Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2019 Dec 16;8:e52465. doi: 10.7554/eLife.52465

Navigating the garden of forking paths for data exclusions in fear conditioning research

Tina B Lonsdorf 1,, Maren Klingelhöfer-Jens 1, Marta Andreatta 2,3, Tom Beckers 4, Anastasia Chalkia 4, Anna Gerlicher 5, Valerie L Jentsch 6, Shira Meir Drexler 6, Gaetan Mertens 7, Jan Richter 8, Rachel Sjouwerman 1, Julia Wendt 9, Christian J Merz 6
Editors: Floris P de Lange10, Alexander Shackman11
PMCID: PMC6989118  PMID: 31841112

Abstract

In this report, we illustrate the considerable impact of researcher degrees of freedom with respect to exclusion of participants in paradigms with a learning element. We illustrate this empirically through case examples from human fear conditioning research, in which the exclusion of ‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’ is common – despite a lack of consensus on how to define these groups. We illustrate the substantial heterogeneity in exclusion criteria identified in a systematic literature search and highlight the potential problems and pitfalls of different definitions through case examples based on re-analyses of existing data sets. On the basis of these studies, we propose a consensus on evidence-based rather than idiosyncratic criteria, including clear guidelines on reporting details. Taken together, we illustrate how flexibility in data collection and analysis can be avoided, which will benefit the robustness and replicability of research findings and can be expected to be applicable to other fields of research that involve a learning element.

Research organism: Human

Introduction

In the past decade, efforts to understand the impact of undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis on research findings have gained momentum – for instance in defining and excluding ‘outliers’ (Simmons et al., 2011). This flexibility has been referred to as ‘researcher degrees of freedom’ (Simmons et al., 2011) or ‘the garden of forking paths’ (Gelman and Loken, 2013) to reflect the fact that each decision during data processing and/or analysis will take the researcher down a different ‘path’. Importantly and concerningly, these different paths can lead to fundamentally different end-points (i.e., results and associated conclusions) despite an identical starting point (i.e., raw data) (Silberzahn et al., 2018). Often, researchers take a certain path without malicious intent to obtain favorable results (e.g., ‘p-hacking’; Head et al., 2015): the decision to follow a certain path may be based on unawareness of alternative paths (due to lack of specific background knowledge) or the researcher following the most obvious path from an individual perspective. The latter is influenced by the scientific environment, the research question at stake or practices previously published by researchers in the field.

Admittedly, there is substantial ambiguity in what constitutes ‘the best decision’ for data analysis, and none of the available options may be necessarily incorrect (Simmons et al., 2011; Silberzahn et al., 2018). More precisely, different paths in the garden of forking paths may be more or less appropriate for different research questions, experimental designs, outcome measures or samples. Consequently, it is notoriously difficult for researchers, particularly those new to a field, to make informed and hence appropriate decisions. As a matter of fact, it is difficult to anticipate the number of different paths available and the consequences of choosing one over the other, or to come up with facts that truly justify choosing one path over the other – even for experts in a field. However, simply choosing a particular path because others chose it before (i.e., adopting published exclusion criteria) can also be highly problematic, as decisions often hinge on study-specific characteristics that do not invariantly apply to other studies.

We argue that it is important to raise awareness to this issue. Specifically, we think that it is critical to discuss both the rationale behind and the consequences associated with taking different analytical paths in general and in specific sub-fields of research. Here, we exemplarily take up this discussion for human fear conditioning research as a case example for tasks with a learning element grounded in recent discussions in science in general (Flake and Fried, 2019; John et al., 2012) and in fear conditioning research specifically (Lonsdorf et al., 2017; Lonsdorf et al., 2019). Fear conditioning is a typical paradigm employed to study (emotional) learning and memory processes with a particularly strong translational perspective (Lonsdorf et al., 2017; Vervliet et al., 2013). Questions addressed in the field of human fear conditioning are often concerned with consolidation, retrieval, generalization or modification of conditioned responses. Hence, it has often been claimed that the study of these processes requires the acquisition of a robust conditioned response as a precondition. Therefore, participants are often (routinely) excluded from analyses if they appear to not have learned (‘non-learners’) or not have been responsive to the experimental stimuli (‘non-responders’) during fear acquisition training, in which one conditional stimulus (CS+) predicts an upcoming aversive unconditioned stimulus (US) and another conditional stimulus does not (CS–) (Lonsdorf et al., 2017; Pavlov, 1927).

Critically, ‘non-learning’ is most often defined as a failure to show discrimination between the CS+ and CS– in skin conductance responses (SCRs) – the most common outcome measure in the field (Lonsdorf et al., 2017). This practice may seem trivial at first glance and has been referred to as exclusion of ‘non-learners’, ‘performance-based exclusion’ or even ‘exclusion of outliers'. Yet, defining a set of characteristics to identify individuals who ‘did not learn’ is operationalized in very heterogeneous ways across studies. The same applies to the criteria that determine what constitutes a’ non-responder’ during fear acquisition training.

In addition to the heterogeneity in operationalization, other problems of performance-based exclusion of participants are worth noting: definitions of ‘non-learners’ are typically based on SCRs only (for exceptions see Ahmed and Lovibond, 2019; Oyarzún et al., 2019) and ‘non-learners’ are typically excluded from all analyses, that is, all experimental phases and outcome measures of a study. As SCRs are not a pure measure of either learning or fear, but rather reflect arousal levels (Hamm et al., 1993) that serve as proxies for fear learning, classification into ‘learners’ and ‘non-learners’ on the basis of this single outcome measure may induce substantial sample bias. First, defining 'non-learning' on one single outcome measure, such as SCRs, ignores the fact that successful CS+/CS– differentiation may be present in other outcome measures (Hamm et al., 1993) such as fear potentiated startle (FPS) or ratings of fear and contingencies (i.e., cognitive awareness of the CS+/US contingencies). As such, ‘non-learning’ as defined on a single outcome measure such as SCRs cannot comprehensively capture ‘non-learning’. Second, the level of responding in SCRs and CS+/CS– discrimination has been shown to be associated with a vast number of individual difference factors (Lonsdorf and Merz, 2017; Boucsein et al., 2012) such as age and sex (for a discussion see Boucsein et al., 2012), ethnicity (Alexandra Kredlow et al., 2017Boucsein et al., 2012), genetic make-up (Garpenstrand et al., 2001), use of oral contraceptives (Merz et al., 2018b) or personality traits (Naveteur and Freixa I Baque, 1987). Consequently, excluding participants from an experiment as 'non-learners' may pre-select specific sub-samples and thus may thus severely hamper the generalizability and interpretation of the findings. Importantly, this practice may be a threat to and a limitation of the clinical translation of findings because it potentially leads to the selective exclusion of specific and highly relevant sub-groups. In fact, a recent meta-analysis suggests that patients suffering from anxiety disorders show overgeneralization of fear responding, which is enhanced when responding to the CS– (Duits et al., 2015), which may lead to reduced CS+/CS– discrimination if the response to the CS+ is comparable.

The concerns discussed above are merely based on theoretical considerations. Below, we aim to address the important and controversial topic of exclusion of ‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’ in human fear conditioning research empirically. We set out to provide an overview and inventory of the exclusion criteria that are currently employed in the field by means of a systematic literature search following PRISMA guidelines (Moher et al., 2009), covering a publication period of six months. Importantly, we distinguish between ‘non-learners’ (based on task performance, that is, CS+/CS– discrimination) and ‘non-responders’ (based on a lack of responsiveness) as assessed using SCRs. We expect the identified criteria for ‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’ to be characterized by noticeable heterogeneity (thus allowing for considerable researcher degrees of freedom) across studies. We thus aim to (1) raise awareness and (2) illustrate the impact of applying different exclusion criteria features (i.e., forking paths) on results and interpretation through case examples exemplified by the re-analyses of existing data sets. Finally, we aim to (3) provide methodologically informed, evidence-based recommendations for future studies with respect to defining and handling ‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’.

Results

Definition of performance-based exclusion of participants (‘non-learners’) and number of participants excluded across studies

Slightly fewer than one fourth of the records (i.e., 22%; 11 out of 50 records comprising 14 individual studies as three records reported two studies each) included in the systematic literature search employed performance-based exclusion of participants (i.e., SCR ‘non-learners’, Figure 1).

Figure 1. Flow chart illustrating the selection of records according to PRISMA guidelines (Moher et al., 2009).

Figure 1.

Note that seven records (14%) employed the definition and exclusion of both ‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’. Examples of irrelevant topics included studies that did not use fear conditioning paradigms (see https://osf.io/uxdhk/ for a documentation of excluded publications).

Strikingly, every single one of these records used an idiosyncratic definition to define ‘non-learners’, yielding a total of eleven different definitions in the short period of six months (see Appendix 1—table 1). The percentages of excluded participants varied from 2% to 74% (Figure 2A) of the respective study sample. Definitions differed in i) the experimental (sub-)phases to which they were applied (i.e., whether the full phase or only the first half, second half or even single trials were considered), ii) the number of trials that the exclusion was based on (varying between one and eight single trials), iii) the CS+/CS– discrimination cutoff applied (varying between <0 µS and <0.1 µS), and iv) the CS+ type (only non-reinforced or all CS+ trials) considered. The different forking paths and their frequency resulting from these combinations are displayed in Figure 2B.

Figure 2. Graphical illustration of the percentage of ‘non-learners’ and forking path analysis across studies.

Figure 2.

(A) Illustration of the percentage of participants excluded (‘non-learners’) based on SCR CS+/CS–discrimination scores across studies included in the systematic literature search (note that these 14 individual studies are derived from 11 different records, as three records reported two individual studies each). Please note that some studies excluded participants on the basis of ‘non-learning’ as well as ‘non-responding’ (cf. Figure 1), and hence the percentages displayed here do not necessarily map onto the percentage of total participants excluded per study. Also note that the study with the highest percentage of excluded participants (i.e., 74%) reported the percentage of excluded participants as a single value that included ‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’. This study is only included here because the largest proportion of exclusions can be expected to result from ‘non-learning’. (B) Sanky plot showing the ‘forking paths’ of performance-based exclusion of participants as 'non-learners', illustrating differences in the experimental phase, number of trials, the SCR CS+/CS– discrimination score in µS used to define a ‘non-learner’, the CS+ type considered (illustrated as the nodes in graded colors) and their combinations used to define 'non-learners' across studies. Path width was scaled in relation to frequency of the combinations. Note that for some ‘nodes’ the percentages do not add up to 100% because of rounding.

The cutoff for CS+ versus CS– discrimination used to identify a ‘non-learner’ varied between <0 µS and <0.1 µS, with most records excluding participants as ‘non-learners’ if they showed either a negative discrimination (<0 µS) and/or no discrimination (≤0 µS). These criteria apply if the SCR amplitude in response to the CS– was higher than and/or equal to the amplitude elicited by the CS+. Furthermore, most records required this criterion to be fulfilled only during the last half or the full fear acquisition training phase. Of note, the number of trials included in the same ‘phase’ category is contingent on the experimental design and hence does not represent a homogeneous category (‘last half’ may include five trials for one study comprising 10 trials in total but 10 trials for a different study employing 20 trials in total.

Applying the identified performance-based exclusion criteria to existing data: a case example

We applied the identified cutoff criteria to an existing data set (Data set 1) to exemplify the part of the sample that would be excluded when applying different cutoff criteria (shown in different colors from yellow to dark blue in Figure 3) based on the most frequently used phase restriction: the last half of fear acquisition training. CS+/CS– discrimination was calculated on the basis of raw (A) or log-transformed, range-corrected (log, rc) scores (B), because it is not usually reported which data are used to classify ‘learners’ vs. ‘non-learners’. Strikingly, the proportion of participants that are excluded is higher when CS+/CS– discrimination is calculated on the basis of raw data rather than log-transformed and range-corrected data (despite employing the same criteria) in particular for the highest ‘non-learner’ <0.01 µS cutoff (76.7% versus 52.6%, respectively) (see Figure 3—figure supplement 1 for details).

Figure 3. Density plots illustrating the frequency of CS+/CS– discrimination scores in a sample of N = 116 (Data set 1) based on the last half of the acquisition phase (including 7 CS+ and 7CS–, 100% reinforcement rate) for (A) SCR raw data and (B) logarithmized and range-corrected (rc; individual trial SCR/SCRmax_across_all_trials) SCR data (as it is typically not reported to which data exclusion criteria are applied).

Color coding (yellow to blue) illustrates which part of the sample would be excluded when applying the performance-based exclusion criteria (i.e. CS+/CS– discrimination) as identified by the systematic literature search. Panel (A) also illustrates two case examples (ID#1 and ID#2) that differ in SCR amplitudes but importantly show the same discrimination ratio between CS+ and CS– (4:1). These two case examples illustrate that high CS+/CS– discrimination cutoffs favor individuals with high SCR amplitudes to remain in the final sub-sample. Data are based on a re-analysis of an unpublished data set recorded in the fMRI environment (Klingelhöfer-Jens M., Kuhn, M. and Lonsdorf, T.B.; unpublished).

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Percentages of participants excluded (Data set 1) when employing the different CS+/CS– discrimination cutoffs (as identified by the systematic literature search and graphically shown in Figure 3B) which are illustrated as density plots in Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

Percentages are calculated on the basis of (A) raw SCR scores or (B) logarithmized and range-corrected scores in Data set 1. Note that the different groups are cumulative (i.e., the darker colored groups also comprise the lighter colored groups).

In addition, we included a case example of two hypothetical individuals that differ in raw SCR amplitudes (ID#1: low and ID#2: high), but importantly show the same discrimination ratio (4:1) between CS+ and CS–(see Figure 3A). These two case examples illustrate that high CS+/CS– discrimination cutoffs, such as excluding individuals with discrimination scores < 0.1 µS as ‘non-learners’, favor individuals with high SCR raw amplitudes.

Unsurprisingly, the exclusion group defined by a CS+/CS– discrimination cutoff <0 µS showed inverse discrimination (CS–>CS+, not significant in raw SCRs [p=0.117]; significant in log,rc SCRs [p = 0.021]). Strikingly and more importantly, most cumulative exclusion groups, as established by defining ‘non-learners’ by the CS+/CS– discrimination different cutoffs in SCRs in the literature, in fact show statistically significant CS+/CS– discrimination (see Appendix 2 for details and a brief discussion).

Note that despite the different color coding, which serves illustrative purposes only, the groups are in practice cumulative. More precisely, the groups illustrated by lighter colors are always contained in the darker colored groups when applying the respective cutoffs. For example, the group excluded when employing a cutoff of <0.1 µS (mid blue) also comprises the groups already excluded for the lower cutoffs of = 0.05 µS (light blue),<0.05 µS (turquoise), = 0 µS (light green) and <0 µS (yellow). For illustrative purposes, the different groups are treated as separate groups in this figure.

Exploratory analyses of consistency of classification (‘learners’ vs. ‘non-learners’) across outcome measures and criteria employed

The convergence of non-discrimination across different outcome measures was investigated by testing for CS+/CS– discrimination in fear ratings in individuals with different amounts of CS+/CS– discrimination in SCRs as defined by the criteria described above. In fact, individuals with non-significant and inverse CS+/CS– discrimination (i.e., ≤0 µS) in SCRs showed significant CS+/CS– discrimination in fear ratings (t31 = 9.69, pbonf_corr < 0.000000001, d = 1.71, see Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Importantly, all cumulative exclusion groups showed significant CS+/CS– discrimination in fear ratings (all p ’s< 0.002, see Appendix 3—table 1).

We also illustrate (Figure 4) that the classification as ‘learners’ and ‘non-learners’ changes if two features (CS+/CS- discrimination cutoff and full vs. last half of acquisition training phase) of the criteria are changed (as illustrated in their full variation in Figure 2B).

Figure 4. Exemplary illustration of individuals (Data set 1) that switch from being classified as ‘learners’ vs. ‘non-learners’ depending on the different CS+/CS– discrimination cutoff level (panels A–D), when calculation of CS+/CS– discrimination is based on either the full fear acquisition phase or the second half of the fear acquisition training (left and right part of each panel, respectively).

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Bar plots (mean ± SE) on which the superimposed individual data points show CS+ and CS– amplitudes (of raw SCR values) and CS+/CS– discrimination in (A) fear ratings and (B) SCRs raw values in the group of ‘non-learners’, as exemplarily defined for this example as a group consisting of individuals in the two lowest SCR CS+/CS– discrimination cutoff groups (i.e., ≤0) in Data set 1.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

This illustrates that individuals who fail to show CS+/CS– discrimination in SCRs (B) may in fact show substantial CS+/CS– discrimination (as an indicator for successful learning) in other outcome measures, as exemplarily illustrated here for fear ratings (A).

The potential sample bias with respect to individual differences induced by employing different performance-based exclusion criteria: a re-analysis of existing data and a case example

Regarding the impact of performance-based exclusion on the pre-selection for certain individual differences, Figure 5 shows that the distributions of trait anxiety were shifted to the left (i.e., towards lower scores) with higher SCR CS+/CS– discrimination cutoffs. More precisely, this means that, in this sample, highly anxious individuals display smaller CS+/CS– discrimination in SCRs, and that excluding individuals who display low discrimination scores will lead to the exclusion of anxious individuals.

Figure 5. A case example illustrating potential sample bias induced by excluding individuals on the basis of CS+/CS– discrimination scores (based on logarithmized, range-corrected (rc) SCR data).

Figure 5.

Scatterplot illustrating the association between trait anxiety (measured via the trait version of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, STAI-T) and CS+/CS– discrimination scores in a sample of N = 268 (Data set 2). Color coding (yellow to blue) illustrates which part of the sample would be excluded when applying the performance-based exclusion criteria (i.e. CS+/CS– discrimination) as identified by the systematic literature search. Note that within this sample, no individuals were identified with CS+/CS– discrimination equaling 0.05 µS. The upper panel illustrates densities for trait anxiety for the different CS+/CS–discrimination groups. The rightmost panel illustrates the density for CS+/CS– discrimination in the full sample. Data are based on a re-analysis of a data set recorded in the behavioral environment (Schiller et al., 2010). Note that despite the different color coding, which serves illustrative purposes only, the groups are in practice cumulative. More precisely, the groups illustrated by lighter colors are always contained in the darker colored groups when applying the respective cutoffs. For example, the group excluded when employing a cutoff of <0.1 µS (mid blue) also comprises the groups already excluded for the lower cutoffs of = 0.05 µS (light blue), <0.05 µS (turquoise), = 0 µS (light green) and <0 µS (yellow). For illustrative purposes, the different groups are treated as separate groups in this figure.

In fact, we observed a main effect of ‘Exclusion group’ on trait anxiety score (F[4,263] = 219.2, p<0.001, ƞP2 = 0.77). All exclusion groups (corresponding to the color coding in Figure 5) differ significantly from each other in their trait anxiety scores (all pbonf_corr ≤ 0.001), except for the group that did not show any CS+/CS– discrimination (=0 µS, light green, however n = 6 only), which showed significantly higher trait anxiety scores (mean ± SD STAI score: 43.8 ± 6.1) than the group with the largest CS+/CS– discrimination only (i.e., ≥0.1 µS, dark blue, n = 88, mean ± SD STAI score: 36.6 ± 8.5, pbonf_corr ≤ 0.001, CI [0.133 to 0.279]). Nevertheless, trait anxiety scores in this group (light green) were not significantly larger than those in the group with the negative discrimination (i.e., <0 µS, yellow, n = 89, mean ± SD STAI score: 40.5 ± 9.7, pbonf_corr = 0.10, CI [−0.004 to 0.142]), the group with a small discrimination score (i.e.,>0 µS but <0.05 µS, light blue, n = 43, mean ± SD STAI score 37.9 ± 7.9, pbonf_corr = 1.0, CI [−0.054 to 0.094]) or the group with the middle discrimination score (i.e., >0.05 µS but <0.1 µS, mid blue, n = 42, mean ± SD STAI score 38 ± 10.2, pbonf_corr = 0.11, CI [−0.005 to 0.145]).

