Skip to main content
PLOS Biology logoLink to PLOS Biology
. 2020 Jan 23;18(1):e3000570. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3000570

Temporal information loss in the macaque early visual system

Gregory D Horwitz 1,*
Editor: Jonathan Demb2
PMCID: PMC6977937  PMID: 31971946

Abstract

Stimuli that modulate neuronal activity are not always detectable, indicating a loss of information between the modulated neurons and perception. To identify where in the macaque visual system information about periodic light modulations is lost, signal-to-noise ratios were compared across simulated cone photoreceptors, lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) neurons, and perceptual judgements. Stimuli were drifting, threshold-contrast Gabor patterns on a photopic background. The sensitivity of LGN neurons, extrapolated to populations, was similar to the monkeys' at low temporal frequencies. At high temporal frequencies, LGN sensitivity exceeded the monkeys' and approached the upper bound set by cone photocurrents. These results confirm a loss of high-frequency information downstream of the LGN. However, this loss accounted for only about 5% of the total. Phototransduction accounted for essentially all of the rest. Together, these results show that low temporal frequency information is lost primarily between the cones and the LGN, whereas high-frequency information is lost primarily within the cones, with a small additional loss downstream of the LGN.


Information about periodic modulation of luminance is lost in the visual system, both peripherally and centrally. This study uses “ideal observer” analyses to reveal that peripheral signal loss dominates, even at the high frequencies at which central loss is maximal.

Introduction

Information, lost from a processing cascade, cannot be regained downstream. The signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) of image representations in the early visual system thus imposes an upper bound on SNR downstream and, ultimately, on perception. Identifying where and how SNR is lost in the visual system constrains models of vision, guides experimental designs, and bring us closer to understanding perception at a mechanistic level. Comparisons between neural and behavioral SNR have been used to constrain models of visual awareness [1]. Discovery of bottlenecks for information transmission has proven valuable in a variety of sensory systems, and a lesson from these studies is that early bottlenecks can have large perceptual effects that generalize well across a wide range of stimulus and task conditions [24].

The visibility of a temporally modulated light depends on its modulation frequency, a fact that has been known for centuries but whose neural basis is poorly understood. A prevalent view is that temporal contrast sensitivity is limited by cortical mechanisms (e.g., [5, 6]). Evidence for this idea is primarily that some neurons in the early visual system respond to flicker above the critical flicker fusion frequency [714]. This logic is flawed; some cortical neurons are exquisitely sensitive to low temporal frequency modulations, suggesting that information loss may not be restricted entirely to high frequencies [15, 16]. Moreover, signal and noise in cone photoreceptors are temporal frequency–dependent [1724]. How much information is lost precortically, relative to how much is lost through cortical processing, is unknown.

To answer this question, I compared the quality of stimulus representation across four levels of the macaque visual system in the common currency of SNR. Signals and noise in cone photoreceptors were simulated on the basis of Poisson statistics (for an ideal observer of photon absorptions) and electrophysiological measurements (for an ideal observer of outer-segment electrical currents) [19, 25, 26]. Lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) neurons were recorded extracellularly. Psychophysical sensitivity was measured with a two-alternative, forced-choice contrast detection task [27]. Critical to the logic of the experiment, stimuli used to probe LGN neurons and simulated cones were at or near the monkeys' detection threshold. Using these stimuli, as opposed to extrapolating from suprathreshold measurements, provides direct insight into the neural signals that mediate contrast detection [28, 29].

Ideal observers, by definition, integrate information over the complete spatiotemporal extent of the stimulus and no further. In contrast, monkeys integrate locally and briefly, within windows that likely depend on spatial and temporal frequency [3032], and their thresholds can be elevated by uncertainty about exactly where and when the stimulus will appear [33]. Stimuli in this study were small relative to psychophysical integration areas measured under similar stimulus conditions [32, 34, 35] but were much longer (666 ms) than psychophysical integration times (40–100 ms) [36, 37]. Long stimulus durations were unavoidable because temporal frequency, the stimulus property of interest, cannot be manipulated in brief stimuli.

Changing the spatiotemporal integration window affects the performance of all three ideal observers (of photon absorptions, cone currents, and LGN action potentials) in similar and expected ways (S1 Fig and S2 Fig). This is because noise in photon absorptions, cone currents, and LGN responses is correlated over only short spatiotemporal scales.

This study produced four main results. First, most of the information loss occurred upstream of the LGN, consistent with previous work [38]. Second, information loss at each stage was temporal frequency–dependent. Low-frequency information was lost primarily between the cones and the LGN, whereas high-frequency information was lost primarily in the cones, making them the principal mediator of flicker fusion. Third, LGN neurons were similarly sensitive to the monkey at low temporal frequencies, indicating near-perfect fidelity of low-frequency signal transmission from the LGN to perception. Fourth, magnocellular and parvocellular LGN neurons were more sensitive than the monkey at high frequencies, approaching the upper bound set by cone photocurrents.

Results

Fifty-three LGN neurons were recorded from two monkeys (19 parvocellular neurons from each, eight magnocellular neurons from monkey 1, and seven magnocellular neurons from monkey 2). Neurons were classified as parvocellular or magnocellular based on location along electrode tracks and responses to white-noise stimuli (Fig 1). Parvocellular neurons were encountered early in each penetration, had temporally protracted spike-triggered averages, and usually exhibited opponency between L- and M-cones. Magnocellular neurons were encountered deeper, had more clearly biphasic spike-triggered averages, and lacked cone opponency.

Fig 1. t-SNE of STAs from 58 LGN neurons [39].

Fig 1

Pixels in the white-noise stimulus that impinged on the RF were averaged to derive a 6 (frames) × 3 (color) temporal–chromatic STA for each neuron. STAs are plotted around the perimeter, and similar STAs have been superimposed. Arrows indicate the correspondence between STAs in time–color and t-SNE representations, which have been manually partitioned into seven clusters (dashed ellipses). Singular value decomposition was used to represent each STA as a 9-element vector prior to t-SNE. The singular value decomposition preserved an average of 78% of the variance in the STAs across neurons (SD = 10%). The axes of the t-SNE plot are arbitrary; only distances between points are meaningful. Data are available at https://github.com/horwitzlab/LGN-temporal-contrast-sensitivity/blob/master/DataByFigure.xlsx. Konio, koniocellular; LGN, lateral geniculate nucleus; Magno, magnocellular; parvo, parvocellular; RF, receptive field; SD, standard deviation; STA, spike-triggered average; t-SNE, t-distributed stochastic neighbor embedding.

Each neuron was stimulated with a series of drifting Gabor patterns. The contrast of each Gabor pattern was adjusted to be at the monkeys' detection threshold (see Methods and [27]). Detection thresholds vary across the visual field, so stimulus contrasts were tailored to each receptive field (RF) location.

LGN responses ranged from unmeasurable to vigorous. An example magnocellular neuron exhibited robust modulations to high-frequency stimulation and weaker modulations to low-frequency stimulation (Fig 2A). An example parvocellular neuron, with a nearby RF, responded relatively weakly (Fig 2B).

Fig 2.

Fig 2

Rasters (black ticks) and peristimulus time histograms (gray traces) from an example magnocellular neuron (A) and an example parvocellular neuron (B) in response to near-threshold, drifting Gabor patterns sorted by temporal frequency. Vertical lines indicate stimulus start and stop times. Numbers at left indicate frequency in Hz, and numbers at right indicate Weber luminance contrast. (C and D) Signal-to-noise ratio (d′) as a function of temporal frequency computed from the responses in (A) and (B), respectively. The gray band spans ±1 standard error, estimated by nonparametric bootstrap (200 resamples). Data are available at https://github.com/horwitzlab/LGN-temporal-contrast-sensitivity/blob/master/DataByFigure.xlsx.

Neuronal responses were converted to d′ values that quantify SNR on the basis of an ideal observer model (Fig 2C and 2D, see Methods). The ideal observer integrates spikes over the entire stimulus duration, providing an upper bound on behavioral sensitivity (it makes no assumptions about the inefficiency of downstream integration). The monkey does not integrate perfectly and may, like humans, have an integration time that contracts with temporal frequency [30, 31, 4042]. The fact that neuronal d′ computed over long stimulus durations can exceed the d′ inferred from the monkeys' behavioral sensitivity is unsurprising [28, 43, 44].

For each neuron, a d′ value was calculated separately for each temporal frequency, and these were averaged across temporal frequencies to obtain a single value per neuron. Below, I describe how this average d′ value depended on neuronal type and RF eccentricity. The relationship between d′ and temporal frequency is addressed in the subsequent section.

SNR as a function of eccentricity

Magnocellular neurons (Fig 3A) had greater d′ values than parvocellular neurons (Fig 3B) at all eccentricities tested. d′ increased with eccentricity for both populations (Fig 3C). The increase in d′ with eccentricity is expected: as the stimulus moved away from the fovea, its contrast increased to compensate for the monkeys' declining psychophysical sensitivity. The slope of this relationship was significantly greater than zero for parvocellular neurons (p = 0.03) and did not differ significantly between magnocellular and parvocellular neurons (p > 0.05).

Fig 3. Dependence of d′ on RF location for monkey 1 (circles) and monkey 2 (triangles).

Fig 3

(A and B) Symbol position represents RF location in visual space, and symbol size represents d′ for magnocellular (A) and parvocellular neurons (B). Scale bar in (A) represents a d′ of 1 and corresponds to circle diameter and triangle base width in (A) and (B). Example neurons from Fig 2 are highlighted with bold outlines. (C) Data from (A) and (B) replotted as a function of retinal eccentricity. (D) Data from (C) multiplied by population scale factors. Open and filled symbols in (C) and (D) represent data from magnocellular and parvocellular neurons, respectively. Symbol size in (C) and (D) is not meaningful. Data are available at https://github.com/horwitzlab/LGN-temporal-contrast-sensitivity/blob/master/DataByFigure.xlsx. H., horizontal; RF, receptive field; V. vertical.

Psychophysical sensitivity declines with eccentricity presumably because the number of stimulated neurons is reduced, not because of a decline in the sensitivity of individual LGN neurons. Under this presumption, scaling d′ of individual neurons to that expected of a population should eliminate the effect of eccentricity. Confirming this intuition, d′ values for neuronal populations (individual neuron d′ values multiplied by their respective population scale factors, see Methods) did not vary with retinal eccentricity for magnocellular or parvocellular neurons (both p > 0.05, Fig 3D).

The temporal-frequency dependence of contrast sensitivity varies little over the central 12° of visual angle [45]. Guided by this observation, the contrasts used to stimulate LGN neurons scaled with eccentricity identically for low and high temporal frequencies [27]. If LGN neurons with peripheral RFs were specialized for encoding high-frequency stimuli, d′ should increase with temporal frequency more steeply at large than at small eccentricities. This prediction was not borne out in the data (linear regressions, p > 0.05 for parvocellular and magnocellular neurons from each monkey).

SNR as a function of temporal frequency

d′ increased with temporal frequency (Fig 4). High-frequency stimuli had high contrast to compensate for the monkeys' insensitivity to them, but the dependence of d′ on temporal frequency was not a trivial consequence of contrast. The monkeys' psychophysical sensitivity peaked at approximately 7 Hz (5.8 Hz for monkey 1 and 7.7 Hz for monkey 2), so stimuli below this frequency had relatively high contrast. Nevertheless, magnocellular d′ was greater at 7.7 Hz than at 1 Hz (Wilcoxon signed-rank tests p < 0.05 for both monkeys). Thus, the increase in temporal frequency from 1 to 7.7 Hz outweighed the decrease in contrast over this range with respect to modulating magnocellular neurons. d′ did not differ significantly between 1 and 7.7 Hz for parvocellular neurons from either monkey (p > 0.05).

Fig 4. d′ values of magnocellular neurons (open symbols) and parvocellular neurons (filled symbols) as a function of temporal frequency.

Fig 4

Points are means and shaded bands are ±1 standard error. The monkeys' psychophysical d′ for all stimuli is approximately 1.27 (dashed line, see Methods). Stimulus contrasts are shown for a location 5° out along the horizontal meridian (gray curve and right ordinate). Data are available at https://github.com/horwitzlab/LGN-temporal-contrast-sensitivity/blob/master/DataByFigure.xlsx.

SNR of individual LGN neurons can be compared to behavioral performance using a standard model from signal detection theory [46]. All stimuli were at or near the monkeys' detection threshold, a level of performance consistent with a psychophysical d′ of 1.27 (see Methods). Parvocellular d′ was consistently below this level, showing that these neurons are individually less sensitive than the monkey. Magnocellular d′ was also lower than this level below 10 Hz. Signals from individual LGN neurons must therefore be pooled to mediate behavioral sensitivity, at least for stimuli < 10 Hz.

SNR of LGN populations

A population scale factor, computed for each recorded neuron, indicates how much more sensitive a population of identically sensitive neurons is (see Methods). The product of each neuron's scale factor and d′ value is its population d′ value. Population d′ values for magnocellular neurons increased over most of the temporal frequency range tested (Fig 5A and 5B). Parvocellular population d′ values increased similarly above 10 Hz and varied little below this frequency (Fig 5C and 5D). At the lowest frequencies tested, magnocellular and parvocellular population d′ values were similar to each other and to the monkeys' psychophysical d′. Above 30 Hz, both magnocellular and parvocellular populations had ≥4 times the SNR required to mediate the monkeys' behavior.

Fig 5.

Fig 5

Population signal-to-noise ratio (d′) of LGN neurons (black), cone currents (red), and photon absorptions (“absorp.”; blue) as a function of temporal frequency. Points are means and bands are ±1 standard error. The monkeys' psychophysical d′ for all stimuli is approximately 1.27 (dashed line). Note the break in the ordinate needed to show the d′ of the photon absorption ideal observer at 45 Hz. Data are available at https://github.com/horwitzlab/LGN-temporal-contrast-sensitivity/blob/master/DataByFigure.xlsx. LGN, lateral geniculate nucleus.

Cone current d′ values decreased monotonically with temporal frequency (Fig 5, red traces), showing that information is transmitted from the cones to behavior with progressively greater efficiency as temporal frequency increases. At the highest frequencies tested, cone currents and LGN neuronal populations had similar d′ values, indicating that high-frequency information is transmitted from the cones to the LGN with near-perfect fidelity. In fact, d′ values from LGN populations in monkey 2 slightly exceeded cone current d′ values (Fig 5B), indicating that one or both were misestimated (SNR cannot increase between cone currents and LGN spikes). Potential explanations include errors in the assumed sizes of RFs, density of cones, or the sensitivity of individual cones in monkey 2 to high-frequency modulations (see Discussion).

High-frequency visual information is lost within the cones and downstream of the LGN. The loss downstream of the LGN can be estimated as the difference between population d′ values and 1.27, which approximates the monkeys' psychophysical d′. The loss within the cones can be estimated as the difference in d′ calculated from photon absorptions and from cone currents. Photon absorptions are effectively instantaneous, so d′ of photon absorptions tracks stimulus contrast, reaching a minimum at approximately 7 Hz (compare blue curves in Fig 5 with gray curves in Fig 4). Stimuli near this frequency are therefore processed with maximal efficiency from photon absorption to behavior, incurring 50% of the total SNR loss through phototransduction, an additional 35% between the cones and the LGN, and the final 15% from the LGN to decision-making circuitry. In contrast, the 45-Hz stimulus is processed relatively inefficiently, losing approximately 95% of its SNR through phototransduction and the remaining 5% between the LGN and decision-making circuitry.

If monkeys were ideal observers of LGN spiking activity, then d′ computed from LGN responses would be flat across temporal frequency (recall that all stimuli were approximately matched for behavioral SNR). To the contrary, d′ increased beyond 7.7 Hz for both magnocellular and parvocellular neurons. LGN responses must be sufficient to mediate the monkeys' behavior at low temporal frequencies. Therefore, high-frequency signals must be lost downstream of the LGN.

Analysis of noise spectra and microsaccades

SNR of parvocellular neurons was low below 10 Hz. This result was not due to high-amplitude noise at these frequencies. LGN spike trains had relatively little power from 1 to 20 Hz in the absence of a modulated stimulus (Fig 6A–6D), consistent with previous data [47] and theory [48].

Fig 6.

Fig 6

(A–D) Power spectra of spike trains on stimulus-absent trials. (E–L) Microsaccade-triggered spike time histograms from stimulus-present trials (E–H) and stimulus-absent trials (I–L). Dark traces are means and shaded bands are ±1 standard error. Data are available at https://github.com/horwitzlab/LGN-temporal-contrast-sensitivity/blob/master/DataByFigure.xlsx. Magno, magnocellular; Parvo, parvocellular.

Saccades within the fixation window produced stereotyped modulations of firing rate that were qualitatively similar on trials in which the stimulus appeared inside the RF (Fig 6E–6H) or did not (Fig 6I–6L). The saccade-locked modulations in firing rate may reflect an extraretinal component to the response [49]. Ocular drifts are not expected to influence LGN responses strongly under the conditions of this experiment. They cause only modest displacements of the 1 cycle per degree stimulus on the RF and are slow relative to the stimulus drift and to microsaccades [49, 50].

Saccade-related modulations in firing rate did not account in any simple way for the temporal power spectrum of LGN spike trains (Fig 6A–6D). The temporal distribution of saccade occurrences was fairly flat and similar between monkeys. Monkey 1 made fewer fixational saccades than monkey 2 (monkey 1: 0.4 saccades/s, monkey 2: 2.9 saccades/s, Mann-Whitney U test, p < 0.0001), and parvocellular neurons from monkey 1 showed the clearest dip in noise power at low temporal frequencies (Fig 6A).

Temporal integration

Reducing the temporal window over which spikes are counted reduces neuronal d′, as expected [44, 51]. The fact that LGN population d′ was close to the monkeys' psychophysical d′ at low temporal frequencies (Fig 5) implies that reducing the stimulus duration should reduce psychophysical sensitivity to low temporal frequency patterns. To test this idea, behavioral detection thresholds were measured for stimuli lasting 666 ms, which is the duration used in the electrophysiological recording experiments, and for stimuli lasting 333 ms. If the monkey integrated stimulus information perfectly, then reducing the stimulus duration to 333 ms should increase detection thresholds by the square root of 2. This predicted threshold change was small relative to the noise in the psychophysical measurements, and reducing stimulus duration did not raise thresholds significantly (one-tailed Mann-Whitney U tests, p > 0.05; Fig 7). Nevertheless, the fact that reducing the stimulus duration had little effect on the monkeys' high-frequency thresholds supports the idea that these signals are integrated inefficiently. These data also attest to the stability of the monkey's psychophysical detection thresholds over the duration of this study (compare points to curves in Fig 7).

Fig 7.

Fig 7

Behavioral detection thresholds from monkey 1 (A) and monkey 2 (B) for stimulus presentations lasting 666 ms (black) and 333 ms (red). Symbols are medians, and error bars span 25th to 75th percentiles. Black curves are predicted thresholds from the model in [27], which is based on a 666-ms presentation time. The red curve is identical to the black curve but shifted up by 2 to represent the threshold expected under perfect integration given a 333-ms stimulus duration. Stimuli in these experiments were presented at 5° on the horizontal meridian. Data are available at https://github.com/horwitzlab/LGN-temporal-contrast-sensitivity/blob/master/DataByFigure.xlsx.

Human observers performing flicker detection tasks appear to integrate over different temporal windows depending on stimulus frequency: long integration windows are used to detect slowly modulating patterns, and short integration windows are used to detect quickly modulating patterns [30, 31, 4042]. Consistent with this observation, an ideal observer of LGN spikes over a single stimulus cycle had an SNR that was nearly constant across temporal frequencies (S2 Fig).

The physiological mechanism by which the duration of psychophysical integration could depend on the temporal frequency of a near-threshold stimulus is unclear. One possibility is that high temporal frequency stimuli engage a physiological process that curtails efficient integration. To explore this possibility, spikes from magnocellular neurons were counted over variable epochs starting at stimulus onset and ending partway through the response. The briefest epoch was identified for which the mean population d′ matched the monkeys' psychophysical d′ (1.27). The duration of this epoch varied inversely with temporal frequency, consistent with the idea that decision-making circuits integrate LGN spikes over a brief epoch for high-frequency stimuli and over a longer epoch for low-frequency stimuli (Fig 8A and 8B).

Fig 8.

Fig 8

Magnocellular population signal-to-noise ratio, d′ (A and B), and mean firing rate (C and D) as a function of temporal frequency (ordinate) and integration time from stimulus onset (abscissa). Data from monkey 1 appear in (A) and (C), and data from monkey 2 appear in (B) and (D). Contours in (A) and (B) indicate the locus of points at which d′ = 1.27. Contour in (C) is at 17 spikes/s. Contour in (D) is at 16 spikes/s. These were selected to maximize the similarity of contours between (A) and (C) and between (B) and (D). Data are available at https://github.com/horwitzlab/LGN-temporal-contrast-sensitivity/blob/master/DataByFigure.xlsx.

Potential neural mechanisms are revealed by an analysis of firing rates during the same epochs. High-frequency stimuli elevated the average firing rate (Fig 8C and 8D). Across temporal frequencies, the average firing rate of magnocellular neurons was nearly constant in spike-counting windows for which the average population d′ matched the monkeys' psychophysical d′. The scaling of psychophysical integration window with temporal frequency may be a consequence of each LGN spike affecting downstream circuitry progressively less as their rate increases.

Discussion

To identify where in the primate visual system information about periodic temporal modulations is lost, SNR was measured and compared across cones, LGN neurons, and behavioral choices. Several results were obtained. The reduction in SNR from LGN neurons to behavior increased with temporal frequency. This result shows that low-frequency modulations are conducted from the LGN to decision-making circuitry more efficiently than high-frequency modulations. The absolute sensitivity of LGN populations was similar to the monkeys' at low temporal frequencies, indicating that transmission of low-frequency signals from the LGN to decision-making circuitry is nearly noise-free.

In response to high-frequency modulations, cone currents and LGN spikes had similar SNR, indicating that high-frequency information is conveyed faithfully from the cones to the LGN. The loss of high-frequency SNR in the visual system is therefore not an obligate consequence of synaptic transmission. High-frequency stimuli elevated LGN firing rates, suggesting that spikes that are close together in time (or space, considering intermixed populations of neurons with nearby or overlapping RFs) are integrated relatively inefficiently by downstream neurons.

Below, the results of this study are compared with those of previous studies, the robustness of the results are analyzed with respect to the choice of stimuli and model assumptions, and the biophysical mechanisms of temporal signal loss in the visual system are discussed.

Relationship to previous studies

Consistent with previous studies, the most striking difference between magnocellular and parvocellular neurons was in contrast gain [5255]. Differences in the temporal dynamics of responses were negligible (for consonant observations, see [5559]). These results show that high temporal frequency, threshold-contrast, periodic luminance modulations do not isolate the magnocellular pathway [6, 6062].

Magnocellular neurons responded more vigorously to the lowest-frequency stimuli used (nominally 1 Hz) than did parvocellular neurons. The temporal contrast envelope of the stimulus (666 ms, with full contrast for only half of this duration) contributed to this result by producing appreciable power at frequencies > 1 Hz. To generate a stimulus with a spectral power distribution more tightly concentrated on 1 Hz would require extending the stimulus duration. The stimulus was already long with respect to psychophysical integration times [63, 64] and intersaccade intervals under natural viewing conditions [65, 66]. Extending the stimulus duration might therefore reveal a greater contribution of parvocellular neurons to detection but would likely be less informative regarding the mechanisms of contrast detection during natural viewing.

Increasing the spatial frequency of the Gabor stimulus would also likely have revealed a greater contribution of parvocellular neurons to detection ([61, 67, 68] but see [69, 70]). To test this idea empirically, the behavioral model used for stimulus contrast selection could be extended to include spatial frequency as an additional parameter. Such an extension could be made by merging the existing model with others from the literature (e.g., [71, 72, 73]). However, increasing spatial frequency increases variability of the response phase within trials produced by fixational eye movements, complicating the analysis of neuronal d′.

Several previous studies have compared contrast detection thresholds of human or monkey observers with the spiking responses of macaque LGN neurons or retinal ganglion cells [13, 14, 28, 29, 43, 7477]. Consistent with the results reported here, magnocellular (or magnocellular-projecting) cells are individually more sensitive than psychophysical observers at high temporal frequencies, whereas single parvocellular (or parvocellular-projecting) cells are generally less sensitive than psychophysical observers [14, 43, 53, 74].

Lee and colleagues [43] compared neurometric thresholds of retinal ganglion cells to contrast detection thresholds of human observers. Consistent with the results reported here, magnocellular-projecting cells were more sensitive than parvocellular-projecting cells across temporal frequencies. However, Lee and colleagues also found that the sensitivity of magnocellular-projecting cells matched psychophysical sensitivity at low and high frequencies, whereas individual magnocellular neurons in the current study were less sensitive than the observer at low frequencies and more sensitive than the observer at high frequencies. Part of this discrepancy is related to how spikes were counted. Lee and colleagues counted spikes over individual stimulus cycles and therefore over progressively less time as temporal frequency increased. Integrating LGN spikes in the current dataset over single stimulus cycles produced d′ values that were closely matched to behavior, consistent with the results of Lee and colleagues (S2 Fig). However, the match between neuronal and behavioral sensitivity in the current study occurred only when populations of LGN neurons were considered. Why Lee and colleagues found a quantitative match between the sensitivity of single magnocellular-projecting cells and observers given their large (4.7°) stimulus is unclear but may be related to differences between the stimulus displays used in the neurophysiological experiments and the psychophysical experiments.

