Skip to main content
Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience logoLink to Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience
. 2014 Apr 16;9:160–171. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2014.04.001

Social cognition, face processing, and oxytocin receptor single nucleotide polymorphisms in typically developing children

Mylissa M Slane a,1,2, Laina G Lusk a,1, KB Boomer b, Abby E Hare c, Margaret K King c, David W Evans a,1,2,*
PMCID: PMC6989749  PMID: 24814480

Graphical abstract

graphic file with name fx1.jpg

Keywords: Social cognition, Oxytocin, Autism spectrum disorder, Typically developing children, Event-related potential-N170

Highlights

  • Behavioral, neural, and genetic markers of ASD are present in typical children.

  • Neural processes during face perception predict scores only for some SC measures.

  • Observed in isolation, OXTR SNPs also produce mixed results.

  • However, rs2254298(A/G) with rs53576(G/G) → consistent increased social impairment.

Abstract

Recent research has provided evidence of a link between behavioral measures of social cognition (SC) and neural and genetic correlates. Differences in face processing and variations in the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene have been associated with SC deficits and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) traits. Much work has examined the qualitative differences between those with ASD and typically developing (TD) individuals, but very little has been done to quantify the natural variation in ASD-like traits in the typical population. The present study examines this variation in TD children using a multidimensional perspective involving behavior assessment, neural electroencephalogram (EEG) testing, and OXTR genotyping. Children completed a series of neurocognitive assessments, provided saliva samples for sequencing, and completed a face processing task while connected to an EEG. No clear pattern emerged for EEG covariates or genotypes for individual OXTR single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs). However, SNPs rs2254298 and rs53576 consistently interacted such that the AG/GG allele combination of these SNPs was associated with poorer performance on neurocognitive measures. These results suggest that neither SNP in isolation is risk-conferring, but rather that the combination of rs2254298(A/G) and rs53576(G/G) confers a deleterious effect on SC across several neurocognitive measures.

1. Introduction

There is growing recognition that traits traditionally associated with neurodevelopmental disorders (NDDs) manifest as quantitative traits in the typical population (Constantino, 2011, Gillberg, 2010, Moreno-De-Luca et al., 2013). In this study, we examined quantitative behavioral and social-cognitive traits associated with NDDs and neuropsychiatric disorders (NPDs; e.g., autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and schizophrenia) in typically developing (TD) children and their parents. We correlated these quantitative traits with neural biomarkers of face processing and genetic variability in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR). Through this method, we extended the observed gene-brain-behavior links in ASD to the typical range of development, highlighting the continuities between clinical and normally distributed traits.

Social cognition (SC) is a broad, adaptive, developmental process that reflects an understanding of the actions, emotions, and intentions of oneself and others (Brothers, 1990, Frith and Frith, 2007, Frith and Frith, 2012, Pinkham et al., 2008). Deficits and delays in SC are common in ASD, even when accounting for overall cognitive ability (Baron-Cohen et al., 1997, Brent et al., 2004, Chan et al., 2011, Klin, 2000). Persons with ASD are more likely to misinterpret mental states, use fewer internal state terms when describing events (Beaumont and Newcombe, 2006, Brent et al., 2004, Castelli et al., 2002, Chan et al., 2011, Shimoni et al., 2012), and are delayed on tasks assessing the ability to impute mental states from images of human eyes (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001a). Subjects with ASD anthropomorphize moving geometric shapes less than TD subjects, most of whom readily infer social meaning based on the movement of these shapes (Klin, 2000). These reduced abilities also exist in other disorders such as schizophrenia (Bell et al., 2010), suggesting that difficulty inferring social meaning is symptomatic of a range of NDDs and NPDs.

SC has also been linked to functional neural correlates. Face perception tasks have been robustly linked to the activation of inferior temporal brain regions, specifically, the fusiform gyrus, which is less active in individuals with ASD during face perception tasks (Schultz et al., 2000, Schultz, 2005). Event-related potential (ERP) studies reveal that relative to TD subjects, individuals with ASD often produce a delayed N170 response to faces (McPartland et al., 2004) and a faster precursor N170 response to objects (Webb et al., 2006). Individuals with ASD also exhibit a muted inversion effect, as evidenced by little or no differences in N170 latencies to upright relative to inverted faces (McPartland et al., 2004). Even in individuals with no family history of ASD, increased atypical social behaviors are associated with reduced N170 amplitudes (Hileman et al., 2011). Similarly, adults that are classified as “introverted” failed to exhibit the inversion effect, whereas “extroverted” individuals demonstrated an inversion effect (Cheung et al., 2010). These findings suggest continuity between atypical social development and typical social processes, thereby highlighting the importance of examining a broader phenotype that extends to unaffected relatives of ASD probands, as well as to the general population.

Some symptoms associated with ASD are ameliorated by administration of oxytocin. Oxytocin is a hormone that is produced by the hypothalamus and acts as a neuromodulator in the brain by activating its receptor, OXTR, in neurons (Anagnostou et al., 2014). Continuous oxytocin infusion decreases the number and severity of repetitive behaviors in adults diagnosed with ASD (Hollander et al., 2003). Intranasal administration of oxytocin increases empathizing ability in males with low levels of social functioning (Bartz et al., 2010) and increases attention paid to the eyes in typical adult males (Guastella et al., 2008). Similarly, following inhalation of oxytocin, adults with ASD exhibited increased social reciprocity and trust and increased their total looking time at facial stimuli, specifically the eyes (Andari et al., 2010). Adults with ASD also exhibited increased ability to comprehend affective speech when given an infusion of oxytocin (Hollander et al., 2007). In children with ASD, intranasal administration of oxytocin has been found to significantly improve emotion recognition on the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test-Revised (RMET) (Guastella et al., 2010). In addition, intranasal oxytocin in children and adolescents with ASD has been shown to differentially affect brain activity in regions associated with reward and face perception, increasing activity during socially relevant judgments and decreasing activity during nonsocial judgments (Gordon et al., 2013).

While these social deficits are characteristic of ASD, it is important to note that much of the individual variation in social ability may be accounted for by subtle variations in single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) within the OXTR gene. Given that several SNPs within OXTR correlate to pair-bonding in humans (Walum et al., 2012) and attachment security in infants (Chen et al., 2011), it is not surprising that they have also been implicated in ASD (Campbell et al., 2011, Jacob et al., 2007, Liu et al., 2010, Wu et al., 2005). Of the thirty SNPs identified in OXTR, rs53576, rs237897, rs1042778, and rs2254298 have been most consistently associated with a variety of traits linked with ASD. In addition, these SNPs are common enough to be found in TD populations, making them an ideal choice for analysis (Bookheimer, 2012). Rs53576 has been associated with the functional connectivity of the hypothalamus (Tost et al., 2010, Wang et al., 2013), differences in maternal and empathic behavior (Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn, 2008, Rodrigues et al., 2009), decreases in optimism, self-esteem (Saphire-Bernstein et al., 2011), positive affect, non-verbal intelligence (Lucht et al., 2009), and empathy (Rodrigues et al., 2009). Rs237897 is strongly associated with facial recognition memory in families with a single child with ASD (Skuse et al., 2014) and rs2254298 is associated with ASD (Jacob et al., 2007), depression (Costa et al., 2009), and social anxiety (Thompson et al., 2011). Positive parenting and functional responses to child stimuli in the orbitofrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, and hippocampus are associated with rs1042778 (Michalska et al., 2014).

Although these SNPs have been shown to be associated with SC, the risk alleles vary due to differing study designs, including variability in measuring social behavior, focus on adults rather than children, variability in the diagnostic status of participants, and the designation of heterozygous individuals as part of the “risk” or “no risk” groups. A recent, large-scale, meta-analysis (Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn, 2014) found no evidence for association of rs2254298 or rs53576 with social behavior or autism, casting even more doubt on the link between these SNPs and SC. The links between OXTR, the brain, and behavior in SC during childhood are complex and far from unequivocal. Despite some convergence in the literature, much remains to be known about the association of OXTR risk alleles with SC and other ASD-associated behaviors, as well as neural function. In the present study, the genetics-brain-behavior links observed in NDDs are extended to TD children. First, we evaluated multiple ASD traits reflecting adaptive and maladaptive social behavior and repetitive behaviors using dimensional, quantitative scales and subscales that are sensitive to these traits in TD children. We then evaluate these traits relative to the neural processing of face perception and to four OXTR SNPs (rs2254298, rs53576, rs1042778, rs237897). We hypothesize that presence/absence of OXTR risk alleles and the neural processing of face perception will predict performance on behavioral measures of SC.

2. Methods

2.1. Subjects

Sixty-six children were recruited from local public schools in rural central Pennsylvania and from the Bucknell University Message Center. Each family received monetary compensation for their participation. Experimental procedures were approved by the Bucknell University IRB. Written consent was obtained from parents and verbal assent was obtained from children. Parents completed several parent-report forms on their children using the online survey tool, Qualtrics® (Provo, Utah). These measures included a demographics form, the Social Responsiveness Scale 2 (SRS-2): School Age (Constantino and Gruber, 2012), and the Adaptive Behavior Assessment System 2nd edition ABAS 2 Parent Form: Ages 5–21 (Harrison and Oakland, 2003). The SRS-2 served as a parent-reported, quantitative measure of ASD-like traits, whereas the ABAS 2 assessed a range of adaptive behaviors.

In the lab, parents completed a paper form of the Child Behavior Checklist 4–18 (CBCL) (Achenbach and Rescorla, 2001), a parent-report checklist of maladaptive behaviors. Cognitive levels were assessed for the first three children with the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test 2 (KBIT-2) and with the Shipley Institute of Living Scale 2 (SILS 2) (Shipley et al., 2009) for all other children. The SILS 2 is a valid and reliable measure of IQ designed to assess crystalized knowledge and fluid reasoning. Administration of the SILS 2 in this study included two sections: a vocabulary questionnaire and a series of mental rotation tasks. This measure was chosen for its brief administration time (about 25 min). Children completed a paper version of this measure in the lab. In addition, children completed the Children's RMET (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001b). In this task, they were presented with a series of pictures on the computer featuring only an individual's eyes and were asked to determine what the person is thinking or feeling by selecting one of four possible answers. Children also completed the Social Attribution Task Multiple Choice Version (SAT-MC) (Klin and Jones, 2006), a task that presents a 61 s video of three geometric shapes in dynamic movement that generally elicits anthropomorphic social attributions. This measure of theory of mind and social attribution correlates with ASD social deficits (Klin, 2000).

In order to ensure the non-clinical status of the sample, all children (n = 5) and parents (n = 3) who were taking medication for psychiatric conditions at the time of testing were removed from analysis. Additionally, any child or parent with a family history of autism or Asperger's syndrome was removed from analysis (n = 3). One child was removed from analysis due to an abnormally high SRS-2 score (>3 SDs above the mean). During analysis, three children who were heterozygous (A/G) for rs53576 and rs2254298 were removed from analysis due to the small sample size and insufficient power. Forty-eight children (23 female) between the ages of 6 and 15 were included in the study analysis after exclusions. The sample was largely Caucasian (95.8%). Participants were of average or above average intelligence with average scores on the SRS-2, ABAS 2 composites, and CBCL (Table 1).

Table 1.

Means and standard deviations of sample characteristics.

N Mean (SD)
Demographic characteristics
Parent age 24 40.79 (5.62)
Child age 48 10.04 (2.50)
IQ
Parent verbal IQ 24 110.58 (10.30)
Child verbal IQ 48 118.71 (12.33)
Parent nonverbal IQ 24 113.42 (13.43)
Child nonverbal IQ 48 115.06 (12.16)
Parent FSIQ 24 114.50 (10.92)
Child FSIQ 48 120.46 (12.77)
Measures of ASD-traits and social functioning
Parent SRS-2 score 24 44.67 (4.62)
Child SRS-2 score 48 46.56 (6.63)
RMET total 45 18.25 (3.59)
SAT-MC total 48 12.96 (3.63)
Measures of adaptive/maladaptive functioning
CBCL anxious depressed 43 53.88 (5.93)
CBCL withdrawn depressed 43 53.30 (5.59)
CBCL somatic complaints 43 53.14 (4.99)
CBCL social problems 43 51.56 (2.50)
CBCL thought problems 43 52.74 (3.75)
CBCL attention problems 43 53.40 (4.73)
CBCL rule breaking 43 52.12 (3.25)
CBCL aggressive behavior 43 52.40 (3.57)
CBCL internalizing total 43 48.28 (9.58)
CBCL externalizing total 43 45.84 (8.97)
CBCL total 43 46.70 (7.91)
ABAS general adaptive composite 41 102.46 (16.24)
ABAS conceptual composite 43 103.95 (14.56)
ABAS social composite 46 107.61 (12.21)
ABAS practical composite 42 99.10 (16.17)

Note. Ns vary due to missing/incomplete data for one or more participants or incomplete individual items required for composite calculation and/or too many “guess” indications.

2.2. Electroencephalogram (EEG)

Children were fitted with a 32 channel Ag-AgCl electrode fabric cap arranged in the international 10–20 system, grounded at site AFz (ANT, Enschede, Netherlands). Stimuli were presented on a Dell computer monitor (16 in.) and consisted of 100 images of upright and inverted faces (50 each) and 200 images of upright faces and houses (100 each) (Herrington et al., 2011). All images were presented individually in grayscale in the center of the screen, and all faces were depicted with a neutral expression. Each image was presented for 500 ms with a 1000 ms inter-stimulus interval, creating a total stimulus length of 2.50 min for the upright-inverted paradigm and 5.00 min for the houses-faces paradigm.

ERP data were analyzed with ASA (Advanced Source Analysis) version 4.7.3. Signals were recorded at a sampling rate of 512 Hz, filtered continuously with a low cutoff frequency of 0.3 and a high cutoff frequency of 30 Hz. Averaging epochs were set to −100 ms before to 250 ms after stimulus presentation and impedances were maintained below 10 kΩ. Baseline correction was applied with a sample baseline duration of 100 ms and a 0 ms offset before event; time interval for correction was set to event duration. ASA's artifact detection removed any artifacts that were outside of the accepted range of −70 to 70 μV. DC correction was also applied. Data were analyzed using both positive and negative amplitudes and latencies of N170 occurring between 150 ms and 245 ms after stimulus presentation. Electrode sites P7 and P8 were used for analysis. To safeguard against unreliable data due to increased movement in children, an inclusion criterion of at least 50% valid events for the upright-inverted and houses-faces paradigms was established. Participants had to have at least 25/50 valid events for both upright and inverted faces and at least 50/100 valid events for both faces and houses to be included in the respective analyses. If reaching the criterion number of events required deactivation of more than five electrodes, the data were considered unreliable given that all electrodes are used as reference points to one another.

Normal or enhanced face perception is measured by larger amplitudes, and in the case of the N170, a larger amplitude is expressed by more negative values (−μV). Faster response time (shorter latencies of the N170) is also a marker of enhanced face processing, so both amplitudes and latencies are expected to predict social behavior. Conversely, images of houses elicit a slower, less negative N170 relative to images of faces in individuals with normal or enhanced face perception. Finally, images of inverted faces tend to elicit a slower but more negative N170 relative to images of upright faces. It is expected in this study that N170 responses characteristic of typical or enhanced face perception for each stimulus type will also be indicative of increased SC as measured by behavioral tasks and parent-report measures.

2.3. DNA collection and genotyping

Saliva samples were collected using Oragene DISCOVER (OGR-500) collection kits (DNA Genotek Inc., Kanata, Canada). DNA was extracted using QIAsymphony (QIAGEN, Duesseldorf, Germany) with protocol CF1000, CR2276 ID366 V1 and DSP Virus/Pathogen Midi Kit Version 1 (Ref 937055, 2013-07). The DNA was eluted in 120 μl water, and the concentration and purity ratios were assessed using the NanoDrop 2000 UV-Vis Spectrophotometer (ThermoScientific, Wilmington, DE). Genotyping was performed on the following SNPs: rs53576, rs237897, rs1042778, and rs2254298, using the Applied Biosystems 7500 Fast Real-time PCR System (Life Technologies Corp., Grand Island, NY; V 2.0.5) and the standard cycling protocol for the 96-well reaction plate format. Briefly, each 10 μl reaction consisted of TaqMan Genotyping Master Mix, a TaqMan SNP Genotyping Assay (Applied Biosystems, Foster City, CA), 10 ng of genomic DNA, and DNase-free water, leaving a minimum of 2 wells as no-template controls.

2.4. Statistical analysis

Grand averaging was conducted for all EEG data, which were then mean-centered so that each slope could be interpreted independently of the other covariates. The data were modeled using a univariate linear mixed model (LMM) controlling for within family covariance (with a compound symmetric covariance structure) with backwards elimination using an elimination criterion of p > .200; the repeated component was included to control for the covariance between siblings. The following dependent variables were modeled separately: SRS-2 Social Avoidance subscale score (SRS-2-SA), SRS-2 Emotion Recognition subscale score (SRS-2-ER), SRS-2 Interpersonal Relatedness subscale score (SRS-2-IR), SRS-2 Insistence on Sameness subscale score (SRS-2-IS), SRS-2 Repetitive Mannerisms subscale score (SRS-2-RM), ABAS 2 Social Composite score, CBCL Social Problems score, RMET total score, and SAT-MC total score. EEG amplitude and latency independent variables were modeled separately due to differences in scaling, so that each model began with 8 covariates: P7 and P8 minimum amplitude (or latency) for houses and for faces, and P7 and P8 minimum amplitude (or latency) for upright faces and for inverted faces. The four SNP variables were entered as factors (rs1042778, rs2254298, rs53576, rs237897) along with sex. Significance levels were adjusted by the Bonferroni method to account for multiple comparisons.

If sex was found to be significant after completion of a model, sex differences in EEG measures were examined by adding the interactions of each remaining EEG component and sex as additional covariates in the model. Modeling then continued with backwards elimination using an elimination criterion of p > .200. When modeling was complete, any significant interactions between EEG components and sex were reported and sex differences were examined.

During initial analysis, an interaction between rs2254298 and rs53576 was discovered for several DVs. Thus an interaction variable was created and the data were re-modeled with the interaction variable, rs237897, and rs1042778 serving as factors. If the interaction was not significant at the end of the modeling process, the data were re-modeled. The same procedure was used, but the interaction was removed and rs2254298 and rs53576 were entered as factors to allow for the expression of main effects. In order to account for a strong correlation between children's age and both SAT-MC scores (r = .563, p < .001) and RMET scores (r = .518, p < .001), age was entered as a covariate for all models involving these two measures.

2.5. Parent data

A subset of data was also collected on parents. Social behavior was assessed using the SRS-2: Adult Self-Report and IQ was assessed using the SILS 2. Saliva samples were collected and analyzed using the same methods outlined above for children. Only parents of children who were included in the child analysis were included in the adult analysis. Similar to children, any parent who was using psychotropic mediation or had a family history of autism or Asperger's Syndrome was eliminated from analysis. Additionally, three parents were removed due to missing SRS-2 data. Finally, parents with rs2254298 and rs53576 (AA/GG and AG/AG) genotypes were removed from analysis due to insufficient sample size. Parent SRS-2 and SNP data were analyzed separately from child data using similar univariate LMM methods. The following dependent variables were modeled separately: SRS-2-SA, SRS-2-ER, SRS-2-IR, SRS-2-IS, and SRS-2-RM. The SNP variables rs237897, rs1042778, and the interaction between rs2254298 and rs53576 were entered as factors along with sex.

3. Results

3.1. OXTR SNPs

In the children, genotype frequencies (n = 48) for each SNP were as follows: rs1042778: TT:9, GT:28, GG:11; rs2254298: AA:0, AG:8, GG:40; rs53576: AA:4, AG:21 GG:23; rs237897: AA:11, AG:18, GG:19. No SNPs were removed due to departures from Hardy Weinberg Equilibrium (HWE) (p < .01). No SNPs indicated linkage disequilibrium using an r2 cutoff of 0.8.

A significant and consistent interaction occurred between rs2254298 and rs53576. When EEG amplitude variables were entered as covariates, the combination of rs2254298(A/G) and rs53576(G/G) was associated with the worst CBCL Social Problems subscale scores, SRS-2-SA, SRS-2-ER, and SRS-2-IR scores. When EEG latency variables were entered as covariates, the presence of rs2254298(A/G) and rs53576(G/G) was associated with the worst RMET, SRS-2-SA, SRS-2-ER, and SRS-2-IR scores. In the only exceptions to the pattern, children with rs2254298(A/G) and rs53576(G/G) had the second worst scores on the RMET with amplitude covariates and the second worst scores on the SAT-MC with latency covariates. See Table 2 for significance values and Table 3 for full details on pairwise comparisons.

Table 2.

Significant SNP covariates by dependent variables.

SNP factors Dependent variables
ABAS social CBCL social RMET total SAT-MC SRS-SA SRS-ER SRS-IR SRS-RM SRS-IS
Amplitude covariates
rs1042778 .004 ns ns .002 .013 .010 ns ns ns
rs237897 ns ns ns ns ns ns ns .019 ns
rs2254298*rs53576 ns .022 .002 ns .016 .004 .003 ns ns
Latency covariates
rs1042778 ns ns ns .015 .003 ns ns ns .008
rs2254298 ns ns ns ns ns ns ns ns .006
rs237897 ns ns ns ns ns ns ns .001 ns
rs2254298*rs53576 ns ns .006 .012 .003 .010 .020 ns ns

Note. Table includes p values for all significant SNP factors. SNPs modeled with amplitude covariates are presented in the top half of the table and SNPs modeled with latency covariates are presented in the bottom half of the table. ABAS social = Adaptive Behavior Assessment System 2 Social Composite score; CBCL social = Child Behavior Checklist Social Problems score; RMET = Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test-Revised score; SAT-MC = Social Attribution Task Multiple Choice version total score; SRS = Social Responsiveness Scale 2; SRS-SA = SRS-2 Social Avoidance score; SRS-ER = SRS-2 Emotion Recognition score; SRS-IR = SRS-2 Interpersonal Relatedness score; SRS-RM = SRS-2 Repetitive Mannerisms score; SRS-IS = SRS-2 Insistence on Sameness score. * = interaction. ns = not significant, p > .05.

Table 3.

Estimated model adjusted means and standard errors of significant rs2254298 and rs53576 interactions.

SNP
rs22554298/rs53576
CBCL social RMET total SAT-MC SRS-SA SRS-ER SRS-IR
Amplitude covariates
GG_GG 51.04 (0.72)a 21.38 (1.58)ab ns 1.21 (0.38)a 4.59 (5.93)a 0.72 (0.39)a
GG_AG 50.65 (0.71)a 16.36 (1.17)a ns 1.32 (0.32)a 17.53 (4.28)ab 0.74 (0.34)a
GG_AA 52.24 (1.64)ab 21.75 (1.47)b ns 2.27 (0.72)ab 5.64 (4.85)ab 2.28 (0.68)ab
AG_GG 54.65 (0.95)b 19.66 (1.76)ab ns 3.41 (0.60)b 21.78 (6.19)b 3.10 (0.52)b
Latency covariates
GG_GG ns 18.76 (1.15)ab 9.11 (1.40)a 1.40 (0.42)a 8.68 (2.86)a 0.43 (0.43)a
GG_AG ns 18.23 (0.89)a 16.41 (1.02)b 1.46 (0.39)a 15.17 (2.71)ab 1.24 (0.39)ab
GG_AA ns 23.32 (1.46)b 18.24 (1.92)b 2.21 (0.85)ab 7.34 (4.08)a 1.70 (0.68)ab
AG_GG ns 17.27 (1.33)a 12.28 (1.62)ab 3.99 (0.58)b 27.98 (4.28)b 2.96 (0.61)b

Note. Table presents genotype estimated model adjusted means and standard errors for significant rs2254298/rs53576 interactions. Values that share the same superscript are not significantly different from one another. Interactions with amplitude covariates are presented in the top half of the table and interactions with latency covariates are presented in the bottom half of the table. CBCL social = Child Behavior Checklist Social Problems score; RMET = Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test-Revised score; SAT-MC = Social Attribution Task Multiple Choice version total score; SRS = Social Responsiveness Scale 2; SRS SA = SRS-2 Social Avoidance score; SRS-ER = SRS-2 Emotion Recognition score; SRS-IR = SRS-2 Interpersonal Relatedness score.

Children with rs2254298(A/G) and rs53576(G/G) scored significantly worse on the SRS-2-SA (amplitude and latency covariates), SRS-2-IR (amplitude covariates) and the CBCL Social Problems subscale (amplitude covariates) than children with rs2254298(G/G) and rs53576(G/G or A/G) combinations. They also scored significantly worse on the SRS-2-ER (latency covariates) than children with rs2254298(G/G) and rs53576(G/G or A/A) combinations. Similarly, children with an rs2254298(A/G) and rs53576(G/G) combination scored significantly worse on the RMET (latency covariates) than those with an rs2254298(G/G) and rs53576(A/A) combination. Finally, children with the AG/GG genotype also scored significantly worse on the SRS-2-ER (amplitude covariates) and SRS-2-IR (latency covariates) than children with an rs2254298(G/G) and rs53576(G/G) combination. Interactions are plotted in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Summary of significant interactions between rs53576 and rs2254298. This figure depicts the 10 interactions for which individuals with an rs2254298(A/G) and rs53576(G/G) combination received the poorest scores. The y-axis represents estimated marginal means for various measures of SC and the x-axis represents the genotype combinations for rs2254298/rs53576. Values with the same superscript are not significantly different from one another and vertical lines represent standard errors. Interactions modeled with amplitude covariates are presented on the left and interactions modeled with latency covariates are presented on the right.

In addition to the interaction effects of rs2254298 and rs53576, a main effect emerged indicating that children with an rs2254298(A/G) combination received significantly worse scores on the SRS-2-IS (latency covariates) than those with the rs2254298(G/G) combination. Main effects also emerged for rs1042778 and rs237897. When both EEG amplitudes and latencies were entered as covariates, results were mixed, with the poorest performing genotype for rs1042778 varying between SC measures. In contrast, children with the rs237897(G/G) combination received significantly worse scores on the SRS-2-RM with both amplitude and latency EEG covariates. For full details, see Table 2, Table 4.

Table 4.

SNP allele estimated model adjusted means and standard errors for significant dependent variables.

