Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2020 Jan 24;15(1):e0228044. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0228044

When weight is an encumbrance; avoidance of stairs by different demographic groups

Frank F Eves 1,*
Editor: Lee Smith2
PMCID: PMC6980638  PMID: 31978202

Abstract

Background

Locomotion is an energy costly behaviour, particularly when it entails raising weight against gravity. Minimization of locomotor costs appears a universal default. Avoidance of stair climbing helps humans minimise their energetic costs. In public access settings, demographic subgroups that raise more ‘dead’ weight than their comparison groups when climbing are more likely to avoid stairs by choosing the escalator. Individuals who minimise stair costs at work, however, can accumulate a deficit in energy expenditure in daily life with potential implications for weight gain. This paper tests the generality of avoidance of stairs in pedestrians encumbered by additional weight in three studies.

Methods

Pedestrian choices for stairs or the alternative were audited by trained observers who coded weight status, presence of large bags and sex for each pedestrian. Sex-specific silhouettes for BMIs of 25 facilitated coding of weight status. Choices between stairs and a lift to ascend and descend were coded in seven buildings (n = 26,981) and at an outdoor city centre site with the same alternatives (n = 7,433). A further study audited choices to ascend when the alternative to stairs was a sloped ramp in two locations (n = 16,297). Analyses employed bootstrapped logistic regression (1000 samples).

Results

At work and the city centre site, the overweight, those carrying a large bag and females avoided both stair climbing and descent more frequently than their comparison groups. The final study revealed greater avoidance of stairs in these demographic subgroups when the alternative means of ascent was a sloped ramp.

Discussion

Minimization of the physiological costs of transport-related walking biases behaviour towards avoidance of stair usage when an alternative is available. Weight carried is an encumbrance that can deter stair usage during daily life. This minimization of physical activity costs runs counter to public health initiatives to increase activity to improve population health.

Introduction

Use of stairs, instead of escalators or lifts, is one of a range of transport-related behaviours that allow accumulation of incidental physical activity during daily life. Although minimum bouts of 10 minutes duration were recommended for cardio-respiratory benefits, the most recent evidence review concluded that short duration bouts were beneficial [1]. For body weight, all physical activity requires energy expenditure that counters intake that can lead to weight gain. Stair climbing is an energy demanding behaviour that involves raising body weight against gravity; it requires 9.6 times the energy expenditure of rest outside the laboratory [2]. An 80 kg individual going upstairs in their own home ten extra times each day for a year expends the energy equivalent to three pounds of fat [3]. Repeated for ten years, expenditure equivalent to over two stone could be accumulated. Conversely, an individual avoiding climbing ten, 3m flights of stairs each day could accumulate reduced expenditure relative to intake. Weight gain has many potential drivers; this paper summarises data for one of them, avoidance of energy expenditure as part of daily life by avoiding stair climbing.

Although interventions to increase stair climbing are part of current public health strategy [47], the primary goal of seminal research on stair climbing was an ‘unobtrusive measure of physical activity in natural settings’ (page 1540 [8]). Auditing of choice between stairs and escalators unobtrusively assessed physical activity choices in overweight and obese individuals in a shopping mall, an airport, as well as bus and train stations. Both Meyers et al., (1980) and Brownell et al., (1980) reported less frequent stair climbing in overweight than healthy weight pedestrians. A recent review of public access settings confirms these reports; all nine studies with relevant data report significantly less frequent stair climbing in overweight pedestrians [9]. Consistently, overweight pedestrians, offered the opportunity to avoid energy expenditure, do so more frequently than healthy weight individuals. What the data also reveal is that most healthy weight pedestrians avoid the stairs, 91.0% in the original research by Brownell and colleagues (n = 47,548 [8]) and 92.4% prior to intervention in 15 different shopping mall studies (n = 355,069 [9]). While avoidance of energy expenditure on stairs is more frequent in the overweight, it is common to all pedestrians, including healthy weight ones. This prevalence of avoidance reflects a bias to minimise the energetic costs of locomotion (see general discussion).

To climb stairs, an individual must raise their body weight against gravity. Raising weight when climbing stairs entails two and a half times the energetic costs of purposeful walking [2]. Any additional weight carried will increase the energetic cost. Individuals carrying large bags and females also avoid stairs more than their comparison groups [9]. An average woman has a greater percentage of her weight as body fat (25%) than an average man (12.5%); she would raise more ‘dead’ weight against gravity for the same set of stairs [10]. Climbing is more energetically taxing on the resources of overweight pedestrians, those carrying large bags and females because of the ‘dead’ weight they must carry upwards.

Avoidance of stairs at work

While avoidance of stair climbing has consequences for energy balance, a choice between stairs and escalators in a public access setting such as a station is a relatively infrequent daily event. At work, however, stairs and their alternative the lift are a more frequently encountered choice. An individual at work who avoids stairs more than a colleague could accumulate a disparity in energy expenditure as part of daily life with obvious potential consequences for individual differences in weight gain. Employed individuals spend half of their waking life at work [11]. As a result, choice behaviour at work may better address the generality of the avoidance posed by the original researchers of stair choice. This paper provides data on the effects of demographic grouping on avoidance of stair climbing initially at work and then in public access settings, to test for the generality of the effects of demographics on avoidance.

Preliminary study

Table 1 summarises the results of a preliminary literature review on previously reported significant effects of the demographics of weight status, carrying a large bag and being female on choice between stairs and lifts at work (S1 File; Effects of demographic grouping on workplace stair avoidance).

Table 1. Summary of previous significant effects in workplaces of demographics on avoidance of stair climbing.

Demographic (number of studies)
Weight Status (n = 2) Carrying large bag (n = 5) Sex (n = 21)
OW >a HW = HW > OW Bag > No bag = No Bag > Bag F > M = M > F
n = 2 0 0 3 2 0 9 7 5

a > indicates that one group avoided stairs significantly more than the other, e.g. F > M means females avoided stairs significantly more than males. = indicates no significant differences between the demographic groups for the studies in that column, OW = overweight, HW = healthy weight, F = females, M = males.

As can be seen from Table 1, the available data were sparse. The two studies testing effects of weight status, and three of the five recording the presence of large bags, reported greater avoidance of stairs in overweight and encumbered pedestrians respectively, a pattern consistent with greater avoidance by these demographic groups in public access settings. Effects for sex on avoidance were mixed. Nine reported that females avoided stairs more than males, five reported the opposite disparity and seven no significant difference. Analysis of this distribution revealed that females at work did not avoid stairs significantly more than males (binomial test p = .808). As reported in S1 File, there were no obvious co-occurrences of sex differences in avoidance with measurement or analysis choices that might explain the mixed evidence. The anomalies of greater avoidance in men might reflect differing locations by gender of meaningful journey ends within the building. For comparison, the significant effects of demographic grouping in public access studies [9] revealed that females avoided stairs more than males in 32 out of 42 studies (p = .0005), the overweight more than the healthy weight (n = 9/9, p = .002), and those carrying large bags more than the unencumbered (n = 14/17, p = .006).

One further point from the additional material is informative. The percentage choosing the lift at baseline was included in the table to illustrate the much less frequent avoidance at work than in public access settings [12]. The sample size weighted average of the avoidance, 61.0%, meant that choosing to expend energy climbing at work was more than five times more frequent than it was in shopping malls (39.0%; n = 181,168 vs. 7.6%; n = 355,069 [9]). Biases against expenditure were reduced at work. Provision of a lift to ascend, as opposed to an escalator in a mall, is the most plausible explanation for this discrepancy between contexts [12,13]. In a public access setting, choice of either the stairs or an adjacent escalator typically incurs a minimal time penalty. At work, however, a pedestrian will have to wait for any lift not at their floor, slowing the journey [13]. Time is important to pedestrians in public access settings [1315] and at work [16,17]. When the mechanised alternative is a lift, the stairs may provide a quicker option. More available lifts in a workplace, however, will reduce waiting times and stair avoidance is increased as a result [13,18]. Multivariate analyses of the first study formally test effects of lift availability and a second structural determinant, direction of travel, independent of the effects of demographic grouping.

At 9.6 metabolic equivalents (METs) of the resting state, stair climbing is a vigorous activity that requires twice the energy of stair descent (4.9 METs [2]). Direction of travel on the stairs is relevant to energy costs that might be avoided. If avoidance is to generalise, however, one would expect effects of demographic grouping for descent as well as ascent; walking down stairs costs more than standing in a lift. In public access settings, only two studies provide data. Meyer et al., (1980) reported more frequent avoidance of stair descent for obese than healthy weight pedestrians [8] whereas Webb and Eves (2007) reported greater avoidance in females and those carrying large bags [19].

Study 1

Given the paucity of available data on demographics at work, and the mixed data for sex, the first study reports data from workplace observations. The data test for the generality of demographic effects on avoidance by coding, and separately analysing, stair ascent and descent. Based on the sparse public access data, demographic differences were predicted for both directions of travel and less avoidance was predicted for the lower energetic cost of descending.

Methods

Ethical approval for the studies was obtained from the University of Birmingham ethics subcommittee. The ethics committee did not require the consent of observed pedestrians. Approval to observe the employees in the first study was obtained from the management of the firms.

Inconspicuous observers monitored stair and lift choices (up n = 14,607; down n = 12,334) from 9:00am to 4:00pm at the ground floor in seven separate buildings for 8–12 days. (The number of observations, lifts and floors in each building and the percentage stair use are summarised in S2 File). Following training, observers coded choice, direction of travel, gender and presence of large bags/boxes using previously employed criteria [20]; large bags were sizes greater than a briefcase or small rucksack. The silhouettes for male and female BMIs of 25 were watermarks on the coding sheet to facilitate coding of weight status (c.f. [3,21]). Multiple coding of lift and stair choices revealed excellent Kappas [κ] for method of ascent (κ = 0.99), weight status (κ = 0.94), presence of large bags (κ = 0.92) and sex (κ = 0.99).

Analyses

The tabulated data contain percentage avoidance with 95% confidence intervals (CIs) to facilitate inspection; non-overlapping CIs indicate differences between the compared groups and conditions. Formal analyses employed logistic regression with bootstrapping (1000 samples) to control for the potential non-independence of observations at work. Stair/lift choice was the dichotomous dependent variable and the potential predictor variables were direction of travel, weight status and sex. A second structural aspect, number of lifts, was also included as more frequent avoidance occurs as lift availability increases [13,18]. Buildings with one lift were compared with those providing two. Although there are also effects of number of floors on stair use (e.g. 18]), taller buildings typically have more lifts to accommodate the greater number of employees. As a result, number of lifts and floors co-occur and inclusion of both variables in a relatively small data set of different buildings was precluded by this multi-collinearity.

Results

Table 2 contains the percentage avoidance of stairs (95% CIs) broken down by number of lifts, direction of travel and demographic group. Consistent effects of structural aspects of the building were found. Avoidance of stairs was more frequent when two lifts were available relative to one, and when going up relative to coming down. Superimposed on these structural influences were effects of demographics; avoidance of stairs was more common in the overweight, those carrying large bags and females than their comparison groups. As is clear from the non-overlapping CIs, effects of demographic group were present at each level of either structural aspect. The overweight avoided stairs more frequently than those of healthy weight, irrespective of lift availability or direction of travel. Presence of large bags was infrequent in the data set (3.3%), excluding it from formal analyses.

Table 2. Percentage avoidance of stairs (95% CI)a by lift number, direction of travel and demographic group.

Demographic Total n = One lift Total n = Two lifts
Up Down Up Down
Overweight 2,067 49.2 (45.9,52.4) 22.3 (19.1,25.6) 3,973 53.4 (50.9,55.7) 34.7 (32.2,37.3)
Healthy weight 3,873 28.8 (26.6,31.1) 13.1 (11.3,15.0) 17,068 49.1 (47.9,50.3) 30.3 (29.2,31.5)
Large bag 130 87.0 (74.4,94.2) 80.3 (65.6,90.0) 751 81.9 (77.0,86.0) 76.6 (71.1,81.4)
No bag 5,810 35.2 (33.3,37.1) 14.5 (13.0,16.1) 20,290 48.8 (47.7,49.9) 29.4 (28.3,30.4)
Female 3,500 41.8 (39.3,44.4) 20.3 (18.1,22.7) 11,944 53.0 (51.6,54.4) 35.9 (34.5,37.4)
Male 2,440 28.4 (25.6,31.2) 9.9 (7.9,12.1) 9,097 45.7 (44.1,47.3) 25.1 (23.7,26.7)

a; CI = confidence interval.

A preliminary omnibus analysis revealed more avoidance travelling up than down (OR = 3.22, 95% CI = 2.82, 3.67, p < .001). Analyses, summarised in Table 3, were performed separately for direction of travel.

Table 3. Effects of demographics and lift availability on stair avoidance for ascent and descent in workplaces.

Variable Up OR (95% CI)a Down OR (95% CI)
Overweight > Healthy weight 2.43***b (2.09, 2.82) 2.01*** (1.60, 2.52)
Females > Males 1.90*** (1.62, 2.21) 3.02*** (2.31, 3.95)
Two lifts > One lift 2.96*** (2.54, 3.45) 4.70*** (3.61, 6.14)
Weight status x lift interaction 0.48*** (0.40, 0.57) 0.59*** (0.46, 0.76)
Gender x lift interaction 0.73*** (0.62, 0.87) 0.59*** (0.45, 0.79)

a; OR = odds ratio, CI = confidence interval.

b; * = p < .05, ** = p < .01, *** = p < .001.

As can be seen from the table, the overweight and females avoided stairs more than their comparison groups for both ascent and descent. Follow-up of the interactions between lift availability and weight status revealed more frequent avoidance of climbing with two lifts in healthy weight individuals (OR = 2.43, 95% CI = 2.18, 2.70, p<0.001) but no significant effect in the overweight (OR = 1.14, 95% CI = 0.99, 1.32, p = 0.08). For descent, two lifts were associated with a greater increase in avoidance for healthy weight (OR = 3.21, 95% CI = 2.75, 3.76, p<0.001) than overweight individuals (OR = 1.88, 95% CI = 1.54, 2.29, p<0.001). Additionally, there was more frequent avoidance with two lifts in males and females (all prob. < .001), with the ORs for males numerically greater than those for females in both directions.

Discussion

In summary, the overweight, females and those carrying large bags avoided stairs more than their comparison groups. Overall, it appears that any tendencies for avoidance were enhanced by increases in the energy cost of the behaviour (up = 46.8%; down = 28.0%) and the availability of the method of avoidance (two lifts = 41.2%, one lift = 27.4%), with one exception. Avoidance of stair climbing by overweight individuals did not increase significantly when two lifts were available.

At work, avoidance of stair climbing was considerably less frequent than in public access settings, echoing research summarised in additional file, Table 1. Similarly, avoidance of stair descent with one lift (14.5%) was less frequent than when the alternative was an escalator (88.3%) in the same region of the UK [19]. Potential waiting time for a lift provides a plausible explanation for these differences in frequency of avoidance of lifts relative to escalators [6,13,22]. Additionally, pedestrians minimise the distance between destinations for level walking [23] and stair negotiation [19,24]. The layout of the building may influence choice. The stair exit could be closer to a photocopier on the left of the building whereas the lift exit may be closer to the office of a valued colleague on the right. Any distance minimization by an employee seeking a specific destination in the building could dilute effects of demographics. Further, stair choice at work is a frequently encountered option for most employees. Habitual behaviours develop when they are performed regularly in consistent contexts such as the workplace [2527]. It seems likely that habits for stair and lift choice would develop for individual employees and might further dilute effects of demographics at each choice.

Study 2

Introduction

Occasionally in public access settings, a staircase is paired with a lift rather than an escalator. At such a site, the transition between levels is simply part of the journey. The location of both the top and bottom of either the stairs or the lift is essentially the same, a pause on the way to a range of possible destinations encircling the exit. Distance minimization may have minor effects on choice. Additionally, public access stairs will be encountered less frequently than those at work and habitual choice less likely to develop. The second study used a choice between stairs and a lift that connected pavements on different levels to test for the generality of effects of demographics when a lift was available outside of work. Based on study 1, demographic differences were predicted for both directions of travel and less avoidance was predicted for the lower energetic cost of descending.

Methods

Observations of pedestrians ascending (n = 4,100) and descending (n = 3,333) a 43-step staircase (8.60m high) wrapped around a single central lift at an outdoor city centre site were made on eight days between 12:30 and 4:00pm. The entrance to lift was clearly visible to pedestrians at the top of the site but partially concealed from pedestrians approaching from the bottom unless a preceding pedestrian chose it. Trained observers situated at the top of the site coded pedestrians using the same criteria as the workplace data. One observer coded ascent and one descent within any time period. The Kappas were excellent for method of ascent (κ = 0.98), weight status (κ = 0.87), presence of large bags (κ = 0.92) and sex (κ = 0.96). Bootstrapped analyses (1000 samples) employed logistic regression with stair/escalator choice as the dichotomous dependent variable and the potential predictor variables of direction of travel, sex, presence of large bags and weight status.

Results

A preliminary omnibus analysis revealed the expected main effect for direction of travel (OR = 1.29, 95% CI = 1.06, 1.56, p = .02). Table 4 contains the percentage avoidance of stairs (95% CIs) broken down by direction of travel and demographic group. The ORs (95% CIs) for the effects of demographic group for each direction are included in the table. Large bags were carried in 9.0% of the pedestrians and included in formal analyses.

Table 4. Percentage avoidance of stairs (95% CI) outdoors, separated by direction of travel and demographic group.

Demographic Total n = One lift
Up Down
Overweight 1,562 45.5 (41.7, 49.3) 36.9 (32.7, 41.1)
Healthy weight 5,871 22.3 (20.6, 23.9) 17.0 (14.7, 17.9)
ORa (95% CI) 3.59***b (3.01, 4.27) 3.67*** (2.99, 4.49)
Large bag 669 48.9 (43.6, 54.1) 53.7 (45.4, 61.4)
No Bag 6,764 24.5 (22.9, 26.1) 18.3 (16.8, 19.9)
OR (95% CI) 3.69*** (2.97, 4.60) 7.54*** (5.50, 10.28)
Female 3,099 39.1 (36.4, 41.8) 27.9 (25.2, 30.6)
Male 4,334 19.1 (17.3, 20.9) 14.9 (13.1, 16.8)
OR (95% CI) 2.38*** (2.05, 2.77) 1.93*** (1.60, 2.30)

a; OR = odds ratio, CI = confidence interval for effects of that demographic.

b; * = p < .05, ** = p < .01, *** = p < .001.

As at work, stair avoidance was more common for ascent (27.3%, 95% CI = 25.7, 28.9) than descent (20.5%, 95% CI = 18.9, 22.1). Once again, the overweight, those carrying large bags and females avoided stairs more than their comparison groups, with effects superimposed on effects of direction of travel. Confidence intervals for demographic subgroups did not overlap in either direction. For weight status, similar differential rates of avoidance between healthy weight and overweight pedestrians occurred for ascent and descent.

Discussion

Avoidance at this outdoor site echoed that found at work; effects of demographic group were superimposed on more frequent avoidance of ascent than descent. The general pattern of effects at work does not appear to be confined to a work environment where habitual choice might develop. Nonetheless, there were some differences between the two tested contexts. The frequency of avoidance of climbing increases with increasing height of the climb at work, consistent with the increased energetic cost [12,13,18]. The climb to the next level for the city centre site, 8.60m, was considerably higher than the average in the workplace data for the next floor, 3.45m, yet there was less frequent avoidance for the greater ascent (27.3%) than for one lift at work (35.2%). This less frequent avoidance could reflect either a lift that was partially concealed from pedestrians using the site or journeys of more than one floor in the workplace data. For descent, the lift was clearly visible at the top of the climb and the pattern expected from the difference in height appeared; there was more avoidance of descent at the higher outdoor site (20.5%) than at work (14.5%).

Fig 1 summarises the percentage avoidance of stairs for ascent and descent in healthy weight and overweight pedestrians in both studies. The overweight consistently avoided stairs more than those of healthy weight. The difference between the subgroups was of smaller magnitude when two lifts were available. For ascent, increased avoidance by healthy weight individuals with two lifts narrowed the gap between subgroups; changes for the overweight subgroup were not statistically significant. For the single lift at the outdoor site, the overweight avoided descent considerably more frequently than healthy weight pedestrians, with the greater magnitude difference than seen at work consistent with differences in energetic cost between sites. One simple fact would summarise these data and the earlier public access studies [9]. There was a generalised propensity for the overweight to avoid expending energy on stairs when a mechanised alternative was available.

Fig 1. Avoidance of stairs for ascent and descent separated by weight status and number of lifts (OW = overweight, HW = healthy weight).

Fig 1

Study 3

Introduction

In modern cities, sloped ramps can be provided as an alternative to stairs in some public access settings. This provision allows equal access for wheelchair users who cannot negotiate stairs. The final study investigated stair versus slope choice, and the associated demographics, at two such sites to provide replicated observations for the unusual outcome. Stair climbing requires up to three times the peak forces at the knee as level walking [28]. The more gradual climbing available on slopes requires less flexion of the knee and, hence, allows more gradual force production [29].

Chamberlain Square in Birmingham (UK) features a set of stairs opposite the entrance to the square and a sloping ramp around the periphery of the square that reaches the same destination as the top of the stairs (see S3 File for a picture of the site). Pedestrians at this site can avoid the stairs by walking up the slope. Preliminary observations at this site using standardised criteria [20] revealed that 41.2% (95% CI = 40.6, 42.0) avoided the stairs, with avoidance more common in females and those carrying large bags (n = 14,513; Eves, unpublished). The second site was at one exit from the University of Birmingham campus towards University station (see S3 File). A short flight of stairs on the direct route to the station contrasted with a slope that allowed avoidance of the stairs.

Methods

Observations of pedestrians travelling to the exit from Chamberlain Square (n = 13,768; 13 days) and towards the university station (n = 2,529; eight days) were made between 9:50am and 4:00pm. Only ascending pedestrians were coded. In Chamberlain Square, trained observers at the exit of the site coded pedestrians using the same criteria as the workplace data. One observer coded stair users and one coded slope users within any time period. For University station, a single observer alternated between stair and slope coding each 30 minutes. Double coding revealed excellent Kappas for method of ascent (κ = 0.98), weight status (κ = 0.84), presence of large bags (κ = 0.80) and sex (κ = 0.98). Bootstrapped analyses (1000 samples) employed logistic regression with stair/slope choice as the dichotomous dependent variable and the potential predictor variables of sex and weight status. Pedestrians carrying large bags were infrequent in both settings and excluded from formal analyses (Chamberlain Square = 1.8%; University = 1.7%).

Results

Overall, avoidance of the stairs was less frequent in Chamberlain Square, 47.3% (95% CI = 46.4, 48.3), than when approaching University station, 65.2% (95% CI = 63.0, 67.3). Table 5 contains the percentage avoidance (95% CI) broken down by demographic group at both sites, with the ORs (95% CIs) for the effects of demographic group.

Table 5. Percentage avoidance of stairs (95% CI) by choosing slopes at Chamberlain Square and University station by demographic group.

Demographic Total
n =
Total
n =
Chamberlain Square University Station
Overweight 1,860 70.1 (67.6, 72.4) 244 79.9 (73.3, 85.2)
Healthy weight 11,908 43.8 (42.8, 44.8) 2,285 63.6 (61.3, 65.8)
OR (95% CI)a 2.93***b (2.63, 3.25) 2.22*** (1.60, 3.07)
Large bag 245 76.7 (69.9, 82.4) 48 85.4 (69.2, 94.3)
No bag 13,523 46.8 (45.9, 47.8) 2,481 64.8 (62.6, 66.9)
OR (95% CI) c - -
Female 6,074 51.2 (49.7, 52.6) 1,308 70.8 (67.9, 73.6)
Male 7,694 44.3 (43.1, 45.6) 1,221 59.1 (55.9, 62.3)
OR (95% CI) 1.24*** (1.16, 1.33) 1.65*** (1.40, 1.95)

a; OR = odds ratio, CI = confidence interval for each effect of demographic group.

b; * = p < .05, ** = p < .01, *** = p < .001.

c; insufficient data for inclusion in the analyses.

At both sites, the overweight and females avoided stairs more than their comparison groups. Confidence intervals for demographic subgroups did not overlap at either site. The effects of sex were of greater magnitude at University station than at Chamberlain Square.

Discussion

As with lifts, avoidance of stairs by choosing a sloped ascent was frequent. The lower rates of avoidance in Chamberlain Square than the station may reflect the greater detour in the square to choose the slope. Avoidance in the square required 76m of walking versus 48.4m for the direct route across the square; at the station, the discrepancy was smaller, 46.1m versus 42.6m. Inevitably, any detour would increase journey times and, typically, pedestrians seek to minimise time and distance [14,15,23,24]. Less frequent avoidance in the square may reflect the greater temporal cost of the indirect route. Nonetheless, stairs are a more energy efficient means of raising weight against gravity than slopes of equivalent angle [29]. Avoiding stairs by choosing a slope will increase both temporal and energetic costs, unlike the reduced costs with mechanised alternatives. This result of avoidance, despite increased cost, may reflect the more gradual force production possible on the slope to achieve the ascent; the actual height of the climb was the same for both alternatives.

General discussion

The studies in this paper reveal consistent effects of demographic grouping on stair avoidance when an alternative is available. In workplaces, and at an outdoor site where the alternative was a lift, overweight pedestrians, those carrying large bags and females were more likely to avoid stairs than their comparison groups. In the final study, this pattern of avoidance occurred where the alternative method of ascent was a sloped ramp. Taken together with a previous summary of avoidance with escalators [9], these studies expand on the original question posed by Brownell and co-workers about the effects of weight status on avoidance of stair use [7,8]. Overweight pedestrians negotiating the built environment are more likely to avoid the physical activity of stair use as part of daily life than healthy weight pedestrians. So are females and those carrying large bags. A bias to minimise the costs of active transport provides a plausible explanation for this generality.

Minimizing energetic cost

During locomotion, humans naturally optimise energetic cost. They adopt a step width, step length and step frequency for walking and choose a step length and frequency for running, all of which minimise the total metabolic cost for completion of the behaviour (see [30]). Humans have an optimal speed for walking and running that minimises the energetic cost per unit distance [31,32], as do other animals [33,34]. Minimisation of transport costs may be a universal default. This minimization requires repeated iterations to optimise the behaviour. Minimisation of transport costs is learnt, linked to the changes in the visual consequences of forward motion [30,3538]. All of the above studies were for locomotion on the level. Stair climbing, at two and half times the energetic cost of purposeful walking [2], is a metabolically costly barrier encountered during active transport. A consistent bias for pedestrians to minimise the cost incurred by climbing is evident; in shopping malls where journey time is less of an issue than in stations, 92.4% avoid stairs (n = 355,069 [9]). Raising body weight against gravity is energetically costly and appears to be minimised by other animals [39,40]. Energy expenditure serves three main functions, basal metabolic rate, diet-related expenditure on ingested food, and energy for physical activity [41]. Human basal metabolic rate requires 60% of the recommended daily intake and utilizing food a further 6–12% [41]. At least two thirds of recommended intake are required for these recurrent costs of maintaining function that must be met. The only modifiable part of the equation linking intake and expenditure is the remaining third of intake available for movements of the body; it has been estimated that 89% of these movements involve walking [41]. Transport-related walking has deep evolutionary roots. Minimising the proportion of total intake required for transport would be biologically advantageous [3034].

Nonetheless, the final pair of studies demonstrated that avoidance was not synonymous with minimisation of expenditure. Choosing a sloped ascent increased both temporal and energetic costs. Walking up a slope allows a more gradual force production during the ascent; peak forces at the knee are reduced compared to stairs [28,29]. Similarly, a modified climbing gait in older individuals reduces the forces at the knee and the ankle to a lower proportion of their maximal capacity [42]. Older climbers have reduced resources for climbing and adopt a reduced, and more gradual, increase in force over time for each step [42]. Choosing the slope in the final pair of studies would allow individuals with reduced resources for climbing to maintain output at a lower proportion of total resources, despite increases in energetic and temporal costs. Aggregated avoidance with ramped ascent, 50.1% (95% CI = 49.3, 50.9) exceeded that when the lift was the alternative, 33.5% (95% CI = 33.0, 34.0). Preference for more gradual resource expenditure could increase avoidance when a ramp was available whereas unwillingness to wait for a lift could decrease it.

Perception of stair slope

Recent research on the perception of the slope of stairs provides clues to the mechanisms that may underlie avoidance based on climbing resources. Perception of the angle of hills and stairs is exaggerated in explicit awareness; a 10˚ hill is reported to be about 30˚ and a 23˚ staircase reported to be 45˚ [4345]. In experimental studies, fatigue from an exhausting run [4547], carrying extra weight [46] and depleted glucose resources [48], all result in further exaggerations of reported angle. While effects of experimental demand have been proposed as an alternative explanation [49,50], quasi-experimental studies confirmed effects of depleted resources [51] and additional weight carried [43,52] where demand was absent. Travelers waiting for their trains were recruited to complete an interview about the environment [43,51,52]; there was no experiment. Proffitt argued that perception of slope was ‘embodied’ in that resources for climbing influenced explicit perception [44]. Embodied effects of resources facilitate physical activity choices without individuals having to specifically consider resource availability [44].

Echoing the behavioural differences documented here, overweight pedestrians, those wearing heavy bags and females all reported potential climbs as steeper than their comparison groups [43,4547,51,52]. Estimates of stair steepness scaled by the deadweight of fat mass that would be carried [51]. As noted earlier, females have, on average, a greater percentage of their weight as body fat (25%) than males (12.5%) and hence are encumbered by more deadweight [10]. Consistently, females reported slopes as steeper than males did [43,45,47,49,50]. Further, stairs were reported as steeper by pedestrians who avoided them by choosing the escalator, even when potential effects of demographics were controlled by stratified sampling and statistical adjustment [43]. Perceived steepness appears to be an environmental cue linked to resources that can deter climbing when an alternative is available [43].

At work, the stairs may not be directly visible when a lift is chosen so perception of steepness cannot directly influence choice. Nonetheless, individuals learn about the potential cost of climbing from experience, biasing subsequent choices. As resources change, behaviour, and the associated perception, echo these changes. Body mass is composed of fat free mass and fat mass. Fat free mass that provides resources for climbing was unrelated to perceived steepness [52]. Rather, it was fat mass, i.e. deadweight that must be carried, that was linked to perception [52]. Further, only changes in fat mass, not fat free mass, were related to changes in perception of steepness over a year later [52]. Similar calibration of perception occurred for changes in body size during pregnancy [53] and loss of leg strength with ageing [54]; changes without experience were ineffective [53]. Obese individuals walk less than lean participants, with daily walking distance negatively correlated with body fat [55]. In the only truly experimental study of weight change, increases in body mass, 78% of which was fat, reduced the distance walked equally for healthy and overweight participants [55]; resources changed behaviour. In both longitudinal perception studies, body change over time influenced perception [52,53]. No study has altered perception to change behaviour. The simplest conclusion is that weight carried deters behaviour [55], consistent with the direction of effects in other research [5658]. Avoidance of stairs seems likely to be a consequence of weight carried. The role of learning in this process, and the potential mediator of perception, is unknown.

Limitations

It is a curiosity that one strength of these data, direct auditing of behaviour, is accompanied by a limitation. Auditing provides matchless accuracy about the actual behaviour performed; accelerometers, for example, cannot identify behaviour. Observational studies of stair use allow a test of the biasing effects of extra weight carried because of the ability to clearly identify the behavioural choice made. Stair climbing is a vigorous member of the family of active transport behaviours. The energetic cost of stair climbing is clear. Work done to raise weight against gravity is relatively independent of the rate of climbing. Height of the climb, not speed, primarily determines cost. Climbing at 60 steps.min-1 required 8.7 METs (Eves & White, unpublished) whereas climbing at almost twice that speed, 110 steps.min-1, cost 9.6 METs [2]. Effects of weight on the lower intensity activity of stair descent here, and on ‘objectively’ measured walking [55], physical activity [57] and sitting time [56], indicate a generalised effect of weight carried on physical activity choices. Nonetheless, auditing will imperfectly measure demographic differences. Sex is generally straightforward but weight status, and the additional weight of a large bag, must be imprecise categories, even when silhouettes optimise coding for weight status [3,21]. The commonality of effects of weight carried on avoidance across different settings, however, does not suggest imprecision in measurement has impeded the research. The fact that demographic differences in avoidance behaviour are linked to perception of an environmental cue that promotes avoidance indicates some triangulation on the question.

Conclusions

Overweight pedestrians, those carrying large bags and females were more likely to avoid stairs when a lift was available at work and outdoors, as well as when the alternative to stairs was a sloped ascent. Weight carried can be an encumbrance that deters participation in the incidental physical activity of stair usage during daily life. Increased physical activity, including stair climbing, is a current target of public health in the developed world to improve population health [1,5961]. A focus on environments that would encourage active transport such as walking is prominent in current approaches [6264]. Mechanisms that minimise the costs of active transport can run counter to public health initiatives to increase physical activity as part of daily life.

Supporting information

S1 File. Previous studies of the effects of demographic grouping on stair avoidance at work.

(PDF)

S2 File. Building characteristics for study 1.

(PDF)

S3 File. Chamberlain Square and University Station.

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

I thank Mike White, Janice Thompson and Doug Carroll for helpful comments on earlier versions of this paper, and the statistician, Roger Holder, for the recommendation to use bootstrapped analyses. I thank Birmingham and Coventry City Councils, Severn Trent, Pilkingtons and Birmingham Chamber of Commerce for permission to use their buildings as well as Birmingham City council for permission to make observations at the outdoor site and in Chamberlain Square. Finally, I thank the third-year project students without whom these observations would not have been collected.

Data Availability

All files are available at https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.w0vt4b8m5.

Funding Statement

FFE G0802070/91321 Medical Research Council, UK The funders had no role in study design, data colection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Jakicic JM, Kraus WE, Powell KE, Campbell WN, Janz KF, et al. , for the 2018 Physical Activity Guidelines Advisory Committee. Association between bout duration of physical activity and health: Systematic review. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2019;51: 1213–1219. 10.1249/MSS.0000000000001933 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Teh KC, Aziz AR. Heart rate, oxygen uptake, and energy cost of ascending and descending the stairs. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2002;34: 695–99. 10.1097/00005768-200204000-00021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Lewis A, Eves FF. Specific effects of a calorific expenditure intervention in overweight commuters. Ann Behav Med. 2011;42: 257–61. 10.1007/s12160-011-9283-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.National Institute of Clinical Excellence, public health guidance 13 (2008) Workplace health promotion. www.nice.org.uk
  • 5.Soler RE, Leeks KD, Buchanan LR, Brownson RC, Heath GW, Hopkins DH. Point-of-decision prompts to increase stair use; a systematic review update. Am J Prev Med. 2010;38: S292–S300. 10.1016/j.amepre.2009.10.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Eves FF. Effects of point-of-decision prompts for stair use depend on the alternative. Am J Prev Med. 2010;38: 573–574. 10.1016/j.amepre.2010.02.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Brownell KD, Stunkard A, Albaum J. Evaluation and modification of exercise patterns in the natural environment. Am J Psychiat. 1980;137: 1540–1545. 10.1176/ajp.137.12.1540 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Meyers AW, Stunkard AJ, Coll M, Cooke CJ. Stairs, escalators and obesity. Behav Mod. 1980;4: 355–359. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Eves FF. Is there any Proffitt in stair climbing? A headcount of studies testing for demographic differences in choice of stairs. Psychonom Bull Rev. 2014;21: 71–79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.McCardle WD, Katch FI, Katch VL. Exercise Physiology, 6th Edition Philadelphia, US: Lippincott, Williams & Wilkins; 2007. 10.1159/000097953 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Dishman RK, Oldenburg B, O’Neal H, Shephard RJ. Worksite physical activity interventions. Am J Prev Med. 1998;15: 344–61. 10.1016/s0749-3797(98)00077-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Eves FF, Webb OJ. Worksite interventions to increase stair climbing; Reasons for caution. Prev Med. 2006;43: 4–7. 10.1016/j.ypmed.2006.03.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Olander E, Eves FF. Elevator availability and its impact on stair use in a workplace. J Envir Psychol. 2011. a;31: 200–206. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Kerr J, Eves FF, Carroll D. Getting more people on the stairs: The impact of a new message format. J Health Psychol. 2001. a;6: 495–500. 10.1177/135910530100600503 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Eves FF, Lewis AL, Griffin C. Modeling effects of stair width on rates of stair climbing in a train station. Prev Med. 2008;47: 270–272. 10.1016/j.ypmed.2007.12.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Kerr J, Eves FF, Carroll D. Can posters prompt stair use in a worksite environment? J Occ Health. 2001. b;43: 205–207. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Blake H, Lee S, Stanton T, Gorely T. Workplace intervention to promote stair-use in an NHS setting. Int J Workplace Health Manag. 2008;1: 162–175. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Lewis A, Eves FF. Prompt before the choice is made; Effects of a stair climbing intervention in university buildings. Brit J Health Psych. 2012;17: 631–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Webb OJ, Eves FF. Effects of environmental changes in a stair climbing intervention: generalization to stair descent. Am J Health Prom. 2007;22: 38–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Kerr J, Eves FF, Carroll D. Six-month observational study of prompted stair climbing. Prev Med. 2011. c;33: 422–427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Eves FF, Webb OJ, Mutrie N. A workplace intervention to promote stair climbing: greater effects in the overweight. Obes. 2006;14: 2210–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Eves FF. All choices are not equal; effects of context on point-of-choice prompts for stair climbing. Obes Rev. 2008;9: 83–84. 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2007.00372.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Gärling T, Gärling E. Distance minimization in downtown pedestrian shopping. Environ Plan A. 1988;20: 547–554. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Eves FF, Olander EK, Nicoll G, Puig-Ribera A, Griffin C. Increasing stair climbing in a train station; effects of contextual variables and visibility. J Envir Psychol. 2009;29: 300–03. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Ouellette JA, Wood W. Habit and intention in everyday life: The multiple processes by which past behavior predicts future behavior. Psychol Bull. 1998;124: 54–74. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Verplanken B, Aarts H. Habit, attitude and planned behaviour: Is habit an empty construct or an interesting case of goal-directed automaticity. In Stroebe W, Hewstone M. (Eds.), Europe Rev Soc Psychol. 1999;10: p. 101–134. Chichester, England: John Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Eves FF, Puig-Ribera A. Learnt effects of environmental cues on transport-related walking; disrupting habits with health promotion? PLoS ONE. 2019;14: e0220308 10.1371/journal.pone.0220308 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Riener R, Rabuffetti M, Frigo C. Stair ascent and descent at different inclinations. Gait Posture. 2002;15: 32–44. 10.1016/s0966-6362(01)00162-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Corlett E, Hutcheson C, DeLugan M, Rogozenski J. Ramps or stairs: The choice using physiological and biomechanic criteria. Applied Ergonomics. 1972;3: 195–201. 10.1016/0003-6870(72)90100-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Srinivasan M. Optimal speed for walking and running, and walking on a moving walkway. Chaos. 2009;19: 026112 1–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Selinger JC, O’Connor SM, Wong JD, Donelan JM. (2015). Humans can continuously optimize energetic cost during walking. Current Biol. 2015;25: 2452–2456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Steudel-Numbers KL, Wall-Scheffler CM. Optimal running speed and the evolution of hominin hunting strategies. J Human Evol. 2009;56: 355–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Hoyt DF, Taylor CR. Gait and energetics of locomotion in horses. Nature. 1981;292; 239–40. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Nishii J. Legged insects select the optimal locomotor pattern based on energetic cost. Biol Cyber. 2000;83: 435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Prokop T, Schubert M, Berger W. Visual influences on human locomotion. Exper Brain Res. 1997;114: 63–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Reiser JJ, Pick HL, Ashmead DH, Garing AE. Calibration of human locomotion and models of perceptual-motor organisation. J Exper Psychol; Human Perc Perf. 1995;21: 480–497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.White E, Shockley K, Riley MA. Multimodally specified energy expenditure and action-based distance judgments. Psychon Bull Rev. 2013;20: 1371–1377. 10.3758/s13423-013-0462-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Zadra JR. Proffitt DR. Optic flow is calibrated to walking effort. Psychon Bull Rev. 2016;23: 1491–1496. 10.3758/s13423-016-1017-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Thorpe SKS, Crompton RH, McNeil Alexander R. Orangutans use compliant branches to lower the energetic cost of locomotion. Biol Letters. 2007;3: 253–56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Wall J, Douglas-Hamilton I, Vollrath F. Elephants avoid costly mountaineering. Current Biol. 2006;16: R527–529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Levine JA, Kotz CM. NEAT–non-exercise activity thermogenesis–egocentric and geocentric environmental factors vs. biological regulation. Acta Physiol Scand. 2005;184: 309–318. 10.1111/j.1365-201X.2005.01467.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Reeves ND, Spanjaard M, Mohagheghi AA, Baltzopoulos V, Maganaris CN. Older adults employ alternative strategies to operate within their maximum capabilities when ascending stairs. J Electromyog Kinesiol. 2009;19: e57–e68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Eves FF. Thorpe SKS, Lewis A, Taylor-Covill GAH. Does perceived steepness deter stair climbing when an alternative is available? Psychonom Bull Rev. 2014;21: 637–644. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Proffitt DR. Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspec Psychol Sci. 2006;1: 110–122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Proffitt DR, Bhalla M, Gossweiler R, Midgett J. Perceiving geographical slant. Psychonom Bull Rev. 1995;2: 409–428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Bhalla M, Proffitt DR. Visual-motor recalibration in geographical slant perception. J Exper Psychol; Human Perc Perf. 1999;25: 1076–1096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Taylor-Covill GAH, Eves FF. Slant perception on stairs and screens: Effects of sex and fatigue in a laboratory environment. Perc. 2013;42: 459–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Schnall S, Zadra JR, Proffitt DR. Direct evidence for the economy of action: Glucose and the perception of geographical slant. Perc. 2010;39: 464–482. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Durgin F.H, Baird J.A, Greenburg M, Russell R, Shaughnessy K, Waymouth S. Who is being deceived? The experimental demands of wearing a backpack. Psychonom Bull Rev. 2009;16: 964–969. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Durgin FH, Klein B, Spiegel A, Strawser CJ, Williams M. The social psychology of perception experiments: Hills, backpacks, glucose and the problem of generalizability. J Exper Psychol; Human Perc Perf. 2012;38: 1582–1595. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Taylor-Covill GAH, Eves FF. When what we need influences what we see: Choice of energetic replenishment is linked with perceived steepness. J Exper Psychol; Human Perc Perf. 2014;40: 915–919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Taylor-Covill GAH, Eves FF. Carrying a biological ‘backpack’; quasi-experimental effects of weight status and body fat change on perceived steepness. J Exper Psychol; Human Perc Perf. 2016;42: 331–338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Franchak JM, Adolph KE. Gut estimates: Pregnant women adapt to changing possibilities for squeezing through doorways. Attention, Perc Psychophysics. 2014;26: 460–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Konczak J, Meeuwsen HJ, Cress ME. Changing affordances in stair climbing: The perception of maximum climbability in young and older adults. J Exper Psychol; Human Perc Perf. 1992;18: 691–697. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Levine JA, McCrady SK, Lanningham-Foster LM, Kane PH, Foster RC, Manohar CU. The role of free-living daily walking in human weight gain and obesity. Diabet. 2008;57: 548–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Ekelund U, Brage S, Besson H, Sharp S, Wareham NJ. Time spent being sedentary and weight gain in healthy adults: reverse or bidirectional causality? Am J Clin Nutr. 2008;88: 612–7. 10.1093/ajcn/88.3.612 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Metcalf BS, Hosking J, Jeffery AN, Voss LD, Henley W, et al. Fatness leads to inactivity but inactivity does not lead to fatness: a longitudinal study in children (EarlyBird 45). Arch Dis Child. 2011;96: 942–47. 10.1136/adc.2009.175927 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Pedisic Z, Grunseit A, Ding D, Chau JY, Banks E, et al. High Sitting Time or Obesity: Which Came First? Bidirectional Association in a Longitudinal Study of 31,787 Australian Adults. Obes. 2014;22: 2126–2130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Physical Activity Guidelines Advisory Committee. Physical Activity Guidelines Advisory Committee scientific report. Part F. Chapter 11 Promoting Regular Physical Activity. Department of Health and Human Services: Washington, DC, U.S, 2018; pp. 1–104. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.National Health Service, UK (2018). Physical activity guidelines for adults. Retrieved from https://www.nhs.uk/live-well/exercise/
  • 61.U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (2018). Physical Activity Guidelines for Americans, 2nd edition [pdf]. Retrieved from https://health.gov/paguidelines/second-edition/pdf/Physical_Activity_Guidelines_2nd_edition.pdf
  • 62.Sallis JF, Cervero RB, Ascher W, Henderson KA, Kraft MK, Kerr J. An ecological approach to creating more physically active communities. Ann Rev Public Health 2006;27: 297–322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Bauman AE, Reis RS, Sallis JF, Wells JC, Loos RJF, Martin BW. Correlates of physical activity: why are some people physically active and others not? Lancet 2012;380: 258–71. 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60735-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Sallis JF, Cerin E, Conway TL, Adams MA, Frank LD, Pratt M, et al. Physical activity in relation to urban environments in 14 cities worldwide: a cross-sectional study. Lancet 2016;387: 2207–17. 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)01284-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Lee Smith

20 Nov 2019

PONE-D-19-27601

When weight is an encumbrance; avoidance of stairs by different demographic groups.

PLOS ONE

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

==============================

This is a fine paper, however, one reviewer has requested minor revisions we would like you to address before acceptance.

==============================

We would appreciate receiving your revised manuscript by Dec 26 2019 11:59PM. When you are ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter.

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). This letter should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Manuscript'.

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Lee Smith

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

1. When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at http://www.journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and http://www.journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please reorganise your manuscript and incorporate the sections of each separate study into a unique section. For more information please visit https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines .

3. We note that you have stated that you will provide repository information for your data at acceptance. Should your manuscript be accepted for publication, we will hold it until you provide the relevant accession numbers or DOIs necessary to access your data. If you wish to make changes to your Data Availability statement, please describe these changes in your cover letter and we will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide.

4. Please upload a copy of Figure 2, to which you refer in your text on page 17. If the figure is no longer to be included as part of the submission please remove all reference to it within the text.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This paper reports a literature review, and three new empirical studies, that together demonstrate that those for whom stair use is most physiologically costly are most likely to avoid using the stairs. The paper is well-written and the studies use a rigorous methodology, including direct observation of thousands of participants in real-world settings. The paper furthers understanding of reasons for stair use, or rather avoidance thereof, and has important implications for public health policy and practice. I have only very minor suggestions for improvement.

Abstract: Please state explicitly the number of studies presented in the manuscript.

Abstract, Discussion subsection: ‘minimization of physiological costs of transport-related walking biases behaviour towards avoidance of stairs when an alternative is available’. This statement seems an over-generalisation; presumably it is not just any ‘alternative’ that must be available, but rather a lower-cost alternative.

Abstract and throughout paper: I found the term ‘lifestyle physical activity’ to describe stair use slightly awkward. While an accurate depiction, applying the term ‘lifestyle’ to the mundane act of climbing stairs has the effect (to me, at least) of inflating its importance and meaning to individuals. Might this be changed to ‘incidental physical activity’ or ‘instrumental physical activity’ instead?

Abstract, Discussion subsection: Both here, and in the Conclusion, the author argues that observed striving to minimize costs ‘runs counter to public health initiatives’. Which initiatives, and how does cost minimisation conflict with such initiatives? This point could be expanded on in the General Discussion.

Introduction, p4: The sentence beginning ‘For body weight, all physical activity contributes…’ is difficult to comprehend. Please simplify.

Introduction, p4: What is meant by the term ‘in the field’ in the first paragraph? Can this be deleted?

Preliminary study: This study summarises a ‘preliminary literature review’, but the methods used to conduct the review are not reported. Assuming this review was carried out for the purpose of this paper, the sources within the literature review should be explicitly stated, together with an indication of the process of identifying these sources.

Preliminary study, p6: The author reports here that while ‘nine studies reported that females avoided stairs more than males, five reported the opposite disparity’. Why should the opposite disparity have been found? This seems at odds with the arguments forwarded in the Introduction regarding groups most likely to avoid stair use, so should be considered in more depth.

Study 1, p11: Replace the term ‘no significant effect’ with ‘no effect’.

Reviewer #2: Excellent paper, very minor comments included.

Abstract: "This minimization of costs runs counter to public health initiatives that targets weight gain." Please specify what "this minimization of costs" strands for.

Gender and sex: throughout the paper, please check for consistency and use sex (male/female) or gender (men and women) or clearly define the different use of each. Currently sex and gender seem to be used interchangeably.

Method: pleaae define "large bags"

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Benjamin Gardner

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files to be viewed.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2020 Jan 24;15(1):e0228044. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0228044.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


5 Dec 2019

I thank the reviewers, and the editor, for their generally positive comments. I have responded to each of query below.

Reviewer #1: This paper reports a literature review, and three new empirical studies, that together demonstrate that those for whom stair use is most physiologically costly are most likely to avoid using the stairs. The paper is well-written and the studies use a rigorous methodology, including direct observation of thousands of participants in real-world settings. The paper furthers understanding of reasons for stair use, or rather avoidance thereof, and has important implications for public health policy and practice. I have only very minor suggestions for improvement.

I thank the reviewer for their kind comments.

Abstract: Please state explicitly the number of studies presented in the manuscript.

Now included in the abstract as requested.

Abstract, Discussion subsection: ‘minimization of physiological costs of transport-related walking biases behaviour towards avoidance of stairs when an alternative is available’. This statement seems an over-generalisation; presumably it is not just any ‘alternative’ that must be available, but rather a lower-cost alternative.

I can understand why the reviewer queries this statement. While the first two studies entail minimization of the energetic costs of active transport, choosing a ramp to ascend increases both energetic and temporal costs at both locations in the final study. In this case, it is likely that rate of expenditure is the key variable as I explain in the associated discussion section.

Abstract and throughout paper: I found the term ‘lifestyle physical activity’ to describe stair use slightly awkward. While an accurate depiction, applying the term ‘lifestyle’ to the mundane act of climbing stairs has the effect (to me, at least) of inflating its importance and meaning to individuals. Might this be changed to ‘incidental physical activity’ or ‘instrumental physical activity’ instead?

I understand the reviewer’s concern here and have changed the usage to either ‘the lifestyle physical activity of active transport’ or ‘incidental physical activity of stair climbing/usage as part of daily life’.

The paper reports a bias to avoid stairs as a result of extra weight carried. This is true for both climbing and the less intense activity of stair descent. Such a bias is also seen for the only experimental data for walking on the level (Levine et al., 2008) and the epidemiological studies of Metcalf et al., (2011) and Ekelund et al., (2008) that demonstrate that extra weight reduces what are termed ‘objective measures’ of physical activity. Observational studies of stair use allow a test of the biasing effects of extra weight carried because of the ability to clearly identify the behavioural choice made. I think that these effects generalise and have made this point explicitly in the discussion section (page 22, line 289–292, page 23 line 295-298). I thank the reviewer for the greater clarity about my underlying assumptions and the implications of the data resulting from this requested clarification.

Abstract, Discussion subsection: Both here, and in the Conclusion, the author argues that observed striving to minimize costs ‘runs counter to public health initiatives’. Which initiatives, and how does cost minimisation conflict with such initiatives? This point could be expanded on in the General Discussion.

I apologise for the oversight. I now make explicit in the general discussion that increased physical activity is a current target of public health in the developed world and provide appropriate references (page 23, line 311-314).

Introduction, p4: The sentence beginning ‘For body weight, all physical activity contributes…’ is difficult to comprehend. Please simplify.

I have reworded the sentence to read ‘For body weight, all physical activity requires energy expenditure that counters intake that can lead to weight gain.’ I hope this is now clearer.

Introduction, p4: What is meant by the term ‘in the field’ in the first paragraph? Can this be deleted?

I have removed the phrase and replaced it with ‘outside the laboratory’ to make explicit that this was not a laboratory study but rather one that used an 11-story building.

Preliminary study: This study summarises a ‘preliminary literature review’, but the methods used to conduct the review are not reported. Assuming this review was carried out for the purpose of this paper, the sources within the literature review should be explicitly stated, together with an indication of the process of identifying these sources.

The information about the process and sources for the preliminary literature review are presented in the supplementary file S1.

Preliminary study, p6: The author reports here that while ‘nine studies reported that females avoided stairs more than males, five reported the opposite disparity’. Why should the opposite disparity have been found? This seems at odds with the arguments forwarded in the Introduction regarding groups most likely to avoid stair use, so should be considered in more depth.

I agree with the reviewer that greater avoidance in men is puzzling. I have no clear explanation but have inserted the sentence. ‘The anomalies of greater avoidance in men might reflect differing locations by gender of meaningful journey ends within the building.’ (page 6, line 137ff).

Study 1, p11: Replace the term ‘no significant effect’ with ‘no effect’.

I have resisted this suggestion. The OR of 1.14, (95% CI = 0.99, 1.32, p=0.08) is close to significance and I feel that the unqualified ‘no effect’ would be misleading.

Reviewer #2: Excellent paper, very minor comments included.

I thank the reviewer for their kind comment.

Abstract: "This minimization of costs runs counter to public health initiatives that targets weight gain." Please specify what "this minimization of costs" strands for.

The sentence has been altered to now read ‘This minimisation of physical activity costs runs counter to public health initiatives to increase activity to improve population health.’

Gender and sex: throughout the paper, please check for consistency and use sex (male/female) or gender (men and women) or clearly define the different use of each. Currently sex and gender seem to be used interchangeably.

I thank the reviewer for their vigilance. I have amended the manuscript to make explicit that I am referring to sex.

Method: pleaae define "large bags"

I now make explicit that ‘large bags were sizes greater than a briefcase or small rucksack.’

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Lee Smith

7 Jan 2020

When weight is an encumbrance; avoidance of stairs by different demographic groups.

We are pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it complies with all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you will receive an e-mail containing information on the amendments required prior to publication. When all required modifications have been addressed, you will receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will proceed to our production department and be scheduled for publication.

Shortly after the formal acceptance letter is sent, an invoice for payment will follow. To ensure an efficient production and billing process, please log into Editorial Manager at https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the "Update My Information" link at the top of the page, and update your user information. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to enable them to help maximize its impact. If they will be preparing press materials for this manuscript, you must inform our press team as soon as possible and no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

With kind regards,

Lee Smith

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Thank you for adequately responding to the reviewer comments. I have now recommended that this paper is accepted.

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Lee Smith

8 Jan 2020

PONE-D-19-27601R1

When weight is an encumbrance; avoidance of stairs by different demographic groups.

Dear Dr. Eves:

I am pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper at this point, to enable them to help maximize its impact. If they will be preparing press materials for this manuscript, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

For any other questions or concerns, please email plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE.

With kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Lee Smith

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 File. Previous studies of the effects of demographic grouping on stair avoidance at work.

    (PDF)

    S2 File. Building characteristics for study 1.

    (PDF)

    S3 File. Chamberlain Square and University Station.

    (PDF)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All files are available at https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.w0vt4b8m5.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES