Abstract
Impulse control disorder (ICD) is a major non-motor complication of Parkinson's disease (PD) with often devastating consequences for patients' quality of life. In this study, we aimed to characterize the phenotype of impulsivity in PD and its neuroanatomical correlates.
Methods: Thirty-seven PD patients (15 patients with ICD, 22 patients without ICD) and 36 healthy controls underwent a neuropsychological battery. The test battery consisted of anxiety and depression scales, self-report measures of impulsivity (Barratt scale and UPPS-P), behavioral measures of impulsive action (Go/No-Go task, Stop signal task) and impulsive choice (Delay discounting, Iowa gambling task), and measures of cognitive abilities (working memory, attention, executive function). Patients and controls underwent structural MRI scanning.
Results: Patients with ICD had significantly higher levels of self-reported impulsivity (Barratt scale and Lack of perseverance from UPPS-P) in comparison with healthy controls and non-impulsive PD patients, but they performed similarly in behavioral tasks, except for the Iowa gambling task. In this task, patients with ICD made significantly less risky decisions than patients without ICD and healthy controls. Patients without ICD did not differ from healthy controls in self-reported impulsivity or behavioral measurements. Both patient groups were more anxious and depressive than healthy controls. MRI scanning revealed structural differences in cortical areas related to impulse control in both patient groups. Patients without ICD had lower volumes and cortical thickness of bilateral inferior frontal gyrus. Patients with ICD had higher volumes of right caudal anterior cingulate and rostral middle frontal cortex.
Conclusions: Despite the presence of ICD as confirmed by both clinical follow-up and self-reported impulsivity scales and supported by structural differences in various neural nodes related to inhibitory control and reward processing, patients with ICD performed no worse than healthy controls in various behavioral tasks previously hypothesized as robust impulsivity measures. These results call for caution against impetuous interpretation of behavioral tests, since various factors may and will influence the ultimate outcomes, be it the lack of sensitivity in specific, limited ICD subtypes, excessive caution of ICD patients during testing due to previous negative experience rendering simplistic tasks insufficient, or other, as of now unknown aspects, calling for further research.
Keywords: impulse control disorder, Parkinson's disease, impulsive action, impulsive choice, structural MRI, Iowa gambling task, delay discounting task, stop signal task
Introduction
Parkinson's disease (PD) is a neurodegenerative disease characterized not only by motor symptoms as rigidity, hypokinesis, and tremor, but also by a variety of non-motor deficits. Among these non-motor symptoms, impulse control disorder (ICD) is the one that has a devastating effect on the quality of patients' life. In the population of PD patients, ICD manifests itself in a wide spectrum of impulsive behaviors such as pathological gambling, binge eating, hypersexuality, excessive shopping, and also repetitive excessive behaviors like punding (stereotyped purposeless repetitive behavior), hobbyism (internet use, reading, art work), walk-abouts (purposeless wandering), and hoarding (1, 2).
ICD in PD is usually believed to be a consequence of dopaminergic treatment (2, 3), but recent studies claim that there is an interaction of medication influence with underlying vulnerability to impulsive behavior (4, 5). Therefore, it is important to describe the behavioral pattern and neurobiological correlates of impulsivity in PD to track possible correlates of ICD in PD patients. Impulsivity is a heterogeneous concept, which can be understood as a personality trait or as a consequence of a neurobiological function deficit. Behavioral models of impulsivity distinguish impulsive action, which means inability to inhibit prepotent or unwanted actions (waiting impulsivity) or inability to stop ongoing action (stopping impulsivity), and impulsive choice, which includes aspects like high sensitivity to reward, risk taking, and preference of small immediate rewards to long-term gains (6). Impulsivity as a personality trait is measured by self-reported questionnaires, such as the Barratt scale or the UPPS-P scale; impulsivity as neurobiological deficit is measured by behavioral tasks.
Research using self-reported methods concluded that people with certain personality traits are in higher risk of developing ICD. Specifically, associations between ICD and higher novelty/sensation seeking, compulsivity, depression, and anxiety were found (3, 7–9). Impulsive personal traits have been previously associated with ICD in PD. Patients with ICD manifest elevated self-impulsivity in the Barratt scale (9, 10). Other risk factors for developing ICD in PD patients are younger age/younger age of disease onset, family history of behavior such as gambling or alcoholism, being man, and being single (2, 8, 11–14).
Previous research revealed increased impulsive choice, i.e., increased tendency to risky or disadvantageous decisions in general PD population (i.e., ICD status was not reported) (15–23) or in PD-ICD patients specifically (9, 24, 25). A tendency to irrational choices and early decisions in the Bead task, a test of reflection impulsivity, was observed in PD-ICD patients (26). These patients collected less information than PD patients without ICD or healthy controls before making a decision. However, results are not consistent. Study of Voon et al. (27) reported that PD-ICD patients have greater tendency to risk in comparison with patients without ICD, but only when there is only gain possibility, not in a situation with possibility of loss. Several studies did not find any differences in choice impulsivity between PD-ICD patients and healthy controls or PD patients without ICD (28–30) or in general PD population (31, 32). One study reported only statistical trend toward more risky decisions in the PD-ICD group in the Iowa gambling task (10). No significant differences in the performance between patients with and without ICD were reported in the Balloon Analog Risk Task (33–35). Ambivalent results were obtained regarding learning from the negative feedback. Several studies found lower sensitivity to negative feedback in PD-ICD (33, 36, 37), but others found no differences between ICD and patients without ICD or healthy controls (11, 35). Moreover, PD-ICD patients without medication showed decreased learning from negative feedback and increased learning from positive feedback compared to the PD-ICD patients on medication (11). Patients with ICD on dopamine agonists make faster decisions and more impulsive choices than patients off medication (28) and show enhanced sensitivity to risk (27).
Several studies reported increased stopping impulsivity (38–41) and waiting impulsivity (42) in the general PD population. However, there are also reports of intact impulsive action in PD patients without ICD (43) and in PD patients in general (ICD not specified) (44). Studies conducted on PD-ICD patients brought evidence of intact impulsive action (10, 45, 46); one study reported even faster Stop signal reaction time (SSRT), which means better ability to stop ongoing actions, i.e., lower stopping impulsivity in PD-ICD patients (43).
Structural imaging methods found evidence of cortical thickness abnormalities in PD-ICD in the structures related to impulse control and decision making, namely, the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), and corpus callosum (47–51). A gray-matter volume loss in amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex and volume changes in nucleus accumbens (NAcc) and in amygdala were observed in PD-ICD (21, 47, 48, 52).
There are only few studies directly comparing PD patients with and without ICD in impulsivity domains. Moreover, individual studies usually do not target multiple impulsive domains simultaneously, despite the heterogeneous nature of impulsivity. The aim of our study was to describe the phenotype of impulsivity in PD patients with and without ICD by testing multiple impulsivity domains. We included three groups into our research: PD patients with ICD (PD-ICD), PD patients without ICD (PD-nonICD) and healthy controls (HC). We used self-reported impulsivity questionnaires (the UPPS-P and the Barratt scale), behavioral tasks for assessment of impulsive action (Go/No-Go task, Stop signal task), and impulsive choice (Iowa gambling task, Delay discounting task). We also measured depression, anxiety, and cognitive components influencing performance in impulsivity tasks (attention, working memory, and executive functioning). Moreover, structural magnetic resonance images (MRI) were obtained. We analyzed several brain regions, which were previously in the literature linked to impulsivity, inhibitory control, and decision making (Table 3).
Based on previous research, we hypothesized that PD patients with ICD, but not those without ICD, show greater impulsivity in self-reported scales. Further, we hypothesized that only PD-ICD patients show increased impulsivity in the Sensation seeking subscale of UPPS-P in comparison with healthy controls. We did not make any specific hypotheses for the other UPPS-P dimensions due to the lack of literature. We expect greater impulsive choice in the PD-ICD population, but intact impulsive action. No specific hypotheses were made about impulsive action and impulsive choice in patients without ICD due to conflicting literature and lack of literature targeting specifically PD patients without ICD. We expected PD-ICD patients to show brain structure abnormalities in comparison with healthy controls in the prefrontal cortex (orbitofrontal cortex, ACC, and DLPFC) and NAcc.
Methods
Subjects
Thirty-seven patients with Parkinson‘s disease and 36 healthy controls took part in this study; the groups were matched by age, gender, education, and laterality. All patients were recruited from the University Hospital of St. Anne, Brno, Czech Republic. Healthy volunteers were recruited by advertising in local newspapers. Patients aged 25–75 with a diagnosis of Parkinson‘s disease based on the UK Brain Bank Criteria (53) were recruited in the study. All patients were under dopaminergic medication for at least 12 months preceding the examination and had stable doses at least for 4 weeks before testing. Exclusion criteria for all subjects were neurological or systemic disorder with effect on brain function (except PD in the patient groups), lesions in MRI scans, comorbid psychotic disease, affective disease or autism spectrum disorder, mental retardation, cognitive deficit (MMSE under 27), severe depression, and substance abuse.
Prior to the testing, data about age of the PD onset, Hoehn and Yahr stage, and medication calculated as levodopa equivalent dose were obtained, and cognition was examined using MMSE. Healthy volunteers were assessed by the Mini international neuropsychiatric interview [MINI; (54)] to exclude any subjects with psychiatric symptoms. The patients had no self-reported cognitive problems; none of them scored below 28 in Mini-Mental State Examination [MMSE; (55)]. Most of the patients were in stage 2 or 3 according to the Hoehn and Yahr Scale (56). Fifteen patients had ICD, and 22 patients were without ICD. Patients with ICD were selected by a neurologist based on interview with the patient and presence of ICD signs in their medical records. Only patients with severe behavioral problems connected to ICD were recruited. The patients showed the following ICD symptoms: gambling (5 patients), binge eating (3 patients), hypersexuality (2 patients), hobbyism (1 patient), compulsive buying (1 patient), hoarding (1 patient), punding (1 patient), pedantry (1 patient), and excessive cleaning (1 patient). Some of the patients showed more than one symptom of ICD. The study was approved by the Institutional Ethical Committee. All participants signed the informed consent prior to the beginning of the procedure. Characteristics of the sample are summarized in Table 1.
Table 1.
PD-ICD (n = 15) | PD-nonICD (n = 22) | Healthy controls (n = 36) | Statistics value, p | Effect size | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Gender M/F | 11/4 | 10/12 | 19/17 | ||
Age | 59.27 (8.88) | 69.18 (5.47) | 66.14 (7.70) | F(2, 70) = 8.214, p = 0.001 | |
Education | 6.7, 13.3, 46.7, 33.4 | 4.5, 13.6, 68.2, 13.6 | 0, 16.7, 55.6, 27.7 | = 0.658, p = 0.658 | |
Socioeconomic status | 6.7, 33.3, 46.7, 6.7 | 13.6, 22.7, 54.5, 9.0 | 2.8, 19.4, 52.8, 22.2 | = 3.902, p = 0.142 | |
Neurologic data | d | ||||
H a Y stage | 2.53 (0.64) | 2.48 (0.66) | p = 0.799, t = 0.256 | d = 0.085927 | |
Age of onset | 50.80 (9.64) | 62.55 (6.25) | p < 0.001, t = −4.160 | d = 1.445757 | |
Diseases duration | 8.87 (4.17) | 6.95 (4.63) | p = 0.208, t = 1.282 | d = 0.433676 | |
L-dopa equivalent dose | 1289.75 (543.97) | 1025.46 (567.18) | p = 0.166, t = 1.414 | d = 0.475602 | |
Neuropsychological data | η2 | ||||
MADRS | 3.73 (5.68) | 3.05 (3.84) | 0.69 (1.83) | F(2, 70) = 5.202, p = 0.008 | η2 = 0.129 |
SAS score | 36.73 (5.40) | 35.73 (5.25) | 28.50 (6.59) | F(2, 70) = 14.980, p < 0.001 | η2 = 0.300 |
Education (%), Primary, lower secondary, higher secondary, college.
Socioeconomic status (%), insufficient, unsatisfactory, satisfactory, very satisfactory.
MADRS, Montgomery Asberg Depression Rating Scale; SAS, Zung Self-reported Anxiety Scale; M, male; F, female.
Experimental Procedure
Subjects underwent a neuropsychological battery of tests and questionnaires and performed four computerized behavioral tasks. The test battery included the Barratt scale (57) and the UPPS-P scale (58–60) for measuring self-reported impulsivity, the Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale (61), the Zung Self-Rating Anxiety Scale (62), and standardized measures of cognitive functions. Waiting impulsivity was measured by the Go/No-Go task (GNG), stopping impulsivity was measured by the Stop signal task (SST), and impulsive choice was assessed by the Delay discounting task (DDT) and the Iowa gambling task (IGT). Results are summarized in Table 2. Behavioral tests were created in E-Prime 2.0 software. The whole testing procedure took ~2.5 h.
Table 2.
PD-ICD (n = 15) | PD-nonICD (n = 22) | Healthy controls (n = 36) | Statistic value, p | Effect size | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Barrat scale | 60.47 (7.54) | 54.14 (5.60) | 54.31 (6.48) | F(2, 70) = 5.537, p = 0.006 | η2 = 0.137 |
UPPS-P PRE | 20.60 (5.05) | 19.77 (4.36) | 18.67 (4.03) | F(2, 70) = 1.162, p = 0.319 | η2 = 0.032 |
UPPS-P PER | 20.33 (5.08) | 19.14 (3.58) | 16.83 (3.48) | F(2, 70) = 5.140, p = 0.008 | η2 = 0.128 |
UPPS-P SS | 22.07 (6.78) | 23.64 (6.76) | 26.25 (6.85) | F(2, 70) = 2.317, p = 0.106 | η2 = 0.062 |
UPPS-P NU | 27.27 (6.44) | 25.18 (6.54) | 25.51 (6.22) | F(2, 69) = 0.537, p = 0.587 | η2 = 0.015 |
UPPS-P PU | 28.33 (7.51) | 28.27 (6.99) | 26.69 (8.25) | F(2, 70) = 0.391, p = 0.678 | η2 = 0.011 |
Go omissions % | 18.95 (5.57) | 18.76 (7,80) | 16.94 (5.96) | F(2, 70) = 0.782, p = 0.461 | η2 = 0.022 |
Go RT | 419.36 (50.98) | 418.03 (48.11) | 402.83 (56.48) | F(2, 70) = 0.808, p = 0.450 | η2 = 0.023 |
No-Go commissions % | 18.65 (12.36) | 25.97 (19.51) | 22.45 (17.21) | F(2, 70) = 0.827, p = 0.442 | η2 = 0.023 |
SSRT | 298.46 (125.45) | 242.36 (115.86) | 281.97 (80.90) | F(2, 69) = 1.580, p = 0.213 | η2 = 0.044 |
DD k 990 | 0.022 (0.046) | 0.061 (0.125) | 0.019 (0.050) | F(2, 65) = 1.886, p = 0.160 | η2 = 0.026 |
DD k 24900 | 0.005 (0.0103) | 0.077 (0.385) | 0.004 (0.005) | F(2, 69) = 0.626, p = 0.538 | η2 = 0.018 |
DD AUC 990 | 0.61 (0.31) | 0.43 (0.29) | 0.58 (0.34) | F(2, 68) = 1.854, p = 0.164 | η2 = 0.052 |
DD AUC 24900 | 0.76 (0.25) | 0.71 (0.23) | 0.76 (0.30) | F(2, 68) = 0.251, p = 0.779 | η2 = 0.007 |
IGT NET score 1st part | 13.80 (29.33) | 3.18 (25.80) | 7.17 (25.28) | F(2, 70) = 0.729, p = 0.486 | η2 = 0.020 |
IGT NET score 2nd part | 39.07 (44.89) | −0.91 (33.84) | 15.00 (37.43) | F(2, 70) = 4.925, p = 0.010 | η2 = 0.123 |
IGT B % 1st part | 24.55 (9.03) | 32.55 (10.72) | 31.00 (11.27) | F(2, 70) = 2.723, p = 0.073 | η2 = 0.072 |
IGT B % 2nd part | 16.86 (13.74) | 36.64 (17.04) | 30.58 (15.98) | F(2, 69) = 6.696, p = 0.002 | η2 = 0.163 |
Digit span | 14.47 (2.72) | 14.18 (3.00) | 14.67 (3.87) | F(2, 68) = 0.138, p = 0.872 | η2 = 0.004 |
d2 speed | 119.07 (24.99) | 106.80 (24.50) | 126.06 (19.99) | F(2, 66) = 4.624, p = 0.013 | η2 = 0.123 |
d2 accuracy (error %) | 9.25 (8.78) | 8.67 (6.06) | 9.39 (6.21) | F(2, 66) = 0.074, p = 0.929 | η2 = 0.002 |
ToL moves | 34.79 (17.08) | 40.90 (24.29) | 31.08 (19.67) | F(2, 68) = 1.493, p = 0.232 | η2 = 0.042 |
ToL init. time | 137.21 (71.74) | 91.25 (42.32) | 109.81 (55.98) | F(2, 68) = 2.841, p = 0.065 | η2 = 0.077 |
ToL exec. time | 337.71 (164.86) | 360.10 (175.52) | 278.33 (141.59) | F(2, 68) = 2.000, p = 0.143 | η2 = 0.056 |
PRE, Lack of premeditation; PER, Lack of perseverance; SS, Sensation seeking; NU, Negative urgency; PU, positive urgency; Go RT, Go reaction time; SSRT, Stop signal reaction time; DD, delay discounting; AUC, Area under the curve; IGT, Iowa gambling task; IGT B %, percentage of B deck cards selections; ToL, Tower of London; moves, total number of moves made during the task; ToL inic. time; ToL exec. time.
Impulsivity Questionnaires
We used the validated Czech translation of the Barratt scale for assessing impulsivity (57). This method includes aspects of non-planning impulsiveness, attentional impulsiveness, and motor impulsiveness. However, the three-factor structure has been questioned [e.g., (63, 64)]; therefore, only total score for this scale was calculated. Self-reported impulsivity was also measured by the Czech validated translation of the UPPS-P scale (58–60). The questionnaire consists of five subscales: Lack of premeditation (11 items), Lack of perseverance (10 items), Sensation seeking (12 items), Negative urgency (12 items), and Positive urgency (14 items), where urgency indicates the tendency to act impulsively under influence of emotions.
Behavioral Tasks
GNG Task
The GNG task contained two types of stimuli, the letter A was a Go stimulus and the letter B was a No-Go stimulus. Stimuli were in white color presented on the black screen. Subjects had to quickly react by pressing a key when letter A appeared and avoid the reaction when letter B appeared. A fixation cross preceded each stimulus. The whole task consisted of four blocks with 48 trials in each block. All subjects performed a short practice before the testing. The stimulus duration was 0.4 s; fixation cross duration varied from 1.1 to 2.6 s. Three parameters were analyzed from this task: No-Go commission errors percentage (percentage of erroneous key press after No-Go trial), Go omission errors percentage (percentage of Go trials erroneously followed by no key press), and Go reaction time (average reaction time on correct Go trials). Go stimuli occurred in 83% and No-Go stimuli occurred in 17%; this ratio makes the task more cognitively demanding and the subjects prone to make more commission errors.
SST
Stimuli in the SST were white arrows pointing to the left or to the right; subjects had to press an arrow key pointing to the same direction as the arrow presented on the black screen. However, the subjects had to stop their reaction and press no key when the stimulus was followed by a visual stop signal (the arrow turned red). Stop signals appeared in 25% of trials. Delays in the presentation of stop signal (stop signal delay, SSD) varied during the task in order to prevent the subject from developing response pattern, the starting latency was 200 ms. When the subject succeeded in the trial, the latency increased by 45 ms; when the subject failed, the latency decreased by 45 ms. Each stimulus lasted until the subject reacted by pressing a key or for 1 s if there was no reaction from the subject. A fixation cross appeared before every stimulus; the duration of the cross was again variable between 1.1 and 2.6 s. The task consisted of four blocks with 48 trials in each block; a short practice preceded the testing procedure. The SSRT was calculated by subtracting the average SSD from the average Go reaction time. SSRT refers to the time that the subject needs to stop his/her reaction; the longer time needed, the more difficult it is to interrupt one's own actions; i.e., higher SSRT is linked with higher impulsivity.
DD
In the DD task, subjects were asked to choose between two possible rewards in every trial—a smaller but immediate reward or higher but delayed reward. For example, Would you prefer to receive 510 CZK today or 990 CZK in a week? There were five possible delays—1 day, 1 week, 1 month, 3 months, and 6 months—and two delayed reward levels 990 CZK (around 40 EUR) and 24900 CZK (around 980 EUR). These delays and rewards were set according to the pilot study. Questions with different combinations of delays and delayed rewards were presented in random order and the subject was asked if he or she prefers the immediate or the delayed reward. The amount of immediate reward varied in intervals of 20 CZK (in case of smaller delayed reward) or 500 CZK (in case of bigger delayed reward) until an indifference point, where the subjective value of the immediate reward is equal to the subjective value of the delayed reward, was found.
Two parameters were calculated for each delayed reward (k parameter and area under the curve). The hyperbolic discount parameter (k) was calculated by the equation DR = I/(k*D), where DR is delayed reward, I is immediate reward, and D is delay. Naturally, as the reward delay increases, the subjective value of this reward decreases (65). Larger values of k indicate steeper decline of the subjective value and therefore greater impulsivity. The second parameter was the area under the curve; the curve is estimated by the connection of indifference points for each delay reward. The lower the AUC means the steeper discounting and the higher impulsive choice (66).
IGT
The computerized version of IGT consisted of 200 trials (67, 68). In every trial, subjects had to make a series of choices between four card decks by pressing the key with number of the chosen deck. Two of the decks are disadvantageous (A and B) and two decks are advantageous (C and D). Subjects were informed that some of the decks were better than the other and they were instructed to play until the game ends and try to win as much money as possible. However, participants didn't know which decks are bad or good, how many trials had the game, or risk of losses in each deck. Impulsive behavior is associated with decks A and B. Deck A contains high immediate rewards with high risk of loss, and deck B contains high immediate rewards with low risk of very high loss. IGT net score was calculated by subtraction of disadvantageous deck choices from advantageous deck choices [(C + D) − (A + B)]. We also compared the relative frequency of A and B deck choices.
Cognitive Abilities and Executive Functions
For working memory, the total score of Digit span subtest from WAIS-III (69) was analyzed. Three parameters from Tower of London (70) were calculated for assessment of executive functions—total move score, total initiation time, and total execution time. Attention was measured by test d2-R (71) with two analyzed parameters—speed (total number of processed items) and accuracy (percentage of omission and commission errors).
MRI Data Acquisition and Analysis
After the behavioral testing, the patients and controls underwent MRI scanning. Ten patients and eight controls did not undergo the acquisition due to MRI contraindications or inability to tolerate the procedure. The scanning was performed using a 3-T MRI scanner, SIEMENS MAGNETOM Prisma syngo (Siemens Medical Systems, Erlangen, Germany) at the Central European Institute of Technology of Masaryk University, Brno, Czech Republic. A high-resolution T1-weighted scan was acquired using the following parameters: MPRAGE sequence with repetition time = 2,300 ms, echo time = 2.34 ms, flip angle = 8°, voxel size 1.00 × 1.00 × 1.00 mm, matrix 240 × 224 × 224. Further three patients were excluded from the scanning because of excessive head movement resulting to significant motion artifacts not compatible with the automatic processing pipeline. The final size of the sample that competed MRI session was 16 PD-nonICD patients, 8 PD-ICD patients, and 28 healthy controls.
Volumetric segmentation was performed in this T1-weighted scan using the standard automated pipeline (72) implemented in the FreeSurfer image analysis suite, version 5.3.0 (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/). The accuracy of the segmentation in each subject was visually inspected by a trained operator (P.F.). The volumes (adjusted for the estimated intracranial volume) and surface areas (in case of cortical regions) of several regions of interest (Table 3) and the volume of the whole brain, as provided by the automatic segmentation algorithm of FreeSurfer, were compared between the PD-nonICD, PD-ICD patients and healthy controls using one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Bonferroni post-hoc multiple-comparison correction.
Table 3.
Anatomical region | Side |
---|---|
Caudate | R,L |
Putamen | R,L |
Nucleus accumbens | R,L |
Accumbens area | R,L |
Caudal anterior cingulate | R,L |
Rostral anterior cingulate | R,L |
Caudal middle frontal | R,L |
Rostral middle frontal | R,L |
Lateral orbitofrontal | R,L |
Medial orbitofrontal | R,L |
Pars orbitalis | R,L |
Pars triangularis | R,L |
Precentral gyrus | R,L |
Insula | R,L |
L, left; R, right.
We performed statistical comparisons (two-sample t-tests) of subgroup of patients who underwent MRI scanning with the subgroup of patients who did not undergo MRI scanning to be sure that the MRI subgroup was not significantly different from the original patient group in the behavioral, demographic, and other measured parameters.
Statistical Analysis
Data were analyzed in IBM SPSS Statistics 24 software. We compared demographic characteristics, performance in computerized tasks, cognitive tasks, scores from questionnaires, and MRI parameters between groups of healthy controls, Parkinson patients with ICD, and Parkinson patients without ICD. The data from computerized behavioral tasks, questionnaires, and cognitive tasks were compared by one-way ANOVA without covariates. In cases of significant group differences, Tukey's post-hoc tests were applied. Sociodemographic characteristics (education, socioeconomic status) were analyzed by Kruskal–Wallis H-test; neurological data of the two patient groups were analyzed by independent t-tests. IGT net scores of the first and the second part of the task were analyzed in jamovi software by repeated measures ANOVA with Tukey's post-hoc tests with time (first vs. second part of the task) as within-subject factor and group as between-subject factor. Results are reported at p < 0.05 level of significance.
Results
Demographic Data
The three groups did not differ in education or socioeconomic status (Table 1). The groups were also matched by age; the healthy controls were selected to have similar age to patients with 2 years tolerance. However, there was a difference in age between groups; patients with ICD were significantly younger than those without ICD and healthy controls (PD-nonICD vs. PD-ICD: p < 0.001, PD-ICD vs. HC: p = 0.009). The PD-ICD patients were younger than PD-nonICD patients at the time of the disease onset (PD-ICD vs. PD-nonICD: p < 0.001). There were no differences in disease duration, levodopa equivalent dose, or Hoehn and Yahr stage between clinical groups.
The group of patients who underwent MRI scanning did not differ from the group of patients without MRI in the cognitive abilities, behavioral parameters, or self-reported impulsivity. However, the groups significantly differed in age (p = 0.028, group with MRI mean = 67.56, SD = 6.4; group without MRI mean = 58.7, SD = 10.5).
Depression and Anxiety
There was a significant effect of group on scores of anxiety and depression. Both clinical groups (PD-ICD and PD-nonICD) had higher scores of depression than healthy controls (MADRS score PD-ICD vs. HC: p = 0.018, PD-nonICD vs. HC: p = 0.043). Both clinical groups also had significantly higher levels of anxiety than healthy controls (SAS score PD-ICD vs. HC: p < 0.001, PD-nonICD vs. HC: p < 0.001). No significant differences were detected between PD-nonICD and PD-ICD patients in anxiety or depression.
Impulsivity Questionnaires
There were significant group differences in the Barratt scale score. The PD-ICD group scored the highest on the Barratt scale, which means that PD-ICD patients were more impulsive than PD-nonICD patients (p = 0.013) and controls (p = 0.008). The PD-nonICD group and control group did not differ in the Barratt scale score (p = 0.995). In the UPPS-P scale, patients with ICD showed elevated score, as compared to healthy controls, in Lack of perseverance (p = 0.017). No differences between the groups were found in the other UPPS-P subscales. The PD-nonICD group did not differ from healthy controls in any of the self-reported impulsivity measurements and did not differ from PD-ICD except for the Barratt scale score.
Behavioral Tasks
There was no significant effect of group on performance in GNG, DDT, or SST (Table 2). Both groups of patients had similar RTs, accuracy, SSRT, and discounting parameters as healthy controls. Group differences were found in performance in IGT. Analysis revealed significant effects of time, group, and time vs. group interaction on the IGT net score [(C + D) − (A + B)]. There were no significant differences in the NET score in the first part of the task, but the PD-ICD group had significantly higher NET score than the PD-nonICD group (p = 0.007) in the second part of the task. No significant differences were found between healthy controls and PD-nonICD or between healthy controls and PD-ICD. There was significant effect of time vs. group interaction [F(2, 70) = 3.36, p = 0.041]. Post-hoc tests revealed that only the PD-ICD group improved their NET score in the second half of the task; the difference was marginally significant in PD-ICD (p = 0.055) and not significant in other groups (HC: p = 0.733, PD-nonICD: p = 0.993). We further made analysis of particular deck selections in the first and second half of the task. There was a group difference in B deck preference in the second half of the task; specifically, the PD-ICD group selected cards from the B deck less likely than the PD-nonICD group (PD-ICD vs. PD-nonICD: p = 0.002) and healthy controls (PD-ICD vs. HC: p = 0.021). No significant differences were found in A deck selection relative frequency.
Cognitive Abilities and Executive Functions
The three groups did not differ in working memory (Digit span). ANOVA revealed significant group differences in processing speed in d2 test. Patients without ICD were significantly slower than healthy controls (PD-nonICD vs. HC: p = 0.009). No significant differences were found between the PD-ICD patients and healthy controls or between the two patients' groups in the accuracy in d2 test. No significant differences were detected in total move score and total execution time in the Tower of London. However, we observed a trend in total initiation time between the PD-nonICD and PD-ICD group. The PD-ICD group showed longer incitation time than PD-nonICD group, but the comparison was only marginally significant (PD-nonICD vs. PD-ICD p = 0.052). All groups worked with similar accuracy in the test.
MRI
PD-nonICD patients differed from HC in the estimated intracranial volume (eICV) of the right nucleus accumbens area (p = 0.002), bilateral pars orbitalis (p < 0.001), the left pars triangularis (p = 0.002), and the left precentral gyrus (p = 0.032) (Table 4); PD-nonICD patients showed decrease of volumes in all of these regions (All presented p-values are Bonferroni-corrected for multiple comparisons.) PD-nonICD patients also showed cortical thinning of the left pars orbitalis (p = 0.020) in comparison with HC. PD-ICD patients in comparison with HC showed increase of the right caudal anterior cingulate area (p = 0.003) and right rostral middle frontal area (p = 0.018) and also increase of total volumes of these regions (right caudal ACC, p = 0.010, right rostral middle frontal p = 0.007). However, differences in eICV volumes in these areas did not reach significance in the case of PD-ICD group. Both groups—PD-nonICD and PD-ICD—showed a decrease in the eICV volume of the right precentral gyrus in comparison with HC, but none of the results reached significance in post-hoc testing (p > 0.05). In the comparison of PD-nonICD and PD-ICD, only total volumes of bilateral pars orbitalis (right p = 0.029, left p = 0.039) and right caudal anterior cingulate (p = 0.041) differed, with PD-ICD patients having greater volumes. Comparison of cortical areas, eICV volumes, or cortical thickness between the two patient groups did not bring any significant results.
Table 4.
Volumes | p | F | Significant contrasts |
---|---|---|---|
Right accumbens area | 0.022 | 4.130 | PD-nonICDxHC |
lh pars orbitalis | 0.013 | 4.737 | PD-nonICDxHC, PD-nonICDXPD-ICD |
rh caudal anterior cingulate | 0.012 | 4.850 | PD-nonICDXPD-ICD, PD-ICDxHC |
rh pars orbitalis | 0.005 | 5.841 | PD-nonICDXPD-ICD, PD-nonICDxHC |
rh rostral middle frontal | 0.009 | 5.202 | PD-ICDxHC |
Volumes eICV | |||
Right accumbens area | 0.003 | 6.688 | PD-nonICDxHC |
lh pars orbitalis | 0.000 | 10.515 | PD-nonICDxHC |
lh pars triangularis | 0.002 | 7.074 | PD-nonICDxHC |
lh precentral gyrus | 0.013 | 4.718 | PD-nonICDxHC |
rh pars orbitalis | 0.000 | 13.144 | PD-nonICDxHC |
rh precentral gyrus | 0.021 | 4.160 | No significant contrast |
Thickness | |||
lh pars orbitalis | 0.021 | 4.171 | PD-nonICDxHC |
Area | |||
rh caudal anterior cingulate | 0.005 | 6.006 | PD-ICDxHC |
rh rostral middle frontal | 0.022 | 4.146 | PD-ICDxHC |
Volumes eICV, estimated intracranial volume; lh, left hemisphere; rh, right hemisphere.
Discussion
In the study, several possible factors associated with ICD were compared across the groups (anxiety, depression, age, age of onset, disease stage, and duration). Among these factors, only age of onset and age differed between the ICD and non-ICD group. Patients with ICD were younger and had lower age of disease onset than patients without ICD. Our results support previous research reporting association between PD-ICD and younger disease onset or younger age (2, 11–14).
Elevated self-reported impulsivity in the Barratt scale in PD-ICD in comparison with HC and PD-nonICD is consistent with previous research in this population (9, 10, 13, 73). In the UPPS-P scale, our results did not confirm our hypothesis regarding elevated Sensation seeking in PD-ICD. The absence of differences in Sensation seeking in PD-ICD might be surprising, when some other studies found associations of higher Sensation/Novelty seeking with ICD in this population (7–9, 13) [but see Evans et al. (12)]. The differences might be due to using various measurement tools with different concepts of Sensation seeking. Regarding the UPPS-P dimensions, we found only one study that targeted these dimensions in PD-ICD (7). This work reported increased impulsivity in Lack of premeditation, Urgency, and Sensation seeking in PD-ICD in contrast with HC and elevated Sensation seeking in contrast with PD-nonICD patients. However, only the shortened 16-item UPPS scale was used and the scale was answered by close relatives of the patients not the patients themselves, which may cause inconsistency of their results with our findings. PD-ICD patients in contrast with HC showed elevated score only in the Lack of perseverance. Perseverance is defined as “the ability to remain focused on a task that may be boring and/or difficult.” Lack of perseverance has been associated with poor resistance to distraction, harm avoidance, poor concentration on boring or difficult tasks, lower responsibility perception, and difficulties in dealing with frustration (74). Lower perseverance was reported in people with obsessive–compulsive symptoms, compulsive buying (74–77), bulimia (78), and self-injury behavior (79). One study found lower perseverance in PD patients with and without ICD (7). PD-ICD patients in our sample did not manifest higher levels of Urgency, which represents the aspect of emotional impulsivity. Taken together with no impairments in impulsive choice, it seems that impulsivity in PD-ICD is not elevated by strong emotions unlike, for example, borderline disorder, where impulsivity is more prominent in intense emotional state (6, 80, 81). PD-ICD patients are more likely to avoid harm and lower their effort in boring or difficult situations. They have impaired ability to maintain long-term goal and facing the obstacles; when facing difficulties, they rather abandon the goal. Even though UPPS-P questionnaire is the most up-to-date personality model of impulsivity, it is not used in studies conducted on PD-ICD populations. Future research with larger sample sizes studying PD-ICD in the context of UPPS-P impulsivity model would be beneficial.
In accordance with our hypothesis, PD-ICD patients in our study did not show impairments in impulsive action (waiting and stopping impulsivity). Results were also negative for the non-impulsive group. Previous studies of impulsive action in general PD population brought mixed results (38–44). There might be several factors responsive for the inconsistency in impulsive action among studies. Differences in results can be caused by cognitive impairments in patients' groups, heterogeneity in the patients' samples, and by differences in task designs. Patients in studies have different clinical characteristics such as different disease stage and duration, cognitive impairments, and different severity of anxiety and depression. Some of the studies included patients with cognitive impairments and elevated depression in their samples (38, 45). Presence of cognitive deficits and depression were linked to lower efficacy of inhibitory performance in impulsive action tasks (38, 82). Further, patients with older age and later disease stages have greater difficulties when inhibiting their responses (38). Thus, it is possible that increased impulsive action observed in some studies in the previous literature might have been influenced more by impaired cognitive functioning or psychomotor slowing associated with markedly increased depression, rather than with increased impulsivity itself.
We expected elevated choice impulsivity in PD-ICD in comparison with healthy controls, but our results did not support this expectation. None of the clinical groups differed from healthy controls on Delay discounting. Unimpaired performance of PD-ICD patients on Delay discounting is in contrast with two studies (9, 24), which found elevated discounting in PD patients with ICD in comparison with HC and non-impulsive PD patients. However, PD-ICD patients in these studies had either lower IQ or working memory deficits or were more depressed than non-impulsive patients. All of these factors were previously associated with greater discounting (83–86). Consistently with our findings, another study (28) reported similar delay discounting comparing PD-ICD and HC. The results of DD studies in PD-ICD population remain inconsistent. It is again possible that presence of cognitive impairments or elevated depression could influence performance in this task in previous studies. Future research should focus on relationship of depression, cognitive deficits, and behavioral measures of impulsivity in this population.
In the IGT in our study, patients with ICD performed surprisingly the best from all groups. PD-ICD as the only group improved their performance in the second part of the task and made significantly less risky choices. PD-ICD patients differed from PD-nonICD and HC in B deck card preference. B deck is a deck with low frequent but very high losses and high frequent gains. This deck is disadvantageous in long-term outcome. Therefore, according to the basic IGT assumption, the frequency of B deck choice declines during the task in a healthy population (67). This basic IGT assumption is being questioned, since the high preference of B deck choices observed in a healthy population in many studies suggests that it is rather the gain–loss frequency than the long-term outcome that influences decision-making in IGT in a healthy population (87). Our results suggest that PD-ICD patients, unlike HC and patients without ICD, are less sensitive to gain-loss frequency and more sensitive to high punishments. Previous studies brought evidence of impaired IGT performance in PD patients (ICD not specified) (16–23, 88) and in pathological gamblers with Parkinson (25). The difference between our study and that of Rossi et al. (25) is that we also included other forms of ICD beside pathological gambling. We recruited some patients with gambling history into our sample. It is possible that these patients with gambling history were more aware of potential risk because of their previous problems and they already developed some strategies, which helped them during the task. In contrast with previous studies, we used a prolonged protocol consisting of 200 trials. The differences between groups were more prominent in the second part of the tasks where PD-ICD patients chose the cards from the disadvantageous deck less frequently than in the first part. It seems that this prolonged version is more sensitive to describe learning from the consequences of the traditional 100 card version. We further analyzed not only the NET score but also the preference of particular packages. This four-deck approach seems to be useful, because it provides more information about decision strategies of participants.
Having in mind the age differences between groups of patients in our study, the role of age in the context of IGT results should be considered. PD-ICD in our study performed better than PD-nonICD, and they were also younger than PD-nonICD patients. Biars et al. (29) reported that increasing age negatively influenced IGT performance. Some studies found older adults to have greater tendency to less advantageous decisions in IGT than younger adults (89, 90). It is possible that older adults use different cognitive strategies and are not able to learn from the feedback as effectively as younger adults (89). On the other hand, studies reporting impaired performance in the PD or PD-ICD group in IGT found no significant age differences between compared groups (16, 18, 20–22, 25). Further investigation in this field would be beneficial.
As for cognitive functions, patients did not show any significant impairments except slower processing speed in the d2 test in PD non-ICD in contrast with HC. It may be surprising, because a decline of cognitive functions is linked to PD and cognitive impairment was previously find in patients with ICD (5). However, only patients with high MMSE scores were recruited, and therefore, those with potential cognitive problems were excluded from the testing. The slower processing speed of PD-nonICD patients in contrast to HC in d2 can be attributed to age, because the PD-nonICD group was the oldest one. Slower processing speed can also indicate attentional problems. It is possible that PD-nonICD patients are able to complete the task with similar accuracy as other experimental groups, but it costs them more effort; as a result, they need more time to process the same amount of items.
Although there were no impairments in PD-ICD on behavioral tasks, MRI scanning revealed structural differences between the three groups in the structures involved in inhibitory control and decision making. PD-ICD patients showed volume increase of ACC in comparison to PD patients and healthy controls. Our results correspond with previous research, which found increased thickness of ACC cortex in PD-ICD (48, 49, 51). Decreased functional connectivity of ACC with nucleus accumbens was previously associated with impulsive–compulsive behavior in PD (51). ACC is an important component of the reward system. It is involved in subjective evaluation of immediate and delayed rewards in delay discounting (91). Some researchers suggest that this structure is also important for conflict monitoring in situations with low predictability and high error rate; when the subject is required to adjust behavior after error response, it is needed for learning from the negative feedback (49, 92). Considering an increase in ACC volume together with the improvements in PD-ICD group during the second part of the IGT task, it suggests that PD-ICD patients were the most sensitive to the negative feedback. PD-ICD patients in our study showed an increase of volume and area in the right rostral middle frontal region in comparison with HC. Middle frontal region abnormalities in PD-ICD population were previously reported Biundo et al. (47) and Yoo et al. (50), who observed cortical thinning in PD-ICD. Increased activity of the right middle frontal gyrus was previously observed during impulsive action tasks (93). This region is associated with working memory, norm- or rule-related behavior, and making strategic decisions in social context (94, 95).
However, our results are not entirely consistent with previous research. One study of PD-ICD patients reported no structural differences between PD-ICD and PD-nonICD or HC (96). Recent research brought also evidence of structural differences in orbitofrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens in PD-ICD (47–49), but our group comparisons were negative regarding these regions. Differences in anatomical findings across the studies might be caused by different analysis approaches and further by the presence of cognitive impaired individuals in some of the studies, as well as by variability of ICD patients in behavioral manifestations of ICD, in disease duration and disease progression. Disease progression and symptom severity are very important factors. For example, PD-ICD patients in the study of Pellicano et al. (48), which observed more extensive structural changes than our study, were in more advanced stage and showed more severe disease symptoms than non-impulsive patients. On the other hand, the study of Ricciardi et al. (96), which did not find any structural differences, included patients in earlier stage of the disease and with shorter disease duration than our study. Some studies tested patients with mild cognitive impairment (47, 49). Most of the studies included patients with variable ICD symptoms often with more than one manifestation of ICD. We also included variable ICD sample into our study. It is possible that particular subtypes of impulsive behavior like gambling, hypersexuality, or binge eating differ from each other in behavioral impulsivity or in their neurobiological correlates.
Patients without ICD differed from healthy controls in the regions of inferior frontal gyrus, right nucleus accumbens, and left precentral gyrus. These findings also indicate that patients without ICD have some brain pathology in regions relevant for impulsivity. This corresponds with previous MRI research conducted on the general PD population (42, 97–100).
This study has several limitations. First of all, the research sample consisted of a small number of patients due to the inclusion of only patients with severe impulsive problems and avoiding patients with cognitive deficit. Even though the prevalence of ICD in the population is higher, we decided to include only patients with severe ICD, which has a truly observable effect on their day-to-day life (e.g., substantial financial losses due to gambling). We deliberately decided to exclude patients with subclinical or borderline behavioral problems, because PD patients often manifest with a variety of behavioral problems, but only problems that differ from premorbid level of everyday functioning should be considered as a disorder. Hence, the inclusion of borderline patients would affect the validity of the study.
Secondly there is heterogeneity in our patient sample—we included patients with various manifestations of ICD. Future research studies could separately analyze individual subtypes of PD-ICD. Another limitation is the age difference between the PD-ICD and PD non-ICD patients. This factor needs to be considered, and caution should be exercised in interpretation of group differences. Moreover, not all of our patients were able to successfully undergo MRI testing; therefore, the sample for MRI testing was smaller than the sample for behavioral testing. However, the statistical comparison did not find any differences in self-reported impulsivity and behavioral performance between the subgroup of patients who underwent the MRI assessment and the group of patients without MRI testing. The last factor that may have influenced the results was the presence of some patients with a history of ICD. It is possible that previous personal experience could influence the behavior of these patients during the testing.
Conclusion
The discrepancy between the presence of florid ICD signs, both in clinical follow-up and in self-reported impulsivity scales, and the comparable or even better performance of PD-ICD patients in rather well-established behavioral tasks is rather intriguing. This stalemate is further accentuated by significant structural differences in various neural nodes related to inhibitory control and reward processing in PD-ICD patients. Be it the lack of sensitivity in specific, limited ICD subtypes, excessive caution of ICD patients due to previous negative experience rendering simplistic tasks insufficient, or other, as of now unknown aspects, the issue of impulsivity in PD definitely warrants further research, ultimately to allow proactive prevention of this debilitating complication of PD.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Etická komise Masarykovy univerzity. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Contributions
PH, PF, PL, MS, MBal, MBar, and TK participated in designing the project and defining the aims and hypotheses. PF, RŠ, MBal, and MBar were responsible for patient recruitment and neurological examinations. PH and PL performed neuropsychological testing. PH and PF performed the data analysis and wrote the manuscript draft. All co-authors provided their comments to the manuscript draft.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We acknowledge the core facility MAFIL of CEITEC MU supported by the CzechBioImaging large RI project (LM2015062 funded by MEYS CR) for their support with obtaining scientific data presented in this paper.
Footnotes
Funding. This study was supported by the Ministry of Health of the Czech Republic grant 15-30062A and the EU H2020 Marie Skłodowska RISE project #691110 (MICROBRADAM).
References
- 1.Evans AH, Katzenschlager R, Paviour D, O'Sullivan JD, Appel S, Lawrence AD, et al. Punding in Parkinson's disease: Its relation to the dopamine dysregulation syndrome. Mov Disord. (2004) 19:397–405. 10.1002/mds.20045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Weintraub D, Koester J, Potenza MN, Siderowf AD, Stacy M, Voon V, et al. Impulse Control Disorders in Parkinson Disease. Arch Neurol. (2010) 67:589–95. 10.1001/archneurol.2010.65 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Gallagher DA, O'Sullivan SS, Evans AH, Lees AJ, Schrag A. Pathological gambling in Parkinson's disease: Risk factors and differences from dopamine dysregulation. An analysis of published case series. Mov Disord. (2007) 22:1757–63. 10.1002/mds.21611 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Evans AH, Strafella AP, Weintraub D, Stacy M. Impulsive and compulsive behaviors in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2009) 24:1561–70. 10.1002/mds.22505 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Leeman RF, Potenza MN. Impulse control disorders in Parkinson's disease: clinical characteristics and implications. Neuropsychiatry. (2011) 1:133–47. 10.2217/npy.11.11 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Linhartová P, Širuček J, Ejova A, Barteček R, Theiner P, Kašpárek T. Dimensions of impulsivity in healthy people, patients with borderline personality disorder, and patients with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Atten Disord. (2019). 10.1017/S0033291719001892. [Epub ahead of print]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Bayard S, Joly E, Ghisletta P, Rossignol A, Herades Y, Geny C, et al. A multidimensional approach to impulsivity in Parkinson's disease: measurement and structural invariance of the UPPS Impulsive Behaviour Scale. Psychol Med. (2016) 46:2931–1. 10.1017/S0033291716001586 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Bódi N, Kéri S, Nagy H, Moustafa A, Myers CE, Daw N, et al. Reward-learning and the novelty-seeking personality: a between-and within-subjects study of the effects of dopamine agonists on young parkinsons patients. Brain. (2009) 132:2385–95. 10.1093/brain/awp094 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Voon V, Sohr M, Lang AE, Potenza MN, Siderowf AD, Whetteckey J, et al. Impulse control disorders in parkinson disease: a multicenter case-control study. Ann Neurol. (2011) 69:986–96. 10.1002/ana.22356 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Bentivoglio AR, Baldonero E, Ricciardi L, De Nigris F, Daniele A. Neuropsychological features of patients with Parkinson's disease and impulse control disorders. Neurol Sci. (2013) 34:1207–13. 10.1007/s10072-012-1224-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Djamshidian A, Jha A, O'Sullivan SS, Silveira-Moriyama L, Jacobson C, Brown P, et al. Risk and learning in impulsive and nonimpulsive patients with Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2010) 25:2203–10. 10.1002/mds.23247 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Evans AH, Lawrence AD, Potts J, MacGregor L, Katzenschlager R, Shaw K, et al. Relationship between impulsive sensation seeking traits, smoking, alcohol and caffeine intake, and Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2006) 77:317–21. 10.1136/jnnp.2005.065417 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Voon V, Thomsen T, Miyasaki JM, de Souza M, Shafro A, Fox SH, et al. Factors associated with dopaminergic drug–related pathological gambling in Parkinson Disease. Arch Neurol. (2007) 64:212. 10.1001/archneur.64.2.212 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Weintraub D, Siderowf AD, Potenza MN, Goveas J, Morales KHD, Duda JE, et al. Dopamine agonist use is associated with impulse control disorders in Parkinson's Disease NIH public access. Arch Neurol. (2006) 63:969–73. 10.1001/archneur.63.7.969 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Brand M, Labudda K, Kalbe E. Decision-making impairments in patients with Parkinson's disease. Behav Neurol. (2004) 15:77–85. 10.1155/2004/578354 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kobayakawa M, Tsuruya N, Kawamura M. Sensitivity to reward and punishment in Parkinson's disease: An analysis of behavioral patterns using a modified version of the Iowa gambling task. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2010) 16:453–7. 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2010.04.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Mimura M, Oeda R, Kawamura M. Impaired decision-making in Parkinson's disease. Park Relat Disord. (2006) 12:169–75. 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2005.12.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Pagonabarraga J, García-Sánchez C, Llebaria G, Pascual-Sedano B, Gironell A, Kulisevsky J. Controlled study of decision-making and cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2007) 22:1430–5. 10.1002/mds.21457 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Perretta JG, Pari G, Beninger RJ. Effects of Parkinson disease on two putative nondeclarative learning tasks: probabilistic classification and gambling. Cogn Behav Neurol. (2005) 18:185–92. 10.1097/01.wnn.0000187939.81541.1d [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Gescheidt T, Czekóová K, Urbánek T, Mareček R, Mikl M, Kubíková R, et al. Iowa Gambling Task in patients with early-onset Parkinson's disease: strategy analysis. Neurol Sci. (2012) 33:1329–35. 10.1007/s10072-012-1086-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Ibarretxe-Bilbao N, Junque C, Tolosa E, Marti MJ, Valldeoriola F, Bargallo N, et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of impaired decision-making and facial emotion recognition in early Parkinson's disease. Eur J Neurosci. (2009) 30:1162–71. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06892.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Mapelli D, Di Rosa E, Cavalletti M, Schiff S, Tamburin S. Decision and dopaminergic system: an ERPs study of Iowa gambling task in Parkinson's disease. Front Psychol. (2014) 5:684. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00684 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kobayakawa M, Tsuruya N, Kawamura M. Decision-making performance in Parkinson's disease correlates with lateral orbitofrontal volume. J Neurol Sci. (2017) 372:232–8. 10.1016/j.jns.2016.11.046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Housden CR, O'Sullivan SS, Joyce EM, Lees AJ, Roiser JP. Intact reward learning but elevated delay discounting in Parkinson's disease patients with impulsive-compulsive spectrum behaviors. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2010) 35:2155–64. 10.1038/npp.2010.84 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Rossi M, Gerschcovich ER, De Achaval D, Perez-Lloret S, Cerquetti D, Cammarota A, et al. Decision-making in Parkinson's disease patients with and without pathological gambling. Eur J Neurol. (2010) 17:97–102. 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2009.02792.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Djamshidian A, O'Sullivan SS, Sanotsky Y, Sharman S, Matviyenko Y, Foltynie T, et al. Decision making, impulsivity, and addictions: do Parkinson's disease patients jump to conclusions? Mov Disord. (2012) 27:1137–45. 10.1002/mds.25105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Voon V, Gao J, Brezing C, Symmonds M, Ekanayake V, Fernandez H, et al. Dopamine agonists and risk: impulse control disorders in Parkinson's; disease. Brain. (2011) 134:1438–46. 10.1093/brain/awr080 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Voon V, Reynolds B, Brezing C, Gallea C, Skaljic M, Ekanayake V, et al. Impulsive choice and response in dopamine agonist-related impulse control behaviors. Psychopharmacology. (2010) 207:645–59. 10.1007/s00213-009-1697-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Biars JW, Johnson NL, Nespeca M, Busch RM, Kubu CS, Floden DP. Iowa gambling task performance in parkinson disease patients with impulse control disorders. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2019) 34:310–8. 10.1093/arclin/acy036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Poletti M, Frosini D, Lucetti C, Dotto Del P, Ceravolo R, Bonuccelli U. Decision making in de novo Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2010) 25:1432–6. 10.1002/mds.23098 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Czernecki V, Pillon B, Houeto JL, Pochon JB, Levy R. Motivation, reward, and Parkinson's disease: Influence of dopatherapy. Neuropsychologia. (2002) 40:2257–67. 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00108-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Euteneuer F, Schaefer F, Stuermer R, Boucsein W, Timmermann L, Barbe MT, et al. Dissociation of decision-making under ambiguity and decision-making under risk in patients with Parkinson's disease: a neuropsychological and psychophysiological study. Neuropsychologia. (2009) 47:2882–90. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Martini A, Ellis SJ, Grange JA, Tamburin S, Dal Lago D, Vianello G, et al. Risky decision-making and affective features of impulse control disorders in Parkinson's disease. J Neural Transm. (2018) 125:131–43. 10.1007/s00702-017-1807-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Rao H, Mamikonyan E, Detre JA, Siderowf AD, Stern MB, Potenza MN, et al. Decreased ventral striatal activity with impulse control disorders in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2010) 25:1660–9. 10.1002/mds.23147 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Claassen DO, van den Wildenberg WPM, Ridderinkhof KR, Jessup CK, Harrison MB, Wooten GF, et al. The risky business of dopamine agonists in Parkinson disease and impulse control disorders. Behav Neurosci. (2011) 125:492–500. 10.1037/a0023795 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Leplow B, Sepke M, Schönfeld R, Pohl J, Oelsner H, Latzko L, et al. Impaired learning of punishments in Parkinson's disease with and without impulse control disorder. J Neural Transm. (2017) 124:217–25. 10.1007/s00702-016-1648-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Piray P, Zeighami Y, Bahrami F, Eissa AM, Hewedi DH, Moustafa AA. Impulse control disorders in Parkinson's disease are associated with dysfunction in stimulus valuation but not action valuation. J Neurosci. (2014) 34:7814–24. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4063-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Gauggel S, Rieger M, Feghoff T-A. Inhibition of ongoing responses in patients with Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2004) 75:539–44. 10.1136/jnnp.2003.016469 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Obeso I, Wilkinson L, Casabona E, Bringas ML, Álvarez M, Álvarez L, et al. Deficits in inhibitory control and conflict resolution on cognitive and motor tasks in Parkinson's disease. Exp Brain Res. (2011) 212:371–84. 10.1007/s00221-011-2736-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Obeso I, Wilkinson L, Jahanshahi M. Levodopa medication does not influence motor inhibition or conflict resolution in a conditional stop-signal task in Parkinson's disease. Exp Brain Res. (2011) 213:435–45. 10.1007/s00221-011-2793-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Tolleson C, Turchan M, van Wouwe N, Isaacs D, Phibbs F, Wylie S. Parkinson's Disease subtypes show distinct tradeoffs between response initiation and inhibition latencies. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2017) 23:665–74. 10.1017/S1355617717000467 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.O'Callaghan C, Naismith SL, Hodges JR, Lewis SJG, Hornberger M. Fronto-striatal atrophy correlates of inhibitory dysfunction in parkinson's disease versus behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia. Cortex. (2013) 49:1833–43. 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.12.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Claassen DO, van den Wildenberg WPM, Harrison MB, van Wouwe NC, Kanoff K, Neimat JS, et al. Proficient motor impulse control in Parkinson disease patients with impulsive and compulsive behaviors. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (2015) 129:19–25. 10.1016/j.pbb.2014.11.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Manza P, Schwartz G, Masson M, Kann S, Volkow ND, Li CR, et al. Levodopa improves response inhibition and enhances striatal activation in early-stage Parkinson's disease. Neurobiol Aging. (2018) 66:12–22. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.02.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Baglio F, Blasi V, Falini A, Farina E, Mantovani F, Olivotto F, et al. Functional brain changes in early Parkinson's disease during motor response and motor inhibition. Neurobiol Aging. (2011) 32:115–24. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.12.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Wylie SA, Claassen DO, Huizenga HM, Schewel KD, Ridderinkhof KR, Bashore TR, et al. Dopamine agonists and the suppression of impulsive motor actions in Parkinson's Disease. J Cogn Neurosci. (2012) 24:1709 10.1162/jocn_a_00241 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Biundo R, Weis L, Facchini S, Formento-Dojot P, Vallelunga A, Pilleri M, et al. Patterns of cortical thickness associated with impulse control disorders in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2015) 30:688–95. 10.1002/mds.26154 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Pellicano C, Niccolini F, Wu K, O'Sullivan SS, Lawrence AD, Lees AJ, et al. Morphometric changes in the reward system of Parkinson's disease patients with impulse control disorders. J Neurol. (2015) 262:2653–61. 10.1007/s00415-015-7892-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Tessitore A, Santangelo G, De Micco R, Vitale C, Giordano A, Raimo S, et al. Cortical thickness changes in patients with Parkinson's disease and impulse control disorders. Park Relat Disord. (2016) 24:119–25. 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2015.10.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Yoo HS, Yun HJ, Chung SJ, Sunwoo MK, Lee JM, Sohn YH, et al. Patterns of neuropsychological profile and cortical thinning in Parkinson's disease with punding. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0134468. 10.1371/journal.pone.0134468 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Hammes J, Theis H, Giehl K, Hoenig MC, Greuel A, Tittgemeyer M, et al. Dopamine metabolism of the nucleus accumbens and fronto-striatal connectivity modulate impulse control. Brain. (2019) 142:733–43. 10.1093/brain/awz007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Cerasa A, Salsone M, Nigro S, Chiriaco C, Donzuso G, Bosco D, et al. Cortical volume and folding abnormalities in Parkinson's disease patients with pathological gambling. Park Relat Disord. (2014) 20:1209–14. 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2014.09.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Kilford L, Lees AJ. Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson's disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (1992) 55:181–4. 10.1136/jnnp.55.3.181 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Sheehan D, Lecrubier Y. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. (1998) 59:22–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Folstein MF, Folstein SE, Mchugh PR. “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiat Res. (1975) 12:189–98. 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Hoehn MM, Yahr MD. Parkinsonism: onset, progression, and mortality. Neurology. (1967) 17:427. 10.1212/WNL.17.5.427 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J Clin Psychol. (1995) 51:768–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Whiteside SP, Lynam DR. The Five Factor Model and impulsivity: using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personal Individ Diff. (2001) 30:669–89. 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00064-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Cyders MA, Smith GT. Mood-based rash action and its components: positive and negative urgency. Pers Individ Dif . (2007) 43:839–50. 10.1016/j.paid.2007.02.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Linhartová P, Širuček J, Barteček R, Theiner P, Jerábková B, Daniela R. Czech versions of impulsivity self-report scales and their psychometric properties. Ces a Slov Psychiatr. (2017) 113:149–57. [Google Scholar]
- 61.Montgomery SA, Asberg M. A New depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. Br J Psychiatry. (1979) 134:382–9. 10.1192/bjp.134.4.382 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Zung WWK. A rating instrument for anxiety disorders. Psychosomatics. (1971) 12:371–9. 10.1016/S0033-3182(71)71479-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Reise SP, Moore TM, Sabb FW, Brown AK, London ED. The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11: reassessment of its structure in a community sample. Psychol Assess. (2013) 25:631–42. 10.1037/a0032161 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Steinberg L, Sharp C, Stanford MS, Tharp AT. New tricks for an old measure: the development of the barratt impulsiveness scale-brief (BIS-Brief). Psychol Assess. (2013) 25:216–26. 10.1037/t21455-000 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Madden G, Bickel WK, Jacobs EA. Discounting of delayed rewards in opioid-dependent outpatients: exponential or hyperbolic discounting functions? Effects on infusion rate on the discriminative effects of opioids view project. Artic Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. (1999) 7:284–93. 10.1037/1064-1297.7.3.284 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Myerson J, Green L, Warusawitharana M. Area under the curve as a measure of discounting. J Exp Anal Behav. (2001) 76:235–43. 10.1901/jeab.2001.76-235 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW. Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition. (1994) 50:7–15. 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Odum AL. Delay discounting: I'm a k, You're a k. J Exp Anal Bahav. (2011) 96:427–39. 10.1901/jeab.2011.96-423 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Wechsler D. WAIS-3: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale: Administration and Scoring Manual. New York, NY: Psychological Corporation; (1997). 10.1037/t49755-000 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Culbertson W, Zillmer E. Tower of London Drexel University. Toronto, ON: Multi-Health Systems; (2005). [Google Scholar]
- 71.Brickenkamp R, Schmidt-Atzert L, Liepmann D, Hoskovcová S CD. d2 Attention Test Revised. Prague: Hogrefe – Testcentrum; (2014). [Google Scholar]
- 72.Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, Albert M, Dieterich M, Haselgrove C, et al. Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron. (2002) 33:341–55. 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00569-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Isaias IU, Siri C, Cilia R, de Gaspari D, Pezzoli G, Antonini A. The relationship between impulsivity and impulse control disorders in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2008) 23:411–5. 10.1002/mds.21872 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Savvidou LG, Fagundo AB, Fernández-Aranda F, Granero R, Claes L, Mallorquí-Baqué N, et al. Is gambling disorder associated with impulsivity traits measured by the UPPS-P and is this association moderated by sex and age? Compr Psychiatry. (2017) 72:106–13. 10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.10.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Billieux J, Rochat L, Rebetez MML, Van der Linden M. Are all facets of impulsivity related to self-reported compulsive buying behavior? Pers Individ Dif . (2008) 44:1432–42. 10.1016/j.paid.2007.12.011 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Williams AD, Grisham JR. Impulsivity, emotion regulation, and mindful attentional focus in compulsive buying. Cognit Ther Res. (2012) 36:451–7. 10.1007/s10608-011-9384-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Zermatten A, Van der Linden M. Impulsivity in non-clinical persons with obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Pers Individ Dif . (2008) 44:1824–30. 10.1016/j.paid.2008.01.025 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Claes L, Vandereycken W, Vertommen H. Impulsivity-related traits in eating disorder patients. Pers Individ Dif . (2005) 39:739–49. 10.1016/j.paid.2005.02.022 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Riley EN, Combs JL, Jordan CE, Smith GT. Negative urgency and lack of perseverance: identification of differential pathways of onset and maintenance risk in the longitudinal prediction of nonsuicidal self-injury. Behav Ther. (2015) 46:439–48. 10.1016/j.beth.2015.03.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Cackowski S, Reitz A-C, Ende G, Kleindienst N, Bohus M, Schmahl C, et al. Impact of stress on different components of impulsivity in borderline personality disorder. Psychol Med. (2014) 44:3329–40. 10.1017/S0033291714000427 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Krause-Utz A, Cackowski S, Daffner S, Sobanski E, Plichta MM, Bohus M, et al. Delay discounting and response disinhibition under acute experimental stress in women with borderline personality disorder and adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychol Med. (2016) 46:3137–49. 10.1017/S0033291716001677 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Nigg JT. Annual research review: on the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. (2017) 58:361–83. 10.1111/jcpp.12675 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Imhoff S, Harris M, Weiser J, Reynolds B. Delay discounting by depressed and non-depressed adolescent smokers and non-smokers. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2014) 135:152–5. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.11.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Patros CHG, Sweeney K, Mahone EM, Mostofsky SH, Rosch KS. Greater delay discounting among girls, but not boys, with ADHD correlates with cognitive control. Child Neuropsychol. (2018) 24:1026–46. 10.1080/09297049.2017.1359525 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Pulcu E, Trotter PD, Thomas EJ, McFarquhar M, Juhasz G, Sahakian BJ, et al. Temporal discounting in major depressive disorder. Psychol Med. (2014) 44:1825–34. 10.1017/S0033291713002584 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Szuhany KL, MacKenzie D, Otto MW. The impact of depressed mood, working memory capacity, and priming on delay discounting. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2018) 60:37–41. 10.1016/j.jbtep.2018.03.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Lin C-H, Chiu Y-C, Lee P-L, Hsieh J-C. Is deck B a disadvantageous deck in the Iowa gambling task? Behav Brain Funct. (2007) 3:16. 10.1186/1744-9081-3-16 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Gescheidt T, Mareček R, Mikl M, Czekóová K, Urbánek T, Vaníček J, et al. Functional anatomy of outcome evaluation during Iowa Gambling Task performance in patients with Parkinson's disease: an fMRI study. Neurol Sci. (2013) 34:2159–66. 10.1007/s10072-013-1439-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Fein G, Mcgillivray S, Finn P. Older adults make less advantageous decisions than younger adults: cognitive and psychological correlates. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2007) 13:480–9. 10.1017/S135561770707052X [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Di Rosa E, Mapelli D, Arcara G, Amodio P, Tamburin S, Schiff S. Aging and risky decision-making: new ERP evidence from the Iowa gambling task. Neurosci Lett. (2017) 640:93–8. 10.1016/j.neulet.2017.01.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Kable JW, Glimcher PW. The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice. Nat Neurosci. (2007) 10:1625–33. 10.1038/nn2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Nieuwenhuis S, Yeung N, van den Wildenberg W, Ridderinkhof KR. Electrophysiological correlates of anterior cingulate function in a go/no-go task: Effects of response conflict and trial type frequency. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. (2003) 3:17–26. 10.3758/CABN.3.1.17 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Zheng D, Oka T, Bokura H, Yamaguchi S. The key locus of common response inhibition network for No-go and stop signals. J Cogn Neurosci. (2008) 20:1434–42. 10.1162/jocn.2008.20100 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Simmonds DJ, Pekar JJ, Mostofsky SH. Meta-analysis of Go/No-go tasks demonstrating that fMRI activation associated with response inhibition is task-dependent. Neuropsychologia. (2008) 46:224–32. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.07.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Steinbeis N, Bernhardt BC, Singer T. Impulse control and underlying functions of the left DLPFC mediate age-related and age-independent individual differences in strategic social behavior. Neuron. (2012) 73:1040–51. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.12.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Ricciardi L, Lambert C, De Micco R, Morgante F, Edwards M. Can we predict development of impulsive-compulsive behaviours in Parkinson's disease? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2018) 89:476–81. 10.1136/jnnp-2017-317007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Gerrits NJHM, van Loenhoud AC, van den Berg SF, Berendse HW, Foncke EMJ, Klein M, et al. Cortical thickness, surface area and subcortical volume differentially contribute to cognitive heterogeneity in Parkinson's Disease. PLoS ONE. (2016) 11:e0148852. 10.1371/journal.pone.0148852 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Guimarães RP, Santos MCA, Dagher A, Campos LS, Azevedo P, Piovesana LG, et al. Pattern of reduced functional connectivity and structural abnormalities in Parkinson's disease: an exploratory study. Front Neurol. (2017) 7:243. 10.3389/fneur.2016.00243 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Pan PL, Song W, Shang HF. Voxel-wise meta-analysis of gray matter abnormalities in idiopathic Parkinson's disease. Eur J Neurol. (2012) 19:199–206. 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2011.03474.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Filip P, Linhartová P, Hlavatá P, Šumec R, BalᎠM, Bareš M, et al. Disruption of multiple distinctive neural networks associated with impulse control disorder in Parkinson's Disease. Front Hum Neurosci. (2018) 12:462. 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00462 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.