Jiang and colleagues [28, 29] recorded from LGN neurons in monkeys performing a contrast detection task. They found that pooling neurons in proportion to their SNR, as was done in the current study, provided the best fits to the psychophysical data. Pools of 50–200 LGN neurons were sufficient to match behavioral sensitivity, except over the shortest stimulus durations. These pools are presumably smaller than the number of LGN neurons that were activated by the stimuli they used, which in some experiments was a 2°-diameter disk. The use of choice probabilities to constrain the pooling model, which is based on debatable assumptions about the role of feedforward noise in perceptual decision-making, may have underestimated the size of neuronal pools [78].

Most of the information loss that limits the detection of a 100-ms light pulse occurs in the retina [38]. Note added in proofs: This result extends to the detection of Gabor patterns in the presence of fixational eye movements [141]. Approximately half of this loss occurs within the cones or at the first synapse. In the current study, approximately half of the information loss occurred within the cones for stimuli below 6 Hz. A similar fraction was lost between cone currents and LGN spikes (Fig 9). Together, these results show that under a range of conditions, contrast sensitivity is limited by the retina, that phototransduction accounts for approximately half the information loss, and that the conversion of graded membrane voltage in first-order retinal neurons to spiking outputs accounts for most of the other half.

Fig 9. Fractional signal-to-noise ratio as a function of temporal frequency and stage of the visual system.

Fig 9

Data are identical to those shown in Fig 5 but have been shifted and scaled so that d′ of the photon absorption ideal observer is 1 and behavioral d′ is 0. Filled-in regions represent signal-to-noise loss between pairs of consecutive stages: from photon absorption to cone currents (white), from cone currents to LGN spikes (gray), and from LGN spikes to behavior (black). Data were averaged across monkeys and across magnocellular and parvocellular populations. Parvocellular neurons were omitted below 1.5 Hz because including them reduced the d′ of LGN populations below the theoretical floor of 1.27. Eliminating parvocellular neurons entirely increased d′ of LGN populations across temporal frequencies (cross-hatched area). Data are available at https://github.com/horwitzlab/LGN-temporal-contrast-sensitivity/blob/master/DataByFigure.xlsx. LGN, lateral geniculate nucleus.

Sensitivity of the analyses to assumptions

LGN responses in this study were analyzed with parametric models. The accuracy of the modeling results depends on the accuracy of the parameter values. Below are discussed the parameters that have the greatest impact on the results, the data that constrain them, and how their values affect the conclusions of this study.

The LGN population model assumes that the correlations between LGN neurons are independent of temporal frequency. This assumption is supported by the fact that many of the factors thought to underlie correlated activity in the LGN—RF overlap, eye movements, and feedback from cortex—are unlikely to depend much, if at all, on the temporal frequency of a near-threshold stimulus. A decisive test of this assumption will require simultaneous recordings of multiple LGN neurons.

If the assumption is correct, estimated population d′ values are probably close to their true values. Larger population d′ values would render LGN neurons more sensitive than the cones inside their RFs, which is logically impossible. Substantially lower population d′ values would render LGN neurons insufficiently sensitive to mediate the monkeys' behavioral performance at low temporal frequencies. If the assumption is incorrect, then the factorization of population d′ into a population scale factor and an individual neuronal d′, on which this analysis depends, is invalid.

The temporal properties of RF centers and surrounds differ, and the way temporal frequency affects information transmission may depend on stimulus size. In this experiment, stimulus size was fixed, but temporal frequency varied, affecting the relative phase and strength of drive from the RF center and surround. We did not model LGN centers and surrounds explicitly. Instead, every recorded neuron was subject to center-surround interactions and thus so were the statistical replicas in the population analyses.

Population scale factors depend on RF sizes and interneuronal correlations. Values for these parameters were taken from the literature (mostly retinal, see Methods and Table 1). The anticorrelation assumed to exist between ON and OFF mosaics affected the results negligibly; eliminating it changed population d′ values by <20%. RF sizes and interneuronal correlations within a mosaic were more influential. S3 Fig shows the results of analyses in which these parameters were changed over a broad but credible range. Halving the RF diameter (increasing the density of RFs by a factor of 4) increased the population scale factor by a factor of approximately 2. Consequently, SNR in the LGN exceeded cone current SNR at the highest temporal frequencies, which is unrealistic. Doubling the RF diameter had the opposite effect.

Table 1. Ideal observer model parameters and sources.

Parameter Value or text reference Source(s) Notes
LGN population model
Scale factor to convert nasal to equivalent temporal eccentricity 0.61 [54, 79]
Magnocellular RF diameter Eq 13 in text [53] Similar to but lower than other estimates [54, 79, 80]
Parvocellular RF diameter Eq 14 in text
[81] Similar to other estimates [79, 80, 82]
Distance between RF centers within a mosaic 2 SDs [83]
RF truncation 2 SDs [84] a
Correlation between neighboring neurons within a mosaic 0.29 [28, 8588] Monkey retinal ganglion cells: 0.1 to 0.3; cat LGN: 0.2 to 0.5; monkey LGN: 0.02
Correlation (between mosaics) −0.05 [8789] Monkey retinal ganglion cells: −0.02 to −0.3
Cone model
mm per degree on monkey retina 0.233 [90]
Cone density (temporal, superior, and inferior retina) 0.967.6e−1.2220x+35997.9e−0.1567x+9993.6e−0.0258x [91] y in cones/mm2 and x in mmb
Cone density (nasal retina) 176662.4e−7.9473x+94490.8e−0.3518x+18676.1e−0.0236x [92] y in cones/mm2 and x in mmc
Axial cone collecting area 0.6 μm2 [93, 94]
Cone noise power spectrum 0.16(1+(ω55)2)4+0.045(1+(ω290)2)1.8 d ω is frequency in Hz.
Cone temporal impulse response 0.6745((t0.0216)31+(t0.0216)3)et0.0299cos(2πt0.5311+68.3628π360) [19]
(Eq 6)
t is time in s.
Cone gain 0.321+(x2250) [19]
Eq 7 [25] Eq 11
x is in R*/s.

aLee (1999) [84] suggests that retinal ganglion cell RFs are slightly larger (2.8–4.2 SDs).

bOriginal equation (in Goodchild et al., 1996, Table 2 [91]) appears to be missing a factor of 1,000, which is included here.

cData digitally captured and fitted.

Abbreviations: LGN, lateral geniculate nucleus; RF, receptive field; SD, standard deviation.

The correlation between neighboring LGN neurons within a mosaic was assumed to be 0.29, which is the normalized overlap between two 2D Gaussian RFs, separated by 1 standard deviation (SD), and truncated at 2 SDs. This value is consistent with data from anesthetized cat (0.2 < r < 0.55) [85, 86] and ex vivo monkey retina (0.12 < r < 0.3) [87, 89] but greatly exceeds estimates from awake monkey LGN (r = 0.02) [28]. Identifying the value of this parameter more precisely will require additional data. Reducing this correlation to zero, which is as low as it can reasonably go, inflated population scale factors by approximately 60% (S3 Fig).

Increasing the overlap between LGN RFs by truncating them at 3 SDs instead of 2 (consistent with [84]) increased the correlation between neighboring neurons from 0.29 to 0.36 but affected the population scale factor by only about 10%. Under the model, as RFs are enlarged, each one receives greater contrast, and therefore, each replica neuron carries greater signal (corresponding to larger values of μ in Eq 12). This increase in signal is compensated by an increase in shared noise across neurons.

Estimates of magnocellular RF sizes came directly from a large set of physiological measurements [53]. An alternative data set, which is sparser at the relevant eccentricities, indicates that magnocellular RF diameters are more than twice as large as assumed, resulting in substantially lower magnocellular population d′ values (similar to the blue traces in S3A Fig and S3B Fig) [54]. Using the parvocellular RF size estimates from [53] produced population d′ values that were too low to mediate the monkeys' detection of low-frequency, antiphase modulations of the L- and M-cones, which is unrealistic [95]. Instead, midget RF sizes were taken as proxy for parvocellular RF sizes [81].

d′ values computed from LGN responses in monkey 2 exceeded the upper bound set by the cones. This logical impossibility represents a flaw in the LGN model, the cone model, or both. When the analysis was restricted to individual LGN neurons and the cones assumed to be inside their monocular RFs, LGN d′ values still exceeded cone d′ values, obviating concerns that misestimates of LGN pool sizes or interneuronal correlations were responsible (S1 Fig). Correction could be achieved by doubling the number of cones used in the simulations or by increasing LGN RF diameters by 50% (S3 Fig). Individual differences on this order are possible [96, 97]. Alternatively, cones in monkey 2 might have greater high-frequency sensitivity than the modeled cones [19, 25]. The high-frequency sensitivity of cones is variable [23], but individual differences in cone sensitivity have not been reported.

LGN neurons may have responded differently during task performance, when psychophysical thresholds were measured, and during fixation when electrophysiological recordings were made. The efficiency of transmission from retinal ganglion cells to LGN neurons varies with arousal level [98], flicker detection thresholds vary with attentiveness [99], and preferred temporal frequencies of LGN neurons drop with inattention in rabbits [100]. Nevertheless, the monkeys were required to fixate in both conditions, which requires arousal [101], and effects of attention on primate LGN neurons are small [70, 102, 103]. Moreover, neurometric thresholds of LGN neurons during passive fixation are similar to those during contrast detection [28].

Loci and mechanisms of temporal information loss in the visual system

Visual information is lost in the eye and in the cortex. The amount of information lost at each stage is temporal frequency–dependent. At 45 Hz, information loss in the cones accounted for approximately 95% of the total and increased at higher frequencies. This result shows that phototransduction, not cortical processing, is the primary mediator of flicker fusion [104, 105].

The biophysical mechanisms of high-frequency filtering in the cones are thought to be related to photopigment quenching [106], and much of the high-frequency noise in cone currents is thought to be due to the spontaneous activation of phosphodiesterase [19]. The loss of low-frequency information in the eye is thought to be due to center-surround RF organization [107, 108] and contrast gain control [109, 110] in addition to phototransduction. Light adaptation mechanisms may also contribute [111115].The biophysical mechanisms of cortical filtering are less well understood but may include spike rate adaptation [116], synaptic depression [117119], and slow feedforward excitation and dominant inhibition in a push-pull arrangement [120].

Spikes from individual LGN neurons drive spikes in downstream area V1 more effectively when they are close together in time than when they are farther apart [86, 121]. Nevertheless, rapidly modulated stimuli in this study produced high firing rates in the LGN that were transmitted to behavior inefficiently (Fig 8). These two observations can be reconciled if the dynamics of cortical networks play the greater role in the attenuation of high-frequency periodic signals than the fatigue of individual LGN afferents.

Temporally protracted, periodic stimuli—like the drifting Gabor patterns used in this study—occur rarely in nature, and the visual system is not under evolutionary pressure to transmit them with high fidelity. Brief, aperiodic luminance changes are more relevant to behavior, and these are transmitted faithfully [42]. Information about both types of stimuli are probably transmitted over the same neural pathways. Perhaps the mechanisms that arrest visual responses with temporal precision require time to recover, precluding the encoding of repeated cycles.

Some neurons in area V1 entrain to stimulus modulations at frequencies too high to be seen [710, 122]. These neurons may be located primarily in the input layers, where preferred temporal frequencies are only slightly lower than those in the LGN [59]. Important remaining questions include how much high-frequency information is lost between the LGN and the output of V1 and between the output of V1 and behavior. Answering these questions is facilitated by the observation that, at low temporal frequencies, SNR in the LGN is closely matched to behavioral SNR. At low temporal frequencies, SNR in V1 must be similar to both of these.

In summary, the transmission of low-frequency, periodic luminance information is degraded primarily between cone photocurrents and LGN spiking responses. Low-frequency information loss in the cortex, under the conditions of this study, is negligible. High-frequency information loss in the cortex exceeds low-frequency information loss, but it accounts for only a few percent of the total.

Methods

Ethics statement

All procedures were approved by the University of Washington Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (protocol #4167–01) and were performed in accordance with the recommendations of the Weatherall report [123].

Electrophysiological data collection

Single LGN units were recorded from two macaque monkeys (male, Macaca mulatta). Visual stimuli were displayed on a rear-projection screen, 61 cm in front of the monkey, illuminated by a digital light projector (Propixx, VPixx) that updated at 240 Hz. Throughout each trial, the monkey was required to fixate on a 0.2° × 0.2° black fixation point within a 1° × 1° window. Eye position was tracked with a scleral search coil (Riverbend Instruments).

Each LGN neuron was characterized with a white-noise stimulus in which the three projector primaries varied independently across space and time according to Gaussian distributions [124, 125]. The mean of the stimulus distribution was identical to the background (Y = 130 cd/m2, x = 0.3, y = 0.3). For 22 neurons, the white-noise stimulus consisted of 0.14° squares arranged in a 10 × 10 grid; for 31 neurons, squares were 0.09° and arranged in a 12 × 12 grid. The stimulus updated every four frames (60 Hz). Spike-triggered averaging was conducted to classify each neuron as magnocellular, parvocellular, or koniocellular and to locate the center of the RF. A small number of neurons that were difficult to classify were passed over during recording or omitted from analysis. The four blue-yellow koniocellular neurons in Fig 1 are not analyzed in this paper.

Following white-noise characterization, neurons were stimulated with upward-drifting, horizontally oriented Gabor patterns with spatial frequency of 1 cycle per degree, an envelope of 0.15° (SD), and a temporal frequency of 1 to 60 Hz in 15 logarithmically spaced steps. Monkey 1 was unable to detect the 60-Hz stimulus, so for this animal the maximum frequency was 45 Hz. The contrast of the Gabor ramped up linearly over 167 ms, remained constant for 334 ms, and then ramped down over 167 ms. The Gabor stimulus was positioned on the display so that its center was at the peak of the spike-triggered average.

Each Gabor stimulus was at or near the monkey's psychophysical detection threshold. Detection thresholds were estimated on the basis of a model that was fitted to behavioral data collected before the electrophysiology experiments began (monkeys 1 and 2 in the current study are monkeys 1 and 2 from Gelfand and Horwitz, 2018 [27]). A small set of behavioral data was collected after the electrophysiological experiments to confirm threshold stationarity (Fig 7). Gabor stimuli varied pseudorandomly in temporal frequency and color direction (L + M or L − M) from trial to trial. A stimulus-absent condition was interleaved to measure baseline spiking statistics. After each stimulus had been shown at least 10 times, the neuron was recharacterized with white noise to confirm recording stability. This procedure, white noise followed by Gabor stimulation, was repeated as many times as possible while the monkey continued to fixate and electrical isolation was maintained (1–3 times). S-cones of the 10° standard observer [126] were unmodulated, giving the L + M stimulus a small chromatic component that the author was unable to see under most of the conditions of this experiment. Responses to L − M modulations are not analyzed in this paper.

Behavioral data collection

Behavioral methods have been described previously [27]. Briefly, monkeys performed a two-alternative, forced-choice detection task. Each trial began when the monkey fixated on a black dot on the rear-projection screen. Five hundred ms later, the Gabor stimulus appeared in the left or right hemifield. The stimulus duration was 666 ms except in a few sessions when it was reduced to 333 ms to probe psychophysical temporal integration efficiency. After the stimulus presentation, the fixation point disappeared, and two targets appeared at 2° on the horizontal meridian. The monkey was required to indicate within 700 ms whether the stimulus had appeared on the left or right by making a saccade to the corresponding target. Stimulus contrast was adjusted by the QUEST staircase procedure [127]. Behavioral data and model fitting code are available at https://github.com/horwitzlab/monkey-temporal-contrast-sensitivity.

Data analysis

The central goal of this study was to compare the SNR of Gabor stimulus representation across four stages of the visual system: cone photoreceptors (photon absorptions and electrical currents), LGN neurons (spikes), and psychophysical decisions (responses in a two-alternative, forced-choice task). The SNR at each stage was quantified as d′, which is the difference between the means of two distributions, representing a noisy representation of a Gabor stimulus and a noisy representation of a blank screen, divided by their SDs, which are assumed to be equal [128]. Reducing spatiotemporal activity patterns to the scalar values used in this calculation required ideal observer models, which are described below. Parameter values used in these models are provided in Table 1.

SNR: Behavior

Behavioral detection thresholds were defined as the contrast at which the monkey answered 82% of the trials correctly. This level of performance corresponds to a d′ of 1.27 under a standard model [46]. Under this model, a draw from each of two independent Gaussian distributions represents evidence that the stimulus appeared at each of two locations. The distribution corresponding to the greater draw is chosen as the location containing the stimulus. The choice is correct with probability 0.82 when the means of the distributions are separated by 1.27 SDs. All stimuli used in the recording experiments were at or near the monkeys' detection threshold, so d′ at the level of psychophysical decision-making was assumed to be approximately 1.27 for all of them.

Estimates of behavioral threshold, and therefore behavioral d′, are noisy. The average cross-validated prediction error of contrast detection thresholds under the model of [27] was 14%. A 14% change in contrast around threshold corresponds to a change from 82% correct choices to 79%–84%, assuming a Weibull psychometric function with a slope of 3 [129]. The range 79%–84% corresponds to the d′ range 0.89–1.7. This range is not a confidence interval, but it provides some intuition regarding the likely error around the point estimate of d′ = 1.27.

SNR: Cone photoreceptor populations

d′ for an ideal observer of photon absorptions was calculated as follows: A mosaic of cones was simulated with a density that varied with retinal eccentricity ([25, 91], see Table 1). The number of photons absorbed, per cone, per frame, was assumed to follow a Poisson distribution. Poisson counts were weighted over space and time using a spatiotemporal weighting function that was identical to the stimulus. This weighting function is optimal for this observer because photon absorptions are instantaneous, local, and independent. The sum of these weighted Poisson counts is approximately Gaussian with nearly the same SD on stimulus-present and stimulus-absent trials (less than 0.1% difference). d′ was calculated from these distributions.

d′ was also calculated for an ideal observer of noisy electrical currents across cone outer-segment membranes as described previously [19, 25]. Currents were modeled as the sum of a deterministic component (the convolution of the stimulus with a fixed temporal kernel) and a stochastic component (a Gaussian process, assumed independent across cones). The deterministic component was computed across the entire cone mosaic, separately in response to a stimulus or a blank, and then projected onto a matched filter (equal to the mean-subtracted deterministic component in response to the stimulus). The difference between these two scalar values is the numerator of d′.

The denominator was calculated in the Fourier domain. The Fourier transform of the spatiotemporal weighting function was multiplied by the cone current noise amplitude spectrum, which was replicated at each pixel. The average Fourier power was computed across temporal frequencies to obtain the variance in cone current at each pixel using Plancherel's theorem. These variances were then scaled by the number of cones in each pixel and summed. The square root of the variance was the denominator of d′. Code implementing these calculations is available at https://github.com/horwitzlab/LGN-temporal-contrast-sensitivity.

SNR: Individual LGN neurons

Under the conditions of this study, every recorded LGN neuron responded dominantly at the fundamental frequency of the stimulus. The signal carried by each neuron was therefore quantified as the amplitude of spike rate modulation at this frequency. Signal and noise were quantified identically except that signal was computed from responses on stimulus-present trials and noise was computed from responses on stimulus-absent trials.

The modulation amplitude of each spike train, v(t), at the fundamental frequency of the stimulus in cosine and sine phase can be written:

x=t=0τcos(2πωt)v(t)dty=t=0τsin(2πωt)v(t)dt, (1–2)

where ω is the temporal frequency of the Gabor in Hz, and τ is the duration of the stimulus in s.

The squared euclidean distance, x2 + y2, is a conventional measure of the response modulation energy (e.g., [130]). The distribution of this quantity is complicated even when spikes are unrelated to the stimulus. For example, the expected value of x and y, assuming uniformly distributed spike times, is

μx=nsin(2πωτ)2πωτμy=n1cos(2πωτ)2πωτ (3–4)

where n is the number of spikes. As n increases, so do μx and μy. The covariance between x and y is

Σ=[abbc]a=n[sin(4πωτ)8πωt+12]+[n(n1)sin(2πωτ)2(2πωτ)2]μx2b=n[sin(2πωτ)24πωt]+[n(n1)sin(πωτ)3cos(πωτ)(πωτ)2]μxμyc=n[sin(4πωτ)8πωt+12]+[n(n1)sin(πωτ)4(πωτ)2]μy2. (5–8)

The covariance (b) is far from zero when sine and cosine functions, evaluated over the stimulus duration, are not orthogonal. As a result, the value of x2 + y2 depends on the phase of the neural response.

A measure based on Mahalanobis distance, as opposed to euclidean distance, mitigates these shortcomings:

dM2=[xμxyμy]Σ1[xμxyμy]. (9)

For an unmodulated spike train, the distribution of dM2 varies relatively little with the number of spikes or stimulus frequency. It is still far from Gaussian, however, which complicates the calculation of d′ values. Transforming dM2 values makes their distribution approximately Gaussian:

dist=f(g(dM2)) (10)

where f is the inverse cumulative density of the standard normal distribution and g is the cumulative density of the χ2(2) distribution. To compute d′ from LGN spike trains, dist values were calculated and averaged separately for stimulus-present and stimulus-absent trials, and the difference between these means was divided by the pooled SD. Code implementing these calculations is available at https://github.com/horwitzlab/LGN-temporal-contrast-sensitivity.

An alternative measure of SNR is the area under the receiver operating characteristic (ROC) [131]. The area under the ROC, computed directly from dM2 values on stimulus-present and stimulus-absent trials, was closely related to d′ (S4 Fig). Note that the ROC is invariant to the transformation in Eq 10 and thus is identical for dist and dM2 values.

SNR: LGN populations

For each recorded LGN neuron, a scale factor was calculated that quantifies how much more sensitive a population of similar neurons is expected to be. The scale factor depends on the sizes and spatial organization of RFs and noise correlations between the neurons. These parameters were taken largely from the literature on primate retinal ganglion cells, which are similar to LGN neurons and have been studied more thoroughly. No parameter was fitted to the data. See Table 1 for parameter values and sources.

LGN RFs were assumed to have Gaussian spatial profiles truncated at 2 SDs [132, 133]. RF surrounds were not explicitly modeled (see Discussion: Sensitivity of the analyses to the assumptions). RFs within a cell type (e.g., ON magnocellular neurons) were assumed to form a hexagonal mosaic with an inter-RF spacing of 2 SDs [83]. Each RF therefore touches its six neighbors at the 1 SD boundary.

A vector of normalized signal strengths, μ, with one element per neuron (one real neuron and the rest, statistical replicas), represents how strongly each neuron within a cell type is driven by the Gabor stimulus. Signal strength was defined as the overlap between each neuron's RF and the spatial envelope of the Gabor stimulus. Each neuron's signal strength was normalized by division by the maximum, which is equal to the overlap between the envelope of the Gabor stimulus and the centermost RF. The centermost RF was presumed to belong to the real neuron because the Gabor stimulus was centered on its RF during data collection.

A covariance matrix, ∑pop, characterized noise across the neuronal population. Noise was assumed to be identical for each LGN neuron, so values on the main diagonal of ∑pop were set to 1. Interneuronal correlations within a mosaic were assumed to be equal to the normalized overlap between their RFs [8789]. Neighboring neurons had a correlation of 0.29, and neurons whose RFs did not overlap had a correlation of zero.

To detect the stimulus, responses were combined across neurons with weights [134]:

w=Σpop1μ. (11)

The net signal was therefore wTμ, and the net noise was wTpop1w [135], leading to the SNR

wTμwTpop1w.

Signal and noise were pooled across two identical and weakly anticorrelated (r = −0.05 [87]) mosaics of neurons (e.g., representing ON and OFF neurons) and from a second, independent eye. The final scale factor was therefore

Populationscalefactor=4wTμ4.2wTΣpop1w. (12)

The number of neurons in each mosaic is implicit in the number of elements of the vector, μ. The factor of 4 in the numerator represents the signal gain achieved by integrating over 4 mosaics of neurons (ON and OFF in the right and left eyes). The 4.2 in the denominator represents the increase in variance produced by integrating over the same 4 mosaics of neurons, and the 0.2 reflects the extra variance due to correlations between ON and OFF mosaics in each eye. If every neuron's responses were independent and identically distributed, the population scale factor would simplify to 4wTμ4wTw=4(1T1)4(1T1)=4n4n=4n, where n is the number of neurons in each mosaic and 1 represents an n-dimensional vector of ones. Thus, consistent with standard results, the SNR of a sum of n independent, identically distributed neurons is n times greater than a single one.

RF size estimates: Magnocellular neurons

Population scale factors depend on RF sizes, which vary with eccentricity. Magnocellular RF eccentricities and sizes were digitally captured from Derrington and Lennie [53] (S5A Fig, Fig 6 from [53]). Captured eccentricities in the nasal retina were multiplied by 0.61 to express all RF locations in degrees of equivalent temporal eccentricity [54, 90], a manipulation that brought captured nasal and temporal RF diameters into close registration. RF diameters were multiplied by 2/2 to transform from Derrington and Lennie's convention (width of a fitted Gaussian at 1/e height) to mine (2 SDs of the same Gaussian fit).

Magnocellular RF diameter increased with temporal equivalent eccentricity. The equation of the least-squares best-fit line was:

log10(RFdiameter)=0.03446x1.24594, (13)

where x is temporal equivalent RF eccentricity in degrees of visual angle and RFdiameter is 2 SDs of an assumed Gaussian RF profile, also in degrees of visual angle. This formula was used to estimate the RF size of each recorded magnocellular neuron and statistical replicas. Neuronal pool sizes used in the population analyses are given in S5B Fig.

RF size estimates: Parvocellular neurons

Derrington and Lennie [53] also measured parvocellular RF sizes, but imperfect optics likely inflated their estimates [84, 136]. Parvocellular RF sizes were therefore estimated from midget retinal ganglion cell RF sizes. Midget cells provide the driving input to parvocellular LGN neurons, and convergence from midget cells to parvocellular LGN neurons appears to be minimal [137139].

Parvocellular RF sizes were estimated on the basis of a model of midget RF density in the human retina [81]. Under this model, midget RF density varies with eccentricity along the temporal retina according to

density=29609(1+x41.03)1×[0.9729(1+x1.084)2+(10.9729)exp(x7.633)], (14)

where x is retinal eccentricity in degrees.

RFs are assumed to form a hexagonal mosaic. The spacing between adjacent midget RFs within each mosaic is therefore approximately

RFspacing=23density2, (15)

where density is halved to account for the fact that the model in Eq 14 includes both ON and OFF cells (see Eq 9 of [81] and associated text). The spacing between neighboring RF centers was assumed to be 2 SDs of a Gaussian profile, so RF spacing dictates RF size. Midget RFs are slightly larger in humans than in macaques [82]. Multiplying RFspacing by 0.8 accounted for this difference and led to a 4:1 ratio between parasol and midget RF areas at large eccentricities, which is a reasonable approximation [83, 140].

During LGN recordings, monkeys viewed the stimuli binocularly to match the viewing conditions used during the psychophysical experiments [27]. On the assumption that roughly half of the neurons had RFs in the nasal retina, and the other half had RFs in the temporal retina, the horizontal component of each recorded LGN neuron's RF position was divided by 0.8 to undo half of the multiplicative scale factor (0.61) used to equate RF sizes between nasal and temporal retinae. This minor adjustment had minimal impact on the results.

Supporting information

S1 Fig

Signal-to-noise ratios (d′ values) of individual LGN neurons (black), currents in cones within their RFs (red), and photon absorptions in the same cones (cyan). Points are means and shaded bands are ±1 SEM. Dashed line indicates the signal-to-noise ratio assumed at the level of behavior on the basis of performance in a two-alternative, forced-choice contrast detection task (d′ = 1.27 or 82% correct). This analysis is identical to the analysis in Fig 5 of the main text except that the spatial integration window of the ideal observers was constrained to a single monocular LGN RF. This analysis does not require assumptions about pools of LGN neurons or interneuronal correlations. Notice that the relative signal-to-noise ratio is similar in small spatial integration windows (this figure) and windows that were, on average, 74 times larger (Fig 5). The mean signal-to-noise ratio across individual LGN neurons (black traces) also appears in Fig 4 of the main text. Data are available at https://github.com/horwitzlab/LGN-temporal-contrast-sensitivity/blob/master/DataByFigure.xlsx. LGN, lateral geniculate nucleus; RF, receptive field.

(PDF)

S2 Fig

Signal-to-noise ratios (population d′ values, see Methods) calculated from single cycles of LGN neuronal responses (black) and simulated cone currents (red) as a function of temporal frequency. Points are means and shaded bands are ±1 SEM. Dashed line indicates the signal-to-noise ratio assumed at the level of behavior (d′ = 1.27 or 82% accuracy). Reducing the temporal integration window to include a single stimulus cycle resulted in signal-to-noise ratios being roughly constant across temporal frequency. (A) Magnocellular data from monkey 1. (B) Magnocellular data from monkey 2. (C) Parvocellular data from monkey 1. (D) Parvocellular data from monkey 2. Data are available at https://github.com/horwitzlab/LGN-temporal-contrast-sensitivity/blob/master/DataByFigure.xlsx. LGN, lateral geniculate nucleus.

(PDF)

S3 Fig

Signal-to-noise ratios (d′ values) of cone currents (red) and LGN populations under several pooling models. Conventions are identical to Fig 5 of the main text. LGN population scale factors were computed as described in the main text (black), in the absence of within-mosaic interneuronal correlations (yellow), after doubling RF diameter (blue), or after halving RF diameter (purple). All of these manipulations affect population scale factors and therefore scale the d′ curves. (A) Magnocellular data from monkey 1. (B) Magnocellular data from monkey 2. (C) Parvocellular data from monkey 1. (D) Parvocellular data from monkey 2. Data are available at https://github.com/horwitzlab/LGN-temporal-contrast-sensitivity/blob/master/DataByFigure.xlsx. LGN, lateral geniculate nucleus. RF, receptive field.

(PDF)

S4 Fig. Area under the ROC (ordinate) as a function of d′ (abscissa).

ROC curves were computed from dM2 values (see Methods, Eq 9). Each symbol represents the data from a single LGN neuron and a single temporal frequency. Data from parvocellular (gray) and magnocellular (black) neurons are shown from monkey 1 (circles) and monkey 2 (triangles). The red curve is the prediction from Gaussian signal and noise distributions with identical SD. Data are available at https://github.com/horwitzlab/LGN-temporal-contrast-sensitivity/blob/master/DataByFigure.xlsx. LGN, lateral geniculate nucleus; ROC, receiver operating characteristic; SD, standard deviation.

(PDF)

S5 Fig

(A) Magnocellular and parvocellular RF diameter (2 SDs of a Gaussian fit) as a function of temporal equivalent retinal eccentricity. Points are RFs in the temporal (+) and nasal (circles) retinae from Derrington and Lennie [53]. The solid line is a least-squares fit. Parvocellular RF sizes were estimated from the model proposed by Watson [81] and shifted down by a factor of 0.8 to account for the smaller RF sizes of macaques [82] (dashed curve). (B) Number of LGN neurons assumed in each ideal observer pool as a function of eccentricity. Pool sizes were calculated on the basis of RF size, the assumption of a hexagonal RF lattice, and the retinal size of the stimulus (a Gabor with 0.15° SD envelope, truncated at ±2 SDs). Open circles represent magnocellular neurons and closed symbols represent parvocellular neurons. Circles and triangles represent neurons from monkeys 1 and 2, respectively. Data are available at https://github.com/horwitzlab/LGN-temporal-contrast-sensitivity/blob/master/DataByFigure.xlsx. LGN, lateral geniculate nucleus; RF, receptive field; SD, standard deviation.

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

I am grateful to Emily Gelfand for help with monkey surgeries, training, and behavioral data collection; Zack Lindbloom-Brown for computer programming; Fred Rieke for helpful discussions; Juan Angueyra and Charlie Hass for the cone current model; and the veterinary staff at the Washington National Primate Center for animal care and husbandry. Greg Field, Abhishek De, and Lisa McConnell provided helpful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript.

Abbreviations

LGN

lateral geniculate nucleus

RF

receptive field

SNR

signal-to-noise ratio

t-SNE

t-distributed stochastic neighbor embedding

Data Availability

Data and analysis code are available at https://github.com/horwitzlab/LGN-temporal-contrast-sensitivity.

Funding Statement

Funding was provided by grants EY018849 from the National Eye Institute and OD010425 from the Office of Research Infrastructure Programs. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Crick F, Koch C. Are we aware of neural activity in primary visual cortex? Nature. 1995;375(6527):121–3. Epub 1995/05/11. 10.1038/375121a0 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Delgutte B. Physiological models for basic auditory percepts Auditory computation. New York, NY: Springer; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Buchsbaum G, Gottschalk A. Trichromacy, opponent colours coding and optimum colour information transmission in the retina. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1983;220(1218):89–113. 10.1098/rspb.1983.0090 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Werner G, Mountcastle VB. Neural activity in mechanoreceptive cutaneous afferents: Stimulus-response relations, Weber functions, and information transmission. J Neurophysiol. 1965;28:359–97. 10.1152/jn.1965.28.2.359 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Stetson C, Fiesta MP, Eagleman DM. Does time really slow down during a frightening event? PLoS ONE. 2007;2(12). 10.1371/journal.pone.0001295 WOS:000207459500014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Skottun BC. On using very high temporal frequencies to isolate magnocellular contributions to psychophysical tasks. Neuropsychologia. 2013;51(8):1556–60. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.05.009 WOS:000321938800020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Gur M, Snodderly DM. A dissociation between brain activity and perception: Chromatically opponent cortical neurons signal chromatic flicker that is not perceived. Vision Res. 1997;37(4):377–82. Epub 1997/02/01. 10.1016/s0042-6989(96)00183-6 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Krolak-Salmon P, Henaff MA, Tallon-Baudry C, Yvert B, Guenot M, Vighetto A, et al. Human lateral geniculate nucleus and visual cortex respond to screen flicker. Ann Neurol. 2003;53(1):73–80. Epub 2003/01/02. 10.1002/ana.10403 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Williams PE, Mechler F, Gordon J, Shapley R, Hawken MJ. Entrainment to video displays in primary visual cortex of macaque and humans. J Neurosci. 2004;24(38):8278–88. Epub 2004/09/24. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2716-04.2004 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Vul E, MacLeod DI. Contingent aftereffects distinguish conscious and preconscious color processing. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9(7):873–4. 10.1038/nn1723 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Falconbridge M, Ware A, MacLeod DI. Imperceptibly rapid contrast modulations processed in cortex: Evidence from psychophysics. J Vis. 2010;10(8):21 Epub 2010/10/05. 10.1167/10.8.21 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Yeh T, Lee BB, Kremers J. Temporal response of ganglion cells of the macaque retina to cone-specific modulation. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 1995;12(3):456–64. Epub 1995/03/01. 10.1364/josaa.12.000456 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Lee BB, Pokorny J, Smith VC, Martin PR, Valberg A. Luminance and chromatic modulation sensitivity of macaque ganglion cells and human observers. J Opt Soc Am A. 1990;7(12):2223–36. Epub 1990/12/01. 10.1364/josaa.7.002223 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Lee BB, Martin PR, Valberg A. Sensitivity of macaque retinal ganglion cells to chromatic and luminance flicker. J Physiol. 1989;414:223–43. Epub 1989/07/01. 10.1113/jphysiol.1989.sp017685 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Hass CA, Horwitz GD. V1 mechanisms underlying chromatic contrast detection. J Neurophysiol. 2013;109(10):2483–94. Epub 2013/03/01. 10.1152/jn.00671.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Palmer C, Cheng SY, Seidemann E. Linking neuronal and behavioral performance in a reaction-time visual detection task. J Neurosci. 2007;27(30):8122–37. Epub 2007/07/27. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1940-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Allen D, Hess RF. Is the visual field temporally homogeneous? Vision Res. 1992;32(6):1075–84. 10.1016/0042-6989(92)90008-7 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Baron WS, Boynton RM. Response of primate cones to sinusoidally flickering homochromatic stimuli. J Physiol. 1975;246(2):311–31. 10.1113/jphysiol.1975.sp010892 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Angueyra JM, Rieke F. Origin and effect of phototransduction noise in primate cone photoreceptors. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16(11):1692–700. Epub 2013/10/08. 10.1038/nn.3534 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Seiple W, Holopigian K. Outer-retina locus of increased flicker sensitivity of the peripheral retina. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 1996;13(3):658–66. 10.1364/josaa.13.000658 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Seiple W, Holopigian K, Greenstein V, Hood DC. Temporal frequency dependent adaptation at the level of the outer retina in humans. Vision Res. 1992;32(11):2043–8. 10.1016/0042-6989(92)90065-q . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Schnapf JL, Nunn BJ, Meister M, Baylor DA. Visual transduction in cones of the monkey Macaca fascicularis. J Physiol. 1990;427:681–713. 10.1113/jphysiol.1990.sp018193 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Sinha R, Hoon M, Baudin J, Okawa H, Wong ROL, Rieke F. Cellular and circuit mechanisms shaping the perceptual properties of the primate fovea. Cell. 2017;168(3):413–26 e12. 10.1016/j.cell.2017.01.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Tyler CW. Analysis of visual modulation sensitivity. II. Peripheral retina and the role of photoreceptor dimensions. J Opt Soc Am A. 1985;2(3):393–8. 10.1364/josaa.2.000393 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Hass CA, Angueyra JM, Lindbloom-Brown Z, Rieke F, Horwitz GD. Chromatic detection from cone photoreceptors to V1 neurons to behavior in rhesus monkeys. J Vis. 2015;15(15):1 10.1167/15.15.1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Geisler WS. Sequential ideal-observer analysis of visual discriminations. Psychological Review. 1989;96(2):267–314. 10.1037/0033-295x.96.2.267 WOS:A1989T957000005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Gelfand EC, Horwitz GD. Model of parafoveal chromatic and luminance temporal contrast sensitivity of humans and monkeys. J Vis. 2018;18(12):1 10.1167/18.12.1 30383213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Jiang Y, Yampolsky D, Purushothaman G, Casagrande VA. Perceptual decision related activity in the lateral geniculate nucleus. J Neurophysiol. 2015;114(1):717–35. 10.1152/jn.00068.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Jiang Y, Purushothaman G, Casagrande VA. A computational relationship between thalamic sensory neural responses and contrast perception. Front Neural Circuits. 2015;9:54 10.3389/fncir.2015.00054 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Rovamo J, Raninen A, Lukkarinen S, Donner K. Flicker sensitivity as a function of spectral density of external white temporal noise. Vision Res. 1996;36(23):3767–74. 10.1016/0042-6989(96)00108-3 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Rovamo J, Kukkonen H, Raninen A, Donner K. Efficiency of temporal integration of sinusoidal flicker. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2003;44(11):5049–55. 10.1167/iovs.02-1082 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Howell ER, Hess RF. The functional area for summation to threshold for sinusoidal gratings. Vision Res. 1978;18(4):369–74. 10.1016/0042-6989(78)90045-7 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Pelli DG. Uncertainty explains many aspects of visual contrast detection and discrimination. J Opt Soc Am A. 1985;2(9):1508–32. 10.1364/josaa.2.001508 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Koenderink JJ, Bouman MA, Bueno de Mesquita AE, Slappendel S. Perimetry of contrast detection thresholds of moving spatial sine wave patterns. III. The target extent as a sensitivity controlling parameter. J Opt Soc Am. 1978;68(6):854–60. 10.1364/josa.68.000854 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Barlow HB. Temporal and spatial summation in human vision at different background intensities. The Journal of Physiology. 1958;141(2):337–50. 10.1113/jphysiol.1958.sp005978 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Gorea A, Tyler CW. New look at Bloch's law for contrast. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, Optics and Image Science. 1986;3(1):52–61. 10.1364/josaa.3.000052 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Watson AB. Temporal sensitivity. Handbook of Perception and Human Performance. 1986;1(6):1–43. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Borghuis BG, Sterling P, Smith RG. Loss of sensitivity in an analog neural circuit. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29(10):3045–58. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5071-08.2009 WOS:000264093200006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.van der Maaten L, Hinton G. Visualizing data using t-SNE. J Mach Learn Res. 2008;9:2579–605. WOS:000262637600007. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Snowden RJ, Braddick OJ. The temporal integration and resolution of velocity signals. Vision Res. 1991;31(5):907–14. 10.1016/0042-6989(91)90156-y . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.De Bruyn B, Orban GA. Human velocity and direction discrimination measured with random dot patterns. Vision Res. 1988;28(12):1323–35. 10.1016/0042-6989(88)90064-8 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Borghuis BG, Tadin D, Lankheet MJ, Lappin JS, van de Grind WA. Temporal limits to visual motion processing: Psychophysics and neurophysiology. Vision. 2019;3(5). 10.3390/vision3010005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Lee BB, Sun H, Zucchini W. The temporal properties of the response of macaque ganglion cells and central mechanisms of flicker detection. J Vis. 2007;7(14):11–6. Epub 2008/01/26. 10.1167/7.14.11 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Cohen MR, Newsome WT. Estimates of the contribution of single neurons to perception depend on timescale and noise correlation. J Neurosci. 2009;29(20):6635–48. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5179-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Wright MJ, Johnston A. Spatiotemporal contrast sensitivity and visual field locus. Vision Res. 1983;23(10):983–9. 10.1016/0042-6989(83)90008-1 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Green DM, Swets JA. Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: Wiley; 1966. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Mukherjee P, Kaplan E. Dynamics of neurons in the cat lateral geniculate nucleus: In vivo electrophysiology and computational modeling. J Neurophysiol. 1995;74(3):1222–43. 10.1152/jn.1995.74.3.1222 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Troy JB, Robson JG. Steady discharges of X and Y retinal ganglion cells of cat under photopic illuminance. Vis Neurosci. 1992;9(6):535–53. 10.1017/s0952523800001784 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Snodderly DM. A physiological perspective on fixational eye movements. Vision Res. 2016;118:31–47. 10.1016/j.visres.2014.12.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Martinez-Conde S, Macknik SL, Hubel DH. The role of fixational eye movements in visual perception. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5(3):229–40. Epub 2004/02/21. 10.1038/nrn1348 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Britten KH, Shadlen MN, Newsome WT, Movshon JA. The analysis of visual motion: A comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performance. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1992;12(12):4745–65. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-12-04745.1992 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Shapley R. Visual sensitivity and parallel retinocortical channels. Annu Rev Psychol. 1990;41:635–58. 10.1146/annurev.ps.41.020190.003223 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Derrington AM, Lennie P. Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivities of neurones in lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque. J Physiol. 1984;357:219–40. Epub 1984/12/01. 10.1113/jphysiol.1984.sp015498 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Croner LJ, Kaplan E. Receptive fields of P and M ganglion cells across the primate retina. Vision Res. 1995;35(1):7–24. 10.1016/0042-6989(94)e0066-t . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Levitt JB, Schumer RA, Sherman SM, Spear PD, Movshon JA. Visual response properties of neurons in the LGN of normally reared and visually deprived macaque monkeys. J Neurophysiol. 2001;85(5):2111–29. 10.1152/jn.2001.85.5.2111 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Purpura K, Tranchina D, Kaplan E, Shapley RM. Light adaptation in the primate retina: Analysis of changes in gain and dynamics of monkey retinal ganglion cells. Vis Neurosci. 1990;4(1):75–93. Epub 1990/01/01. 10.1017/s0952523800002789 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Gielen CCAM. Spatio-temporal and chromatic properties of visual neurones in the rhesus monkey geniculate nucleus [dissertation]. Nijmegen: University of Nijmegen; 1980.
  • 58.Kaplan E, Shapley RM. X and Y cells in the lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque monkeys. J Physiol. 1982;330:125–43. Epub 1982/09/01. 10.1113/jphysiol.1982.sp014333 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Hawken MJ, Shapley RM, Grosof DH. Temporal-frequency selectivity in monkey visual cortex. Vis Neurosci. 1996;13(3):477–92. Epub 1996/05/01. 10.1017/s0952523800008154 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Laycock R, Crewther DP, Crewther SG. Abrupt and ramped flicker-defined form shows evidence for a large magnocellular impairment in dyslexia. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50(8):2107–13. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.05.012 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Skottun BC. The magnocellular deficit theory of dyslexia: The evidence from contrast sensitivity. Vision Res. 2000;40(1):111–27. 10.1016/s0042-6989(99)00170-4 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Johnston A, Bruno A, Watanabe J, Quansah B, Patel N, Dakin S, et al. Visually-based temporal distortion in dyslexia. Vision research. 2008;48(17):1852–8. 10.1016/j.visres.2008.04.029 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.King-Smith PE, Carden D. Luminance and opponent-color contributions to visual detection and adaptation and to temporal and spatial integration. J Opt Soc Am. 1976;66(7):709–17. 10.1364/josa.66.000709 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Roufs JA. Dynamic properties of vision. II. Theoretical relationships between flicker and flash thresholds. Vision Res. 1972;12(2):279–92. 10.1016/0042-6989(72)90118-6 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Wang HX, Freeman J, Merriam EP, Hasson U, Heeger DJ. Temporal eye movement strategies during naturalistic viewing. J Vis. 2012;12(1):16 10.1167/12.1.16 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3273487. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Yarbus AL. Eye Movements and Vision. New York, NY: Plenus Press; 1967. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Merigan WH, Maunsell JH. Macaque vision after magnocellular lateral geniculate lesions. Vis Neurosci. 1990;5(4):347–52. 10.1017/s0952523800000432 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Merigan WH, Katz LM, Maunsell JH. The effects of parvocellular lateral geniculate lesions on the acuity and contrast sensitivity of macaque monkeys. J Neurosci. 1991;11(4):994–1001. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-04-00994.1991 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Hicks TP, Lee BB, Vidyasagar TR. The responses of cells in macaque lateral geniculate nucleus to sinusoidal gratings. J Physiol. 1983;337:183–200. 10.1113/jphysiol.1983.sp014619 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Alitto HJ, Moore BDt, Rathbun DL, Usrey WM. A comparison of visual responses in the lateral geniculate nucleus of alert and anaesthetized macaque monkeys. J Physiol. 2011;589(Pt 1):87–99. 10.1113/jphysiol.2010.190538 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Watson AB, Ahumada AJ Jr. A standard model for foveal detection of spatial contrast. J Vis. 2005;5(9):717–40. 10.1167/5.9.6 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Virsu V, Rovamo J. Visual resolution, contrast sensitivity, and the cortical magnification factor. Exp Brain Res. 1979;37(3):475–94. 10.1007/bf00236818 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Raninen A, Rovamo J. Perimetry of critical flicker frequency in human rod and cone vision. Vision Res. 1986;26(8):1249–55. 10.1016/0042-6989(86)90105-7 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Crook JM, Lee BB, Tigwell DA, Valberg A. Thresholds to chromatic spots of cells in the macaque geniculate nucleus as compared to detection sensitivity in man. J Physiol. 1987;392:193–211. 10.1113/jphysiol.1987.sp016776 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Kremers J, Lee BB, Kaiser PK. Sensitivity of macaque retinal ganglion cells and human observers to combined luminance and chromatic temporal modulation. J Opt Soc Am A. 1992;9(9):1477–85. Epub 1992/09/01. 10.1364/josaa.9.001477 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Spekreijse H, van Norren D, van den Berg TJ. Flicker responses in monkey lateral geniculate nucleus and human perception of flicker. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1971;68(11):2802–5. Epub 1971/11/01. 10.1073/pnas.68.11.2802 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Lee BB, Martin PR, Valberg A, Kremers J. Physiological mechanisms underlying psychophysical sensitivity to combined luminance and chromatic modulation. J Opt Soc Am A. 1993;10(6):1403–12. Epub 1993/06/01. 10.1364/josaa.10.001403 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Nienborg H, Cumming B. Correlations between the activity of sensory neurons and behavior: How much do they tell us about a neuron's causality? Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2010;20(3):376–81. 10.1016/j.conb.2010.05.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Watanabe M, Rodieck RW. Parasol and midget ganglion cells of the primate retina. J Comp Neurol. 1989;289(3):434–54. 10.1002/cne.902890308 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Perry VH, Oehler R, Cowey A. Retinal ganglion cells that project to the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus in the macaque monkey. Neuroscience. 1984;12(4):1101–23. Epub 1984/08/01. 10.1016/0306-4522(84)90006-x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Watson AB. A formula for human retinal ganglion cell receptive field density as a function of visual field location. J Vis. 2014;14(7). 10.1167/14.7.15 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Dacey DM, Petersen MR. Dendritic field size and morphology of midget and parasol ganglion cells of the human retina. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1992;89(20):9666–70. 10.1073/pnas.89.20.9666 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Gauthier JL, Field GD, Sher A, Shlens J, Greschner M, Litke AM, et al. Uniform signal redundancy of parasol and midget ganglion cells in primate retina. J Neurosci. 2009;29(14):4675–80. Epub 2009/04/10. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5294-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Lee BB. Receptor inputs to primate ganglion cells In: Gegenfurtner KR, Sharpe LT, editors. Color Vision: From Genes to Perception. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 1999. p. 203–17. [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Dan Y, Alonso JM, Usrey WM, Reid RC. Coding of visual information by precisely correlated spikes in the lateral geniculate nucleus. Nature neuroscience. 1998;1(6):501–7. 10.1038/2217 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Alonso JM, Usrey WM, Reid RC. Precisely correlated firing in cells of the lateral geniculate nucleus. Nature. 1996;383(6603):815–9. Epub 1996/10/31. 10.1038/383815a0 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Greschner M, Shlens J, Bakolitsa C, Field GD, Gauthier JL, Jepson LH, et al. Correlated firing among major ganglion cell types in primate retina. J Physiol. 2011;589(Pt 1):75–86. 10.1113/jphysiol.2010.193888 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Ala-Laurila P, Greschner M, Chichilnisky EJ, Rieke F. Cone photoreceptor contributions to noise and correlations in the retinal output. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14(10):1309–16. 10.1038/nn.2927 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Trong PK, Rieke F. Origin of correlated activity between parasol retinal ganglion cells. Nat Neurosci. 2008;11(11):1343–51. 10.1038/nn.2199 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Perry VH, Cowey A. The ganglion cell and cone distributions in the monkey's retina: Implications for central magnification factors. Vision Res. 1985;25(12):1795–810. 10.1016/0042-6989(85)90004-5 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Goodchild AK, Ghosh KK, Martin PR. Comparison of photoreceptor spatial density and ganglion cell morphology in the retina of human, macaque monkey, cat, and the marmoset Callithrix jacchus. J Comp Neurol. 1996;366(1):55–75. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Packer O, Hendrickson AE, Curcio CA. Photoreceptor topography of the retina in the adult pigtail macaque (Macaca nemestrina). J Comp Neurol. 1989;288(1):165–83. 10.1002/cne.902880113 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Schneeweis DM, Schnapf JL. The photovoltage of macaque cone photoreceptors: Adaptation, noise, and kinetics. J Neurosci. 1999;19(4):1203–16. Epub 1999/02/10. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-04-01203.1999 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Schneeweis DM, Schnapf JL. Photovoltage of rods and cones in the macaque retina. Science. 1995;268(5213):1053–6. 10.1126/science.7754386 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Lennie P. Roles of M and P pathways In: Shapley R, Lam DM, editors. Contrast Sensitivity. 5. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1993. p. 201–14. [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Curcio CA, Sloan KR Jr., Packer O, Hendrickson AE, Kalina RE. Distribution of cones in human and monkey retina: Individual variability and radial asymmetry. Science. 1987;236(4801):579–82. 10.1126/science.3576186 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Van Essen DC, Newsome WT, Maunsell JH. The visual field representation in striate cortex of the macaque monkey: Asymmetries, anisotropies, and individual variability. Vision Res. 1984;24(5):429–48. 10.1016/0042-6989(84)90041-5 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Kaplan E, Mukherjee P, Shapley R. Information filtering in the lateral geniculate nucleus In: Shapley R, Lam DM, editors. Contrast Sensitivity. 5. Cambridge, MA: MIT press; 1993. p. 183–200. [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Carmel D, Saker P, Rees G, Lavie N. Perceptual load modulates conscious flicker perception. J Vis. 2007;7(14):14 1–3. Epub 2008/01/26. 10.1167/7.14.14 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Bezdudnaya T, Cano M, Bereshpolova Y, Stoelzel CR, Alonso JM, Swadlow HA. Thalamic burst mode and inattention in the awake LGNd. Neuron. 2006;49(3):421–32. 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.01.010 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Di Stasi LL, McCamy MB, Catena A, Macknik SL, Canas JJ, Martinez-Conde S. Microsaccade and drift dynamics reflect mental fatigue. Eur J Neurosci. 2013;38(3):2389–98. 10.1111/ejn.12248 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Sperling HG, Crawford ML, Espinoza S. Threshold spectral sensitivity of single neurons in the lateral geniculate nucleus and of performing monkeys. Mod Probl Ophthalmol. 1978;19:2–18. . [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Bender DB, Youakim M. Effect of attentive fixation in macaque thalamus and cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2001;85(1):219–34. 10.1152/jn.2001.85.1.219 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Kelly DH. Spatio-temporal frequency characteristics of color-vision mechanisms. J Opt Soc Am. 1974;64(7):983–90. 10.1364/josa.64.000983 WOS:A1974T643900012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Rovamo J, Raninen A, Donner K. The effects of temporal noise and retinal illuminance on foveal flicker sensitivity. Vision research. 1999;39(3):533–50. 10.1016/s0042-6989(98)00120-5 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Matthews HR, Sampath AP. Photopigment quenching is Ca2+ dependent and controls response duration in salamander L-cone photoreceptors. J Gen Physiol. 2010;135(4):355–66. 10.1085/jgp.200910394 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Barten PGJ. Spatiotemporal model for the contrast sensitivity of the human eye and its temporal aspects. Human vision, visual processing, and digital display IV. 1993;1913:2–15. [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Robson JG. Spatial and temporal contrast-sensitivity functions of visual system. J Opt Soc Am. 1966;56(8):1141–&. 10.1364/Josa.56.001141 WOS:A19668113400031. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Shapley RM, Victor JD. How the contrast gain control modifies the frequency responses of cat retinal ganglion cells. J Physiol. 1981;318:161–79. 10.1113/jphysiol.1981.sp013856 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Benardete EA, Kaplan E. The dynamics of primate M retinal ganglion cells. Vis Neurosci. 1999;16(2):355–68. Epub 1999/06/15. 10.1017/s0952523899162151 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Yeh T, Lee BB, Kremers J. The time course of adaptation in macaque retinal ganglion cells. Vision Res. 1996;36(7):913–31. 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00332-0 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Hayhoe MM, Benimoff NI, Hood DC. The time-course of multiplicative and subtractive adaptation process. Vision Res. 1987;27(11):1981–96. 10.1016/0042-6989(87)90062-9 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Enroth-Cugell C, Shapley RM. Adaptation and dynamics of cat retinal ganglion cells. J Physiol. 1973;233(2):271–309. 10.1113/jphysiol.1973.sp010308 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Clark DA, Benichou R, Meister M, Azeredo da Silveira R. Dynamical adaptation in photoreceptors. PLoS Comput Biol. 2013;9(11):e1003289 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003289 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Korenbrot JI. Speed, sensitivity, and stability of the light response in rod and cone photoreceptors: Facts and models. Progress in Retinal and Eye Research. 2012;31(5):442–66. 10.1016/j.preteyeres.2012.05.002 WOS:000307089400004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Suarez H, Koch C, Douglas R. Modeling direction selectivity of simple cells in striate visual cortex within the framework of the canonical microcircuit. J Neurosci. 1995;15(10):6700–19. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-10-06700.1995 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Chance FS, Nelson SB, Abbott LF. Synaptic depression and the temporal response characteristics of V1 cells. J Neurosci. 1998;18(12):4785–99. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-12-04785.1998 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Carandini M, Heeger DJ, Senn W. A synaptic explanation of suppression in visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2002;22(22):10053–65. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-22-10053.2002 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Fortune ES, Rose GJ. Short-term synaptic plasticity as a temporal filter. Trends Neurosci. 2001;24(7):381–5. 10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01835-x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Krukowski AE, Miller KD. Thalamocortical NMDA conductances and intracortical inhibition can explain cortical temporal tuning. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4(4):424–30. 10.1038/86084 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Usrey WM, Alonso JM, Reid RC. Synaptic interactions between thalamic inputs to simple cells in cat visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2000;20(14):5461–7. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-14-05461.2000 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Jiang Y, Zhou K, He S. Human visual cortex responds to invisible chromatic flicker. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10(5):657–62. Epub 2007/03/31. 10.1038/nn1879 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Weatherall D. The use of non-human primates in research. London: 2006.
  • 124.Chichilnisky EJ. A simple white noise analysis of neuronal light responses. Network. 2001;12(2):199–213. . [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Horwitz GD, Chichilnisky EJ, Albright TD. Cone inputs to simple and complex cells in V1 of awake macaque. J Neurophysiol. 2007;97(4):3070–81. Epub 2007/02/17. 10.1152/jn.00965.2006 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Stockman A, MacLeod DI, Johnson NE. Spectral sensitivities of the human cones. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 1993;10(12):2491–521. Epub 1993/12/01. 10.1364/josaa.10.002491 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Watson AB, Pelli DG. QUEST: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method. Percept Psychophys. 1983;33(2):113–20. Epub 1983/02/01. 10.3758/bf03202828 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Tanner WP Jr., Swets JA. A decision-making theory of visual detection. Psychol Rev. 1954;61(6):401–9. 10.1037/h0058700 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Wallis SA, Baker DH, Meese TS, Georgeson MA. The slope of the psychometric function and non-stationarity of thresholds in spatiotemporal contrast vision. Vision Res. 2013;76:1–10. 10.1016/j.visres.2012.09.019 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Croner LJ, Purpura K, Kaplan E. Response variability in retinal ganglion cells of primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1993;90(17):8128–30. 10.1073/pnas.90.17.8128 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Tolhurst DJ, Movshon JA, Dean AF. The statistical reliability of signals in single neurons in cat and monkey visual cortex. Vision Res. 1983;23(8):775–85. 10.1016/0042-6989(83)90200-6 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Rodieck RW. Quantitative analysis of cat retinal ganglion cell response to visual stimuli. Vision Res. 1965;5(11):583–601. 10.1016/0042-6989(65)90033-7 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Li PH, Field GD, Greschner M, Ahn D, Gunning DE, Mathieson K, et al. Retinal representation of the elementary visual signal. Neuron. 2014;81(1):130–9. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.043 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Rabbani S. Derivation of the matched filter as highest SNR linear filter. 2006. Available from: https://srabbani.com/matched_filter.pdf.
  • 135.Leon-Garcia A. Probability and Random Processes for Electrical Engineering. 2nd ed Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Campbell FW, Gubisch RW. Optical quality of the human eye. J Physiol. 1966;186(3):558–78. 10.1113/jphysiol.1966.sp008056 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Sincich LC, Adams DL, Economides JR, Horton JC. Transmission of spike trains at the retinogeniculate synapse. J Neurosci. 2007;27(10):2683–92. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5077-06.2007 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Sincich LC, Zhang Y, Tiruveedhula P, Horton JC, Roorda A. Resolving single cone inputs to visual receptive fields. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12(8):967–9. 10.1038/nn.2352 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Usrey WM, Reppas JB, Reid RC. Specificity and strength of retinogeniculate connections. J Neurophysiol. 1999;82(6):3527–40. 10.1152/jn.1999.82.6.3527 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Gauthier JL, Field GD, Sher A, Greschner M, Shlens J, Litke AM, et al. Receptive fields in primate retina are coordinated to sample visual space more uniformly. PLoS Biol. 2009;7(4):e1000063 Epub 2009/04/10. 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000063 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Cottaris NP, Wandell BA, Rieke F, Brainard DH. A computational observer model of spatial contrast sensitivity: Effects of photocurrent encoding, fixational eye movements and inference engine. bioRxiv 759811 [Preprint]. 2019. 10.1101/759811 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Gabriel Gasque

4 Nov 2019

Dear Greg,

Thank you for submitting your revised manuscript entitled "Temporal information loss in the macaque early visual system" for re-consideration as a Research Article by PLOS Biology. As I mentioned previously, all reviewers have now submitted their comments and the decision is almost ready.

However, before we can communicate it, we need you to complete your submission by providing the metadata that is required for full assessment. (I should have asked you to do this *before* I sent the paper to reviewers, but I forgot – I am really sorry).

Please login to Editorial Manager where you will find the paper in the 'Submissions Needing Revisions' folder on your homepage. Please click 'Revise Submission' from the Action Links and complete all additional questions in the submission questionnaire.

Please re-submit your manuscript within two working days, i.e. by Nov 06 2019 11:59PM.

Login to Editorial Manager here: https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology

During resubmission, you will be invited to opt-in to posting your pre-review manuscript as a bioRxiv preprint. Visit http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/preprints for full details. If you consent to posting your current manuscript as a preprint, please upload a single Preprint PDF when you re-submit.

Once your full submission is complete, your paper will undergo a series of checks. Once your manuscript has passed all checks, I'll do the assessment and will communicate the final decision.

Feel free to email us at plosbiology@plos.org if you have any queries relating to your submission.

Kind regards,

Gabriel Gasque, Ph.D.,

Senior Editor

PLOS Biology

Decision Letter 1

Gabriel Gasque

5 Nov 2019

Dear Greg,

Thank you for re-submitting your revised Research Article entitled "Temporal information loss in the macaque early visual system" for publication in PLOS Biology. I have now obtained advice from the original reviewers and have discussed their comments with the Academic Editor.

Based on the reviews, we will probably accept this manuscript for publication, assuming that you will modify the manuscript to address the remaining points raised by reviewers 1 and 2. Please also make sure to address the data and other policy-related requests noted at the end of this email.

Please submit a file detailing your responses to the editorial requests and a point-by-point response to all of the reviewers' comments that indicates the changes you have made to the manuscript. In addition to a clean copy of the manuscript, please upload a 'track-changes' version of your manuscript that specifies the edits made. This should be uploaded as a "Related" file type.

We expect to receive your revised manuscript within two weeks. Your revisions should address the specific points made by each reviewer. In addition to the remaining revisions and before we will be able to formally accept your manuscript and consider it "in press", we also need to ensure that your article conforms to our guidelines. A member of our team will be in touch shortly with a set of requests. As we can't proceed until these requirements are met, your swift response will help prevent delays to publication.

*Copyediting*

Upon acceptance of your article, your final files will be copyedited and typeset into the final PDF. While you will have an opportunity to review these files as proofs, PLOS will only permit corrections to spelling or significant scientific errors. Therefore, please take this final revision time to assess and make any remaining major changes to your manuscript.

NOTE: If Supporting Information files are included with your article, note that these are not copyedited and will be published as they are submitted. Please ensure that these files are legible and of high quality (at least 300 dpi) in an easily accessible file format. For this reason, please be aware that any references listed in an SI file will not be indexed. For more information, see our Supporting Information guidelines:

https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/supporting-information

*Published Peer Review History*

Please note that you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out. Please see here for more details:

https://blogs.plos.org/plos/2019/05/plos-journals-now-open-for-published-peer-review/

*Early Version*

Please note that an uncorrected proof of your manuscript will be published online ahead of the final version, unless you opted out when submitting your manuscript. If, for any reason, you do not want an earlier version of your manuscript published online, uncheck the box. Should you, your institution's press office or the journal office choose to press release your paper, you will automatically be opted out of early publication. We ask that you notify us as soon as possible if you or your institution is planning to press release the article.

*Submitting Your Revision*

To submit your revision, please go to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/ and log in as an Author. Click the link labelled 'Submissions Needing Revision' to find your submission record. Your revised submission must include a cover letter, a Response to Reviewers file that provides a detailed response to the reviewers' comments (if applicable), and a track-changes file indicating any changes that you have made to the manuscript.

Please do not hesitate to contact me should you have any questions.

Sincerely,

Gabriel Gasque, Ph.D.,

Senior Editor

PLOS Biology

------------------------------------------------------------------------

ETHICS STATEMENT:

The Ethics Statements in the submission form and Methods section of your manuscript should match verbatim. Please ensure that any changes are made to both versions.

-- Please include an ID number of the protocol(s) approved by the University of Washington Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.

------------------------------------------------------------------------

DATA POLICY:

You may be aware of the PLOS Data Policy, which requires that all data be made available without restriction: http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/data-availability. For more information, please also see this editorial: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001797

Note that we do not require all raw data. Rather, we ask for all individual quantitative observations that underlie the data summarized in the figures and results of your paper. For an example see here: http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pbio.1001908#s5

These data can be made available in one of the following forms:

1) Supplementary files (e.g., excel). Please ensure that all data files are uploaded as 'Supporting Information' and are invariably referred to (in the manuscript, figure legends, and the Description field when uploading your files) using the following format verbatim: S1 Data, S2 Data, etc. Multiple panels of a single or even several figures can be included as multiple sheets in one excel file that is saved using exactly the following convention: S1_Data.xlsx (using an underscore).

2) Deposition in a publicly available repository. Please also provide the accession code or a reviewer link so that we may view your data before publication. Please annotate your data files sufficiently so they can be directly linked to each figure displaying quantitative data.

Regardless of the method selected, please ensure that you provide the individual numerical values that underlie the summary data displayed in the following figures: 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, S1, S2, S3, S4, and S5.

Please also ensure that the figure legends in your manuscript include information on where the underlying data can be found, and ensure your supplemental data file/s has a legend.

Please ensure that your Data Statement in the submission system accurately describes where your data can be found.

------------------------------------------------------------------------

Reviewer remarks:

Reviewer #1: I appreciate the improvements made to the manuscript. The author has clarified the key points made in my review of the previous submission. I have just one remaining point.

This study uses Ideal Observer-type models to assess loss of information at the level of the photoreceptors and LGN. Previous work assessed in detail the loss of visual information across the retina, from the photoreceptors to the ganglion cells, also using Ideal Observer type model analysis (Borghuis et al., 2009, J Neurosci. 2009, 'Loss of sensitivity in an analog neural circuit'; currently not cited in the manuscript). This study should be cited in Introduction, and it should be made clear in Discussion how the current results relate to those of the prior study. For example, while the prior study did not assess loss across frequency bands, it appears that claims about the origin and magnitude of losses (for example, within the photoreceptors based on assumed quantum efficiency as reported) can, and should be, compared.

Reviewer #2: I thought this was a strong manuscript on the first round, reporting a large body of careful integrative analysis directed at an important question.

The most substantive comment in the reviews, consistent across the reviewers, was the need for a closer discussion of how sensitive the broad conclusions drawn are to specific assumptions and uncertainty about those assumptions, particular stimulus duration. The revision is responsive to this concern. Although some uncertainties of necessity remain, I feel these are now brought out in a manner that appropriately balances between the conclusions the author wishes to draw and what we should worry about with respect to those conclusions.

The author has also considered and responsibly addressed my more specific commments from the initial round of review.

Overall, then, I am in favor of publication of the manuscript in its current form, or something very close ot it, in PLoS Biology.

My one suggestion is that perhaps the discussion could benefit from a simple summary figure that divided up the information loss across the three stages (photoisomerizations to photocurrent, photocurrent to LGN, LGN to behavior) as a function of temporal frequency. Such a plot would allow easy grasp of the main messages of the paper. Figure 5 provides this type of information, but perhaps in the discussion a version that expressed the result in terms of percentage information loss, that averaged across the two monkeys, and that combined the information carried by parvo/magno into one single plot would be helpful to go along with the words.

Reviewer #3: In this revised manuscript, Horwitz has adequately addressed my concerns in the previous version. I have no further comments. I would like to suggest that for future revised submissions, the author submit a version with changes highlighted. This will make the reviewer's life easier and the review process faster.

Decision Letter 2

Gabriel Gasque

5 Dec 2019

Dear Dr Horwitz,

On behalf of my colleagues and the Academic Editor, Jonathan Demb, I am pleased to inform you that we will be delighted to publish your Research Article in PLOS Biology.

The files will now enter our production system. You will receive a copyedited version of the manuscript, along with your figures for a final review. You will be given two business days to review and approve the copyedit. Then, within a week, you will receive a PDF proof of your typeset article. You will have two days to review the PDF and make any final corrections. If there is a chance that you'll be unavailable during the copy editing/proof review period, please provide us with contact details of one of the other authors whom you nominate to handle these stages on your behalf. This will ensure that any requested corrections reach the production department in time for publication.

PRESS

We frequently collaborate with press offices. If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper at this point, to enable them to help maximise its impact. If the press office is planning to promote your findings, we would be grateful if they could coordinate with biologypress@plos.org. If you have not yet opted out of the early version process, we ask that you notify us immediately of any press plans so that we may do so on your behalf.

We also ask that you take this opportunity to read our Embargo Policy regarding the discussion, promotion and media coverage of work that is yet to be published by PLOS. As your manuscript is not yet published, it is bound by the conditions of our Embargo Policy. Please be aware that this policy is in place both to ensure that any press coverage of your article is fully substantiated and to provide a direct link between such coverage and the published work. For full details of our Embargo Policy, please visit http://www.plos.org/about/media-inquiries/embargo-policy/.

Thank you again for submitting your manuscript to PLOS Biology and for your support of Open Access publishing. Please do not hesitate to contact me if I can provide any assistance during the production process.

Kind regards,

Hannah Harwood

Publication Assistant,

PLOS Biology

on behalf of

Gabriel Gasque,

Senior Editor

PLOS Biology

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Fig

    Signal-to-noise ratios (d′ values) of individual LGN neurons (black), currents in cones within their RFs (red), and photon absorptions in the same cones (cyan). Points are means and shaded bands are ±1 SEM. Dashed line indicates the signal-to-noise ratio assumed at the level of behavior on the basis of performance in a two-alternative, forced-choice contrast detection task (d′ = 1.27 or 82% correct). This analysis is identical to the analysis in Fig 5 of the main text except that the spatial integration window of the ideal observers was constrained to a single monocular LGN RF. This analysis does not require assumptions about pools of LGN neurons or interneuronal correlations. Notice that the relative signal-to-noise ratio is similar in small spatial integration windows (this figure) and windows that were, on average, 74 times larger (Fig 5). The mean signal-to-noise ratio across individual LGN neurons (black traces) also appears in Fig 4 of the main text. Data are available at https://github.com/horwitzlab/LGN-temporal-contrast-sensitivity/blob/master/DataByFigure.xlsx. LGN, lateral geniculate nucleus; RF, receptive field.

    (PDF)

    S2 Fig

    Signal-to-noise ratios (population d′ values, see Methods) calculated from single cycles of LGN neuronal responses (black) and simulated cone currents (red) as a function of temporal frequency. Points are means and shaded bands are ±1 SEM. Dashed line indicates the signal-to-noise ratio assumed at the level of behavior (d′ = 1.27 or 82% accuracy). Reducing the temporal integration window to include a single stimulus cycle resulted in signal-to-noise ratios being roughly constant across temporal frequency. (A) Magnocellular data from monkey 1. (B) Magnocellular data from monkey 2. (C) Parvocellular data from monkey 1. (D) Parvocellular data from monkey 2. Data are available at https://github.com/horwitzlab/LGN-temporal-contrast-sensitivity/blob/master/DataByFigure.xlsx. LGN, lateral geniculate nucleus.

    (PDF)

    S3 Fig

    Signal-to-noise ratios (d′ values) of cone currents (red) and LGN populations under several pooling models. Conventions are identical to Fig 5 of the main text. LGN population scale factors were computed as described in the main text (black), in the absence of within-mosaic interneuronal correlations (yellow), after doubling RF diameter (blue), or after halving RF diameter (purple). All of these manipulations affect population scale factors and therefore scale the d′ curves. (A) Magnocellular data from monkey 1. (B) Magnocellular data from monkey 2. (C) Parvocellular data from monkey 1. (D) Parvocellular data from monkey 2. Data are available at https://github.com/horwitzlab/LGN-temporal-contrast-sensitivity/blob/master/DataByFigure.xlsx. LGN, lateral geniculate nucleus. RF, receptive field.

    (PDF)

    S4 Fig. Area under the ROC (ordinate) as a function of d′ (abscissa).

    ROC curves were computed from dM2 values (see Methods, Eq 9). Each symbol represents the data from a single LGN neuron and a single temporal frequency. Data from parvocellular (gray) and magnocellular (black) neurons are shown from monkey 1 (circles) and monkey 2 (triangles). The red curve is the prediction from Gaussian signal and noise distributions with identical SD. Data are available at https://github.com/horwitzlab/LGN-temporal-contrast-sensitivity/blob/master/DataByFigure.xlsx. LGN, lateral geniculate nucleus; ROC, receiver operating characteristic; SD, standard deviation.

    (PDF)

    S5 Fig

    (A) Magnocellular and parvocellular RF diameter (2 SDs of a Gaussian fit) as a function of temporal equivalent retinal eccentricity. Points are RFs in the temporal (+) and nasal (circles) retinae from Derrington and Lennie [53]. The solid line is a least-squares fit. Parvocellular RF sizes were estimated from the model proposed by Watson [81] and shifted down by a factor of 0.8 to account for the smaller RF sizes of macaques [82] (dashed curve). (B) Number of LGN neurons assumed in each ideal observer pool as a function of eccentricity. Pool sizes were calculated on the basis of RF size, the assumption of a hexagonal RF lattice, and the retinal size of the stimulus (a Gabor with 0.15° SD envelope, truncated at ±2 SDs). Open circles represent magnocellular neurons and closed symbols represent parvocellular neurons. Circles and triangles represent neurons from monkeys 1 and 2, respectively. Data are available at https://github.com/horwitzlab/LGN-temporal-contrast-sensitivity/blob/master/DataByFigure.xlsx. LGN, lateral geniculate nucleus; RF, receptive field; SD, standard deviation.

    (PDF)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: ReviewerFigure1.pdf

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: ResponseToReviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    Data and analysis code are available at https://github.com/horwitzlab/LGN-temporal-contrast-sensitivity.


    Articles from PLoS Biology are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES