Abstract
Accurate sensory processing during movement requires the animal to distinguish between external (exafferent) and self-generated (reafferent) stimuli to maintain sensitivity to biologically relevant cues. The lateral line system in fishes is a mechanosensory organ that experiences reafferent sensory feedback, via detection of fluid motion relative to the body generated during behaviors such as swimming. For the first time in larval zebrafish (Danio rerio), we employed simultaneous recordings of lateral line and motor activity to reveal the activity of afferent neurons arising from endogenous feedback from hindbrain efferent neurons during locomotion. Frequency of spontaneous spiking in posterior lateral line afferent neurons decreased during motor activity and was absent for more than half of swimming trials. Targeted photoablation of efferent neurons abolished the afferent inhibition that was correlated to swimming, indicating that inhibitory efferent neurons are necessary for modulating lateral line sensitivity during locomotion. We monitored calcium activity with Tg(elav13:GCaMP6s) fish and found synchronous activity between putative cholinergic efferent neurons and motor neurons. We examined correlates of motor activity to determine which may best predict the attenuation of afferent activity and therefore what components of the motor signal are translated through the corollary discharge. Swim duration was most strongly correlated to the change in afferent spike frequency. Attenuated spike frequency persisted past the end of the fictive swim bout, suggesting that corollary discharge also affects the glide phase of burst and glide locomotion. The duration of the glide in which spike frequency was attenuated increased with swim duration but decreased with motor frequency. Our results detail a neuromodulatory mechanism in larval zebrafish that adaptively filters self-generated flow stimuli during both the active and passive phases of locomotion.
NEW & NOTEWORTHY For the first time in vivo, we quantify the endogenous effect of efferent activity on afferent gain control in a vertebrate hair cell system during and after locomotion. We believe that this pervasive effect has been underestimated when afferent activity of octavolateralis systems is characterized in the current literature. We further identify a refractory period out of phase with efferent control and place this gain mechanism in the context of gliding behavior of freely moving animals.
Keywords: cholinergic, corollary discharge, efference copy, hair cells, locomotion
INTRODUCTION
Sensory systems inform animals about their environment, gathering information essential for directing behavior. Motor behaviors create sensory artifacts that the animal must then disentangle from relevant cues from their environment. In this way, it is critical that sensory systems need to be able to differentiate between external stimuli (exafference) and self-generated stimuli (reafference) to maintain an accurate perception of the sensory landscape (Sperry 1950; von Holst and Mittelstaedt 1950). For example, animals that rely on sound production for communication, such as the cricket, release self-generated acoustic stimuli that can saturate their auditory sensors and cause temporary deafness (Crapse and Sommer 2008; Poulet and Hedwig 2006). A corollary discharge of the sound-producing signal is relayed to the sensory receptor to modulate or interrupt the processing of the reafferent stimulus. By eliminating any ambiguity concerning the origin of a stimulus, animals can better interpret exafferent sensory information by avoiding reafferent sensory overload.
Fishes sense perturbations in the fluid environment through the lateral line, a sensory organ on the body surface that translates fluid motion relative to the body into neural signals essential for navigation (Olszewski, et al. 2012; Oteiza et al. 2017; Suli et al. 2012), predator avoidance, prey capture (McHenry et al. 2009; Stewart et al. 2013), and schooling (Mekdara et al. 2018). However, since water motions are also self-generated by behaviors such as swimming (Ayali et al. 2009; Mensinger et al. 2019; Palmer et al. 2003), respiration (Montgomery et al. 1996; Montgomery and Bodznick 1994; Palmer et al. 2003), and feeding (Palmer et al. 2005), fishes should possess a mechanism to discriminate such self-generated signals from environmental signals.
The origin of this discriminatory capacity is thought to lie in the neural architecture of the lateral line circuit (Russell and Roberts 1972). The neuromast receptors that compose the lateral line comprise mechanosensory hair cells innervated by afferent and efferent neurons (Dow et al. 2018; Harris et al. 1970). Afferent neurons (hereafter referred to as afferents) transmit evoked action potentials in response to water motion that deflects the hair cell cilia, resulting in membrane depolarization, rapid glutamate exocytosis, and increased afferent spike rates (Obholzer et al. 2008). Afferents also transmit spontaneous action potentials (spikes) in the absence of mechanical stimuli due to glutamate leakage from the hair cell basal membrane onto their postsynaptic terminals (Keen and Hudspeth 2006; Li et al. 2009; Liao and Haehnel 2012), which is an intrinsic property shown to contribute to maintaining sensitivity (Kiang et al. 1965; Manley and Robertson 1976).
Efferent neuron projections from the brain have long been hypothesized to be a principle mechanism for filtering sensory reafference via efferent copy or corollary discharge (Sperry 1950; von Holst and Mittelstaedt 1950). Cholinergic efferent neurons (herein referred to as efferents), projecting from the hindbrain, discharge parallel to motor commands that attenuate lateral line activity (Flock and Russell 1973; Russell 1971; Russell and Roberts 1972), as well as activity in homologous octavolateralis systems (i.e., electrosensitive ampullary organ, the inner ear, vestibular system; Bell 1981, 2001; Brichta and Goldberg 2000; Crapse and Sommer 2008; Montgomery 1984; Weeg et al. 2005). The corollary discharge can function to maintain sensitivity to stimulus frequencies during the production of motor commands (Bell 1989; Bodznick et al. 1999; Crapse and Sommer 2008; Poulet and Hedwig 2006). A history of work has been devoted to untangling this critical aspect of sensory systems, often by employing in vitro preparations with chemical or electrical stimulation that likely did not match endogenous levels (Flock and Russell 1973; Montgomery 1984; Roberts and Russell 1972; Russell 1971; Russell and Roberts 1972; Tricas and Highstein 1990; Weeg et al. 2005). We expand on this foundational work by investigating the effects of the corollary discharge on lateral line activity in vivo, where native levels of feedback are preserved, and by defining which parameters of the motor command contribute to lateral line inhibition.
For the first time, we investigate how sensory information from the posterior lateral line of larval zebrafish (Danio rerio) is modulated by hindbrain efferents during swimming. Using optogenetic and electrophysiological approaches, we verified the parallel activity of efferents and motor neurons, and quantified changes in spontaneous afferent activity during swimming motor commands. To determine the functional role of efferents during swimming, we ablated hindbrain efferents to look for changes in the afferent activity patterns. We next investigated how afferent modulation varies across swim intensities and durations. Last, we show that the modulatory effect persists after locomotion and estimated its influence on sensing during the glide phase of intermittent swimming behaviors. Our in vivo experimental paradigm is poised to provide a better understanding of sensory feedback during locomotion.
METHODS
Animals.
Experiments were performed on 4- to 7-day postfertilization (dpf) zebrafish (D. rerio), which were raised in 10% Hanks’ solution (137 mM NaCl, 5.4 mM KCl, 0.25 mM Na2HPO4, 0.44 mM KH2PO4, 1.3 mM CaCl2, 1.0 mM MgSO4, 4.2 mM NaHCO4; pH 7.3) at 27°C according to protocols approved by the University of Florida Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. All larvae displayed a range of swim behaviors (Masino and Fetcho 2005). Blood flow was monitored continuously throughout each experiment as a metric for animal health.
Calcium imaging.
We first verified the cholinergic identity of efferent neurons by backfilling them with tetramethylrhodamine isothiocynate (TRITC; 3 kDa; Molecular Probes, Eugene, OR) in Islet:GFP larvae, as described previously (Smith et al. 2014). To image the activity of hindbrain cholinergic efferent neurons, we electroporated TRITC bilaterally into the efferent axons running along the lateral line nerve at the site of the cleithrum in 20 Tg(HUC:GCaMP6s) larvae (Zebrafish International Resource Center, Eugene, OR) embedded in agar. Motor neurons were labeled by electroporating TRITC into the axial musculature. Fish were then gently freed from the agar and allowed to recover for 24 h. Fish were then remounted in agar dorsal surface down and imaged on a Leica SP5 confocal microscope (Leica Microsystems, Wetzlar, Germany). Scanning was performed at 0.39–0.78 Hz, depending on the line speed and scan area. Calcium activity was quantified by a region of interest within identified TRITC-labeled cells in ImageJ (v1.48; National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD). We monitored calcium changes in motor neuron activity to indicate swimming. In cases where individual motor neurons did not show calcium changes, we analyze average calcium signal in the spinal cord as a proxy for overall motor activity. Background fluorescence was subtracted by a rolling-ball method implemented in ImageJ, and fluorescence was converted to F/Fo. Baseline fluorescence was manually subtracted using Clampfit software (Molecular Devices, Sunnyvale, CA).
Electrophysiology.
Before recordings, larvae were paralyzed by immersion in 10 µL of 1 mg/mL α-bungarotoxin (Reptile World Serpentarium, St. Cloud, FL) in 10% Hanks’ solution. Although α-bungarotoxin is an antagonist of the nicotinic acetylcholine receptor (nAChR) α9-subunit in mammalian cochlear hair cells, its effects have been shown to be reversible in Xenopus embryos after a 10-min washout (Elgoyhen et al. 1994). Once paralyzed, larvae were then transferred into extracellular solution (134 mM NaCl, 2.9 mM KCl, 1.2 mM MgCl2, 2.1 mM CaCl2, 10 mM glucose, 10 mM HEPES buffer; pH 7.8, adjusted with NaOH) and pinned with etched tungsten pins through their dorsal notochord into a Sylgard-bottom dish.
Multiunit extracellular recordings of the posterior lateral line afferent ganglion were made in wild-type ABTü fish (n = 29). Electrodes (30-µm tip diameter) were pulled from borosilicate glass (model G150F-3, 0.86-mm inner diameter, 1.50-mm outer diameter; Warner Instruments, Hamden, CT) on a model P-97 Flaming/Brown micropipette puller (Sutter Instruments, Novato, CA). The recording pipette was filled with extracellular solution and brought into contact with afferent somata by applying −50 mmHg pressure (pneumatic transducer tester, model DPM1B; Fluke Biomedical Instruments, Everett, WA). Negative pressure was released to atmospheric (0 mmHg) once a stable recording was achieved. This approach allowed us to record the activity of multiple posterior lateral line afferents and increase our chances of observing any effect during motor activity.
Along with afferent recordings, simultaneous extracellular ventral root (VR) recordings were made through the skin (Masino and Fetcho 2005) between myotomes 6 and 10, where myotome 1 is the anterior-most muscle block. VR pipettes were beveled and polished to a >60-µm tip diameter using a MF-830 microforge (Narishige International, Amityville, NY). VR pipettes were held at −100 mmHg during recordings. Spontaneous swimming activity was recorded simultaneously with evoked swimming commands, which were initiated by light or an electrical pulse (60 V for 20 ms; model DS2A-mk.II constant voltage isolated stimulator; Digitimer Ltd., Welwyn Garden, UK). All recordings were sampled at 20 kHz and amplified with a gain of 1,000 in Axoclamp 700B, digitized with Digidata 1440A, and saved in pClamp10 (Molecular Devices).
All recordings were analyzed in MATLAB (vR2016b) using custom-written scripts. Both spontaneous afferent spikes and swimming motor activity were identified by using a combination of spike parameters such as threshold, minimum duration (0.01 ms), and minimum interspike interval (ISI; 0.001 ms). Motor bursts within a swim bout were then defined by any activity that had a minimum of two spikes within 0.1 ms of each other and lasted a minimum of 5 ms. All swim bouts that were selected for analysis had a minimum of three bursts with a maximum interburst interval of 200 ms. We observed 2,336 total swim bouts, of which 45.8% were preceded by intervals of inactivity in the afferent neurons. These periods of inactivity made it challenging to interpret changes in afferent activity, so we restricted the data set to only include swim bouts that were preceded by a minimum of one afferent spike (n = 1,263 swim bouts). This period of monitored afferent activity was set to match the time interval of the subsequent swim bout. For the rest of this report, this period is termed “pre-swim,” with the same time period following a swim bout termed “post-swim.” The interval between the end of a swim bout and the first afferent spike is termed the “refractory period.”
Efferent neuron ablations.
ABTü wild-type fish (n = 8) were mounted on their sides in 1.6% low-melting-point agarose and covered in 0.02% MS-222 in Hanks’ solution. The somata of efferent neurons were backfilled as previously by TRITC (3 kDa) or fluorescein isothiocyanate (FITC; 3–5 kDa; Sigma-Aldrich, Darmstadt, Germany) and allowed to recover for 2–4 h. The near-ultraviolet laser was focused at a depth corresponding to the maximum intensity of each neuron’s soma, to ensure we were targeting its center, on a Leica SP5 TSM laser scanning confocal microscope. Labeled efferents were imaged at 532 nm, and we applied the FRAP Wizard tool in Leica application software to target individual cells. We ablated target cells with a 30-s exposure to the near-ultraviolet laser line (458 nm), and successful targeting was typically confirmed by quenching of the backfilled dye. This method has been successfully applied and validated in similar systems (Liu and Fetcho 1999; Soustelle et al. 2008). Fish were then freed from agar and were allowed to swim freely and recover overnight. Electrophysiological recordings were performed to simultaneously record afferent activity and motor activity in ablated animals. If swimming has no impact on spike rate, then the null hypothesis is that the slope of the line of best fit forced through zero will not be significantly different from unity. Conversely, a departure from this slope indicates that swimming has a significant impact on spike rates: a higher slope corresponds to increased spike rates, and lower slope corresponds to suppressed spike rates.
Behavioral test.
ABTü wild-type fish (4–7 dpf, n = 50) were housed in a 400-mL beaker in 200 mL of Hanks’ solution. Intermittent swimming behavior of larvae was recorded from above using a high-speed video camera (200 frames/s, 512 × 512 pixel resolution, 1,600-ms exposure time; Phantom Miro M310; Vision Research Inc., Wayne, NJ) and bottom lit by a light-emitting diode (LED) array (Ikan Corp., Houston, TX) through a diffuser. To prevent microcurrents arising from thermal gradients caused by the LED lighting (which we found to prevent accurate measurements of the glide phase), we introduced a fan to circulate air and offset the heat. Video capture was triggered to record the swim and the glide of individual larvae from initiation of the tail beat until lateral movement ceased. Videos (n = 112) were then processed using custom-written MATLAB programs to track the body kinematics of larvae, frame by frame, and quantify tail-beat frequency and swim duration. The glide period was defined as the time from the end of the tail beat to the end of passive forward movement.
Analysis.
All statistics were performed using custom-written models in the R language (vR2016b; R Development Core Team) using packages car, visreg, reshape2, plyr, dplyr, ggplot2, gridExtra, minpack.lm, nlstools, investr, and cowplot (Auguie 2017; Baty et al. 2015; Breheny and Burchett 2017; Elzhov et al. 2016; Fox and Weisberg 2011; Greenwell and Schubert Kabban 2014; Wickham 2007, 2011, 2016; Wickham et al. 2019; Wilke 2019). We were able to measure changes in calcium activity in 9 of 10 efferents, and these neurons were subsequently analyzed. We analyzed the correlation between the time courses of efferent neurons and motor neuron calcium activity using cross-correlations of the respective time series. Pre-swim, swim, and post-swim spike rate were calculated by taking the number of spikes within the respective period over its duration. Swim frequency was calculated by taking the number of bursts within a swim bout over the duration of the swim bout, and duty cycle was calculated by taking the sum of the swim burst durations over the swim bout’s total duration. Relative spike rate was calculated by taking the swim spike rate over the pre-swim spike rate. All variables were averaged by individual (n = 29). The precision of estimates for each individual is partly a function of the number of swims, so we analyzed variable relationships using weighted regressions, with individual weights equal to the square root of the number of swims. We log transformed variables in which the mean and the variance were correlated. To quantify the inhibition of the afferent spike frequency during swimming, we tested for a significant difference in afferent spike frequency during swimming compared with nonswimming periods using a paired-sample Student’s t test.
Differences in afferent spike rates across the various periods of interest (pre-swim, swim, and post-swim) were tested by two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA), followed by Tukey’s post hoc test to detect significant differences in spike rates between swim periods or treatments. Linear models were used to detect relationships between spike rate during swimming and other independent variables (e.g., spike rate pre-swim and motor parameters). To quantitatively define the refractory period, we restricted the data to individuals that had a minimum of 20 swims and sampled spike times within 500 ms following the termination of the swim. We used a modified logistic function to model asymmetrical data from parameters of post-swim spike rates to predict the duration of the refractory period, according to the equation
where the response y at time t is due to exponential growth with constant k until a maximum ymax is reached at time tmax, after which it grows as μ (Werker and Jaggard 1997). The model was bootstrap simulated for 100 iterations.
We used behavioral data to quantify the relationships between tail-beat frequency and swim duration to glide duration, as described above. We then calculated the expected refractory period duration of the behavioral motor parameters from the line equations produced by linear models of the ventral root motor parameters. With the expected refractory period and respective glide duration, we calculated the proportion of time the refractory period overlapped the glide phase and quantified the relationship between tail-beat frequency and swim duration to that proportion. Data are means ± SE. Statistical significance is reported at α = 0.05.
RESULTS
We first quantified the strength of the association between efferent neuron activity and motor activity. Our optogenetic approach confirmed that efferent neurons on both left and right sides of the hindbrain are active during swimming. Cross-correlation coefficients of efferent neuron (n = 9) and motor activity (n = 6) time profiles ranged from 0.44 to 0.95, with a mean of 0.66. In one example of an individual with four neurons labeled on each side, the correlation coefficient was 0.89 (Fig. 1). We only observed strong calcium activity in efferent neurons during swimming. Therefore, we performed ventral motor root recordings to signal the timing of efferent activity and to dissect the components of the motor signal that are transmitted to the lateral line during the corollary discharge.
Spontaneous afferent activity was reduced during swimming but was not fully inhibited in all cases (Fig. 2). The distribution of spike rates was shifted toward smaller values, which results in a significant decrease in afferent spike rate from when the larvae were inactive (8.93 ± 0.2 Hz) compared with when they were swimming (3.81 ± 0.2 Hz; P < 0.001, t = −21.72, df = 1.263; n = 1,263; Fig. 2D). We examined spike rates during swimming relative to the pre-swim period to examine patterns of relative inhibition. In a majority of swim bouts, there was a complete absence of afferent activity (relative spike rate = 0, n = 620/1,263, 50.7%), but there were also instances of partial decrease (relative spike rate between 0 and 1, n = 267/1,263, 21.1%), no decrease (relative spike rate = 1, n = 240/1,263, 19.0%), and an increase in spike rate (relative spike rate > 1, n = 116/1,263, 9.2%; Fig. 3, A and B). Discrete peaks in relative spike rates arise due to the integer number of spikes during swimming, often due to multiples of one.
To more closely probe the dynamics of afferent activity, we examined how spike rate varies before, during, and after the swim. Swimming spike rates were lower than in the immediate pre-swim period (8.94 ± 0.2 Hz, relative decrease 57%) and the post-swim period (5.34 ± 0.2 Hz, relative decrease 40%). Post-swim spike rate was itself lower than the pre-swim spike rate (Tukey’s post hoc tests across groups, P < 0.001). Spike rates preceding a complete decrease (8.45 ± 0.2 Hz), no decrease (6.94 ± 0.3 Hz), or an increase in spike rate (7.15 ± 0.5 Hz) were significantly lower than spike rates preceding partial inhibition (12.64 ± 0.4 Hz; F3,1262 = 50.03, P < 0.001; Fig. 3C).
We expected that ablating the efferent neurons would diminish or abolish the swim-associated inhibition of spike rate. Following efferent ablation, we observed some decrease in afferent spike rates, but to a lesser extent than in non-ablated fish, which was not statistically discriminated from pre-swim spike rates of either group (Tukey’s post hoc test; ablated, pre-swim: 8.92 ± 0.268 Hz; ablated, swimming: 5.65 ± 0.3 Hz; Fig. 4B). Alternatively, we can examine the correlation between nonswimming and swimming spike rates for evidence of the effects of suppression. If swimming has no impact on spike rates, then the line of best fit will be unity because, on average, swim intervals are equally likely to be higher or lower than nonswimming intervals. Average spike rates during and before a swim were positively correlated in control fish (r2 = 0.61, F1,35 = 54.48, P < 0.001), and the slope of the line indicates a fractional suppression of 56% that is significantly less than unity [slope 0.44, confidence interval (CI) = 0.32–0.57; Fig. 4C]. Conversely, in ablated fish, spike rates during swimming are not distinguishable from unity (slope 1.12, CI = 0.78–1.46), indicating that spike rates during swimming intervals differ from those during nonswimming intervals no differently than chance.
We sought to determine what aspects of the age-dependent motor pattern may contribute to the corollary discharge transmitted to the lateral line. Across ages, the average swim frequency, duty cycle, and swim duration were 27.01 ± 6.0 Hz, 0.101 ± 0.002, and 280 ± 6.0 ms, respectively. Swim frequency and duty cycle both increased with age, whereas swim duration decreased with age (r2 = 0.378, F2,27 = 16.41, P < 0.001; r2 = 0.437, F2,27 = 20.93, P < 0.001; and r2 = 0.166, F2,27 = 5.377, P = 0.028, respectively; Fig. 5, A–C). Relative changes in spike rate during swimming did not change with age (r2 = 0.047, F2,27 = 1.331, P = 0.259). Swim frequency and duty cycle were not correlated to changes in spike rate (r2 = 0.099, F2,26 = 1.431, P = 0.231 and r2 = 0.047, F2,26 = 0.645, P = 0.932, respectively; Fig. 6, A and B), but the spike rate was predictably lower for longer swim durations (r2 = 0.186, F2,26 = 2.971, P = 0.045; Fig. 6C). Mixed effect models revealed no interaction between frequency, duty cycle, and duration with individuals.
We characterized the reduction of afferent spike rate following a swim to define the refractory period and its contribution to post-swim spike dynamics. In control treatments, the time interval for post-swim spike rate to return to spontaneous levels was similar to the timing of when the first spike would be predicted to occur after a swim when a nonlinear least-squares model was used with a logistic function fit to spike rate (Fig. 7, A and Bi). The same model indicated that the post-swim was relatively unchanged in ablation treatments. The density estimates of the time of the first spike after the swim was log-normally distributed in control treatments (Fig. 7Bii). In contrast, the time of the first spike in the ablation treatments could not be determined, resulting in a uniform distribution (Fig. 7Bii). The mean time to the first post-swim spike (0.149 ± 0.004 s) was significantly later than the midpoint of the best-fit equation (tmid; CI = 0.105–0.119 s) and within 95% confidence of the asymptotic spike rate (CI = 0.092–0.156 s).
The mean value for the time to the first post-swim spike occurred within the confidence interval of 95% of the asymptotic spike rate, indicating that the timing of the first post-swim spike relative to the termination of the swim is a reliable predictor of refractory period duration (Fig. 7, Bi and Bii). Spike rate following ablation did not exhibit a quantifiable refractory period. Because the first post-swim spike time appears to coincide with the population-averaged refractory period, we use it to explore post-swim dynamics in control larvae. Refractory period duration was negatively correlated to the swimming frequency (r2 = 0.296, F2,26 = 5.467, P = 0.010; Fig. 7Ci), was not correlated with duty cycle (r2 = 0.088, F2,26 = 1.255, P = 0.998), and was positively correlated with swim duration (r2 = 0.315, F2,26 = 5.97, P = 0.007; Fig. 7Cii). Refractory period duration also increased as the level of inhibition increased (r2 = 0.391, F2,26 = 8.335, P = 0.001).
We quantified the swimming kinematics and glide duration of freely moving larvae to examine potential relationships between motor and sensory properties of the swim. Although refractory period duration was dependent on swimming frequency and duration, glide duration (0.49 ± 0.02 s) was independent of both tail-beat frequency (r2 < 0.001, F1,94 = 0.014, P = 0.905; Fig. 8B) and swim duration (r2 = 0.015, F1,109 = 1.69, P = 0.196; Fig. 8C). Consequently, the proportion of the glide affected by the refractory period was expected to decrease with faster tail-beat frequencies (r2 = 0.25, F1,94 = 31.35, P < 0.001; Fig. 8D) and increased with longer swim durations (r2 = 0.05, F1,109 = 6.09, P = 0.015; Fig. 8E). On average, the modeled refractory period accounts for the first 30.4% of the glide phase.
DISCUSSION
When animals move, their sensory receptors should, in theory, respond to a stimulus regardless of whether it is generated by the external environment or from the animal’s own movements. These movements can create sensory overload during locomotion and potentially mask stimuli important for survival, such as those generated by predators or prey. One solution would be for the animal to adjust its receptor sensitivity in anticipation of moving the body platform in which the sensory structures are embedded (Crapse and Sommer 2008; Sperry 1950; von Holst and Mittelstaedt 1950).
The spontaneous activity of zebrafish lateral line afferent neurons may be related to the maintenance of sensitivity. In other sensory systems, the degree of spontaneous firing is positively correlated with sensitivity to external stimuli (Douglass et al. 1993; Kiang et al. 1965; Manley and Robertson 1976; Yu et al. 2004). In this study we describe the properties of a circuit that may regulate lateral line sensitivity during swimming motor commands (Feitl et al. 2010; Mensinger et al. 2019). We found that during 70% of swims, there was a reduction in spontaneous afferent activity. This highlights the importance of recording both afferent and motor activity together. Otherwise, spontaneous afferent activity will be underestimated due to the influence of swim-correlated inhibitory efferent activity (Dawkins and Sewell 2004; Harris and Milne 1966; Haehnel-Taguchi et al. 2014; Liao and Haehnel 2012; Song et al. 2018; Trapani and Nicolson 2011). This is particularly problematic when one is deciphering the underlying dynamics of afferents; intermittent swims can explain apparent state transition in afferent spike intervals (Song et al. 2018). The variability in spike rate response between afferent neurons (Haehnel-Taguchi et al. 2014; Levi et al. 2015) may also be influenced by efferent modulation. Additionally, we show that spike rate modulation is not limited to the swim period, but exhibits a nonlinear return of pre-swim spike rates after the swim ends. Characterizations of afferent activity should therefore also account for the delayed return of intrinsic spike properties after a swim.
Cholinergic efferents exert modulatory effects on the activity of the lateral line and other octavolateralis systems, such as the vestibular and auditory system. This has been demonstrated by galvanic stimulation to efferents, bath-applied agonists, and genetic manipulation, all of which lead to a decrease in afferent activity (Dawkins et al. 2005; Flock and Russell 1973; Russell 1971; Russell and Roberts 1972; Sewell and Starr 1991; Vetter et al. 2007). These methods do not specifically target the efferent system or capture the intrinsic efferent activity. We note that although exogenous manipulations can elucidate the general function of physiological processes, they may be inadequate when tasked with revealing the nuanced in vivo dynamics inherent in endogenous systems. Whereas traditional methods can document the general effect of the efferent system, they struggle to reveal its function in a natural context, where native circuit architecture and neurotransmitter levels are preserved. Using optogenetics, we verified that cholinergic hindbrain efferents are synchronously active with spinal motor neurons during swimming (Bricaud et al. 2001; Chagnaud et al. 2015). We targeted and ablated these efferents and found that the magnitude of suppression was greatly decreased compared with controls. Residual suppression was expected because our method could not discriminate whether all efferents in the population were ablated. We found that efferents are necessary to inhibit afferent activity because when ablated, there was no inhibition of afferent activity. However, efferents are not sufficient in compensating for the physiological heterogeneity of afferent neurons that contributes to variation in the level of inhibition. This is because even when efferents were intact, afferent activity was not always completely absent during swimming.
To survive, many animals must move across a wide range of speeds. In many aquatic animals, an increased swimming frequency proportionally reduces afferent firing rate, likely due to the input of efferent neurons conveying this frequency information from active motor units (Chagnaud et al. 2015; Flock and Russell 1973; Hänzi et al. 2015; Roberts and Russell 1972; Russell 1971; Russell and Roberts 1972). In zebrafish, these speeds are achieved by increases in tail-beat frequency (Li et al. 2012), which are generated in a tonotopic manner by shifts in activity by distinct groups of interneurons and motor neurons in the spinal cord (McLean et al. 2007; McLean and Fetcho 2009; Mendell 2005). In paralyzed preparations, the tail-beat frequency is reflected in motor burst frequency (McLean et al. 2008). We examined variables of the motor command to better understand which ones might be copied by the efferent neurons. If motor burst frequency is the same as tail-beat frequency, which is the parameter commonly used to measure swim speed in zebrafish (Buss and Drapeau 2001; Gabriel, et al. 2011; Liao and Fetcho 2008; McLean et al. 2008; Severi et al. 2014), then we would expect to see a relationship between motor burst frequency and afferent spike rate. However, we found no changes in afferent spike rate across swimming frequencies.
As larval zebrafish mature, swimming and other motor behaviors can change (McLean et al. 2008; Müller and van Leeuwen 2004). In our hands, we found that average swim frequency increases with age, whereas swim duration decreases with age. We expected these age-related motor differences to be reflected in efferent modulation but found no significant correlations. Despite the changes in motor commands with age the inhibitory effect remains constant. This suggests that the efferent system acts as a corollary discharge with limited temporal information from higher order motor units rather than an efference copy that encodes the timing from local motor units (Straka et al. 2018). We investigated using a limited age range, but we suggest expanding to more mature fish to determine if the relationship between motor command and inhibition changes with development.
Swim duration had the highest correlation to afferent spike rate. Longer swim durations resulted in lower afferent spike frequencies, suggesting that information is filtered at the periphery during extended swim bouts. Swim duration reflects an underlying mechanism of locomotor drive that corresponds to the strength of efferent inhibition. Our findings are consistent with auditory efferents that carry corollary discharges of vocalization motor commands in midshipman fish (Porichthys notatus). Auditory efferents have been shown to be adapted for the transmission of call duration information (Chagnaud et al. 2011; Chagnaud and Bass 2013; Weeg et al. 2005). In midshipman, there is evidence that the corollary discharge originates in the pre-pacemaker cells of the hindbrain, which control gross timing (i.e., duration) of the motor command (Chagnaud et al. 2011; Chagnaud and Bass 2013), whereas control of motor command frequency occurs downstream of the corollary discharge. Additionally, weak encoding of swim frequency in the periphery may be due to persistent postsynaptic hair cell or afferent responses (e.g., neurotransmitter washout) in tadpole and toadfish (Boyle et al. 2009; Chagnaud et al. 2015; Highstein and Baker 1986).
We found that the reduction of afferent spike rate persisted beyond the offset of swimming, revealing that the influence of efferent activity is not confined only to periods of active motor commands. This reduction was substantial, returning to intrinsic spontaneous spike rates after a well-defined refractory period. The refractory period is likely the product of either the interruption of the spontaneous spike pattern or a persistent hyperpolarizing effect of the efferents (Art et al. 1982; Flock and Russell 1976). This observation is supported by the fact that the refractory period was correlated to relative spike rate during swimming, swim frequency, and duration, suggesting that reduced spike rate is related to motor correlates within the efferent signal.
The refractory period has important implications for sensing in freely swimming larval fishes that employ intermittent burst-glide swimming patterns. The glide behavior following swim bouts may allow efficient sensing, because the straightened body mitigates complex wake interactions that might interfere with responses to external stimuli (Feitl et al. 2010; Kramer and McLaughlin 2001; McHenry and Lauder 2005; Tan et al. 2011). We found that the glide duration is not directly influenced by tail-beat frequency or swim duration but can still be influenced by other factors such as differences in mechanical scaling (McHenry and Lauder 2005; Müller et al. 2000) or viscosity of the fluid environment at different depths (Anderson et al. 2001). Because the motor activity influences the refractory period and the duration of the glide phase is relatively constant across swimming parameters, on average, the refractory period accounts for 30% of the glide duration. The relationship between the refractory:glide duration and tail-beat frequency or swim duration reveals that fast, short swim bouts minimize lateral line desensitization during the glide period. In light of our results, we must consider that gliding fish are at least partially desensitized to hydrodynamic signals, and this desensitization is influenced by the preceding swim. The refractory period may compensate for reafference generated by inertial passive movement, but it may also attenuate the lateral line during crucial opportunities for flow stimuli sampling. For example, the blind Mexican cavefish (Astyanax mexicanus) performs active flow sensing during the glide phase, sampling the flow field to detect and avoid obstacles (Teyke 1985; Windsor et al. 2008), but the refractory period may limit this behavioral specialization during the initial part of the glide. Therefore, the long-standing idea that intermittent or burst-and-glide swimming exists for sensory advantage is potentially inflated (Feitl et al. 2010; Tan et al. 2011). Neurophysiological mechanisms must be taken into consideration when one is attempting to interpret how movement strategies may influence the sensory capabilities of animals.
We still know relatively little about efferent modulation in freely swimming fishes, where hydrodynamic stimuli from swimming can further impact afferent activity, but we now have a greater appreciation for the complex relationship between sensory and motor systems. In freely moving fish, afferent spike frequencies increase both during swimming and during feeding strikes (Ayali et al. 2009; Mensinger et al. 2019; Palmer et al. 2003, 2005; Russell and Roberts 1974). This is consistent with our observation that efferent activity only partially suppresses spontaneous spike rates. To determine how efferents modulate evoked spike rates and ultimately relate to behavior, it is necessary to examine the magnitude of reafference in the absence of efferent activity, as we have done in the current study. We have found that characterizing even basic properties of the afferent signal crucially depends on knowledge of the swim cycle. With the advent of new technologies and advances in both model systems and comparative approaches, neurophysiologists are primed to look deeper into the mechanisms of sensory feedback in vivo during locomotion and behavior.
GRANTS
We gratefully acknowledge support from National Institutes of Health Grant DC010809, National Science Foundation Grant IOS1257150, and the Whitney Laboratory for Marine Biosciences (to J. C. Liao).
DISCLOSURES
No conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise, are declared by the authors.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
E.T.L. and J.C.L conceived and designed research; E.T.L. and D.A.S. performed experiments; E.T.L. and D.A.S. analyzed data; E.T.L, D.A.S., and J.C.L. interpreted results of experiments; E.T.L. prepared figures; E.T.L. drafted manuscript; E.T.L., D.A.S., and J.C.L. edited and revised manuscript; E.T.L., D.A.S., and J.C.L. approved final version of manuscript.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank James Strother for generously sharing Tg(HUC:GCaMP6s) fish during preliminary experiments, and we thank Otar Akanyeti for programming assistance and comments on the manuscript.
REFERENCES
- Anderson EJ, McGillis WR, Grosenbaugh MA. The boundary layer of swimming fish. J Exp Biol 204: 81–102, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Art JJ, Crawford AC, Fettiplace R, Fuchs PA. Efferent regulation of hair cells in the turtle cochlea. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 216: 377–384, 1982. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1982.0081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Auguie B. gridExtra: Miscellaneous Functions for “Grid” Graphics. R package version 2.3 Vienna, Austria: R Core Team, 2017. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=gridExtra. [Google Scholar]
- Ayali A, Gelman S, Tytell ED, Cohen AH. Lateral-line activity during undulatory body motions suggests a feedback link in closed-loop control of sea lamprey swimming. Can J Zool 87: 671–683, 2009. doi: 10.1139/Z09-050. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Baty F, Ritz C, Charles S, Brutsche M, Flandrois JP, Delignette-Muller ML. A toolbox for nonlinear regression in R: the package nlstools. J Stat Softw 66: 1–21, 2015. doi: 10.18637/jss.v066.i05. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bell CC. An efference copy which is modified by reafferent input. Science 214: 450–453, 1981. doi: 10.1126/science.7291985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bell CC. Sensory coding and corollary discharge effects in mormyrid electric fish. J Exp Biol 146: 229–253, 1989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bell CC. Memory-based expectations in electrosensory systems. Curr Opin Neurobiol 11: 481–487, 2001. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00238-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bodznick D, Montgomery JC, Carey M. Adaptive mechanisms in the elasmobranch hindbrain. J Exp Biol 202: 1357–1364, 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyle R, Rabbitt RD, Highstein SM. Efferent control of hair cell and afferent responses in the semicircular canals. J Neurophysiol 102: 1513–1525, 2009. doi: 10.1152/jn.91367.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breheny P, Burchett W. Visualization of regression models using visreg. R J 9: 56–71, 2017. doi: 10.32614/RJ-2017-046. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bricaud O, Chaar V, Dambly-Chaudière C, Ghysen A. Early efferent innervation of the zebrafish lateral line. J Comp Neurol 434: 253–261, 2001. doi: 10.1002/cne.1175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brichta AM, Goldberg JM. Responses to efferent activation and excitatory response-intensity relations of turtle posterior-crista afferents. J Neurophysiol 83: 1224–1242, 2000. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.83.3.1224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buss RR, Drapeau P. Synaptic drive to motoneurons during fictive swimming in the developing zebrafish. J Neurophysiol 86: 197–210, 2001. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.86.1.197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chagnaud BP, Baker R, Bass AH. Vocalization frequency and duration are coded in separate hindbrain nuclei. Nat Commun 2: 346, 2011. doi: 10.1038/ncomms1349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chagnaud BP, Banchi R, Simmers J, Straka H. Spinal corollary discharge modulates motion sensing during vertebrate locomotion. Nat Commun 6: 7982, 2015. doi: 10.1038/ncomms8982. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chagnaud BP, Bass AH. Vocal corollary discharge communicates call duration to vertebrate auditory system. J Neurosci 33: 18775–18780, 2013. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3140-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crapse TB, Sommer MA. Corollary discharge across the animal kingdom. Nat Rev Neurosci 9: 587–600, 2008. doi: 10.1038/nrn2457. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dawkins R, Keller SL, Sewell WF. Pharmacology of acetylcholine-mediated cell signaling in the lateral line organ following efferent stimulation. J Neurophysiol 93: 2541–2551, 2005. doi: 10.1152/jn.01283.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dawkins R, Sewell WF. Afferent synaptic transmission in a hair cell organ: pharmacological and physiological analysis of the role of the extended refractory period. J Neurophysiol 92: 1105–1115, 2004. doi: 10.1152/jn.01107.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Douglass JK, Wilkens L, Pantazelou E, Moss F. Noise enhancement of information transfer in crayfish mechanoreceptors by stochastic resonance. Nature 365: 337–340, 1993. doi: 10.1038/365337a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dow E, Jacobo A, Hossain S, Siletti K, Hudspeth AJ. Connectomics of the zebrafish’s lateral-line neuromast reveals wiring and miswiring in a simple microcircuit. eLife 7: e33988, 2018. doi: 10.7554/eLife.33988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elgoyhen AB, Johnson DS, Boulter J, Vetter DE, Heinemann S. α9: an acetylcholine receptor with novel pharmacological properties expressed in rat cochlear hair cells. Cell 79: 705–715, 1994. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(94)90555-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elzhov TV, Mullen KM, Spiess AN, Bolker B. Minpack.lm: R Interface to the Levenberg-Marquardt Nonlinear Least-Squares Algorithm Found in MINPACK, Plus Support for Bounds. R package version 1.2–1 Vienna, Austria: R Core Team, 2016. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=minpack.lm. [Google Scholar]
- Feitl KE, Ngo V, McHenry MJ. Are fish less responsive to a flow stimulus when swimming? J Exp Biol 213: 3131–3137, 2010. doi: 10.1242/jeb.045518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flock A, Russell I. Inhibition by efferent nerve fibres: action on hair cells and afferent synaptic transmission in the lateral line canal organ of the burbot Lota lota. J Physiol 257: 45–62, 1976. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1976.sp011355. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flock A, Russell IJ. The post-synaptic action of efferent fibres in the lateral line organ of the burbot Lota lota. J Physiol 235: 591–605, 1973. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1973.sp010406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox J, Weisberg S. An R Companion to Applied Regression (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 2011. https://socialsciences.mcmaster.ca/jfox/Books/Companion/. [Google Scholar]
- Gabriel JP, Ausborn J, Ampatzis K, Mahmood R, Eklöf-Ljunggren E, El Manira A. Principles governing recruitment of motoneurons during swimming in zebrafish. Nat Neurosci 14: 93–99, 2011. doi: 10.1038/nn.2704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenwell BM, Schubert Kabban CM. investr: an R package for inverse estimation. R J 6: 90–100, 2014. doi: 10.32614/RJ-2014-009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Haehnel-Taguchi M, Akanyeti O, Liao JC. Afferent and motoneuron activity in response to single neuromast stimulation in the posterior lateral line of larval zebrafish. J Neurophysiol 112: 1329–1339, 2014. doi: 10.1152/jn.00274.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hänzi S, Banchi R, Straka H, Chagnaud BP. Locomotor corollary activation of trigeminal motoneurons: coupling of discrete motor behaviors. J Exp Biol 218: 1748–1758, 2015. doi: 10.1242/jeb.120824. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris GG, Frishkopf LS, Flock A. Receptor potentials from hair cells of the lateral line. Science 167: 76–79, 1970. doi: 10.1126/science.167.3914.76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris GG, Milne DC. Input-output characteristics of the lateral-line sense organs of Xenopus laevis. J Acoust Soc Am 40: 32–42, 1966. doi: 10.1121/1.1910060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Highstein SM, Baker R. Organization of the efferent vestibular nuclei and nerves of the toadfish, Opsanus tau. J Comp Neurol 243: 309–325, 1986. doi: 10.1002/cne.902430303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keen EC, Hudspeth AJ. Transfer characteristics of the hair cell’s afferent synapse. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103: 5537–5542, 2006. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601103103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiang NY, Watanabe T, Thomas E, Clark L. Discharge Patterns of Single Fibers in the Cat’s Auditory Nerve. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1965. [Google Scholar]
- Kramer DL, McLaughlin RL. The behavioral ecology of intermittent locomotion. Integr Comp Biol 41: 137–153, 2001. doi: 10.1093/icb/41.2.137. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Levi R, Akanyeti O, Ballo A, Liao JC. Frequency response properties of primary afferent neurons in the posterior lateral line system of larval zebrafish. J Neurophysiol 113: 657–668, 2015. doi: 10.1152/jn.00414.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li G, Müller UK, van Leeuwen JL, Liu H. Body dynamics and hydrodynamics of swimming fish larvae: a computational study. J Exp Biol 215: 4015–4033, 2012. doi: 10.1242/jeb.071837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li GL, Keen E, Andor-Ardó D, Hudspeth AJ, von Gersdorff H. The unitary event underlying multiquantal EPSCs at a hair cell’s ribbon synapse. J Neurosci 29: 7558–7568, 2009. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0514-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liao JC, Fetcho JR. Shared versus specialized glycinergic spinal interneurons in axial motor circuits of larval zebrafish. J Neurosci 28: 12982–12992, 2008. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3330-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liao JC, Haehnel M. Physiology of afferent neurons in larval zebrafish provides a functional framework for lateral line somatotopy. J Neurophysiol 107: 2615–2623, 2012. doi: 10.1152/jn.01108.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu KS, Fetcho JR. Laser ablations reveal functional relationships of segmental hindbrain neurons in zebrafish. Neuron 23: 325–335, 1999. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80783-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manley GA, Robertson D. Analysis of spontaneous activity of auditory neurones in the spiral ganglion of the guinea-pig cochlea. J Physiol 258: 323–336, 1976. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1976.sp011422. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masino MA, Fetcho JR. Fictive swimming motor patterns in wild type and mutant larval zebrafish. J Neurophysiol 93: 3177–3188, 2005. doi: 10.1152/jn.01248.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McHenry MJ, Feitl KE, Strother JA, Van Trump WJ. Larval zebrafish rapidly sense the water flow of a predator’s strike. Biol Lett 5: 477–479, 2009. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2009.0048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McHenry MJ, Lauder GV. The mechanical scaling of coasting in zebrafish (Danio rerio). J Exp Biol 208: 2289–2301, 2005. doi: 10.1242/jeb.01642. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLean DL, Fan J, Higashijima S, Hale ME, Fetcho JR. A topographic map of recruitment in spinal cord. Nature 446: 71–75, 2007. doi: 10.1038/nature05588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLean DL, Fetcho JR. Spinal interneurons differentiate sequentially from those driving the fastest swimming movements in larval zebrafish to those driving the slowest ones. J Neurosci 29: 13566–13577, 2009. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3277-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLean DL, Masino MA, Koh IY, Lindquist WB, Fetcho JR. Continuous shifts in the active set of spinal interneurons during changes in locomotor speed. Nat Neurosci 11: 1419–1429, 2008. doi: 10.1038/nn.2225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mekdara PJ, Schwalbe MAB, Coughlin LL, Tytell ED. The effects of lateral line ablation and regeneration in schooling giant danios. J Exp Biol 221: jeb175166, 2018. doi: 10.1242/jeb.175166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mendell LM. The size principle: a rule describing the recruitment of motoneurons. J Neurophysiol 93: 3024–3026, 2005. doi: 10.1152/classicessays.00025.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mensinger AF, Van Wert JC, Rogers LS. Lateral line sensitivity in free-swimming toadfish Opsanus tau. J Exp Biol 222: jeb190587, 2019. doi: 10.1242/jeb.190587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Montgomery J, Bodznick D, Halstead M. Hindbrain signal processing in the lateral line system of the dwarf scorpionfish Scopeana papillosus. J Exp Biol 199: 893–899, 1996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Montgomery JC. Noise cancellation in the electrosensory system of the thornback ray; common mode rejection of input produced by the animal’s own ventilatory movement. J Comp Physiol 155: 103–111, 1984. doi: 10.1007/BF00610935. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Montgomery JC, Bodznick D. An adaptive filter that cancels self-induced noise in the electrosensory and lateral line mechanosensory systems of fish. Neurosci Lett 174: 145–148, 1994. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(94)90007-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller UK, Stamhuis EJ, Videler JJ. Hydrodynamics of unsteady fish swimming and the effects of body size: comparing the flow fields of fish larvae and adults. J Exp Biol 203: 193–206, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller UK, van Leeuwen JL. Swimming of larval zebrafish: ontogeny of body waves and implications for locomotory development. J Exp Biol 207: 853–868, 2004. doi: 10.1242/jeb.00821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Obholzer N, Wolfson S, Trapani JG, Mo W, Nechiporuk A, Busch-Nentwich E, Seiler C, Sidi S, Söllner C, Duncan RN, Boehland A, Nicolson T. Vesicular glutamate transporter 3 is required for synaptic transmission in zebrafish hair cells. J Neurosci 28: 2110–2118, 2008. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5230-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olszewski J, Haehnel M, Taguchi M, Liao JC. Zebrafish larvae exhibit rheotaxis and can escape a continuous suction source using their lateral line. PLoS One 7: e36661, 2012. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036661. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oteiza P, Odstrcil I, Lauder G, Portugues R, Engert F. A novel mechanism for mechanosensory-based rheotaxis in larval zebrafish. Nature 547: 445–448, 2017. [Erratum in Nature 549: 292, 2017.] doi: 10.1038/nature23014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palmer LM, Deffenbaugh M, Mensinger AF. Sensitivity of the anterior lateral line to natural stimuli in the oyster toadfish, Opsanus tau (Linnaeus). J Exp Biol 208: 3441–3450, 2005. doi: 10.1242/jeb.01766. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palmer LM, Giuffrida BA, Mensinger AF. Neural recordings from the lateral line in free-swimming toadfish, Opsanus tau. Biol Bull 205: 216–218, 2003. doi: 10.2307/1543260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poulet JF, Hedwig B. The cellular basis of a corollary discharge. Science 311: 518–522, 2006. doi: 10.1126/science.1120847. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts BL, Russell IJ. The activity of lateral-line efferent neurones in stationary and swimming dogfish. J Exp Biol 57: 435–448, 1972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Russell IJ. The pharmacology of efferent synapses in the lateral-line system of Xenopus laevis. J Exp Biol 54: 643–658, 1971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Russell IJ, Roberts BL. Inhibition of spontaneous lateral-line activity of efferent nerve stimulation. J Exp Biol 57: 77–82, 1972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Russell IJ, Roberts BL. Active reduction of lateral-line sensitivity in swimming dogfish. J Comp Physiol 94: 7–15, 1974. doi: 10.1007/BF00610153. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Severi KE, Portugues R, Marques JC, O’Malley DM, Orger MB, Engert F. Neural control and modulation of swimming speed in the larval zebrafish. Neuron 83: 692–707, 2014. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.06.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sewell WF, Starr PA. Effects of calcitonin gene-related peptide and efferent nerve stimulation on afferent transmission in the lateral line organ. J Neurophysiol 65: 1158–1169, 1991. doi: 10.1152/jn.1991.65.5.1158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith CA, Haehnel-Taguchi M, Liao JC. Regional specialization of posterior lateral line efferent neurons in the hindbrain of larval zebrafish. Integr Comp Biol 54: E351, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Song S, Lee JA, Kiselev I, Iyengar V, Trapani JG, Tania N. Mathematical modeling and analyses of interspike-intervals of spontaneous activity in afferent neurons of the zebrafish lateral line. Sci Rep 8: 14851, 2018. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-33064-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Soustelle L, Aigouy B, Asensio ML, Giangrande A. UV laser mediated cell selective destruction by confocal microscopy. Neural Dev 3: 11, 2008. doi: 10.1186/1749-8104-3-11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sperry RW. Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. J Comp Physiol Psychol 43: 482–489, 1950. doi: 10.1037/h0055479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stewart WJ, Cardenas GS, McHenry MJ. Zebrafish larvae evade predators by sensing water flow. J Exp Biol 216: 388–398, 2013. doi: 10.1242/jeb.072751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Straka H, Simmers J, Chagnaud BP. A new perspective on predictive motor signaling. Curr Biol 28: R232–R243, 2018. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.01.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suli A, Watson GM, Rubel EW, Raible DW. Rheotaxis in larval zebrafish is mediated by lateral line mechanosensory hair cells. PLoS One 7: e29727, 2012. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029727. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tan D, Patton P, Coombs S. Do blind cavefish have behavioral specializations for active flow-sensing? J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 197: 743–754, 2011. doi: 10.1007/s00359-011-0637-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teyke T. Collision with and avoidance of obstacles by blind cave fish Anophtichthys jordani (Characidae). J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 31: 1614–1623, 1985. doi: 10.1007/bf01350081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trapani JG, Nicolson T. Mechanism of spontaneous activity in afferent neurons of the zebrafish lateral-line organ. J Neurosci 31: 1614–1623, 2011. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3369-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tricas TC, Highstein SM. Visually mediated inhibition of lateral line primary afferent activity by the octavolateralis efferent system during predation in the free-swimming toadfish, Opsanus tau. Exp Brain Res 83: 233–236, 1990. doi: 10.1007/BF00232215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vetter DE, Katz E, Maison SF, Taranda J, Turcan S, Ballestero J, Liberman MC, Elgoyhen AB, Boulter J. The α10 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor subunit is required for normal synaptic function and integrity of the olivocochlear system. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104: 20594–20599, 2007. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0708545105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- von Holst E, Mittelstaedt H. Das Reafferenzprinzip (Wechselwirkung zwischen Zentralnervensystem und Peripherie) [The principle of reafference: interactions between the central nervous system and the peripheral organs]. Naturwissenschaften 37: 463–576, 1950. [Google Scholar]
- Weeg MS, Land BR, Bass AH. Vocal pathways modulate efferent neurons to the inner ear and lateral line. J Neurosci 25: 5967–5974, 2005. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0019-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Werker AR, Jaggard KW. Modeling asymmetrical growth curves that rise and then fall: applications to foliage dynamics of sugar beet (Beta vulgaris L.). Ann Bot 79: 657–665, 1997. doi: 10.1006/anbo.1997.0387. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wickham H. Reshaping data with the reshape package. J Stat Softw 21: 1–20, 2007. doi: 10.18637/jss.v021.i12. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wickham H. The split-apply-combine strategy for data analysis. J Stat Softw 40: 1–29, 2011. doi: 10.18637/jss.v040.i01. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wickham H. Elegant graphics for Data Analysis. New York: Springer, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Wickham H, Francois R, Henry L, Muller K. Dplyr: a Grammar of Data Manipulation. R package version 0.8.0.1 Vienna, Austria: R Core Team, 2019. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=dplyr. [Google Scholar]
- Wilke CO. Cowplot: Streamlined Plot Theme and Plot Annotations for ‘ggplot2’. R package version 0.9.4 Vienna, Austria: R Core Team, 2019. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=cowplot. [Google Scholar]
- Windsor SP, Tan D, Montgomery JC. Swimming kinematics and hydrodynamic imaging in the blind Mexican cave fish (Astyanax fasciatus). J Exp Biol 211: 2950–2959, 2008. doi: 10.1242/jeb.020453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yu CR, Power J, Barnea G, O’Donnell S, Brown HE, Osborne J, Axel R, Gogos JA. Spontaneous neural activity is required for the establishment and maintenance of the olfactory sensory map. Neuron 42: 553–566, 2004. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00224-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]