Abstract
Impulsivity is a multidimensional construct. Although gambling disorder (GD) has been associated with high impulsivity, impulsivity across multiple domains has not been thoroughly investigated in this population. We first aimed to examine whether associations between three facets of impulsivity (response impulsivity, choice impulsivity and impulsive tendency) varied between GD patients and healthy controls (HC). We next aimed to evaluate relationships between these three types of impulsivity, as proposed by theoretical models of impulsivity, and their associations with GD severity. The sample included 97 treatment-seeking adult men with GD, diagnosed according to DSM-5 criteria, and 32 male HCs recruited from the general population. Greater impulsivity in all three domains was found in men with GD in comparison to men without GD. Associations between impulsivity facets were found in both groups, with response impulsivity being the only domain associated with GD severity. Our findings confirm that multiple domains of impulsivity are relevant in GD. Future studies should examine the extent to which treatments aimed at targeting specific aspects of impulsivity improve outcomes.
Subject terms: Psychology, Human behaviour
Introduction
Although impulsivity has been proposed as a multifactorial construct1, there is still a lack of consensus regarding its definition and the independence of impulsivity domains2. Impulsivity has been defined as a tendency to respond with little forethought, often with disregard to the negative consequences to the impulsive individual or others3. Impulsivity has been found to factor into multiple forms, including response and choice forms, that can be measured across species4–7. While multiple theoretical models have been proposed different types of impulsivity, the proposal by MacKillop et al.8 is widely used and validated in different populations. This model posits that impulsivity can be partitioned into three main domains: response impulsivity, choice impulsivity and impulsive tendencies.
Response impulsivity, also termed impulsive action or motor impulsivity, involves impairments in delaying, withholding or interrupting inappropriate responses4,9. High levels of this type of impulsivity have been associated with gambling disorder (GD), with GD participants demonstrating differences in response impulsivity in comparison with healthy control (HC) participants10, including within treatment-seeking samples11. Multiple studies suggest gambling severity is positively correlated with motor impulsivity9,12,13 and one recent meta-analysis found GD to be associated with significant impairments in motor and attentional inhibition14.
Delay discounting relates to impulsive choice and the extent to which an individual prefers a smaller-sooner over a larger-later reward5,15. In the case of GD, cognitive disturbances related to risk-reward decision making have been reported16,17, and individuals with GD tend to discount rewards more steeply than controls18–21. Increasing evidence supports that people with gambling problems prefer to obtain an economic reward in the most immediate way possible that provides them with the sensation that they are winning and able to continue betting, rather than receiving larger amounts of money in a more distant time22. However, some research on GD severity and choice impulsivity has been inconsistent. Some studies suggest that GD severity and age may be the best statistical predictors of individual differences in delay-discounting rates22–24 and a recent meta-analysis of impulsivity in GD found evidence of a significant publication bias in delay discounting studies in GD populations14. However, some others have found that choice impulsivity cannot discriminate between individuals with problematic gambling and those with GD13,25. Further, some theoretically related constructs (e.g., learning to make advantageous choices during a risk/reward decision-making task) have found GD to associate with disadvantageous decision-making whereas others have not26.
A third form of impulsivity, henceforth termed “impulsive tendency” (also known as impulsivity trait), has been proposed. For example, the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11) has been found to factor into several domains including those relating to motor, non-planning and inattention27,28. While the BIS-11 has been studied across diagnostic groups (including in studies linking the measure to biological measures like brain structure in GD, drug addiction and non-addicted states), concerns have been raised regarding inconsistencies in factor structure across studies29, including within GD samples30. On the other hand, the UPPS-P model, derived from the extant literature and updated over time, proposes five factors of impulsivity: (lack of) perseverance, (lack of) premeditation, positive and negative urgency, and sensation-seeking31,32. Empirical studies have repeatedly reported an association between GD and impulsive tendencies33–36. In particular, higher lack of perseverance, and positive and negative urgency levels have been found to be the features that best distinguish between patients with GD and HC37,38. Similarly, lack of premeditation is positively associated with poor decision-making and an inability to identify the possible negative financial outcomes that might stem from taking risks, a relevant feature in patients with GD20,39. Other studies highlight that urgency levels, characterized by the tendency to act rashly when experiencing extreme moods, are linked with GD severity and other impulsive behaviors40,41. It should also be noted that impulsivity stemming from enduring personality characteristics that lead gamblers to focus on short-term gains (i.e. trait impulsivity) have been found to be more strongly linked to problem gambling rather than momentary cognitive or affective disinhibition (i.e. state impulsivity)42.
The existing body of research on impulsivity and GD suggests an association between impulsive response, choice and tendency. Some studies uphold that sensation-seeking, lack of premeditation and urgency could be linked with choice impulsivity and response impulsivity2. Similarly, a correlation between impulsive tendencies and choice impulsivity has been described in GD, suggesting that individuals with GD who perceive themselves as being more prone to behaving impulsively may also make impulsive choices23. This correlation was also found in another study, but only in young patients with GD22. Relatedly, Kräplin et al.43 found that urgency and premeditation were specifically associated with disadvantageous decision-making. It also remains unclear whether impulsivity levels are associated with treatment outcome. Some research has found high levels of impulsive traits to be associated with relapse and dropout from treatment40, whereas other studies have found greater awareness of gambling-related problems to be associated with positive outcomes44. Finally, another study found GD to be associated with response impulsivity and choice impulsivity, although only the latter was linked with GD severity13.
At present, questions remain regarding relationships between response impulsivity, choice impulsivity, impulsive tendency and gambling. Empirical studies are needed to examine the multidimensional nature of these impulsive phenotypes in greater depth8, and whether the interrelatedness of these domains differ between those with GD and those without. As such, the aim of this study was two-fold. Our first aim was to examine whether the associations between three facets of impulsivity (response impulsivity, choice impulsivity and impulsive tendency) varied between GD patients and HC. Our second aim was to evaluate the intercorrelatedness of these three types of impulsivity in GD, and their association with GD severity. We hypothesized that GD, as compared to HC participants, would exhibit greater impulsivity in all three domains, and that response impulsivity, choice impulsivity, and impulsive tendency would correlate with one another to varying degrees in the GD group, and GD severity would relate to impulsivity in the GD group.
Material and Methods
Participants and procedure
An initial sample of 193 patients diagnosed with GD from the Department of Psychiatry at our University Hospital, consecutively recruited between September 2017 and April 2018, was included in the study. Only patients who sought treatment for GD as a primary mental health concern and who met DSM-5 GD criteria45 were included. Patients were voluntarily referred to our GD Unit through general practitioners or via other healthcare professionals.
Regarding sociodemographic features, data suggest a negative correlation between impulsivity and chronological age46,47, and higher impulsivity levels in males48. For this reason, in the present study, male participants aged between 18 and 50 years were included (which define the range of young- to middle-age adulthood). Into the research area of gambling related problems, the definition of age thresholds for elderly substantively vary, being the lower bound usually between age 50+ to 70+ years across researches49. In fact, most studies outline that it is precisely the transition from middle-age adulthood to older age (around 50 years-old) the critical phase with relevant adjustments and changes which can significantly affect the gambling habits, being the most relevant risk factors for GD some socio-demographical variations (e.g. employment retirement, financial disadvantages or social isolation)50,51, the age-related neurological vulnerabilities in the mechanisms related with behavioral regulation and diminished executive functioning typical of elderly52–54, and the physical and psychological unhealthy typical of the senior age (such as chronic medical conditions, limited mobility, anxiety or depression)55.
From this sample, 96 cases were excluded because they did not meet the inclusion criteria for this study: they were over 50 (n = 42), suffered from a comorbid mental disorder (i.e. schizophrenia or other psychotic disorders) (n = 17), did not meet DSM-5 criteria for GD (n = 5), were female (n = 22), or could not participate for practical reasons (n = 10). The final sample was made up of 97 treatment-seeking adult men. No comorbid conditions characterized by high levels of impulsivity were found in the study sample, except for substance use disorder (reported by n = 8 GD patients).
Experienced psychologists and psychiatrists conducted face-to-face clinical interviews to assess clinical and demographic variables, such as education level, origin or civil status. Patients were diagnosed with GD according to DSM-5 criteria45.
Participants, before initiating outpatient treatment, individually completed all the questionnaires utilized in this study. Neuropsychological measures were completed under the supervision of a staff psychologist on the same day as the rest of the assessment.
Our study sample also incorporated 32 HC participants recruited using word of mouth. The exclusion criteria for the HC group included a lifetime history of GD, being female (to avoid introducing bias in the study design) or not being within the established age range (between 18 and 50 years, inclusive). The comparison group was recruited from the surrounding community. The evaluation protocol was identical to that of the clinical group in that the participants were all evaluated on the same day.
Measures
GD severity
DSM-5 Criteria45. Patients were diagnosed with gambling disorder if they met DSM-5 criteria45, which consist of nine different criteria and the presence of the disorder is set at a cut-off point of 4 or more.
South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS). This self-report 20-item screening questionnaire discriminates between probable pathological, problem and non-problem gamblers56. The Spanish validation used in this work showed excellent internal consistency (α = 0.94) and test–retest reliability (r = 0.98)57.
Response impulsivity
Conners’ Continuous Performance Test, 2nd edition (CPT-II). The CPT-II is a computer-based task that involves participants pressing the space bar in response to visual stimuli (i.e., letters on a computer screen) that are presented over a span of 14 min58. The CPT-II provides information about the participants’ omission and commission error rates, reaction time, and response variability, which represent an assessment of sustained attention and inhibitory control. Higher scores on the CPT-II indicate worse performance.
Choice impulsivity
Delay discounting task. This 27-item self-administered tool was used to measure individual inter-temporal discount rates (k), providing a set of alternative choices between a smaller, immediate monetary reward and a larger, delayed monetary reward59. Each question was designed to correspond to a different k value, which constitutes the measure of discounting rate and represents the amount of discounting of the later reward that renders it equal to the smaller reward. Respondents’ answers are placed on reference discounting curves, where placement amid steeper curves indicates higher levels of choice impulsivity. Single k parameter-estimates can be obtained not only for an overall rate of discounting, but also for items with small, medium and large monetary rewards59. K values can range from 0 (selection of the delayed reward option for all items, or no discounting) to 0.25 (selection of the immediate reward option for all items, or always discounting). According to many studies using the delay discounting task (also termed the Monetary Choice Questionnaire)22,60, the distributions of k values were normalized using square root transformation.
Impulsive tendencies
Impulsive Behavior Scale (UPPS-P). The UPPS-P measures five facets of impulsive behavior through self-report on 59 items: negative urgency; positive urgency; lack of premeditation; lack of perseverance; and sensation-seeking32. The Spanish-language adaptation of the UPPS-P showed good reliability (Cronbach’s α between 0.79 and 0.93) and external validity61.
Ethics
The present study was carried out in accordance with the latest version of the Declaration of Helsinki. The University Hospital Clinical Research Ethics Committee approved the study, and signed informed consent was obtained from all participants.
Statistical analysis
Statistical analyses were conducted with Stata15 for Windows. The comparison between the impulsivity measures between the groups (HC versus GD) was based on analysis of variance adjusted for the participants’ ages, education levels and presence of substances use (ANCOVA). Associations between variables (impulsivity measures and GD severity measures) were estimated through partial correlation coefficients, also adjusted for age, and education and substance use. In addition to the correlational analysis, multiple regression models were used to obtain a predictive model of the GD severity (defined as the dependent variable) based on impulsivity measures. These models were generated in two step/blocks: (a) first block entered and set the covariates age and education level; (b) the second block automatically selected the significant contributors to GD severity from the impulsivity measures through a stepwise procedure. The incremental predictive capacity of impulsivity on the criteria was valued with change/increase in the R2 coefficient.
In this study, effect sizes for mean comparisons were obtained through Cohen’s d coefficient, considering 0.5 > |d| > 0.20 to be a small effect, 0.8 > |d| > 0.5 to be a moderate effect and |d| > 0.8 to be a large effect62. In addition, and due to the strong association between the sample size and significance tests for correlation estimates, 0.24 > |r| > 0.10 was considered to be small, 0.37 > |r| > 0.24 to be medium and |r| > 0.37 to be large (these thresholds corresponds to Cohen’s d values of 0.20, 0.50 and 0.80 respectively63.
Finally, increases in the type-I error due to multiple comparisons was controlled using the Finner method, a procedure included in family-wise-error-rate stepwise systems, which has been reported to be more appropriate than Bonferroni correction64.
Results
Sample description
The mean age for the HC group was 31.3 years old (SD = 6.6). Most participants had completed secondary school (53.1%), 37.5% had a university education and 9.4% a primary school level of education. Most were born in Spain (87.5%) and were employed (71.9%).
The mean age for the GD group was 35.0 years (SD = 8.8). Most had a primary school level of education (55.7% versus 40.2% secondary school and 4.1% university). Most were born in Spain (87.6%) and were employed (72.2%).
Significant differences were found between groups in terms of education level (χ2 = 34.2, df = 2, p < 0.001) and age (T = 2.2, df = 128, p < 0.030). Thus, we controlled for these factors in subsequent between-group analyses. No differences between the groups were found for marital status (χ2 = 4.58, df = 2, p = 0.106), immigration status (χ2 = 0.01, df = 1, p = 0.985) and employment status (χ2 = 0.02, df = 1, p = 0.975).
Comparisons between the groups: ANCOVA
ANCOVAs confirmed that the GD group demonstrated greater GD severity than the HC group (Table 1). The GD group also demonstrated more commission errors on the CPT, demonstrated steeper discounting rates, and scored higher on all UPPS-P subscales (Table 1).
Table 1.
