Abstract
Recent research in mice indicates that luminance-independent fluctuations in pupil size predict variability in spontaneous and evoked activity of single neurons in auditory and visual cortex. These findings suggest that pupil is an indicator of large-scale changes in arousal state that affect sensory processing. However, it is not known whether pupil-related state also influences the selectivity of auditory neurons. We recorded pupil size and single-unit spiking activity in the primary auditory cortex (A1) of nonanesthetized male and female ferrets during presentation of natural vocalizations and tone stimuli that allow measurement of frequency and level tuning. Neurons showed a systematic increase in both spontaneous and sound-evoked activity when pupil was large, as well as desynchronization and a decrease in trial-to-trial variability. Relationships between pupil size and firing rate were nonmonotonic in some cells. In most neurons, several measurements of tuning, including acoustic threshold, spectral bandwidth, and best frequency, remained stable across large changes in pupil size. Across the population, however, there was a small but significant decrease in acoustic threshold when pupil was dilated. In some recordings, we observed rapid, saccade-like eye movements during sustained pupil constriction, which may indicate sleep. Including the presence of this state as a separate variable in a regression model of neural variability accounted for some, but not all, of the variability and nonmonotonicity associated with changes in pupil size.
NEW & NOTEWORTHY Cortical neurons vary in their response to repeated stimuli, and some portion of the variability is due to fluctuations in network state. By simultaneously recording pupil and single-neuron activity in auditory cortex of ferrets, we provide new evidence that network state affects the excitability of auditory neurons, but not sensory selectivity. In addition, we report the occurrence of possible sleep states, adding to evidence that pupil provides an index of both sleep and physiological arousal.
Keywords: arousal, auditory cortex, pupillometry, single-unit physiology, sleep
INTRODUCTION
Although the iris primarily functions to regulate the formation of images on the retina, pupil dilation and constriction are also a result of autonomic nervous activity unrelated to visual stimuli (Loewenfeld and Lowenstein 1999). Growing evidence suggests that these autonomic changes in pupil diameter are an indicator of changes in internal state that affect processing in sensory cortex (McGinley et al. 2015a; Reimer et al. 2014; Vinck et al. 2015). In mice, pupil dilation is correlated with transitions from stillness to walking (McGinley et al. 2015a; Vinck et al. 2015), which increase firing rates in visual cortex (Niell and Stryker 2010) and decrease firing rates in auditory cortex (Schneider et al. 2014). Dilated pupil is associated with increases in high-frequency local field potential (LFP) activity, a physiological signature of alertness, in sensory cortex (Harris and Thiele 2011; Vinck et al. 2015). In mouse auditory cortex, intermediate pupil diameter is also associated with increases in the gain of sound-evoked responses, decreased spontaneous activity, and stable, hyperpolarized subthreshold membrane potential, which have been hypothesized to reflect an “optimal state” for detecting sensory stimuli (McGinley et al. 2015a).
Pupil dilation has also been correlated with changes in sensory selectivity. In visual cortex, orientation tuning becomes more selective when pupil dilates, suggesting that sensory representations are more precise in some internal brain states (Reimer et al. 2014). However, it is not known if pupil-associated state influences the selectivity of neurons in the auditory system. Some population-level associations between pupil size and cortical activity do differ between sensory systems (Shimaoka et al. 2018). Data from mouse auditory cortex, which suggest that pupil size is nonmonotonically related to neural activity (McGinley et al. 2015a), conflict with data from mouse visual cortex, which show either monotonic increases in neural activity when pupil dilates (Reimer et al. 2014; Vinck et al. 2015) or simultaneous increases and decreases in different neurons recorded at the same time (Stringer et al. 2019). These observations may reflect functional differences between the auditory and visual systems.
To explore effects of pupil-associated state on sensory coding in auditory cortex, we recorded pupil size and single-unit spiking activity in the primary auditory cortex (A1) of head-restrained, nonanesthetized ferrets. To study effects of internal state on gain and spontaneous activity, we presented a large number of repetitions (up to 120) of ferret vocalizations. To study state effects on sensory selectivity, we presented random sequences of tone pips at various frequencies and levels. Each stimulus set therefore allowed in-depth exploration of a distinct feature of auditory responses, as well as comparison to previous work in the visual system (Reimer et al. 2014). We characterized effects of pupil by comparing responses to each distinct sound as pupil size varied. Neurons typically showed an increase in baseline firing rate as well as the gain and reliability of sound-evoked activity when pupil was large, although there was also evidence for nonmonotonic effects of pupil-associated state in some neurons. In most neurons, parametric measurements of tuning, including acoustic threshold, spectral bandwidth, and best frequency, remained stable across large changes in pupil size. Across the population, however, we observed a small decrease in acoustic threshold when pupil was dilated.
In some recordings, we observed increased saccadic eye movements during periods of sustained pupil constriction, possibly indicating sleep onset. Including the presence of this state as a separate variable in a regression model of neural variability accounted for some, but not all, of the variability associated with changes in pupil size. The stability of best frequency across large changes in pupil size (and associated changes in gain and spontaneous activity) contrasts with task-related plasticity of receptive fields in ferret auditory cortex (David et al. 2012; Fritz et al. 2003), suggesting a difference between effects of engaging in tasks that require listening and variation within passive states.
METHODS
All procedures were approved by the Oregon Health and Science University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee and conform to standards of the Association for Assessment and Accreditation of Laboratory Animal Care.
Neurophysiology.
Five young adult ferrets (4 males and 1 female; Marshall Farms) were implanted with a stainless steel head post and acrylic cap to permit head fixation and access to auditory cortex. After recovery (1–2 wk), animals were habituated to a head-fixed posture and presentation of auditory stimuli. Following habituation, a small (1–2 mm) craniotomy was opened over primary auditory cortex (A1). In 66 recordings, data were collected using one to four independently positioned tungsten microelectrodes (FHC; 1–5 MΩ) advanced by independent microdrives (Electrode Positioning System; Alpha Omega). In three recordings, data were collected using a single-shank 64-channel silicon microelectrode array, spanning 1 mm (Masmanidis Laboratory, University of California, Los Angeles) (Du et al. 2011).
After a unit was isolated during tungsten recordings, its auditory responsiveness was tested using tones or narrowband noise bursts. If the site showed no response, the electrode was lowered to search for another site. During depth-array recordings, the probe was lowered into auditory cortex until auditory-evoked activity was observed across the span of recorded channels. Amplified and digitized signals were stored using open-source data acquisition software (tungsten, MANTA: Englitz et al. 2013; depth array, Open Ephys: Siegle et al. 2017). Ferrets were observed by video monitor during recordings. If a unit became indistinguishable from the noise floor following a body movement, the recording was stopped and the five trials preceding the putative isolation loss (28 to 40 s of recording time) were excluded from analysis. A1 recording sites were identified by characteristic short latency, frequency selectivity, and tonotopic gradients of single- or multiunit activity across multiple penetrations (Bizley et al. 2005; Elgueda et al. 2019).
Spike sorting.
For tungsten electrode recordings, events were extracted from the continuous electrophysiological signal using a principal components analysis and k-means clustering (Meska-PCA: David et al. 2009). Single-unit isolation was quantified from cluster overlap as the fraction of spikes likely to be produced by a single neuron rather than another unit. For depth-array recordings, single-unit spikes were sorted using polytrode sorting software (Kilosort: Pachitariu et al. 2016). Only units maintaining stable isolation >95% through the experiment were considered single neurons for analysis.
Pupillometry.
During neurophysiological recordings, infrared video of one eye was collected for offline measurement of pupil size. Video was collected using either 1) a commercial closed-circuit television camera fitted with a lens (to provide magnification of the eye) and an infrared filter (Heliopan ES 49 RG 780) or 2) an open-source camera (Adafruit TTL serial camera) fitted with a lens (M12 Lenses PT-2514BMP 25.0 mm) whose focal length allowed placement of the camera 10 cm from the ferret’s eye. To improve contrast, the imaged eye was illuminated by a bank of infrared light-emitting diodes (LEDs). Ambient luminance was provided using a ring light (AmScope LED-144S). At the start of each recording day, the intensity of the ring light was set to a level (~1,500 lux measured at the recorded eye) chosen to give a maximum dynamic range of pupil sizes. Light intensity remained fixed across the recording session.
Pupil size was measured using custom MATLAB (version R2016b) software (code available at https://bitbucket.org/lbhb/baphy). For each video, an intensity threshold was selected to capture pupil pixels and exclude the surrounding image. During initial recordings, the threshold was selected manually. We observed that the intensity histogram is multimodal with the first peak (reflecting the darkest pixels) generally corresponding to the pupil. In later recordings, we therefore automatically updated the threshold of each frame to position it at the first valley in the frame’s intensity histogram. Each frame was smoothed by a Gaussian filter before thresholding and then segmented by Moore boundary tracing. The segment with the largest area was identified as pupil. We measured pupil size as the length of the minor axis of an ellipse fit to this region using a direct least-squares method. To avoid identifying shadows at the edge of the eye or other dark regions of the image as pupil, we restricted the search for the largest area segment to a rectangular region of interest surrounding the pupil identified in the preceding frame (Nguyen and Stark 1993). Measurements were calibrated by comparing the width of each ferret’s eye (in mm) and the width of the ferret eye as it appeared in each video (in pixels), which gave a conversion factor of pixels to mm.
The frame rate of the cameras used in the experiments varied from 10 to 30 frames/s. To compensate for variability in camera frame rate, we recorded a time stamp at the start and end of each trial and then interpolated measurements of pupil size to match the sampling of the simultaneously recorded neural data. This procedure ensured that the two data streams (video and neural recording) remained synchronized throughout each recording.
Blink artifacts were identified by rapid, transient changes in pupil size (McGinley et al. 2015a). The derivative of the pupil trace was taken, and bins with derivatives more than 6 SD from the mean were marked. Blinks were identified within these bins by screening for decreases in pupil size followed by increases. Data during a 1-s period surrounding the blink was then removed from the trace and replaced by a linear interpolation of the pupil size immediately before and after the blink.
When pupil and neural data were being compared, a 750-ms offset was applied to pupil trace to account for the lagged relationship between changes in pupil size and neural activity in auditory cortex to allow for comparison with previous research (McGinley et al. 2015a). Setting the offset at values between 0 and 1.5 s did not affect the mean accuracy of the pupil-based regression models of neural activity described below (data not shown).
To measure changes in eye position, we calculated the Euclidean distance between the center of the ellipse fit to the pupil in each video frame and then multiplied by frame rate to find eye speed. We identified putative sleep states by screening for a combination of tonically constricted pupil and a high rate of saccades. Sleep states were identified by selecting parameter values for each recording (maximum pupil size, maximum pupil standard deviation, minimum saccade speed, minimum saccade rate) that marked abrupt state transitions in the traces of pupil size and eye speed. Gaps of less than 15 s between sleep episodes were considered artifacts and coded as sleep. We restricted our analysis to sleep episodes with a duration of at least 30 s.
Acoustic stimuli.
Stimulus presentation was controlled by custom MATLAB software (code available at https://bitbucket.org/lbhb/baphy). Digital acoustic signals were transformed to analog (National Instruments), amplified (Crown), and delivered through a free-field speaker (Manger).
During initial recording sessions, ferrets were presented with two 3-s species-specific vocalizations: an infant call and an adult aggression call. Stimuli were recorded in a sound-attenuating chamber using a commercial digital recorder (44-kHz sampling; Tascam DR-400). No animal that produced the recorded vocalizations was used in the study. Each vocalization was repeated up to 120 times. The order of vocalizations was randomized on each repetition. Each repetition was preceded by a 2-s silent period to allow measurement of spontaneous neural activity on each trial.
To improve sampling of internal states, recording was paused for several seconds after the 40th and 80th sound repetitions, during which the ferret was roused by an unexpected sound (e.g., turning the doorknob of the recording booth), which often resulted in pupil dilation. A stepwise regression analysis (see below) using the number of trials since each pause to predict neural activity suggests that the time since this event did not predict neural activity independent of pupil size (data not shown).
During later recording sessions, ferrets were presented with sequences of 10-ms tone pips with varying frequency (4–6 octaves surrounding best frequency, ranging over 32 Hz to 40 kHz) and level (0 to 65 dB SPL, in 5- or 10-dB SPL steps). Each pip sequence was 6 s long, preceded by a 2-s silent period to allow measurement of spontaneous neural activity. Tone-pip sequences were repeated multiple times in each recording (repetitions: 28 ± 2, mean ± SE). During some recordings, tone-pip sequences were interleaved with ferret vocalizations to improve sampling of pupil states in each data set.
Data analysis: gain and baseline firing rate.
Single-trial neural and pupil data for ferret vocalization recordings were binned at 4 Hz. Peristimulus time histogram (PSTH) responses to each vocalization were calculated by aligning spike activity to stimulus onset and averaging across presentations. To isolate evoked activity, the mean spike rate during the 2 s of silence preceding stimulus onset was subtracted from the PSTH.
Neural and pupil data for responses to tone-pip sequences were binned at 100 Hz. A frequency response area (FRA) was calculated by taking the mean spike rate for each sound during a window from 10 to 60 ms following pip onset. To isolate evoked activity, the mean spike rate during the 2 s of silence preceding tone-pip sequence onset was subtracted from the FRA.
We used multivariate regression models to examine effects of pupil state on spontaneous and sound-evoked activity. We modeled the response to each presentation of a tone pip or vocalization as a function of average spontaneous rate over the entire recording, average response to the stimulus across all trials, and pupil diameter during each stimulus presentation.
The baseline-gain model accounted for a linear relationship between pupil diameter and both spontaneous and evoked spike rate. According to this model, single-trial activity, ri(t,s), is
where r0(t,s) is the mean time-varying PSTH response evoked by stimulus s (either one vocalization or one tone pip at a single frequency and level), m0 is the mean spontaneous rate, and pi(t,s) is pupil size observed simultaneously to ri(t,s). The model coefficients indicate how the neuron’s overall spiking activity (baseline, β2) and the scaling of the cell’s sound-evoked response (gain, β3) vary with pupil size. To cast all β parameters in units of pupil−1, we normalized the baseline terms by m0. We therefore refer to this as the “baseline-gain” or “first-order baseline-gain” model.
Unless otherwise specified, model parameters were fit by linear, least-squares regression using 20-fold cross-validation. The median model parameters across all 20 fits was used to compare coefficients across cells. Model accuracy for a given cell was quantified by the squared Pearson correlation between actual single-trial firing rates and firing rates predicted using the cross-validated model coefficients. Significance of model fits was assessed using a permutation test. A cell with a significant fit was one in which a fit to real pupil data showed accuracy greater than a model fit to pupil data shuffled across trials for at least 50 of 1,000 shuffles (i.e., P < 0.05, permutation test).
To determine what aspects of the baseline-gain model were important for the capturing the relationship between pupil and firing rate, we compared it with several variants. The baseline-only model allowed only the baseline rate, but not the gain of the evoked response, to scale with pupil,
The gain-only model allowed only the gain of the evoked response, and not the baseline rate, to scale with pupil,
We also considered nonlinear relationships between pupil and spike rate. The second-order baseline model allowed the baseline rate to scale with the square of pupil size, allowing nonmonotonic effects of internal state,
The second-order baseline-gain model allowed both the baseline rate and gain to scale with the square of pupil size,
We measured the performance of each model by comparing its ability to predict single-trial data to the PSTH or FRA alone, without any information on pupil size (the null model),
For each cell and each model, we measured the percent improvement in accuracy (i.e., the difference in squared Pearson correlation coefficient, R2, between the model predictions and actual data) over the null model,
Each model’s performance was quantified as the median improvement in accuracy across all cells that showed a significant fit for any tested model (P < 0.05, permutation test). For comparing model performance across collections of neurons, we performed a Wilcoxon signed-rank test (sign test) between the distribution of prediction correlations across neurons for each model.
Data analysis: nonmonotonicity.
A previous report (McGinley et al. 2015a) observed nonmonotonic relationships between pupil size and neural activity in mouse auditory cortex. Following its language, we refer to a neuron with a maximum firing rate at intermediate pupil sizes and a lesser firing rate at small and large pupil sizes as an “inverted U.” We refer to a neuron with a minimum firing rate at intermediate pupil sizes as a “U.”
We restricted our analysis of nonmonotonicity to neurons that showed a significant fit for the second-order baseline-gain model relative to the same model fit to shuffled pupil data (n = 79/114 neurons). The second-order terms of this model allow for the possibility of nonmonotonic relationships between pupil size and firing rate.
We used a segmented linear model to test for nonmonotonic relationships between pupil size and firing rate in single neurons (Simonsohn 2018). The segmented model is similar to the baseline-gain model (see above), except that data are partitioned into intervals at a breakpoint in pupil size. The fit parameters of the model may differ on either side of the breakpoint. This allows a variety of relationships between pupil and firing rate, including linear increases and decreases, U’s, and inverted U’s.
The segmented model included separate breakpoints for pupil-related effects on baseline and gain (β0, β4). Both breakpoints were fit simultaneously with other free parameters in the model. Baseline and gain effects were combined to estimate the neuron’s firing rate, i.e.,
and
The constraints on the two shift parameters ensure that the functions will be continuous at their breakpoints. Model parameters were fit by nonlinear least squares using the trust-region-reflective algorithm and 20-fold cross-validation. A change in the sign of pupil coefficients across the breakpoint (β2 vs. β3 or β6 vs. β7) indicates a nonmonotonic relationship between pupil and firing rate. The direction of the sign change indicates if a cell is a U or inverted U. Data were preprocessed as described (see Data analysis: gain and baseline firing rate).
To identify nonmonotonic neurons, we compared the accuracy of the segmented linear model with that of an identical model that was constrained to be monotonic. That is, the slope coefficients related to baseline (β2,β3) and gain (β6,β7) were constrained to have the same sign on both sides of the breakpoint. If the nonconstrained model showed a sign change as well as an improvement in accuracy over the constrained model, we classified the cell as showing a nonmonotonic trend.
To identify significantly nonmonotonic neurons, we performed a permutation test. We randomly shuffled half the firing rate predictions between the constrained and nonconstrained models, and then calculated the difference in accuracy between the shuffled prediction rates. Significantly nonmonotonic neurons were those in which fewer than 50 of 1,000 shuffles showed a greater difference in accuracy than that observed between the constrained and nonconstrained models (P < 0.05 of a chance improvement).
Data analysis: neural response variability.
We used several measurements (Fano factor, between-trial reliability, and noise correlation) to characterize the variability of neural responses across trials. In all cases, we used data from the repeated vocalizations experiment and compared variability across pupil states by grouping trials based on whether the mean pupil size preceding stimulus onset was above or below the median pupil size observed during the recording. Data for the noise correlation and reliability analyses were binned at 4 Hz for consistency with regression analyses. Data for the Fano factor analysis were binned at 100 Hz to bring out faster temporal dynamics of the neural response to sound. Two neurons with extremely low evoked spike rates produced undefined peristimulus Fano factors (variance of 0 divided by mean of 0) and were therefore excluded from the analysis. Reliability was calculated as the mean trial Pearson correlation of neural responses across trials (McGinley et al. 2015a). Spike rates from the response to each stimulus were z-scored before noise correlations were calculated.
Data analysis: frequency and level tuning.
To assess changes in frequency and level tuning associated with pupil state, neural responses to tone-pip sequences were binned at 100 Hz. Data from each tone-pip recording were divided into two bins (large pupil and small pupil) based on the median pupil size during the recording. Data from tone-pip sequences were analyzed using 1) the 2-s silence preceding each tone-pip sequence (spontaneous activity), 2) the interval from 60 to 10 ms preceding onset of each tone pip (prestimulus activity), and 3) the interval from 10 to 60 ms after the onset of each tone pip (stimulus-evoked activity). Because the FRA was assessed by using a cloud of tone pips presented in rapid succession, the cell’s firing rate during the 50 ms preceding sound onset (i.e., prestimulus activity) was a better measure of the cell’s baseline response for subthreshold levels than spontaneous activity.
For display, examples of FRAs were calculated taking the mean stimulus-evoked spike rate for each tone pip, smoothing using a two-dimensional box filter with dimensions equivalent to 15 dB and 1.5 octaves, and subtracting the mean spontaneous activity across all tone-pip sequences in the recording. To measure changes in spontaneous and driven rates in the tone-pip data without exploring changes in tuning, we used the mean spontaneous rate and the difference between the mean stimulus-evoked and prestimulus firing rates. To measure possible changes in sensory tuning, we then fit hierarchical regression models to the prestimulus and stimulus-evoked spike data for each tone pip.
The hierarchical models were implemented in STAN (https://mc-stan.org) and fit using Bayesian inference. In contrast to conventional model fitting, Bayesian analysis allows for simple calculations of credible intervals on derived parameters (e.g., parameters that are mathematical functions of fitted coefficients), is more robust to outliers through the use of Poisson likelihoods for spike count data, and offers simple construction of realistic hierarchical models (Gelman et al. 2003). Each model was fit four times for 1,000 samples after a 1,000-sample iteration burn-in period. Posterior samples were combined across all chains for inference.
Data analysis: rate-level functions.
To estimate changes in level tuning linked to internal state, we first calculated characteristic frequency (CF) before binning data by pupil size. An FRA for all data from the recording was calculated using data from the stimulus-evoked epoch and smoothed with a two-dimensional box filter with dimensions equivalent to 15 dB and 1.5 octaves. A separate standard error was calculated for each coefficient of the FRA by measuring variance across repetitions of tone pips at that frequency and level. The minimum sound level that evoked a response at least 2 SE above the mean spike rate during the 100 ms preceding pip onset, measured across all pips, was calculated for each frequency. CF was defined as the frequency that required the minimum sound level to evoke a response.
For each cell, we went back to the unsmoothed FRA data, binned the tone-evoked responses according to pupil size, and generated rate-level functions. Rate-level functions were smoothed by averaging the response across the three frequencies closest to CF. Rate-level functions for individual cells were fit using a function with three parameters, baseline firing rate, b, slope, s, and threshold, θ, for each pupil condition:
where n is the number of spikes observed in a time interval, t, for cell, c, and pupil condition, p, on the ith observation of stimulus level, l. Because the time interval varied across cells, depending on how long we maintained stable recordings, incorporating this information into our model gave greater weight to data points with longer time intervals.
For levels less than threshold, firing rate was expected to be equal to the baseline firing rate. For stimulus levels greater than threshold, the cell could have an excitatory (sc > 0) or inhibitory (sc < 0) response.
The distribution of baseline rates across the cells in the small pupil condition were modeled by a Gamma prior, bc,0 ~ Gamma(bα,bβ), with bα ~ Gamma(0.5,0.1) and bβ ~ Gamma(0.1,0.1). (Because the expected value of the Gamma distribution is α/β, we can compute the posterior for the average spontaneous rate of the population as bα/bβ when the pupil is small and bα/bβ × bgμ when the pupil is large.) We set the priors for α and β after inspecting the distributions of spontaneous rates in the small and large pupil condition. Although prior work (McGinley et al. 2015a) suggests that we would expect to see a difference in spontaneous rate between the pupil conditions, we chose an unbiased prior for the ratio (bc,1/bc,0) such that the mean and standard deviation were Normal(1,1) and HalfNormal(1), respectively.
The distribution of slopes across cells were modeled using a Normal prior, sc,0 ~ Normal(sμ,sσ), and the priors for the population mean and standard deviation were modeled themselves as sμ ~ Normal(0.1,0.1) and sσ ~ HalfNormal(0.1). The distribution of thresholds across cells was modeled using a Normal prior, θc,0 ~ Normal(θμ,θσ) with priors for the population mean and standard deviation modeled as θμ ~ Normal(40,5) and θσ ~ HalfNormal(5). To minimize potential bias, unbiased priors were used for parameters representing the difference between pupil conditions. Specifically, the difference in slope (sc,1 − sc,0) was modeled with a Normal prior with mean and standard deviation of Normal(0,0.1) and HalfNormal(0.1), respectively. The difference in threshold (θc,1 − θc,0) was modeled with a Normal prior with mean and standard deviation of Normal(0,5) and HalfNormal(5), respectively.
Data analysis: frequency-tuning curves.
To quantify changes in frequency tuning linked to internal state, we first found best level (i.e., the level that evoked the maximum rate across all frequencies) using the FRA measured across all pupil-size bins. We then extracted frequency tuning curves (FTCs) at best level from the large-pupil and small-pupil FRAs. FTCs were smoothed by averaging across best level and the two neighboring levels in the FRA.
We assumed that the FTC for individual cells, c, and pupil condition, p, could be expressed as a Gaussian function with four parameters, baseline firing rate, b, gain, g, best frequency, BF, and bandwidth, BW:
where n is the evoked rate for the cell, pupil condition (small vs. large), and stimulus frequency, f, on the ith observation. As in the rate-level model, the cell’s firing rate during the 50 ms preceding sound onset was a better measure of the cell’s baseline response for subthreshold levels, and the spontaneous rate model was also incorporated into this model to improve our estimate of b.
The distribution of gains across cells was modeled using a Normal prior, with priors for the population mean and standard deviation modeled as gμ ~ Normal(10,1) and gσ = HalfNormal(10). The distribution of best frequency across cells was modeled using a Normal prior, with priors for the population mean and standard deviation modeled as BFμ ~ Normal(10,1) and BFσ = HalfNormal(1). The distribution of bandwidth across cells was modeled using a Normal prior, with priors for the population mean and standard deviation modeled as BWμ ~ Normal(−0.5,0.1) and BWσ = HalfNormal(0.25).
To avoid bias in estimates of pupil-related changes, uninformative Normal priors were used for parameters representing the difference between small and large pupil conditions. Specifically, the logarithm of the ratio in gain (gc,1/gc,0) was a Normal prior with a mean and standard deviation of Normal(0,0.1) and HalfNormal(0.1), respectively. The logarithm of the ratio in bandwidth (BWc,1/BWc,0) had a mean and standard deviation of Normal(0,0.1) and HalfNormal(0.25), respectively. For the ratio in gain and ratio in bandwidth, exp(0) = 1 such that the expected value of the prior was no effect of pupil size on gain or bandwidth. The difference in best frequency (BFc,1 − BFc,0) had a mean and standard deviation of Normal(0,0.1) and HalfNormal(0.1), respectively.
Data analysis: sleep states.
To test for neural correlates of sleep states, we defined a binary variable that indicated the presence or absence of sleep, sleepi(t,s), and added it as a term in the baseline-gain model (see above),
We also tested a sleep-only model in which baseline and gain were modulated only by sleep state,
Preprocessing and model fitting and evaluation were completed as for the baseline-gain model (see Data analysis: gain and baseline firing rate). The model was fit to neural data from responses to vocalizations.
Data analysis: local field potential.
Local field potential (LFP) signals from each recording were bandpass filtered using a forward and reverse filtered second-order Butterworth filter into delta (1–4 Hz), theta (4–7 Hz), alpha (7–14 Hz), beta (15–30 Hz), low gamma (30–60 Hz), and high gamma (60–100 Hz) bands (Yüzgeç et al. 2018). The instantaneous amplitude of each bandpass-filtered signal was calculated by computing the magnitude of its Hilbert transform, low-pass filtering with a cutoff at 1 Hz, and taking the mean amplitude across all electrodes.
Data analysis: sound-evoked pupil dilation.
To calculate sound-evoked pupil dilation (Δp), pupil traces were binned at 30 Hz, and the mean pupil size during the 2-s silence preceding each vocalization on each trial (p0) was subtracted from the raw trace before averaging across repetitions of the same sound. For regression analysis of the effect of sound-evoked dilations, we fit a model that included separate baseline and gain terms for the effect of the mean prestimulus pupil size on each trial and the change from prestimulus pupil size in each bin,
We compared the accuracy of this model’s results with the first-order gain model, using the model comparison procedure described above.
Experimental design and statistical analyses.
Statistical tests used to assess whether model fits were significantly better than chance (i.e., whether they accounted for any auditory response, tuning parameter, or state-related changes in neural activity) and to compare predictions across models are described above, together with other aspects of the models. Tests for correlations between data across conditions are reported as the unsquared Pearson correlation coefficient (r) and a t test for significance. Tests for differences between conditions are reported using two-tailed Wilcoxon rank-sum tests (rank-sum test), Wilcoxon signed-rank tests (sign test), or t tests (paired or unpaired t test).
Code.
Custom software for stimulus presentation and pupil analysis is available at https://bitbucket.org/lbhb/baphy. MATLAB and Python code for statistical analyses is available from the corresponding author on request.
RESULTS
Dilated pupil is correlated with increases in the spontaneous activity and gain of neurons in auditory cortex.
To examine the relationship between internal brain state and neural representation of natural sounds, we simultaneously recorded infrared video of the eye and single-unit spiking activity in the primary auditory cortex (A1) of head-restrained, nonanesthetized ferrets (Fig. 1A; n = 114 neurons from 46 recording sites in 3 ferrets). Pupil size could be extracted from the video, providing a measure of internal state (McGinley et al. 2015a; Reimer et al. 2014; Vinck et al. 2015). To gather data on the response to the same sounds across a wide range of states, our stimulus consisted of a small number of sounds (2 ferret vocalizations) repeated many times (up to 120 repetitions per recording, 77 ± 40 repetitions, mean ± SD). To further increase our sampling of internal states, we paused this presentation at set times in the recording to rouse the ferret using auditory stimuli (see methods). Although lighting conditions were held static during recordings, we also observed large changes in pupil size across timescales of tens of seconds to minutes (Fig. 1B).
Visual inspection of spike rasters suggested that trial-to-trial variability in neural activity sometimes tracked changes in pupil size (Fig. 1C). To quantify the association between pupil size and firing rate, we fit a linear model that allowed baseline spike rate and gain of sound-evoked responses to depend on pupil size (baseline-gain model, see methods; Fig. 1D). In a majority of neurons (n = 75/114, 66%), this model was significantly more accurate at predicting single-trial neural activity than a control model where pupil was shuffled in time [i.e., the prediction was based only on the neuron’s peristimulus time histogram (PSTH) response to each vocalization; Fig. 1E; P < 0.05, permutation test]. The degree of improvement was not correlated with the prediction accuracy of the control model alone (r = −0.08, P = 0.5, t test), suggesting that the influence of pupil on trial-to-trial variability did not depend on a neuron’s overall auditory responsiveness.
The baseline-gain model included separate terms for effects of internal state on gain and baseline firing rate, both of which tended to be positive, indicating an increase in firing rate with increasing pupil size (Fig. 1F). Both the baseline and gain terms contributed to model prediction accuracy, compared with models that included only one term (Fig. 1G and Table 1). Adding second-order terms improved median prediction accuracy further and increased the number of neurons that showed a significant fit from 75 to 79 [Fig. 1G and Table 1; median increase in accuracy over PSTH only = 0.7% (gain model), 1.3% (baseline), 2.5% (gain and baseline), 2.4% (gain, baseline, and baseline squared), 3.7% (gain, baseline, baseline squared, and gain squared)].
Table 1.
Baseline | Gain | Baseline-Gain | 2nd-Order Baseline | 2nd-Order Baseline-Gain | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
−0.009§ | −0.009§ | −0.02§ | −0.01§ | −0.02§ | Null |
0.0001† | −0.006§ | −0.002‡ | −0.01§ | Baseline | |
−0.006§ | −0.002† | −0.01§ | Gain | ||
−0.004* | −0.004‡ | Baseline-gain | |||
−0.008§ | 2nd-Order Baseline |
Stepwise regression of pupil-based models of neural activity applied to vocalization data set. Values are the difference in median accuracy across the population of neurons with significant fit for any model (n = 92/114). Bold text indicates comparisons for which the difference is significant at alpha level 0.05 with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.
P < 0.05;
P < 0.01;
P < 0.001;
P < 0.0001 (sign test).
Given that pupil size was more variable in some recordings than others, we wondered whether we were unable to detect effects of internal state in some neurons simply because the animal’s pupil size remained fairly stable during the recording. Variability of pupil size during the recording was greater for neurons that showed a significant effect of pupil (median SD = 0.4 mm, n = 79 neurons) than for neurons that did not (median SD = 0.2 mm, n = 35 neurons; P = 1e-5, rank-sum test). Among neurons that showed a significant effect of pupil on firing rate, there was a correlation between the variance of pupil during the recording and the degree of improvement of the model over a model based on PSTH response alone (n = 79 neurons; r = 0.37, P = 4e-4, t test). Thus the measurement of 66% of neurons showing a significant effect of state on activity represents a lower bound on the frequency of neurons showing pupil-related effects. To confirm this point, we split the data set into high-variability and low-variability recordings based on the variance of pupil within a recording relative to the median variance across all recordings. A greater percentage of neurons in the high-variance subset were modulated by pupil (number of modulated neurons: 31/35 modulated in high-variance data set vs. 48/79 in the low-variance data set, P = 3e-03, chi-square test). The median improvement in variance explained by the pupil model over a PSTH-based model was greater for the high-variance than for the low-variance subset (change in R2: 0.01 for low variance vs. 0.13 for high variance, P = 2e-04, rank-sum test). These results suggest that the level of pupil modulation would be greater if all neurons were drawn from recordings with the greatest possible range of pupil states.
The models were fit to data that included sound-evoked activity as well as activity during the silent intervals before and after each vocalization. The baseline term of the models could reflect either modulation of spontaneous activity or a stimulus-dependent modulation of sound-evoked firing rate that did not scale with stimulus strength. To distinguish between these possibilities, we fit a model with only a baseline term to data from the prestimulus epoch alone, ignoring sound-evoked activity. We then fit a model that included terms for both baseline and gain to the peristimulus epoch alone, ignoring spontaneous activity. The magnitude of the baseline terms in the two models was correlated (n = 75 neurons; r = 0.55, P = 4e-08, t test), suggesting that the baseline term of the model reflected primarily modulation of spontaneous activity rather than (or in addition to) stimulus-evoked activity.
Auditory neurons encode sensory stimuli more reliably when pupil is large.
Previous research in mouse auditory and visual cortex suggests that the degree to which neural activity is dominated by sensory input depends on pupil-indexed state (McGinley et al. 2015a; Reimer et al. 2014; Vinck et al. 2015). To replicate and extend these results, we split the data from each recording into two groups depending on whether pupil size was greater or less than the median pupil size observed in the recording. We then calculated several metrics of neural variability across stimulus repetitions (Figs. 2–4). When pupil was large, local field potential (LFP) showed a consistent shift toward greater power at high frequency (Fig. 2A), and pairs of simultaneously recorded neurons showed a small decrease in stimulus-independent correlated activity [Fig. 2B; n = 88 neurons forming 430 pairs; mean noise correlation = 0.03 (large pupil), 0.04 (small pupil); P = 1.1e-03, paired t test].
Previous research also indicates that the level of pupil-indexed arousal is associated with changes in the reliability of neurons in auditory cortex, where reliability is defined as the mean cross-correlation between spiking responses to the same stimulus in the same neuron across repetitions (McGinley et al. 2015a). To test for this effect, we calculated the correlation between the spiking responses of each vocalization (Fig. 3, A and B). The mean correlation was greater during trials when pupil was large (Fig. 3C; n = 114 neurons; P = 5e-8, paired t test). This difference in reliability was preserved in the subset of cells that showed modulation of neural activity by pupil-indexed state under our second-order baseline-gain model (Fig. 3D; n = 79 neurons; P = 5e-8, paired t test) but disappeared when only cells that did not show pupil-associated modulation under this model were compared (Fig. 3D; n = 35 neurons; P = 0.2, paired t test). These results suggest that pupil-associated state affects both the mean rate at which auditory neurons respond to sound and the variability of their responses, and that these effects occur in the same population of cells.
Finally, dilated pupil was also associated with a decrease in a third metric of neural variability, the Fano factor (Fig. 4). We noted a decrease in Fano factor upon stimulus onset and following large increases in sound amplitude during the stimulus (Fig. 4A), the latter of which was not reported in previous studies that examined the effect of static stimuli on neural variability (Churchland et al. 2010). Interestingly, the decrease in Fano factor associated with pupil size was present in both spontaneous and sound-driven activity [Fig. 4B; n = 112 neurons; P = 3e-05 (prestimulus Fano factor), P = 7e-08 (peristimulus Fano factor), paired t test]. Moreover, the effects of stimulus onset on Fano factor was independent of pupil size [Fig. 4B; n = 112 neurons; P = 0.9 (prestimulus Fano factor − peristimulus Fano factor), paired t test], suggesting differences between the effects of pupil-indexed arousal and previously studied effects of stimulus onset on neural variability (Churchland et al. 2010).
Nonmonotonic effects of pupil on firing rate are observed in some A1 neurons.
Arousal can have a nonmonotonic effect on behavior: both learning (Diamond et al. 2007; Yerkes and Dodson 1908) and task performance (Aston-Jones and Cohen 2005; Murphy et al. 2011; van Kempen et al. 2019) are suboptimal in minimally and maximally aroused states. A previous study found that multiunit spiking activity in mouse auditory cortex showed a similarly nonmonotonic relation between pupil and firing rate: on average, spontaneous activity was lowest, and gain highest, at intermediate pupil diameter, suggesting that this range of pupil sizes was optimal for detection of auditory signals (McGinley et al. 2015a). We found examples of nonmonotonic pupil size-firing rate relationships in some cells (Fig. 5A). To test for their prevalence, we fit data on neural responses to ferret vocalizations with a segmented regression model (Simonsohn 2018) that predicted a linear effect of pupil on neural activity but allowed the slope of the fit to take two different values depending on pupil size (Fig. 5B). A difference in the sign of the slope between segments indicated a nonmonotonic relationship between pupil size and spiking activity. We then compared the accuracy of this model to a similar model in which slope could vary between segments but where both line segments were constrained to have the same sign (i.e., the fit could not assume the shape of a U or an inverted U).
Although a large proportion of neurons that showed effects of pupil-associated state also showed a trend toward nonmonotonicity in either baseline firing rate or gain [Fig. 5C; n = 38/79 (baseline), n = 41/79 (gain)], allowing for a nonmonotonic segmented fit significantly improved the accuracy of the model for a smaller fraction of cells [n = 15/79 (baseline), 14/79 (gain), P < 0.05, permutation test]. Among neurons that showed a trend toward nonmonotonicity, more showed trends toward U in baseline firing rate than inverted U, and more showed an inverted U than U in gain (Fig. 5C), suggestive of previous results comparing spontaneous and sound-evoked activity in mouse A1 (McGinley et al. 2015a).
Frequency and level tuning show no or small dependence on pupil size.
To examine the effect of changes in internal state on stimulus selectivity, we recorded the responses of A1 neurons to tone pips at a variety of frequencies and levels (Fig. 6, A–C; 114 neurons from 40 recording sites in 4 ferrets). To test for changes in frequency and level selectivity, we divided the data from each cell into large-pupil and small-pupil bins based on the median pupil size observed during the recording (Fig. 6, A and B) and constructed frequency response areas (FRAs) from the data in each bin (Fig. 6C). FRAs showed a variety of patterns typical of the auditory system (Bizley et al. 2005), including a sound-level response threshold and broadening of spectral bandwidth at higher sound levels (Fig. 6C).
Comparing data from the large- and small-pupil bins showed a systematic increase in spontaneous activity and the response to tones across all levels and frequencies [Fig. 6, B, D, and E; n = 114 neurons; P = 1e-4 (spontaneous rate), P = 0.03 (driven rate), sign test]. To confirm that this result did not depend on our division of the data into large- and small-pupil bins, and to further examine changes in driven rate, we used linear regression to predict the response to tones based on pupil size and the average FRA of the neuron (see methods). As it did for vocalizations, pupil size predicted trial-to-trial variability in a subpopulation of cells (Fig. 6F; n = 57/114 neurons, 50%; P < 0.05, permutation test). Most state-modulated neurons showed enhanced gain and baseline firing rate when pupil was large (Fig. 6G), suggesting that pupil-indexed state acted to shift and multiplicatively scale the neuron’s auditory tuning curve.
Does the stimulus selectivity of auditory neurons change depending on pupil-indexed state? To test for changes in level tuning, we again divided data at the median pupil value in each recording and then fit a hinge function (see methods) to the characteristic frequency rate-level function in each pupil condition (Fig. 7, A and B). Across the population, there was a significant decrease in threshold when pupil was large (mean change: −2.65 dB, 90% credible interval: −4.00 to −1.33 dB) but no change in the slope of the rate-level function (mean change: 0.05 spikes/s per dB, 90% credible interval: −0.07 to 0.15 spikes/s per dB SPL). Consistent with previous analyses, we also observed an increase in the baseline firing rate of the cells when pupil was large (mean change: 1.68 spikes/s, 90% credible interval: 0.80 to 2.59 spikes/s). At the single-cell level, most neurons showed a significant change in baseline firing rate (n = 68/114), but few showed significant changes in slope (n = 12/114) or threshold (n = 8/114). The low number of cells showing significant changes in threshold may be due to the small effect size (−2.65-dB average across the population) and the uncertainty in model fits for individual cells.
To test for changes in frequency tuning, we fit a Gaussian function to the best-level frequency tuning curve (FTC) in each pupil condition (Fig. 7, C and D). To isolate changes in frequency tuning (i.e., bandwidth) from nonspecific changes in baseline firing rate or gain, our model included a multiplicative gain term and additive offset terms. Consistent with data from previous analysis, the gain and baseline parameters of the FTC showed a systematic increase across the population when pupil was large (gain: mean large-to-small ratio = 1.12, 90% credible interval = 1.06 to 1.18; baseline: mean change = 2.02 spikes/s, 90% credible interval = 1.20 to 2.75). There was no change in the spectral bandwidth of the FTC when pupil was large (mean large-to-small ratio = 1.01, 90% credible interval = 0.94 to 1.07). There was also no change in mean best frequency across pupil conditions (mean change = −0.02 octaves, 90% credible interval = −0.09 to 0.05). The number of neurons that showed a significant change in each parameter was greater for gain (n = 27/114) and baseline firing rate (n = 78/114) than it was for bandwidth (n = 14/114) or best frequency (n = 4/114). Thus, although A1 neurons did sometimes show pupil-dependent changes in response threshold or other tuning parameters, these changes were relatively small compared with the changes in baseline activity and response gain.
Sleep states account for additional neural variability.
Although we did not explicitly seek to study sleep state, during some recordings we observed periods of tonically constricted pupil accompanied by an increase in saccade-like eye movements (Fig. 8, A and B; Supplemental Movies S1 and S2, see https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.9859358). We initially speculated that these bouts represented rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, based on a previous report correlating constricted pupil and REM sleep in mice (Yüzgeç et al. 2018). However, the delta-to-theta ratio of LFP, a signature of REM sleep (Yüzgeç et al. 2018), did not show a systematic change between putative REM bouts and recording segments with pupil size in the same range (n = 26 recordings; P = 0.8, sign-rank test). Instead, the increase in eye movements was accompanied by a decrease in alpha power (Fig. 8C; n = 26 recordings; P = 6e-09, sign-rank test), suggesting that the eye movements indicated sleep onset (Silber et al. 2007).
Given that neural responses to sound in auditory cortex are preserved during natural sleep states but sometimes suppressed or enhanced (Brugge and Merzenich 1973; Issa and Wang 2008), we wondered if these brief sleep episodes might show changes in firing rate distinct from those associated with changes in pupil size. We therefore fit linear regression models that included putative sleep state, pupil size, or both sleep state and pupil size as predictors of neural activity (Fig. 8, D and E, and Table 2). Sleep state significantly predicted neural activity in 81% of neurons recorded in experiments that included one or more sleep episodes (n = 38/47 neurons). Across the population of neurons with a significant fit for any tested model (n = 44), sleep state predicted less variability than pupil, which in turn predicted less variability than both pupil and sleep state [Fig. 8D and Table 2; median change in accuracy over PSTH-only model: 1.5% (sleep), 8.4% (pupil), 11% (pupil and sleep)]. There was no correlation between the duration of sleep during the recording and the improvement in accuracy of the sleep-based model (r = 0.2, P = 0.16, t test), suggesting that sleep effects vary across neurons.
Table 2.
Sleep | Pupil (Baseline-Gain) |
Sleep and Pupil | |
---|---|---|---|
−0.2§ | −0.03§ | −0.04§ | Null |
−0.01‡ | −0.02§ | Sleep | |
−0.006† | Pupil (Baseline-Gain) |
Stepwise comparison of accuracy of pupil and/or sleep-state based models of neural activity (see methods) applied to all vocalization data including sleep episodes (n = 47 neurons). Values are median accuracy for population of neurons with significant fit for any model (n = 44/47 neurons). Bold text indicates the difference is significant at alpha level 0.05 with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.
P < 0.01;
P < 0.001;
P < 0.0001 (sign test).
In some neurons, visual inspection of spiking activity and pupil state indicated that sleep episodes were associated with a nonmonotonic change in firing rates (Fig. 8D). To quantify this effect, we compared trials recorded during sleep episodes with non-sleep trials falling in an equivalent range of pupil sizes (Fig. 9, A and B). Across the population, there was no significant difference in spontaneous rate, sound-evoked rate, or reliability between these two conditions [Fig. 9C; n = 47 neurons with recordings that included sleep episodes; P = 0.16 (spontaneous rate), P = 0.38 (evoked rate), P = 0.17 (reliability), sign test]. However, a subpopulation of neurons did show a difference in spontaneous or sound-evoked rates at the single-cell level [Fig. 9C; n = 7/47 (spontaneous rate), n = 15/47 (evoked rate); P < 0.05, unpaired t test]. To further examine the effect of sleep state, we removed data from trials recorded during sleep episodes from the data set and refit the segmented regression model initially used to test for nonmonotonic effects. Fitting to data that excluded sleep state reduced the number of neurons that showed nonmonotonic effects in both baseline and gain (Fig. 9D), indicating that sleep accounted for nonmonotonic effects in some neurons [n = 7/42 (change in nonmonotonicity of baseline effect), n = 6/42 (change in nonmonotonicity of gain effect), n = 42 neurons with recordings that included sleep episodes and a significant fit for second-order baseline-gain model].
Sound-selective pupil dilations.
To test whether the presentation of sound stimuli themselves affected the animals’ internal state, we aligned pupil traces to sound onset and calculated the mean pupil dilation in response to ferret vocalizations (Fig. 10A). We observed a sound-selective pupil dilation in response to one vocalization during the 2.5 s of silence following sound offset [Fig. 10, B and C; n = 46 recordings; P = 0.1 (vocalization-related difference in peristimulus pupil size), P = 6e-03 (poststimulus pupil size), paired t test]. These sound-evoked pupil dilations did not predict neural activity with greater accuracy than the raw pupil trace, suggesting that they do not influence neural activity in a distinct way from slower, intrinsically generated fluctuations in pupil diameter (Fig. 10D; n = 81/114 neurons; P = 0.5, sign test).
DISCUSSION
Pupil size is an indicator of central neuromodulatory processes related to arousal that affect processing in sensory cortex (McGinley et al. 2015b; Reimer et al. 2016). Our results show that pupil size is correlated with changes in the gain and baseline firing rate of neurons in the primary auditory cortex (A1) of nonanesthetized ferrets. Nonmonotonic effects of changes in pupil-indexed state were observed in some neurons, but the majority of effects were monotonic and showed a positive correlation between pupil size and spike rate. Across our population of recorded neurons, pupil size was also correlated with increases in the reliability of responses to sound, small decreases in acoustic threshold, and no change in spectral bandwidth or best frequency. The changes in gain that we observed suggest that pupil size tracks the gross level of activity evoked by auditory stimuli: sounds become more salient to the rest of the brain when pupil is large. Changes in threshold and decreased neural variability may also support a more sensitive or precise representation of sound in high-arousal states.
Stability of sensory tuning across pupil states.
In mouse visual cortex, single neurons’ orientation selectivity increases when pupil is dilating rather than constricting: the neurons’ response to their preferred direction, but not the orthogonal direction, increases (Reimer et al. 2014). Although the impact of this change in orientation selectivity on tuning bandwidth has not been quantified, it suggests a narrowing of orientation tuning in the mean response across the population (Reimer et al. 2014). Our observation of stable tuning bandwidth in A1 may therefore indicate differences between how pupil-indexed states affect sensory selectivity in visual and auditory cortex.
Our results are also relevant to previous work on behavioral modulation of sensory receptive fields. Studies of task engagement effects on A1 reveal enhancement or suppression of neural responses to task-relevant sound features, including frequency (David et al. 2012; Fritz et al. 2003), amplitude modulation (Niwa et al. 2012), and spatial position (Lee and Middlebrooks 2011), as well as generalized changes in excitability (Miller et al. 1972; Otazu et al. 2009). We found that best frequency is stable across large changes in pupil size, in contrast to previous studies of task-related plasticity in ferret primary auditory cortex that show frequency-specific effects (David et al. 2012; Fritz et al. 2003), suggesting a key difference between tasks that involve manipulation of the behavioral relevance of specific sound features and uncontrolled variation within passive states. Our data therefore suggest that effects of task engagement on frequency tuning in primary auditory cortex are not the result of nonspecific increases in arousal, but instead involve separate mechanisms, such as feedback from frontal cortex (Fritz et al. 2010) or pairing activation of neuromodulatory systems involved in pupil dilation with specific auditory stimuli (Froemke et al. 2007; Martins and Froemke 2015). The random sequences of tones used to characterize tuning would not pair release of acetylcholine with specific tone frequencies, and therefore would not be expected to gate shifts in tuning (Weinberger 2004).
Changes in neural excitability and variability.
We found an increase in both spontaneous and sound-evoked firing rate when pupil was large. In addition, we found shifts in multiple metrics of neural variability, including Fano factor, reliability, and noise correlations, as well as a shift toward desynchronized neural activity associated with high-arousal states (Harris and Thiele 2011). Our results therefore support previous studies that found an influence of pupil-indexed arousal on encoding of stimuli in early sensory cortex (McGinley et al. 2015a; Vinck et al. 2015).
A previous study in mouse auditory cortex found that intermediate pupil diameter was associated with maximum evoked responses to sound and minimal spontaneous activity (McGinley et al. 2015a). Although we observed some cells with nonmonotonic relationships between pupil size and spike rate, this was the not the predominant pattern in our sample. In addition, we found that the effect of pupil-associated state usually had the same sign for both spontaneous and sound-evoked activity within a single neuron, in contrast to earlier results suggesting that intermediate pupil sizes were associated with opposite changes in spontaneous and evoked activity (McGinley et al. 2015a).
Several factors could explain this inconsistency between studies. The difference could reflect sampling of cortical layers. The earlier study targeted layers 4/5. We did not target any particular layer, but our method of recording auditory cells as the electrode advanced through cortex may have biased our sample toward more superficial layers. It is also possible that, compared with the earlier study, we tended to sample a different range of cortical states, due to either differences in experimental technique (the earlier study recorded head-restrained animals on a treadmill, whereas we did not use a treadmill) or differences in the behavioral patterns of ferrets and mice. Ferrets were not able to run and rarely showed substantial motor activity during the recordings. Thus they may not have achieved the very high arousal state observed during bouts of running and other motor activity in mice.
Pupil as an index of arousal or cognitive engagement.
We used absolute pupil size as a measurement of cortical state. Because the variations in pupil size we observed occurred outside a controlled behavior, we have characterized them as a measurement of physiological arousal rather than inferring changes in a particular cognitive state. However, mechanisms like those we observed may underlie correlations between absolute pupil size and the efficiency of responses to sensory stimuli in some tasks (Beatty 1982a; van Kempen et al. 2019; Murphy et al. 2011). Our work complements human pupillometry’s traditional focus on small, rapidly decaying changes in pupil size that coincide with behavioral events (Beatty 1982b; Kahneman and Beatty 1966). The amplitude of these task-evoked dilations depends on a variety of cognitive variables (Zekveld et al. 2018), some of which may be related to the sound-selective pupil dilation we observed in response to pup cries over adult aggression calls. Work on sound-evoked pupil dilation in owls indicates that pupil adapts to repeated sounds (Bala and Takahashi 2000). Studies that use a larger number of stimuli with fewer sound repetitions per session may therefore provide more insight into neural correlates of sound-evoked pupil dilation, as may studies that use controlled behaviors rather than passive listening.
Pupil as an index of noradrenergic tone.
Activity in neuromodulatory centers, particularly the noradrenergic and cholinergic systems, has been proposed as a mechanism underlying correlations between pupil size and the level of neural activity in sensory cortex (McGinley et al. 2015b; Reimer et al. 2016). Evidence from multiple laboratories, species, and experimental techniques indicates that central release of noradrenaline from locus coeruleus (LC) is causally involved in pupil dilation and that noradrenaline release in sensory cortex accompanies pupil dilation (Aston-Jones and Cohen 2005; de Gee et al. 2017; Joshi et al. 2016; Larsen et al. 2018; Liu et al. 2017; Lovett-Barron et al. 2017; Murphy et al. 2014; Reimer et al. 2016).
Despite the substantial evidence linking pupil size to noradrenergic tone, the gain increases we observed during pupil dilation do not directly match in vivo measurements of the effects of noradrenaline in auditory cortex. Iontophoresis of noradrenaline increases the signal-to-noise ratio of some auditory cortex neurons via a decrease in spontaneous, but not sound-evoked, activity (Foote et al. 1975; Manunta and Edeline 1997, 1999). The gain increases we observe persist after subtraction of mean spontaneous activity and are present when data from the evoked period alone are analyzed. It is possible that iontophoresis does not replicate the spatial distribution or temporal dynamics of noradrenaline release in nonanesthetized animals. It is also possible that the gain increases we observed were a result of multiple modulatory systems acting on auditory cortex, both directly and indirectly. Activity in the cholinergic and dopaminergic systems are also correlated with changes in pupil size (de Gee et al. 2017; Larsen et al. 2018; Reimer et al. 2016). Given that pupil size is correlated with activity in multiple neuromodulatory systems, our failure to directly replicate results from spatially localized, single-neuromodulator experiments is unsurprising and indicates the complexity of neuromodulation under more natural conditions.
Involvement of other cortical and subcortical brain regions.
Some of the trial-to-trial variability we observed in auditory cortex may also be due to feedback from motor cortex (Schneider et al. 2014). Pupil size varies even when mice are still, and this explains more variability in neural activity than locomotion (McGinley et al. 2015a). However, in nonanesthetized mice, motor activities such as running, whisking, and licking a water reward are associated with pupil dilation (Lee and Margolis 2016; Stringer et al. 2019). Detailed tracking of face and body movements explains more variability than pupil alone in mouse visual cortex (Stringer et al. 2019) and dorsal cortex (Musall et al. 2019). We did not track detailed motor behavior in the current study, and thus the extent to which motor activity predicts trial-to-trial variability in our data is not known. However, our results add to the evidence that pupil is one of several variables related to movement that can be used to predict cortical activity.
Pupil-related changes in neural activity are not restricted to the cortex. The same study that showed evidence of a causal relationship between pupil dilation and LC activity showed a similar relationship between pupil dilation and activity in superior and inferior colliculus (IC), including spiking preceding pupil dilation by hundreds of milliseconds and pupil dilation induced by microstimulation (Joshi et al. 2016). Some of the effects of arousal state we observed are likely to be inherited from auditory signals that must pass through IC before reaching cortex.
Sleep states during head-restrained recordings.
Recent results in head-restrained mice showed fluctuations between awake, rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, and non-REM sleep states associated with changes in pupil size, with constricted pupil associated with REM sleep (Yüzgeç et al. 2018). We observed epochs of tonically constricted pupil accompanied by an abrupt increase in saccade rate. Although alpha power consistently increased as pupil constricted, this trend reversed itself during these epochs. We speculate that over the course of lengthy, head-restrained passive recordings, the ferrets may become progressively more drowsy and that the high-saccade, low-alpha epochs indicate sleep onset.
The pattern of constricted pupil, saccades, and lack of eyelid closure we observed may also be an artifact of head-restrained recording. Methods for recording the activity of single neurons in freely moving animals can be supplemented with head-mounted cameras to track pupil (Meyer et al. 2018). It is likely that these techniques will yield data on changes in internal state more relevant to natural environments.
Our observation of putative sleep states, like previous data from mice (Yüzgeç et al. 2018), complicates the suggestion that the pupil size of head-restrained animals reveals a continuum of awake arousal states analogous to, but distinct from, substates of sleep (McGinley et al. 2015b). Thus a full characterization of arousal states must incorporate both pupillometry and other physiological measurements that can distinguish between drowsy waking states and sleep.
GRANTS
This work was supported by National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders Grants R01 DC0495, R21 DC016969, and F31 DC016204 and by the ARCS Foundation Oregon Chapter.
DISCLOSURES
B. N. Buran receives financial compensation for programming, data analysis, experiment design, and tutoring services for government agencies, academic institutions, and private companies in addition to his part-time work at Oregon Health and Science University. None of the other authors any conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise, to declare.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Z.P.S. and S.V.D. conceived and designed research; Z.P.S. performed experiments; Z.P.S. and B.N.B. analyzed data; Z.P.S. and S.V.D. interpreted results of experiments; Z.P.S. and B.N.B. prepared figures; Z.P.S. and B.N.B. drafted manuscript; Z.P.S., B.N.B., and S.V.D. edited and revised manuscript; Z.P.S., B.N.B., and S.V.D. approved final version of manuscript.
ENDNOTE
At the request of the authors, readers are herein alerted to the fact that additional materials related to this manuscript may be found at https://bitbucket.org/lbhb/baphy. These materials are not a part of this manuscript, and have not undergone peer review by the American Physiological Society (APS). APS and the journal editors take no responsibility for these materials, for the website address, or for any links to or from it.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank Daniela Saderi, Luke A. Shaheen, and Sean Slee for training, advice, and assistance with neurophysiological recording, Ulysses Duckler for animal care, and Matthew J. McGinley, Avinash Bala, and Robert Peterka for technical assistance with pupillometry.
REFERENCES
- Aston-Jones G, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annu Rev Neurosci 28: 403–450, 2005. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beatty J. Task-evoked pupillary responses, processing load, and the structure of processing resources. Psychol Bull 91: 276–292, 1982a. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.91.2.276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beatty J. Phasic not tonic pupillary responses vary with auditory vigilance performance. Psychophysiology 19: 167–172, 1982b. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1982.tb02540.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bizley JK, Nodal FR, Nelken I, King AJ. Functional organization of ferret auditory cortex. Cereb Cortex 15: 1637–1653, 2005. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Churchland MM, Yu BM, Cunningham JP, Sugrue LP, Cohen MR, Corrado GS, Newsome WT, Clark AM, Hosseini P, Scott BB, Bradley DC, Smith MA, Kohn A, Movshon JA, Armstrong KM, Moore T, Chang SW, Snyder LH, Lisberger SG, Priebe NJ, Finn IM, Ferster D, Ryu SI, Santhanam G, Sahani M, Shenoy KV. Stimulus onset quenches neural variability: a widespread cortical phenomenon. Nat Neurosci 13: 369–378, 2010. doi: 10.1038/nn.2501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- David SV, Fritz JB, Shamma SA. Task reward structure shapes rapid receptive field plasticity in auditory cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109: 2144–2149, 2012. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1117717109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Gee JW, Colizoli O, Kloosterman NA, Knapen T, Nieuwenhuis S, Donner TH. Dynamic modulation of decision biases by brainstem arousal systems. eLife 6: e23232, 2017. doi: 10.7554/eLife.23232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diamond DM, Campbell AM, Park CR, Halonen J, Zoladz PR. The temporal dynamics model of emotional memory processing: a synthesis on the neurobiological basis of stress-induced amnesia, flashbulb and traumatic memories, and the Yerkes-Dodson law. Neural Plast 2007: 60803, 2007. doi: 10.1155/2007/60803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Du J, Blanche TJ, Harrison RR, Lester HA, Masmanidis SC. Multiplexed, high density electrophysiology with nanofabricated neural probes. PLoS One 6: e26204, 2011. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0026204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elgueda D, Duque D, Radtke-Schuller S, Yin P, David SV, Shamma SA, Fritz JB. State-dependent encoding of sound and behavioral meaning in a tertiary region of the ferret auditory cortex. Nat Neurosci 22: 447–459, 2019. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0317-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Englitz B, David SV, Sorenson MD, Shamma SA. MANTA—an open-source, high density electrophysiology recording suite for MATLAB. Front Neural Circuits 7: 69, 2013. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foote SL, Freedman R, Oliver AP. Effects of putative neurotransmitters on neuronal activity in monkey auditory cortex. Brain Res 86: 229–242, 1975. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(75)90699-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fritz J, Shamma S, Elhilali M, Klein D. Rapid task-related plasticity of spectrotemporal receptive fields in primary auditory cortex. Nat Neurosci 6: 1216–1223, 2003. doi: 10.1038/nn1141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fritz JB, David SV, Radtke-Schuller S, Yin P, Shamma SA. Adaptive, behaviorally gated, persistent encoding of task-relevant auditory information in ferret frontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 13: 1011–1019, 2010. doi: 10.1038/nn.2598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Froemke RC, Merzenich MM, Schreiner CE. A synaptic memory trace for cortical receptive field plasticity. Nature 450: 425–429, 2007. doi: 10.1038/nature06289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gelman A, Carlin JB, Stern HS, Rubin DB. Bayesian Data Analysis (2nd ed.). Boca Raton, FL: Taylor & Francis, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Harris KD, Thiele A. Cortical state and attention. Nat Rev Neurosci 12: 509–523, 2011. doi: 10.1038/nrn3084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Issa EB, Wang X. Sensory responses during sleep in primate primary and secondary auditory cortex. J Neurosci 28: 14467–14480, 2008. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3086-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joshi S, Li Y, Kalwani RM, Gold JI. Relationships between pupil diameter and neuronal activity in the locus coeruleus, colliculi, and cingulate cortex. Neuron 89: 221–234, 2016. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kahneman D, Beatty J. Pupil diameter and load on memory. Science 154: 1583–1585, 1966. doi: 10.1126/science.154.3756.1583. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larsen RS, Turschak E, Daigle T, Zeng H, Zhuang J, Waters J. Activation of neuromodulatory axon projections in primary visual cortex during periods of locomotion and pupil dilation (Preprint). bioRxiv 502013, 2018. doi: 10.1101/502013. [DOI]
- Lee CC, Middlebrooks JC. Auditory cortex spatial sensitivity sharpens during task performance. Nat Neurosci 14: 108–114, 2011. doi: 10.1038/nn.2713. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee CR, Margolis DJ. Pupil dynamics reflect behavioral choice and learning in a go/nogo tactile decision-making task in mice. Front Behav Neurosci 10: 200, 2016. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu Y, Rodenkirch C, Moskowitz N, Schriver B, Wang Q. Dynamic lateralization of pupil dilation evoked by locus coeruleus activation results from sympathetic, not parasympathetic, contributions. Cell Rep 20: 3099–3112, 2017. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2017.08.094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loewenfeld IE, Lowenstein O. The Pupil: Anatomy, Physiology, and Clinical Applications. Oxford: Butterworth-Heinemann, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Lovett-Barron M, Andalman AS, Allen WE, Vesuna S, Kauvar I, Burns VM, Deisseroth K. Ancestral circuits for the coordinated modulation of brain state. Cell 171: 1411–1423.e17, 2017. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2017.10.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manunta Y, Edeline JM. Effects of noradrenaline on frequency tuning of rat auditory cortex neurons. Eur J Neurosci 9: 833–847, 1997. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1997.tb01433.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manunta Y, Edeline JM. Effects of noradrenaline on frequency tuning of auditory cortex neurons during wakefulness and slow-wave sleep. Eur J Neurosci 11: 2134–2150, 1999. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00633.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martins AR, Froemke RC. Coordinated forms of noradrenergic plasticity in the locus coeruleus and primary auditory cortex. Nat Neurosci 18: 1483–1492, 2015. doi: 10.1038/nn.4090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGinley MJ, David SV, McCormick DA. Cortical membrane potential signature of optimal states for sensory signal detection. Neuron 87: 179–192, 2015a. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGinley MJ, Vinck M, Reimer J, Batista-Brito R, Zagha E, Cadwell CR, Tolias AS, Cardin JA, McCormick DA. Waking state: Rapid variations modulate neural and behavioral responses. Neuron 87: 1143–1161, 2015b. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer AF, Poort J, O’Keefe J, Sahani M, Linden JF. A head-mounted camera system integrates detailed behavioral monitoring with multichannel electrophysiology in freely moving mice. Neuron 100: 46–60.e7, 2018. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.09.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller JM, Sutton D, Pfingst B, Ryan A, Beaton R, Gourevitch G. Single cell activity in the auditory cortex of Rhesus monkeys: behavioral dependency. Science 177: 449–451, 1972. doi: 10.1126/science.177.4047.449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy PR, O’Connell RG, O’Sullivan M, Robertson IH, Balsters JH. Pupil diameter covaries with BOLD activity in human locus coeruleus. Hum Brain Mapp 35: 4140–4154, 2014. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy PR, Robertson IH, Balsters JH, O’connell RG. Pupillometry and P3 index the locus coeruleus-noradrenergic arousal function in humans. Psychophysiology 48: 1532–1543, 2011. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01226.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Musall S, Kaufman MT, Gluf S, Churchland AK. Movement-related activity dominates cortex during sensory-guided decision making. Nat Neurosci 22: 1677–1686, 2019. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0502-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nguyen AH, Stark LW. Model control of image processing: pupillometry. Comput Med Imaging Graph 17: 21–33, 1993. doi: 10.1016/0895-6111(93)90071-T. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niell CM, Stryker MP. Modulation of visual responses by behavioral state in mouse visual cortex. Neuron 65: 472–479, 2010. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niwa M, Johnson JS, O’Connor KN, Sutter ML. Active engagement improves primary auditory cortical neurons’ ability to discriminate temporal modulation. J Neurosci 32: 9323–9334, 2012. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5832-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Otazu GH, Tai LH, Yang Y, Zador AM. Engaging in an auditory task suppresses responses in auditory cortex. Nat Neurosci 12: 646–654, 2009. doi: 10.1038/nn.2306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pachitariu M, Steinmetz N, Kadir S, Carandini M, Harris KD. Kilosort: realtime spike-sorting for extracellular electrophysiology with hundreds of channels (Preprint). bioRxiv 061481, 2016. doi: 10.1101/061481. [DOI]
- Reimer J, Froudarakis E, Cadwell CR, Yatsenko D, Denfield GH, Tolias AS. Pupil fluctuations track fast switching of cortical states during quiet wakefulness. Neuron 84: 355–362, 2014. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.09.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reimer J, McGinley MJ, Liu Y, Rodenkirch C, Wang Q, McCormick DA, Tolias AS. Pupil fluctuations track rapid changes in adrenergic and cholinergic activity in cortex. Nat Commun 7: 13289, 2016. doi: 10.1038/ncomms13289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider DM, Nelson A, Mooney R. A synaptic and circuit basis for corollary discharge in the auditory cortex. Nature 513: 189–194, 2014. doi: 10.1038/nature13724. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shimaoka D, Harris KD, Carandini M. Effects of arousal on mouse sensory cortex depend on modality. Cell Rep 22: 3160–3167, 2018. [Erratum in Cell Rep 25: 3230, 2018.] doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.02.092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siegle JH, López AC, Patel YA, Abramov K, Ohayon S, Voigts J. Open Ephys: an open-source, plugin-based platform for multichannel electrophysiology. J Neural Eng 14: 045003, 2017. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/aa5eea. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silber MH, Ancoli-Israel S, Bonnet MH, Chokroverty S, Grigg-Damberger MM, Hirshkowitz M, Kapen S, Keenan SA, Kryger MH, Penzel T, Pressman MR, Iber C. The visual scoring of sleep in adults. J Clin Sleep Med 3: 121–131, 2007. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simonsohn U. Two-lines: a valid alternative to the invalid testing of u-shaped relationships with quadratic regressions. Adv Methods Pract Psychol Sci 1: 538–555, 2018. doi: 10.1177/2515245918805755. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stringer C, Pachitariu M, Steinmetz N, Reddy CB, Carandini M, Harris KD. Spontaneous behaviors drive multidimensional, brainwide activity. Science 364: 255, 2019. doi: 10.1126/science.aav7893. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Kempen J, Loughnane GM, Newman DP, Kelly SP, Thiele A, O’Connell RG, Bellgrove MA. Behavioural and neural signatures of perceptual decision-making are modulated by pupil-linked arousal. eLife 8: e42541, 2019. doi: 10.7554/eLife.42541. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vinck M, Batista-Brito R, Knoblich U, Cardin JA. Arousal and locomotion make distinct contributions to cortical activity patterns and visual encoding. Neuron 86: 740–754, 2015. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weinberger NM. Specific long-term memory traces in primary auditory cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci 5: 279–290, 2004. doi: 10.1038/nrn1366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yerkes RM, Dodson JD. The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. J Comp Neurol Psychol 18: 459–482, 1908. doi: 10.1002/cne.920180503. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Yüzgeç Ö, Prsa M, Zimmermann R, Huber D. Pupil size coupling to cortical states protects the stability of deep sleep via parasympathetic modulation. Curr Biol 28: 392–400.e3, 2018. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.12.049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zekveld AA, Koelewijn T, Kramer SE. The pupil dilation response to auditory stimuli: current state of knowledge. Trends Hear 22: 2331216518777174, 2018. doi: 10.1177/2331216518777174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]