SNP ABAS social SAT-MC SRS-SA SRS-ER SRS-RM SRS-IS
Amplitude covariates
rs1042778
GG 105.58 (2.87)a 17.19 (1.28)a 2.93 (0.58)a 5.69 (3.02)a ns ns
GT 103.82 (2.75)a 13.34 (0.69)b 1.15 (0.27)b 13.85 (2.62)b ns ns
TT 115.42 (3.23)b 15.45 (1.31)ab 2.07 (0.55)ab 17.62 (3.80)b ns ns
rs237897
GG ns ns ns ns 2.46 (0.39)a ns
AG ns ns ns ns 0.81 (0.40)b ns
AA ns ns ns ns 0.92 (0.49)ab ns
Latency covariates
rs1042778
GG ns 13.45 (1.16)ab 3.10 (0.53)a ns ns 6.07 (1.32)ab
GT ns 12.27 (0.60)a 1.18 (0.31)b ns ns 5.51 (0.81)a
TT ns 16.32 (1.20)b 2.52 (0.62)ab ns ns 10.57 (1.42)b
rs237897
GG ns ns ns ns 2.61 (0.39)a ns
AG ns ns ns ns 0.73 (0.41)b ns
AA ns ns ns ns 0.41 (0.51)b ns
rs2254298
GG ns ns ns ns ns 5.06 (0.65)a
AG ns ns ns ns ns 9.71 (1.47)b

Note. Table presents genotype estimated model adjusted means and standard errors for significant SNP factors. Values that share the same superscript are not significantly different from one another. Interactions with amplitude covariates are presented in the top half of the table and interactions with latency covariates are presented in the bottom half of the table. ABAS Social = Adaptive Behavior Assessment System 2 Social Composite score; SAT-MC = Social Attribution Task-Multiple Choice version total score; SRS = Social Responsiveness Scale 2; SRS-SA = SRS-2 Social Avoidance score; SRS-ER = SRS-2 Emotion Recognition score; SRS-RM = SRS-2 Repetitive Mannerisms score; SRS-IS = SRS-2 Insistence on Sameness score.

3.2. EEG

Whereas the genetic findings of this study – that the rs53576 and rs2254298 SNPs interact to predict adaptive and maladaptive social behavior – were clear and consistent, the EEG findings were varied and equivocal. Although nearly all of the EEG variables predicted scores on measures of social cognition, no consistent pattern emerged as to the directions of these predictions. As Table 5 indicates, approximately half of the significant EEG covariates predicted SC in the expected direction (green): increased face sensitivity in the neural measures predicted better social cognition. This pattern persisted even among the covariates with higher beta values (bold).

Table 5.

Significant EEG covariates by dependent variables.

graphic file with name fx2.gif
graphic file with name fx3.gif

Note. SNPs modeled with amplitude covariates are presented in the top half of the table and SNPs modeled with latency covariates are presented in the bottom half of the table. Male and female values are reported separately for significant EEG*Sex interactions. ABAS Social = Adaptive Behavior Assessment System 2 Social Composite score; CBCL Social = Child Behavior Checklist Social Problems score; RMET = Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test-Revised total score; SAT-MC = Social Attribution Task Multiple Choice version total score; SRS = Social Responsiveness Scale 2; SRS-SA = SRS-2 Social Avoidance score; SRS-ER = SRS-2 Emotion Recognition score; SRS-IR = SRS-2 Interpersonal Relatedness score; SRS-RM = SRS-2 Repetitive Mannerisms score; SRS-IS = SRS-2 Insistence on Sameness score. * = interaction. ns = not significant, p > .05. Bold = beta > 0.4.

The most consistent pattern emerging from the EEG data involved a significant interaction between sex and three EEG amplitude covariates (upright faces at electrode P8, and inverted faces at electrodes P7 and P8) when SRS-2-IS was the dependent variable. At all three of these sites, females exhibited a stronger correlation between the EEG covariate and SRS-2-IS scores than males. In addition, the beta values for females reached significance while those for males did not. Although the direction of the relationship differed among the three EEG covariates (negative or unexpected for inverted faces at P8 and positive or expected for the others), the beta values were consistently stronger in females than in males. This suggests that the SRS-2-IS subscale, measuring an insistence on sameness and order, is predicted by the identified EEG correlates in females but not in males.

3.3. Parents

In the parents, genotype frequencies were as follows: rs1042778: TT:6, GT:13, GG:5; rs2254298: AA:0, AG:4, GG:20; rs53576: AA:3, AG:14, GG:7; rs237897: AA:6, AG:11, GG:7. Although only significant for the IR subscale (p = .019), all subscales of the SRS-2 (IR, SA, IS, RM, ER) showed a trend in which a genotype of AG for rs2254298 and GG for rs53576 had the highest scores. The lack of significance may be explained by the small sample size for parents (n = 24 overall and n = 4 for the AG/GG genotype in these two SNPs). However, it should be noted that the trends seen in the parent data correspond to the significant findings in the child data.

4. Discussion

The present study examined SC in TD children using multiple quantitative trait measures and several methodological approaches including parent report, performance-based tasks, neural response, and genotype analysis. We found a clear association between rs2254298, rs53576, and several measures of SC, including the CBCL Social Problems subscale, RMET, SAT-MC, and several subscales of the SRS-2 (SA, ER, and IR).

A significant and consistent interaction arose between rs53576 and rs2254298 such that children with rs2254298(A/G) and rs53576(G/G) received the worst scores (i.e. those reflecting decreased social cognition) on several measures of SC, including the CBCL Social Problems subscale, RMET (latency covariates), SRS-2-SA, SRS-2-ER, and SRS-2-IR. Several interactions were significant for both amplitude and latency (RMET, SRS-2-SA and SRS-2-IR), while the CBCL Social Problems subscale was only significant with amplitude covariates and the SAT-MC was only significant with latency covariates. In addition, children with rs2254298(A/G) and rs53576(G/G) also received the second lowest scores on the RMET (amplitude covariates) and the SAT-MC (latency covariates), indicating more ASD-like performance. Though only significant for scores on the IR subscale of the SRS-2, parents showed a similar pattern in which those with rs2254298(A/G) and rs53576(G/G) had the highest or most ASD-like scores for all subscales of the SRS-2.

These findings complement a recent meta-analysis study (Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn, 2014) that examined the associations between social behavior/autism status and rs2254298 or rs53576, independently, and found no significant association between any outcome measure and these SNPs across approximately 18,000 participants for rs53576 and 13,000 participants for rs2254298. Our results suggest that it is the combination and interaction of rs2254298(A/G) and rs53576(G/G) that predisposes an individual to a risk for poorer SC and increased ASD-like tendencies, not a single SNP. Thus, our results extend those reported by the meta-analysis and indicate that the combination of rs2254298(A/G) and rs53576(G/G), and not either SNP in isolation, poses the greatest risk for poorer SC. To our knowledge, the present study is the first to provide evidence of this interaction, and this finding merits further exploration on larger samples.

In addition to the interaction, a main effect emerged for rs2254298 such that the AG combination is associated with significantly more ASD-like behavior than the GG combination. Previous studies have had mixed findings regarding the “risk” allele of rs2254298. Some studies have found the A allele to be associated with ASD (Liu et al., 2010, Wu et al., 2005) and higher Autism Quotient scores in males (Chen and Johnson, 2012). Furthermore, the A allele is associated with greater levels of depression, attachment anxiety (Chen and Johnson, 2012), and exacerbated physical and social anxiety in girls who experience increased early adversity (Thompson et al., 2011). However, other studies have found the G allele of rs2254298 to be associated with ASD (Jacob et al., 2007, Lerer et al., 2008) and lower empathic concern in children with schizophrenia (Montag et al., 2012). Thus, no true “risk” allele can be ascertained from the literature, but in this study the A/G genotype for rs2254298 was associated with poorer SC.

In contrast, no main effects emerged for rs53576 on the measures of SC. Other studies examining the association of rs53576 with SC have produced mixed results, with some studies suggesting that lower SC is associated with the A allele and other studies suggesting it is associated with the G allele. For example, studies have shown the A allele of rs53576 to be associated with ASD (Wu et al., 2005), lower sensitive responsiveness (Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn, 2008), lower positive affect (Lucht et al., 2009), lower scores on the RMET (Rodrigues et al., 2009), higher emotional suppression (Kim et al., 2011), less affiliative nonverbal cues (Kogan et al., 2011), less optimism and self-esteem (Saphire-Bernstein et al., 2011), less emotional support seeking in highly distressed Americans (Kim et al., 2010), and less trusting behavior (Krueger et al., 2012). However, other studies have shown the G allele of rs53576 to be associated with unipolar depression (Costa et al., 2009), poorer social cognitive ability (Park et al., 2010), and higher satisfaction after betrayal (Tabak et al., 2013). Here, we found no such link. More importantly, our results indicate that it is not a particular genotype for a single SNP in isolation that poses the greatest risk for poorer SC but rather the combination of rs2254298(A/G) and rs53576(G/G).

4.1. Conclusions and future directions

We have demonstrated significant associations between quantitative measures of SC and ASD-like traits, neurological measures of face perception abilities, and two OXTR SNPs in TD children. Through the use of a multi-dimensional approach, we have shown that behaviors and OXTR SNPs associated with ASD are present in TD children. Similar to previous literature, we have shown that no one genotype for any of the four SNPs sequenced can truly be designated as a risk for poorer SC in isolation. Rather we have shown that the AG/GG genotype combination for rs2254298 and rs53576 confers the greatest risk for social impairments in TD children. These findings corroborate previous work implicating these SNPs in social behavior, empathy, and social anxiety that are governed by a host of neural substrates (i.e., the hypothalamus, orbitofrontal cortex, anterior cingulum and the hippocampus; see Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn, 2008, Michalska et al., 2014, Rodrigues et al., 2009, Thompson et al., 2011, Tost et al., 2010, Wang et al., 2013). However, given our small sample sizes and restriction of our sample to children, the generalizability of these findings is limited. Thus, this finding will need to be replicated and expanded in future studies.

ASD is a complex, multi-faceted disorder and only through equally complex and multi-faceted approaches can we hope to understand the full nature of the disorder. The quantitative nature of our measures made it possible to detect individual differences in SC, making them useful tools for ASD research given the wide range of abilities in both those who are diagnosed and those who are considered TD. There is a large degree of overlap between various neurodevelopmental disorders, and in many cases, there are more similarities than differences. Even the previously “distinct” genetic markers of disorders have been found to overlap within differing conditions (Moreno-De-Luca et al., 2013). Thus, given the extensive overlap between disorders neurologically, phenotypically, and genetically, it seems logical that the field should shift toward a theoretical structure that allows for the expression and identification of these similarities using the previously proposed paradigm of developmental brain dysfunction (Moreno-De-Luca et al., 2013). This concept has been proposed as an overarching framework that encompasses all neurodevelopmental disorders with various etiologies and manifestations (Moreno-De-Luca et al., 2013). The use of this broad and flexible model allows for a more comprehensive assessment of an individual's needs and the institution of a treatment plan designed to emphasize his/her strengths while improving deficits manifested not only through primary diagnoses, but through any comorbid conditions or symptoms as well.

Given that non-clinical variations of ASD exist within the typical population and are manifest in multiple domains of functioning, it is logical to move toward the direction of spectrums for neurodevelopmental disorders. If a multi-dimensional assessment technique can identify such a large degree of variability within TD individuals, it stands to reason that it could be a useful tool for the treatment of various forms of neurodevelopmental disorders when viewed through the framework of developmental brain dysfunction.

4.2. Limitations

As this study was drawn from a small geographic region, it may not be representative of the population and should be replicated in a larger sample in the future. Many of the behavioral measures relied upon parent reports, which may introduce bias. However, these parent-report measures are normed, standardized, and are assessed based on performance relative to age-matched peers, and offer an important perspective on behavioral profiles. The study also involved a relatively small sample size of 48 TD children, which became smaller when they were divided into the appropriate interaction subgroups (rs2254298/rs53576: AG/GG = 8; GG/AA = 4; GG/AG = 21; GG/GG = 15). While the sample sizes were large enough to detect differences, they warrant caution in terms of interpreting and generalizing these findings.

The participants were selected because they were considered to be TD and thus none of their scores were expected to fall within the clinical range. Despite this, we found a large degree of variation in scores on SC measures and have demonstrated that those individuals with an rs2254298(A/G) and rs53576(G/G) combination received the lowest scores on four unique (6 total when the SRS-2 subscales are considered separately) measures of SC. These children are not at risk for developing autism or other social problems, but through the use of quantitative measures sensitive enough to detect individual differences, we have demonstrated that these children exhibit poorer SC phenotypes than their peers. It is important to note, however, that the models linking cortical function during face perception tasks to other measures of SC produced mixed results, possibly owing to the relatively small sample size. But it may also be that the mixed findings are due to real differences in the clinical manifestation of ASD and the milder range of social deficits that are observed in the general population. Thus, while we advocate a quantitative approach, more research is needed to examine the continuities and discontinuities in the clinical presentation of ASD and the normal range of ASD-like behaviors found in non-clinical populations.

Funding

This work was supported by a Bucknell University Graduate Summer Research Stipend.

Conflicts of interest statement

We declare no conflicts of interest for this work.

References

  1. Achenbach T.M., Rescorla L.A. University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families; Burlington, VT: 2001. Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms & Profiles. [Google Scholar]
  2. Anagnostou E., Soorya L., Brian J., Dupuis A., Mankad D., Smile S., Jacob S. Intranasal oxytocin in the treatment of autism spectrum disorders: a review of literature and early safety and efficacy data in youth. Brain Res. 2014;(February) doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.01.049. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Andari E., Duhamel J., Zalla T., Herbrecht E., Leboyer M., Sirigu A. Promoting social behavior with oxytocin in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2010;107(9):4389–4394. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0910249107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bakermans-Kranenburg M.J., van IJzendoorn M.H. Oxytocin receptor (OXTR) and serotonin transporter (5-HTT) genes associated with observed parenting. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2008;3(2):128–134. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bakermans-Kranenburg M.J., van IJzendoorn M.H. A sociability gene? Meta-analysis of oxytocin receptor genotype effects in humans. Psychiatr. Genet. 2014;24(2):45–51. doi: 10.1097/YPG.0b013e3283643684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Baron-Cohen S., Jolliffe T., Mortimore C., Robertson M. Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 1997;38(7):813–822. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01599.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Baron-Cohen S., Wheelwright S., Hill J., Raste Y., Plumb I. The reading the mind in the eyes test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2001;42(2):241–251. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Baron-Cohen S., Wheelwright S., Spong A., Scahill V., Lawson J. Are intuitive physics and intuitive psychology independent? A test with children with Asperger syndrome. J. Dev. Learn. Disord. 2001;5(1):47–78. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bartz J.A., Zaki J., Bolger N., Hollander E., Ludwig N.N., Kolevzon A., Ochsner K.N. Oxytocin selectively improves empathic accuracy. Psychol. Sci. 2010;21(10):1426–1428. doi: 10.1177/0956797610383439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Beaumont R., Newcombe P. Theory of mind and central coherence in adults with high-functioning autism or Asperger syndrome. Autism. 2006;10(4):365–382. doi: 10.1177/1362361306064416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bell M., Fiszdon J., Greig T., Wexler B. Social attribution test – multiple choice (SAT-MC) in schizophrenia: comparison with community sample and relationship to neurocognitive, social cognitive and symptom measures. Schizophr. Res. 2010;122(1-3):164–171. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.03.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bookheimer S. Oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) impacts salience network connectivity in children with and without ASD. Platform Presentation at the International Meeting for Autism Research; Toronto, Canada ; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  13. Brent E., Rios P., Happé F., Charman T. Performance of children with autism spectrum disorder on advanced theory of mind tasks. Autism. 2004;8(3):283–299. doi: 10.1177/1362361304045217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Brothers L. The neural basis of primate social communication. Motiv. Emotion. 1990;14(2):81–91. [Google Scholar]
  15. Campbell D.B., Datta D., Jones S.T., Lee E.B., Sutcliffe J.S., Hammock E.A., Levitt P. Association of oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene variants with multiple phenotype domains of autism spectrum disorder. J. Neurodev. Disord. 2011;3(2):101–112. doi: 10.1007/s11689-010-9071-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Castelli F., Frith C., Happé F., Frith U. Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain. 2002;125(8):1839–1849. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Chan R.C., Hu Z., Cui J., Wang Y., McAlonan G.M. Social attribution in children with high functioning autism and Asperger syndrome: an exploratory study in the Chinese setting. Res. Autism Spect. Disord. 2011;5(4):1538–1548. [Google Scholar]
  18. Chen F.S., Barth M.E., Johnson S.L., Gotlib I.H., Johnson S.C. Oxytocin receptor (OXTR) polymorphisms and attachment in human infants. Front. Psychol. 2011;2:200. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Chen F.S., Johnson S.C. An oxytocin receptor gene variant predicts attachment anxiety in females and autism-spectrum traits in males. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 2012;3(1):93–99. [Google Scholar]
  20. Cheung C.H., Rutherford H.J., Mayes L.C., McPartland J.C. Neural responses to faces reflect social personality traits. Soc. Neurosci. 2010;5(4):351–359. doi: 10.1080/17470911003597377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Constantino J.N., Gruber C.P. 2nd ed. Western Psychological Services; Torrance, CA: 2012. Social Responsiveness Scale, Second Edition (SRS-2) [Google Scholar]
  22. Constantino J.N. The quantitative nature of autistic social impairment. Pediatr. Res. 2011;69:55R–62R. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e318212ec6e. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Costa B., Pini S., Gabelloni P., Abelli M., Lari L., Cardini A., Muti M., Gesi C., Landi S., Galderisi S., Mucci A., Lucacchini A., Cassano G.B., Martini C. Oxytocin receptor polymorphisms and adult attachment style in patients with depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34(10):1506–1514. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Frith C.D., Frith U. Social cognition in humans. Curr. Biol. 2007;17(16):R724–R732. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.05.068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Frith C.D., Frith U. Mechanisms of social cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2012;63:287–313. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gillberg C. The ESSENCE in child psychiatry: early symptomatic syndromes eliciting neurodevelopmental clinical examinations. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2010;31(6):1543–1551. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2010.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gordon I., Vander Wyk B.C., Bennett R.H., Cordeaux C., Lucas M.V., Eilbott J.A., Zagoory-Sharon O., Leckman J.F., Feldman R., Pelphrey K.A. Oxytocin enhances brain function in children with autism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2013;10(52):20953–20958. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1312857110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Guastella A.J., Einfeld S.L., Gray K.M., Rinehart N.J., Tonge B.J., Lambert T.J., Hickie I.B. Intranasal oxytocin improves emotion recognition for youth with autism spectrum disorders. Biol. Psychiatry. 2010;67(7):692–694. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.09.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Guastella A.J., Mitchell P.B., Dadds M.R. Oxytocin increases gaze to the eye region of human faces. Biol. Psychiatry. 2008;63(1):3–5. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.06.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Harrison P., Oakland T. 2nd ed. Western Psychological Services; Torrance, CA: 2003. Adaptive Behavior Assessment System. [Google Scholar]
  31. Herrington J.D., Taylor J.M., Grupe D.W., Curby K.M., Schultz R.T. Bidirectional communication between amygdala and fusiform gyrus during facial recognition. Neuroimage. 2011;56(4):2348–2355. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.03.072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hileman C.M., Henderson H., Mundy P., Newell L., Jaime M. Developmental and individual differences on the P1 and N170 ERP components in children with and without autism. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2011;36(2):214–236. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2010.549870. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hollander E., Bartz J., Chaplin W., Phillips A., Sumner J., Soorya L., Anagnostou E., Wasserman S. Oxytocin increases retention of social cognition in autism. Biol. Psychiatry. 2007;61(4):498–503. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.05.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hollander E., Novotny S., Hanratty M., Yaffe R., DeCaria C.M., Aronowitz B.R., Mosovich S. Oxytocin infusion reduces repetitive behaviors in adults with autistic and Asperger's disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28(1):193–198. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Jacob S., Brune C.W., Carter C., Leventhal B.L., Lord C., Cook E.H., Jr. Association of the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) in Caucasian children and adolescents with autism. Neurosci. Lett. 2007;417(1):6–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kim H.S., Sherman D.K., Mojaverian T., Sasaki J.Y., Park J., Suh E.M., Taylor S.E. Gene-culture interaction: oxytocin receptor polymorphism (OXTR) and emotion regulation. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 2011;2:665–672. [Google Scholar]
  37. Kim H.S., Sherman D.K., Sasaki J.Y., Xu J., Chu T.Q., Ryu C., Suh E.M., Graham K., Taylor S.E. Culture, distress, and oxytocin receptor polymorphism (OXTR) interact to influence emotional support seeking. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2010;107(36):15717–15721. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010830107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Klin A. Attributing social meaning to ambiguous visual stimuli in higher-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome: the social attribution task. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2000;41(7):831–846. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Klin A., Jones W. Attributing social and physical meaning to ambiguous visual displays in individuals with higher-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Brain Cogn. 2006;61(1):40–53. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2005.12.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kogan A., Saslow L.R., Impett E.A., Oveis C., Keltner D., Saturn S.R. Thin-slicing study of the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene and the evaluation and expression of the prosocial disposition. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2011;108(48):19189–19192. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1112658108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Krueger F., Parasuraman R., Iyengar V., Thornburg M., Weel J., Lin M., Clarke E., McCabe K., Lipsky R.H. Oxytocin receptor genetic variation promotes human trust behavior. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012;6:4. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Lerer E., Levi S., Salomon S., Darvasi A., Yirmiya N., Ebstein R. Association between the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene and autism: relationship to Vineland adaptive behavior scales and cognition. Mol. Psychiatry. 2008;13(10):980–988. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Liu X., Kawamura Y., Shimada T., Otowa T., Koishi S., Sugiyama T., Nishida H., Hashimoto O., Nakagami R., Tochigi M., Umekage T., Kano Y., Miyagawa T., Kato N., Tokunaga K., Sasaki T. Association of the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene polymorphisms with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in the Japanese population. J. Hum. Genet. 2010;55(3):137–141. doi: 10.1038/jhg.2009.140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Lucht M.J., Barnow S., Sonnenfeld C., Rosenberger A., Grabe H.J., Schroeder W., Völzke H., Freyberger H., Herrmann J., Kroemer F.H., Rosskopf H.D. Associations between the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) and affect, loneliness and intelligence in normal subjects. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry. 2009;33(5):860–866. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. McPartland J., Dawson G., Webb S.J., Panagiotides H., Carver L.J. Event-related brain potentials reveal anomalies in temporal processing of faces in autism spectrum disorder. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discipl. 2004;45(7):1235–1245. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00318.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Michalska K.J., Decety J., Liu C., Chen Q., Martz M.E., Jacob S., Hipwell A.E., Lee S.S., Chronis-Tuscano A., Waldman I.D., Lahey B.B. Genetic imaging of the association of oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) polymorphisms with positive maternal parenting. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2014;3(8):1–10. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Montag C., Brockmann E., Lehmann A., Müller D.J., Rujescu D., Gallinat J. Association between oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms and self-rated ‘empathic concern’ in schizophrenia. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(12):e51882. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051882. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Moreno-De-Luca A., Myers S.M., Challman T.D., Moreno-De-Luca D., Evans D.W., Ledbetter D.H. Developmental brain dysfunction: revival and expansion of old concepts based on new genetic evidence. Lancet Neurol. 2013;12(4):406–414. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(13)70011-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Park J., Willmott M., Vetuz G., Toye C., Kirley A., Hawi Z., Brookes K.J., Gill M., Kent L. Evidence that genetic variation in the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene influences social cognition in ADHD. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry. 2010;34(4):697–702. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.03.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Pinkham A.E., Hopfinger J.B., Pelphrey K.A., Piven J., Penn D.L. Neural bases for impaired social cognition in schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorders. Schizophr. Res. 2008;99(1):164–175. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.10.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Rodrigues S.M., Saslow L.R., Garcia N., John O.P., Keltner D. Oxytocin receptor genetic variation relates to empathy and stress reactivity in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2009;106(50):21437–21441. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0909579106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Saphire-Bernstein S., Way B.M., Kim H.S., Sherman D.K., Taylor S.E. Oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) is related to psychological resources. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2011;108(37):15118–15122. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1113137108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Schultz R.T. Developmental deficits in social perception in autism: the role of the amygdala and fusiform face area. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 2005;23(2):125–141. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.12.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Schultz R.T., Gauthier I., Klin A., Fulbright R.K., Anderson A.W., Volkmar F., Skudlarski P., Lacadie C., Cohen D.J., Gore J.C. Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity during face discrimination among individuals with autism and Asperger syndrome. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2000;57(4):331. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.4.331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Shimoni H.N., Weizman A., Yoran R.H., Raviv A. Theory of mind, severity of autistic symptoms and parental correlates in children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome. Psychiatry Res. 2012;197(1):85–89. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.02.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Shipley W.C., Gruber C.P., Martin T.A., Klein A.M. Western Psychological Services; Torrance, CA: 2009. Shipley-2 Manual. [Google Scholar]
  57. Skuse D.H., Lori A., Cubells J.F., Lee I., Conneely K.N., Puura K., Lehtimäki T., Binder E.B., Young L.J. Common polymorphism in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) is associated with human social recognition skills. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2014;111(5):1987–1992. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1302985111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Tabak B.A., McCullough M.E., Carver C.S., Pedersen E.J., Cuccaro M.L. Variation in oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) polymorphisms is associated with emotional and behavioral reactions to betrayal. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2013 doi: 10.1093/scan/nst042. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Thompson R.J., Parker K.J., Hallmayer J.F., Waugh C.E., Gotlib I.H. Oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism (rs2254298) interacts with familial risk for psychopathology to predict symptoms of depression and anxiety in adolescent girls. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2011;36(1):144–147. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Tost H., Kolachana B., Hakimi S., Lemaitre H., Verchinski B.A., Mattay V.S., Weinberger D.R., Meyer-Lindenberg A. A common allele in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) impacts prosocial temperament and human hypothalamic-limbic structure and function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2010;107(31):13936–13941. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1003296107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Walum H., Lichtenstein P., Neiderhiser J.M., Reiss D., Ganiban J.M., Spotts E.L., Pedersen N.L., Anckarsäter H., Larsson H., Westberg L. Variation in the oxytocin receptor gene is associated with pair-bonding and social behavior. Biol. Psychiatry. 2012;71(5):419–426. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Wang J., Qin W., Liu B., Wang D., Zhang Y., Jiang T., Yu C. Variant in OXTR gene and functional connectivity of the hypothalamus in normal subjects. Neuroimage. 2013;81:199–204. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Webb S.J., Dawson G., Bernier R., Panagiotides H. ERP evidence of atypical face processing in young children with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2006;36(7):881–890. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0126-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Wu S., Jia M., Ruan Y., Liu J., Guo Y., Shuang M., Gong X., Zhang Y., Yang X., Zhang D. Positive association of the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) with autism in the Chinese Han population. Biol. Psychiatry. 2005;58(1):74–77. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES