Abstract
The corticospinal tract (CST) plays an important role in controlling voluntary movement. Because the CST has a long trajectory throughout the brain toward the spinal cord, many axon guidance molecules are required to navigate the axons correctly during development. Previously, we found that double-knockout (DKO) mouse embryos lacking the heparan sulfate endosulfatases, Sulf1 and Sulf2, showed axon guidance defects of the CST owing to the abnormal accumulation of Slit2 protein on the brain surface. However, postnatal development of the CST, especially the pyramidal decussation and spinal cord projection, could not be assessed because DKO mice on a C57BL/6 background died soon after birth. We recently found that Sulf1/2 DKO mice on a mixed C57BL/6 and CD-1/ICR background can survive into adulthood and therefore investigated the anatomy and function of the CST in the adult DKO mice. In Sulf1/2 DKO mice, abnormal dorsal deviation of the CST fibers on the midbrain surface persisted after maturation of the CST. At the pyramidal decussation, some CST fibers located near the midline crossed the midline, whereas others located more laterally extended ipsilaterally. In the spinal cord, the crossed CST fibers descended in the dorsal funiculus on the contralateral side and entered the contralateral gray matter normally, whereas the uncrossed fibers descended in the lateral funiculus on the ipsilateral side and entered the ipsilateral gray matter. As a result, the CST fibers that originated from 1 side of the brain projected bilaterally in the DKO spinal cord. Consistently, microstimulation of 1 side of the motor cortex evoked electromyogram responses only in the contralateral forelimb muscles of the wild-type mice, whereas the same stimulation evoked bilateral responses in the DKO mice. The functional consequences of the CST defects in the Sulf1/2 DKO mice were examined using the grid-walking, staircase, and single pellet-reaching tests, which have been used to evaluate motor function in mice. Compared with the wild-type mice, the Sulf1/2 DKO mice showed impaired performance in these tests, indicating deficits in motor function. These findings suggest that disruption of Sulf1/2 genes leads to both anatomical and functional defects of the CST.
Keywords: heparan sulfate, Sulfatase 1, Sulfatase 2, knockout mouse, corticospinal tract, pyramidal decussation, motor movement, bilateral projection
Introduction
The corticospinal tract (CST) plays a critical role in controlling voluntary movement (Lemon, 2008; Welniarz et al., 2017a). It is the longest tract in the central nervous system, originating in layer 5 pyramidal neurons in the sensorimotor cortex and terminating in the spinal cord. In rodents, the CST fibers pass through the internal capsule and cerebral peduncle, extend onto the ventral medulla, cross the midline at the pyramidal decussation dorsally, and further descend in the dorsal funiculus of the spinal cord contralaterally. Before reaching the spinal cord, they send collateral branches to the striatum, superior colliculus, red nucleus, pontine gray nucleus, and dorsal column nuclei (Wang et al., 2018). In the spinal cord, the CST fibers project to the dorsal and intermediate portions of the gray matter and innervate interneurons, which in turn control motor neurons (Lemon, 2008; Welniarz et al., 2017a).
Because the CST axons extend throughout the brain during development, a number of axon guidance molecules are required to navigate them to their targets correctly (Canty and Murphy, 2008; Leyva-Díaz and López-Bendito, 2013; Welniarz et al., 2017a). For example, Slit and its receptor Robo guide the axons by preventing them from entering the hypothalamic area and crossing the midline. Netrin-1 and its receptors DCC/Unc5, Sema6A and its receptors Plexin A3/A4, and the cell adhesion molecules L1 and NCAM are implicated in forming the pyramidal decussation (Dahme et al., 1997; Cohen et al., 1998; Fransen et al., 1998; Finger et al., 2002; Rolf et al., 2002; Faulkner et al., 2008; Rünker et al., 2008). In the spinal cord, Wnt and its receptor Ryk promote caudal growth (Liu et al., 2005), whereas ephrin and its receptor Eph are involved in topographical branching and innervation (Dottori et al., 1998; Kullander et al., 2001a, b; Yokoyama et al., 2001).
The heparan sulfate endosulfatases, Sulfatase 1 (Sulf1) and Sulfatase 2 (Sulf2), are extracellular enzymes that remove sulfate groups at the 6-O position of the glucosamine in heparan sulfate (HS) (Lamanna et al., 2007). Because Sulf-mediated desulfation occurs mainly in the highly sulfated regions of HS, which are required for interaction with signaling molecules, Sulfs can regulate cellular signaling positively or negatively. Previously, we showed that Sulf1/2 double-knockout (DKO) mice have defects in the CST, whereas Sulf1 or Sulf2 single-knockout (KO) mice appear normal (Okada et al., 2017). Mechanistically, in Sulf1/2 DKO mice, abnormal accumulation of Slit2 protein in the basement membrane of the ventral brain surface, which is caused by the increase in 6-O-sulfated HS, results in aberrant dorsal deflection of the CST axons on the lateral surface of the midbrain. Most of the axons that extend abnormally in the dorsal direction return to the medulla, whereas a part of them enter the superior and inferior colliculi. In the medulla, the CST fibers are defasciculated and positioned more laterally when compared with those of controls. Because DKO mice on a C57BL/6 background die within a day of birth, we could not assess the postnatal development of the CST. Specifically, it remained unclear whether pyramidal decussation and spinal cord projection occur normally.
Recently we found that for unknown reasons, Sulf1/2 DKO mice survived into adulthood after outcrossing to the CD-1/ICR strain. Thus, we examined the CST trajectory in the adult Sulf1/2 DKO brain using mice on a mixed genetic background. We found that the abnormal dorsal projection of the CST fibers on the midbrain surface that is observed in the embryonic brain is present in the adult brain. In addition, the DKO mice have abnormal pyramidal decussation and aberrant bilateral projections in the spinal cord. Consistently, stimulation of 1 side of the motor cortex evokes bilateral responses in the forelimb muscles of the DKO mice. Furthermore, we found that the DKO mice perform poorly in skilled reaching and grasping tasks, indicating that they have impaired motor movements.
Materials and Methods
Mice
Sulf1 and Sulf2 KO mice were generated using homologous recombination in 129/Ola-derived ES cells and maintained on a C57BL/6N background by mating offspring of mice that had been backcrossed to C57BL/6N for 5 successive generations (N5 generation), as described previously (Nagamine et al., 2012). These mice were mated with the outbred CD-1/ICR strain (F1 generation), and the offspring was further mated with the N5 generation of C57BL/6N (F2 generation). In this study, offspring of F1 or F2 on a mixed genetic background of C57BL/6N and CD-1/ICR (≒50%:50% or 75%:25%) were used. The genotypes were determined by PCR of genomic DNAs isolated from the tails. Wild-type and DKO mice (both from the mixed genetic background) were used, unless otherwise stated. All animal experiments were approved by and performed according to the guidelines of the Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Tsukuba.
Preparation of the Brain and Spinal Cord Sections
Mice were deeply anesthetized by intraperitoneal (i.p.) injection of an excess amount of pentobarbital and transcardially perfused with 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in phosphate-buffered saline (PBS). The brain and spinal cord were dissected and immersed in the same fixative overnight at 4°C. After being rinsed 3 times in PBS, the tissues were immersed in 20% sucrose in PBS for cryoprotection and embedded in Tissue-Tek O.C.T. compound (Sakura Finetek Japan, Tokyo, Japan). Coronal sections (50 μm thick) were cut on a cryostat (CM 1850; Leica Biosystems, Wetzlar, Germany), and the sections were collected in PBS. Free-floating sections were washed three times in PBS and once in PBS with 0.1% Tween-20 (PBT) and serially dehydrated through 25–80% methanol/PBT and then 80% methanol/20% dimethyl sulfoxide (DMSO). To block endogenous peroxidase activity, the sections were incubated in 3% H2O2/80% methanol/20% DMSO for 30 min. After rehydration, the sections were subjected to further processing.
Immunohistochemistry
Protein kinase C gamma (PKCγ) immunohistochemistry was done as previously described (Okada et al., 2019). Briefly, the brain sections of mice (16–56 weeks old) were incubated twice overnight with anti-PKCγ antibody (1:200; Frontier Institute, Hokkaido, Japan) in 0.5% blocking reagent (Roche Diagnostics, Mannheim, Germany) in PBT at 4°C. The sections were washed with PBT for 15 min six times and then incubated with biotin-conjugated anti-rabbit IgG antibody (1:600; Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA, United States) in 0.5% blocking reagent in PBT for 2 h. After being washed with PBT for 15 min five times and with Tris-buffered saline (TBS) supplemented with 0.1% Tween-20 (TBST) for 15 min, the sections were incubated with avidin-biotin peroxidase complex (ABC; Vectastain Elite ABC kit; Vector Laboratories) for 30 min. The sections were washed with 1% Tween-20/TBS for 20 min, and then with TBST for 20 min twice, and were thereafter kept in the same solution at 4°C overnight. The sections were incubated with 3,3′-diaminobenzidine (DAB; Vector Laboratories) for 10 min. All steps were performed at room temperature unless otherwise indicated.
Anterograde Tracing of the CST
Anterograde tracing of the CST was done using biotinylated dextran amine (BDA) as follows. Adult mice of either sex (9–45 weeks old, 17.1–53.3 g) were anesthetized by i.p. injection of pentobarbital (50 mg/kg body weight), and placed in a stereotaxic frame (David Kopf Instruments, Tujunga, CA, United States). After the scalp was incised, a burr hole was made using a dental drill. After the dura was removed, a Neuros syringe (model 75RN; Hamilton Laboratories, Reno, NV, United States) was inserted into the target area in the right sensorimotor cortex. The stereotaxic coordinates (anterior-posterior [AP] to the bregma; medial-lateral [ML] from the midline; dorsal-ventral [DV] from the pial surface, in mm) were +1.2 AP, +1.5 ML, 0.7 DV for the forelimb area, and −1.2 AP, +1.0 ML, 0.7 DV for the hindlimb area. Systematic injection to the sites at +1.2, 0, and −1.2 AP and from 0.5 to 3.0 ML was also performed. BDA (10,000 MW, lysine-fixable; Molecular Probes, Eugene, OR, United States) was dissolved in PBS (10%) and 0.5 μl was injected over 5 min. Five min after the injection was completed, the tip was slowly removed from the cortex, and the mice were returned to their cages.
To examine the labeled fibers in the brain and spinal cord, the mice were transcardially perfused with 4% PFA/PBS at 8 and 15 days after the injection, respectively. The brain sections, prepared as described previously, were incubated with ABC for 30 min and subjected to the DAB reaction for 20 min at room temperature. The spinal cord sections (approximately C2 and L1–3 levels) were further subjected to tyramide signal amplification (TSA). For this, after ABC treatment, the sections were incubated with biotin tyramide (TSA Biotin system; PerkinElmer, Waltham, MA, United States) for 10 min. After washing with TBST, the sections were again incubated with ABC for 30 min. After washing with TBST, the signals were visualized by incubation with DAB for 20 min at room temperature. The sections were mounted on MAS-coated slide glasses (Matsunami Glass Industry, Osaka, Japan), dehydrated through an ethanol series, and cleared in xylene, and the coverslips were mounted using Poly-mount (Polysciences, Warrington, PA, United States). Images were observed and recorded using microscopes (Axioplan 2; Carl Zeiss Microscopy, Jena, Germany and BZ-8000; Keyence, Osaka, Japan). In all cases, the precise location of the BDA injection site in the target area was confirmed by examining the stained sections.
3D Reconstruction
3D reconstruction of the serial sections was done as described previously (Okada et al., 2019). Briefly, the 2D images of the serial sections were aligned using AutoAligner alignment software (Bitplane, Zürich, Switzerland) on the basis of the shape of the sections and the location of the signals. Stacks of the aligned images were imported into Imaris software (Bitplane) and transformed into 3D images.
Quantification of Labeled Fibers
To examine the distribution of the BDA-labeled fibers in the spinal cord, the BDA signals in the spinal cord sections were quantified using ImageJ software1. Digital images were obtained using a microscope (Axioplan 2) with a 5 × objective lens and a cooled CCD camera (VB-7010, Keyence). Bright-field images were converted to 8-bit grayscale, and the colors of the images were subsequently inverted. The threshold of the background signal was determined using the triangle algorithm (Zack et al., 1977) in the Auto Threshold method of ImageJ software. The integrated density, the sum of the values of the signals above the threshold, was measured for each region of interest (ROI). To quantify the signals in the descending tract, ROIs were drawn to outline the contralateral dorsal funiculus (cDF) and ipsilateral lateral funiculus (iLF) (Figure 6A) using dark-field illumination, which can highlight the border between the white and gray matter. Percent integrated density was calculated as the ratio of the integrated density in each ROI to the sum of the integrated density in the cDF and iLF of the cervical and lumbar cord regions. To quantify the signal in the contralateral and ipsilateral side, the gray matter was divided into right and left halves. To quantify the signal along the dorsoventral axis, the gray matter was divided into quarters and ROIs were drawn to outline the dorsal quarter, intermediate half, and ventral quarter on both sides (Figure 6A). Percent integrated density was calculated as the ratio of the integrated density in each ROI to the sum of the integrated density in the gray matter.
Intracortical Microstimulation and Electromyography
Intracortical microstimulation and electromyogram (EMG) recordings were performed using the methods reported previously (Li and Waters, 1991; Serradj et al., 2014; Gu et al., 2017; Ueno et al., 2018) with some modifications. Briefly, after injection of atropine sulfate (0.05 mg/kg body weight, i.p.), the mice were anesthetized by injection of ketamine (100 mg/kg body weight, i.p.). During surgery, isoflurane inhalation was used in conjunction. One-fifth the amount of ketamine was further added when a pain-induced reflex or spontaneous movement was observed during surgery and recording. The mouse was placed in a stereotaxic frame (Narishige, Tokyo, Japan) and craniotomy was performed. A polyurethane-coated coaxial microelectrode (200 μm diameter, 50 kΩ tip resistance; Unique Medical, Tokyo, Japan) was inserted into the motor cortex (+0.75 AP, +1.5 ML, 0.6–1.0 DV) and 14 square-pulses of current stimulations (200-μs duration, 3-ms intervals, 20–100 μA) were delivered every 1 s using a pulse generator (SEN-7103M; Nihon Kohden, Tokyo, Japan) and an isolator (SS-401J, Nihon Kohden). When forelimb movements were observed, the EMG responses were recorded differentially using nichrome wire electrodes (tip 0.5 mm deinsulated) inserted into the bilateral biceps and triceps muscles with an amplifier (MEG-6108, Nihon Kohden) with low- and high-frequency cutoffs of <150 Hz and >3 kHz, respectively. EMG responses to 4 square-pulses of current stimulation (200-μs duration, 3-ms intervals, every 550 ms, 20–100 μA) to the motor cortex were recorded using an AD converter (model 1401 plus; Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, United Kingdom) and Spike2 software (ver. 7.2; Cambridge Electronic Design). The EMGs were rectified and averaged 500 times with triggering by microstimulations. At the end of the experiment, electric lesions (20 μA, 10 s) were made to mark the location of the recording sites. The mouse was transcardially perfused with 4% PFA under deep anesthesia and the brain was histologically examined.
Grid-Walking Test
The grid-walking test was performed essentially as described previously (Starkey et al., 2005). Male mice (11–12 weeks old) were placed on an elevated wire grid (32 × 20 cm square with 11 × 11 mm grids, placed 50 cm from the floor) and allowed to explore freely for 3 min. Behavior was recorded using a digital camera (EX-FR100; Casio, Tokyo, Japan) at 30 frames per sec and scored later. An investigator blinded to the mouse genotype counted the number of foot-fault errors, which were scored when one of the limbs fell below the grid surface (Figure 8A). Because the mice moved their forelimbs four times more frequently than their hindlimbs in this test, we independently analyzed the first 200 steps taken by the forelimbs and the first 50 steps taken by the hindlimbs on both sides. The foot-fault rate (% foot fault) was calculated by dividing the numbers of the foot-fault errors by 200 for the forelimbs and 50 for the hindlimbs.
Staircase Test
The staircase test was performed as described previously with minor modifications (Montoya et al., 1991; Baird et al., 2001). A staircase apparatus for mice (model 80301) was purchased from Melquest (Toyama, Japan). It consists of a start chamber with a clear, hinged lid and a narrow corridor with a central platform and a double staircase (Figure 8C′). The central platform extends along the length of the corridor, and a removable double staircase with 8 steps on each side can be inserted into the space between the platform and walls. Food pellets (Dustless Precision Pellet, 20 mg; Bioserv, Flemington, NJ, United States) are baited in the shallow well of each step (Figure 8C), and the mouse retrieves the pellets on either side only with the forelimb of the same side because it cannot turn around in the corridor.
At the beginning, male mice (11–12 weeks old) were familiarized to the food pellets by 55 mg per g body weight per d being placed in their cages on 3 consecutive days. On the next day, the mice were habituated to the staircase box for 30 min with four pellets baited along the platform as well as two pellets baited on each step of both sides. From this day, the mice were deprived of food for 18–20 h before the daily test session. Over the next 4 days (test session), the mice were placed in the apparatus with single pellets baited on each well in the lower seven steps on both sides. During the test time (30 min), the mice were allowed to enter the corridor and reach, retrieve, and eat the pellets freely. At the end of each session, an experimenter checked the number and place of the remaining pellets to calculate the following values and to evaluate skilled function. Pellets baited on the second step were not scored because many of the mice used their tongues to retrieve pellets (wild-type, n = 5/6; DKO, n = 6/6), but pellets were still presented on these steps because otherwise the mice seemed to lose their motivation to reach the pellets in the lower steps. The “number of pellets collected” (scores from 0 to 12) was calculated by subtracting the number of the remaining pellets from the total number of pellets baited from the third to eighth well on both sides, which indicated the number of pellets successfully eaten by the mouse during the session. The “maximum distance reached” (scores from 0 to 6) indicated the deepest well reached by the mouse (the larger of the right or left) regardless of whether it ate, dropped, or knocked down the pellets; scores 1–6 indicate that the mouse reached the third to eighth well, respectively. The “success rate” was calculated by dividing the “number of pellets collected” by the sum of the wells reached by the mouse on both sides. The mouse’s behavior was recorded using a digital camera for later analysis.
Single Pellet-Reaching Test
The single pellet-reaching test was performed as described previously, with minor modifications (Farr and Whishaw, 2002; Chen et al., 2014). The apparatus was a clear box made of plexiglass (20 cm high, 15 cm deep, 8.5 cm wide, measured from the outside, and 5 mm thick) that had three vertical slits (13 cm high, 0.5 cm wide): one central slit on one side and two lateral slits (2.5 cm lateral to the center) on the opposite side. At the beginning, male mice (13–15 weeks old) were habituated to the food pellets (Dustless Precision pellet, 20 mg; Bioserv) for 2 days, by 55 mg per g body weight per day being placed in their cages. On the next day, the mice were familiarized to the apparatus with 20 pellets placed in it for 20 min. From this day, the amount of laboratory chow was adjusted to maintain approximately 90% of the free-feeding weight. A shaping session started on the following day. The mice were placed in the apparatus with a tilted food tray affixed to the front of the center slit and filled with pellets. In this session, the mice used both forelimbs to reach for the pellets. The shaping session was finished when the mouse performed 20 reaching attempts within 20 min. If the mouse did not perform 20 reaching attempts within 20 min, the shaping session was conducted again on the next day. If the mouse could not complete shaping within 5 days, it was excluded from further testing (1 of 10 wild-type mice and 1 of 11 DKO mice). The preferred limb was determined by counting which forelimb the mice used more frequently (>50%) in the shaping session. The test sessions started the day after shaping and lasted for 8 days, one session per day. A wild-type mouse died for unknown reasons after completing the shaping session and thus was excluded from further analysis.
In the test sessions, the mice were placed in the apparatus with the double-slit side facing downward. A holding plate (10 mm high) was affixed to the front wall of the apparatus. To place a pellet at the same position consistently, two divots were made: 6.5 mm from the front wall and 4 mm medial to the center of each lateral slit. A single pellet was baited on the divot of the preferred side of each mouse. Because the mice pronated the forelimb medially to reach for the pellet, the medial displacement of the divot encouraged them to reach with the preferred limb. A daily test session was finished when 30 reaching attempts were performed or the time limit of 20 min was exceeded. Behavior was recorded using a digital camera at 30 frames per sec for later analysis. Reaching attempts were classified into four categories: success, drop, loss, and failure. “Success” means that the mouse successfully grasped the pellet and brought it into its mouth. “Drop” means that the mouse grasped the pellet and dropped it inside the chamber before putting it into its mouth. “Loss” means that the mouse grasped the pellet and dropped it outside the chamber and thus could not eat it. “Failure” means that the mouse missed, touched, or knocked the pellet and failed to grasp it eventually. “Failure” also includes drawing the pellet without grasping regardless of whether the mouse ultimately brought the pellet into its mouth. The rates for success, drop, loss, and failure were calculated by dividing the numbers of success, drop, loss, and failure by the respective total attempts.
To examine the reaching movement trajectories of the forelimbs, five successful reaches of each mouse were analyzed using the recorded video. All the mice except 1 Sulf1/2 DKO mouse achieved at least five successful reaches during the total test period. If a mouse achieved five or more successful reaches on the last test day, the first five successful reaches on the day were analyzed. If a mouse achieved less than 5 successful reaches on the last test day, successful reaches on the previous days were included for analysis. The positions of the distal tip of the second digit, second metacarpophalangeal joint, and wrist were marked separately in single frames and each trajectory was calculated using MTrackJ2 (Meijering et al., 2012), a plugin for ImageJ software. To analyze the velocity profiles of the reaching movements, the positions of the distal tip of the second digit in the five consecutive frames (P1–P5) in a successful reach, in which P3 corresponds to the points of pellet grasping, were marked in the video, and the velocity between 2 points (vi means the velocity between Pi and Pi+1) was measured.
Rotarod Test
The rotarod test was performed using a rotarod apparatus (ENV-577M; Med Associate Inc, Fairfax, VT, United States). Five mice were put on a rod and the length of time that each remained on the rotating rod was measured. The mice were subjected to a total of 6 sessions on consecutive days (three sessions per day), with accelerating speed (4–40 rpm) over 5 min.
Open Field Test
The open field test was performed using a square arena [OF-36(M)SQ; 500 × 500 mm, wall height of 400 mm; Muromachi Kikai, Tokyo, Japan] and a video tracking system (ANY-maze; Stoelting, Wood Dale, IL, United States). Each mouse was allowed to walk in the arena freely for 30 min, and the total distance traveled was measured.
Hot Plate Test
The hot plate test was performed as previously described, with minor modifications (O’Callaghan and Holtzman, 1975). A male mouse (17–18 weeks old) was placed on a hot plate maintained at 55°C (FHP-450; Tokyo Garasu Kikai, Tokyo, Japan), and the latency to jump, paw-shaking or paw-licking was recorded.
Statistical Analysis
All the statistical tests were performed using Prism 4.0c (GraphPad Software; San Diego, CA, United States). Statistical significance in the staircase test, single pellet-reaching task, and BDA tracing study was evaluated using one-way or two-way repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) or two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc tests. Statistical significance in the grid walking test, hot plate test, and velocity analysis was evaluated using the Mann–Whitney U test.
Results
CST Defects in the Adult Sulf1/2 DKO Brain Revealed by PKCγ Staining
To examine the CST trajectory in the adult brain, we first performed immunohistochemistry for PKCγ, a marker for CST fibers in the adult mouse brain (Mori et al., 1990; Ding et al., 2005; Joshi et al., 2008) because this staining is useful to overview all the CST fibers. In the wild-type mice, the CST fibers formed the cerebral peduncle in the caudal forebrain (Figure 1A1), passed ventrally (Figures 1B1,C1), and extended medially toward the pons (Figures 1D1–F1). In the medulla, the CST fibers formed the pyramidal tract (Figure 2A1), extended toward the midline (Figure 2B1), decussated (Figures 2C1,D1), and entered the contralateral dorsal funiculus of the spinal cord (Figure 2E1). In the DKO mice, the CST fibers appeared almost normal up to the ventral midbrain (Figures 1A2–C2), whereas at the level of the pons, the abnormal fibers extended dorsally on the surface of the midbrain (Figures 1D2–F2). In addition, a small number of the fibers projected toward the superior colliculus through the thalamus (Figures 1B2–E2, 1B2′–E2′, arrowheads). In the medulla, the pyramidal tract became flattened and laterally widened (Figures 2A2,B2). A part of the fibers near the midline crossed the midline, whereas others located more laterally extended to the ventrolateral surface of the medulla (Figures 2C2–E2).
To examine the midline crossing, we reconstructed 3D images of the CST trajectory from the PKCγ-stained brain sections. In the wild-type mice, the CST fibers ran close to the midline and crossed contralaterally at the pyramidal decussation (Figures 2F–H). Most of the fibers entered the dorsal funiculus, whereas a few fibers, which form the dorsolateral CST (Steward et al., 2004), descended in a more lateral position (Figures 2E1,H, dlcst). In contrast, the Sulf1/2 DKO mice showed defects in the pyramidal decussation (Figures 2J–L and Supplementary Figure S1). First, the CST fibers were located more laterally than those in the wild-type mice: the largest distances between the midline and the medial border of the CST fibers in the medulla were 161.1 ± 18.7 μm in the wild-type mice (n = 6 CSTs from three mice; Figure 2I) and 414.1 ± 17.6 μm in the DKO mice (n = 6 CSTs from three mice; Figure 2M), whereas those between the midline and the lateral borders of the CST fibers were 693.0 ± 14.2 μm in the wild-type mice and 1212.6 ± 28.1 μm in the DKO mice (Figures 2I,M). Secondly, in the DKO mice, the laterally deviated fibers descended ipsilaterally without midline crossing, whereas the medially located fibers crossed the midline (Figures 2K,L). The outermost fibers that crossed the midline in the DKO mice were 765.3 ± 36.9 μm apart from the midline (Figure 2M), which was close to the distance of the outermost fibers from the midline in the wild-type mice.
BDA Tracing of the CST Fibers in the Sulf1/2 DKO Brain
Given that PKCγ staining is also positive in non-CST neurons, to see the CST trajectory more specifically, we next performed anterograde tracing of the CST fibers using BDA in wild-type (n = 15) and Sulf1/2 DKO (n = 21) mice. BDA was stereotaxically injected into the primary motor area and subsequently the CST trajectories were visualized by detecting the BDA-positive fibers by means of the avidin-biotin peroxidase complex (ABC) and DAB reaction. In both the wild-type and the DKO brains, the labeled fibers extended into the internal capsule and cerebral peduncle, with their branches projecting to the striatum, thalamus, red nucleus, and pretectum (Figures 3A1–C1,A2–C2). At the level of the pons, in the wild-type mice, the CST fibers turned medially from the lateral surface of the brain (Figure 3D1), ran beneath the pons, sent branches to the pons (Figure 3E1, open arrow), and descended caudally to form the pyramidal tract (Figure 3F1). In the DKO mice, abnormal CST fibers were found on the lateral surface of the midbrain (Figures 3D2–F2). In the caudal pons, a flattened and widened pyramidal tract was observed (Figure 3F2).
In the tectum of the wild-type mice, BDA-positive collaterals were observed in the superior colliculus (Figure 3F1). These fibers ran through the thalamus and formed a dense clump at the lateral aspect of the superior colliculus (Figures 3F1′,F1″) and subsequently projected medially into the intermediate layer of the superior colliculus (Figure 3F1′). In the DKO brain, a similar distribution of the labeled fibers in the intermediate layer was observed, although the density of the fibers was much higher than in the wild-type mice (Figures 3F2,F2′,F2″). In addition, a thick bundle of the labeled fibers traversed the deep layer of the superior colliculus (n = 12/21, Figures 3F2′,F2″).
In the medulla of the wild-type mice, the labeled fibers crossed the midline (Figures 3H1,H1′,I1). In the DKO mice, the labeled fibers split into 2 bundles. The lateral bundle extended in the ventrolateral position ipsilaterally (n = 21/21; Figures 3G2–I2, black arrowheads), whereas the medial bundle reached the midline at the pyramidal decussation and most of the fibers crossed the midline (Figures 3H2–I2, open arrowheads). In some cases, a few fibers reached the midline and returned to the ipsilateral side (n = 8/21; Figures 3H2–I2, blue arrowheads); projection to the ipsilateral dorsal funiculus was also observed in the wild-type mice (n = 2/15, data not shown), but the frequency and number of these fibers were higher in the DKO mice. Other fibers entered the ventral funiculus on the ipsilateral side in the DKO mice (n = 9/21; Figure 3H2′, red arrowhead); a few fibers were also seen in the ipsilateral ventral funiculus of the wild-type mice (n = 2/15, data not shown). These analyses clearly demonstrated the presence of midline crossing errors in the Sulf1/2 DKO mouse (see Supplementary Table S1 for summary).
Next, we performed 3D reconstruction of the BDA-labeled CST fibers in the whole brain. In the wild-type mice, the CST fibers descended through the internal capsule, cerebral peduncle, and pyramidal tract and crossed the midline at the pyramidal decussation (Figures 4A,B). It was apparent that the CST fibers entered the contralateral dorsal funiculus (Figure 4B). In addition to the main tract, collateral branches and their fiber terminals were observed in the striatum, thalamus, superior colliculus, pons, and dorsal column nuclei (Figure 4A; Catsman-Berrevoets and Kuypers, 1981; O’Leary et al., 1990; Lévesque et al., 1996; Wang et al., 2018). In the DKO mice, the most prominent abnormality was the U-shaped trajectory in the midbrain (n = 21/21; Figures 4C,E; arrows). After the fibers extended dorsally toward the superior colliculus, most of them returned to the pons. These data clearly showed that the aberrant U-shaped detour of the CST fibers observed in the embryonic DKO brain persisted into adulthood in all the DKO mice. In the 3D reconstruction images, variable decussation defects were observed in the DKO mice. Consistent with the PKCγ staining results, the lateral bundle extended ipsilaterally (Figures 4D,F, white arrowheads), whereas the medial bundle reached the midline and some of the fibers crossed the midline (Figures 4D,F, open arrowheads) and others returned to the ipsilateral side (Figure 4D, blue arrowhead). Furthermore, defasciculation of the CST fibers in the cerebral peduncle was also observed (n = 9/21; Figure 4E, arrowheads). Information about all the mice analyzed and their phenotypes is summarized in Supplementary Table S1.
Bilateral CST Fiber Projections in the Spinal Cord of Sulf1/2 DKO Mice
Next, we examined the projection of the CST fibers in the spinal cord. When BDA was injected stereotaxically into the forelimb area of the Primary motor cortex (M1) of the wild-type mice (Figure 5A; a representative stereotaxic coordinate, +1.2 AP, +1.5 ML, 0.7 DV, in mm), the labeled fibers were observed in the ventromedial portion of the dorsal funiculus in the cervical spinal cord on the contralateral side of the injection (Figure 5C). The fibers entered the intermediate portion of the gray matter (Figure 5C) and their terminals showed bouton-like structures (Figures 5C′,C″, open arrows). Thus, the CST fiber projection was almost exclusively confined to the contralateral side in the wild-type mice. Few signals reached the lumbar spinal cord (Figure 5D).
In the DKO mice, the labeled fibers descended in the lateral funiculus on the ipsilateral side in addition to in the dorsal funiculus on the contralateral side (Figure 5G). The fibers in the lateral funiculus entered the ipsilateral gray matter in a medial direction (Figure 5G′, arrowheads), whereas the fibers in the dorsal funiculus entered the contralateral gray matter in a ventrolateral direction (Figure 5G). In addition, a few fibers crossed the midline in the spinal cord (Supplementary Figure S2). The terminals of the fibers from both the lateral and the dorsal funiculi had bouton-like structures (Figure 5G″, arrows). Thus, the CST fiber projection was bilateral in the DKO mice. Few signals reached the lumbar spinal cord (Figure 5H), indicating that the CST fibers originating from the forelimb area projected to their inherent projection levels in the DKO mice.
When BDA was injected into the hindlimb area of the M1 of the wild-type mice (Figure 5B; a representative stereotaxic coordinate, −1.2 AP, +1.0 ML, 0.7 DV), the labeled fibers passed into the contralateral dorsal funiculus at the cervical level with few projecting into the gray matter (Figure 5E). At the lumbar level, the fibers in the dorsal funiculus projected into the dorsal portion of the gray matter (Figures 5F,F′,F″). In the DKO mice, the labeled fibers descended in both the ipsilateral lateral funiculus and the contralateral dorsal funiculus (Figures 5I,J). Projection into the gray matter was scarce in the cervical spinal cord (Figure 5I), whereas bilateral projection to the dorsal portion was observed in the lumbar spinal cord (Figures 5J,J′,J″). These findings suggest that the CST fibers of the DKO mice terminate at the appropriate levels of the spinal cord even though the trajectories are aberrant.
To compare the differences in the CST projection between the wild-type and DKO mice precisely, we quantitated the distribution of the BDA-labeled fibers in the spinal cord. Because the extent of labeling differed among individuals, we measured the total number of signals present in the cervical or lumbar spinal cord and calculated the percentages of the signals in the regions of interest (ROIs). To this end, we used the integrated density, the sum of the values of the signals above the threshold, which was determined using ImageJ software. The ratio of the labeled fibers in each ROI among all the fibers in the white or gray matter was shown as the percent integrated density, the ratio of the integrated density in each ROI to the sum of integrated density in the white or gray matter in the cervical or lumbar spinal cord (please refer to “Quantification of labeled axons” in the section “Materials and Methods” for a detailed description of the method). We first compared the percentages of the normal fibers in the contralateral dorsal funiculus (cDF) and of the abnormal fibers in the ipsilateral lateral funiculus (iLF, Figure 6A). Quantitative analyses demonstrated that the CST fibers were confined to the cDF in the wild-type mice, whereas they were present more in the iLF and less in the cDF in the Sulf1/2 DKO mice (Figures 6B,C). Next, we compared the laterality. The CST fibers projected to the gray matter on the contralateral side in the wild-type mice, whereas they projected bilaterally in the DKO mice (Figures 6D,E). Finally, we compared the dorsoventral distribution of the CST fibers. For this purpose, we divided the spinal cord into four parts along the dorsoventral axis and compared the distribution in the dorsal quarter, intermediate half, and ventral quarter (Figure 6A). This analysis showed that when the signals on both sides were added, the dorsoventral distribution within the gray matter was almost the same in the wild-type and DKO mice (Figures 6F,G).
Topographical Organization of the Motor Area in Sulf1/2 DKO Mice
We then wondered whether the topographical organization of the motor cortex is the same in wild-type and Sulf1/2 DKO mice. To address this question, we performed systematic injection of BDA at different sites throughout the sensorimotor cortex (Supplementary Figure S3) and examined the projection in the cervical and lumbar spinal cord regions. Dense projection to the cervical spinal cord was found when BDA was injected at +1.2 and 0 AP, from 1.0 to 2.0 ML in both the wild-type and the DKO mice (Supplementary Figure S3). Projection to the lumbar spinal cord was found when BDA was injected at 0 AP, from 1.0 to 1.5 ML, and at −1.2 AP, from 0.5 to 1.5 ML in both the wild-type and the DKO mice (Supplementary Figure S4), indicating that the motor area projecting to the lumbar spinal cord was caudal and medial to that projecting to the cervical spinal cord, with overlap between them, which is consistent with the motor cortex map determined by electrical or optogenetic stimulation (Li and Waters, 1991; Ayling et al., 2009; Tennant et al., 2011). Thus, the overall topographical organization of the motor area of the wild-type and DKO mice was roughly the same. These data also suggest that in DKO mice, the CST fibers take two routes through the contralateral dorsal funiculus and the ipsilateral lateral funiculus regardless of the positions of their origin in the motor cortex and that they terminate at their inherent levels regardless of the route they take.
M1-Stimulated Bilateral Responses in Sulf1/2 DKO Mice
Our BDA tracing studies clearly demonstrated that the CST fibers projected bilaterally in the spinal cord of the Sulf1/2 DKO mice. This observation led us to wonder whether the uncrossed fibers projecting to the ipsilateral spinal cord in the DKO mice control motor neuron activity. To test this, we measured the EMG responses after intracortical microstimulation of the M1. An anesthetized mouse was placed in a stereotaxic frame and a stimulating electrode was inserted into the M1 area. Electrical stimulation of the M1 on one side evoked contralateral forelimb movements in the wild-type mice, whereas the same stimulation evoked bilateral movements in the DKO mice. Consistent with the forelimb movements, when EMG responses were observed, activation of the EMG after four pulses of intracortical microstimulations was observed only from the contralateral muscles of the wild-type mice, whereas bilateral responses were detected in the DKO mice (Figure 7 and Supplementary Figure S5). These results show that the CST fibers are functionally connected to both the ipsilateral and the contralateral motor neurons, thereby controlling bilateral motor neuron activity in the DKO mouse.
Deficits in Fine Motor Movements in Sulf1/2 DKO Mice
The Sulf1/2 DKO mice appeared to behave normally: they did not show gait disturbance, ataxia, or abnormal movements. Consistent with this, the DKO mice performed normally in the rotarod and open field tests (Supplementary Figures S6A,B), suggesting that gross motor movements were normal in the DKO mice. In addition, in the hot plate test, the wild-type and DKO mice showed no significant differences in the time taken to avoid painful thermal stimuli, indicating that temperature and pain sensation was normal in the DKO mice (Supplementary Figure S6C). To examine possible deficits in fine motor movements in Sulf1/2 DKO mice, we performed three different behavioral tests that have been used for evaluating the motor functions of normal mice and disease models (Brooks and Dunnett, 2009; Schönfeld et al., 2017). First, we performed a grid-walking test that can assess sensorimotor function and motor coordination (Starkey et al., 2005). In this test, the mice were made to walk on an elevated wire grid for 3 min and foot-fault errors were analyzed (Figure 8A). The rate for the foot fault was higher in the Sulf1/2 DKO mice than in the wild-type mice: the difference was significant in the hindlimbs but not in the forelimbs (Figure 8B).
We next performed a double staircase test (Montoya et al., 1991; Baird et al., 2001). To this end, we used an apparatus that has a start chamber and a narrow corridor with a central platform and a double staircase (Figure 8C′). The removable staircase has eight steps on each side, and small food pellets are placed in the shallow well of each step (Figure 8C). In this apparatus, a mouse can retrieve pellets on either side using the forelimb of the same side (Figure 8D, arrows), enabling evaluation of the mouse’s ability to reach the pellets with the respective forelimbs. A food-deprived mouse was placed in the apparatus with single pellets baited on each well on both sides, and the mouse was allowed to retrieve pellets freely for 30 min over 4 days. At the end of each session, the number and place of the remaining pellets were examined. The number of pellets that the mouse ate successfully (“number of pellets collected”) was lower in the DKO mice than in the wild-type mice (Figure 8E). The “success rate,” which was calculated by dividing the “number of pellets collected” by the sum of the wells reached by the mouse on both sides (Figure 8F), was also lower in the DKO mice than in the wild-type mice throughout the test period (Figure 8G), suggesting motor deficits in forelimb movement.
We then performed a single pellet-reaching test (Farr and Whishaw, 2002; Chen et al., 2014). In this test, the mice were trained to use a forelimb to retrieve a small pellet through a narrow slit of a clear box (Figures 9A,B). First, a food-deprived mouse was trained to retrieve pellets from the center slit until the mouse performed 20 reaching attempts within 20 min. Because the mouse used both forelimbs, the preferred limb was determined. In the subsequent test sessions (one session per day, for 8 days), a single pellet was placed in a divot at the fixed position (Figure 9B) to force the mouse to retrieve the pellet with the preferred forelimb. The rate at which the mouse successfully grasped a pellet and brought it into its mouth was higher in the wild-type mice than in the DKO mice (Figure 9C), indicating impaired performance in pellet retrieval in the DKO mice. The success rate became higher during the test sessions in the wild-type mice but not in the DKO mice (Figure 9C and Supplementary Figure S7).
To examine why the DKO mice were less successful at reaching the pellets, we performed kinematic measures of the trajectory and velocity of the forelimb movement (Whishaw, 1996). The positions of the distal tip of the second digit, second metacarpophalangeal (MCP) joint, and wrist were marked separately in single frames and each trajectory was examined (Figure 9D). As shown in Figure 9E, the trajectories of the digit and MCP joint, but not of the wrist, in the successful reaches were shorter in the DKO mice than in the wild-type mice. In addition, the speed of the digit near the pellet was slowed down in the wild-type mice but accelerated in the DKO mice (Figures 9F–K). This analysis revealed that DKO mice use different goal-directed action strategies even in successful reaches.
Discussion
In this study, by taking advantage of the survival into adulthood of Sulf1/2 DKO mice on a mixed genetic background, we demonstrated several aspects of the CST defects in the adult brain. First, the abnormal dorsal extension of the CST fibers toward the superior colliculus, which was observed in DKO embryos (Okada et al., 2017), persisted in the adult brain. Second, at the pyramidal decussation, some fibers located close to the midline crossed the midline, whereas others located more laterally did not. Third, in the spinal cord, the crossed fibers descended in the contralateral dorsal funiculus and entered the contralateral gray matter, whereas the uncrossed fibers descended in the lateral funiculus on the ipsilateral side and entered the ipsilateral gray matter. These results showed that the CST fibers of Sulf1/2 DKO mice project bilaterally in the spinal cord. Consistently, electric stimulation of M1 neurons on 1 side evoked bilateral EMG responses in the DKO mice. We also demonstrated impaired performance of the DKO mice in behavioral tests used for evaluating motor functions, suggesting deficits in their motor movement.
Pyramidal Decussation Defects
Various CST defects at the pyramidal decussation were reported in mutant mice (Canty and Murphy, 2008; Leyva-Díaz and López-Bendito, 2013; Welniarz et al., 2017a). These mutant mice include mutants for Semaphorin 6A (Sema6a) and its receptors Plexin A3 (Plxna3)/Plexin A4 (Plxna4; Faulkner et al., 2008; Rünker et al., 2008); Netrin receptors DCC (Kanga mutant expressing a truncated DCC protein [Dcckanga]) and UNC5C (rostral cerebellar malformation mutant, Unc5crcm; Finger et al., 2002); and cell adhesion molecules NCAM (Ncam1; Rolf et al., 2002) and L1 (L1cam; Dahme et al., 1997; Cohen et al., 1998).
In the Sema6a, Plxna3, or Plxna4 KO mice and the Plxna3/Plxna4 DKO mice, a part of the CST fibers deviated from the midline and ran down the ventrolateral position of the medulla, whereas those near the midline decussated normally (Faulkner et al., 2008; Rünker et al., 2008). The authors of those studies argued that the phenotype was caused by the loss of the constraint of the CST fibers to the midline, which is normally brought about by the repulsive signal of Sema6A in the inferior olive (Faulkner et al., 2008; Rünker et al., 2008). The CST abnormalities of these KO mice seem similar to those of the Sulf1/2 DKO mice. However, the Sema6a KO mice showed aberrant circumferential projection of the CST fibers in the medulla (Okada et al., 2019), which was never observed in the Sulf1/2 DKO mice, indicating that the molecular mechanisms causing the decussation defects differ for Sema6a-Plxna3/a4 and Sulf1/2 mutant mice.
In the Dcckanga mutant mice, the CST fibers spread into the medial and lateral bundles just before the pyramidal decussation and neither of them crossed the midline (Finger et al., 2002; Welniarz et al., 2017b). The medial bundle extended ipsilaterally in the ventral funiculus, whereas the lateral bundle extended ipsilaterally in the ventral portion of the lateral funiculus (Finger et al., 2002). Thus, the CST defects in the Dcckanga mutant mice seem to differ from those in the Sulf1/2 DKO mice. In the Unc5crcm mutant mice, the CST fibers formed two distinct bundles at the pyramidal decussation: one bundle in the normal position near the midline and another located more laterally. Whilst the fibers of the former crossed the midline normally, most entered the dorsal gray matter adjacent to the dorsal funiculus instead of entering into the dorsal funiculus (Finger et al., 2002). Thus, the properties of the crossed fibers differ from those in the Sulf1/2 DKO mice. By contrast, the fibers of the laterally located bundle descended ipsilaterally in the outermost region of the lateral funiculus of the spinal cord (Finger et al., 2002). Therefore, the decussation error phenotype in the Unc5c mutant mice is similar to that in the Sulf1/2 DKO mice. In this regard, it is interesting that different types of HS selective for DCC and UNC-5 are assumed to lead to the formation of the DCC/DCC and DCC/UNC-5 complex with Netrin-1, thereby causing axonal attraction and repulsion, respectively (Finci et al., 2014). Sulf1/2-mediated modification of HS may affect HS-dependent formation of the netrin receptor complex and consequent netrin-mediated responses.
In the L1cam and Ncam1 KO mice, a significant portion of the CST fibers failed to cross the midline and entered the ipsilateral dorsal funiculus (Cohen et al., 1998; Rolf et al., 2002). In addition, in the Ncam1 KO mice, the CST fibers frequently extended laterally instead of growing dorsally (Rolf et al., 2002). In the L1cam KO mice, the CST fibers were reduced in number and not observed caudal to the cervical spinal cord (Dahme et al., 1997; Cohen et al., 1998). These phenotypes differ largely from those observed in the Sulf1/2 DKO mice.
These comparisons revealed that none of the mice with loss-of-function mutations in one of these genes showed the same CST phenotype as that of the Sulf1/2 DKO mice. It is possible that the gain-of-function mutations in these genes are related to the phenotype of the DKO mice or that the combination of mutations in more than one gene causes the phenotype.
In the Sulf1/2 DKO mice, the CST fibers within approximately 700 μm of the midline decussated almost normally, whereas the fibers outside this border did not, suggesting that the midline-crossing signals may operate within this range. Viewed from this standpoint, it is likely that the location of the CST fibers in the medulla after their return from the aberrant detour in the midbrain may determine whether they cross the midline. Lateral positioning of the CST fibers may be influenced by Slit-Robo signaling, as proposed in mutant Drosophila and mouse studies (Rajagopalan et al., 2000; Simpson et al., 2000; Farmer et al., 2008; Jaworski et al., 2010).
Descending CST Fibers in the Lateral Funiculus of the Spinal Cord
The CST is a major descending pathway that appeared in mammals evolutionarily. Although the anatomy of the CST is similar across species, the location of its fibers in the spinal cord is different from one species to another (Lemon, 2008; Welniarz et al., 2017a). In rodents, the crossed fibers are located in the ventral portion of the dorsal funiculus, whereas a small number of uncrossed fibers are present in the ventral funiculus (Brösamle and Schwab, 1997). Despite the different locations, the crossed and uncrossed fibers originate from the same cortical areas (Brösamle and Schwab, 1997). In primates, by contrast, the crossed fibers are located in the dorsolateral funiculus, whereas the uncrossed fibers are located in the ventral and dorsolateral funiculi (Welniarz et al., 2017a). In Sulf1/2 DKO mice and in Dcc and Unc5c mutants, the uncrossed fibers descend in the lateral funiculus, suggesting that it provides a permissive substrate for CST axon elongation beyond species. It will thus be interesting to investigate the molecular mechanisms that determine the trajectory choice, especially the genetic elements that influence species differences, as shown in a previous study that revealed the mechanism underlying manual dexterity (Gu et al., 2017). In addition, our finding that the CST fibers of the Sulf1/2 DKO mice terminate in their inherent levels irrespective of the route they take suggests the presence of mechanisms that stop the projection of the growing axons at the appropriate levels of the spinal cord.
Bilateral Projection of the CST Fibers in the Spinal Cord
In rodents, the majority of CST fibers cross the midline at the pyramidal decussation and project to the contralateral spinal cord (Welniarz et al., 2017a). In Sulf1/2 DKO mice, in addition to the normal, contralateral projection of the crossed fibers, the uncrossed fibers enter the ipsilateral spinal cord, resulting in bilateral projection. Our EMG studies showed that microstimulation of the M1 on 1 side evoked comparable responses in the bilateral forelimb muscles, indicating that the uncrossed fibers also form functional synapses and control the ipsilateral motor activity.
Bilateral CST projection was also observed in other mutant mice that lack ephrin B3 (Efnb3), EphA4 (Epha4), or α-chimerin (Chn1), although the location of the abnormal midline crossing differs between these mice and Sulf1/2 DKO mice. In the above mutants, the CST fibers cross the midline at the pyramidal decussation and descend in the contralateral dorsal funiculus normally, but a considerable amount of the fibers cross the midline again after entering the gray matter, resulting in bilateral projection (Dottori et al., 1998; Kullander et al., 2001a, b; Yokoyama et al., 2001; Iwasato et al., 2007). In addition to the misprojection of the CST fibers, axons of the interneurons in the spinal cord cross the midline abnormally as a result of the loss of the midline barrier signal by ephrin B3. Interestingly, these mutant mice show a hopping gait phenotype, which was thought to be caused by the inability to perform left-right alternate movement during locomotion (Kullander et al., 2003; Borgius et al., 2014; Serradj et al., 2014; Katori et al., 2017). Spinal cord-specific deletion of Epha4 or Chn1 led to a hopping gait, whereas forebrain-specific disruption of the same genes did not lead to the phenotype (Borgius et al., 2014; Katori et al., 2017), suggesting that a hopping gait is caused by abnormal midline crossing of interneurons in the spinal cord. Given that Sulf1/2 DKO mice did not show a hopping gait, they did not have deficits in the locomotor circuit, although a few CST fibers crossed the midline in the spinal cord in those mice.
Interestingly, neurological conditions similar to the anatomical and functional CST abnormalities found in the Sulf1/2 DKO mice are observed in patients with congenital mirror movement (CMM). CMM is a rare genetic disorder that is characterized by involuntary movements on one side of the body induced by intentional movements on the opposite side (Galléa et al., 2011; Cox et al., 2012; Welniarz et al., 2017a) as the result of mutations in the DCC, RAD51, and NTN1 (netrin-1) genes (Srour et al., 2010; Depienne et al., 2012; Méneret et al., 2017). In some CMM patients, the proportion of the uncrossed CST fibers at the pyramidal decussation was increased and unilateral M1 stimulation elicited bilateral responses (Méneret et al., 2017; Welniarz et al., 2017b). Therefore, the patterns of the anatomical and physiological deficits are almost the same as those in the Sulf1/2 DKO mice, although these mice did not show apparent mirror movement in their cage or in our behavioral tests. However, forebrain-specific EphA4 KO mice, which did not show a hopping gait in normal stereotypical locomotion despite the existence of abnormal midline crossing of the CST fibers in the spinal cord, showed abnormal synchronization of left and right forelimb movement in adaptive locomotion over obstacles (Serradj et al., 2014). Thus, it would be interesting to examine whether Sulf1/2 DKO mice show synchronized movement in the same behavioral test. It may be useful to elucidate the contribution of midline-crossing abnormality to mirror movement and to address the question whether midline-crossing errors at the pyramidal decussation and in the spinal cord have different consequences for motor control in left-right synchronization.
Impaired Motor Movement
The CST plays an important role in cortical control of spinal motor neuron activity (Lemon, 2008; Welniarz et al., 2017a). Because it is a major pathway for voluntary movement, its dysfunction leads to motor impairment. In mice, lesions of the motor cortex, surgical dissection of the pyramidal tract, or optogenetic silencing of the corticospinal neurons led to motor deficits (Baird et al., 2001; Farr and Whishaw, 2002; Starkey et al., 2005; Ueno et al., 2018). The Sulf1/2 DKO mice showed impaired performance in the staircase test and single pellet-reaching tests, although their gross movements and locomotion appeared to be normal, indicating deficits in fine motor movement. In the DKO mice, in the single pellet-reaching test, the trajectories of the distal component of the forelimb and speed control near the target were different from those in the wild-type mice, indicating that the DKO mice use different goal-directed action strategies. We examined the correlation between the parameters obtained from the kinematic measurement in the single pellet-reaching test (performance, trajectory length, and speed) and the anatomical defects of the CST (signal intensity of the PKCγ staining in the contralateral/ipsilateral and dorsal/lateral funiculi) in the DKO mice, but we could not find any significant correlation between them. Analysis of the synaptic connections in the spinal cord may be necessary, although it appears to be difficult to attribute some aspect of the behavioral deficits to simple anatomical phenotypes.
In rodents, CST axons originate from the motor, somatosensory, parietal, cingulate, visual, and prefrontal regions and mediate many different functions (Lemon, 2008; Welniarz et al., 2017a). Therefore, movement impairment in Sulf1/2 DKO mice may be caused by deficits of CST function that include descending control of afferent inputs and gating and gain control of the spinal reflex in addition to excitation of motor neurons (Lemon, 2008; Welniarz et al., 2017a). Involvement of the CST fibers originating from the somatosensory cortex or other areas that affect their performance (Lemon, 2008; Wang et al., 2017; Ueno et al., 2018) should also be considered. Furthermore, it is also important to note that sensory feedback to the CST plays a critical role in controlling movement (Seki et al., 2003; Bui et al., 2013; Bourane et al., 2015). Although the sensory function assessed by the hot plate test was normal in the DKO mice, the possibility cannot be excluded that the sensory deficits due to the abnormality of the CST fiber projections from the sensory cortex affected the performance in the behavioral tests. To understand the mechanism of impaired motor movements in Sulf1/2 DKO mice, it will be important to determine whether motor deficits are caused by reduction in the number of normal crossed fibers or by interference in the normal functions of the crossed fibers by the presence of abnormal ipsilateral fibers.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.
Ethics Statement
The animal study was reviewed and approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Tsukuba.
Author Contributions
SA, TO, KK-M, and MM designed the research and performed the experiments. SA, MA, and KK-M performed the rotarod and open field tests. TD, TK, and MM performed the EGM recording. SA, TO, and TD analyzed the data. AT supervised SA and evaluated the clinical implications. SA, TO, KK-M, and MM wrote the manuscript. All the authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We thank Drs. Noriyuki Higo, Hiroshi Nishimaru, Hiroshi Kameda, Satoshi Fukuda, Naoyuki Murabe, Naohito Terui, and Marika Kato for useful comments and Flaminia Miyamasu for critical reading of the manuscript. We also thank Hitomi Yuyama, Nana Kagaya, Moe Imaizumi, and Ayaka Tsukakoshi for their support with the experiments. The apparatuses for the grid-walking test and single pellet-reaching test were made by the Research Facility Center for Science and Technology, University of Tsukuba.
Abbreviations
- ABC
avidin-biotin-peroxidase complex
- AP
anterior-posterior
- BDA
biotinylated dextran amine
- CMM
congenital mirror movement
- CST
corticospinal tract
- DAB
3,3′-diaminobenzidine
- DF
dorsal funiculus
- DKO
double knockout
- DMSO
dimethyl sulfoxide
- DV
dorsal-ventral
- EMG
electromyogram
- HS
heparan sulfate
- i.p.
intraperitoneal
- KO
knockout
- LF
lateral funiculus
- M1
primary motor cortex
- ML
medial-lateral
- PBS
phosphate-buffered saline
- PBT
PBS with 0.1% Tween-20
- PFA
paraformaldehyde
- PKC γ
protein kinase C gamma
- ROI
region of interest
- Sulf1
Sulfatase 1
- TBS
Tris-buffered saline
- TBST
Tris-buffered saline supplemented with 0.1% Tween-20
- TSA
tyramide signal amplification.
Funding. This work was partly supported by Kakenhi grants (Grant Numbers 22123006 and 25293065) from MEXT and the JSPS and by research grants from the Takeda Science Foundation and the Naito Foundation.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnmol.2019.00333/full#supplementary-material
References
- Ayling O. G., Harrison T. C., Boyd J. D., Goroshkov A., Murphy T. H. (2009). Automated light-based mapping of motor cortex by photoactivation of channelrhodopsin-2 transgenic mice. Nat. Methods 6 219–224. 10.1038/nmeth.1303 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baird A. L., Meldrum A., Dunnett S. B. (2001). The staircase test of skilled reaching in mice. Brain Res. Bull. 54 243–250. 10.1016/s0361-9230(00)00457-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borgius L., Nishimaru H., Caldeira V., Kunugise Y., Low P., Reig R., et al. (2014). Spinal glutamatergic neurons defined by EphA4 signaling are essential components of normal locomotor circuits. J. Neurosci. 34 3841–3853. 10.1523/jneurosci.4992-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bourane S., Grossmann K. S., Britz O., Dalet A., Del Barrio M. G., Stam F. J., et al. (2015). Identification of a spinal circuit for light touch and fine motor control. Cell 160 503–515. 10.1016/j.cell.2015.01.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks S. P., Dunnett S. B. (2009). Tests to assess motor phenotype in mice: a user’s guide. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10 519–529. 10.1038/nrn2652 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brösamle C., Schwab M. E. (1997). Cells of origin, course, and termination patterns of the ventral, uncrossed component of the mature rat corticospinal tract. J. Comp. Neurol. 386 293–303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bui T. V., Akay T., Loubani O., Hnasko T. S., Jessell T. M., Brownstone R. M. (2013). Circuits for grasping: spinal dI3 interneurons mediate cutaneous control of motor behavior. Neuron 78 191–204. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.02.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Canty A. J., Murphy M. (2008). Molecular mechanisms of axon guidance in the developing corticospinal tract. Prog. Neurobiol. 85 214–235. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2008.02.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Catsman-Berrevoets C. E., Kuypers H. G. (1981). A search for corticospinal collaterals to thalamus and mesencephalon by means of multiple retrograde fluorescent tracers in cat and rat. Brain Res. 218 15–33. 10.1016/0006-8993(81)90986-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen C.-C., Gilmore A., Zuo Y. (2014). Study motor skill learning by single-pellet reaching tasks in mice. J. Vis. Exp. 85:e51238. 10.3791/51238 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen N. R., Taylor J. S., Scott L. B., Guillery R. W., Soriano P., Furley A. J. (1998). Errors in corticospinal axon guidance in mice lacking the neural cell adhesion molecule L1. Curr. Biol. 8 26–33. 10.1016/s0960-9822(98)70017-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cox B. C., Cincotta M., Espay A. J. (2012). Mirror movements in movement disorders: a review. Tremor Other. Hyperkinet. Mov. 2:tre–02–59–398–1. 10.7916/d8vq31dz [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dahme M., Bartsch U., Martini R., Anliker B., Schachner M., Mantei N. (1997). Disruption of the mouse L1 gene leads to malformations of the nervous system. Nat. Genet. 17 346–349. 10.1038/ng1197-346 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Depienne C., Bouteiller D., Meneret A., Billot S., Groppa S., Klebe S., et al. (2012). RAD51 haploinsufficiency causes congenital mirror movements in humans. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 90 301–307. 10.1016/j.ajhg.2011.12.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ding Y. Q., Xiang C. X., Chen Z. F. (2005). Generation and characterization of the PKC gamma-Cre mouse line. Genesis 43 28–33. 10.1002/gene.20151 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dottori M., Hartley L., Galea M., Paxinos G., Polizzotto M., Kilpatrick T., et al. (1998). EphA4 (Sek1) receptor tyrosine kinase is required for the development of the corticospinal tract. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95 13248–13253. 10.1073/pnas.95.22.13248 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farmer W. T., Altick A. L., Nural H. F., Dugan J. P., Kidd T., Charron F., et al. (2008). Pioneer longitudinal axons navigate using floor plate and Slit/Robo signals. Development 135 3643–3653. 10.1242/dev.023325 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farr T. D., Whishaw I. Q. (2002). Quantitative and qualitative impairments in skilled reaching in the mouse (Mus musculus) after a focal motor cortex stroke. Stroke 33 1869–1875. 10.1161/01.str.0000020714.48349.4e [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faulkner R. L., Low L. K., Liu X. B., Coble J., Jones E. G., Cheng H. J. (2008). Dorsal turning of motor corticospinal axons at the pyramidal decussation requires plexin signaling. Neural Dev. 3:21. 10.1186/1749-8104-3-21 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finci L. I., Kruger N., Sun X., Zhang J., Chegkazi M., Wu Y., et al. (2014). The crystal structure of netrin-1 in complex with DCC reveals the bifunctionality of netrin-1 as a guidance cue. Neuron 83 839–849. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.07.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finger J. H., Bronson R. T., Harris B., Johnson K., Przyborski S. A., Ackerman S. L. (2002). The netrin 1 receptors Unc5h3 and Dcc are necessary at multiple choice points for the guidance of corticospinal tract axons. J. Neurosci. 22 10346–10356. 10.1523/jneurosci.22-23-10346.2002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fransen E., D’Hooge R., Van Camp G., Verhoye M., Sijbers J., Reyniers E., et al. (1998). L1 knockout mice show dilated ventricles, vermis hypoplasia and impaired exploration patterns. Hum. Mol. Genet. 7 999–1009. 10.1093/hmg/7.6.999 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galléa C., Popa T., Billot S., Meneret A., Depienne C., Roze E. (2011). Congenital mirror movements: a clue to understanding bimanual motor control. J. Neurol. 258 1911–1919. 10.1007/s00415-011-6107-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gu Z., Kalambogias J., Yoshioka S., Han W., Li Z., Kawasawa Y. I., et al. (2017). Control of species-dependent cortico-motoneuronal connections underlying manual dexterity. Science 357 400–404. 10.1126/science.aan3721 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iwasato T., Katoh H., Nishimaru H., Ishikawa Y., Inoue H., Saito Y. M., et al. (2007). Rac-GAP alpha-chimerin regulates motor-circuit formation as a key mediator of EphrinB3/EphA4 forward signaling. Cell 130 742–753. 10.1016/j.cell.2007.07.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaworski A., Long H., Tessier-Lavigne M. (2010). Collaborative and specialized functions of Robo1 and Robo2 in spinal commissural axon guidance. J. Neurosci. 30 9445–9453. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6290-09.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joshi P. S., Molyneaux B. J., Feng L., Xie X., Macklis J. D., Gan L. (2008). Bhlhb5 regulates the postmitotic acquisition of area identities in layers II-V of the developing neocortex. Neuron 60 258–272. 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.08.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katori S., Noguchi-Katori Y., Itohara S., Iwasato T. (2017). Spinal RacGAP alpha-Chimaerin is required to establish the midline barrier for proper corticospinal axon guidance. J. Neurosci. 37 7682–7699. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3123-16.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kullander K., Butt S. J., Lebret J. M., Lundfald L., Restrepo C. E., Rydstrom A., et al. (2003). Role of EphA4 and EphrinB3 in local neuronal circuits that control walking. Science 299 1889–1892. 10.1126/science.1079641 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kullander K., Croll S. D., Zimmer M., Pan L., McClain J., Hughes V., et al. (2001a). Ephrin-B3 is the midline barrier that prevents corticospinal tract axons from recrossing, allowing for unilateral motor control. Genes Dev. 15 877–888. 10.1101/gad.868901 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kullander K., Mather N. K., Diella F., Dottori M., Boyd A. W., Klein R. (2001b). Kinase-dependent and kinase-independent functions of EphA4 receptors in major axon tract formation in vivo. Neuron 29 73–84. 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00181-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lamanna W. C., Kalus I., Padva M., Baldwin R. J., Merry C. L., Dierks T. (2007). The heparanome–the enigma of encoding and decoding heparan sulfate sulfation. J. Biotechnol. 129 290–307. 10.1016/j.jbiotec.2007.01.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lemon R. N. (2008). Descending pathways in motor control. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 31 195–218. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125547 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lévesque M., Charara A., Gagnon S., Parent A., Deschenes M. (1996). Corticostriatal projections from layer V cells in rat are collaterals of long-range corticofugal axons. Brain Res. 709 311–315. 10.1016/0006-8993(95)01333-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leyva-Díaz E., López-Bendito G. (2013). In and out from the cortex: development of major forebrain connections. Neuroscience 254 26–44. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.08.070 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li C. X., Waters R. S. (1991). Organization of the mouse motor cortex studied by retrograde tracing and intracortical microstimulation (ICMS) mapping. Can. J. Neurol. Sci. 18 28–38. 10.1017/s0317167100031267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu Y., Shi J., Lu C. C., Wang Z. B., Lyuksyutova A. I., Song X. J., et al. (2005). Ryk-mediated Wnt repulsion regulates posterior-directed growth of corticospinal tract. Nat. Neurosci. 8 1151–1159. 10.1038/nn1520 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meijering E., Dzyubachyk O., Smal I. (2012). Methods for cell and particle tracking. Methods Enzymol. 504 183–200. 10.1016/b978-0-12-391857-4.00009-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Méneret A., Franz E. A., Trouillard O., Oliver T. C., Zagar Y., Robertson S. P., et al. (2017). Mutations in the netrin-1 gene cause congenital mirror movements. J. Clin. Invest. 127 3923–3936. 10.1172/jci95442 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Montoya C. P., Campbell-Hope L. J., Pemberton K. D., Dunnett S. B. (1991). The “staircase test”: a measure of independent forelimb reaching and grasping abilities in rats. J. Neurosci. Methods 36 219–228. 10.1016/0165-0270(91)90048-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mori M., Kose A., Tsujino T., Tanaka C. (1990). Immunocytochemical localization of protein kinase C subspecies in the rat spinal cord: light and electron microscopic study. J. Comp. Neurol. 299 167–177. 10.1002/cne.902990204 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nagamine S., Tamba M., Ishimine H., Araki K., Shiomi K., Okada T., et al. (2012). Organ-specific sulfation patterns of heparan sulfate generated by extracellular sulfatases Sulf1 and Sulf2 in mice. J. Biol. Chem. 287 9579–9590. 10.1074/jbc.M111.290262 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Callaghan J. P., Holtzman S. G. (1975). Quantification of the analgesic activity of narcotic antagonists by a modified hot-plate procedure. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 192 497–505. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Okada T., Keino-Masu K., Nagamine S., Kametani F., Ohto T., Hasegawa M., et al. (2017). Desulfation of heparan sulfate by Sulf1 and Sulf2 is required for corticospinal tract formation. Sci. Rep. 7:13847. 10.1038/s41598-017-14185-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Okada T., Keino-Masu K., Suto F., Mitchell K. J., Masu M. (2019). Remarkable complexity and variability of corticospinal tract defects in adult Semaphorin 6A knockout mice. Brain Res. 1710 209–219. 10.1016/j.brainres.2018.12.041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Leary D. D., Bicknese A. R., De Carlos J. A., Heffner C. D., Koester S. E., Kutka L. J., et al. (1990). Target selection by cortical axons: alternative mechanisms to establish axonal connections in the developing brain. Cold Spring Harb. Symp. Quant. Biol. 55 453–468. 10.1101/sqb.1990.055.01.045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rajagopalan S., Vivancos V., Nicolas E., Dickson B. J. (2000). Selecting a longitudinal pathway: robo receptors specify the lateral position of axons in the Drosophila CNS. Cell 103 1033–1045. 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)00207-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolf B., Bastmeyer M., Schachner M., Bartsch U. (2002). Pathfinding errors of corticospinal axons in neural cell adhesion molecule-deficient mice. J. Neurosci. 22 8357–8362. 10.1523/jneurosci.22-19-08357.2002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rünker A. E., Little G. E., Suto F., Fujisawa H., Mitchell K. J. (2008). Semaphorin-6A controls guidance of corticospinal tract axons at multiple choice points. Neural Dev. 3:34. 10.1186/1749-8104-3-34 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schönfeld L. M., Dooley D., Jahanshahi A., Temel Y., Hendrix S. (2017). Evaluating rodent motor functions: which tests to choose? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 83 298–312. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.10.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seki K., Perlmutter S. I., Fetz E. E. (2003). Sensory input to primate spinal cord is presynaptically inhibited during voluntary movement. Nat. Neurosci. 6 1309–1316. 10.1038/nn1154 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Serradj N., Paixao S., Sobocki T., Feinberg M., Klein R., Kullander K., et al. (2014). EphA4-mediated ipsilateral corticospinal tract misprojections are necessary for bilateral voluntary movements but not bilateral stereotypic locomotion. J. Neurosci. 34 5211–5221. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4848-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simpson J. H., Bland K. S., Fetter R. D., Goodman C. S. (2000). Short-range and long-range guidance by Slit and its Robo receptors: a combinatorial code of Robo receptors controls lateral position. Cell 103 1019–1032. 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)00206-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Srour M., Riviere J. B., Pham J. M., Dube M. P., Girard S., Morin S., et al. (2010). Mutations in DCC cause congenital mirror movements. Science 328:592. 10.1126/science.1186463 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Starkey M. L., Barritt A. W., Yip P. K., Davies M., Hamers F. P., McMahon S. B., et al. (2005). Assessing behavioural function following a pyramidotomy lesion of the corticospinal tract in adult mice. Exp. Neurol. 195 524–539. 10.1016/j.expneurol.2005.06.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steward O., Zheng B., Ho C., Anderson K., Tessier-Lavigne M. (2004). The dorsolateral corticospinal tract in mice: an alternative route for corticospinal input to caudal segments following dorsal column lesions. J. Comp. Neurol. 472 463–477. 10.1002/cne.20090 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tennant K. A., Adkins D. L., Donlan N. A., Asay A. L., Thomas N., Kleim J. A., et al. (2011). The organization of the forelimb representation of the C57BL/6 mouse motor cortex as defined by intracortical microstimulation and cytoarchitecture. Cereb. Cortex 21 865–876. 10.1093/cercor/bhq159 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ueno M., Nakamura Y., Li J., Gu Z., Niehaus J., Maezawa M., et al. (2018). Corticospinal circuits from the sensory and motor cortices differentially regulate skilled movements through distinct spinal interneurons. Cell Rep. 23 1286.e7–1300.e7. 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.03.137 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang X., Liu Y., Li X., Zhang Z., Yang H., Zhang Y., et al. (2017). Deconstruction of corticospinal circuits for goal-directed motor skills. Cell 171 440.e14–455.e14. 10.1016/j.cell.2017.08.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Z., Maunze B., Wang Y., Tsoulfas P., Blackmore M. G. (2018). Global connectivity and function of descending spinal input revealed by 3D microscopy and retrograde transduction. J. Neurosci. 38 10566–10581. 10.1523/jneurosci.1196-18.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Welniarz Q., Dusart I., Roze E. (2017a). The corticospinal tract: evolution, development, and human disorders. Dev. Neurobiol. 77 810–829. 10.1002/dneu.22455 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Welniarz Q., Morel M. P., Pourchet O., Gallea C., Lamy J. C., Cincotta M., et al. (2017b). Non cell-autonomous role of DCC in the guidance of the corticospinal tract at the midline. Sci. Rep. 7:410. 10.1038/s41598-017-00514-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whishaw I. Q. (1996). An endpoint, descriptive, and kinematic comparison of skilled reaching in mice (Mus musculus) with rats (Rattus norvegicus). Behav. Brain Res. 78 101–111. 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00236-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yokoyama N., Romero M. I., Cowan C. A., Galvan P., Helmbacher F., Charnay P., et al. (2001). Forward signaling mediated by ephrin-B3 prevents contralateral corticospinal axons from recrossing the spinal cord midline. Neuron 29 85–97. 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00182-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zack G. W., Rogers W. E., Latt S. A. (1977). Automatic measurement of sister chromatid exchange frequency. J. Histochem. Cytochem. 25 741–753. 10.1177/25.7.70454 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.