and behavioral techniques), but unchanged in participants who received the control condition (i.e. information on pain theory, physiology, and processing). Subsequent analysis of the percent change in odor threshold showed a trend-level effect of group (F[5, 64] = 1.98, p = .09, ηp2 = .13), and significant differences between the Anodal-CBI and both the Sham-Control (p = .02) and Anodal-Control (p = .03) groups. Moreover, there were trend-level differences between the Sham-Control and both the Sham-CBI (p = .09) and Cathodal-CBI (p = .06) groups, as well as between the Cathodal-CBI and the Anodal-Control groups (p = .09) (see Figure 1). In contrast to the significant interaction of CBI and time, there was no interaction between tDCS and time (F[2, 62] = .11, p = .90), nor any interaction between tDCS, CBI, and time (F[2, 62] = .32, p = .73).
Figure 1.
Mean (SEM) percent change from baseline in the detection (threshold score) of phenyl ethyl alcohol (PEA), a neutral, rose-like, odor. Participants were randomized to either anodal, cathodal, or sham transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) aimed at the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), plus a brief cognitive behavioral intervention (CBI) or a control condition. Participants who underwent CBI, regardless of tDCS condition, demonstrated a significant (or trend-level) difference in the percent change in odor sensitivity compared to participants in the pure control condition (Sham-Control). * = p < .10 and ** = p < .05
Because ASI scores were highly non-normal in distribution, Spearman’s rank-order correlation coefficients were used. Results showed no significant associations between AS and odor threshold at baseline or post-intervention in the overall sample (all p-values > .22). However, severity of AS related to a greater reduction in odor sensitivity in those that received CBI (ρ = .364, p = .035), but was unrelated to changes in odor sensitivity in those that received the control intervention (ρ = −.093, p = .59) (see Figure 2).
Figure 2.
Results indicated a significant correlation between anxiety sensitivity (measured by the Anxiety Sensitivity Index, ASI) and reduction in odor sensitivity after a brief cognitive behavioral intervention (CBI) for pain (ρ = .364, p = .035; solid trend line). This relationship was not found for participants who underwent the control intervention (ρ = −.093, p = .59; dashed trend line).
DISCUSSION
Results of the current study indicate that brief CBI for pain tolerance, but not tDCS aimed at DLPFC, associated with reduced odor sensitivity in healthy adults. In addition, more severe anxiety sensitivity associated with greater decreases in odor sensitivity in persons who received CBI. These results suggest that at least some features of CBT for somatic issues may influence olfactory sensitivity, particularly in persons with high anxiety sensitivity.
Indeed, anxiety sensitivity is related to the fear of aversive bodily sensations (93). Studies have shown that persons high in anxiety sensitivity are less tolerant of pain (15, 94), more perceptive of anxiety-related sensations (95), and more sensitive to threat (96), including threat-related odors (1). Yet, because PEA is a somewhat neutral odor, it is unlikely that CBI influenced participants’ perceptual thresholds via their cognitive appraisals of its threat-relevance. A more likely explanation, based in part upon the highly overlapping limbic circuitry that sub-serves both emotional and olfactory processes (97, 98), is that CBI was associated with dampening of participants’ overall limbic activity and thus gain on olfactory processing, particularly in those with higher baseline anxiety sensitivity. We know from our previous work that anxiety sensitivity associates with odor hypersensitivity (1), and from the work of other investigators that high rates of anxiety and psychological distress exist within MCS (25–28, 59, 99, 100). Thus, the findings from the present study lend additional support for the notion that odor hypersensitivity/MCS is linked to affective psychopathology, while also suggesting that CBT may be an effective treatment for odor hypersensitivity, especially in anxious populations like MCS.
Modulation of DLPFC activity via tDCS, while effective for pain tolerance (70), was not associated with a change in odor sensitivity. Though preliminary, this perhaps indicates that neither activation nor deactivation of DLPFC influences the detection of small-to-modest differences in odor concentration. It may also indicate that prefrontal modulation of odor and pain appear to be sub-served via distinct neural circuitry. Indeed, while studies have shown that damage to prefrontal cortex (i.e., alcoholic Korsakoff’s disease) impairs odor detection (101), other work indicates that the involvement of the frontal cortices in olfaction is mostly constrained to the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) (102–104). Yet this seemingly disparate finding for prefrontal modulation of pain and odors does not rule out the possibility that separate DLPFC and OFC pathways impinge upon, or regulate, a final common pathway underlying the core features of chronic pain, odor hypersensitivity (i.e. MCS), and other disorders of central sensitization.
While the present study was not specifically designed to determine the neural substrates of pain versus odor sensitivity, future studies should address this topic and also whether tDCS and/or CBI impact odor processing of more intense and/or noxious odors (e.g. sniff volume could replace detection as the measure of olfactory function). Several limitations to the current study should also be addressed in future investigations. First, sample size in the current study was relatively small, given that groups consisted of between 11 and 14 participants. While it would be ideal to conduct clinical trials with cell sizes of at least 30, the current pilot study was well powered to detect medium effect sizes. Nevertheless, there still could have been meaningful effects that our study was underpowered to detect. On the other hand, the fact that we tested multiple hypotheses increased the risk of Type I error. Given the exploratory nature of this study, future analyses would need to be well powered and more stringent by correcting for multiple comparisons. In addition, because the current sample was composed of healthy participants without clinically significant odor hypersensitivity, it is unclear whether CBT for somatic processes and/or tDCS aimed at different neural circuits would be clinically robust in MCS. Along the same lines, the CBI in the current study was very brief, and while successful for reducing odor sensitivity acutely, may not have long-term therapeutic effects. It is also likely that the CBI was too brief to be deemed a treatment, and its effects on odor sensitivity may be due to other effects such as distraction. Despite these limitations however, this exploratory study lends support for a more rigorous examination of CBT as a behavioral intervention for individuals suffering from pathological odor hypersensitivity.
While future research may eventually elucidate mechanisms of change, we can conclude that cognitive-behavioral techniques for somatic processes have some initial promise for treating issues related to heightened odor sensitivity, a core feature of MCS. Thus the clinical implication of these findings is that clinicians could implement CBT techniques in an attempt to modulate odor sensitivity in persons with MCS. For example, clinicians could educate their patients that problematic odor sensitivity can be managed, was well as draw upon common CBT techniques to discourage maladaptive thoughts and problematic avoidance. The present findings may also have implications beyond MCS, extending a range of somatic issues (i.e. perceptual sensitivities) that are found across multiple forms of psychopathology including autism spectrum disorders (3), Tourette’s disorder (2), obsessive-compulsive disorder (105), trichotillomania (106, 107), attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (108–110), posttraumatic stress disorder (111–113), panic disorder (115), generalized anxiety disorder (115), and social phobia (115).
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
The authors would like to thank the participants in this study as well as Abigail Powers, Alok Madan, Megan Hilbert, Scott Reeves, Mark George, and Mark Nash.
Funding for this study was provided by NIH/NCATS Grant Number UL1TR000062.
Abbreviations:
- MCS
Multiple Chemical Sensitivity
- CBT
Cognitive Behavior Therapy
- CBI
cognitive behavioral intervention
- DLPFC
Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex
- tDCS
transcranial direct current stimulation
- PEA
brief, phenyl-ethyl alcohol
- STT™
Smell Threshold Test™
- ASI-3
Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3
- AS
anxiety sensitivity
Footnotes
All authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.
REFERENCES
- 1.Houghton DC, Howard S, Paquet C, Uhde TW, Schlosser R, Cortese BM. Odor sensitivity impairment: A behavioral marker of psychological distress? CNS Spectrums, in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Houghton DC, Capriotti MR, Conelea CA, Woods DW. Sensory phenomena in Tourette syndrome: Their role in symptom formation and treatment. Curr Dev Disord Rep 2014;1:245–251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Leekam SR, Nieto C, Libbj SJ, Wing L, Gould J. Describing the sensory abnormalities of children and adults with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 2007;37:894–910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.da Silva Prado H, do Rosario MC, Lee J, Hounie AG, Shavitt RG, Miguel EC. Sensory phenomena in obsessive-compulsive disorder and tic disorders: A review of the literature. CNS Spectrums 2008;13:425–432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Miller LJ, Nielsen DM, Schoen SA, Brett-Green BA. Perspectives on sensory processing disorder: A call for translational research. Front Int Neurosci 2009;3:1–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Bailer J, Witthöft M, Rist F. The chemical odor sensitivity scale: Reliability and validity of a screening instrument for idiopathic environmental intolerance. J Psychosom Res 2006;61: 71–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kreutzer R, Neutra RR, Lashuay N. Prevalence of people reporting sensitivities to chemicals in a population based survey. Am J Epidemiol 1999;150:1–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Meggs WJ, Dunn KA, Dunn KA, Goodman PE, Davidoff AL. Prevalence and nature of allergy and chemical sensitivity in a general population. Arch Environ Health 1996;51:275–282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Cullen MR. Multiple chemical sensitivities: Summary and directions for future investigators. J Occup Med 1987;2:801–804. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Miller CS. Toxicant-induced loss of tolerance—an emerging theory of disease? Environ Health Persp 1997;105:445–453. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Sparks PJ, Daniell W, Black DW, Kipen HM, Altman LC, Simon GE, Terr AI. Multiple chemical sensitivity syndrome: A clinical perspective. I. Case definition, theories of pathogenesis, and research needs. J Occup Med 1994;36:718–730. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Park J, Knudson S. Medically unexplained physical symptoms. Health Rep 2007;18:43–47. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Tran MTD, Arendt-Nielsen L, Kupers R, Elberling J. Multiple chemical sensitivity: On the scent of central sensitization. Int J Hyg Environ Health 2013;216:202–210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Clauw DJ. Fibromyalgia and related conditions. Mayo Clin Proc 2015:90:680–692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Asmundson GJ, Taylor S. Role of anxiety sensitivity in pain-related fear and avoidance. J Behav Med 1996;19:577–586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Holst H, Arendt-Nielsen L, Mosbech H, Elberling J. Increased capsaicin-induced secondary hyperalgesia in patients with multiple chemical sensitivity. Clin J Pain 2011;27:156–162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Aaron LA, Burke MM, Buchwald D. Overlapping conditions among patients with chronic fatigue syndrome, fibromyalgia, and temporomandibular disorder. Arch Intern Med 2000;160:221–227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Yunus MB. Central sensitivity syndromes: A new paradigm and group nosology for fibromyalgia and overlapping conditions, and the related issue of disease versus illness. Semin Arthritis Rheum 2008;37:339–352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Miller CS. Toxicant-induced loss of tolerance—an emerging theory of disease?. Environ Health Perspect 1997;105:445–453. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Pall ML. Elevated nitric oxide/peroxynitrite theory of multiple chemical sensitivity: Central role of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors in the sensitivity mechanism. Environ Health Perspect 2003;111:1461–1464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.McKeown-Eyssen G, Baines C, Cole DE, Riley N, Tyndale RF, Marshall L, Jazmaji V. Case-control study of genotypes in multiple chemical sensitivity syndrome: CYP2D6, NAT1, NAT2, PON1, PON2, and MTHFR. Int J Epidemiol 2004;33:971–978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Meggs WJ. Multiple chemical sensitivities and the immune syndrome. Toxicol Ind Health 1992;8:203–214. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Meggs WJ. Neurogenic inflammation and sensitivity to environmental chemicals. Environ Health Perspect 1993;101:234–238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Donnay AH. On the recognition of multiple chemical sensitivity in medical literature and government policy. Int J Toxicol 1999;18:383–392. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Bornschein S, Hausteiner C, Zilker T, Förstl H. Psychiatric and somatic disorders and multiple chemical sensitivity (MCS) in 264 ‘environmental patients’. Psychol Med 2002;32:1387–1394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Poonai NP, Antony MM, Binkley KE, Stenn P, Swinson RP, Corey P, Silverman FS, Tarlo SM. Psychological features of subjects with idiopathic environmental intolerance. J Psychosom Res 2001;51:537–541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Simon GE, Katon WJ, Sparks PJ. Allergic to life: Psychological factors in environmental illness. Am J Psychiatry 1990;147:901–906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Simon GE. Psychiatric symptoms in multiple chemical sensitivity. Toxicol Ind Health 1994;10:487–496. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Henningsson M, Sundbom E. Conversion disorder and multiple chemical sensitivity: A comparative study of psychological defense strategies. Percept Mot Skills 2000;91:803–818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Staudenmayer H, Selner ME, Selner JC. Adult sequelae of childhood abuse presenting as environmental illness. Ann Allergy 1993;71:538–546. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Brodsky CM. ‘Allergic to everything’: A medical subculture. Psychosomatics 1983;24:731–732. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Gomez RL, Schvaneveldt RW, Staudenmayer H. Assessing beliefs about ‘environmental illness/multiple chemical sensitivity’. J Health Psychol 1996;1:107–123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Hauge CR, Rasmussen A, Piet J, Bonde JP, Jensen C, Sumbundu A, Skovbjerg S. Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT) for multiple chemical sensitivity (MCS): Results from a randomized controlled trial with 1 year follow-up. J Psychosom Res 2015;79:628–634. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Sampalli T, Berlasso E, Fox R, Petter M. A controlled study of the effect of a mindfulness-based stress reduction technique in women with multiple chemical sensitivity, chronic fatigue syndrome, and fibromyalgia. J Multidiscip Healthc 2009;2:53–59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Skovbjerg S, Hauge CR, Rasmussen A, Winkel P, Elberling J. Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy to treat multiple chemical sensitivities: A randomized pilot trial. Scand J Psychol 2012;53:233–238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Guglielmi RS, Cox DJ, Spyker DA. Behavioral treatment of phobic avoidance in multiple chemical sensitivity. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 1994;25:197–209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Stenn P, Binkley. Successful outcome in a patient with chemical sensitivity: Treatment with psychological desensitization and selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor. Psychosomatics 1998;39:547–550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Spyker DA. Multiple chemical sensitivities—Syndrome and solution. J Toxicol: Clin Toxicol 1995;2:95–99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Bernstein RE, Angell KL, Dehle CM. A brief course of cognitive behavioural therapy for the treatment of misophonia: A case example. Cogn Behav Ther 2013;6:1–13. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Dozier TH. Counterconditioning treatment for misophonia. Clin Case Stud 2015;14:374–387. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Dozier TH. Treating the initial physical reflex of misophonia with the neural repatterning technique: A counterconditioning procedure. Psychological Thought 2015;8:189–210. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Johnson PL, Webber TA, Wu MS, Lewin AB, Murphy TK, Storch EA. When selective audiovisual stimuli become unbearable: A case series on pediatric misophonia. Neuropsychiatry 2013;3:569–575. [Google Scholar]
- 43.McGuire JF, Wu MS, Storch EA. Cognitive-behavioral therapy for 2 youths with misophonia. J Clin Psychiatry 2015;76: 573–574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Webber TA, Johnson PL, Storch EA. Pediatric misophonia with comorbid obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorders. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 2014;36:231–e1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Butler AC, Chapman JE, Forman EM, Beck AT. The empirical status of cognitive-behavioral therapy: A review of meta-analyses. Clin Psychol Rev 2006;26:17–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Ehde DM, Dillworth TM, Turner JA. Cognitive-behavioral therapy for individuals with chronic pain: Efficacy, innovations, and directions for research. Am Psychol 2014;69:153–166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.McBeth J, Prescott G, Scotland G, Lovell K, Keeley P, Hannaford P, McNamee P, Symmons DPM, Woby S, Gkazinou C, Beasley M, Macfarlane GJ. Cognitive behavior therapy, exercise, or both for treating chronic widespread pain. Arch Intern Med 2012;172:48–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Morley S, Eccleston C, Williams A. Systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials of cognitive behavior therapy and behavior therapy for chronic pain in adults, excluding headache. Pain 1999;80:1–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Bennett R, Nelson D. Cognitive behavioral therapy for fibromyalgia. Nature Rev Rheum 2006;2:416–424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Goldenberg DL, Kaplan KH, Nadeau MG, Brodeur C, Smith S, Schmid CH. A controlled study of a stress-reduction, cognitive-behavioral treatment program in fibromyalgia. J Musculoskel Pain 1994;2:53–66. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Lera S, Gelman SM, López MJ, Abenoza M, Zorrilla JG, Castro-Fornieles J, Salamero M. Multidisciplinary treatment of fibromyalgia: Does cognitive behavior therapy increase the response to treatment? J Psychosom Res 2009;67:443–441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Mengshoel AM, Forseth KO, Haugen M, Walle-Hansen R, Forre O. Multidisciplinary approach to fibromyalgia: A pilot study. Clin Rheum 1995;14:165–170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Nielson WR, Walker C, McCain GA. Cognitive behavioral treatment of fibromyalgia syndrome: Preliminary findings. J Rheum 1992;19:98–103. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Nezu AM, Nezu CM, Lombardo ER. Cognitive-behavior therapy for medically unexplained symptoms: A critical review of the treatment literature. Behav Ther 2001;32:537–583. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Singh BB, Berman BM, Hadhazy VA, Creamer P. A pilot study of cognitive behavioral therapy in fibromyalgia. Alt Ther Health Med 1998;4:67–70. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Thieme K, Flor H, Turk DC. Psychological pain treatment in fibromyalgia syndrome: Efficacy of operant behavioural and cognitive behavioural treatments. Arthr Res Ther 2006;8:R121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Williams DA, Cary MA, Groner KH, Chaplin W, Glazer LJ, Rodriguez AM, Clauw DJ. Improving physical functional status in patients with fibromyalgia: A brief cognitive behavioral intervention. J Rheum 2002;29:1280–1286. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Turk DC, Okifuji A. Psychological factors in chronic pain: Evolution and revolution. J Consul Clin Psychol 2002;70:678–690. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Devriese S, Winters W, Stegen K, Van Diest I, Veulemans H, Nemery B, Eelen P, Van de Woestijne K, Van den Bergh O. Generalization of acquired somatic symptoms in response to odors: A pavlovian perspective on multiple chemical sensitivity. Psychosom Med 2000;62:751–759. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Alobid I, Nogué S, Izquierdo-Dominguez A, Centellas S, Bernal-Sprekelsen M, Mullol J. Multiple chemical sensitivity worsens quality of life and cognitive and sensorial features of sense of smell. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol 2014;271:3203–3208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Borckardt JJ, Smith AR, Hutcheson K, Johnson K, Nahas Z, Anderson B, Schneider MB, Reeves ST, George MS. Reducing pain and unpleasantness during repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. J ECT 2006;22:259–264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Borckardt JJ, Reeves ST, Weinstein M, Smith AR, Shelley N, Kozel FA, Nahas Z, Byrne KT, Morgan J, George MS.Significant analgesic effects of one session of postoperative left prefrontal cortex repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation: A replication study. Brain Stim 2008;2:122–127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Borckardt JJ, Reeves ST, Frohman H, Madan A, Jensen MP, Patterson D, Barth K, Smith AR, Gracely R, George MS. Fast left prefrontal rTMS acutely suppresses analgesic effects of perceived controllability on the emotional component of pain experience. Pain 2011;152:182–187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Borckardt JJ, Glaser JA, Carver KC, Reeves ST, George MS, Fredrich S, Hohenberger J. Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) in the management of acute post-spine surgery pain: A prospective randomized controlled trial. Spine J 2013;13:S119. [Google Scholar]
- 65.Borckardt JJ, Reeves ST, Kotlowski P, Abernathy JH, Field LC, Dong L, Frohman H, Moore H, Ryan K, Madan A, George MS. Fast left prefrontal rTMS reduces post-gastric bypass surgery pain: Findings from a large-scale, double-blind, sham-controlled clinical trial. Brain Stim 2014;7:42–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Khedr EM, Kotb H, Kamel NF, Ahmed MA, Sadek R, Rothwell JC. Longlasting antalgic effects of daily sessions of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in central and peripheral neuropathic pain. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2005;76:833–838. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Mylius V, Borckardt JJ, Lefaucheur JP Noninvasive cortical modulation of experimental pain. Pain 2012;153:1350–1363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Short EB, Borckart JJ, Anderson BS, Frohman H, Beam W, Reeves ST, George MS. Ten sessions of adjunctive left prefrontal rTMS significantly reduces fibromyalgia pain: A randomized, controlled pilot study. Pain 2011;152:2477–2484. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Boggio PS, Zaghi S, Fregni F. Modulation of emotions associated with images of human pain using anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Neuropsychologia 2009;47:212–217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Powers A, Madan A, Hilbert M, Reeves ST, George M, Nash MR, Borckardt JJ. Effects of combining a brief cognitive intervention with transcranial direct current stimulation on pain tolerance: A randomized controlled pilot study. Pain Med 2017;19:677–685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Tran MTD, Skovbjerg S, Arendt-Nielsen L, Christensen KB, Elberling J. Transcranial pulsed electromagnetic fields for multiple chemical sensitivity: Study protocol for a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Trials 2013;14:256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Guo X, Gao G, Wang X, Li L, Li W, Liang Q, Zhang H. Effects of bilateral deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus on olfactory function in Parkinson’s disease patients. Stereotact Funct Neurosurg 2008;86:237–244. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Henkin RI, Potolicchio SJ, Levy LM. Improvement in smell and taste dysfunction after repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Am J Otolaryngol 2011;32:38–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Hummel T, Jahnke U, Sommer U, Reichmann H, Müller A. Olfactory function in patients with idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: Effects of deep brain stimulation in the subthalamic nucleus. J Neural Transm 2005;112:669–676. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Sandyk R Treatment with AC pulsed electromagnetic fields improves olfactory function in Parkinson’s disease. Int J Neurosci 1999;97:225–233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Andersson L, Claeson AS, Nyberg L, Nordin S. Short-term olfactory sensitization involves brain networks relevant for pain, and indicates chemical intolerance. Int J Hyg Environ Health 2017;220:503–509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Zald DH. The human amygdala and the emotional evaluation of sensory stimuli. Bran Res Brain Res Rev 2003;41:88–123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Cortese BM, McConnell PA, Froeliger B, Leslie K, Uhde TW. Burning odor-elicited anxiety in OEF/OIF combat veterans: Inverse relationship to gray matter volume in olfactory cortex. J Psychiatric Res 2015;70:58–66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Sensonics, Inc. Smell Threshold TestTM. Haddon Heights, NJ, USA [Google Scholar]
- 80.Doty RL. The Smell Threshold Test Administration Manual. 2nd Edition. Haddon Heights, NJ: Sensonics, Inc; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- 81.Pierce JD. Doty RL, Amoore JE. Analysis of position of trial sequence and type of diluent on the detection threshold for phenyl ethyl alcohol using a single staircase method. Percept Mot Skills, 1996;82:451–458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Taylor S, Zvolensky MJ, Cox BJ, Deacon B, Heimberg RG, Ledley DR, Abramowitz JS, Holaway RM, Sandin B, Stewart SH, Coles M, Eng W, Daly E, Arrindell WA, Bouvard M, Cardenas SJ. Robust dimensions of anxiety sensitivity: Development and initial validation of the Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3. Psychol Assess 2007;19:176–188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Osman A, Gutierrez PM, Smith K, Fang Q, Lozano G, Devine A. The anxiety sensitivity index-3: Analyses of dimensions, reliability estimates, and correlates in nonclinical samples. J Pers Assess 2010;92:45–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Beam W, Borckardt JJ, Reeves ST, George MS. An efficient and accurate new method for locating the F3 position for prefrontal TMS applications. Brain Stim 2002;2:50–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Scheldrup M, Greenwood PM, McKendrick R, Strohl J, Bikson M, Alam M, McKinley RA, Parasuraman R. Transcranial direct current stimulation facilitates cognitive multi-task performance differentially depending on anode location and subtask. Front Human Neurosci 2014;8:665. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Ambrus GG, Al-Moyed H, Chaieb L, Sarp L, Antal A, Paulus W. The fade in – Short stimulation – Fade out approach to sham tDCS – Reliable at 1 mA for naïve and experienced subjects, but not investigators. Brain Stim 2012;5:499–504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Brunoni AR, Schestatsky P, Lotufo PA, Benseñor IM, Fregni F. Comparison of blinding effectiveness between sham tDCS and placebo sertraline in a 6-week major depression randomized clinical trial. Clin Neurophysiol 2014;125:298–305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Fregni F, Nitsche MA, Loo CK, Brunoni AR, Marangolo P, Leite J, Carvalho S, Bolognini N, Caumo W, Paik NJ, Simis M, Ueda K, Ekhitari H, Luu P, Tucker DM, Tyler WJ, Brunelin J, Datta A, Juan CH, Venkatasubramanian G, Boggio PS, Bikson M. Regulatory considerations for the clinical and research use of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS): Review and recommendations from an expert panel. Clin Res Regul Affairs 2015;32:22–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Moore CG, Carter RE, Nietert PJ, Stewart PW. Recommendations for planning pilot studies in clinical and translational research. Clin Transl Sci 2011;4:332–337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Singh V, Rana RK, Singhal R. Analysis of repeated measurement data in the clinical trials. J Ayurveda Integr Med 2013;4:77–81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Vickers AJ. Analysis of variance is easily misapplied in the analysis of randomized trials: A critique and discussion of alternative statistical approaches. Psychosom Med 2005;67:652–655. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Schmider E, Ziegler M, Danay E, Beyer L, Bühner M. Is it really robust? Reinvestigating the robustness of ANOVA against violations of the normal distribution assumption. Methodology 2010;6:147–151. [Google Scholar]
- 93.Reiss S Expectancy model of fear, anxiety, and panic. Clin Psychol Rev 1991;11:141–153. [Google Scholar]
- 94.Schmidt NB, Cook JH. Effects of anxiety sensitivity on anxiety and pain during a cold pressor challenge in patients with panic disorder. Behav Res Ther 1999;37:313–323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Sturges LV, Goetsch VL. Psychophysiological reactivity and heartbeat awareness in anxiety sensitivity. J Anx Disord 1996;10:283–294. [Google Scholar]
- 96.Nelson BD, Hodges A, Hajcak G, Shankman SA. Anxiety sensitivity and the anticipation of predictable and unpredictable threat: Evidence from the startle response and event-related potentials. J Anx Disord 2015;33:62–71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.LeDoux J Rethinking the emotional brain. Neuron 2012;73:653–676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Price JL. Olfactory system In n G, ed. The Human Nervous System. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1990. [Google Scholar]
- 99.Bell IR, Miller CS, Schwartz GE, Peterson JM, Amend D. Neuropsychiatric and somatic characteristics of young adults with and without self-reported chemical odor intolerance and chemical sensitivity. Arch Environ Health 1996;51:9–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Bell IR, Schwartz GE, Peterson JM, Amend D. Symptom and personality profiles of young adults from a college student population with self-reported illness from food and chemicals. J Am Coll Nutr 1993;12: 693–702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Potter H, Butters N. An assessment of olfactory deficits in patients with damage to prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia 1980;18:621–628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Gottfried JA, Zald DH. On the scent of human olfactory orbitofrontal cortex: Meta-analysis and comparison to non-human primates. Brain Res Rev 2005;50:287–304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Levy LM, Henkin RI, Hutter A, Lin CS, Martins D, Schellinger D. Functional MRI of human olfaction. J Comput Assist Tomogr 1997;21:849–856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Zald DH, Pardo JV. Emotion, olfaction, and the human amygdala: Amygdala activation during aversive olfactory stimulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci 1997;94:4119–4124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Anderson AK, Christoff K, Stappen I, Panitz D, Ghahremani DG, Glover G, Gabrieli JDE, Sobel N. Dissociated neural representations of intensity and valence in human olfaction. Nature Neurosci 2003;6:196–202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Atanasova B, Graux J, El Hage W, Hommet C, Camus V, Belzung C. Olfaction: A potential cognitive marker of psychiatric disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2008;32:1315–1325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Rieke EF, Anderson D. Adolscent/Adult Sensory Profile and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Occup Ther 2009;63:138–145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Falkenstein MJ, Conelea CA, Garner LE, Haaga DAF. Sensory over-responsivity in trichotillomania (hair-pulling disorder). Psychiatry Res 2018;260:207–218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Houghton DC, Alexander MR, Bauer CC, Woods DW. Abnormal perceptual sensitivity in body-focused repetitive behaviors. Comp Psychiatry 2018;82:45–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Ghanizadeh A Sensory processing problems in children with ADHD: A systematic review. Psychiatry Investig 2011;8:89–94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Lane SJ, Reynolds S, Thacker L. Sensory over-responsivity and ADHD: Differentiating using electrodermal responses, cortisol, and anxiety. Front Integr Neurosci 2010;4:8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Reynolds S, Lane SJ. Sensory overresponsivity and anxiety in children with ADHD. Am J Occup Ther 2009;63:433–440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Cortese BM, Schumann AY, Howell AN, McConnell PA, Yang QX, Uhde TW. Preliminary evidence for differential olfactory and trigeminal processing in combat veterans with and without PTSD. NeuroImage: Clin 2018;17:378–387. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Wilkerson AK, Uhde TW, Leslie K, Freeman WC, LaRowe SD, Schumann A, Cortese BM. Paradoxical olfactory function in combat veterans: The role of PTSD and odor factors. Mil Psychol 2018;30:120–130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Daniels JK, Vermetten E. Odor-induced recall of emotional memories in PTSD-Review and new paradigm for research. Exp Neurol 2016;284:168–180 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Clepce M, Reich K, Gossler A, Kornhuber J, Theurauf N. Olfactory abnormalities in anxiety disorders. Neurosci Lett 2012;511:43–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]