Abstract
Metastable brain dynamics are characterized by abrupt, jump-like modulations so that the neural activity in single trials appears to unfold as a sequence of discrete, quasi-stationary ‘states’. Evidence that cortical neural activity unfolds as a sequence of metastable states is accumulating at fast pace. Metastable activity occurs both in response to an external stimulus and during ongoing, self-generated activity. These spontaneous metastable states are increasingly found to subserve internal representations that are not locked to external triggers, including states of deliberations, attention and expectation. Moreover, decoding stimuli or decisions via metastable states can be carried out trial-by-trial. Focusing on metastability will allow us to shift our perspective on neural coding from traditional concepts based on trial-averaging to models based on dynamic ensemble representations.
Recent theoretical work has started to characterize the mechanistic origin and potential roles of metastable representations. In this article we review recent findings on metastable activity, how it may arise in biologically realistic models, and its potential role for representing internal states as well as relevant task variables.
Introduction
Brain circuits consist of large neural networks, where populations of neurons are recurrently coupled via synaptic connections. These circuits can be interpreted as dynamical systems with many coupled degrees of freedom and therefore capable of generating a wealth of dynamical behaviors on very diverse timescales [1]. Those include transient relaxations towards a point or line attractor, oscillatory patterns, chaotic dynamics or metastable activity [2,3]. These dynamical behaviors have been implicated in important brain functions. Transient relaxation to stable neural activity (such as a point or line attractor) may subserve memory [4,5] and perceptual decisions [6,7]. Oscillatory dynamics may subserve respiration, locomotion, and other rhythmic forms of behavior [8,9]. Chaotic dynamics amplify random perturbations and, when successfully tamed by learning, can generate complex computations [10,11]. Metastable dynamics, initially characterized in the presence of a sensory stimulus [12,13], might underlie a range of internal computations during cognitive tasks [14••,15,16••,17••].
Relaxations to an attractor, oscillations and chaotic dynamics typically describe neural activity as smoothly varying over time [18,19]. In contrast, metastable dynamics are characterized by abrupt, jump-like modulations so that neural activity appears to unfold as a sequence of discrete, quasi-stationary ‘states’. Metastable activity is being found in an increasing number of brain structures of different species engaged in a variety of tasks, and is the focus of this opinion. The intuitive appeal of metastable activity is that it resonates with our intuition that our thoughts and actions proceed along a sequence of distinct episodes, as we scan alternatives and ponder potential options during everyday tasks. It is natural to think that such episodes are being represented in transient but well-defined neural patterns in our brains. Recent models have started to clarify how metastable activity could emerge spontaneously as a collective phenomenon via attractor dynamics in spiking networks [20••,21,22••,23••]. These models have proved powerful tools to investigate the circuit origin of metastable states and their potential role in sensory and cognitive processes.
In this article we review recent progress in the analysis of metastable state sequences, focusing on issues of detection, modeling and interpretation.
Observing metastable activity
Metastable states were first characterized in electrophys-iological recordings from the prefrontal cortex of monkeys performing a delayed localization task [12,24,25]. Spike counts from simultaneously recorded neurons were analyzed with a hidden Markov model (HMM), a popular latent variable model for analyzing sequential data arising, for example, in time series analysis, speech recognition [26], DNA sequence analysis [27], behavioral analysis [28], and many other problems [29,30].
Under the most common application of HMM to neural data, population activity from simultaneously recorded neurons unfolds through a sequence of ‘hidden’ states, each state being a set of ensemble firing rates (Figure 1a). Stochastic transitions between states occur at random times according to an underlying Markov chain, and neurons discharge as Poisson processes with state-dependent firing rates, though the model can be extended to include refractory periods and other history-dependent factors [31]. The number of hidden states may be selected using information criteria [15,32,33••], cross-validation [14••], or nonparametric Bayesian methods [34,35••]. Several measures have been used to assess the goodness-of-fit of inferred sequences, including variance explained [14••], comparison to shuffled datasets [16••], and comparison of single-neuron transitions to ensemble state transition [22••]. The strength of HMM is to allow a principled, unsupervised method for segmenting neural activity into a sequence of discrete metastable states, and its use has led to intriguing new observations in ensemble dynamics, as we discuss next.
Consistent features of metastable activity
HMM and related procedures for detecting hidden states have established a number of consistent features across datasets, animals and tasks. Ensemble activity unfolds as a sequence of metastable states, each lasting from a few hundred ms to a second or more, with sharp transitions among the states [12,13,15,22••] (Figure 1b). The transitions are typically one order of magnitude faster than the state durations, are close to their theoretically observable lower bound, and are not an artifact of the HMM [12,13,15]. When the start of the sequence is aligned to an external event such as stimulus onset, specific state sequences are observed (Figure 1b), raising the possibility that stimuli are coded by dynamic state sequences [12,13].
Among the most significant early results of applying HMM to the analysis of neural data were the demonstration that (i) the hidden states identified in response to a stimulus tend to recur during most of the recorded activity, not just in response to the stimulus, and (ii) pairwise correlations among simultaneously recorded neurons depend on an underlying global activity state, and not just on neural connectivity [12,24]. These two observations are very relevant for current issues in neuroscience, including the origin and role of ongoing activity [36,37], the importance of the connectome [38,39], and the meaning of correlations [40-42].
It is worth remarking that the ability of HMM analysis to segment the neural activity into distinct states in an unsupervised manner – that is, without knowledge of external triggers such as stimulus timings, – has been crucial to obtaining the results reviewed above. It is also important to point out that the HMM analysis on ensemble data allows to decode the neural activity on a trial-by-trial basis, avoiding the necessity of averaging across trials. Most findings discussed in this review would go undetected if data were averaged across trials, because ensemble transitions occur at different times in each trial. This latter feature also allows the time-warping of state sequences, explaining why, for example, the method is so useful for speech recognition and for the analysis of neural activity during birdsongs [43]. This could also explain part of the trial-to-trial variability observed in cortex after aligning spike trains across different trials to, for example, stimulus onset [13].
Metastability of ongoing cortical activity
One central question in contemporary neuroscience is why ongoing cortical activity is so rich in structure and resembles so closely stimulus-evoked activity [36,37,44-49]. The origin of ongoing activity and the nature of its interaction with evoked activity can potentially reveal much about the way cortical networks are functionally organized and how this may support flexible coding (see Refs. [36,37] for two recent proposals). However, quantifying precisely the similarities and differences between ongoing and evoked activity has proved difficult. A recent HMM analysis of ongoing spiking activity in the gustatory cortex of behaving rats has demonstrated that ongoing activity, similarly to evoked activity, unfolds as a sequence of metastable states [22••]. Transitions among states are characterized by a partial degree of coordination, so that ongoing activity is neither completely asynchronous nor completely synchronized. Ongoing and stimulus-evoked activity share most of the same states, although some of the states occur mostly during ongoing activity and others during evoked activity [33••]. About 50% of the ensemble neurons exhibit three or more different firing rates across states, that is, they are ‘multi-stable’ rather than bistable; and this fraction is substantially reduced by stimulation [22••]. These findings have all been captured by a spiking network model of metastability that might shed light into the link between ongoing and evoked activity (see section ‘Spiking network models of metastable activity’).
Metastable sequences as a substrate for internal computations
Do metastable states have precise and specific meanings as neural correlates of external or internally generated events? Visual [12], gustatory [13,22••] and vibro-tactile stimuli [15] seem represented by reliable sequences of HMM states in primate and rodent cortical areas in the context of different tasks.
Even more remarkably, though, aspects of the metastable dynamics seem to subserve internal states of attention, expectation, and internal deliberations during decisions, and can be used to predict behavior. In monkeys performing a selective attention task, the probability of detecting a change in the attended stimulus was significantly greater when occurring during an ‘ON’ state (characterized by vigorous multi-unit activity in area V4) than during an ‘OFF’ state (characterized by faint multi-unit activity) [14••] (Figure 2a). Similarly, different hidden states were preferentially associated to different task conditions in a human working memory task, with occupancy rate in each state predicting better performance in the corresponding task [35••]. Moreover, changes in patterns of functional connectivity across many brain areas co-occurred more reliably with state transitions than with external triggers, suggesting that metastable states supported by flexible patterns of functional connectivity may reflect internal representations of task demands.
A similar relationship between state occupancy rate and decisions has been found in monkeys performing a choice task between differentially rewarded stimuli [17••]. Ensemble activity in orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) mostly switched between two states representing the values of the stimuli currently being offered, with larger occupancy rate predicting the behavioral decision (Figure 2b, left). Intriguingly, slower decisions (reflecting longer internal deliberation) occurred when the states had similar occupancy rates and were found on trials of the same kind (thus reflecting subjective preferences rather than actual task difficulty; Figure 2b, right). These findings are reminiscent of those on dynamic changes of mind [50] and complement earlier intriguing results on the possible meaning of state transitions, which may underlie the sudden realization that task rules have changed [51] or reflect the difficulty of a vibro-tactile discrimination [15].
In the rat hippocampus, HMM states were recently found to represent the position in a linear track and in an open field during a navigation task [16••] (Figure 2c), an alternative view to decoding spatial maps based on single neurons place fields [52]. The state sequences were observed in area CA1 during hippocampal sharp wave ripples when the animal was not exploring the track, and could be used to infer a spatial map of the environment without any reference to external locations [16••].
In rat gustatory cortex, state sequences after stimulus delivery proceed faster when the delivery is expected compared to when it is not expected [33••], which may explain the faster decoding of expected stimuli found in this area [53] (a spiking network model explanation of this phenomenon is discussed in the next section).
The examples discussed in this subsection make a convincing case that metastable sequences underlie a variety of important cognitive processes, and more examples will likely be found across different cognitive domains not yet explored with latent state models.
Spiking network models of metastable activity
In addition to statistical descriptions in terms of HMM and related latent state models, mechanistic accounts of metastable activity based on biologically plausible networks of spiking neurons have also been given. We shall mostly focus on efforts that have purposefully combined spiking network modeling with HMM [22••,33••,54,55], as we believe that such an approach is the most promising to understand the potential origin and function of metastable activity in neural circuits. Metastable activity naturally occurs when multiple hidden states are attractive fixed points of the neural dynamics which are either inherently unstable, or can be destabilized by internal noise or external perturbations [56-58]. Such fixed points would attract the dynamic trajectories of the neural activity and force them to linger in their neighborhood for a finite amount of time. It is natural, therefore, to look for models wherein metastability is caused by the coexistence of multiple attractor states.
Metastable activity in clustered networks of spiking neurons
Among the class of attractor neural networks with multiple attractor states, spiking network models allow to build biologically plausible models capable of generating metastable state sequences. Recurrent networks with strong synaptic connections can produce highly temporally fluctuating activity [59-67], often the signature of chaotic activity. However, to endow a network of spiking neurons with endogenously generated metastable states, a specific partition of the network in subpopulations of neurons seems required [20••,21,22••,23••]. The clustered architecture has emerged as an effective way to generate metastability. The models studied so far comprise excitatory and inhibitory spiking neurons with (typically) the excitatory neurons organized in clusters, as seems the case in cortical circuits [68-70]. The common feature of these models is that the average synaptic strength (and/or connectivity) inside clusters is larger than between clusters (Figure 3a, right). A mean field analysis of these models [4,22••,54] shows that multiple attractor states are possible depending on the average intra-cluster synaptic weight (ICW; Figure 3b). Above a critical ICW value (dashed green line in Figure 3b), multiple configurations of the network emerge wherein neurons in clusters tend to occupy several states with different values of mean firing rates (grey and red lines in Figure 3b), where the firing rates depend on the number of active clusters. In an infinite network, those configurations would be stable attractors; but in a finite network, or in the presence of sufficient external noise, those configurations become metastable. Note that if the ICW is large enough (e.g. full green line in Figure 3b), the configuration with no active clusters is unstable even in the infinite network, and at least one cluster must be active at any given time.
It is unclear whether suitable connectivity patterns exist such that metastable itinerant dynamics is possible also in a deterministic network of infinite size [71]. In the following, we assume that fluctuations originate in finite size effects in a deterministic network [20••,22••], and later consider alternative models (see subsection ‘Variations on the clustered architecture’).
In finite clustered networks above the critical ICW value, a combination of erratic spiking activity and recurrent inhibition can ignite an itinerant exploration of multiple configurations (as in Figure 3a, right), whose number and features depend on network parameters such as the ICW. When an HMM analysis is run on these networks, segmentation of the spontaneous ongoing activity in discrete states is found (Figure 3c). Such metastable activity is modified, but not removed, by an external stimulus. Since the firing rates depend on the number of active clusters, the neurons exhibit multiple firing rates across different HMM states, as found experimentally in rat gustatory cortex [22••]. Note that the neurons themselves can have graded activity; hence, their multistability is an emergent property of the collective network’s dynamics and not an intrinsic property of the units.
Predictions of the clustered spiking network
Clustered spiking networks capture many statistical and dynamical features of the data, including a stimulus-induced reduction of: (i) trial-to-trial variability [20••,21,72], multistability of the firing rates across states [22••], and neural dimensionality [54], that is, the minimal number of effective dimensions (between one and the number of neurons) necessary to describe the ensemble dynamics [73-75]. It is worth noticing that while the stimulus-driven reduction of trial-to-trial variability has been a catalyst for the development of the clustered network model [20••,21], the reduction of multistability and dimensionality was found to be a naturally emergent property of the model which led to finding the corresponding properties in the data.
These predictions capture aspects of the data and thus help to corroborate the validity of the model. However, two other recent predictions link metastability directly with sensory and cognitive functions. The first is that visiting metastable states during ongoing activity will help to maintain learned stimulus responses and improve performance, suggesting yet another role for ongoing activity. This prediction emerged from an attempt to understand how neural clusters and metastable states can emerge during learning, and revealed the need to complement synaptic plasticity with homeostatic mechanisms [76-78]. The second prediction has led to the proposal that a state of expectation could be understood as an acceleration of metastable dynamics [33••]. When rats are trained to respond to taste stimuli, the identity of the stimuli can be decoded faster from the neural activity if stimulus delivery is expected compared to when stimuli are randomly, and passively, administered [53]. In the model, the onset of a predictive cue modifies the network’s landscape of metastable configurations so as to make state transitions more frequent. Faster state transitions, in turn, induce faster onset of stimulus-coding states after stimulus delivery, an effect uncovered by an HMM analysis of the model and confirmed in the data.
Variations on the clustered architecture
Variations of the clustered network architecture and its generated dynamics can explain related kinds of metastable activity such as perceptual bistability [79,80], the emergence of specific slow oscillations known as ‘up’ and ‘down’ states [81], or networks that can sustain bidirectional sequence propagation at slow and tunable speed [82]. These types of metastable dynamics are different from those reviewed earlier in subtle, but significant, ways.
Clustered spiking networks have also been used to uncover subtle differences between mechanisms for decision making. Specifically, spiking networks with multiple attractors built for decision making [7,83] can work in two different modes, the ‘ramping’ mode, characterized by gradual stimulus-driven firing rate increases, and the ‘jumping’ mode [55,56], wherein the stimulus ignites a sequence of metastable states. By leveraging a mechanism reminiscent of stochastic resonance, the jumping mode can outperform the ramping mode (underlying the so-called ‘integration-to-bound’ models [84]) during perceptual decision-making in the face of sensory noise.
Finally, we briefly mention that models with endogenously generated activity are not necessary to produce metastable activity. External fluctuations or the presence of firing rate adaptation both can create instability and lead to metastability in a finite network with multiple attractors [85,86] (including ‘up’ and ‘down’ states at low firing rate, [87]). Sometimes both ingredients are required to reproduce the statistics of the empirical data [87,88]. Also, it is possible to obtain stimulus-evoked quenching of variability in non-metastable networks, such as networks with chaotic attractors [89] or the supralinear stabilized network (SSN) [90-92]. These models capture the reduction of variability via different mechanisms and make different predictions, such as the tuning-dependent modulation of variability observed in primary visual cortex and captured by the SSN [92].
Conclusions
We have reviewed recent progress made in elucidating the nature and the role of metastable states in brain function. Thanks to the availability of ensemble data from behaving animals, the study of metastable activity is changing the way we think about neural coding for both external events and internal representations.
We have also reviewed a class of spiking network models that promise to provide a mechanistic understanding of the origin and function of metastability. These models have demonstrated the importance of a clustered architecture for the spontaneous generation of metastability, the coexistence of ongoing and evoked metastability, the benefits of metastable sequences to decision making, and the explanation of faster coding of expected events via the acceleration of metastable dynamics. Because of their biological plausibility, not only can these models shed light on the mechanisms, but they can also be powerful tools for analyzing metastable states, predict causal relationships to behavior, and help to design new experimental studies.
In the future, it will be important to establish a causal relationship between metastable states and behavior, by manipulating neural activity during behavioral tasks while recording from large ensembles across multiple sites. It will also be important to establish the emergence of neural clusters and metastable dynamics though development or learning. Recurrent spiking network models, in conjunction with latent-state models such as HMM, are likely to play an important role in his endeavor.
Acknowledgements
The authors acknowledge support from the National Science Foundation (NSF grant IIS-1161852 to GLC), the National Institutes of Health (NIH/NIHDCD R01DC015234 to AF and NIH/NIDCD K25DC013557 to LM) and the Swartz Foundation (Award 66438 to LM) during the development of some of the ideas and concepts discussed in this article.
Footnotes
Conflict of interest statement
Nothing declared.
References and recommended reading
Papers of particular interest, published within the period of review, have been highlighted as:
•• of outstanding interest
- 1.Bernacchia A, Seo H, Lee D, Wang X-J: A reservoir of time constants for memory traces in cortical neurons. Nat Neurosci 2011, 14:366–372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Rabinovich MI, Varona P, Selverston AI, Abarbanel HDI: Dynamical principles in neuroscience. Rev Mod Phys 2006, 78:1213–1265. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Miller P: Dynamical systems, attractors, and neural circuits. F1000Res 2016, 5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Amit DJ, Brunel N: Model of global spontaneous activity and local structured activity during delay periods in the cerebral cortex. Cereb Cortex 1997, 7:237–252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Seung HS: How the brain keeps the eyes still. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1996, 93:13339–13344. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Mante V, Sussillo D, Shenoy KV, Newsome WT: Context-dependent computation by recurrent dynamics in prefrontal cortex. Nature 2013, 503:78–84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Wang X-J: Probabilistic decision making by slow reverberation in cortical circuits. Neuron 2002, 36:955–968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Wang X-J: Neurophysiological and computational principles of cortical rhythms in cognition. Physiol Rev 2010, 90:1195–1268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Buzsaki G: Rhythms of the Brain. Oxford University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Sussillo D, Abbott LF: Generating coherent patterns of activity from chaotic neural networks. Neuron 2009, 63:544–557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Laje R, Buonomano DV: Robust timing and motor patterns by taming chaos in recurrent neural networks. Nat Neurosci 2013, 16:925–933. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Abeles M, Bergman H, Gat I, Meilijson I, Seidemann E, Tishby N, Vaadia E: Cortical activity flips among quasi-stationary states. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1995, 92:8616–8620. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Jones LM, Fontanini A, Sadacca BF, Miller P, Katz DB: Natural stimuli evoke dynamic sequences of states in sensory cortical ensembles. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2007, 104:18772–18777. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.••.Engel TA, Steinmetz NA, Gieselmann MA, Thiele A, Moore T, Boahen K: Selective modulation of cortical state during spatial attention. Science 2016, 354:1140–1144.In area V4 of behaving monkeys, ON and OFF metastable states are related to global changes in cortical state (associated with arousal) as well as local selective attention demands, with better performance when a stimulus change occurs during ON states compared to OFF states.
- 15.Ponce-Alvarez A, Nácher V, Luna R, Riehle A, Romo R: Dynamics of cortical neuronal ensembles transit from decision making to storage for later report. J Neurosci 2012, 32:11956–11969. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.••.Maboudi K, Ackermann E, de Jong LW, Pfeiffer BE, Foster D, Diba K, Kemere C: Uncovering temporal structure in hippocampal output patterns. eLife 2018, 7.Spontaneous metastable states observed in CA1 during population burst events (when the animal is idling) reconstruct a map of place fields evoked during locomotion.
- 17.••.Rich EL, Wallis JD: Decoding subjective decisions from orbitofrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 2016, 19:973–980.In the OFC of monkeys, periods of deliberation during a choice task are associated with sequences of states representing the values (rewards) associated to the options being offered in the current choice. The longer the presence of one state compared to the others, the quicker the behavioral decision, regardless of the presumed difficulty of the choice according to the similarity in values. It is also shown that ensembles of OFC neurons can represent the value of unchosen options (a fact undetected in single neurons).
- 18.Churchland MM, Cunningham JP, Kaufman MT, Foster JD, Nuyujukian P, Ryu SI, Shenoy KV: Neural population dynamics during reaching. Nature 2012, 487:51–56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Mazor O, Laurent G: Transient dynamics versus fixed points in odor representations by locust antennal lobe projection neurons. Neuron 2005, 48:661–673. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.••.Litwin-Kumar A, Doiron B: Slow dynamics and high variability in balanced cortical networks with clustered connections. Nat Neurosci 2012, 15:1498–1505.This theoretical work demonstrated that a balanced spiking network with clustered architecture can generate slow metastable activity via internally generated fluctuations.
- 21.Deco G, Hugues E: Neural network mechanisms underlying stimulus driven variability reduction. PLoS Comput Biol 2012, 8: e1002395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.••.Mazzucato L, Fontanini A, La Camera G: Dynamics of multistable states during ongoing and evoked cortical activity. J Neurosci 2015, 35:8214–8231.This study demonstrates that ongoing activity in rat gustatory cortex unfolds as a sequence of metastable states that is compatible with the dynamics of a clustered spiking network model. The model predicts key features of both ongoing and evoked activity, including a stimulus-induced reduction of multistability in single neurons.
- 23.••.Schaub MT, Billeh YN, Anastassiou CA, Koch C, Barahona M: Emergence of slow-switching assemblies in structured neuronal networks. PLoS Comput Biol 2015, 11 :e1004196.This paper presents the link between metastable activity and the eigen-value spectrum of the synaptic matrix in a clustered spiking network. When the model is augmented with a hierarchical architecture, transitions among metastable states at higher level in the hierarchy occur on slower timescales than transitions among the states at lower level of the hierarchy.
- 24.Seidemann E, Meilijson I, Abeles M, Bergman H, Vaadia E: Simultaneously recorded single units in the frontal cortex go through sequences of discrete and stable states in monkeys performing a delayed localization task. J Neurosci 1996, 16:752–768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Gat I, Tishby N: Statistical modeling of cell-assemblies activities in associative cortex of behaving monkeys. Neural Information Processing Systems. 1993:945–953. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Rabiner LR: A tutorial on hidden Markov models and selected applications in speech recognition. Proc IEEE 1989, 77:257–286. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Durbin R, Eddy SR, Krogh A, Mitchison G: Biological Sequence Analysis: Probabilistic Models of Proteins and Nucleic Acids. Cambridge University Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Wiltschko AB, Johnson MJ, Iurilli G, Peterson RE, Katon JM, Pashkovski SL, Abraira VE, Adams RP, Datta SR: Mapping subsecond structure in mouse behavior. Neuron 2015, 88:1121–1135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Zucchini W, MacDonald IL, Langrock R: Hidden Markov Models for Time Series: An Introduction Using R. edn 2 CRC Press; 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Dymarski P (Ed): Hidden Markov Models, Theory and Applications. InTech; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Escola S, Fontanini A, Katz D, Paninski L: Hidden Markov models for the stimulus-response relationships of multistate neural systems. Neural Comput 2011, 23:1071–1132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Sadacca BF, Mukherjee N, Vladusich T, Li JX, Katz DB, Miller P: The behavioral relevance of cortical neural ensemble responses emerges suddenly. J Neurosci 2016, 36:655–669. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.••.Mazzucato L, La Camera G, Fontanini A: Expectation-induced modulation of metastable activity underlies faster coding of sensory stimuli. Nat Neurosci 2019, 22:787–796.This paper shows that expectation induces faster coding of taste stimuli in rat gustatory cortex by modulating the timescale of ensemble metastable dynamics. A spiking network model explains the phenomenon in terms of decreased energy barriers between metastable configurations of the network’s activity. Shallower barriers cause faster transitions to stimulus-coding states identified with an HMM analysis, accelerating stimulus coding.
- 34.Linderman SW, Johnson MJ, Wilson MA, Chen Z: A Bayesian nonparametric approach for uncovering rat hippocampal population codes during spatial navigation. J Neurosci Methods 2016, 263:36–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.••.Taghia J, Cai W, Ryali S, Kochalka J, Nicholas J, Chen T, Menon V: Uncovering hidden brain state dynamics that regulate performance and decision-making during cognition. Nat Commun 2018, 9:2505.An HMM analysis of fMRI data in 122 human subjects performing a working memory task with multiple task conditions reveals the existence of metastable states preferentially associated to different task conditions. The hidden states are, in turn, associated to different patterns of functional connectivity across many brain areas.
- 36.Orbán G, Berkes P, Fiser J, Lengyel M: Neural variability and sampling-based probabilistic representations in the visual cortex. Neuron 2016, 92:530–543. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Stringer C, Pachitariu M, Steinmetz N, Reddy CB: Spontaneous behaviors drive multidimensional, brain-wide activity. Science 2019, 364 eaav7893. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Swanson LW, Lichtman JW: From Cajal to connectome and beyond. Annu Rev Neurosci 2016, 39:197–216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Bargmann CI: Beyond the connectome: how neuromodulators shape neural circuits. Bioessays 2012, 34:458–465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Ostojic S, Brunel N, Hakim V: How connectivity, background activity, and synaptic properties shape the cross-correlation between spike trains. J Neurosci 2009, 29:10234–10253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Doiron B, Litwin-Kumar A, Rosenbaum R, Ocker GK, Josić K: The mechanics of state-dependent neural correlations. Nat Neurosci 2016, 19:383–393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Rosenbaum R, Tchumatchenko T, Moreno-Bote R: Correlated neuronal activity and its relationship to coding, dynamics and network architecture. Front Comput Neurosci 2014, 8:102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Blaettler F, Kollmorgen S, Herbst J, Hahnloser R: Hidden Markov models in the neurosciences Hidden Markov Models, Theory and Applications. InTech; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Arieli A, Sterkin A, Grinvald A, Aertsen A: Dynamics of ongoing activity: explanation of the large variability in evoked cortical responses. Science 1996, 273:1868–1871. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Kenet T, Bibitchkov D, Tsodyks M, Grinvald A, Arieli A: Spontaneously emerging cortical representations of visual attributes. Nature 2003, 425:954–956. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Deco G, Jirsa VK: Ongoing cortical activity at rest: criticality, multistability, and ghost attractors. J Neurosci 2012, 32:3366–3375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Luczak A, Barthó P, Harris KD: Spontaneous events outline the realm of possible sensory responses in neocortical populations. Neuron 2009, 62:413–425. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Tsodyks M, Kenet T, Grinvald A, Arieli A: Linking spontaneous activity of single cortical neurons and the underlying functional architecture. Science 1999, 286:1943–1946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Ringach DL: Spontaneous and driven cortical activity: implications for computation. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2009, 19:439–444. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Kiani R, Cueva CJ, Reppas JB, Newsome WT: Dynamics of neural population responses in prefrontal cortex indicate changes of mind on single trials. Curr Biol 2014, 24:1542–1547. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Durstewitz D, Vittoz NM, Floresco SB, Seamans JK: Abrupt transitions between prefrontal neural ensemble states accompany behavioral transitions during rule learning. Neuron 2010, 66:438–448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Moser EI, Moser M-B, McNaughton BL: Spatial representation in the hippocampal formation: a history. Nat Neurosci 2017, 20:1448–1464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Samuelsen CL, Gardner MPH, Fontanini A: Effects of cuetriggered expectation on cortical processing of taste. Neuron 2012, 74:410–422. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Mazzucato L, Fontanini A, La Camera G: Stimuli reduce the dimensionality of cortical activity. Front Syst Neurosci 2016, 10:11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Miller P, Katz DB: Stochastic transitions between neural states in taste processing and decision-making. J Neurosci 2010, 30:2559–2570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Miller P, Katz DB: Stochastic transitions between states of neural activity. The Dynamic Brain. Oxford University Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Miller P: Itinerancy between attractor states in neural systems. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2016, 40:14–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Rabinovich MI, Huerta R, Varona P, Afraimovich VS: Transient cognitive dynamics, metastability, and decision making. PLoS Comput Biol 2008, 4:e1000072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Sompolinsky H, Crisanti A, Sommers HJ: Chaos in random neural networks. Phys Rev Lett 1988, 61:259–262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Ostojic S: Two types of asynchronous activity in networks of excitatory and inhibitory spiking neurons. Nat Neurosci 2014, 17:594–600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Huang C, Doiron B: Once upon a (slow) time in the land of recurrent neuronal networks … . Curr Opin Neurobiol 2017, 46:31–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Harish O, Hansel D: Asynchronous rate chaos in spiking neuronal circuits. PLoS Comput Biol 2015, 11:e1004266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Mastrogiuseppe F, Ostojic S: Intrinsically-generated fluctuating activity in excitatory-inhibitory networks. PLoS Comput Biol 2017, 13:e1005498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Kadmon J, Sompolinsky H: Transition to chaos in random neuronal networks. Phys Rev X 2015, 5 041030. [Google Scholar]
- 65.Wieland S, Bernardi D, Schwalger T, Lindner B: Slow fluctuations in recurrent networks of spiking neurons. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys 2015, 92 040901. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Crisanti A, Sompolinsky H: Path integral approach to random neural networks. Phys Rev E 2018, 98 062120. [Google Scholar]
- 67.Stern M, Sompolinsky H, Abbott LF: Dynamics of random neural networks with bistable units. Phys Rev E 2014, 90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Song S, Sjöström PJ, Reigl M, Nelson S, Chklovskii DB: Highly nonrandom features of synaptic connectivity in local cortical circuits. PLoS Biol 2005, 3:e68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Perin R, Berger TK, Markram H: A synaptic organizing principle for cortical neuronal groups. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2011, 108:5419–5424. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Kiani R, Cueva CJ, Reppas JB, Peixoto D, Ryu SI, Newsome WT: Natural grouping of neural responses reveals spatially segregated clusters in prearcuate cortex. Neuron 2015, 85:1359–1373. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Doiron B, Litwin-Kumar A: Balanced neural architecture and the idling brain. Front Comput Neurosci 2014, 8:56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Churchland MM, Yu BM, Cunningham JP, Sugrue LP, Cohen MR, Corrado GS, Newsome WT, Clark AM, Hosseini P, Scott BB et al. : Stimulus onset quenches neural variability: a widespread cortical phenomenon. Nat Neurosci 2010, 13:369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Williamson RC, Cowley BR, Litwin-Kumar A, Doiron B, Kohn A, Smith MA, Yu BM: Scaling properties of dimensionality reduction for neural populations and network models. PLoS Comput Biol 2016, 12:e1005141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Gao P, Trautmann E, Yu BM, Santhanam G, Ryu S, Shenoy K, Ganguli S: A theory of multineuronal dimensionality, dynamics and measurement. bioRxiv 2017. 10.1101/214262.214262. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Abbott LF, Rajan K, Sompolinsky H: Interactions between intrinsic and stimulus-evoked activity in recurrent neural networks In The Dynamic Brain: An Exploration of Neuronal Variability and its Functional Significance. Edited by Ding M, Glanzman D. Oxford UP: Oxford; 2011:65–82. [Google Scholar]
- 76.Ocker GK, Litwin-Kumar A, Doiron B: Self-organization of microcircuits in networks of spiking neurons with plastic synapses. PLoS Comput Biol 2015, 11:e1004458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Litwin-Kumar A, Doiron B: Formation and maintenance of neuronal assemblies through synaptic plasticity. Nat Commun 2014, 5:5319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Zenke F, Agnes EJ, Gerstner W: Diverse synaptic plasticity mechanisms orchestrated to form and retrieve memories in spiking neural networks. Nat Commun 2015, 6:6922. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Moreno-Bote R, Rinzel J, Rubin N: Noise-induced alternations in an attractor network model of perceptual bistability. J Neurophysiol 2007, 98:1125–1139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Cao R, Pastukhov A, Mattia M, Braun J: Collective activity of many bistable assemblies reproduces characteristic dynamics of multistable perception. J Neurosci 2016, 36:6957–6972. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Setareh H, Deger M, Petersen CCH, Gerstner W: Cortical dynamics in presence of assemblies of densely connected weight-hub neurons. Front Comput Neurosci 2017, 11:52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Setareh H, Deger M, Gerstner W: Excitable neuronal assemblies with adaptation as a building block of brain circuits for velocity-controlled signal propagation. PLoS Comput Biol 2018, 14:e1006216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Wang X-J: Decision making in recurrent neuronal circuits. Neuron 2008, 60:215–234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Bogacz R, Brown E, Moehlis J, Holmes P, Cohen JD: The physics of optimal decision making: a formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks. Psychol Rev 2006, 113:700–765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Giugliano M, La Camera G, Fusi S, Senn W: The response of cortical neurons to in vivo-like input current: theory and experiment: II. Time-varying and spatially distributed inputs. Biol Cybern 2008, 99:303–318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Russo E, Treves A: Cortical free-association dynamics: distinct phases of a latching network. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys 2012, 85 051920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Jercog D, Roxin A, Barthó P, Luczak A, Compte A, de la Rocha J: UP-DOWN cortical dynamics reflect state transitions in a bistable network. eLife 2017, 6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Shpiro A, Moreno-Bote R, Rubin N, Rinzel J: Balance between noise and adaptation in competition models of perceptual bistability. J Comput Neurosci 2009, 27:37–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Rajan K, Abbott LF, Sompolinsky H: Stimulus-dependent suppression of chaos in recurrent neural networks. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys 2010, 82 011903. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Rubin DB, Van Hooser SD, Miller KD: The stabilized supralinear network: a unifying circuit motif underlying multi-input integration in sensory cortex. Neuron 2015, 85:402–417. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Ahmadian Y, Rubin DB, Miller KD: Analysis of the stabilized supralinear network. Neural Comput 2013, 25:1994–2037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Hennequin G, Ahmadian Y, Rubin DB, Lengyel M, Miller KD: The dynamical regime of sensory cortex: stable dynamics around a single stimulus-tuned attractor account for patterns of noise variability. Neuron 2018, 98:846–860.e5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]