α | Control n = 32 | Gambling Disorder n = 97 | F | df | p | |d| | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | ||||||
Gambling severity | |||||||||
DSM-5 total criteria | 0.934 | 0.06 | 0.25 | 7.24 | 1.66 | 591.45 | 1/127 | <0.001* | 6.05† |
SOGS total score | 0.822 | 0.22 | 0.42 | 11.44 | 2.85 | 490.21 | 1/127 | <0.001* | 5.51† |
1Response impulsivity, CPT | |||||||||
Omissions | 2.09 | 1.76 | 2.29 | 5.73 | 0.03 | 1/125 | 0.868 | 0.05 | |
Commissions | 12.78 | 7.29 | 21.78 | 16.78 | 6.43 | 1/125 | 0.012* | 0.70† | |
Hit Reaction Time | 388.72 | 38.71 | 392.59 | 331.46 | 0.01 | 1/125 | 0.955 | 0.02 | |
Perseveration | −0.03 | 0.74 | 1.18 | 4.07 | 2.06 | 1/125 | 0.154 | 0.41 | |
1Delay discounting | |||||||||
K, overall square root | 0.1360 | 0.0782 | 0.1997 | 0.1553 | 3.91 | 1/125 | 0.047* | 0.52† | |
1Impulsivity tendency, UPPS-P | |||||||||
Lack of premeditation | 0.872 | 20.33 | 4.80 | 24.69 | 6.59 | 8.64 | 1/125 | 0.004* | 0.76† |
Lack of perseverance | 0.783 | 17.68 | 4.37 | 22.69 | 4.99 | 18.55 | 1/125 | <0.001* | 1.07† |
Sensation-seeking | 0.864 | 25.39 | 8.20 | 30.04 | 8.47 | 5.56 | 1/125 | 0.043* | 0.56† |
Positive urgency | 0.942 | 20.06 | 5.92 | 32.24 | 10.47 | 28.10 | 1/125 | <0.001* | 1.43† |
Negative urgency | 0.909 | 20.77 | 5.72 | 32.89 | 7.76 | 47.75 | 1/125 | <0.001* | 1.78† |
Note. SQRT: Square root. SD: standard deviation. α: Cronbach’s alpha in the sample.
1Results adjusted for age and education levels.
*Bold: significant comparison (0.05 level). †Bold: effect size in the moderate (|d| > 0.50) to high range (|d| > 0.80).
p-values include Finner’s correction for multiple comparisons.
Associations between impulsivity measures
The upper part of Table 2 contains the correlation matrix (partial correlations adjusted for age, education level and substance use) measuring associations between the impulsivity measures in the GD group. Positive coefficients in the moderate to high range were obtained between choice impulsivity and CPT-related omissions and perseveration and between the UPPS-P positive urgency and negative urgency measures. Positive associations were also obtained between most UPPS-P subscales.
Table 2.
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | CPT omissions | — | 0.03 | −0.04 | 0.47† | 0.28† | 0.07 | 0.06 | 0.09 | 0.22 | 0.12 |
2 | CPT commissions | 0.37† | — | −0.07 | 0.03 | 0.01 | 0.04 | 0.04 | 0.01 | 0.09 | 0.02 |
3 | CPT hit reaction time | −0.07 | −0.61† | — | −0.02 | 0.14 | 0.07 | 0.06 | −0.09 | −0.03 | −0.03 |
4 | CPT perseveration | 0.36† | 0.45† | −0.11 | — | 0.24† | −0.05 | 0.01 | 0.02 | 0.11 | 0.06 |
5 | k-Overall square root | 0.10 | −0.15 | 0.27† | −0.07 | — | 0.20 | 0.13 | 0.08 | 0.37† | 0.27† |
6 | UPPS-P Premeditation | 0.14 | 0.09 | −0.06 | 0.17 | 0.33† | — | 0.47† | −0.08 | 0.20 | 0.24† |
7 | UPPS-P Perseverance | 0.13 | 0.30† | −0.40† | 0.18 | 0.04 | 0.45† | — | −0.19 | 0.15 | 0.25† |
8 | UPPS-P Sensation | −0.02 | −0.03 | 0.15 | 0.08 | 0.09 | 0.20 | −0.11 | — | 0.36† | 0.32† |
9 | UPPS-P Positive Urge | 0.16 | 0.23 | −0.13 | 0.40 | 0.43† | 0.35† | 0.17 | 0.06 | — | 0.76† |
10 | UPPS-P Negative Urge | −0.05 | 0.09 | −0.34† | 0.03 | 0.26† | 0.37† | 0.24† | −0.08 | 0.64† | — |
Note.†Bold: effect size in the moderate (|r| > 0.24) to high range (|r| > 0.37).
Upper part of the table: correlations estimated in the GD group (n = 97).
Lower part of the table, italic font: correlations estimated in the HC group (n = 32).
The lower part of Table 2 contains the correlation matrix for the HC group. In this subsample, choice impulsivity positively correlated with the CPT hit-reaction-time, and with the UPPS-P premeditation, positive urgency and negative urgency measures. Other correlations emerged: (a) CPT commissions positively correlated with UPPS-P perseverance; (b) CPT hit-reaction time negatively correlated with UPPS-P perseverance and negative urgency measures; (c) CPT perseverance positively correlated with UPPS-P positive urgency; and, (d) CPT measures correlated with one another (except for perseveration and hit-reaction-time), as well as several UPPS-P measures (Table 3). Choice impulsivity measures correlated with UPPS-P measures of premeditation and positive and negative urgency. Positive associations were also obtained between many UPPS-P subscales, and between CPT omissions and commissions measures.
Table 3.
GD group (n = 97) | HC group (n = 32) | |||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Total score | 1DSM-5 criteria | Total score | ||||||||||
DSM-5 | SOGS | C1 | C2 | C3 | C4 | C5 | C6 | C7 | C8 | DSM-5 | SOGS | |
Response impulsivity, CPT | ||||||||||||
Omissions | 0.11 | 0.16 | 0.18 | 0.07 | −0.06 | 0.10 | 0.03 | 0.02 | 0.10 | −0.10 | −0.05 | 0.13 |
Commissions | 0.11 | 0.09 | 0.10 | 0.08 | 0.00 | 0.03 | 0.24† | 0.02 | −0.07 | 0.04 | 0.40† | 0.18 |
Hit Reaction Time | 0.10 | −0.07 | 0.07 | 0.05 | 0.03 | 0.06 | 0.05 | 0.04 | 0.04 | 0.04 | −0.35† | −0.09 |
Perseveration | 0.01 | 0.08 | 0.15 | 0.08 | 0.02 | −0.06 | −0.16 | 0.07 | 0.05 | −0.25 | −0.12 | −0.21 |
Choice impulsivity, DELAY | ||||||||||||
SQRT_K-Overall | 0.19 | 0.35† | 0.24† | 0.12 | −0.04 | 0.30† | 0.03 | −0.05 | 0.12 | −0.06 | −0.05 | 0.44† |
Impulsivity tendency, UPPS-P | ||||||||||||
Lack of premeditation | 0.27† | 0.21 | 0.10 | 0.10 | 0.14 | 0.22 | 0.19 | −0.08 | 0.12 | 0.18 | −0.06 | −0.02 |
Lack of perseverance | 0.36† | 0.34† | 0.30† | 0.40† | 0.24† | 0.12 | 0.18 | −0.08 | 0.14 | 0.07 | 0.34† | 0.04 |
Sensation seeking | 0.21 | 0.16 | 0.25† | 0.14 | 0.08 | 0.04 | 0.20 | 0.10 | −0.13 | 0.05 | −0.20 | 0.01 |
Positive urgency | 0.17 | 0.22 | 0.30† | 0.32† | 0.19 | 0.28† | 0.23 | −0.04 | 0.12 | −0.02 | 0.05 | 0.24† |
Negative urgency | 0.16 | 0.05 | 0.15 | 0.26† | 0.25† | 0.24† | 0.26† | −0.08 | 0.01 | −0.06 | 0.15 | 0.16 |
Note. SQRT: Square root. †Bold: effect size in the moderate (|r| > 0.24) to high range (|r| > 0.37).
1DSM-5 criteria for GD: C1 = gamble with increasing amounts of money, C2 = irritability, C3 = unsuccessful efforts to control, C4 = preoccupations, C5 = gamble as a way of escaping, C6 = after losing returns other days, C7 = lies related to gambling activity, C8 = social impairment. 1Point-biserial correlation estimates.
Associations between impulsivity and GD severity
The first block of Table 3 contains the partial correlations (adjusted for age, education levels and substance use) between impulsivity measures with GD severity (total DSM-5 criteria and SOGS total) and each DSM-5 criteria in the GD group (point-biserial correlations were obtained for examining the relationships between impulsivity measure with each DSM-5 criterion). Results show that the DSM-5 total criteria for GD positively correlated with UPPS-P lack of premeditation and lack of perseverance, while the SOGS total score was positively associated with delay discounting and UPSS-P lack of perseverance. Figure 1 contains the scatterplot between overall delay discounting scores (k) and SOGS scores. Regarding the association between the impulsivity measures with each DSM-5 criteria for GD, choice impulsivity positively correlated with criterion 1 “gamble with increasing amounts of money” and criterion 4 “preoccupied for gambling activity”, CPT commissions score was related to criterion 5 “gambling as a way of escaping” and impulsivity tendency measured with the UPPS-P scales tended to correlate with criterion 1 to 5.
In the HC group (second block of Table 3), DSM-5 total GD criteria positively correlated with CPT commissions and with the UPPS-P lack of perseverance; in this group, the SOGS total score positively correlated with measures of delay discounting and UPSS-P positive urgency. The matrix correlation was not obtained for impulsivity with each DSM-5 criteria in this group due the extremely low prevalence for the gambling symptoms in this subsample.
Table 4 contains the predictive regression models for GD severity based on the impulsivity measures and adjusted for age and education levels in the GD group. The first model was obtained for the number of DSM-5 criteria for GD, and adjusted for age and education level. The significant predictors of higher severity were lack of perseverance and positive urgency. The second model, obtained for the SOGS total score, retained as significant predictors the delay impulsivity, lack of perseverance, sensation seeking and positive urgency.
Table 4.
Criterion: DSM-5 total criteria | B | SE | Beta | T | p | 95%CI B | ΔR2 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
First step/block (covariates) | 0.009 | |||||||
Age (years-old) | 0.012 | 0.020 | 0.063 | 0.605 | 0.547 | −0.027 | 0.051 | |
Education level | −0.088 | 0.140 | −0.066 | −0.630 | 0.530 | −0.367 | 0.190 | |
Second step/block | 0.222 | |||||||
Age (years-old) | 0.019 | 0.017 | 0.103 | 1.101 | 0.274 | −0.015 | 0.054 | |
Education level | −0.158 | 0.126 | −0.118 | −1.255 | 0.213 | −0.409 | 0.092 | |
UPPS-P Lack of perseverance | 0.100 | 0.031 | 0.301 | 3.182 | 0.002 | 0.038 | 0.162 | |
UPPS-P Positive urgency | 0.052 | 0.015 | 0.325 | 3.492 | 0.001 | 0.022 | 0.081 | |
Criterion: SOGS total | B | SE | Beta | T | p | 95%CI B | ΔR2 | |
First step/block (covariates) | 0.025 | |||||||
Age (years-old) | 0.042 | 0.033 | 0.130 | 1.261 | 0.210 | −0.024 | 0.108 | |
Education level | −0.175 | 0.240 | −0.075 | −0.728 | 0.468 | −0.651 | 0.301 | |
Second step/block | 0.270 | |||||||
Age (years-old) | 0.063 | 0.031 | 0.196 | 2.070 | 0.041 | 0.003 | 0.124 | |
Education level | −0.298 | 0.215 | −0.129 | −1.386 | 0.169 | −0.726 | 0.130 | |
Delay: SQRT_K-Overall | 5.987 | 1.716 | 0.326 | 3.488 | 0.001 | 2.577 | 9.397 | |
UPPS-P Lack of perseverance | 0.229 | 0.056 | 0.399 | 4.105 | <0.001 | 0.118 | 0.340 | |
UPPS-P Sensation seeking | 0.095 | 0.035 | 0.279 | 2.728 | 0.008 | 0.026 | 0.163 | |
UPPS-P Positive urgency | −0.082 | 0.037 | −0.222 | −2.183 | 0.032 | −0.156 | −0.007 |
Note. SQRT: Square root. GD group (n = 97).
Discussion
The present study analyzed whether associations between response impulsivity, choice impulsivity and impulsive tendency varied between GD patients and HC. Moreover, the interrelationship among these three types of impulsivity, and their associations with GD severity in GD, were examined. Our hypotheses were largely supported, and the implications of the findings are discussed below. In the present study, 8.24% of the sample included also presented substance abuse. This comorbidity has been recorded as the most frequent in gamblers65. In contrast, other disorders related to gambling behavior and impulsivity, such as eating disorders66, or impulse control disorders67, were not found in the current sample.
Regarding response impulsivity, patients with GD reported greater commission errors, defined as incorrect responses towards non-target stimulus, in comparison with HC participants. This result dovetails with previous studies finding that patients with GD are more prone to commit execution errors when facing no-go stimuli9 and a recent meta-analysis identifying motor impairments in individuals with GD14. This leads us to postulate that response impulsivity is linked to a deficit in inhibitory control, which could partially explain difficulties in reducing or eliminating gambling behavior.
The findings of this study also showed that, in terms of choice impulsivity, patients with GD presented greater delay discounting than did HC participants. This finding is consistent with other studies highlighting that patients with GD differ from HC when making monetary decisions, showing a biased tendency to discount rewards more rapidly and to select smaller, sooner amounts of money18. These results may partly relate to why patients with GD may choose bets for more immediate gains, despite the negative consequences that such gambling may entail19.
Furthermore, higher levels of all assessed dimensions of impulsive tendencies were observed in the GD relative to the HC group. This result is partially in line with previous studies that found higher impulsive tendencies, although results have varied between groups in sensation-seeking tendencies37 and lack of perseverance38. These differences between groups could be explained due, to some extent, to the strong associations between these impulsive dimensions and essential GD clinical features, such as cognitive distortions37,68,69, gambling choices70,71, emotion-regulation impairment72, or GD-related illegal acts34. Sensation seeking as well as urgency, which appear to be related with GD severity in some studies, including ours36,38,73, are strongly associated with emotional factors74. It is well known that difficulties in recognizing and dealing with different emotions are risk factors for the onset and maintenance of GD75,76. With respect to the predictors of greater severity, positive urgency and lack of perseverance were found to be the most related when assessed with the DSM-5 criteria45. Nevertheless, regarding the SOGS56, the most notable dimensions were positive urgency, sensation seeking and lack of perseverance. Similarly, Savvidou et al. (2017)77 found that in a large sample of men and women significant severity predictors were negative urgency and sensation seeking. Furthermore, a recent study69 found that lack of perseverance predicted treatment dropout, and negative urgency was linked to relapses. Regarding the lack of a predictive relationship of the negative urgency and severity of the disorder, it could be hypothesized that negative urgency, that is, gambling when feeling negative emotions, is more associated with female gamblers78,79, and that, in general, it may be a factor more linked to the maintenance of the gambling behavior rather than to the severity of the disorder.
Another finding to emerge from the present study is the association between the three impulsivity domains. Considering the clinical group, our results identified an association between choice impulsivity and impulsivity tendencies, with urgency being the dimension which had the greatest association with delay discounting. This finding is consistent with earlier studies highlighting a significant correlation between these two impulsivity facets22,37. This is in contrast to the HC group, which presented a significant association between delay discounting and lack of premeditation. Our results also indicate a positive correlation between response impulsivity, as assessed on the CPT, and choice impulsivity. This finding seems to be partially consistent with other research which found weak or no relationships between most facets of response and choice impulsivity8,80. The finding that there are several domains of response and choice impulsivity is consistent with the multifactorial frameworks of impulsivity.
Finally, a significant association was found between GD severity and two of the impulsivity facets, impulsive tendencies and choice impulsivity, which is consistent with other findings24,40. Even though a recent meta-analysis identified associations between motor impulsivity and GD9, our study failed to identify a significant association between response inhibition and GD severity among GD patients. This finding suggests that choice impulsivity, impulsive tendencies and response impulsivity could be considered as three separable entities, although the former two in particular seem to be partly inter-related. However, an impaired ability to inhibit motor responses does seem to be associated with greater disorder symptomatology in GD.
From a clinical perspective, it could be postulated that the results of the present study may inform potential treatment targets in the future. Specific adjuvant interventions to address the facets of impulsivity associated with GD severity could potentially improve treatment outcomes4,5. In this sense, technologically based interventions represent a new frontier for treatment, from the computerized adaptation of neurocognitive tasks to evaluate these processes, such as cognitive and attentional bias81, to the use of mobile applications to condition the selection of healthy foods obesity82, or serious games for the treatment of impulsivity in gambling disorder83, in eating disorders84,85 and in other mental disorders86. It has been observed that the use of therapeutic video games, as an additional therapeutic tool can treat difficulties in emotional regulation and impulsivity84. Moreover, this modality of treatment increases the motivation of the patient and decreases dropout rates83.
Limitations and future research
The results of this study should be considered in light of its limitations. First, the sample was entirely male. Some studies carried out in healthy participants have found gender-related differences in impulsive tendencies87, choice impulsivity88 and response impulsivity89. Future studies would benefit from including women and comparing both groups from a three-factor impulsivity perspective8. Second, the number of patients with GD in the present study was higher than the number of HC participants and there was a lack of group matching on the demographic measures. Future studies should include larger and more balanced HC samples. Third, both delay discounting and impulsive tendencies were evaluated through self-report assessments, and self-report and behavioral measures of impulsivity (even within the same domain) may weakly correlate or be uncorrelated90. Further, the extent to which these self-report measures may relate to decision-making processes that may be sensitive to contextual factors and may involve irrational and spontaneous aspects requires additional investigation. Relatedly, although the differences found in the present study would be also related with differences in baseline general IQ91, the present study did not measure, report or control for IQ scores. Fourth, the results should be interpreted cautiously given that separate instruments (some self-report and some behavioral) were used to evaluate each type of impulsivity, and poor concordance between self-report and behavioral measures of impulsivity has been reported92. The same could be argued for the measures used to assess gambling severity. The DSM only provides a measure for the absence or presence of gambling symptoms and does not take factors such as frequency or breadth of gambling activities into account. Differing findings between the SOGS and the DSM with regards to impulsivity measures could be attributed to this fact. Fifth, previous research has suggested that, in the case of choice impulsivity, delay discounting levels in people with GD vary according to whether they are in a gambling context or not93. Relatedly, some individuals with GD may have a contextual control over discounting, choosing delayed rewards in order to avoid spending money immediately through gambling behavior94.
Future research should examine facets of impulsivity in different contexts and in relation to individual differences in gambling-related cognitions68, in order to obtain a more precise evaluation of how different aspects of impulsivity relate to gambling behaviors. Moreover, validated instruments were not used to screen psychiatric morbidities in the HC group. Longitudinal research is needed to understand changes in impulsivity over the course of addiction, particularly as changes in impulsivity may relate importantly to treatment outcomes11,95. Finally, this study was carried out with the aim to assess the specific contribution of impulsivity domains on GD, and therefore other comorbid conditions were excluded from the clinical and control subsamples. Other studies should address what is the impulsivity pattern in heterogeneous samples of patients with GD but also other comorbidities, particularly those clinical conditions that scientific literature has strongly related to impulsivity. Future research also should include comprehensive methods for accurately testing, screening and diagnosing the presence of these other comorbid mental problems, with the aim to know the specific contribution of impulsivity in each condition as well as the interactions between them.
Conclusions
Taken together, one of the more significant findings to emerge from this study is the confirmation that impulsivity is not a singular construct in the case of GD and these domains of impulsivity are intercorrelated. In the study at hand, we studied three different impulsivity domains: choice impulsivity, trait impulsivity and motor impulsivity and the first two appear to be interrelated. The current data also highlight the interrelationship between these impulsivity facets and GD severity, suggesting that motor response impulsivity is not directly associated with GD severity. Assessing multiple facets of impulsivity within an individual subject complements recent research highlighting the pooled effects of multiple studies identifying robust deficits in multiple cognitive domains in individuals with GD14 and offers a path forward for research examining candidate cognitive vulnerability features in gambling populations.
Acknowledgements
Financial support was received through the Ministerio de Economía y Competitividad (PSI2015- 68701-R and RTI2018-101837-B-100). FIS PI14/00290, FIS PI17/01167, and 18MSP001 - 2017I067 received aid from the Ministerio de Sanidad, Servicios Sociales e Igualdad. CIBER Fisiología Obesidad y Nutrición (CIBERobn) and CIBER Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), both of which are initiatives of ISCIII. MLM, TMM and CVA are supported each one by a predoctoral Grant of the Ministerio de Educación, Cultura y Deporte (FPU15/0291; FPU16/02087; FPU16/01453). Dr. Potenza’s involvement was supported by a National Center for Responsible Gaming Center of Excellence grant and by the Connecticut Council on Problem Gambling and the Connecticut Department of Mental Health and Addiction Services.