Definition of ‘non-responders' and numberof participants excluded across studies

Thirty-two percent (i.e., 16 records) of the records in our systematic literature search included a definition and exclusion of ‘non-responders’, with percentages of participants excluded as a result of non-responding ranging between 0% and 14% (see Figure 6A). A single study (Chauret et al., 2014; Oyarzún et al., 2012) reported % ‘non-learners’ and % ‘non-responders’ as a single value (see Appendix 1—table 2). The definitions differed in: i) the stimulus type(s) used to define ‘non-responding’ (CS+ reinforced, CS+ unreinforced, all CS+s, CS–, US), ii) the SCR minimum amplitude criterion used to define a responder (varying between 0.01 µS and 0.05 µS; visual inspection), and iii) the percentage of trials for which these criteria have to be met (see Figure 6B and Appendix 1—table 2), as well as a combination thereof.

Figure 6. Graphical illustration of the percentage of ‘non-responders’ and forking path analysis across studies.

Figure 6.

(A) Illustration of the percentage of participants excluded from each study as a result of ‘ SCR non-responding’ to (i) the conditioned stimuli (i.e., CS+ and CS–), (ii) the US, (iii) the CS+ (which also comprises a study that used the CXT+, i.e. context), (iv) the CS+, CS– and US or (v) a pre-experimental test. Note that these 18 individual studies are derived from 16 different records, two of which included two different studies that used the same criteria. Note that some studies excluded participants on the basis of ‘non-learning’ as well as ‘non-responding’, and hence the percentages displayed here do not necessarily map onto the percentage of total participants excluded from each study. Also note that a single study (Schiller et al., 2018) is not included in this visualization because it reported % ‘non-learners’ and % ‘non-responders’ as a single value. This value has been included in the visualization of ‘non-learners’ (Figure 2) as these are expected to represent the largest proportion. (B) Sanky plot illustrating the stimulus type (pre-experiment refers to determination of 'responding' in an unrelated phase prior to the experiment), the minimally required response amplitude in µS (note that ‘visual’ refers to visual inspection of the data without a clear-cut amplitude cutoff, NA refers to no criterion applied) illustrated as the nodes in graded colors and their combinations that lead to classification as a ‘non-responder’. Path width was scaled in relation to frequency of the combinations. Note that for some ‘nodes’ the percentages do not add up to 100% because of rounding.

‘Non-responding’ was most commonly defined as not showing a sufficient number of responses to the conditioned stimuli (CS+ and CS–), less frequently by the absence of responses to the US or any stimulus (CS+, CS- or US), and in two cases by the absence of responses to the CS+ or context (CXT+) specifically (see Figure 6B). Not surprisingly, the percentage of excluded participants differed substantially depending on the stimulus type used to define ‘non-responding’ (CS based, 0–10%; CS+/CXT+ based, 10–11%; US based, 0–4%; CS and US based, 11–14%; pre-experimental test based, 5%; Figure 6A).

Despite these differences in the stimulus types used to define ‘non-responding’ in the first place, studies differed widely in the amplitude cutoff criterion to be exceeded in order to qualify as a response (see Figure 6B) as well as in the percentage of trials in which this cutoff had to be met (see Appendix 1—table 2).

The question of what (physiological) ‘non-responders’ during fear acquisition training are and how to identify them might be elucidated by investigating the number of ‘non-responses’ across trial types (CS and US) across data sets, and whether ‘non-responding’ to the US predicts ‘non-responding’ to the CS or vice versa. As expected from Figure 6A, the number of ‘non-responses’ to the US was low (as was also the case in our data [10%, Data set 1]), while the number of ‘non-responses’ to the CS (48.29%) was substantially higher – in particular for the CS– (58.6%; CS+ ‘non-responses’: 37.9%, see Figure 7A). This pattern, exemplarily illustrated here in one data set is representative of a larger number of data sets (see Appendix 4, table 1 for details). Furthermore, in our data (Data set 1), all individuals that did not react to the US in more than two thirds of the US trials also showed no responses to the CS (n = 3 of N = 119). To summarize, this provides the first evidence that ‘non-responding’ to the US may predict ‘non-responding’ to the CS but not vice versa. Furthermore, our data also suggest a positive correlation between the number of ‘non-responses’ to the US and the number of ‘non-responses’ to the CS (see Figure 7B for statistics).

Figure 7. Percentage of no-responses across stimuli and correlation between CS and US non-responses.

Figure 7.

(A) Bar plot displaying the number of ‘non-responses’ to the CS+, CS–, across both CS and to the US across all participants in Data set 1 (see Appendix 4—table 1 for percentages across different data sets). (B) Scatterplot illustrating the number of ‘non-responses’ (i.e., zero-responses, here defined by an amplitude <0.01 µS) to the US presentations (total of 14 presentations) and the CS+ (red) and CS– (blue) responses (14 presentations each) for each participant in Data set 1. For completeness sake, ‘non-responses’ across CS types are illustrated in gray (CS+ and CS– combined, total of 28 presentations). Lines illustrate the Spearman correlation (rs) between ‘non-responses’ to the US and ‘non-responses’ to the CS+, CS– and both CS, with corresponding correlation coefficients (font color corresponds to CS type) included in the figure.

Discussion

In this article, we showed that participant exclusion in fear conditioning research is common (i.e., 40% of records included) and characterized by substantial operationalizational heterogeneity of definitions for ‘non-learners’ and (physiological) ‘non-responders’. Furthermore, we provide case-examples that illustrate: i) the futility of some definitions of ‘non-learners’ (i.e., when those classified as ‘non-learners’ in fact show significant discrimination on both ratings and SCRs as illustrated in Appendix 3 and Appendix 2, respectively) when applied to our data; and ii) the potential sample bias induced by excluding ‘non-learners’ with respect to individual differences. Furthermore, we provide an overview of SCR ‘non-responses’ to different stimulus types (CS+, CS– and US) across different data sets (see Appendix 4—tables 1 and 2) as a guide for developing evidence-based criteria to define ‘non-responders’. Together, we believe that this work contributes to: i) raising awareness of some of the problems associated with performance-based exclusion of participants (‘non-learners’) and of how this exclusion is implemented, ii) facilitating decision-making on which criteria to employ and not to employ, iii) enhancing transparency and clarity in future publications, and thereby iv) fostering reproducibility and robustness as well as clinical translation in the field of fear conditioning research and beyond.

‘Non-learners’: conclusions, caveats and considerations

Operationalizational heterogeneity is illustrated by every single record in our systematic literature search (covering a six months period) that employed definitions of ‘non-learners’ using a set of idiosyncratic criteria. The true number of definitions in the field applied over decades will be even substantially larger. In the records included here, 6–52% of participants were excluded (disregarding one study reporting percentages of ‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’ together with 74%; cf. Figure 2A), which substantially exceed the percentages recently put forward for ‘non-learning’ exclusions (Marin et al., 2019) that were suggested to lie between 4% (Chauret et al., 2014) and 19% (Oyarzún et al., 2012).

If several thousand analytical pipelines can be applied, the likelihood of false positives is high (Munafò et al., 2017) and the temptation of their opportunistic (ab)use must be considered a threat. Hence, a constructive discussion on where to go from here and how to not get lost in the garden of forking paths is important. This being said, we do acknowledge that certain research questions or the use of different recording equipment (robust lab equipment vs. novel mobile devices such as smartwatches) may potentially require distinct data-processing pipelines and the exclusion of certain observations (Silberzahn et al., 2018; Simmons et al., 2011), and hence it is not desirable to propose rigid and fixed rules for generic adoption. Procedural differences, in particular the inclusion of outcome measures that require certain triggers to elicit a response (such as startle responses or ratings) have also been shown to impact on the learning process itself (Sjouwerman et al., 2016). Rather, we call for a reconsideration of methods in the field and want to raise awareness to the pitfalls of adopting exclusion criteria from previously published work without critical evaluation of whether these apply meaningfully to one’s own research. Furthermore, we want to promote the adoption of transparent reporting of data processing, recording and analyses and strive to suggest standards in the field to reduce heterogeneity based on idiosyncratic customs rather than methodological and theoretical considerations (see Box 1).

Box 1.

List of reporting details, potential difficulties and recommendations when excluding ‘non-learners’ (performance-based exclusion) and/or ‘non-responders’ with a focus on SCRs.

Please note that this Box can be annotated online.

(A) General reporting details
What to report? Why is this considered important? What can go wrong or be ambiguous? Recommendations on how to proceed
Details on data recording and response quantification pipeline • because differences in data recording and quantification (i.e., response scoring) can make a substantial difference • report recording equipment and all settings used (e.g., filter)
• report software used for response quantification
• report precise details of response quantification
Minimal response criterion (μS) to define a valid SCR • to define valid responses • minimally detectable amplitude (e.g., 0.01, 0.02, 0.03, 0.05 µS, etc.) may be sample- and equipment-specific
• no clear recommendations (existing guidelines provide a range of 0.01 to 0.05 µS) because this is influenced by noise level and equipment
• test different minimal response criteria in the data set and define the cutoff empirically. In our experience (Data set 1), a cutoff was easily determined empirically by visually inspecting responses at different cutoffs (e.g., <0.01 µS, between 0.01 µS and 0.02 µS) and by evaluating their discrimination from noise
Whether the first CS+ and/or the first CS– trial is included or not, and information on trial sequence • no learning can be evident in the first trial, as the first US may occur at the earliest at the end of the CS+ and hence after the scoring window for the CS+-induced SCR
• if the first trial is a CS–, no learning can have taken place as the US has not been presented yet
• inclusion of the first trial (or the first trials in partial reinforcement protocols) may thus artificially reduce CS+/CS– discrimination
• in fully randomized partial reinforcement protocols, US presentations may cluster in the first or last half of the acquisition training, which will impact on CS+/CS–discrimination in SCRs • careful experimental design with respect to trial-sequences (in particular in partial reinforcement protocols)
• report whether the first trial for both CS+ and CS– is excluded because it may induce noise and bias CS+/CS– discrimination towards non-discrimination and as the first trial is sensitive to trial sequence effects
Precise number of trials considered (if applicable for each trial type including reinforced and non-reinforced CS+ trials in case of partial reinforcement) • often difficult/ambiguous to infer this information from the 'Materials and methods' section of a reporta
• number of trials that the ‘last half’ or ‘full phase’ refers to is contingent on experimental design and hence ambiguous and imprecise (see Figure 2B)
• precision in reporting rather than relying on the reader making the right inferences
• specify clearly the number of trials per stimulus type that are comprised in the ‘last half’ or ‘full phase’
• provide a justification (theoretical and/or empirical) for this decisionb
Details of whether results were based on raw or transformed data • typically, transformations are required to allow interpretation of the reported results and to meet the assumptions of commonly statistical models • report details of transformation (e.g., logarithmized [log/LN], range-corrected, square-root) including the number of trials considered (for each stimulus type) and the sequence of transformations applied and specific formula (e.g., for range-correction)
• provide justification for any applied transformation (e.g., violation of assumption of normal distribution of residuals)
Precise number of excluded participants and specific reasons • often difficult/ambiguous to infer this information from the 'Materials and methods' section of a reporta • different researchers have different opinions on what ‘exclusion’ is (e.g., having individuals discontinue after a first experimental day based on performance should be considered and reported as exclusion) • report a breakdown of specific reasons for exclusions with respective n’s
(B) Specific reporting details for exclusion of ‘non-learners’
What to report? Why is this considered important? What can go wrong or be ambiguous? Recommendations on how to proceed
CS+/CS– discrimination is calculated on the basis of raw SCR or transformed (e.g., logarithmized [log/LN], range-corrected, square-root) scores • the same criteria lead to different proportions of excluded individuals when applying them to raw or transformed data (see Figure 3A and B) • exact details of transformations (optimally calculation formulas) need to be included for full transparency and reproducibility
Minimal differential (CS+ vs. CS) cutoff for ‘non-learning’ in μS • different cutoffs lead to very different proportions of individuals excluded (see Figure 3) • exact details on cutoffs need to be included for full transparency and reproducibility
On what outcome measures is ‘non-learning’ determined? • ‘non-learners’ do not necessarily converge across different outcome measures (Appendix 3, Figure 4—figure supplement 1) • all outcome measures recorded need to be reported • ‘non-learning’ should not be based on a single outcome measure or a clear justification needs to be provided as to why a single measure is considered meaningful
If ‘non-learning’ is determined by responding during fear acquisition training, which trial types and number of trials per trial type were considered? • depending on the criteria employed, the same individual may be classified as ‘learner’ or ‘non-learner’ (see Figure 4) • classification as ‘non-learner’ should be based on differential scores (CS+ vs. CS–), and the number of trials included for this calculation should be clearly justified. Providing a generally valid recommendation regarding the number of trials to be included is difficult because it critically depends on experimental design choices
If ‘non-learning ‘criteria are used, do they differ from criteria that the researcher or the research group used in previous publications? If yes, why were the criteria changed? • provide explicit justifications on why different criteria were used previously and presently • report differences between present and previous criteria used including references and justifications
Did ‘non-learners’ really fail to learn? • important as a manipulation check but note that the absence of a statistically significant CS+/CS– discrimination effect in a group on average cannot be taken to imply that all individuals in this group do not show meaningful CS+/CS–discrimination • individuals classified as ‘non-learners’ may in fact show significant CS+/CS– discrimination in SCRs (see Appendix 2) or in other outcome measures (see Figure 3—figure supplement 1 and Appendix 4) and hence fail the manipulation check • do the groups classified as ‘non-learners’ and ‘learners’ differ significantly in discrimination, and do ‘non-learners’ really not discriminate in SCRs and other outcome measures? Report the data on this group graphically and/or statistically in the supplementary material (do not report the full sample with and without exclusions only)
Are results contingent on the exclusion of ‘non-learners’? • important to allow for transparency and to evaluate the impact of the results • it is not clearly defined when results differ meaningfully when excluding and including ‘non-learners’ • provide results with and without exclusion of ‘non-learners’
• additional analyses can be provided as supplementary material. When results are not contingent on the exclusion of ‘non-learners’, it is sufficient to mention this briefly in the results of the main manuscript (e.g., results are not contingent on the exclusion of ‘non-learners’)
• if the results of the main analyses and hence the main conclusions change when ‘non-learners’ are excluded, this needs to be included in the main manuscript , and the implications need to be adequately discussed. Please note that this does not necessarily invalidate findings but can refine them
Descriptive statistics for excluded ‘non-learners’ • important to allow for transparency and evaluation of the potential sample biases introduced • report sex, age, anxiety levels, awareness
(C) Specific reporting details for exclusions of ‘non-responders’
What to report? Why is this considered important? What can go wrong or be ambiguous? Recommendations on how to proceed
Whether ‘non-responses’ are calculated on the basis of raw SCR or transformed (e.g., logarithmized [log/LN], range-corrected, square-root) scores • the same criteria lead to different proportions of excluded individuals when applying to raw or transformed data (see Figure 3A and B) • exact details of transformations (optimally calculation formulas) need to be included for full transparency and reproducibility
Minimal cutoff for ‘non-responses’ in μS • it is often difficult/ambiguous to infer this information from the 'Materials and methods' section of a reporta
• higher cutoffs could unnecessarily reduce the sample size
• exact details on cutoffs need to be included for full transparency and reproducibility
Was ‘non-responding’ determined in a pre-experimental phase such as forced-breathing or US calibration? • determining ‘non-responding’ during a pre-experimental phase may help to detect malfunctioning of the equipment and allow this to be corrected prior to data acquisition
• classification of ‘non-responders’ independent of the experimental task and its specifications (e.g., number of US presentations)
• electrodes may detach between the pre-experimental phase and fear acquisition training • report details of pre-experimental phase
• classification in SCR ‘non-responders’ should be based on a pre-experimental phase if no US presentations occur during the experiment, such as in case of threat of shock experiments, observational conditioning, extinction or return of fear tests
If ‘non-responding’ is determined by responding during fear acquisition training, what trial types are considered? • frequency of ‘non-responding’ differs substantially between different stimuli (CS and US) but also between CS+ and CS– (see Figure 7A) • ‘non-responding’ to the US may be due to technical failure (i.e., no US was administered) • classification in SCR ‘non-responders’ should not be based on SCRS elicited by CS (CS+, CS– or both), but should be based on US responding
• a question on the estimated number of US presented during fear acquisition training (and all other phases) may serve as a manipulation check
Descriptive statistics for excluded ‘non-responder’ • important to allow for transparency and evaluation of the potential sample biases introduced • report sex, age, anxiety levels, awareness

a based on our experience with extracting this information from literature identified in the systematic literature search reported in this manuscript.

‘others have done this previously’ is not an acceptable justification in our point of view.

Yet, there are many other critical considerations worth discussing beyond the heterogeneous criteria used to define ‘non-learners’ and their impact on the outcome of statistical tests:

First, ‘performance-based exclusion of participants’ is often based on a single outcome measure (typically SCRs), despite multiple measures being recorded (for exceptions see Ahmed and Lovibond, 2019; Belleau et al., 2018; Oyarzún et al., 2012). Importantly, ‘fear learning’ cannot be reliably inferred by means of SCRs, because SCRs capture arousal-related processes and can only be used as a proxy to infer ‘fear learning’ as fear is closely linked to arousal (Hamm and Weike, 2005). Relatedly, the fact that physiological proxies of ‘fear’ do not map onto ‘fear’ itself has been discussed extensively (LeDoux, 2012; LeDoux, 2014).

Second, but related, individuals that fail to show CS+/CS– discrimination in SCRs may show substantial discrimination, as an indicator of successful learning, in other outcome measures such as ratings of fear, US expectancy or fear potentiated startle (Hamm and Weike, 2005; Marin et al., 2019), as illustrated here for fear ratings (see Figure 4—figure supplement 1 and Appendix 3—table 1).

Third, a common justification for excluding ‘non-learners’ is that it is not possible to investigate extinction- or return-of-fear-related phenomena in individuals who ‘did not learn’. To our knowledge, there is some evidence (Craske et al., 2008; Plendl and Wotjak, 2010; Prenoveau et al., 2013) that this theoretical assumption does not necessarily hold true, (i.e., CS+/CS– discrimination during fear acquisition training does not necessarily predict CS+/CS– discrimination during other experimental phases) (Gerlicher et al., 2019). An empirical investigation of this, however, would go beyond this manuscript’s scope.

Fourth, we provided empirical evidence that those classified as a group of ‘non-learners’ in SCRs in the literature (sometimes referred to as ‘outliers’) on the basis of the identified definitions in fact displayed significant CS+/CS– discrimination when applied to our own data. An exception to this was using cut offs in differential responding of <0.05 µS (note, however, that a non-significant CS+/CS– discrimination effect in the group of ‘non-learners’ as a whole cannot be taken as evidence that all individuals in this group do not in fact display meaningful or statistically significant CS+/CS– discrimination). Hence, in addition to the many conceptual problems we raised here, the operationalization of ‘non-learning’ in the field failed its critical manipulation check given that those classified as ‘non-learners’ show clear evidence of learning as a group (i.e., CS+/CS– discrimination, see Appendix 2—table 1).

Fifth, we illustrate a concerning sample bias that is introduced by performance-based participant exclusion. CS+/CS– discrimination in SCRs during fear acquisition training has been linked to a number of individual difference factors (Lonsdorf and Merz, 2017) and, naturally, selecting participants on the basis of SCR CS+/CS– discrimination will also select them on the basis of these individual differences (illustrated by our case example on trait anxiety, Figure 5). In our case example, we illustrate that excluding ‘non-learners’ biases the sample towards low anxiety scores, which hampers the generalizability and replicability of findings: i) the effect may only exist in low-anxiety individuals but not in the general population, and ii) as fear acquisition is a clinically relevant paradigm, pre-selection in favor of low-anxiety individuals might represent a threat to the clinical translation of the findings. Many studies in the field of fear conditioning research aim to develop behavioral or pharmacological manipulations to enhance treatment effects or aim to study mechanisms that are relevant for clinical fear and anxiety. Hence, it is highly problematic that these studies may exclude individuals who show response patterns that mimic responses typically observed in anxiety patients when excluding ‘non-learners’. In fact, patients suffering from anxiety disorders have been shown to be characterized by generalization of fear from the CS+ to the CS– (Duits et al., 2015).

Sixth, as illustrated by our case example (Figure 3), high CS+/CS– discrimination cutoffs generally favor individuals with high SCR amplitudes despite potentially identical ratios between CS+ and CS– amplitudes, which may introduce a sampling bias for individuals characterized by high arousal levels that probably have biological underpinnings. Relatedly, future studies need to empirically address which criteria for SCR transformation and exclusions are more or less sensitive to baseline differences (for an example from startle responding see Bradford et al., 2015; Grillon and Baas, 2002).

In summary, in light of the many (potential) problems associated with performance-based exclusion of participants, we forcefully echo Marin et al.’s conclusion that one needs "to be cautious when excluding SCR non-learners and to consider the potential implications of such exclusion when interpreting the findings from studies of conditioned fear" (Marin et al., 2019, abstract). Routinely, excluding participants who are intentionally or unintentionally characterized by specific individual differences represents a major threat to generalizability, replicability and potentially clinical translation of findings, as results might be contingent on a specific sub-sample and specific sample characteristics. This is also true when researchers are interested in the study of general processes. Furthermore, by excluding these individuals from further analyses, we may miss the opportunity to understand why some individuals do not show discrimination between the CS+ and the CS– in SCRs (or other outcome measures) or whether this lack of discrimination is maintained across subsequent experimental phases. It can be speculated that this lack of discrimination may carry meaningful information – at least for a subsample.

‘Non-responders’: conclusions, caveats and considerations

In addition to ‘non-learners’, ‘non-responders’ are also often excluded during fear conditioning research. We showed that the definition of ‘non-responders’, like that of ‘non-learners’, varies widely across studies. Heterogeneity in definitions manifests in different cutoff criteria for what is considered a valid response, the number of trials and the stimulus type(s) considered (Appendix 1—table 2, Figure 6). Surprisingly, most definitions are based on CS responses (i.e., SCRs to the CS+ and/or CS–) and only few are based on US responses. This highlights a potentially problematic overlap between ‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’: ‘non-responding’ to the CS (i.e., CS+ and CS– or CS+ only) is not necessarily indicative of physiological ‘non-responding’ – especially if high cutoffs are used. In fact, ‘non-responding’ to the CS may, or at least in some cases, reflect the absence of learning-based patterns in physiological responding – which may carry important information. Having observed the striking differences in percentages of ‘non-responses’ to the US (10%) and CS (48%) observed in our data (see Figure 7 and Appendix 4—table 1), we suggest that physiological ‘non-responding’ cannot and should not be determined on the basis of the absence of responding to the CS.

More globally, the group of ‘non-responders’, as defined by the criteria identified here, probably lumps together several sub-groups: individuals (1) for whom technical problems resulted in no valid SCRs, (2) who fell asleep or did not pay attention, (3) who cognitively learned the CS+/US contingencies but did not express the expected corresponding responses in SCRs, and (4) who were attentive to the experiment but did not learn the contingencies (i.e., unaware participants) and hence did not show the expected SCR patterns (Tabbert et al., 2011).

In summary, although excluding physiological ‘non-responders’ makes sense (in terms of a manipulation check and independent of the hypothesis), we consider defining ‘non-responders’ on the basis of the absence of SCRs to the CS as problematic (dependent on the hypothesis). We suggest that physiological SCR ‘non-responders’ should be defined on the basis of US responses during fear acquisition training or to strong stimuli during pre-conditioning phases such as US calibration, startle habituation or forced breathing (reliably eliciting strong SCRs). If ‘non-responding’ to the US (during fear acquisition training) is used, it is difficult to suggest a universally valid cutoff with respect to the number or percentage of required valid US responses, because this critically depends on a number of variables such as hardware and sampling rate used. It remains an open question for future work whether data quality of novel mobile devices (e.g., smartwatches) for the acquisition of SCRs differs from traditional, robust lab-based recordings and how this would impact on the frequency of exclusions based on SCRs. Appendix 4 suggests that the cutoff may typically range between 1/3 and 2/3 of valid responses but may be data-set specific. US-based criteria are of course not trivial in multiple-day experiments, in which certain experimental days do not involve the presentation of US or involve few temporally clustered US presentations (i.e., reinstatement), or in paradigms not involving direct exposure to the US (i.e., observational or instructional learning; Haaker et al., 2017). In these cases, the other options listed above are strongly preferred to CS based criteria.

Where do we go from here?

In this work, we have comprehensively illustrated and argued that most of the current definitions employed to define ‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’ have to be considered as theoretically and empirically problematic. It is not sufficient, however, to raise awareness to these problems and the practical question of ‘Where do we go from here?’ remains to be addressed. What can we do to avoid getting lost in the garden of forking paths of exclusion criteria? Here, we would like to offer several solutions to improve practices in the field, which we expect to foster robustness, replicability and potentially clinical translation of findings: (1) transparency in reporting, (2) adopting open science practices, (3) increasing the level and quality of reporting and (4) graphical data presentation, (5) manipulation checks, and (6) fostering critical evaluation. We refer to see Box 1 for specific recommendations.

More precisely, transparency can be enhanced ‘if observations are eliminated, authors must also report what the statistical results are if those observations are included’, as suggested by Simmons and colleagues, nearly a decade ago (Simmons et al., 2011, Table 2). Here, we echo this call that this recommendation should be implemented routinely in data reporting pipelines when employing performance-based participant exclusions (‘non-learners’) in fear conditioning research. We also call for a transparent and adequate reporting in the results (in brief) and discussion section rather than providing this information exclusively in the appendix. This being said, it is important to point out that should a finding turn out to be contingent on the exclusion of ‘non-learners’, this does not necessarily invalidate this finding. On the contrary, it may further specify the finding or hint to possible mechanisms and/or boundary conditions – yet inferences on boundary conditions should be made carefully (Hardwicke and Shanks, 2016). Relatedly, adopting an open science culture will facilitate transparent reporting of exclusion criteria (Nosek et al., 2015) and will minimize the risk of exploiting heterogeneous definitions in the field. Registered reports (Hardwicke and Ioannidis, 2018), publicly available data including those from excluded participants and pre-registration (Munafò et al., 2017) of definitions and analysis pipelines (Ioannidis, 2014), as well as openly accessible lab-specific standard operational protocols (SOPs), may also be helpful.

We acknowledge, however, that transparent reporting and particularly pre-registration of exclusion criteria is not trivial in light of the unsatisfactory quality and level of detail in reporting in the field of fear conditioning research. It was striking that the compilation of exclusion criteria (‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’, see Appendix 1—tables 1 and 2) employed in the records included in our systematic literature search required extensive personal exchange with the authors because the definitions provided were often insufficient, ambiguous or incorrect. It is our responsibility as authors, reviewers and editors to improve these reporting standards to an acceptable level. As a guidance, Box 1 provides a compilation of reporting details that we consider important to include in both pre-registered protocols and publications (an editable online version of Box 1 is available to allow for further development, see Box caption).

Our recommendations to improve the level of reporting details and transparency extends to the graphical illustration of results, which should optimally allow for a complete presentation of data (Weissgerber et al., 2015) without risking obscuring important patterns, providing detailed distributional information rather than merely presenting summary statistics (see Weissgerber et al., 2015 for a discussion). Such visualization options include, for instance, scatterplots, box plots, histograms, violin plots as well as their combination (see also Figure 5) in so called ‘rain cloud plots’ (see Allen et al., 2018 for a tutorial in R, Matlab and Phyton) and utilizing colors or color gradients to visualize different groups of individuals (for instance ‘learners’ and ‘non-learners’) or discrimination scores. This will provide readers with the opportunity to evaluate the presented results and conclusions independently and comprehensively.

Finally, if criteria for ‘non-learners’ or ‘non-responders’ are employed to exclude participants from data analyses (or continuation of the experiment), we recommend that a sanity or manipulation check should be performed to determine whether – for instance - ‘non-learners’ really did not learn (i.e., really do not show significant CS+/CS– discrimination). We have empirically illustrated that most definitions of ‘non-learners’ fail this manipulation check (Appendix 2—table 1). Yet, it may not be feasible in all cases to determine such statistics, as these may not be appropriate for small samples and correspondingly small sub-groups of ‘non-learners’. Relatedly, we urge authors to justify adequately all details of the exclusion criteria (if applied) – both theoretically and practically. Furthermore, we encourage authors, reviewers and editors alike to critically evaluate whether exclusions and applied criteria are warranted in the first place and appropriate in the specific context (vs. mere adopting published or previously employed criteria) and whether these exclusion criteria are transparently reported and discussed if results hinge on them (Steegen et al., 2016).

Furthermore, future work should empirically address the question of how to best define ‘non-learning’ in particular in light of different outcome measures in fear conditioning studies, which capture different aspects of defensive responding (Jentsch et al., 2020; Lonsdorf et al., 2017).

Final remarks

In closing, the field of fear conditioning has been plagued with a lack of consensus on how to define and treat ‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’, which not seldomly impacts review processes and generates unnecessary lengthy discussions for editors, reviewers and authors. We argue that it is neither ethical (due to an excessive waste of tax money and human resources) nor scientifically meaningful to exclude up to two thirds of a sample. If only one third of the population performs ‘as expected’ in the experiment, experimental designs, data recording and processing techniques as well as definitions need to be reconsidered. We have shown that findings derived from such highly selective sub-samples may not generalize to other samples or to the general population, and as a consequence might be a threat to clinical translation. Most problematically, however, findings derived from such highly selective samples have been routinely and invariantly generalized to reflect ‘general principles’ and ‘processes’ in the past. Not surprisingly, such findings have also suffered replication failures. As such, exclusions of ‘non-learners’ can in fact be dangerous if not handled transparently (as suggested above), because they may bias and confuse a whole research field and may push research along a misleading path. Thus, we suggest recommendations and consensus suggestions, and recommend that common practices should be critically evaluated before we adopt them in future work, so that the field follows a path towards more robust and replicable research findings.

Materials and methods

This project has been pre-registered on the Open Science Framework (OSF) (Lonsdorf et al., 2019, March 22; retrieved from https://osf.io/vjse4).

A systematic literature search was performed according to PRISMA guidelines (Moher et al., 2009) covering all publications (including e-pubs ahead of print) in PubMed during the six months prior to the 22nd March 2019, using the following search terms: threat conditioning OR fear conditioning OR threat acquisition OR fear acquisition OR threat learning OR fear learning OR threat memory OR fear memory OR return of fear OR threat extinction OR fear extinction. In case of author corrections, we included the original study that the correction referred to unless this study itself was already included on the basis of the publication date.

From the identified 854 records listed in PubMed, 152 were included in stage 2 screening (abstract) and 86 were retained for stage 3 screening (full text). Finally, 50 records were included (see Figure 1 for details) that reported results for (1) SCRs as an outcome measure from (2) the fear acquisition training phase (3) in human participants.

Extraction of criteria for ‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’

The 50 records were screened in-depth and information derived from each record was entered into a template file agreed on by the authors prior to literature screening (available from the OSF pre-registration https://osf.io/vjse4). We distinguished between ‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’. We considered an exclusion to be an exclusion of ‘non-learners’ if it was based on the key task performance –that is, CS+/CS– discrimination in SCRs. Exclusions were considered as exclusion of ‘non-responders’ when based on general (physiological) responding (i.e., not based on CS+/CS– discrimination). Participants who were explicitly excluded because of clear-cut and well-described technical problems, such as abortion of data recording or electrode disattachment, were not included in any definition. Criteria for defining ‘non-learners’ (see Appendix 1—table 1) and ‘non-responders’ (see Appendix 1—table 2) were extracted if applicable for the respective study. In case information in the publication was insufficient or ambiguous, the corresponding authors were contacted and asked for clarification.

Re-analysis of existing data applying the identified exclusion criteria

One aim of this work was to illustrate empirically the impact of different exclusion criteria on the study outcome and interpretation. To achieve this aim, we initially planned to re-analyze existing data sets and to exclude participants on the basis of the identified definitions, which was expected to demonstrate that results are not robust across the various definitions of ‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’ employed. More precisely, we planned to calculate CS+/CS– discrimination across different data sets for all definitions identified by the systematic literature search and to generate corresponding correlation matrices as well as the percentages of zero and non-responses (see pre-registration: https://osf.io/vjse4). Because the exclusion criteria identified through the systematic literature search were even more heterogeneous than expected, and as it was difficult to agree on a key outcome to quantify the impact of exclusion criteria, we eventually concluded that such extensive re-analyses would not add much to the tabular and graphical illustration of this heterogeneity. Instead, we provide illustrative case examples for: (i) the proportion of individuals excluded on the basis of the identified exclusion criteria for ‘non-learners’ (Figure 3) and (ii) the potential sample bias with respect to individual differences (exploratory aim) induced by employing different exclusion criteria features (i.e., discrimination cutoff; Figure 5). As planned, (iii) we provide the percentage of non-responses to the CS+, CS–, CS+ and CS– combined, and the US across different studies, as well as empirical information on the association between CS and US based non-responding as a base to guide empirical recommendations.

Data processing, statistical analyses and figures were generated with R version 3.6.0 (2019-04-26) using the following packages: cowplot, dplyr, ggplot2 (Wickham, 2009), ggrigdes, car, ez, lsr, psychReport, lubridate, RColorBrewer and flipPlot packages. Sanky plots were generated with help of https://app.displayr.com.

Data sets

Data set 1

Data set 1 is part of the baseline measurement of an ongoing longitudinal fear conditioning study. Here, fear ratings and SCR data from the first test-timepoint (T0) were included (N = 119, 79 females, mean ± SD age of 25 ± 4 years) whereas fMRI data were not used. All participants gave written informed consent to the protocol which was approved by the local ethics committee (PV 5157, Ethics Committee of the General Medical Council Hamburg).

Data set 1 is employed to illustrate a case example for the proportion of participants excluded when employing different CS+/CS– discrimination cutoffs (‘non-learners’, Figure 3) as well as the number of zero-responses across different stimulus types (‘non-responders’) and their association (Figure 7). Furthermore, we aimed to test exploratively whether even in groups defined as ‘non-learners’ a significant CS+/CS– discrimination on SCR and fear ratings can be detected (all results presented in the Appendix are based on Data set 1).

Paradigm and stimuli

The two-day paradigm consisted of habituation and acquisition training (day 1) and extinction training and recall testing (day 2) without any contingency instructions provided. Here, only data from the acquisition training phase (100% reinforcement rate) were used. CS were two light grey fractals, presented 14 times each in a pseudo-randomized order for 6–8 s (mean: 7 s). Visual stimuli were identical for all participants, but allocation to CS+ and CS– was counterbalanced between participants. During inter-trial intervals (ITIs), a white fixation cross was shown for 10–16 s (mean: 13 s). All stimuli were presented on a light gray background and controlled by Presentation software (Version 14.8, Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc, Albany California, USA).

The electrotactile stimulus, serving as US, consisted of three 10 ms electrotactile rectangular pulses with an interpulse interval of 50 ms (onset: 200 ms before CS+ offset) and was administered to the back of the right hand of the participants. It was generated by a Digitimer DS7A constant current stimulator (Welwyn Garden City, Hertfordshire, UK) and delivered through a 1 cm diameter platinum pin surface electrode (Speciality Developments, Bexley, UK). The electrode was attached between the metacarpal bones of the index and middle finger. US intensity was individually calibrated in a standardized step-wise procedure aiming at an unpleasant, but still tolerable level.

SCRs

SCRs were semi-manually scored by using a custom-made computer program (EDA View) as the first response from trough to peak 0.9–3.5 s after CS onset (0.9–2.5 s after US onset) as recommended (Boucsein et al., 2012; Sjouwerman and Lonsdorf, 2019). The maximum rise time was set to 5 s. Data were down-sampled to 10 Hz. Each scored SCR was checked visually, and the scoring suggested by EDA View was corrected if necessary (e.g., the foot or trough was misclassified by the algorithm). Data with recording artifacts or excessive baseline activity (i.e., more than half of the response amplitudes) were treated as missing data points and excluded from the analyses. SCRs below 0.01 μS or the absence of any SCR within the defined time window were classified as non-responses and set to 0. The threshold of 0.01 μS for this data set was determined empirically by visually inspecting response specifically above and below this cutoff, which suggested that in this data set, responses > 0.01 μS can be reliably identified. ‘Non-responders’ (N = 3) were defined as individuals who showed more than two thirds of non-responses to the US (10 or more non-responses out of 14 US trials, see Appendix 4—table 2). Three individuals were classified as ‘non-responders’ and these individuals did not show any responses to the CS either. The three participants classified as ‘non-responders’ (see above) were only excluded for the analyses of ‘non-learners’. Raw SCR amplitudes were normalized by taking the natural logarithm and range-corrected by dividing each logarithmized SCR by the maximum amplitude (maximum SCR to a CS or a US) per participant and day.

Fear ratings

Fear ratings were provided by participants through ratings on a visual analog scale (VAS) on the screen asking ‘how much stress, fear, and tension’ they experienced when they last saw the CS+ and CS–. The fear ratings used for the purpose of this manuscript are those obtained after fear acquisition training (no ratings were acquired during this phase). Answers were given within 5 s on the VAS, which ranged from 0 (answer = none) to 25 (answer = maximum) by using a button box. Pressing the buttons moved a bar on the VAS to the aimed value and answers were logged in by pressing another button. Non-registered ratings were considered as missing values (8.4%).

Statistical analysis

To test whether exclusion groups differ in CS+/CS– discrimination, a mixed ANOVA with CS+/CS– discrimination in SCR or fear ratings as the dependent variable and the between-subjects factor ‘Exclusion group’ and the within-subject factor ‘CS-type’ was performed. Note that it is circular to test for differences in SCR CS+/CS– discrimination between groups that were selected on the basis of different SCR CS+/CS– discrimination cutoffs in the first place. Still, it is relevant to test whether all groups classified as ‘non-learners’ in the literature do indeed fail to show evidence of learning, which would be indicated by a lack of significant CS+/CS– discrimination in SCRs in this case. In essence, this is a test to evaluate whether the exclusion criteria used in the literature do indeed achieve what they purport to do, that is, classify a group of participants that do not show evidence of learning. To test whether these exclusion groups discriminated in SCRs and fear ratings, exclusion groups were cumulated, and t-tests were performed for each cumulative group (see Appendices 2 and 3, respectively). We acknowledge, however, that the absence of a statistically significant CS+/CS– discrimination effect in a group on average cannot be taken to imply that all individuals in this group do not show meaningful CS+/CS– discrimination. As such, this is a rather conservative test. To correct for multiple testing, all p-values deriving from t-tests were adjusted using the Bonferroni procedure. As effect size, Cohen’s d was reported for t-tests and partial eta-squared for ANOVAs. To illustrate the association between the non-responses to the US and the non-responses to the CS, a Spearman rank correlation test was computed.

Data set 2

For the purpose of this manuscript, a final sample of 268 individuals (195 female, mean ± SD age of 25 ± 4 years) was re-analyzed. This sample is reported in a recent pre-print (Sjouwerman et al., 2018) in which we observed an association between trait anxiety and CS+/CS– discrimination in SCRs. Here, the re-analysis and graphical illustration of these data serve the purpose of a case example to illustrate the potential sample bias that may be induced by employing performance-based exclusion (Figure 5).

Paradigm and stimuli

A detailed experimental description is included in the preprint Sjouwerman et al. (2018). In brief, participants underwent a 100% reinforcement fear acquisition training phase in a behavioral laboratory setting, including 9 CS+ and 9 CS– trials, presented for 6 s each. Consequently, 9 US presentations were included that coincided 100 ms prior to CS+ offset. Trials were interleaved by 10–13 s ITIs with a white fixation cross presented on a black background. Black geometrical shapes served as CS, and electrical stimulation delivered by a DS7A electrical stimulator (Digitimer, Welwyn Garden City, UK) onto the outer surface of the right hand served as US. The intensity of the US was individually calibrated with a stair-case procedure in order to reach an unpleasant but tolerable level. Not of interest to the current case example were the acoustic startle probes (95 dB(A) burst of white noise) presented to elicit a startle response in two thirds of all acquisition trials, as well as three fear-rating blocks probed intermittently during fear acquisition training. Startle probes were presented 4 or 5 s post‐CS onset, and 5 or 7 s post‐ITI onset. No contingency instructions were given.

SCRs

SCRs were quantified as the first SCR within 0.9–4.0 s after stimulus onset (CS or US) and were scored semi-manually from trough-to-peak using a custom-made program. Signal increases smaller than 0.02 µS were treated as non-responses, that is set to 0. (Please note that this cut-off was not empirically determined as in Data set 1 but adopted from the previous publication of Data set 2. As we present re-analyses here, we decided not to change the cut-off to maintain comparability.) Responses confounded by recording artifacts, such as responses moving beyond the sampling window, excessive baseline activity, or electrode detachment were treated as missing values. Raw response amplitudes per trial were log-transformed and range-corrected for the maximum CS or US response per participant. Individuals not showing any valid SCR (i.e., missing or zero responses) in more or equal than two thirds (≥6 out of 9, see Appendix 4—table 2) of US trials were treated as physiological ‘non-responders’ (n = 19) and were consequently excluded from graphical illustration and the statistical analysis. In addition, 31 participants were excluded prior to physiological processing, either because of abortion of the experiment or due to technical failures during data acquisition (e.g. errors during saving, overwritten logfile, or missing markers), leaving 307 out of 357 individuals with valid SCR data for fear acquisition training. Of these 307 participants, 39 had incomplete STAI-T data (Spielberger et al., 1983) resulting in a final sample size for this case example of 268 individuals (195 female, mean ± SD age of 25 ± 4 years).

Statistical analysis

To test whether different exclusion groups differ in their mean trait anxiety levels, a univariate ANOVA with STAI-T score as the dependent variable and exclusion group as the independent variable was carried out. Post hoc pairwise t-tests were conducted to compare trait anxiety scores between the different exclusion group levels. The post hoc tests were corrected for multiple testing, using the Bonferroni correction method. 95% family wise confidence levels were determined using TukeyHSD tests.

Acknowledgements

The project is part of a European wide network (EIFEL-ROF network) of researchers working on meta-research topics in the field of fear conditioning research, which is funded by the German Research foundation (DFG; Grant ID LO1980/2-1) to TBL and CJM. Data used for re-analyses in the main manuscript paper are derived from projects funded by the DFG to TBL (Data set 1: Collaborative Research Center project number 44541416 TRR 58, sub-project B07. Data set 2: LO 1980/1-1). Data reported in Appendix 1—table 1 are also funded by the German Research Foundation to CJM (Collaborative Research Center SFB 1280 project number 316803389, sub-project A09) and JW (WE 5873/1–1 and WE 2762/5–1). The authors represent eight different research groups across three European countries (Germany, the Netherlands, and Belgium).

We thank Prof. Dr. Matthias Gamer, University of Würzburg, for providing EDA View for SCR response quantification.

The authors thank Manuel Kuhn, Jan Haaker, Tanja Jovanovic and Dean Mobbs for helpful suggestions during discussions on this project. We also thank Jan Haaker for help with initial literature screening.

Appendix 1

Definition of performance-based exclusion of participants (‘non-learners’) and numbers of participants excluded across studies

Appendix 1—table 1. Summary of criteria used to define ‘non-learners’ across records included in the systematic literature search.

Criteria used to define ‘non-learners’ were identified in eleven records reported in a total of 14 individual studies.

Reference % excluded participants (‘non-learners’) CS+/CS– cut-off (in µS) for ‘non-learners’ N trialsacq total CS+/CS– N trialsacq considered
CS+/CS–
Trials phase (unless otherwise stated, this refers to fear acquisition training) Additional criteria/notes
Ahmed and Lovibond, 2019, Exp. 1 24% <or = 0 3/3 2/2 last two thirds only considered as ‘non-learners’ if applicable to both SCL and ratings
Ahmed and Lovibond, 2019, Exp. 2 16%
Reddan et al., 2018a 35% <or = 0 16/8 8b/8 full phase
Grégoire and Greening, 2019 16% <0.1 13c/8 4c/4 last third participants were also excluded if they did not show equivalent responding to both CS+s (difference > 0.1 µS) or when not showing equal extinction to both CS+s or complete differential extinction to both CS+s vs. CS– (difference > 0.1 µS)
Hu et al., 2018 6% <or = 0 16/10 5/5d
OR 1/1
second halfd
OR last triald
‘non-learners’ discontinued after day 1 of the experiment
Oyarzún et al., 2019, Exp. 1 27% <or = 0e eightc/8 4c/4 second half only considered as ‘non-learners’ if applicable to both SCR and fear-potentiated startle
Oyarzún et al., 2019, Exp. 2 41%
Belleau et al., 2018 2% <0.05 5/5 5/5 full phase only considered as ‘non-learners’ when also failing to show any differential ratings (i.e.,<or = 0 in discrimination)f
Morriss et al., 2018 6% g < or = 0h 12/6 6b/6 full phase only considered as ‘non-learners’ if applicable across all phases (fear acquisition and extinction training, avoidance acquisition and extinction)
Schiller et al., 2018; Schiller et al., 2010, Exp. 1 48%i <0.1/mean SCR to the US 16c/10
13c/8
5b /5
OR 5b /5
OR 1b /1
OR increase from first to last trial
4b /4 OR 4b /4 OR 1b /1
OR increase from first to last trial
first half of acquisition
OR second half OR last trial of acquisition, OR the increase from the first to last trial of acquisition
Schiller et al., 2018; Schiller et al., 2010, Exp. 2 74%i
Nitta et al., 2018 52% <0 13c/8 2c /2 last two trials one additional participant showed strong SCR during re-extinction phase to CS+ and was therefore excluded
Hartley et al., 2019 16% <or = 0.05 6/6 3/3 last half
Hu et al., 2019 16% < 0k 8b/8 4b/4 second half

aPersonal communication with D. Schiller (20.5.2019 and 30.8.2019) confirmed that individuals were classified as ‘non-learners’ when they ‘did not demonstrate greater SCRs to the CS+ relative to the CS– on average across all acquisition trials (n = 24)’ (see 'Materials and methods' section). The personal communication clarified that the statement included in the results section that defines ‘non-learners’ as individuals that “did not demonstrate a discriminatory SCR during acquisition, defined as greater SCR to the CS+ relative to the CS– during either the first or last half of threat-acquisition on average’ was intended to refer to the same procedure (i.e., the full acquisition phase).

bRefers to unreinforced CS+ trials (CS+ trials not followed by the US) only.

cFor each CS+1 and CS+2.

dPersonal communication with D. Schiller (1.5.2019): late acquisition as reported in the publication refers to the last half or last trial. Anyone that had a positive difference (>0.000 µS) in either the second half or last trial of acquisition was kept.

ePersonal communication with J. Oyarzun (21.5.2019): all difference scores > 0 µS were considered as CS+/CS– discrimination.

fPersonal communication with E. Balleau, PhD (5.5.2019): ‘differential ratings’ means that CS+>CS is equal to or below 0 µS was non-differentiation.

gThese were not excluded as results did not change.

hPersonal communication with J. Morriss (15.4.2019): no positive differential response is defined as any number <0 µS.

iPercentages for ‘non-learners’, ‘non-extinguishers’ and ‘non-responders’ reported together.

kPersonal communication with D. Schiller (21.5.2019): zero differences were kept.

Appendix 1—table 2. Summary of criteria used to define ‘non-responders’ across records included in the systematic literature search.

Fifteen records, reporting a total of 17 studies, were identified.

Record % excluded participants
(‘non-responders’)
Cut-off (in µS) for a valid SCR Valid responses in at least % of trials Stimulus type (also referred to as ‘trial’) on which the exclusion is based Additional criteria/notes
Baeuchl et al., 2019 10% >0.01 ≥66% CXT+
Tuominen et al., 2019 12% >0.05 ≥13% CS+ and CS–
Gruss and Keil, 2019 11% visual inspectiona CS+, CS–and US
Sjouwerman and Lonsdorf, 2019 14% ≥0.02 US:≥67%
CS: no valid response in each CS modality
CS+, CS– and US
Grégoire and Greening, 2019 8% >0.02 ≥25%b CS+ and CS–c
Hu et al., 2018 3% ≥0.02 100% CS+ and CS–c ‘non-responders’ discontinued after day 1 of the experiment
Oyarzún et al., 2019, Exp. 1 0% ≥0.02 ≥25% CS+d and CS–c
Oyarzún et al., 2019, Exp. 2 9%
Tani et al., 2019 10% >0.03e 100% CS+
Marin et al., 2019 0%f ≥0.03 ≥10% US
Taylor et al., 2018 5% NA 100% motor testg
Morriss et al., 2018 6% >0.03 ≥90% CS+d and CS–c only applicable if true across all phases/days of the experiment
Schiller et al., 2018; Schiller et al., 2010, Exp. 1 48%h ≥0.02 ≥25% CS+d and CS–
Schiller et al., 2018; Schiller et al., 2010, Exp 2 74%h
Morriss and van Reekum, 2019, Exp. 1 2% >0.03 >90% CS+d and CS–
Morriss and van Reekum, 2019, Exp. 2 2%
Hartley et al., 2019 6% <0.05i ≥33 %i CS+ and CS–c
Hu et al., 2019 4% k ≥0.02 100% k US
Leuchs et al., 2019 4% NA ≥33% CS+ and CS–c only applicable if true across both days of the experiment

aPersonal communication with L. Forest Gruss (29.4.2019): “the determination of non-responders was done, this was done on visual inspection by me through all trials of all individuals. I verified after determining who the lowest, i.e. non-responders were, in the same fashion as the startle non-responders in summing responding over the entire experiment, and this responding falling below a threshold of overall response (~<10%) AND one individual due to lack of response at the end of the trial to the UCS specifically".

bPersonal communication with S.G. Greening (24.4.2019): “non-responders if more than 75% of data were missing (i.e., SCR <0.02 μS) during the training phase. So, that means, if a participant had at least six trials (out of 24) with measurable SCRs (whatever the condition), we kept them (if the other acquisition criteria were OK, see below). If they had five trials or fewer with measurable GSR, we considered them a non-responder and removed them".

cPersonal communications that ‘trial’ or this statement refers to CS+ and CS– trials: S. Greening (24.4.2019), D. Schiller (1.5.2019), J. Oyarzun (21.5.2019), J. Morriss (15.4.2019), C. Hartley (2.5.2019), V. Spoormaker (18.4.2019).

d CS+ unpaired.

ePersonal communication with H. Tani (2.5.2019): only CS+ trials were considered (here as response to the sound or the intrapersonal stimulus).

fPersonal communication with M.-F. Marin (23.4.2019): exclusion criteria were defined, but no participant met these criteria and hence none was excluded.

gPersonal communication with V. Taylor (6.6.2019): clarified that "non-responders’ were identified in a “motor test of SCR responding during the preliminary session. Essentially, they had to compress a ball with the right hand with maximal physical force for a few seconds on about 10 trials, which typically elicits quite large SCRs in subjects. Failure to respond to an SCR to all of these trials was considered a non-responder".

hPercentages for ‘non-learners’, ‘non-extinguishers’ and ‘non-responders’ reported together.

iPersonal communication with C. Hartley (2.5.2019): clarified that “participants were considered non-responder if they had SCR values of 0 for more than 8 of the 12 trials in acquisition (<4 responsive trials)”.

k The percentage of ‘non-responders’ and ‘non-learners’ was reported together without percentages for each category; personal communication with D. Schiller (21.5.2019): in the paper, it is reported that five individuals ‘were excluded due to equipment malfunction (N = 2) or had non-measurable skin conductance response (SCR) to the shock (N = 3)”. It was confirmed that these individuals excluded for non-measurable SCR did not show any responses to any stimulus.

Appendix 2

Applying the identified performance-based exclusion criteria to existing data: a case example

In this case example based on Data set 1 (see main manuscript), we tested whether CS+/CS– discrimination in SCRs does indeed differ between the different exclusion groups as defined by the cut-offs retrieved from the literature (see Figure 2B). Note that this is somewhat circular as exclusion groups are defined by different SCR CS+/CS– cutoffs, which then are used in an analysis in which differential SCRs are the dependent measure. However, that this is exactly what is sometimes done in the literature (see main manuscript).

Still, this is an important manipulation check to test empirically whether those classified in a group of ‘non-learners’ in the literature do indeed show no evidence of learning, which would be indicated by comparable SCRs to the CS+ and the CS– (i.e., no significant discrimination). Here, we test this for cumulative exclusion groups. Note that this is only a rough manipulation check, as a non-significant CS+/CS– discrimination effect in the whole group (e.g., those showing a CS+/CS– discrimination <0.05 µS based on raw scores) cannot be taken as evidence that all individuals in this group do not display meaningful or statistically significant CS+/CS– discrimination. More precisely, half of this group who did not meet the cut-off of 0.05µS in CS+/CS– discrimination do show a negative or zero discrimination score, which may bias the group average score towards non-discrimination. Yet, statistically testing for discrimination within each exclusion group (e.g. specifically in the group showing a discrimination between >0 and < 0.05 µS) is not unproblematic.

Appendix 2—table 1. Results of two-tailed t-tests for differences in SCR CS+/CS– discrimination in Data set 1 for the different cumulative exclusion groups (indicated by the + in the table) based on the criteria identified in the literature with respect to CS+/CS– discrimination cutoffs (in µS).

For completeness sake and as it is not always clear whether CS+/CS– discrimination is based on raw or transformed values, we report results based on analyses of both raw (A) and transformed values (B). P-values for these post-hoc tests are Bonferroni corrected.

A) t-tests: CS+/CS– discrimination based on raw values
Exclusion group
(cumulative)
CS+
M (SD)
CS–
M (SD)
df t pbonf_corr d
<0 0.04 (0.04) 0.07 (0.07) 10 −2.67 .140 0.81
+ = 0 0.02 (0.04) 0.03 (0.05) 33 −2.24 .193 0.38
+ > 0 and < 0.05 0.04 (0.05) 0.03 (0.05) 66 2.14 .219 0.26
+ = 0.05 0.04 (0.05) 0.03 (0.05) 70 2.88 .031 0.34
+ > 0.05 and < 0.1 0.06 (0.06) 0.04 (0.05) 88 5.87 .0000005 0.62
+ ≥ 0.1 0.10 (0.10) 0.04 (0.06) 115 7.87 <0.000000001 0.73
B) t-tests: CS+/CS– discrimination based on log-transformed and range-corrected values
Exclusion group
(cumulative)
CS+
M (SD)
CS–
M (SD)
df t pbonf_corr d
<0 0.09 (0.10) 0.13 (0.11) 13 −3.46 0.025 0.93
+ = 0 0.04 (0.08) 0.06 (0.10) 28 −2.90 0.043 0.54
+ > 0 and < 0.05 0.06 (0.10) 0.07 (0.11) 42 −0.88 >0.999 0.13
+ = 0.05 0.07 (0.10) 0.07 (0.11) 46 −0.06 >0.999 0.01
+ > 0.05 and < 0.1 0.09 (0.11) 0.07 (0.11) 60 2.81 .040 0.36
+ ≥ 0.1 0.21 (0.19) 0.10 (0.11) 115 9.56 <0.000000001 0.89

Appendix 3

Exploratory analyses on consistency of classification (‘learners’ vs. ‘non-learners’) across outcome measures and criteria employed

Throughout the main manuscript and particularly in the discussion, we highlight that differential (CS+>CS–) SCRs alone cannot be taken to infer ‘learning’ (Figure 4—figure supplement 1).

Appendix 3—table 1 provides statistical information on CS+/CS– discrimination in fear ratings in (cumulative) exclusion groups as defined by CS+/CS– discrimination in SCRs.

Appendix 3—table 1. CS+/CS– discrimination in fear ratings in (cumulative) exclusion groups (indicated by the + in the table) as defined by CS+/CS– discrimination in SCRs (based on raw scores).

Exclusion group (cumulative) CS+
M (SD)
CS–
M (SD)
df t pbonf_corr d
<0 15.8 (8.94) 2.45 (4.70) 10 5.37 0.002 1.62
+ = 0 16.6 (7.73) 3.15 (5.82) 31 9.69 <0.000000001 1.71
+ > 0 and < 0.05 16.2 (7.37) 3.06 (5.86) 64 12.8 <0.000000001 1.59
+ = 0.05 16.3 (7.26) 2.96 (5.75) 67 13.4 <0.000000001 1.62
+ > 0.05 and < 0.1 16.5 (6.97) 2.94 (5.47) 84 16.0 <0.000000001 1.74
+ >= 0.1 17.3 (6.64) 3.08 (5.04) 110 20.2 <0.000000001 1.92

Appendix 4

Definition of ‘non-responders ‘and amount of participants excluded across studies

In the main manuscript, we discuss different frequencies of ‘non-responding’ to different experimental stimuli (e.g., US, CS+ and CS– in isolation or in combination), which inherently lead to different exclusion frequencies when classifying ‘non-responders’ on the basis of different types of stimuli. As there is little empirical work on the frequency of ‘non-responses’ to the US, CSs (i.e., CS+ and CS–) and CS+ only to base recommendations on, we compiled this information across 20 different data sets (see Appendix 4—table 1), including information on SCR response quantification specifications (i.e., minimum amplitude, scoring approach) and procedural details during fear acquisition training (i.e., number of CS and US presentations). These data sets were provided by different co-authors involved in this manuscript.

