Abstract
Collective decision-making is important for coordination and synchronization of the activities among group-living animals and the mechanisms guiding such procedure involve a great variety of characteristics of behavior and motivation. This study provides some evidence investigating collective movement initiation in a multi-level social band of the golden snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxellana) located in the Mts. Qinling, China. We collect 1223 datum records relevant to decision initiation from six OMUs. The results indicate that collective movement initiation could be divided into two continual but relatively independent processes: decisions on moving direction and movement implementation. In both processes, adult individuals are more likely to initiate the decision-making, while other adults vote on initiator’s preference, with a threshold, a supporting number required for a success. Thus, voting behavior and quorum fulfillment contribute to a successful decision-making. Adult individuals play important role in making decisions for moving direction and implementation. For a successful collective movement initiation, the individuals being more central in grooming network initiate decisions more frequently than the others, and attract voters more easily. Furthermore, following the initiation, at least four positive voters are required for a direction decision and at least three positive voters are needed for the decision on movement implementation, which could be considered as the threshold of quorum numbers required for a successful decision. This study has provided some very interesting information and scientific evidence in understanding social structure and behaviors of the nonhuman primates with a social structure very similar to humans’. Thus, some results can directly be referred to the comprehension of human social structure and behavior.
Subject terms: Animal behaviour, Behavioural ecology
Introduction
Many animal species have evolutionarily developed their unique social structures under which there are a number of social groups that differ enormously in size, composition and permanence. Group-living animals usually have more benefits in survival and developments than those living alone, through reducing predatory risk, sharing resource, strengthening defense force, cooperative foraging, shared vigilance, and information transfer1. However, individuals within the groups vary in their nutritional requirements, habitat occupation, and ability to monopolize resources, and thus they have different motivations in decision making. If some individuals always prefer their own needs or motivate above others within a group, discord may cause group fragmentation and the loss of group-living advantages2. Thus, individuals must maintain a group cohesion and synchronize activities efficiently in order to continue social benefits of group-living. Under such a situation, some individuals have to try to keep a balance between their benefits and maintaining cohesion within a group. For example, when a wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) group travels from one location to another, some individuals might have to shorten their resting time when the group starts to travel, while others might have to wait until the group finishes feeding before they can depart. Thus, compromises are particularly common in collective group movement3–6.
Collective movement decision-making has gained much attention over the past several decades and demonstrates a wide diffusion across species with alternative evolution levels and social structures, ranging from eusocial insects7,8 to birds9, mammals10,11 and primates12–14. There are, however, generally two kinds of decision patterns in the process of collective movement decision-making. Animals with large groups, such as a flock of birds or school of fish, individuals make collective decisions follow a simple rule to synchronize movement and maintain cohesiveness, adjusting their direction and speed referring to neighboring individuals, known as a self-organizing system3,15. However, regarding stable and cohesive social groups, such as those of lions, wolves, and primates, group members are able to communicate directly, with one or several individual(s) leading group’s decision-making and others following the decision, which is called leadership16.
Researchers have classified such a leadership into two forms: (1). personal leadership that occurs when a single individual uses its high dominant social status or unique experience to lead the group with an “unshared decision”3,17. This occurs to mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei), where the silverback male directs the group by heading to his preferred direction and other group members have no choice but follow the leader18; (2). “shared leadership” in which any group member makes contribution to collective movement and the related decision making, and there is an intermediate form between the two extremes, called “partial leadership”, some group members or sub-groups lead the procedure of decision-making. Partial leadership is the most popularly observed among mammals19, such as in hamadryas baboons (Papio hamadryas), wherein the initiating and decision-making individuals are males, mainly older ones, with the leaders of one-male units (OMUs) that have the most influence in the decision-making process20.
Several factors have been considered to make contribution to different types of leadership. Individual attributes can affect leadership15, such as dominant rank (e.g., rhesus macaque Macaca mulatta21; wolf Canis lupus22; feral horse Equus ferus caballus23), sex preference24–26, age advantage (e.g., African elephants, Loxodonta Africana27), reproductive status (e.g., lactating females28–30), and experience (e.g., homing pigeons Columba livia,4,9,31). Social attributes also can influence the leadership15,32, for instance social organization, individuals with strong dominant hierarchies are most likely to have personal leadership9,21,22,26,33, and those with weak social status normally result to the shared leadership13,21,34; social interaction, individuals with strong social affiliations tend to be followed more often4,35,36; and individual importance within the social network, individuals with higher eigenvector centrality usually initiate successful departures than social individuals32.
A successful collective movement requires several steps for a final achievement, and a pre-departure period is playing an important role to influence group movement37. During the pre-departure period groups make a decision on moving direction or departure time, followed by group members6,26. Two types of pre-departure behavior patterns which are vocalization and voting behavior contribute to decision-making. Regarding the decision on departure time, group members can display their motivations about when to move by simply increasing activity or exhibiting a specific behavior (e.g., vocalizations in Canadian goose, Branta Canadensis38; mountain gorilla, Gorilla beringei beringei26,39). As for decision on moving direction, however, the choice is more complex than simple departure time and involves voting for different direction preferences. For example, when all group members wish to move, and different directions could be preferred, group members must vote for which is their preferred direction20,26,40. These pre-departure behaviors are typical of a shared consensus process that exists in almost all sociable animal species. They usually allow each individual to express its intention to decision making and ultimately plays a role in the launching of departure6,12,41.
A collective decision-making process involves not only the voting behavior but also a quorum threshold7,8,29,42. For example, regarding hamadryas baboon (Papio hamadryas) males repeatedly attempt to exhibit their preferred direction, and influence other group mates, and finally the entire group move to the direction selected once that male has gained the most support20,43. African buffalo (Syncerus caffer) also exhibit similar voting behavior40, Adult cows initially display certain direction orientating behaviors until the group eventually departs for a new grazing location, with a departure in the direction of the most frequently observed orientation. In both cases, a quorum offers a simple and efficient way for animals to achieve a fast and accurate collective decision8,42,44–46.
In collective group movement, a follower is often as important as a initiator in regard to a successful decisions. However, to work out an accurate threshold number for a successful decision is quite difficult, since the initiation may be viewed as failure if no individuals follow21,25,47. A mean number of the followers that determines a successful group movement can vary considerably among species, which may also be changed within a given season or due to the variation of resource abundance35,48. Early studies (wild Propithecus and Eulemur49) set a threshold of 50% of group members following for a successful movement49. However, other studies on other primates indicate that a certain number of followers (quorum) is required for a successful decisions on movement (e.g., five in chacma baboons Papio hamadryas2; three in Verreaux’s sifaka Propithecus verreauxi25). This quorum is an important parameter in decisions making50, and is considered a tradeoff between speed and accuracy during the decision-making process3. In general, a threshold of approximately three followers seems to be sufficient to elicit a group movement48. Since this number is big enough to provide sufficient protection against predators or generate collective knowledge for orientating within the home range and detecting resources.
The golden snub-nosed monkey (Rhinopithecus roxellana) is one of the colobine species endemic to China. It inhabits at high-altitude (1800–3000m) mountainous regions with remarkable seasonal variation of the forests features in central China. It feeds principally on leaves, fruits, buds, bark, lichen, and seeds51. This species is distinct from the other Asian colobines by forming large multi-level societies composed of more than 100 individuals52. The primary social and reproductive unit in its society is the OMU (One Male Unit). Members of an OMU travel and live together, high cohesive association, and remain spatially separated from the other OMUs. An OMU consists of one adult male, several breeding females, and their offspring53,54. Dominant hierarchies exist within the group, which are different between OMUs. However, there is no clear or consistent rank order among adult individuals within the same OMU52, and both intersexual bonds and female-female kin-bonds contribute to the maintenance and cohesion55.
Thus, through this study we analyze the mechanisms of how collective movement in the golden snub-nosed monkeys is conducted by focusing on how such movement is initiated within wild OMUs; who initiates the decision and participates in decision making; how the leadership is formed; and what kind of factors play important role in decision-making. Accordingly, we proposed the following predictions:
Because there is no consistent dominant hierarchies among the individuals within the OMU, we suppose that a shared leadership exists so that any group member is supposed to have a role in leading collective movement.
Due to the affiliative relationships within the group, individuals with higher eigenvector centrality values in grooming networks would be more likely to initiate movement than other individuals with lower values.
Since the number of the followers is important for a successful decision-making, we assume that quorum size is at least three followers elicit group movement.
Methods
All research reported in this manuscript adhered to the Integrative Zoology Principles for the Ethical Treatment of Non Human Primates. All research protocols reported in this manuscript were reviewed and approved by the Chinese Academy of Science. Our research received clearance from, and complied with the protocols approved by animal care committees of Wild Life Conservation Society of ShaanXi Province, China. All research reported in this manuscript adhered to the legal requirements of the Guanyinshan National Reserve, China, in which the work took place.
Study site and subjects
This study was conducted in Dapingyu region of the Guanyinshan National Nature Reserve (GNNR), which is located on southern slopes of the Mts. Qinling, Shaanxi Province, China (107°52′−108°02′E,33°20′−33°44′N) with an elevation of 1150–2574 m above sea level. Vegetation structure varies following the variation of the altitude and is dominated by deciduous broadleaf forest under 1500 m; coniferous and deciduous broadleaf mixed forest between 1500–2200 m; and coniferous forest above 2300 m. The area has a semi-humid montane climate. Average annual rainfall is approximately 924 mm, and average annual temperature is 11.5 °C, with a minimum of −14.3 °C in January and a maximum of 36.4 °C in July. The monkeys are the only residents in the region.
We targeted on Dapingyu Troop (DPT) in GNNR that has been observed since 2009. It composed of breeding band, all-male band, and several solitary males. The home range of the breeding band encompasses 15 km2 in mountainous forest, and is extremely difficult to be followed while they were crossing steep ravines and mountainous terrains. From 2010, we got habituated with this band using semi-provision53, in which individuals were provisioned with approximately 200 g of corn and apples per monkey per day for over a period of 20 days/month.
This targeted breeding band was consisted of 66 individuals belonging to six OMUs, with six adult males, 26 adult females (16 of them gave the birth), five sub-adult individuals, and 13 juveniles. (Table 1)
Table 1.
Age-sex categories | CH unit | CM unit | DG unit | GG unit | PT unit | TY unit | Proportion of category |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Adult male | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 12.00% |
Adult female with infant | 3 | 2 | 3 | 3 | 2 | 3 | 32.00% |
Adult female without infant | 2 | 2 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 4 | 20.00% |
Sub-adult | 1 | 0 | 2 | 0 | 2 | 0 | 10.00% |
Juvenile | 1 | 0 | 3 | 2 | 2 | 5 | 26.00% |
Note: CH, CM, DG, GG, PT, and TY units are the six targeted OMUs.
Behavioral definitions
Initiator
A siting group member turns its body to a certain direction, lasting for at least 10 s during the pre-departure period, or an individual moving at least more than 5m beyond the periphery of the group to a certain direction, without the same movement from the other group members.
Voter
Individuals turn their body to face the direction driven by the initiator during the pre-departure period, or individuals moving to a certain distance in the direction driven by the initiator before the whole OMU is moving.
Pre-departure period
When the whole targeted OMU members are exhibiting free activity, one individual turns its body toward a certain direction, indicating getting ready for departure. While the first individual moves a certain distance, the pre-departure period ends.
Quorum
Minimum number of the voters showing the same behavior as the initiator, and launching collective group behavior.
Direction decision-making (DDM)
OMU members driving the direction to move during the pre-departure period.
Movement implementation decision-making (MDM)
OMU members deciding how to move when the pre-departure period finishes.
Successful direction decision
When the direction initiated by initiator has been followed by the whole group members.
Decision on successful movement implementation
After 50% of the OMU members have joined the movement process.
Behavioral data collection
We conducted the research from April to December 2017, in which we started. The observations from 09:00 am to the time when the whole band left the provision to their sleeping site each day55. In turn, we observed one of the six OMUs per day.
When the targeted OMU in a given day finished feeding and returned to the trees surrounding the provision area, we recorded behavior via scan sampling56, with the information on the activity of all the members recorded. Scans were repeated at 5-min intervals for as long as the OMU was in view. If the whole OMU had moved out of view, the observation site was adjusted to allow a continued data collection next day.
During the afternoon (15:00 to 18:00 pm), while the band was preparing to leave the provision area, we recorded OMU’s movement via a digital camera (Canon G10, Canon Corporation) using all occurrences sampling56, so that were able to get the data based on the recorded behaviors, including times of the events, sequence and name of each individual involved with collective movement.
Data analysis
We classified the subjects into five age-sex categories: adult males (AM), adult females with infants (AFI), adult females without infants (AFN), sub-adults (SA), and juveniles (Juv). As infants were always carried by their mothers during collective movement, any data related to the infants were excluded.
Social status of each individual within the OMUs
As grooming is an important interactive social behavior, it was used to evaluate social relationships among the individuals within each OMU. We used the following formula to calculate grooming index for each individual daily:
where, F(A) and F(B) are the total number of scans for grooming A and B, respectively; FA(B) is the number of the scans in which B is the grooming partner of A when A was scanned; and FB(A) is the number of the scans in which A is the grooming partner of B when B was scanned.
To evaluate the social status regarding a specific individual within the OMU, we calculated it’s the eigenvector centrality coefficient according to grooming index.
Relative leadership distribution
We compared absolute frequency of initiations among the categories using homogeneity chi-square test (the number of initiations per category is divided by a total number of initiations of all the categories). However, absolute frequency does not reflect the probability of a category initiated based on the number of individuals per category, thus, we compared relative leadership among the categories using homogeneity chi-square test (the number of initiations observed per category is divided by the ratio of individuals of this category within a given OMU. This corrected number is then divided by the sum of all corrected number of initiations for all categories). Relative leadership is ranged from 0 to 1, where 0 indicates that the category never leads the group and 1 indicates that the category always leads the group6,12.
Influencing factors on relative leadership
We then clarified whether individual attributes had an effect on the frequency of successful or failed initiations. To do so, we used the generalized linear model (GLM) to examine the effects of age, sex, lactation state, affiliative relationships, and eigenvector centrality on initiation.
Link between direction and movement initiation
Association rule analysis is a popular and well recognized approach in discovering the interesting relationships among the variables collected. Such interesting relationship is usually expressed as X => Y, where X and Y are two disjoint subsets of all available in the database. X is regarded as the antecedent or LHS (left hand side) and Y as the consequent or RHS (right hand side). Interesting rules have to satisfy the constraints on the measures of significance and interestingness, the best-known constraints are minimum thresholds of support and confidence degrees, which are calculated with the following equation.
Support degree=, where XY represents the number of combined XY, all the samples in the database. Confidence degree is defined as when X appears, the probability that Y appears. Confidence degree (X => Y)=.
We analysed the relationship between direction decisions and the decision on movement implementation with association rule analysis. We tried to established association rule between the two relative independent decisions with the Apriori Algorithm model. We set the initiator in direction decision as LHS and the initiator movement implementation decision as RHS. The minimum thresholds on support and confidence are set as 0.1.
Quorum of successful collective movement
We confirmed the minimum number of voters required for a successful direction and the decision on movement implementation, and then performed survival analysis, in which “resultant event” in the model was set as the individual who joined the decision-making.
All statistical tools were from RStudio v1.1.44 (RStudio Team, 2018), with two-tailed test, and with p < 0.05 was the threshold of significance. Average values were expressed as means ± SD. The packages which used in our analysis as follow: the social network analysis used igraph package, survival analysis used survival and survminer packages, the Association rules analysis used Arules package and the GLM analysis used based package in R software.
Results
We recorded 1,223 direction decision initiations (average 203 ± 16.61 for six OMUs), and 865 successful direction decisions that triggered movement implementation initiation (average 144.17 ± 13.85 for six OMUs). Regarding decision-making process, 4.48 ± 1.18 individuals were required for a successful direction decision, and 3.6 ± 1.30 individuals were required for a successful movement implementation decision. As for successful direction decisions, 344 (39.76%) were initiated by adult males, 340 (39.31%) by adult females with infants, 178 (20.58%) by adult females without infants, and 3 (0.35%) by sub-adults. With regard to successful movement implementation, 328 (42.21%) were initiated by adult males, 283 (36.42%) by adult females with infants, 149 (19.18%) by adult females without infants, and 17 (2.19%) by sub-adults. Juvenile category was never observed initiating either decision.
Social relationships within targeted OMUs
The eigenvector centrality coefficient for each individual was calculated from the grooming index. Eigenvector centrality coefficients showed a decreasing rank: adult males> adult females > sub-adults > juveniles (Fig. 1).
Relative leadership driving the initiations of direction and movement implementation
As for decision making on direction and movement implementation, the frequency of successful initiations is not equally distributed across different age-sex categories (Direction decision: relative frequency: χ2 = 670.43, df = 4, p < 0.001; relative leadership: χ2 = 333.96, df = 4, p < 0.001; Movement implementation decision: relative frequency: χ2 = 575.4, df = 4, p < 0.001; relative leadership: χ2 = 439.57, df = 4, p < 0.001). Adult males are the most successful initiators for both decisions, illustrated by the highest likelihood across all categories (28.13% in direction initiation, and 37.91% in movement implementation initiation) and the highest likelihood of leadership success (42.75% in direction initiation, and 54.01% in movement implementation initiation). Conversely, sub-adults are the least successful initiators for both decisions (relative leadership is only 0.4% in direction initiation and 0.34% in movement implementation initiation).The juvenile category never initiated successful decisions (Table 2).
Table 2.
Initiator (category) | Direction decision | Movement implementation decision | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Success | Failure | Leadership | Success | Failure | Leadership | |
Adult male | 0.281 | 0.054 | 0.427 | 0.379 | 0.046 | 0.540 |
Adult female with infant | 0.278 | 0.093 | 0.158 | 0.327 | 0.031 | 0.175 |
Adult female without infant | 0.146 | 0.049 | 0.133 | 0.172 | 0.023 | 0.147 |
Sub-adult | 0.002 | 0.026 | 0.004 | 0.020 | 0.001 | 0.034 |
Juvenile | 0.000 | 0.070 | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.000 |
The frequencies of the failed initiations for both decisions are also not equally distributed across the different age-sex categories (Direction initiation: relative frequency: χ2=53.223, df=4, p<0.001; Movement implementation initiation: relative frequency: χ2=67.114, df=4, p<0.001). Furthermore, relative leadership is not significantly different among the categories (Direction initiation: leadership: χ2=3.546, df=4, p=0.471; Movement implementation initiation: leadership: χ2=7.001, df=4, p=0.136).
Influencing factors on direction and movement success
The GLM was used to test the factors affecting successful direction and movement implementation initiation. As for the decisions on direction and movement implementation, initiator identity (individual attribute) has no significant effect on a successful consensus decisions, and the number of the voters is the key factor driving decision-making processes (Table 3). Regarding the decisions on direction and movement implementation, initiator and voters jointly decide whether there should be collective movement. On the other hand, eigenvector centrality coefficients in grooming network are positively correlated with a successful direction initiation (Table 3).
Table 3.
Direction initiation | Movement implementation initiation | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Estimate | Std. | z | P | Estimate | Std. | z | P | |
Intercept | −9.210 | 1.264 | −7.285 | <0.01 | −12.893 | 4.278 | −3.014 | <0.01 |
AFN | 0.050 | 0.345 | 0.145 | −1.049 | 1.021 | −1.027 | ||
AM | −0.933 | 0.506 | −1.843 | 1.601 | 1.668 | 0.959 | ||
Juv | −17.704 | 871.595 | −0.020 | — | — | — | ||
SA | −0.066 | 0.952 | −0.070 | 0.953 | 2.292 | 0.416 | ||
Direction | 2.114 | 0.152 | 13.940 | <0.01 | 6.247 | 1.295 | 4.824 | <0.01 |
ECC | 9.932 | 2.701 | 3.677 | <0.01 | 3.584 | 7.955 | 0.451 |
Note: AFN: adult females without infant; AM: adult males; Juv: juveniles; SA: sub-adulst; ECC: eigenvector centrality coefficient
Number of voters required for a successful decision-making
Survival analysis was used to determine the number of the voters required for a successful decision on direction and movement implementation for six targeted OMUs (Fig. 2). As for a direction decision, initiation usually fails if voters’s number is under four, and there is a more than 50% of success rate if voters’ number is increased from four to five. With regard to the decision on movement implementation, less voters are required for a success. For example, such a rate is ~35% if voters’ number is less than three; ~65% with at least three voters; and 100% with more than four voters (Fig. 2).
Link rule between direction selection and movement implementation
According to the data on successful collective movement within each targeted OMUs, 10 rules have been found among OMUs; they are GG unit 2, CH unit 1, CM unit 2, DG unit 3, PT unit 2 and without rules from TY unit. Within those rules, 50% (5 rules) from adult males =>adult males, 30% (3 rules) from adult females with infants =>adult males, and 20% (2 rules) from adult males => adult females with infants. (Table 4, Fig. 3).
Table 4.
Target unit | LHS | link | RHS | Support | Confidence |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
CH unit | CH | => | CH | 0.23 | 0.53 |
CM unit | CM | => | CM | 0.27 | 0.59 |
CM unit | BH | => | CM | 0.11 | 0.45 |
DG unit | DG | => | DG | 0.17 | 0.56 |
DG unit | HN | => | DG | 0.14 | 0.57 |
DG unit | DG | => | HN | 0.10 | 0.33 |
GG unit | GG | => | GG | 0.37 | 0.72 |
GG unit | MJ | => | GG | 0.11 | 0.38 |
PT unit | PT | => | PT | 0.17 | 0.36 |
PT unit | PT | => | HL | 0.11 | 0.24 |
Note: LHS means the initiator of direction decision making; RHS means initiator of movement implementation decision making.
Discussion
The results based on nonhuman primates and illustrated in the tables and figures have provided very interesting values and information for us to address the issues proposed in this study. We found that collective movement initiation could be divided into two continual but relatively independent processes; collective movement direction decision occurs before OMU departure, which triggers a decision on which direction to move; the decision on collective movement implementation occurs once the direction has been decided, which triggers the decision on how to implement movement to the direction chosen.
In both processes, leadership is not equally distributed; adult individuals (especially adult males) are more likely to initiate collective direction and movement implementation decisions than any other age-sex categories within the OMU (Table 2). Nevertheless, a successful initiation depends not only on the initiating adult individual, but also on other adult voters. Thus, regarding the monkeys studied, leadership is not formed by a single adult individual that is able to elicit the followers48, but consisted of different adult individuals exhibiting alternative behaviors to be involved with a consensus decision (Fig. 2). Leadership is shared among adult categories; each having a potential chance to participate in consensus decisions, either as an initiator or voter. In contrast, although sub-adults and juveniles can try to initiate a decision, their attempts are almost never successful due to non-support from adult individuals, except for a sub-adult female individual who successfully initiated three direction decisions (1.89%) and 17 movement implementation decisions (11.49%) within the PT OMU. At the beginning of the study, she was categorized as a sub-adult. She was older than the other sub-adult individuals while she was involved with the decision and gave birth in the following breeding season. In other words, she was actually an adult female. If a successful initiation of collective movement can be regarded as a success of the leadership, the golden snub-nosed monkeys expresses a system of partial leadership18. Thus, our results make us exclude Prediction 1.
Partial leadership is most commonly observed among mammals15,19. Regarding some species, the leadership is distributed among individuals, particularly the old ones (e.g. African elephants, Loxodonta Africana27)with better knowledge about the environment27,57. As for other species, leadership is distributed among dominant individuals (e.g., rhesus macaque Macaca mulatta21; wolf Canis lupus22) or specific sexs (white-faced capuchin monkeys24). Previous studies have reported that social relationships can directly affect the distribution of leadership(Tibetan macaques32). In our study, individual attributes from sex and age categories influence successful decision-making differently (Table 3). Eigenvector centrality coefficient that quantifies the attraction of a given individual to the others in the group32,58 is positively correlated with leadership distribution (Table 3), implying that among monkey individuals whose who have better social association with others are more likely to initiate a successful decisions, and attract followers during collective movement. Thus, results supports the Prediction 2.
Although a collective movement initiation is composed of two relatively independent decision-making processes, our study shows that the decisions on collective movement are primarily governed by voting behavior and quorum fulfillment (Fig. 2). As for a direction decision in pre-departure period, an initiator (an OMU adult) turns its body in a particular direction indicating its preferred choice for the movement coming; another adult member in OMU would then vote for this direction by exhibiting the same behavior; and if the number of voting individuals reaches the quorum, the direction decision is made (Fig. 2). With regard to the decision on movement implementation, an initiator (OMU adult) walks a certain distance and waits for the responses from other adults; and if the number of the followers reach a quorum, the entire OMU moves (Fig. 2). Voting behavior and quorums have been reported from many animal species. For example, African buffalo and hamadryas baboon29. In wolves, gorillas, and Tonkean macaques, collective decisions are made by social amplification or selective mimetism, a group decision reaches when the voter number exceeds the minimum quorum41. However, what we found on the golden monkeys are somewhat different; an initiator’s behavior is more similar to that for recruitment and a final decision depends on the number of the voters, but not a majority of the OMU.
We also found that regarding a collective movement initiation, two decision processes are involved in a continuous procedure. This is somehow different from what found in the Tonkean macaques: the transition mechanism linking pre-departure with departure decisions is a continuous procedure in decision-making for a collective movement12. This mechanism is primarily governed by mimetism to notify behaviors during the pre-departure period, with a quorum combined with selective mimetism required at departure. These two processes decide the time and direction to which individuals will move29. In our research, we conducted association rule analysis to determine the link rule between the direction and the decision on a movement implementation. The related results (Table 4 and Fig. 3) indicate that the two decision processes are linked by an adult initiator. That is, the adult individual who initiates a successful direction decision is more motivated to initiate the decision on movement implementation.
Although a quorum is considered to be a simple and efficient way to achieve collective decisions41, a threshold number for a positive decision outcome may vary substantially among species, and may be changed seasonally or resource abundance35,48. Earlier research classified movement initiation as a failure no individuals followed the initiator21,25,47. Based on the threshold of 50% group members are required for successful movement49, however, other studies have indicated that the number of followers (quorum) required for a successful movement decision can vary within a range and is likely taxon-specific35,48 (e.g., five in chacma baboons, Papio hamadryas2; three in Verreaux’s sifaka Propithecus verreauxi25). The demand of a quorum is likely to provide sufficient protection against predators or collective knowledge to orientate within the home range and detect resources48. Furthermore, a quorum can promote decision-making and make it more accurate46. With regard to the golden monkeys, we found that a quorum number affects decision accuracy and success3. This is perhaps due to the fact that the golden snub-nosed monkeys are facing less pressure from the predation in the habitats with condense forests52 and their food resources are seasonally abundant or uniformly distributed52,53. In other words, aggregation for defense and foraging chances seem not to be the main impact factor on a quorum selection. On the other hand, the times for making a decision on moving direction and movement implementation are not significantly different among the six target OMUs. This implies that decision speed is not an important factor required for a quorum number. In regard to decision accuracy, however, a quorum is required for the golden monkeys, although voters’number not a fixed one (ranging from three to five). As long as individuals have employed a quorum rule, the threshold could vary greatly, with less effect on decision making, and the whole group could reach a solution accurately. As a result, our study allow us to accept Prediction 3.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
We gratefully acknowledge Guanyinshan National Nature Reserve, Foping County TouristBureau, and Foping Panda Valley Travel Limited for the permission to carry out this research. This study was funded by the Strategic Priority Research Program of the Chinese Academy of Sciences (XDB310200000 and XDB31020302), Key Project of Natural Science Foundation of China (31730104), National Key Program of Research and Development, Ministry of Science and Technology (2016YFC0503200), National Science Foundation of China (31472014, 31130061, 31270438, and 31470455), and Shaanxi Innovation Capability Support Program (2018PT-04).
Author contributions
Baoguo Li conceived the experiment(s); Chengliang Wang and Xiaowei Wang conducted data analysis and drafted the main manuscript; Ruliang Pan revised the main manuscript; Xiaoguang Qi and Songtao Guo prepared Figures. 1–3; Haitao Zhao and Yi Ren created Tables 1–4, Weiwei Fu and Zirui Zhu committed to data collection.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41598-019-57191-3.
References
- 1.Fichtel, C., Pyritz, L., & Kappeler, P. M. Coordination of Group Movements in Non-human Primates. In M. Boos, M. Kolbe, P. M. Kappeler, & T. Ellwart (Eds.), Coordination in Human and Primate Groups. pp. 37–56. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg (2011).
- 2.Stueckle S, Zinner D. To follow or not to follow: decision-making and leadership during the morning departure in chacma baboons. Animal Behaviour. 2008;75:1995–2004. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.12.012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Conradt L, Roper TJ. Consensus decision-making in animals. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2005;20:449–456. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2005.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.King AJ, Cowlishaw G. Leaders, followers, and group decision-making. Communicative & Integrative Biology. 2009;2:147–150. doi: 10.4161/cib.7562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Sueur C, MacIntosh AJJ, Jacobs AT, Watanabe K, Petit O. Predicting leadership using nutrient requirements and dominance rank of group members. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2013;67:457–470. doi: 10.1007/s00265-012-1466-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Ramos A, Petit O, Longour P, Pasquaretta C, Sueur C. Collective decision-making during group movements in European bison, Bison bonasus. Animal Behaviour. 2015;109:149–160. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.08.016. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Pratt S, Mallon E, Sumpter D, Franks N. Quorum sensing, recruitment, and collective decision-making during colony emigration by the ant Leptothorax albipennis. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2002;52:117–127. doi: 10.1007/s00265-002-0487-x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Seeley TD, Visscher PK. Quorum sensing during nest-site selection by honeybee swarms. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2004;56:594–601. doi: 10.1007/s00265-004-0814-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Flack A, Ákos Z, Nagy M, Vicsek T, Biro D. Robustness of flight leadership relations in pigeons. Animal Behaviour. 2013;86:723–732. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.07.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Espmark Y. Ecology and behaviour of the African buffalo, ecology and behaviour of the African buffalo: Social inequality and decision-making. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 1997;52:191–192. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(96)01116-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Bousquet CAH, Sumpter DJT, Manser MB. Moving calls: a vocal mechanism underlying quorum decisions in cohesive groups. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2011;278:1482–1488. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.1739. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Sueur C. Group decision-making in chacma baboons: leadership, order and communication during movement. BMC Ecology. 2011;11:26. doi: 10.1186/1472-6785-11-26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Fernández VA, Kowalewski M, Zunino GE. Who is coordinating collective movements in black and gold howler monkeys? Primates. 2013;54:191–199. doi: 10.1007/s10329-013-0342-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Fratellone GP, et al. Social connectivity among female Tibetan macaques (Macaca thibetana) increases the speed of collective movements. Primates. 2018;60(3):183–189. doi: 10.1007/s10329-018-0691-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Tokuyama N, Furuichi T. Leadership of old females in collective departures in wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) at Wamba. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2017;71:55. doi: 10.1007/s00265-017-2277-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.King AJ. Follow me! I’m a leader if you do; I’m a failed initiator if you don’t? Behavioural Processes. 2010;84:671–674. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Pyritz LW, Kappeler PM, Fichtel C. Coordination of Group Movements in Wild Red-fronted Lemurs (Eulemur rufifrons): Processes and Influence of Ecological and Reproductive Seasonality. International Journal of Primatology. 2011;32:1325–1347. doi: 10.1007/s10764-011-9549-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Fischer J, Zinner D. Communication and Cognition in Primate Group Movement. International Journal of Primatology. 2011;32:1279–1295. doi: 10.1007/s10764-011-9542-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Smith JE, et al. Collective movements, leadership and consensus costs at reunions in spotted hyaenas. Animal Behaviour. 2015;105:187–200. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.04.023. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Kummer, H. Social Organization of Hamadryas Baboons. A Field Study. Hans Kummer. University of Chicago Press, Chicago (1968).
- 21.Sueur C, Petit O. Shared or unshared consensus decision in macaques? Behavioural Processes. 2008;78:84–92. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Peterson RO, Jacobs AK, Drummer TD, Mech LD, Smith DW. Leadership behavior in relation to dominance and reproductive status in gray wolves, Canis lupus. Canadian Journal of Zoology. 2002;80:1405–1412. doi: 10.1139/z02-124. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Krueger K, Flauger B, Farmer K, Hemelrijk C. Movement initiation in groups of feral horses. Behavioural Processes. 2014;103:91–101. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2013.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Leca JB, Gunst N, Thierry B, Petit O. Distributed leadership in semifree-ranging white-faced capuchin monkeys. Animal Behaviour. 2003;66:1045–1052. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2003.2276. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Trillmich J, Fichtel C, Kappeler P. Coordination of Group Movements in Wild Verreaux’s Sifakas (Propithecus Verreauxi) Behaviour. 2004;141:1103–1120. doi: 10.1163/1568539042664579. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Bourjade M, Sueur C. Shared or unshared consensus for collective movement? Towards methodological concerns. Behavioural Processes. 2010;84:648–652. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.02.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.McComb K, et al. Leadership in elephants: the adaptive value of age. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2011;278:3270–3276. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.0168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Conradt L. Could asynchrony in activity between the sexes cause intersexual social segregation in ruminants? Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences. 1998;265:1359–1368. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1998.0442. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Sueur C, Deneubourg JL, Petit O. Sequence of quorums during collective decision-making in macaques. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2010;64:1875–1885. doi: 10.1007/s00265-010-0999-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Sueur C. Viability of decision-making systems in human and animal groups. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 2012;306:93–103. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2012.04.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Flack A, Pettit B, Freeman R, Guilford T, Biro D. What are leaders made of? The role of individual experience in determining leader–follower relations in homing pigeons. Animal Behaviour. 2012;83:703–709. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.12.018. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Wang X, et al. Social rank versus affiliation: Which is more closely related to leadership of group movements in Tibetan macaques (Macaca thibetana): Leadership in Tibetan Macaques. American Journal of Primatology. 2016;78:816–824. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.King AJ, Johnson DDP, Van Vugt M. The Origins and Evolution of Leadership. Current Biology. 2009;19:911–916. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Squires VR, Daws GT. Leadership and dominance relationships in Merino and Border Leicester sheep. Applied Animal. Ethology. 1975;1:263–274. [Google Scholar]
- 35.King AJ, Douglas CMS, Huchard E, Isaac NJB, Cowlishaw G. Dominance and Affiliation Mediate Despotism in a Social Primate. Current Biology. 2008;18:1833–1838. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.10.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Sueur, C., King, A. J., Pelé, M., & Petit, O. Fast and Accurate Decisions as a Result of Scale-Free Network Properties in Two Primate Species. In T. Gilbert, M. Kirkilionis, & G. Nicolis (Eds.), Proceedings of the European Conference on Complex Systems 2012. pp. 579–584. Cham: Springer International Publishing (2013).
- 37.Petit O, Bon R. Decision-making processes: The case of collective movements. Behavioural Processes. 2010;84:635–647. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Raveling DG. Preflight and Flight Behavior of Canada Geese. The Auk. 1969;86:671–681. doi: 10.2307/4083454. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Harcourt AH, Stewart KJ. Gorillas’ Vocalizations During Rest Periods: Signals of Impending Departure? Behaviour. 1994;130:29–40. doi: 10.1163/156853994X00127. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Prins H. H. T. Ecology and Behaviour of the African Buffalo. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Sueur C, Deneubourg JL, Petit O. From the first intention movement to the last joiner: macaques combine mimetic rules to optimize their collective decisions. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2011;278:1697–1704. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.2084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Ward AJW, Sumpter DJT, Couzin ID, Hart PJB, Krause J. Quorum decision-making facilitates information transfer in fish shoals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2008;105:6948–6953. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0710344105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.King AJ, Sueur C. Where Next? Group Coordination and Collective Decision-making by Primates. International Journal of Primatology. 2011;32:1245–1267. doi: 10.1007/s10764-011-9526-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Franks NR, Dornhaus A, Fitzsimmons JP, Stevens M. Speed versus accuracy in collective decision-making. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences. 2003;270:2457–2463. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2003.2527. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Petit O, Gautrais J, Leca JB, Theraulaz G, Deneubourg JL. Collective decision-making in white-faced capuchin monkeys. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2009;276:3495–3503. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.0983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Sumpter DJ, Pratt SC. Quorum responses and consensus decision-making. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2009;364:743–753. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Jacobs A, Maumy M, Petit O. The influence of social organisation on leadership in brown lemurs (Eulemur fulvus fulvus) in a controlled environment. Behavioural Processes. 2008;79:111–113. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Pyritz LW, King AJ, Sueur C, Fichtel C. Reaching a Consensus: Terminology and Concepts Used in Coordination and Decision-Making Research. International Journal of Primatology. 2011;32:1268–1278. doi: 10.1007/s10764-011-9524-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Erhart EM, Overdorff DJ. Female Coordination of Group Travel in Wild Propithecus and Eulemur. International Journal of Primatology. 1999;20:927–940. doi: 10.1023/A:1020830703012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Pennisi A, Giallongo L. Animal Biopolitics: How Animals Vote. International Journal for the Semiotics of Law - Revue internationale de Sémiotique juridique. 2018;31:491–499. doi: 10.1007/s11196-018-9560-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Guo ST, Li BG, Watanabe K. Diet and activity budget of Rhinopithecus roxellana in the Qinling Mountains, China. Primates. 2007;48:268–276. doi: 10.1007/s10329-007-0048-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Qi XG, Li BG, Garber PA, Ji W, Watanabe K. Social dynamics of the golden snub-nosed monkey (Rhinopithecus roxellana): female transfer and one-male unit succession. American Journal of Primatology. 2009;71:670–679. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Qi, X. G. et al. Satellite telemetry and social modeling offer new insights into the origin of primate multilevel societies. Nature Communications5 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 54.Zhang P, Watanabe K, Li BG, Tan CL. Social organization of Sichuan snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxellana) in the Qinling Mountains, Central China. Primates. 2006;47:374–382. doi: 10.1007/s10329-006-0178-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Wang XW, et al. A newly-found pattern of social relationships among adults within one-male units of golden snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxenalla) in the Qinling Mountains, China. Integrative Zoology. 2013;8:400–409. doi: 10.1111/1749-4877.12026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Altmann J. Observational Study of Behavior: Sampling Methods. Behaviour. 1974;49:227–266. doi: 10.1163/156853974X00534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Berry PSM, Bercovitch FB. Leadership of herd progressions in the Thornicroft’s giraffe of Zambia. African Journal of Ecology. 2015;53:175–182. doi: 10.1111/aje.12173. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Ramos A, Petit O, Longour P, Pasquaretta C, Sueur C. Space Use and Movement Patterns in a Semi-Free-Ranging Herd of European Bison (Bison bonasus) Plos One. 2016;11:e0147404. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0147404. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.