Author contributions
G.M.B., T.S., F.F.A., J.M.M., M.N.P. and S.J.M. designed the study and were involved in developing the research aims. G.M.B., T.S., F.F.A., M.N.P. and S.J.M. aided in the literature search and the framing of the Introduction and Discussion section. R.G. conducted the statistical analysis and interpretation of the results. G.M.B., T.M.M., C.V.A. and M.L.M. contributed to the data collection. G.M.B., T.S., R.G., F.F.A., M.N.P. and S.J.M. were involved in writing, proofreading and approving the final manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript.
Competing interests
None of the authors have any conflicts of interest. Marc Potenza has consulted for Shire, INSYS, Rivermend Health, Opiant/Lakelight Therapeutics, and Jazz Pharmaceuticals; has received research support (to Yale) from Mohegan Sun Casino and the National Center for Responsible Gaming; has participated in surveys, mailings or telephone consultations related to drug addiction, impulse-control disorders or other health topics; has consulted for and/or advised gambling and legal entities on issues related to impulse-control/addictive disorders; has provided clinical care in a problem gambling services program; has performed grant reviews for research-funding agencies; has edited journals and journal sections; has given academic lectures in grand rounds, CME events and other clinical or scientific venues; and has generated books or book chapters for publishers of mental health texts.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Marc N. Potenza, Email: marc.potenza@yale.edu
Susana Jiménez-Murcia, Email: sjimenez@bellvitgehospital.cat.
References
- 1.Mitchell MR, Potenza MN. Recent Insights into the Neurobiology of Impulsivity. Curr. Addict. Reports. 2014;1:309–319. doi: 10.1007/s40429-014-0037-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Hodgins DC, Holub A. Components of Impulsivity in Gambling Disorder. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 2015;13:699–711. doi: 10.1007/s11469-015-9572-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Moeller FG, Barratt ES, Dougherty DM, Schmitz JM, Swann AC. Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2001;158:1783–1793. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Hamilton KR, et al. Rapid-response impulsivity: Definitions, measurement issues, and clinical implications. Personal. Disord. Theory, Res. Treat. 2015;6:168–181. doi: 10.1037/per0000100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hamilton KR, et al. Choice impulsivity: Definitions, measurement issues, and clinical implications. Personal. Disord. Theory, Res. Treat. 2015;6:182–198. doi: 10.1037/per0000099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Fineberg NA, et al. Probing compulsive and impulsive behaviors, from animal models to endophenotypes: A narrative review. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:591–604. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Fineberg NA, et al. New developments in human neurocognition: Clinical, genetic, and brain imaging correlates of impulsivity and compulsivity. CNS Spectrums. 2014;19:69–89. doi: 10.1017/S1092852913000801. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.MacKillop J, et al. The latent structure of impulsivity: impulsive choice, impulsive action, and impulsive personality traits. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2016;233:3361–3370. doi: 10.1007/s00213-016-4372-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Chowdhury NS, Livesey EJ, Blaszczynski A, Harris JA. Pathological Gambling and Motor Impulsivity: A Systematic Review with Meta-Analysis. Journal of Gambling Studies. 2017;33:1213–1239. doi: 10.1007/s10899-017-9683-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kertzman S, et al. Go-no-go performance in pathological gamblers. Psychiatry Res. 2008;161:1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.06.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Grant JE, Chamberlain SR, Odlaug BL, Potenza MN, Kim SW. Memantine shows promise in reducing gambling severity and cognitive inflexibility in pathological gambling: A pilot study. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2010;212:603–612. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-1994-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Odlaug BL, Chamberlain SR, Kim SW, Schreiber LRN, Grant JE. A neurocognitive comparison of cognitive flexibility and response inhibition in gamblers with varying degrees of clinical severity. Psychol. Med. 2011;41:2111–2119. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711000316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Brevers Damien, Cleeremans Axel, Verbruggen Frederick, Bechara Antoine, Kornreich Charles, Verbanck Paul, Noël Xavier. Impulsive Action but Not Impulsive Choice Determines Problem Gambling Severity. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(11):e50647. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0050647. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Ioannidis K, Hook R, Wickham K, Grant JE, Chamberlain SR. Impulsivity in Gambling Disorder and problem gambling: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2019;44:1354–1361. doi: 10.1038/s41386-019-0393-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Madden GJ, Francisco MT, Brewer AT, Stein JS. Delay discounting and gambling. Behav. Processes. 2011;87:43–49. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2011.01.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Potenza MN. The neural bases of cognitive processes in gambling disorder. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2014;18:429–438. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Yau YHC, Potenza MN. Gambling disorder and other behavioral addictions: Recognition and treatment. Harvard Review of Psychiatry. 2015;23:134–146. doi: 10.1097/HRP.0000000000000051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Petry NM. Pathological gamblers, with and without substance use disorders, discount delayed rewards at high rates. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2001;110:482–487. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.110.3.482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Grecucci Alessandro, Giorgetta Cinzia, Rattin Andrea, Guerreschi Cesare, Sanfey Alan G., Bonini Nicolao. Time Devours Things: How Impulsivity and Time Affect Temporal Decisions in Pathological Gamblers. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(10):e109197. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0109197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Mallorquí-Bagué Nuria, Fagundo Ana B., Jimenez-Murcia Susana, de la Torre Rafael, Baños Rosa M., Botella Cristina, Casanueva Felipe F., Crujeiras Ana B., Fernández-García Jose C., Fernández-Real Jose M., Frühbeck Gema, Granero Roser, Rodríguez Amaia, Tolosa-Sola Iris, Ortega Francisco J., Tinahones Francisco J., Alvarez-Moya Eva, Ochoa Cristian, Menchón Jose M., Fernández-Aranda Fernando. Decision Making Impairment: A Shared Vulnerability in Obesity, Gambling Disorder and Substance Use Disorders? PLOS ONE. 2016;11(9):e0163901. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0163901. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Ochoa C, et al. Decision-making deficits in pathological gambling: The role of executive functions, explicit knowledge and impulsivity in relation to decisions made under ambiguity and risk. Am. J. Addict. 2013;22:492–499. doi: 10.1111/j.1521-0391.2013.12061.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Steward T, et al. Delay discounting and impulsivity traits in young and older gambling disorder patients. Addict. Behav. 2017;71:96–103. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Alessi SM, Petry NM. Pathological gambling severity is associated with impulsivity in a delay discounting procedure. Behav. Processes. 2003;64:345–354. doi: 10.1016/S0376-6357(03)00150-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Stea JN, Hodgins DC, Lambert MJ. Relations between delay discounting and low to moderate gambling, cannabis, and alcohol problems among university students. Behav. Processes. 2011;88:202–205. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2011.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.MacKillop J, Anderson EJ, Castelda BA, Mattson RE, Donovick PJ. Divergent validity of measures of cognitive distortions, impulsivity, and time perspective in pathological gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies. 2006;22:339–354. doi: 10.1007/s10899-006-9021-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Balodis IM, et al. Relating neural processing of reward and loss prospect to risky decision-making in individuals with and without gambling disorder. Int. Gambl. Stud. 2018;18:269–285. doi: 10.1080/14459795.2018.1469658. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Yip SW, et al. Gray-matter relationships to diagnostic and transdiagnostic features of drug and behavioral addictions. Addict. Biol. 2018;23:394–402. doi: 10.1111/adb.12492. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the barratt impulsiveness scale. J. Clin. Psychol. 1995;51:768–774. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::AID-JCLP2270510607>3.0.CO;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Reise SP, Moore TM, Sabb FW, Brown AK, London ED. The barratt impulsiveness scale-11: Reassessment of its structure in a community sample. Psychol. Assess. 2013;25:631–642. doi: 10.1037/a0032161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Reid RC, Cyders MA, Moghaddam JF, Fong TW. Psychometric properties of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale in patients with gambling disorders, hypersexuality, and methamphetamine dependence. Addict. Behav. 2014;39:1640–1645. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Berg JM, Latzman RD, Bliwise NG, Lilienfeld SO. Parsing the heterogeneity of impulsivity: A meta-analytic review of the behavioral implications of the UPPS for psychopathology. Psychol. Assess. 2015;27:1129–1146. doi: 10.1037/pas0000111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Whiteside SP, Lynam DR, Miller JD, Reynolds SK. Validation of the UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale: A four factor model of impulsivity. ProQuest Diss. Theses. 2001;574:90–90. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Canale N, Vieno A, Bowden-Jones H, Billieux J. The benefits of using the UPPS model of impulsivity rather than the Big Five when assessing the relationship between personality and problem gambling. Addiction. 2017;112:372–373. doi: 10.1111/add.13641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Mestre-Bach G, et al. Gambling and Impulsivity Traits: A Recipe for Criminal Behavior? Front. Psychiatry. 2018;9:6. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Aragay N, et al. Impulsivity profiles in pathological slot machine gamblers. Compr. Psychiatry. 2018;83:79–83. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2018.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Savvidou LG, et al. Is gambling disorder associated with impulsivity traits measured by the UPPS-P and is this association moderated by sex and age? Compr. Psychiatry. 2017;72:106–113. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Michalczuk R, Bowden-Jones H, Verdejo-Garcia A, Clark L. Impulsivity and cognitive distortions in pathological gamblers attending the UK National Problem Gambling Clinic: A preliminary report. Psychol. Med. 2011;41:2625–2635. doi: 10.1017/S003329171100095X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Billieux J, et al. Investigation of impulsivity in a sample of treatment-seeking pathological gamblers: A multidimensional perspective. Psychiatry Res. 2012;198:291–296. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Navas JF, Verdejo-García A, López-Gómez M, Maldonado A, Perales JC. Gambling with Rose-Tinted Glasses on: Use of Emotion-Regulation Strategies Correlates with Dysfunctional Cognitions in Gambling Disorder Patients. J. Behav. Addict. 2016;5:271–281. doi: 10.1556/2006.5.2016.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Mestre-Bach, G. et al. The predictive capacity of DSM-5 symptom severity and impulsivity on response to cognitive-behavioral therapy for gambling disorder: a 2-year longitudinal study. Eur. Psychiatry (2018). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 41.Grall-Bronnec M, et al. Clinical Profiles as a Function of Level and Type of Impulsivity in a Sample Group of At-risk and Pathological Gamblers Seeking Treatment. J. Gambl. Stud. 2012;28:239–252. doi: 10.1007/s10899-011-9258-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Lai FD, Ip AK, Lee TM. Impulsivity and pathological gambling: Is it a state or a trait problem? BMC Res. Notes. 2011;4:492. doi: 10.1186/1756-0500-4-492. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Kräplin A, et al. Dysfunctional decision-making in pathological gambling: Pattern specificity and the role of impulsivity. Psychiatry Res. 2014;215:675–682. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.12.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Jara-Rizzo MF, et al. Impulsivity and problem awareness predict therapy compliance and dropout from treatment for gambling disorder. Adicciones. 2019;31:147–159. doi: 10.20882/adicciones.1041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition. (2013).
- 46.Galvan, A., Hare, T., Voss, H., Glover, G. & Casey, B. J. Risk-taking and the adolescent brain: Who is at risk? Developmental Science10 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 47.Steinberg L, et al. Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity as indexed by behavior and self-report: evidence for a dual systems model. Dev. Psychol. 2008;44:1764–78. doi: 10.1037/a0012955. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Cyders MA. Impulsivity and the Sexes: Measurement and Structural Invariance of the UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior Scale. Assessment. 2013;20:86–97. doi: 10.1177/1073191111428762. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Tse S, Hong S-I, Wang C-W, Cunningham-Williams RM. Gambling behavior and problems among older adults: a systematic review of empirical studies. J. Gerontol. B. Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2012;67:639–52. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbs068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Tira C, Jackson AC, Tomnay JE. Pathways to late-life problematic gambling in seniors: a grounded theory approach. Gerontologist. 2014;54:1035–48. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnt107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Tirachaimongkol LC, Jackson AC, Tomnay JE. Pathways to Problem Gambling in Seniors. J. Gerontol. Soc. Work. 2010;53:531–546. doi: 10.1080/01634372.2010.487817. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Lorenz RC, et al. Reward anticipation in the adolescent and aging brain. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2014;35:5153–5165. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22540. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Subramaniam M, et al. Prevalence and determinants of gambling disorder among older adults: A systematic review. Addict. Behav. 2015;41:199–209. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.West R, Tiernan BN, Kieffaber PD, Bailey K, Anderson S. The effects of age on the neural correlates of feedback processing in a naturalistic gambling game. Psychophysiology. 2014;51:734–45. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Parke A, Griffiths M, Pattinson J, Keatley D. Age-related physical and psychological vulnerability as pathways to problem gambling in older adults. J. Behav. Addict. 2018;7:137–145. doi: 10.1556/2006.7.2018.18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Lesieur HR, Blume SB. The South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS): a new instrument for the identification of pathological gamblers. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1987;144:1184–8. doi: 10.1176/ajp.144.9.1184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Echeburúa E, Báez C, Fernández J, Páez D. Cuestionario de juego patológico de South Oaks (SOGS): Validación española (South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS): Spanish validation) Análisis Modif. Conduct. 1994;20:769–91. [Google Scholar]
- 58.Conners, C. Conners’ continuous performance test (CPT II) version 5 for windows: Technical guide and software manual. Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health Systems. (2004).
- 59.Kirby KN, Petry NM, Bickel WK. Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 1999;128:78–87. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.128.1.78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Steward T, et al. Delay Discounting of Reward and Impulsivity in Eating Disorders: From Anorexia Nervosa to Binge Eating Disorder. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 2017;25:601–606. doi: 10.1002/erv.2543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Verdejo-García A, Lozano Ó, Moya M, Alcázar MÁ, Pérez-García M. Psychometric properties of a Spanish version of the UPPS–P impulsive behavior scale: Reliability, validity and association with trait and cognitive impulsivity. J. Pers. Assess. 2010;92:70–77. doi: 10.1080/00223890903382369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Kelley K, Preacher KJ. On effect size. Psychol. Methods. 2012;17:137–152. doi: 10.1037/a0028086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Rosnow RL, Rosenthal R. Computing contrasts, effect sizes, and counternulls on other people’s published data: General procedures for research consumers. Psychol. Methods. 1996;1:331–340. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.1.4.331. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Finner H. On a monotonicity problem in step-down multiple test procedures. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 1993;88:920–923. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1993.10476358. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Rash CJ, Weinstock J, Van Patten R. A review of gambling disorder and substance use disorders. Subst. Abuse Rehabil. 2016;7:3–13. doi: 10.2147/SAR.S83460. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Kim HS, von Ranson KM, Hodgins DC, McGrath DS, Tavares H. Demographic, psychiatric, and personality correlates of adults seeking treatment for disordered gambling with a comorbid binge/purge type eating disorder. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 2018;26:508–518. doi: 10.1002/erv.2606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Potenza, M. N., Potenza, M. N. & Hollander, E. Pathological gambling and impulse control disorders. Neuropsychopharmacol. 5TH Gener.
- 68.Del Prete F, et al. The role of affect-driven impulsivity in gambling cognitions: A convenience-sample study with a Spanish version of the Gambling-Related Cognitions Scale. J. Behav. Addict. 2017;6:51–63. doi: 10.1556/2006.6.2017.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Mallorquí-Bagué N, et al. Impulsivity and cognitive distortions in different clinical phenotypes of gambling disorder: Profiles and longitudinal prediction of treatment outcomes. Eur. Psychiatry. 2019;61:9–16. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2019.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Lutri Vittorio, Soldini Emiliano, Ronzitti Silvia, Smith Neil, Clerici Massimo, Blaszczynski Alex, Bowden-Jones Henrietta. Impulsivity and Gambling Type Among Treatment-Seeking Disordered Gamblers: An Explorative Study. Journal of Gambling Studies. 2018;34(4):1341–1354. doi: 10.1007/s10899-018-9764-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Moragas, L. et al. Comparative analysis of distinct phenotypes in gambling disorder based on gambling preferences. BMC Psychiatry15 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 72.Navas JF, et al. Trait and neurobiological underpinnings of negative emotion regulation in gambling disorder. Addiction. 2017;112:1086–1094. doi: 10.1111/add.13751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Nower L, Derevensky JL, Gupta R. The relationship of impulsivity, sensation seeking, coping, and substance use in youth gamblers. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 2004;18:49–55. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.18.1.49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Joseph JE, Liu X, Jiang Y, Lynam D, Kelly TH. Neural Correlates of Emotional Reactivity in Sensation Seeking. Psychol. Sci. 2009;20:215–223. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02283.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Williams AD, Grisham JR, Erskine A, Cassedy E. Deficits in emotion regulation associated with pathological gambling. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 2012;51:223–238. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.2011.02022.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Tárrega S, et al. A Serious Videogame as an Additional Therapy Tool for Training Emotional Regulation and Impulsivity Control in Severe Gambling Disorder. Front. Psychol. 2015;6:1721. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01721. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Savvidou LG, et al. ScienceDirect Is gambling disorder associated with impulsivity traits measured by the UPPS-P and is this association moderated by sex and age? Compr. Psychiatry. 2017;72:106–113. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Martins SS, Lobo DSS, Tavares H, Gentil V. Pathological gambling in women: a review. Revista do Hospital das Clínicas. 2002;57:235–242. doi: 10.1590/S0041-87812002000500008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Ledgerwood DM, Petry NM. Psychological experience of gambling and subtypes of pathological gamblers. Psychiatry Res. 2006;144:17–27. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.08.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Stahl C, et al. Behavioral components of impulsivity. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2014;143:850–886. doi: 10.1037/a0033981. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Boffo, M., Willemen, R., Pronk, T., Wiers, R. W. & Dom, G. Effectiveness of two web-based cognitive bias modification interventions targeting approach and attentional bias in gambling problems: Study protocol for a pilot randomised controlled trial. Trials18 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 82.Kakoschke N, Hawker C, Castine B, de Courten B, Verdejo-Garcia A. Smartphone-based cognitive bias modification training improves healthy food choice in obesity: A pilot study. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 2018;26:526–532. doi: 10.1002/erv.2622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Jiménez-Murcia S, et al. Playmancer project: A serious videogame as an additional therapy tool for eating and impulse control disorders. Annual Review of CyberTherapy and Telemedicine. 2009;7:163–166. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Fernández-Aranda F, et al. Video games as a complementary therapy tool in mental disorders: PlayMancer, a European multicentre study. J. Ment. Heal. 2012;21:364–374. doi: 10.3109/09638237.2012.664302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Giner-Bartolomé, C. et al. Can an intervention based on a serious videogame prior to cognitive behavioral therapy be helpful in bulimia nervosa? A clinical case study. Front. Psychol. 6 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 86.Santamaria JJ, et al. Serious games as additional psychological support: A review of the literature. J. CyberTherapy Rehabil. 2011;4:469–476. [Google Scholar]
- 87.Caravaggio F, et al. Trait impulsiveness is related to smaller post-commissural putamen volumes in males but not females. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2017;46:2253–2264. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Kirby KN, Maraković NN. Delay-discounting probabilistic rewards: Rates decrease as amounts increase. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1996;3:100–104. doi: 10.3758/BF03210748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Lage GM, Albuquerque MR, Fuentes D, Corrêa H, Malloy-Diniz LF. Sex Differences in Dimensions of Impulsivity in a Non-Clinical Sample. Percept. Mot. Skills. 2013;117:601–607. doi: 10.2466/15.19.PMS.117x18z2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Krishnan-Sarin S, et al. Behavioral impulsivity predicts treatment outcome in a smoking cessation program for adolescent smokers. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007;88:79–82. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.09.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Forbush KT, et al. Neuropsychological characteristics and personality traits in pathological gambling. CNS Spectr. 2008;13:306–315. doi: 10.1017/S1092852900016424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Ellingson JM, Potenza MN, Pearlson GD. Methodological factors as a potential source of discordance between self-report and behavioral measures of impulsivity and related constructs. Addict. Behav. 2018;84:126–130. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.04.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Dixon MR, Jacobs EA, Sanders S. Contextual Control of Delay Discounting by Pathological Gamblers. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 2006;39:413–422. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2006.173-05. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Dixon MR, Marley J, Jacobs EA. Delay discounting by pathological gamblers. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 2003;36:449–458. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2003.36-449. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Blanco C, et al. A pilot study of impulsivity and compulsivity in pathological gambling. Psychiatry Res. 2009;167:161–168. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.04.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]