In addition, Appendix 4—table 2 provides information on the number and percentage of individuals in a sample showing SCR ‘non-responses’ to a certain number of US presentations during fear acquisition training as well as mean number and percentage of CS responses (CS refers to the CS+ and CS– combined) in these individuals to guide the development of empirically based criteria to define SCR ‘non-responders’.

Appendix 4—table 1. Overview of SCR response quantification specifications (i.e., min. amplitude, scoring approach) and procedural details during fear acquisition training (i.e., number of CS and US presentations) as well as number (mean and range) and percentage of SCR non-responses towards the different stimuli (US, CS+, CS–, CS).

TTP: trough-to-peak; CS+E: CS+ extinguished; CS+U: CS+ unextinguished, CS: for both the CS+ and CS–.

Reference N Minimum amplitude cutoff (in µS) for valid SCRs Scoring details Number of… ‘Non-responses’ towards…
US CS (CS+/CS–) US (M ± SD, range) US (%) CS+ (M ± SD, range) CS+ (%) CS– (M ± SD, range) CS– (%) CS (M ± SD, range) CS (%)
Jentsch et al., 2020 41 ≥0.02 TTP (max peak), latency 0.5–4 s/1–80.5 s (US/CS) 10 16/16 1.12 ± 1.66 (0–10) 11.22 2.22 ± 3.31 (0–16) 13.87 4.49 ± 3.92 (0–16) 28.05 6.71 ± 6.68 (0–32) 20.96
Hermann et al., 2016 45 ≥0.02 TTP (max peak), latency 0.5–6 s/1–60.5 s (US/CS) 10 (5 for CS+E, 5 for CS+U) 8 CS+E/8 CS+U/16 CS– 0.24 ± 0.88 (0–5) 2.44 2.64 ± 3.49 (0–13);
CS+E: 1.47 ± 2.19 (0–8); CS+U: 1.18 ± 1.80 (0–7)
16.53
CS+E: 18.33;
CS+U: 14.72
8.07 ± 4.14 (0–16) 50.42 10.71 ± 6.65 (0–26) 33.47
Merz et al., 2018a 39 ≥0.02 TTP (max peak), latency 0.5–6 s/1–60.5 s (US/CS) 10 (5 for CS+E, 5 for CS+U) 8 CS+E/8 CS+U/8 CS– 2.08 ± 1.98 (0–8) 20.77 3.36 ± 4.55 (0–16);
CS+E: 1.59 ± 2.35 (0–8);
CS+U: 1.77 ± 2.32 (0–8)
21.00;
CS+E: 19.87;
CS+U: 22.12
2.41 ± 2.27 (0–8) 30.13 5.77 ± 6.49 (0–24) 24.04
Merz et al., 2014 40 ≥0.02 TTP (max peak), latency 0.5–6 s/1–60.5 s (US/CS) 10 (5 for CS+E, 5 for CS+U) 8 CS+E/8 CS+U/16 CS– 0.13 ± 0.33 (0–1) 1.25 1.08 ± 2.04 (0–11);
CS+E: 0.58 ± 1.08 (0–5);
CS+U: 0.50 ± 1.11 (0–6)
6.72;
CS+E: 7.19
CS+U: 6.25
3.13 ± 2.96 (0–11) 19.53 4.20 ± 4.39 (0–21) 13.13
Hamacher-Dang et al., 2015 39 ≥0.02 TTP (max peak), latency 0.5–6 s/1–60.5 s (US/CS) 10 (5 for CS+E, 5 for CS+U) 8 CS+E/8 CS+U/16 CS– 0.23 ± 0.48 (0–2) 2.31 2.33 ± 3.77 (0–12);
CS+E: 1.31 ± 2.21 (0–8);
CS+U: 1.03 ± 1.81 (0–7)
14.58;
CS+E: 16.35;
CS+U: 12.82
3.77 ± 4.20 (0–14) 23.56 6.10 ± 7.71 (0–26) 19.07
Mertens et al., 2019 59 ≥0.02 TTP (max peak), latency 1–8 s (baseline 0–2 s) 10 10/5 0.78 ± 1.69 (0–6) 7.8 4.75 ± 2.97 (0–10) 47.5 2.93 ± 1.66 (0–5) 58.6 7.68 ± 4.30 (0–15) 51.2
Klingel-höfer-Jens et al., unpublished 119 ≥0.01 TTP (first peak), latency 0.9–2.5 s/3.5 s (US/CS) 14 14/14 1.40 ± 2.47 (0–14) 10.0 5.30 ± 4.42 (0–14) 37.9 8.20 ± 3.99 (0–14) 58.6 6.75 ± 4.44 (0–14) 48.2
Gerlicher et al. unpublished 52 ≥0.02 TTP (first peak) latency 0.9–4 s 6 6/6 0.73 ± 1.39 (0–6) 12.18 2.73 ± 2.06 (0–6) 45.5 3.54 ± 1.82 (0–6) 59.0 6.27 ± 3.54 (0–12) 52.24
Gerlicher et al., 2018 39 ≥0.02 TTP (first peak) latency 0.9–4 s 5 10/10 0.33 ± 0.93 (0–5) 6.67 1.05 ± 2.21 (0–10) 10.51 2.36 ± 2.49 (0–10) 23.59 3.41 ± 4.48 (0–20) 17.05
Andreatta et al. unpublished 76 ≥0.02 TTP (first peak) latency 0.8–4 s 16 (8 in analysis due to startle probes) 16/16
(8/8 in analysis due to startle probes)
1.34 ± 1.69 (0–8) 16.78 4.17 ± 2.30 (0–8) 52.14 5.00 ± 1.98 (0–8) 62.50 9.17 ± 3.77 (0–16) 57.32
Wendt et al., 2020 112 ≥0.04 TTP (first peak), latency 0.9–4 s 9 12/12 0.46 ± 1.15 (0–7) 5.06 5.88 ± 3.63 (0–12) 48.96 7.06 ± 3.19 (0–12) 58.85 12.94 ± 6.39 (0–24) 53.91
Wendt et al., 2015 108 ≥0.04 TTP (first peak), latency 0.9–4 s 12 12/12 0.27 ± 0.99 (0–8) 2.24 6.44 ± 3.81 (0–12) 53.63 8.53 ± 2.65 (0–12) 71.06 14.96 ± 6.04 (0–24) 62.35
Drexler et al., 2015 46 ≥0.02 TTP (max peak), latency 1–4.5 s 18 13 CS1+/13 CS2+/13 CS– 2.8 ± 4.18 (0–16) 15.57 9.67 ± 7.64 (0–26);
CS1+: 4.87 ± 4.07 (0–13);
CS2+: 4.80 ± 3.78 (0–13)
37.20;
CS1+:37.45;
CS2+: 36.95
5.26 ± 3.95 (0–13) 40.46 14.93 ± 11.37 (0–39) 38.29
Meir Drexler et al., 2016 73 ≥0.02 TTP (max peak), latency 1–4.5 s 18 13 CS1+/13 CS2+/13 CS– 3.37 ± 4.72 (0–18) 18.72 11.51 ± 7.96 (0–25);
CS1+: 5.78 ± 3.97 (0–13);
CS2+: 5.73 ± 4.22 (0–13)
44.25;
CS1+:44.67;
CS2+:44.04
6.29 ± 3.94 (0–13) 48.36 17.79 ± 11.67 (0–37) 45.62
Meir Drexler and Wolf, 2017 72 ≥0.02 TTP (max peak), latency 1–4.5 s 18 13 CS1+/13 CS2+/13 CS– 1.92 ± 2.96 (0–11) 10.64 9.65 ± 7.21 (0–25);
CS1+: 4.78 ± 3.72 (0–12);
CS2+: 4.88 ± 3.85 (0–13)
37.12;
CS1+: 36.75;
CS2+: 37.50
5.42 ± 3.54 (0–12) 41.66 15.07 ± 10.40 (0–36) 38.63
Drexler et al., 2018 40 ≥0.02 TTP (max peak), latency 1–4.5 s 10 (5 for CS+E, 5 for CS+U) 8 CS+E/8 CS+U/16 CS– 0.32 ± 0.69 (0–3) 3.25 4.17 ± 4.45 (0–16);
CS+E: 2.02 ± 2.47 (0–8);
CS+U: 2.15 ± 2.38 (0–8)
26.09;
CS+E: 25.31;
CS+U: 26.87
6.07 ± 4.37 (0–16) 37.96 10.25 ± 8.24 (1-27) 32.03
Meir Drexler et al., 2019 75 ≥0.02 TTP (max peak), latency 0.5–6 s/1–80.5 s (US/CS) 6 10/10 0.89 ± 01.57 (0–6) 14.88 4.07 ± 3.40 (0–10) 40.66 4.68 ± 3.23 (0–10) 46.8 8.75 ± 6.41 (0–20) 43.73
Chalkia et al., unpublished 238 ≥0.02 TTP (first peak), latency 0.5–4.5 s 6 16/10 (10/10 in analysis, only unrein-forced trials) 0 (0–6) 0 0.03 ± 0.19 (0–10) 0.29 0.05 ± 0.29 (0–10) 0.50 0.08 ± 0.42 (0–20) 0.40
Hollandt et al., unpublished 30 >0.04 TTP (first peak), latency 0.9–4 s 6 10/10 0 0 2.97 ± 2.81 (0–10) 29.67 7.23 ± 2.61 (0–10) 72.33 10.20 ± 4.72 51.0
Sjouwerman et al., 2018 326 ≥0.02 TTP (first peak), latency 0.9–4.5 s 9 9/9 1.38 ± 1.73 (0–9) 15.37 3.11 ± 2.69 (0–9) 34.59 3.77 ± 2.68 (0–9) 41.92 6.87 ± 5.01 (0–18) 38.26

Appendix 4—table 2. Number and percentage of individuals in a sample showing SCR non-responses to a certain number of US presentations during fear acquisition training (exemplarily for one to eight USs#), as well as mean number of and percentage of CS responses (CS refers to the CS+ and CS– combined) in these individuals.

#Here only up to eight USs are included as eight is half of the maximum number of US presentations in the samples included here.

Reference a) n (%) of individuals with 0, 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, and 8 SCRs towards the US. b) M (%) of valid CS responses for these individuals.
0 US 1 US 2 US 3 US 4 US 5 US 6 US 7 US 8 US
Jentsch et al., 2020 a) 1 (2.4%) b) 0 (0%) a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 2 (4.9%) b) 27.5 (85.9%) a) 7 (17.1%) b) 25.4 (79.5%)
Hermann et al., 2016 a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 1 (2%) b) 12 (37.5%) a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 1 (2%) b) 14 (43.7%) a) 0 (0%) b) NA
Merz et al., 2018a a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 1 (2.6%) b) 23.0 (95.8%) a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 2 (5.1%) b) 20.0 (83.3%) a) 3 (7.7%) b) 23.0 (95.8%) a) 1 (2.6%) b) 21.0 (87.5%) a) 5 (12.8%) b) 21.4 (89.1%) a) 9 (23.1%) b) 21.6 (85.6%)
Merz et al., 2014 a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA
Hamacher-Dang et al., 2015 a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 1 (3%) b) 24 (75.0%)
Mertens et al., 2019 a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 4 (6.78%) b) 1.75 (11.67%) a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 2 (3.39%) b) 3.5 (23.33%) a) 2 (3.39%) b) 9 (60%) a) 0 (0%) b) NA
Klingelhöfer-Jens et al., unpublished a) 2 (1.68%) b) 0 (0%) a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 1 (0.84%) b) 10 (35.7%) a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 1 (0.84%) b) 1 (3.57%) a) 2 (1.68%) b) 2 (7.14%) a) 1 (0.84%) b) 0 (0%)
Gerlicher et al., unpublished a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 3 (5.77%) b) 4 (33.33%) a) 5 (9.62%) b) 4.8 (40%) a) 7 (13.46%) b) 6.7 (55.91%) a) 35 (67.31%) b) 6.15 (51.25%) NA NA
Gerlicher et al., 2018 a) 1 (2.56%) b) 0 (0%) a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 2 (5.13%) b) 19.5 (97.5%) a) 4 (10.26%) b) 17.5 (87.50%) a) 32 (82.05%) b) 16.81 (84.05%) NA NA NA
Wendt et al., 2020 a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 1 (0.9%) b)18 (75%) a) 1 (0.9%) b) 24 (100%) a) 1 (0.9%) b) 0 (0%) a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 2 (1.8%) b) 12 (50%) a) 8 (7.1%) b) 13.13 (54.69%) a) 11 (9.9%) b) 11.09 (46.21%)
Wendt et al., 2015 a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 1 (0.9%) b) 18 (75%) a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 1 (0.9%) b) 17 (70.83%) a) 0 (0%) b) NA
Drexler et al., 2015 a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 2 (4.3%) b) 0.5 (1.28%) a) 1 (2.2%) b) 0.0 (0.0%) a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 1 (2.2%) b) 7.0 (17.94%) a) 0 (0%) b) NA
Meir Drexler et al., 2016 a) 1 (1.4%) b) 29.00 (74.35%) a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 2 (2.7%) b) 2.0 (5.12%) a) 1 (1.4%) b) 9.0 (23.07%) a) 1 (1.4%) b) 2.0 (5.12%) a) 1 (1.4%) b) 3.0 (7.69%) a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 4 (5.5%) b) 5.0 (12.82%) a) 2 (2.7%) b) 6.50 (16.66%)
Meir Drexler and Wolf, 2017 a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 1 (1.4%) b) 5.0 (12.82%) a) 1 (1.4%) b) 5.0 (12.82%)
Drexler et al., 2018 a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 1 (2.5%) b) 8 (25%) a) 2 (5.0%) b) 12.5 (39.06%)
Meir Drexler et al., 2019 a) 3 (4.0%) b) 0.33 (1.66%) a) 1 (1.3%) b) 1 (5.0%) a) 4 (5.3%) b) 4.25 (21.25%) a) 2 (2.7%) b) 3.0 (15.0%) a) 3 (4.0%) b) 1.33 (6.66%) a) 19 (25.3%) b) 12.63 (63.15%) a) 43 (57.3%) b) 13.21 (66.04%) a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA
Chalkia et al., unpublished a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 238 (100%) b) 19.92 (99.6%) a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA
Hollandt et al., unpublished a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA NA NA
Sjouwerman et al., 2018 a) 4 (1.23%) b) 0.5 (2.78%) a) 2 (0.61%) b) 2.5 (13.89%) a) 4 (1.23%) b) 4.13 (22.92%) a) 2 (0.61%) b) 7.25 (40.28%) a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA a) 0 (0%) b) NA

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Tina B Lonsdorf, Email: t.lonsdorf@uke.de.

Floris P de Lange, Radboud University, Netherlands.

Alexander Shackman, University of Maryland, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft LO 1980/2-1 to Tina B Lonsdorf, Christian J Merz.

  • Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft LO 1980/1-1 to Tina B Lonsdorf.

  • Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft Project B07 44541416 to Tina B Lonsdorf.

  • Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft 316803389 - SFB1280 to Christian J Merz.

  • Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft WE 5873/1-1 to Julia Wendt.

  • Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft WE 5873/5-1 to Julia Wendt.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Visualization, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Project administration, Writing - review and editing, Concieved the study, Organized the project group, Contributed to data acquisition, Data analysis (systematic literature search, Dataset 1, Dataset 2, Data visualization, Drafting the manuscript, Revised the draft critically and approved the final version to be published.

Data curation, Formal analysis, Visualization, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing, Contributed to data analysis and data visualization (data set 1), Drafting the manuscript, Revised the draft critically and approved the final version to be published.

Formal analysis, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing, Contributed to data acquisition and data analysis (systematic literature search), Drafting the manuscript, Revised the draft critically and approved the final version to be published.

Formal analysis, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing, Contributed to data acquisition and data analysis (systematic literature search), Drafting the manuscript, Revised the draft critically and approved the final version to be published.

Formal analysis, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing, Contributed to data acquisition and data analysis (systematic literature search), Drafting the manuscript, Revised the draft critically and approved the final version to be published.

Formal analysis, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing, Contributed to data acquisition and data analysis (systematic literature search), Drafting the manuscript, Revised the draft critically and approved the final version to be published.

Formal analysis, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing, Contributed to data acquisition and data analysis (systematic literature search), Drafting the manuscript, Revised the draft critically and approved the final version to be published.

Formal analysis, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing, Contributed to data acquisition and data analysis (systematic literature search), Drafting the manuscript, Revised the draft critically and approved the final version to be published.

Formal analysis, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing, Contributed to data acquisition and data analysis (systematic literature search), Drafting the manuscript, Revised the draft critically and approved the final version to be published.

Formal analysis, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing, Contributed to data acquisition and data analysis (systematic literature search), Drafting the manuscript, Revised the draft critically and approved the final version to be published.

Data curation, Formal analysis, Visualization, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing, Contributed to data acquisition, Data analysis (dataset 2), Data visualization, Drafting the manuscript, Revised the draft critically and approved the final version to be published.

Formal analysis, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing, Contributed to data acquisition and data analysis (systematic literature search), Drafting the manuscript, Revised the draft critically and approved the final version to be published.

Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing, Conceived the study, Contributed to data acquisition and data analysis (systematic literature search), Drafting the manuscript, Revised the draft critically and approved the final version to be published.

Ethics

Human subjects: Study 1: All participants gave written informed consent to the protocol which was approved by the local ethics committee (PV 5157, Ethics Committee of the General Medical Council Hamburg). Study 2: All participants gave written informed consent to the protocol which was approved by the Ethical Review Board of the German Psychological Association (TL072015).

Additional files

Transparent reporting form

Data availability

The minimal data sets (data set 1 and data set 2, both represent re-analysis of existing data), which were analysed during the current study, as well as code for figure production are are available at OSF under https://osf.io/mkxqe/ and DOI: https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/MKXQE.

The following datasets were generated:

Tina B Lonsdorf, Maren Klingelhöfer-Jens. 2019. Data_and_code_dataset1. Open Science Framework. w9y8z

Tina B Lonsdorf, Rachel Sjouwerman. 2019. Data_and_code_dataset2. Open Science Framework. 7c5ag

References

  1. Ahmed O, Lovibond PF. Rule-based processes in generalisation and peak shift in human fear conditioning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2019;72:118–131. doi: 10.1177/1747021818766461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alexandra Kredlow M, Pineles SL, Inslicht SS, Marin MF, Milad MR, Otto MW, Orr SP. Assessment of skin conductance in african american and Non-African american participants in studies of conditioned fear. Psychophysiology. 2017;54:1741–1754. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12909. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Allen M, Poggiali D, Whitaker K, Marshall TR, Kievit R. Raincloud plots: a multi-platform tool for robust data visualization. PeerJ. 2018;4:63. doi: 10.7287/peerj.preprints.27137v1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baeuchl C, Hoppstädter M, Meyer P, Flor H. Contingency awareness as a prerequisite for differential contextual fear conditioning. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2019;19:811–828. doi: 10.3758/s13415-018-00666-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Belleau EL, Pedersen WS, Miskovich TA, Helmstetter FJ, Larson CL. Cortico-limbic connectivity changes following fear extinction and relationships with trait anxiety. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2018;13:1037–1046. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsy073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Boucsein W, Fowles DC, Grimnes S, Ben-Shakhar G, roth WT, Dawson ME, Filion DL, Society for Psychophysiological Research Ad Hoc Committee on Electrodermal Measures Publication recommendations for electrodermal measurements. Psychophysiology. 2012;49:1017–1034. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01384.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bradford DE, Starr MJ, Shackman AJ, Curtin JJ. Empirically based comparisons of the reliability and validity of common quantification approaches for eyeblink startle potentiation in humans. Psychophysiology. 2015;52:1669–1681. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12545. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chauret M, La Buissonnière-Ariza V, Lamoureux Tremblay V, Suffren S, Servonnet A, Pine DS, Maheu FS. The conditioning and extinction of fear in youths: what's sex got to do with it? Biological Psychology. 2014;100:97–105. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.06.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Craske MG, Kircanski K, Zelikowsky M, Mystkowski J, Chowdhury N, Baker A. Optimizing inhibitory learning during exposure therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2008;46:5–27. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2007.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Drexler SM, Merz CJ, Hamacher-Dang TC, Tegenthoff M, Wolf OT. Effects of cortisol on reconsolidation of reactivated fear memories. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:3036–3043. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Drexler SM, Merz CJ, Wolf OT. Preextinction stress prevents Context-Related renewal of fear. Behavior Therapy. 2018;49:1008–1019. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2018.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Duits P, Cath DC, Lissek S, Hox JJ, Hamm AO, Engelhard IM, van den Hout MA, Baas JMP. Updated meta-analysis of classical fear conditioning in the anxiety disorders. Depression and Anxiety. 2015;32:239–253. doi: 10.1002/da.22353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Flake JK, Fried E. Measurement schmeasurement: questionable measurement practices and how to avoid them. PsyArXiv. 2019 doi: 10.31234/osf.io/hs7wm. [DOI]
  14. Garpenstrand H, Annas P, Ekblom J, Oreland L, Fredrikson M. Human fear conditioning is related to dopaminergic and serotonergic biological markers. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2001;115:358–364. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.115.2.358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Gelman A, Loken E. The Garden of Forking Paths : Why Multiple Comparisons Can Be a Problem , Even When There Is No “ Fishing Expedition ” or “ P-Hacking ” and the Research Hypothesis Was Posited Ahead of Time. Columbia Statistics; 2013. http://www.stat.columbia.edu/~gelman/research/unpublished/p_hacking.pdf [Google Scholar]
  16. Gerlicher AMV, Tüscher O, Kalisch R. Dopamine-dependent prefrontal reactivations explain long-term benefit of fear extinction. Nature Communications. 2018;9:1–9. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-06785-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Gerlicher AMV, Tüscher O, Kalisch R. L-DOPA improves extinction memory retrieval after successful fear extinction. Psychopharmacology. 2019;236:3401–3412. doi: 10.1007/s00213-019-05301-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Grégoire L, Greening SG. Opening the reconsolidation window using the mind’s eye: Extinction training during reconsolidation disrupts fear memory expression following mental imagery reactivation. Cognition. 2019;183:277–281. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Grillon C, Baas JM. Comments on the use of the startle reflex in psychopharmacological challenges: impact of baseline startle on measurement of fear-potentiated startle. Psychopharmacology. 2002;164:236–238. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1164-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Gruss LF, Keil A. Sympathetic responding to unconditioned stimuli predicts subsequent threat expectancy, orienting, and visuocortical Bias in human aversive pavlovian conditioning. Biological Psychology. 2019;140:64–74. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.11.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Haaker J, Golkar A, Selbing I, Olsson A. Assessment of social transmission of threats in humans using observational fear conditioning. Nature Protocols. 2017;12:1378–1386. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2017.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hamacher-Dang TC, Merz CJ, Wolf OT. Stress following extinction learning leads to a context-dependent return of fear. Psychophysiology. 2015;52:489–498. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hamm AO, Greenwald MK, Bradley MM, Lang PJ. Emotional learning, hedonic change, and the startle probe. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1993;102:453–465. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.102.3.453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hamm AO, Weike AI. The neuropsychology of fear learning and fear regulation. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2005;57:5–14. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hardwicke TE, Ioannidis JPA. Mapping the universe of registered reports. Nature Human Behaviour. 2018;2:793–796. doi: 10.1038/s41562-018-0444-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hardwicke TE, Shanks DR. Reply to walker and Stickgold: proposed boundary conditions on memory reconsolidation will require empirical verification. PNAS. 2016;113:E3993–E3994. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1608235113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hartley CA, Coelho CAO, Boeke E, Ramirez F, Phelps EA. Individual differences in blink rate modulate the effect of instrumental control on subsequent pavlovian responding. Psychopharmacology. 2019;236:87–97. doi: 10.1007/s00213-018-5082-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Head ML, Holman L, Lanfear R, Kahn AT, Jennions MD. The extent and consequences of p-hacking in science. PLOS Biology. 2015;13:e1002106. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hermann A, Stark R, Milad MR, Merz CJ. Renewal of conditioned fear in a novel context is associated with hippocampal activation and connectivity. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2016;11:1411–1421. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hu J, Wang W, Homan P, Wang P, Zheng X, Schiller D. Reminder duration determines threat memory modification in humans. Scientific Reports. 2018;8:8848. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-27252-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hu J, Wang Z, Feng X, Long C, Schiller D. Post-retrieval oxytocin facilitates next day extinction of threat memory in humans. Psychopharmacology. 2019;236:293–301. doi: 10.1007/s00213-018-5074-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Ioannidis JP. How to make more published research true. PLOS Medicine. 2014;11:e1001747. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001747. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Jentsch VL, Wolf OT, Merz CJ. Temporal dynamics of conditioned skin conductance and pupillary responses during fear acquisition and extinction. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2020;147:93–99. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. John LK, Loewenstein G, Prelec D. Measuring the prevalence of questionable research practices with incentives for truth telling. Psychological Science. 2012;23:524–532. doi: 10.1177/0956797611430953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. LeDoux J. Rethinking the emotional brain. Neuron. 2012;73:653–676. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. LeDoux JE. Coming to terms with fear. PNAS. 2014;111:2871–2878. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1400335111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Leuchs L, Schneider M, Spoormaker VI. Measuring the conditioned response: a comparison of pupillometry, skin conductance, and startle electromyography. Psychophysiology. 2019;56:e13283. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Lonsdorf TB, Menz MM, Andreatta M, Fullana MA, Golkar A, Haaker J, Heitland I, Hermann A, Kuhn M, Kruse O, Meir Drexler S, Meulders A, Nees F, Pittig A, Richter J, Römer S, Shiban Y, Schmitz A, Straube B, Vervliet B, Wendt J, Baas JMP, Merz CJ. Don't fear 'fear conditioning': Methodological considerations for the design and analysis of studies on human fear acquisition, extinction, and return of fear. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2017;77:247–285. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.02.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lonsdorf TB, Merz CJ, Fullana MA. Fear extinction retention: is it what we think it is? Biological Psychiatry. 2019;85:1074–1082. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.02.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Lonsdorf TB, Merz CJ. More than just noise: inter-individual differences in fear acquisition, extinction and return of fear in humans - Biological, experiential, temperamental factors, and methodological pitfalls. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2017;80:703–728. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Marin MF, Barbey F, Rosenbaum BL, Hammoud MZ, Orr SP, Milad MR. Absence of conditioned responding in humans: a bad measure or individual differences? Psychophysiology. 2019;57:e13350. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Meir Drexler S, Merz CJ, Hamacher-Dang TC, Wolf OT. Cortisol effects on fear memory reconsolidation in women. Psychopharmacology. 2016;233:2687–2697. doi: 10.1007/s00213-016-4314-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Meir Drexler S, Merz CJ, Lissek S, Tegenthoff M, Wolf OT. Reactivation of the unconditioned stimulus inhibits the return of fear independent of cortisol. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2019;13:254. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Meir Drexler S, Wolf OT. Stress disrupts the reconsolidation of fear memories in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2017;77:95–104. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.11.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Mertens G, Leer A, van Dis EAM, Vermeer L, Steenhuizen A, van der Veen L, Engelhard IM. Secondary extinction reduces reinstatement of threat expectancy and conditioned skin conductance responses in human fear conditioning. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 2019;62:103–111. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2018.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Merz CJ, Hamacher-Dang TC, Wolf OT. Exposure to stress attenuates fear retrieval in healthy men. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2014;41:89–96. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.12.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Merz CJ, Hamacher-Dang TC, Stark R, Wolf OT, Hermann A. Neural underpinnings of cortisol effects on fear extinction. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2018a;43:384–392. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Merz CJ, Kinner VL, Wolf OT. Let's talk about sex … differences in human fear conditioning. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2018b;23:7–12. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.01.021. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  49. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, PRISMA Group Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLOS Medicine. 2009;6:e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Morriss J, Chapman C, Tomlinson S, van Reekum CM. Escape the bear and fall to the lion: the impact of avoidance availability on threat acquisition and extinction. Biological Psychology. 2018;138:73–80. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.08.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Morriss J, van Reekum CM. I feel safe when i know: contingency instruction promotes threat extinction in high intolerance of uncertainty individuals. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2019;116:111–118. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2019.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Munafò MR, Nosek BA, Bishop DVM, Button KS, Chambers CD, Percie du Sert N, Simonsohn U, Wagenmakers E-J, Ware JJ, Ioannidis JPA. A manifesto for reproducible science. Nature Human Behaviour. 2017;1:0021. doi: 10.1038/s41562-016-0021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Naveteur J, Freixa I Baque E. Individual differences in electrodermal activity as a function of subjects' anxiety. Personality and Individual Differences. 1987;8:615–626. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(87)90059-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  54. Nitta Y, Takahashi T, Haitani T, Sugimori E, Kumano H. Avoidance behavior prevents modification of fear memory during reconsolidation. Psychological Reports. 2018;15:33294118811116. doi: 10.1177/0033294118811116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Nosek BA, Alter G, Banks GC, Borsboom D, Bowman SD, Breckler SJ, Buck S, Chambers CD, Chin G, Christensen G, Contestabile M, Dafoe A, Eich E, Freese J, Glennerster R, Goroff D, Green DP, Hesse B, Humphreys M, Ishiyama J, Karlan D, Kraut A, Lupia A, Mabry P, Madon T, Malhotra N, Mayo-Wilson E, McNutt M, Miguel E, Paluck EL, Simonsohn U, Soderberg C, Spellman BA, Turitto J, VandenBos G, Vazire S, Wagenmakers EJ, Wilson R, Yarkoni T. Promoting an open research culture. Science. 2015;348:1422–1425. doi: 10.1126/science.aab2374. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Oyarzún JP, Lopez-Barroso D, Fuentemilla L, Cucurell D, Pedraza C, Rodriguez-Fornells A, de Diego-Balaguer R. Updating fearful memories with extinction training during reconsolidation: a human study using auditory aversive stimuli. PLOS ONE. 2012;7:e38849. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038849. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Oyarzún JP, Càmara E, Kouider S, Fuentemilla L, de Diego-Balaguer R. Implicit but not explicit extinction to threat-conditioned stimulus prevents spontaneous recovery of threat-potentiated startle responses in humans. Brain and Behavior. 2019;9:e01157. doi: 10.1002/brb3.1157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Pavlov I. Conditioned Reflexes. Oxford University Press; 1927. [Google Scholar]
  59. Plendl W, Wotjak CT. Dissociation of within- and between-session extinction of conditioned fear. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:4990–4998. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6038-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Prenoveau JM, Craske MG, Liao B, Ornitz EM. Human fear conditioning and extinction: timing is everything…or is it? Biological Psychology. 2013;92:59–68. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Reddan MC, Wager TD, Schiller D. Attenuating neural threat expression with imagination. Neuron. 2018;100:994–1005. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.10.047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Schiller D, Monfils MH, Raio CM, Johnson DC, Ledoux JE, Phelps EA. Preventing the return of fear in humans using reconsolidation update mechanisms. Nature. 2010;463:49–53. doi: 10.1038/nature08637. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Schiller D, Monfils MH, Raio CM, Johnson DC, LeDoux JE, Phelps EA. Addendum: preventing the return of fear in humans using reconsolidation update mechanisms. Nature. 2018;562:E21. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0405-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Silberzahn R, Uhlmann EL, Martin DP, Anselmi P, Aust F, Awtrey E, Bahník Š, Bai F, Bannard C, Bonnier E, Carlsson R, Cheung F, Christensen G, Clay R, Craig MA, Dalla Rosa A, Dam L, Evans MH, Flores Cervantes I, Fong N, Gamez-Djokic M, Glenz A, Gordon-McKeon S, Heaton TJ, Hederos K, Heene M, Hofelich Mohr AJ, Högden F, Hui K, Johannesson M, Kalodimos J, Kaszubowski E, Kennedy DM, Lei R, Lindsay TA, Liverani S, Madan CR, Molden D, Molleman E, Morey RD, Mulder LB, Nijstad BR, Pope NG, Pope B, Prenoveau JM, Rink F, Robusto E, Roderique H, Sandberg A, Schlüter E, Schönbrodt FD, Sherman MF, Sommer SA, Sotak K, Spain S, Spörlein C, Stafford T, Stefanutti L, Tauber S, Ullrich J, Vianello M, Wagenmakers E-J, Witkowiak M, Yoon S, Nosek BA. Many analysts, one data set: making transparent how variations in analytic choices affect results. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science. 2018;1:337–356. doi: 10.1177/2515245917747646. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  65. Simmons JP, Nelson LD, Simonsohn U. False-positive psychology: undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychological Science. 2011;22:1359–1366. doi: 10.1177/0956797611417632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Sjouwerman R, Niehaus J, Kuhn M, Lonsdorf TB. Don't startle me-Interference of startle probe presentations and intermittent ratings with fear acquisition. Psychophysiology. 2016;53:1889–1899. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12761. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Sjouwerman R, Scharfenort R, Lonsdorf TB. Individual differences in fear learning: specificity to trait-anxiety beyond other measures of negative affect and mediation via amygdala activation. bioRxiv. 2018 doi: 10.1101/233528. [DOI]
  68. Sjouwerman R, Lonsdorf TB. Latency of skin conductance responses across stimulus modalities. Psychophysiology. 2019;56:e13307. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE. Manual for the State‐Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  70. Steegen S, Tuerlinckx F, Gelman A, Vanpaemel W. Increasing transparency through a multiverse analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2016;11:702–712. doi: 10.1177/1745691616658637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Tabbert K, Merz CJ, Klucken T, Schweckendiek J, Vaitl D, Wolf OT, Stark R. Influence of contingency awareness on neural, electrodermal and evaluative responses during fear conditioning. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2011;6:495–506. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Tani H, Tada M, Maeda T, Konishi M, Umeda S, Terasawa Y, Mimura M, Takahashi T, Uchida H. Comparison of emotional processing assessed with fear conditioning by interpersonal conflicts in patients with depression and schizophrenia. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 2019;73:116–125. doi: 10.1111/pcn.12805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Taylor VA, Roy M, Chang L, Gill LN, Mueller C, Rainville P. Reduced Fear-Conditioned pain modulation in experienced meditators: a preliminary study. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2018;80:799–806. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000634. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Tuominen L, Boeke E, DeCross S, Wolthusen RP, Nasr S, Milad M, Vangel M, Tootell R, Holt D. The relationship of perceptual discrimination to neural mechanisms of fear generalization. NeuroImage. 2019;188:445–455. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.12.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Vervliet B, Craske MG, Hermans D. Fear extinction and relapse: state of the art. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2013;9:215–248. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Weissgerber TL, Milic NM, Winham SJ, Garovic VD. Beyond bar and line graphs: time for a new data presentation paradigm. PLOS Biology. 2015;13:e1002128. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Wendt J, Neubert J, Lindner K, Ernst FD, Homuth G, Weike AI, Hamm AO. Genetic influences on the acquisition and inhibition of fear. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2015;98:499–505. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Wendt J, Hufenbach MC, König J, Hamm AO. Effects of verbal instructions and physical threat removal prior to extinction training on the return of conditioned fear. Scientific Reports. 2020;10:1202. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-57934-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Wickham H. Ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. Springer-Verlag; 2009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Alexander Shackman1
Reviewed by: Daniel Bradford, Christine Larson, Nick Balderston

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

Fear is a central dimension of childhood temperament and adult personality. When extreme, fear contributes to the development a spectrum of prevalent and debilitating psychiatric disorders. Mechanistic studies in mice and rats have begun to illuminate the specific molecules, cells, and circuits that control the expression of fear-relevant behaviors, but the relevance of these tantalizing discoveries to the complexities of the human brain and human suffering remain unclear. Human fear conditioning studies represent a crucial means of the addressing this question, yet as Lonsdorf and colleagues make clear in this report, the impact of such studies is limited by several crucial (but seemingly mundane) methodological issues. In particular, they highlight substantial heterogeneity in exclusion criteria for such studies-who does and who does not adequately learn the association between cues and consequences. Based on this, they propose evidence-based clear guidelines for conducting and reporting fear conditioning studies. This report is likely to be of keen interest to a broad spectrum of stakeholders, including the producers, consumers, and funders of human fear research.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Lessons from fear conditioning on exclusions in learning tasks on how to not get lost in the garden of forking paths" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by Dr. Shackman as the Reviewing Editor and Dr. de Lange as the Senior Editor. The following individuals involved in review of your submission have agreed to reveal their identity: Daniel Bradford (Reviewer #1); Christine Larson (Reviewer #2); Nick Balderston (Reviewer #3).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

As the affective neuroscientist Kay Tye recently noted, "Fear resembles a dictator that makes all other brain processes (from cognition to breathing) its slave… Given its critical importance in survival and its authoritarian command over the rest of the brain, fear should be one of the most extensively studied topics in neuroscience" (Mobbs et al. Nature Neurosci 2019). In that same wide-ranging paper, psychiatrist Kerry Ressler reminds us that, "Disorders of fear processing…are among the most common of psychiatric maladies, affecting hundreds of millions of people worldwide. Combined, they are also among the highest in terms of morbidity, loss of work, comorbid psychiatric and medical disorders, and mortality from suicide." Yet the neural systems that control the expression and regulation of fear remain incompletely understood. Considerable progress has been made using animal models, but the relevance of these tantalizing discoveries to the complexities of the human brain and human suffering remain unclear. Human fear conditioning studies represent a crucial means of the addressing this question, yet as Lonsdorf and colleagues make clear in this submission, the inferential impact of such studies is limited by important (but seemingly mundane) methodological issues. In particular, they highlight substantial heterogeneity in exclusion criteria for such studies-who does and who does not adequately learn the association between cues (CS+) and consequences (shocks; UCS). Based on this, they propose evidence-based clear guidelines for conducting and reporting fear conditioning studies.

All 3 reviewers were very enthusiastic about this submission.

• The manuscript has multiple strengths, in my opinion, including the clear writing; the focus on an obvious (to many but not all) problem in a commonly used and published paradigm; the authors' preregistration of analysis; careful and clear presentation of data; and potentially useful suggestions for improvement for the issue at hand. I believe that papers such as this one are long overdue for many subfields. Given the recently increased focus on the credibility of psychological science, this minor is especially timely.

• This is an important and consequential piece of work. The authors provide an extremely important service by surveying the current state of the literature and using an empirical approach to identify potential pitfalls with the current lack of standards and transparency. Fear conditioning is a very commonly used technique. Thus, core decisions, such as which participants to include or not include has the potential to have substantial impact on this literature, and thus for theories of learning and memory, their application to clinical problems such as anxiety and PTSD, and potentially development and improvement of interventions based on conditioning models. The topic of this paper is of clear significance to the field. In addition to significance I also found the approach used by the authors to be rigorous. The sample of papers included was sizeable and systematic. The criteria used in each paper were clearly detailed. The datasets they used to demonstrate the impact of these different exclusion criteria included large samples, minimizing spurious effects. These analyses included many useful contributions, including the dramatic loss of sample with some of the exclusion criteria, how some learning criteria are particularly impactful with respect to individual variation in magnitude of SCR response and trait anxiety (as examples), and the lack of convergence with other measures of learning (e.g., ratings). The data demonstrating the particular utility of identifying non-responders based on the US, but not the CSs, were clearly presented and compelling. The table of recommendations (Box 1) is for the most part extremely comprehensive and useful guidance for scientists in designing studies and analyzing and reporting data and methods. The suggestions for transparency in reporting and not basing identification of non-response on CSs are notably important.

• This is clearly an important topic in a field with little consensus regarding best practices around inclusion/exclusion criteria, experimental design, analysis strategies, etc. They did a good job of identifying and operationally defining the problem under study, and summarizing the literature around the problem. This combined with the thorough re-analysis of their existing data make for a compelling read and convincing case for reform.

The reviewers also outlined some important suggestions for strengthening the report, all of which can be addressed in a revision with a reasonable amount of effort by the authors.

0) The authors should strongly consider editing the title to something shorter and more direct. At present, it reads like 2-3 ideas "mashed" together.

1) The analysis of SCR discrimination as a function of "non-learning" criteria (subsection “Statistical analysis”, subsection “Applying the identified performance-based exclusion criteria to existing data: a case example”, last paragraph, Supplementary Table 3). As I understand it, the authors stratify subjects based on their CS+>CS- difference score, with lower or negative difference scores signifying non-learners. They then calculate for each group in this stratification whether or not the individuals show evidence of learning by looking at differential SCRs and fear ratings. From this, they conclude "Fourth, we provided empirical evidence that those classified as 'non-learners' (sometimes referred to as 'outliers') in SCRs based on the identified definitions ironically displayed significant CS+/CS- discrimination." In the case of differential SCRs, this argument is circular, meaning that the group definitions are dependent upon the outcome measure. Accordingly, exclusion groups will by definition differ from one another in terms of differential SCRs. In addition, exclusion groups with a lower bound > 0 will by definition show significant differential conditioning using SCRs as an outcome. This is problematic, and should be removed from the manuscript entirely. In addition, any conclusions based on this analysis should be rewritten to focus specifically on the data from the fear ratings.

2) In terms of significance for the field, I would specifically like to highlight the author's focus on characterizing the non-learners. While a small group (ideally) these individuals are usually removed from analyses and thus are not well-understood. Thus, as a field we have little understanding of the nature of non-learners. It is very possible that we are missing out on an important opportunity to understand why some individuals don't learn and what the impact of that is, for better or for worse. As an example, what does it mean of fear extinction and generalization of conditioned fear is evident in those who "didn't learn?" This may be a methodological issue, but potentially also has meaningful conceptual implications.

3) In Box 1, Section B, under "Recommendations on how to proceed" for "if 'non-learning' is determined by responding during fear acquisition training, which trial types and number of trials per trial type were considered?" the authors state "classification in SCR 'non-learners' should be based on differential scores (CS+ vs. CS-)." This leaves a lot of wiggle room. Is it possible to be more specific? This still leaves a lot of room for decisions that may result in a biased sample (such as those anxious individuals who also show pretty large responses to the CS-). And what about the number of trials? It seems that at minimal a justification of these choices should be included.

4) The authors remark in a few places that SCR is not, and should not be, the only marker of learning. This begs the question of which dependent variables should be used, how they should be treated, and how they should (or could) be combined. I realize the paper is already long, however, this could benefit from some additional brief commentary. How might researchers choose amongst dependent variables and what might they be optimal for. Would they be weighted equally? For example, some variables may be more relevant for assessing implicit vs. explicit learning. Other variables may introduce new wrinkles – for example fear potentiated startle relies on introducing another aversive stimulus (e.g., noise burst, air puff) in addition to the US. How might this impact learning? Especially fear learning?

5) I am concerned that the many important messages in this manuscript may get lost to many less careful readers due to the wealth of information presented. On one hand, I applaud the authors for being transparent and forthcoming, but I wonder if there are ways to further focus the manuscript. I believe reduction in the overall length (perhaps partly through another careful round of editing to avoid any unnecessary redundancy) would strengthen the impact of the manuscript.

6) One relevant issue not addressed in the manuscript is the rampant inconsistency of exclusionary criteria WITHIN a given lab or set of researchers. If the authors encountered this in their search, it may be worth adding a note about it. If not, I wonder if it would be worth speculating about the meaning of a given researcher repeatedly uses the same paradigm with similar samples multiple times while changing the exclusionary criteria with no justification. This could at least be addressed in the suggestions for improvements in common practices.

7) Given the proliferation of "turn key" systems and rapid adoption of affordable, (e.g., mobile) devices for measuring of psychophysiological signals, many of which have not been sufficiently vetted for their reliability, I believe a slightly greater emphasis (not necessarily much more words but perhaps stronger language)on the potential differences in exclusion of participants based on specific amplifier systems used is warranted. I would predict that some modern systems with their decreased sampling rates and other features might do a poorer job of registering smaller responses and thus might lead to more exclusions if using criteria adopted when using more robust systems. Perhaps this is worth its own study and such could be suggested as a future direction of this work?

8) Given research on differences in skin conductance responses based on sensory domain (e.g., visual versus tactile) I am not sure I agree with the authors' recommendation of using responses to the US as exclusionary criteria in these paradigms. In other words, a given participant may reliability respond to tactile stimuli but not visual stimuli.

9) Regarding recommendations and future directions: there have been calls by some researchers (e.g., https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12481758) and answers by others (e.g., https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4715694/ ) to use manipulations and tests specifically designed to empirically demonstrate proper quantification of measures such as startle. I wonder if something similar would be helpful to determine the proper exclusionary criteria for SCR responses. For example, a manipulation designed vary potential fear conditioning learning (e.g., lesser or greater reinforcement) could be covaried with a manipulation designed to increase or decrease general skin conductance response (e.g., pharmacological or temperature variations). If the authors believe such a discussion of this is beyond the scope of the current manuscript, I would differ to their judgment.

10) I believe it would be helpful for the authors to briefly define "non-responder" on its first use in the paper (Introduction) as this term may be confused with the aforementioned "non-learners" in the same paragraph.

11) Over 400 papers were excluded for having an "irrelevant topic". I may have missed it, but I did not see any examples given of what an "irrelevant topic" was. Knowing this may help readers understand the exclusions.

12) Some spots in the supplementary materials would benefit from just a bit of text to explain the figures, rather than referring back to the main paper (for example Appendix 4—table 2).

13) It's worth considering moving Supplementary Figure 3 to the main text. While this figure is depicting just one of many decisions that may impact subject exclusion, it's a commonly used method, and the graphic depiction of the impact of the heterogeneity of these choices is quite powerful.

14) Because of their import I think it would be helpful to (briefly) more thoroughly describe the ways in which the criteria for non-learners and non-responders (Results) varied across studies. For example, it would be easier to follow if the authors explained a bit more what was meant by "Definitions differed in 1) the experimental (sub-)phases…" and so on.

15) The authors state that this work contributes to "raising awareness to the problems of performance-based exclusion of participants ('non-learners')." This seems a bit too general. The problem isn't necessarily the use of performance-based exclusion, but how that exclusion is done.

16) The authors state, "We have empirically illustrated that most definitions of 'non-learners' fail this manipulation check." Given the large number of findings and points in the manuscript, it would be helpful in the Discussion if statements like this included more detail and context to remind the reader of the finding.

17) In Box 1, General Reporting, under "recommendations for how to proceed" for "minimal response criterion (μS) to define a valid SCR," it would help to have more detail about the means by which an empirical cutoff can be determined.

[Editors' note: further revisions were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for resubmitting your work entitled "Navigating the garden of forking paths for data exclusions in fear conditioning research" for further consideration by eLife. Your revised article has been evaluated by Floris de Lange as the Senior Editor and Alex Shackman as the Reviewing Editor.

I am very happy to accept your paper for revision pending receipt of a revision that addresses, however you see fit, one residual suggestion from the reviewers:

"My main concern in the initial version of the manuscript was the differential SCRs to stratify the sample into exclusion groups, which then are used in an analysis where differential SCRs are the dependent measure (See point 1 in revision), and the conclusion that the authors "provided empirical evidence that those classified as 'non-learners' in SCRs in the literature (sometimes referred to as 'outliers') in SCRs based on the identified definitions ironically displayed significant CS+/CS- discrimination." Although the authors added a caveat to the methods, I find that the conclusions drawn in the sixth paragraph of the subsection “‘Non-learners’: conclusions, caveats and considerations” should be based on a stronger statistical framework. My major point is that it is statistically impossible to not find statistically significant SCR differentiation in an exclusion group who's CS+ > CS- lower bound is > 0. That being said, I understand that the field has used arbitrarily defined cutoffs that could lead to some learners misclassified. I just wonder if there is a statistically stronger approach to make this point. To be clear, I think this is an important piece of work with an impactful message that I would sincerely like to endorse for publication. However, I also understand that this message could be controversial to some, and I would like the authors to present this work in such a way that it can stand up to scrutiny from those that may disagree with the message.

My recommendation would be for the authors to dramatically soften the conclusions in the aforementioned subsection and acknowledge this limitation of their approach in the Discussion section.

Alternatively, if the authors had another method for estimating the number of subjects that each cutoff criteria likely misclassifies (e.g. perhaps by using the error variance of the entire sample as a stand-in for single-subject confidence intervals), these data could be used to support the conclusions mentioned above."

eLife. 2019 Dec 16;8:e52465. doi: 10.7554/eLife.52465.sa2

Author response


The reviewers also outlined some important suggestions for strengthening the report, all of which can be addressed in a revision with a reasonable amount of effort by the authors.

0) The authors should strongly consider editing the title to something shorter and more direct. At present, it reads like 2-3 ideas "mashed" together.

We have changed the title to “Navigating the garden of forking paths for data exclusions in fear conditioning research”.

1) The analysis of SCR discrimination as a function of "non-learning" criteria (subsection “Statistical analysis”, subsection “Applying the identified performance-based exclusion criteria to existing data: a case example”, last paragraph, Supplementary Table 3). As I understand it, the authors stratify subjects based on their CS+>CS- difference score, with lower or negative difference scores signifying non-learners.

We thank the reviewers for highlighting that this issue was not clear. We grouped subjects based on all the different cutoffs in SCR CS+/CS- discrimination (listed in Appendix 2—table 1, section A in the revised version) as identified from the literature search. More precisely, we did not classify individuals with a discrimination score of < 0 µS or = 0 µS as ‘non-learners’. What we did is to classify individuals based on the cutoff criteria identified from the literature and then test if these resulting ‘exclusion groups’ showed evidence of learning (i.e., significant discrimination) or non-learning (non-significant discrimination) in our data.

They then calculate for each group in this stratification whether or not the individuals show evidence of learning by looking at differential SCRs and fear ratings. From this, they conclude "Fourth, we provided empirical evidence that those classified as 'non-learners' (sometimes referred to as 'outliers') in SCRs based on the identified definitions ironically displayed significant CS+/CS- discrimination." In the case of differential SCRs, this argument is circular, meaning that the group definitions are dependent upon the outcome measure. Accordingly, exclusion groups will by definition differ from one another in terms of differential SCRs. In addition, exclusion groups with a lower bound > 0 will by definition show significant differential conditioning using SCRs as an outcome. This is problematic, and should be removed from the manuscript entirely. In addition, any conclusions based on this analysis should be rewritten to focus specifically on the data from the fear ratings.

We agree that the different exclusion groups, which are defined by different SCR CS+/CS- discrimination cutoff scores, can be expected to differ in SCR CS+/CS- discrimination and we also agree that this argument is somewhat circular. We have thus removed part A and B of the table (results of the ANOVAs testing for differences in SCR CS+/CS- discrimination between the exclusion groups). Yet, the fact that some or even most of these “exclusion groups” did show significant CS+/CS- discrimination in SCRs is a major problem in our opinion, as these individuals are excluded from analyses as ‘non-learners’ in the literature. Hence, we think it is an important message that there is in fact strong evidence for learning in SCRs in these groups that are referred to as ‘non-learners’. ‘Non-learners’ should not demonstrate significant discrimination per definition. The exclusion groups, except for those with cutoffs < 0 µS and = 0 µS, all showed significant SCR discrimination in our data as shown in Appendix 2—table 1. Consequentially, they showed evidence of learning in SCRs. We have made this point clearer in the Materials and methods section, the caption to Appendix 2—table 1 and revised the wording of our conclusions.

“To test whether exclusion groups differ in CS+/CS- discrimination, a mixed ANOVA with CS+/CS- discrimination in SCR or fear ratings as dependent variable and the between-subjects factor ‘Exclusion group’ and the within-subject factor ‘CS-type’ was performed. Note that it is circular to test for differences in SCR CS+/CS- discrimination between groups that were selected based on different SCR CS+/CS- discrimination cutoffs in the first place. Still, it is relevant to test whether all groups classified as ‘non-learners’ in the literature indeed fail to show evidence of learning, which would be indicated by a lack of significant CS+/CS- discrimination in SCRs in this case. In essence, this is a test to evaluate whether exclusion criteria used in the literature indeed achieve what they purport to do, that is, classify a group of participants that do not show evidence of learning.”

Appendix 2—table 1: “In this case example based on data set 1 (see main manuscript), we tested whether CS+/CS- discrimination in SCRs indeed differs between the different exclusion groups as defined by the cutoffs retrieved from the literature (see Figure 2B). […] Still, this is an important manipulation check to empirically test whether those classified as ‘non-learners’ in the literature indeed do not show evidence of learning, which would be indicated by comparable SCRs to the CS+ and the CS- (i.e., no significant discrimination).”

Results: “Unsurprisingly, the exclusion group defined by a CS+/CS- discrimination cutoff < 0 µS showed inverse discrimination (CS- > CS+, not significant in raw SCRs: p = 0.117; significant in log, rc SCRs: p = 0.021). Strikingly and more importantly, with the exception of the group formed by defining ‘non-learning’ as a discrimination score of 0 µS (light green), all other exclusion groups, which have been established by defining classified as ‘non-learners’ by the different cutoffs in the literature, showed significantly larger CS+ than CS- SCR amplitudes (i.e., significant CS+/CS- discrimination; raw: all p’s < 0.0009; log, rc: all p’s < 0.0002; see Appendix 2 –Table 1 for details).”

Discussion: “Fourth, we provided empirical evidence that those classified as ‘non-learners’ in the literature (sometimes referred to as ‘outliers’) in SCRs based on the identified definitions ironically displayed significant CS+/CS- discrimination – with the exception of non-learners defined by a cut-off in differential responding of < 0 and = 0 µS. Hence, in addition to the many conceptual problems we raised here, the operationalization of ‘non-learning’ in the field failed the its critical manipulation check, given that those classified as ‘non-learners’ show clear evidence of learning as a group (i.e., CS+/CS- discrimination, see Appendix 2—table 1).”

2) In terms of significance for the field, I would specifically like to highlight the author's focus on characterizing the non-learners. While a small group (ideally) these individuals are usually removed from analyses and thus are not well-understood. Thus, as a field we have little understanding of the nature of non-learners. It is very possible that we are missing out on an important opportunity to understand why some individuals don't learn and what the impact of that is, for better or for worse. As an example, what does it mean of fear extinction and generalization of conditioned fear is evident in those who "didn't learn?" This may be a methodological issue, but potentially also has meaningful conceptual implications.

We thank the reviewers for highlighting this important point, which we have now included in our Discussion. Yet, we would like to highlight that non-discrimination based on a single read-out measure (such as SCRs) cannot be taken as evidence for non-learning. We, however, agree that characterizing individuals that do not discriminate may be highly relevant.

Discussion: “Furthermore, by excluding these individuals from further analyses, we may miss the opportunity to understand why some individuals do not show discrimination between the CS+ and the CS- in SCRs (or other outcome measures) or whether this lack of discrimination is maintained across subsequent experimental phases. It can be speculated that this lack of discrimination may carry meaningful information – at least for a subsample.”

3) In Box 1, Section B, under "Recommendations on how to proceed" for "if 'non-learning' is determined by responding during fear acquisition training, which trial types and number of trials per trial type were considered?" the authors state "classification in SCR 'non-learners' should be based on differential scores (CS+ vs. CS-)." This leaves a lot of wiggle room. Is it possible to be more specific? This still leaves a lot of room for decisions that may result in a biased sample (such as those anxious individuals who also show pretty large responses to the CS-). And what about the number of trials? It seems that at minimal a justification of these choices should be included.

We thank the reviewers for pointing at this issue and have provided more details as suggested. Since it is very difficult to provide a universally applicable recommendation on the number of trials to be included, we follow the suggestion to provide a justification of the choices.

Box 1, section B: “classification as SCR ‘non-learners’ should be based on differential scores (CS+ vs. CS-) and the number of trials included for this calculation should be clearly justified. Providing a generally valid recommendation regarding the number of trials to be included is difficult, since it critically depends on experimental design choices.”

4) The authors remark in a few places that SCR is not, and should not be, the only marker of learning. This begs the question of which dependent variables should be used, how they should be treated, and how they should (or could) be combined. I realize the paper is already long, however, this could benefit from some additional brief commentary. How might researchers choose amongst dependent variables and what might they be optimal for. Would they be weighted equally? For example, some variables may be more relevant for assessing implicit vs. explicit learning. Other variables may introduce new wrinkles – for example fear potentiated startle relies on introducing another aversive stimulus (e.g., noise burst, air puff) in addition to the US. How might this impact learning? Especially fear learning?

We absolutely agree with the reviewers that it would be highly desirable to present a universally valid gold standard of how to quantify learning and the reviewer provides some interesting suggestions. We believe that this is an empirical question that future research needs to address. We have included these thoughts in the Discussion of our revised manuscript.

Discussion: “Furthermore, future work should empirically address the question of how to best define ‘non-learning’ in particular in light of different outcome measures in fear conditioning studies, which capture different aspects of defensive responding (Lonsdorf et al., 2017; Spielberger, Gorsuch and Lushene, 1983)”

With respect to the potential impact of startle on the fear learning process, in the revised manuscript, we now refer to our previous publication (Sjouwerman et al., 2016) that addressed this question empirically. Indeed, the reviewer is correct in assuming that inclusion of startle as an outcome measure (which involves the presentation of an aversive stimulus to trigger the startle response) slows down fear learning.

Discussion: “Procedural differences, in particular the inclusion of outcome measures that require certain triggers to elicit a response (such as startle responses or ratings), have also been shown to impact on the learning process itself (Sjouwerman et al., 2016).”

5) I am concerned that the many important messages in this manuscript may get lost to many less careful readers due to the wealth of information presented. On one hand, I applaud the authors for being transparent and forthcoming, but I wonder if there are ways to further focus the manuscript. I believe reduction in the overall length (perhaps partly through another careful round of editing to avoid any unnecessary redundancy) would strengthen the impact of the manuscript.

We thank the reviewer for bringing forward this concern and have re-read our work carefully. We have shortened and focused the content wherever we saw fit. In addition, we inserted sub-headings to the Discussion to facilitate comprehensibility and guide the readers to the points most relevant to their interests:

“‘Non-learners’: conclusions, caveats and considerations”;

“‘Non-responders’: conclusions, caveats and considerations”;

“Where do we go from here?”;

“Final remarks”.

6) One relevant issue not addressed in the manuscript is the rampant inconsistency of exclusionary criteria within a given lab or set of researchers. If the authors encountered this in their search, it may be worth adding a note about it. If not, I wonder if it would be worth speculating about the meaning of a given researcher repeatedly uses the same paradigm with similar samples multiple times while changing the exclusionary criteria with no justification. This could at least be addressed in the suggestions for improvements in common practices.

The reviewers are absolutely correct to note that there is inconsistent use of criteria not only between but also within research groups and that typically no explicit justification for this is provided. While we are well aware of this, we have deliberately refrained from pointing this out explicitly in our manuscript. We see the major contribution of our work by providing a constructive and evidence-based discussion. Identifying specific individuals or groups for using inconsistent criteria across different publications (and speculating about the underlying reasons) would, in our view, not add anything to the message we want to be heard. We have, however, followed the reviewer’s recommendation to include a note about this in our suggestions. (See Box 1, section B).

7) Given the proliferation of "turn key" systems and rapid adoption of affordable, (e.g., mobile) devices for measuring of psychophysiological signals, many of which have not been sufficiently vetted for their reliability, I believe a slightly greater emphasis (not necessarily much more words but perhaps stronger language)on the potential differences in exclusion of participants based on specific amplifier systems used is warranted. I would predict that some modern systems with their decreased sampling rates and other features might do a poorer job of registering smaller responses and thus might lead to more exclusions if using criteria adopted when using more robust systems. Perhaps this is worth its own study and such could be suggested as a future direction of this work?

We agree with the reviewers that this is a relevant implication for our work and included this in our revised Discussion.

“This being said, we do acknowledge that certain research questions or the use of different recording equipment (robust lab equipment vs. novel mobile devices such as smartwatches) may potentially require distinct data processing pipelines and potentially also exclusion of certain observations (Simmons, Nelson and Simonsohn, 2011; Silberzahn et al., 2018), hence it is not desirable to propose rigid and fixed rules for generic adoption.”

“It is difficult to suggest a universally valid cutoff with respect to the number or percentage of required valid US responses as this critically depends on a number of variables such as hardware and sampling rate used. It remains an open question for future work if data quality of novel mobile devices (e.g., smartwatches) for the acquisition of SCRs differs from traditional, robust, lab-based recordings and how this would impact on the frequency of exclusions based on SCRs.”

8) Given research on differences in skin conductance responses based on sensory domain (e.g., visual versus tactile) I am not sure I agree with the authors' recommendation of using responses to the US as exclusionary criteria in these paradigms. In other words, a given participant may reliability respond to tactile stimuli but not visual stimuli.

We agree that different sensory domains may induce SCRs of different amplitudes (e.g., Etzi and Gallace, 2016; ). In the latter study, tactile stimuli indeed induced larger SCRs with larger amplitudes compared to visual stimuli. Yet, we are not aware of empirical work showing that individuals that reliably respond to tactile stimuli do not reliably respond to visual stimuli or vice versa and hence, this would need to be tested empirically as it seems to be a hypothetical assumption as it stands now.

In our manuscript, we argue that individuals cannot be classified as ‘non-responders’ if they show SCRs to any stimulus – irrespective of stimulus modality or sensory domain. In fear conditioning experiments, the tactile US is the strongest stimulus used. We argue that individuals can be classified as ‘non-responders’ if they do not reliably respond to this strongest stimulus – which reliably elicits SCRs with much larger amplitudes and higher response frequencies than the visual CSs. As there is no empirical work on this topic, we have included Appendix 4—table 1 and 2 which illustrate non-responding rates across different stimuli (CS+, CS-, US) across twenty different experiments. These data illustrate that we can rather reliably infer ‘non-responding’ to the CSs from ‘non-responding’ to the US but not vice versa. We believe that given the above outlined arguments, our recommendation is empirically based. We added further explanations to the Discussion:

“In sum, while excluding physiological ‘non-responders’ makes sense (in terms of a manipulation check and independent of the hypothesis), we consider defining ‘non-responders’ based on the absence of SCRs to the CS as problematic (dependent on the hypothesis).”

9) Regarding recommendations and future directions: there have been calls by some researchers (e.g., https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12481758) and answers by others (e.g., https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4715694/ ) to use manipulations and tests specifically designed to empirically demonstrate proper quantification of measures such as startle. I wonder if something similar would be helpful to determine the proper exclusionary criteria for SCR responses. For example, a manipulation designed vary potential fear conditioning learning (e.g., lesser or greater reinforcement) could be covaried with a manipulation designed to increase or decrease general skin conductance response (e.g., pharmacological or temperature variations). If the authors believe such a discussion of this is beyond the scope of the current manuscript, I would differ to their judgment.

We thank the reviewers for bringing this work to our attention. We have included a discussion on this in our revised manuscript as a suggestion for future studies.

Discussion: “Sixth, as illustrated by our case example (Figure 3), high CS+/CS- discrimination cutoffs generally favor individuals with high SCR amplitudes despite potentially identical ratios between CS+ and CS- amplitudes, which may introduce a sampling bias for individuals characterized by high arousal levels likely linked to biological underpinnings. Relatedly, future studies need to empirically address which criteria for SCR transformation and exclusions are more or less sensitive to baseline differences (for an example from startle responding see Bradford et al., 2015; Grillon et al., 2002).”

10) I believe it would be helpful for the authors to briefly define "non-responder" on its first use in the paper (Introduction) as this term may be confused with the aforementioned "non-learners" in the same paragraph.

We agree that it might be helpful to define ‘non-responders’ in the beginning of the manuscript – yet it is quite difficult, since the definitions differ across studies as outlined in our manuscript. Thus, we have included a definition in our revised manuscript that is somewhat vague but can be used as an umbrella term for the definitions found in the literature.

“Therefore, participants are often (routinely) excluded from analyses if they appear to not have not learned (‘non-learners’) or not have responded been responsive to the experimental stimuli (‘non-responders’) during fear acquisition training”.

11) Over 400 papers were excluded for having an "irrelevant topic". I may have missed it, but I did not see any examples given of what an "irrelevant topic" was. Knowing this may help readers understand the exclusions.

We thank the reviewer for highlighting this topic. We have now provided the relevant details in the revised manuscript and on OSF.

Legend of Figure 1: “Examples of irrelevant topics included studies that did not use fear conditioning paradigms (see https://osf.io/uxdhk/ for a documentation of excluded publications).”

12) Some spots in the supplementary materials would benefit from just a bit of text to explain the figures, rather than referring back to the main paper (for example Appendix 4—table 2).

We thank the reviewer for highlighting this issue and have followed the recommendation to provide more details in the appendix itself. Please note however, that the journal asked us to make substantial changes as it does not accept supplements. As such, the supplement is now split into 4 different appendices and figure supplements to main figures according to journal style.

Appendix 2—table 1: Results for 1.1 Applying the identified performance-based exclusion criteria to existing data: a case example

“In this case example based on data set 1 (see main manuscript), we tested whether CS+/CS- discrimination in SCRs indeed differs between the different exclusion groups as defined by the cutoffs retrieved from the literature (see Figure 2B). […] Still, this is an important manipulation check to empirically test whether those classified as ‘non-learners’ in the literature indeed do not show evidence of learning, which would be indicated by comparable SCRs to the CS+ and the CS- (i.e., no significant discrimination).”

Figure 3—figure supplement 1: “Title: Percentages of participants excluded (data-set 1) when employing the different CS+/CS- discrimination cutoffs (as identified by the systematic literature search and graphically shown in Figure 3B) which are illustrated as density plots in Figure 3. […] Note that the different groups are cumulative (i.e., the darker colored groups also comprise the lighter colored groups).”

Appendix 3: Results for: 1.32. Exploratory analyses on consistency of classification (‘learners’ vs. ‘non-learners’) across outcome measures and criteria employed

Throughout the main manuscript and particularly in the Discussion, we highlight that differential (CS+>CS-) SCRs alone cannot be taken to infer ‘learning’ (Figure 4—figure supplement 1).”

Figure 4—figure supplement 1

“Title: Bar plots (mean ± SE) with superimposed individual data-points showing CS+ and CS- amplitudes (raw SCR values) and CS+/CS- discrimination in (A) fear ratings and (B) SCRs raw values in the group of ‘non-learners’ as exemplarily defined for this example as a group consisting of individuals in the two lowest SCR CS+/CS- discrimination cutoff groups (i.e., ≤ 0) in data set 1. This illustrates that individuals failing to show CS+/CS- discrimination in SCRs (B), may in fact show substantial CS+/CS- discrimination (as an indicator for successful learning) in other outcome measures as exemplarily illustrated here for fear ratings (A).”

Appendix 4: Appendix 4 – Information for: Definition of ‘non-responders ‘and amount of participants excluded across studies

“In the main manuscript, we discuss different frequencies of ‘non-responding’ to different experimental stimuli (e.g., US, CS+ and CS- or in isolation or in combination) which inherently leads to different exclusion frequencies when classifying “non-responders” based on different types of stimuli. […] In addition, Appendix 4—table 2 provides information on the number and percentage of individuals in a sample showing SCR ‘non-responses’ to a certain number of US presentations during fear acquisition training as well as mean number and percentage of CS responses (CS refers to the CS+ and CS- combined) in these individuals to guide the development of empirically-based criteria to define SCR ‘non-responders’.”

13) It's worth considering moving Supplementary Figure 3 to the main text. While this figure is depicting just one of many decisions that may impact subject exclusion, it's a commonly used method, and the graphic depiction of the impact of the heterogeneity of these choices is quite powerful.

We have followed the reviewers’ suggestion and moved this figure to the main manuscript (now Figure 4).

14) Because of their import I think it would be helpful to (briefly) more thoroughly describe the ways in which the criteria for non-learners and non-responders (Results) varied across studies. For example, it would be easier to follow if the authors explained a bit more what was meant by "Definitions differed in 1) the experimental (sub-)phases…" and so on.

We have revised the respective sections accordingly:

Results: “Definitions differed in i) the experimental (sub-)phases to which they were applied (i.e., whether the full phase or only the first half, second half or even single trials were considered), ii) the number of trials that the exclusion was based on (varying between one and eight single trials), iii) the CS+/CS- discrimination cutoff applied (varying between <0 µS and < 0.1 µS) as well as iv) the CS+ type (only non-reinforced or all CS+ trials) considered for the definition. […] Of note, the number of trials included in the same ‘phase’ category is contingent on the experimental design and hence does not represent a homogeneous category (‘last half’ may include 5 trials for one study (i.e., 10 trials in total) but 10 trials for a different study employing 20 trials in total).”

Results: “The definitions differed in i) the stimulus type(s) used to define ‘non-responding’ (CS+ reinforced, CS+ unreinforced, all CS+s, CS-, US), ii) the SCR minimum amplitude criterion to define a responder (varying between 0.01 µS and 0.05 µS, or mere visual inspection), and iii) the percentage of trials for which these criteria have to be met (see Figure 6B and Appendix 1—figure 1) as well as a combination thereof.

‘Non-responding’ was most commonly defined as not showing a sufficient number of responses to the conditioned stimuli (CS+ and CS-), less frequently by the absence of responses to the US, or any stimulus (CS+, CS- or US) and in two cases by the absence of responses to the CS+ or context (CXT+) specifically (see Figure 6B).”

15) The authors state that this work contributes to "raising awareness to the problems of performance-based exclusion of participants ('non-learners')." This seems a bit too general. The problem isn't necessarily the use of performance-based exclusion, but how that exclusion is done.

We agree with the reviewers that it is not only the exclusion per se but also its implementation that is problematic and have revised this sentence accordingly:

Discussion: “Together, we believe that this work contributes to i) raising awareness to some of the problems associated with performance-based exclusion of participants (‘non-learners’) and how this is implemented […]”

16) The authors state, "We have empirically illustrated that most definitions of 'non-learners' fail this manipulation check." Given the large number of findings and points in the manuscript, it would be helpful in the Discussion if statements like this included more detail and context to remind the reader of the finding.

We thank the reviewers for pointing this out. We inserted cross-references to the relevant figures, tables and the Appendix for every discussion point to facilitate comprehensibility and to act as reminders of the respective finding. In addition, we provide more details in our revised manuscript and sub-headings as follows:

Discussion: “Fourth, we provided empirical evidence that those classified as ‘non-learners’ in SCRs in the literature (sometimes referred to as ‘outliers’) based on the identified definitions ironically displayed significant CS+/CS- discrimination – except when using cutoffs in differential responding of < 0 µS and = 0 µS. Hence, in addition to the many conceptual problems we raised here, the operationalization of ‘non-learning’ in the field failed its critical manipulation check, given that those classified as ‘non-learners’ showed clear evidence of learning (i.e., CS+/CS- discrimination, see Appendix 2—table 1).”

“‘Non-learners’: conclusions, caveats and considerations”;

“‘Non-responders’: conclusions, caveats and considerations”;

“Where do we go from here?”;

“Final remarks”.

7) In Box 1, General Reporting, under "recommendations for how to proceed" for "minimal response criterion (μS) to define a valid SCR," it would help to have more detail about the means by which an empirical cutoff can be determined.

We thank the reviewer for bringing this imprecision to our attention and have specified this in the revised manuscript:

Box 1, Section A:

“test different minimal response criteria in the data set and define the cutoff empirically. In our experience (data set 1), a cutoff was easily determined empirically by visually inspecting exemplary SCRs at different cutoffs (e.g., < 0.01 µS, between 0.01 µS and 0.02 µS) and evaluating their discrimination from noise.”

[Editors' note: further revisions were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

I am very happy to accept your paper for revision pending receipt of a revision that addresses, however you see fit, one residual suggestion from the reviewers:

"My main concern in the initial version of the manuscript was the differential SCRs to stratify the sample into exclusion groups, which then are used in an analysis where differential SCRs are the dependent measure (See point 1 in revision), and the conclusion that the authors "provided empirical evidence that those classified as 'non-learners' in SCRs in the literature (sometimes referred to as 'outliers') in SCRs based on the identified definitions ironically displayed significant CS+/CS- discrimination." Although the authors added a caveat to the methods, I find that the conclusions drawn in the sixth paragraph of the subsection “‘Non-learners’: conclusions, caveats and considerations” should be based on a stronger statistical framework. My major point is that it is statistically impossible to not find statistically significant SCR differentiation in an exclusion group who's CS+ > CS- lower bound is > 0. That being said, I understand that the field has used arbitrarily defined cutoffs that could lead to some learners misclassified. I just wonder if there is a statistically stronger approach to make this point. To be clear, I think this is an important piece of work with an impactful message that I would sincerely like to endorse for publication. However, I also understand that this message could be controversial to some, and I would like the authors to present this work in such a way that it can stand up to scrutiny from those that may disagree with the message.

My recommendation would be for the authors to dramatically soften the conclusions in the aforementioned subsection and acknowledge this limitation of their approach in the Discussion section.

Alternatively, if the authors had another method for estimating the number of subjects that each cutoff criteria likely misclassifies (e.g. perhaps by using the error variance of the entire sample as a stand-in for single-subject confidence intervals), these data could be used to support the conclusions mentioned above."

We thank the reviewer for making this clearer for us and proving this feedback in a very constructive way. We agree that it is statistically very unlikely to not find a significant effect in these artificial groups – yet not impossible. We have now replaced these analyses by more conservative analyses using cumulative groups rather than within-exclusion group analyses (see Appendix 2). It is important to note that our previous conclusions still stand, as most of the cumulative exclusion groups still show significant discrimination despite of consisting of a number of individuals with negative and zero discrimination as well (in other words despite of the lower bound not being >0). With respect to the alternative suggestion of the reviewer, we consider this rather difficult as the ground truth is unknown (what is a meaningful discrimination) and hence it is unclear how to define a misclassification. We hope the reviewer and editor agree on this approach.

We have also revised the parts of the manuscript that discussed the methods of our approach or its results.

Appendix 2:Results for 1.1 Applying the identified performance-based exclusion criteria to existing data: a case example

“In this case example based on data set 1 (see main manuscript), we tested whether CS+/CS- discrimination in SCRs indeed differs between the different exclusion groups as defined by the cutoffs retrieved from the literature (see Figure 2B). Note that this is somewhat circular as exclusion groups are defined by different SCR CS+/CS- cutoffs which then are used in an analysis where differential SCRs are the dependent measure. However, that this is exactly what is sometimes done in the literature (see main manuscript).

Still, this is an important manipulation check to empirically test whether those classified as a group of ‘non-learners’ in the literature indeed do not show evidence of learning, which would be indicated by comparable SCRs to the CS+ and the CS- (i.e., no significant discrimination). Here, we test this for cumulative exclusion groups. Note that this is only a rough manipulation check as a non-significant CS+/CS- discrimination effect in the whole group (e.g., those showing a CS+/CS- discrimination <0.05µS based on raw scores) cannot be taken as evidence that all individuals in this group do not display meaningful or statistically significant CS+/CS- discrimination. More precisely, half of this group not meeting the cut-off of 0.05µS in CS+/CS- discrimination do show a negative or zero discrimination score which may bias the group average score towards non-discrimination. Yet, statistically testing for discrimination within each exclusion group (e.g. specifically in the group showing a discrimination between > 0 and < 0.05 µS) is statistically not unproblematic.”

“Strikingly and more importantly, most cumulative exclusion groups, as established by defining ‘non-learners’ by the different CS+/CS- discrimination cutoffs in SCRs in the literature, in fact show statistically significant CS+/CS- discrimination (see Appendix 2 for details and a brief discussion).”

Furthermore, we provide case-examples illustrating i) the futility of some definitions of ‘non-learners’ (i.e., when those classified as ‘non-learners’ in fact show significant discrimination on both ratings and SCRs as illustrated in Appendix 3 and Appendix 2 respectively) when applied to our data”.

“Fourth, we provided empirical evidence that those classified as a group of ‘non-learners’ in SCRs in the literature (sometimes referred to as ‘outliers’) based on the identified definitions in fact displayed significant CS+/CS- discrimination when applied to our own data. […] Hence, in addition to the many conceptual problems we raised here, the operationalization of ‘non-learning’ in the field failed its critical manipulation check given that those classified as ‘non-learners’ show clear evidence of learning as a group (i.e., CS+/CS- discrimination, see Appendix 2—table 1).”

“To test whether these exclusion groups discriminated in SCRs and fear ratings, exclusion groups were cumulated, and t-tests were performed for each cumulative group (see Appendix 2 and 3 respectively). We however acknowledge that the absence of a statistically significant CS+/CS- discrimination effect in a group on average cannot be taken to imply that all individuals in this group do not show meaningful CS+/CS- discrimination. As such, this is a rather conservative test.”

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Tina B Lonsdorf, Maren Klingelhöfer-Jens. 2019. Data_and_code_dataset1. Open Science Framework. w9y8z
    2. Tina B Lonsdorf, Rachel Sjouwerman. 2019. Data_and_code_dataset2. Open Science Framework. 7c5ag

    Supplementary Materials

    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    The minimal data sets (data set 1 and data set 2, both represent re-analysis of existing data), which were analysed during the current study, as well as code for figure production are are available at OSF under https://osf.io/mkxqe/ and DOI: https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/MKXQE.

    The following datasets were generated:

    Tina B Lonsdorf, Maren Klingelhöfer-Jens. 2019. Data_and_code_dataset1. Open Science Framework. w9y8z

    Tina B Lonsdorf, Rachel Sjouwerman. 2019. Data_and_code_dataset2. Open Science Framework. 7c5ag


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES