This cross-sectional study examines the associations of aging and modifiable risk factors for dementia with volume differences in brain regions known to be associated with Alzheimer disease and whether these volume differences are associated with cognitive performance.
Key Points
Question
Are aging and modifiable risk factors for dementia (MRFD) associated with volume differences in brain regions also associated with Alzheimer disease (AD)?
Findings
In this cross-sectional study including 8312 participants, 4 MRFD (hypertension, diabetes, obesity, and frequent alcohol use) were independently and negatively associated with overlapping gray matter regions, including the posterior cingulate cortex, which overlapped with regions that are known to show atrophy in AD. The negative associations of MRFD with spatial memory were associated with mediation through differences in posterior cingulate cortex volume.
Meaning
These findings suggest that common brain regions associated with change with aging and MRFD may explain their joint contributions to cognitive variances in people considered cognitively healthy and in people with AD.
Abstract
Importance
Identifying brain regions associated with risk factors for dementia could guide mechanistic understanding of risk factors associated with Alzheimer disease (AD).
Objectives
To characterize volume changes in brain regions associated with aging and modifiable risk factors for dementia (MRFD) and to test whether volume differences in these regions are associated with cognitive performance.
Design, Setting, and Participants
This cross-sectional study used data from UK Biobank participants who underwent T1-weighted structural brain imaging from August 5, 2014, to October 14, 2016. A voxelwise linear model was applied to test for regional gray matter volume differences associated with aging and MRFD (ie, hypertension, diabetes, obesity, and frequent alcohol use). The potential clinical relevance of these associations was explored by comparing their neuroanatomical distributions with the regional brain atrophy found with AD. Mediation models for risk factors, brain volume differences, and cognitive measures were tested. The primary hypothesis was that common, overlapping regions would be found. Primary analysis was conducted on April 1, 2018.
Main Outcomes and Measures
Gray matter regions that showed relative atrophy associated with AD, aging, and greater numbers of MRFD.
Results
Among 8312 participants (mean [SD] age, 62.4 [7.4] years; 3959 [47.1%] men), aging and 4 major MRFD (ie, hypertension, diabetes, obesity, and frequent alcohol use) had independent negative associations with specific gray matter volumes. These regions overlapped neuroanatomically with those showing lower volumes in participants with AD, including the posterior cingulate cortex, the thalamus, the hippocampus, and the orbitofrontal cortex. Associations between these MRFD and spatial memory were mediated by differences in posterior cingulate cortex volume (β = 0.0014; SE = 0.0006; P = .02).
Conclusions and Relevance
This cross-sectional study identified differences in localized brain gray matter volume associated with aging and MRFD, suggesting regional vulnerabilities. These differences appeared relevant to cognitive performance even among people considered cognitively healthy.
Introduction
The prevalence of dementia, estimated in most countries to be 5% to 7% among people 60 years and older, is increasing globally with greater longevity.1 A likely contributor to rising incidence in dementia other than aging is the global increase in lifestyle diseases, such as hypertension, diabetes, and obesity.2,3,4 These and other modifiable risk factors for dementia (MRFD), such as alcohol use,5 additively contribute approximately one-third of the total risk of Alzheimer disease (AD),6,7 although the pathophysiological mechanisms are not well understood.
Brain atrophy is the most prominent characteristic of brain structural abnormalities in AD. Subtle structural differences similar to those later expressed with AD can be identified in the brains of people who are cognitively healthy (CH) at least a decade before any expected onset of AD.8 Atrophy in brain regions independently associated with early pathology of AD is associated with cognitive decline in individuals who are otherwise CH.9 These and related observations suggest that brain structure is sensitive to factors contributing to late-life cognitive impairment and dementia.
The brain also atrophies with increasing age10,11,12,13,14 at an mean rate of 0.5% per year after age 40 years, although rates of volume loss vary considerably between individuals.10,15,16 Possible explanations for these differences include both genetic variation and differences in exposures to environmental or lifestyle factors that themselves accelerate brain atrophy. Numerous neuroimaging experiments have revealed that the brain atrophy is accelerated by hypertension,13,17,18,19 diabetes,20,21,22,23,24 obesity,25,26 smoking,27,28 low educational attainment,29,30 high alcohol intake,31,32 and inadequate sleep.33,34 However, whether aging and MRFD only influence brain regions sensitive to earlier neuropathological changes with AD or broadly affect all regions of the brain still remains unclear.
We addressed this question using data from the UK Biobank35,36 (UKB) and Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI). The UKB35,36 is a resource for prospective, longitudinal brain magnetic resonance images (MRIs) associated with detailed health and lifestyle information characterizing large numbers of people recruited from general practice registers across the United Kingdom.36 The primary purpose of our study was to determine whether aging and MRFD had independent associations with regional gray matter volumes (GMVs) also associated with AD. We also tested whether associations of aging and multiple MRFD on cognitive impairment are associated with mediation by these regional GMV differences.
Methods
Study Participants
The UKB recruited approximately 500 000 participants aged 40 to 69 years across Great Britain between March 13, 2006, to October 1, 2010.35 Since 2014, a subset of participants underwent brain MRI.36 For this cross-sectional study, we used a data set that included participants who underwent structural brain MRI from August 5, 2014, to October 14, 2016. The UKB received National Research Ethics Approval. All participants provided written informed consent. This study was conducted under terms of UKB access approval and is reported following the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) reporting guideline.
To characterize gray matter regions associated with AD, we used brain MRIs from 100 patients with AD and 133 age-, sex-, and race/ethnicity-matched healthy controls contributing to the ADNI resource.37 The ADNI was launched in 2003 as a public-private partnership, led by Michael W. Weiner, MD. The primary goal of ADNI has been to determine whether serial MRI, positron emission tomography, other biological markers, and clinical and neuropsychological assessments can be combined to measure the progression of mild cognitive impairment and early AD. The ADNI received US ethical approval from 58 study locations, with all participants providing written informed consent. Inclusion and exclusion criteria for the ADNI cohort have been described previously.37
Baseline Characteristics
Information on age, sex, race/ethnicity, MRFD (ie, hypertension, diabetes, obesity, frequency of alcohol use, current smoking, and sleep duration), educational attainment, and history of neurological or psychiatric disease were reported by UKB participants at the time of the imaging assessment. Definitions of MRFD are provided in the eAppendix in the Supplement.25,26,38,39,40,41 Information on histories of neurological or psychiatric diseases was obtained from the International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, Tenth Revision 42 codes in the UKB database (F0-509, F540, F590, F700-890, F990, G40-119, G130-239, G300-419, G460-468, G800-948, G972, I600-I698, and I780). Data on age, sex, and race/ethnicity also were collected from the ADNI database.37
Cognitive Assessment
We used the number of uncorrected matches in the pair matching test,43 the mean time to correctly identify matches in the reaction time test,44 and the number of questions attempted within the time limit for assessment of spatial memory, reaction time, and fluid intelligence45 as described previously46 (see eAppendix in the Supplement). Cognitive assessment data were acquired on the same day of brain MRI acquisition.
Brain MRI Acquisition and Analysis
The MRI acquisition and processing are described in the eAppendix in the Supplement).47,48,49,50,51,52 Briefly, gray matter probabilistic maps were segmented from acquired UKB and ADNI T1 MRIs and normalized to the same standard Montreal Neurological Institute space for voxelwise analyses.
Statistical Analysis
All analyses were performed using Stata statistical software version 14 (StataCorp) unless otherwise stated. Distributions of continuous variables are shown in eFigure 1 in the Supplement. Normally distributed continuous variables were analyzed using the t test, continuous variables which were not distributed normally were analyzed using the Mann-Whitney test, and nominal variables were analyzed using the Pearson χ2 statistic. P values were 2-tailed, and significance levels were set at less than .05 unless otherwise indicated. Familywise error rate (FWER)–corrected P values less than .05 were used as a significance threshold in voxelwise analyses owing to multiple voxel comparisons.53
We first demonstrated independent associations of age and MRFD with total GMV in the UKB cohort (eAppendix in the Supplement). The correlation coefficients across age, MRFD, and the other covariates are shown in eTable 1 in the Supplement. To define the gray matter regions in which volume differences were associated with aging and the MRFD, we performed a voxelwise linear mixed analysis51,54 using a model in which each voxel of the normalized gray matter maps was a dependent variable and age and MRFD were independent variables adjusted for sex, race/ethnicity, educational attainment, and intracranial volume (ICV). We separately contrasted GMVs in the ADNI AD and CH groups on a voxelwise basis to create a binarized mask describing regions showing relative atrophy with AD. These 2 voxelwise analyses in a common brain space allowed for testing whether gray matter regions in which volume differed with aging and MRFD in the UKB population overlapped with those associated with regional atrophy in AD for the ADNI population. This primary analysis was conducted on April 1, 2018.
We also tested whether the association between age and GMV was modified by MRFD in the UKB cohort by entering the product of age and the number of MRFD in the linear regression model with total GMV as the outcome. This interaction was visualized using regressions between normalized total GMV and age for variable numbers of MRFD. Normalized total GMV was calculated for each regression relative to the total GMV to ICV ratio at age 50 years.
Finally, we tested whether associations of aging and multiple MRFD with cognitive impairment were associated with mediation by their brain volume differences using structural equation modeling. Before conducting structural equation modeling, we tested associations of cognitive test scores with brain regions showing differences with aging and MRFD (eAppendix in the Supplement). Associations of aging and MRFD with cognitive test scores also were tested (eAppendix in the Supplement). After testing these associations, structural equation modeling was used to test for mediation of the association of age and numbers of MRFD with spatial memory score by the volume of the posterior cingulate cortex. We chose to explore this mediation using spatial memory score and posterior cingulate cortex volume because of the observed associations of spatial memory with both the number of MRFD and posterior cingulate cortex volume. Direct associations of the MRFD with cognitive function also were tested. Goodness of the model fit was tested using χ2, root mean square error of approximation, Akaike information criterion, comparative fit index, and Tucker-Lewis index.
Results
Characteristics of Participants Selected
Among 9932 participants recruited from the UKB aged 40 to 69 years who had brain MRI data, we excluded 507 individuals lacking cognitive test data, 628 lacking baseline characterization, and 485 individuals with neurological or psychiatric disease. Characteristics of the remaining 8312 participants (mean [SD] age, 62.4 [7.4] years; 3959 [47.1%] men) whose brain images were used in this study are described in the Table. Compared with included participants, the UKB imaging cohort participants whose data were available but who were excluded were older by a mean of 1.2 years (95% CI, 0.8-1.6 years; P < .001), were less likely to be white (8097 white participants [97.4%] vs 1518 white participants [95.8%]; P = .001), and had higher prevalences of risk factors for late-life cognitive impairment, including hypertension (4236 participants [49.0%] vs 718 participants [63.1%]; P < .001), diabetes (400 participants [4.8%] vs 114 participants [7.5%]; P <.001), obesity (1515 participants [18.2%] vs 385 participants [24.2%]; P < .001), current smoking (286 participants [3.4%] vs 126 participants [16.6%]; P < .001), and inadequate sleep (986 participants [11.6%] vs 272 participants [17.8%]; P < .001), and had lower prevalence of college education (3936 participants [47.4%] vs 564 participants [37.2%]; P < .001). Additionally, compared with the included participants, excluded participants had a lower median (interquartile range [IQR]) score for spatial memory (3 [0-5] vs 2 [0-4]; P < .001), higher median (IQR) reaction time (566 [511-636] milliseconds vs 577 [520-655] milliseconds; P < .001), a lower mean (SD) fluid intelligence score (8.82 [2.09] vs 8.64 [2.14]; P < .001), and lower mean ICV volume (difference, 11.5 mL; 95% CI, 3.5-19.5 mL; P = .005), total brain volume, (difference, 20.4 mL; 95% CI, 14.5-26.2 mL; P < .001), total GMV (difference, 13.5 mL; 95% CI, 9.9-17.0 mL; P < .001), and total white matter volume (difference, 6.9 mL; 95% CI, 4.1-9.7 mL; P < .001).
Table. Participants Characteristics.
Characteristic | UK Biobank Cohort, No. (%) | ADNI Cohort, No. (%) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Included Participants, No. (%) (n = 8312) | Excluded Participants, No./Total No. (%) (n = 1620) | Patients With AD (n = 100) | Individuals Who Were CH (n = 133) | |
Nonmodifiable factors | ||||
Age, mean (SD), y | 62.4 (7.4) | 63.6 (7.7) | 74.6 (8.1) | 73.4 (6.3) |
Men | 3959 (47.6) | 770/1620 (47.5) | 57 (57.0) | 65 (48.9) |
White | 8097 (97.4) | 1518/1584 (95.8) | 117 (89.0) | 89 (88.0) |
Intracranial volume, mean (SD), mL | 1547.5 (149.7) | 1536.0 (151.6) | 1500.1 (169.5) | 1488.3 (148.6) |
Overall brain volume measures, mean (SD), mL | ||||
Total brain | 1120.4 (110.0) | 1100.0 (113.5) | 955.5 (112.6) | 1013.9 (116.8) |
Total gray matter | 664.6 (67.0) | 651.2 (68.9) | 557.4 (71.9) | 604.4 (75.3) |
Total white matter | 455.7 (52.9) | 448.8 (54.5) | 398.1 (51.0) | 409.4 (52.1) |
Modifiable risk factors for dementia | ||||
Hypertension | 4236 (49.0) | 718/1138 (63.1) | NA | NA |
Diabetes | 400 (4.8) | 114/1530 (7.5) | NA | NA |
Obesity | 1515 (18.2) | 385/1590 (24.2) | NA | NA |
Current smoking | 286 (3.4) | 126/1527 (8.3) | NA | NA |
Frequent alcohol use | 1478 (17.9) | 257/1549 (16.6) | NA | NA |
Inadequate sleep | 968 (11.6) | 272/1532 (17.8) | NA | NA |
Cognitive test scores and educational attainment | ||||
Mini-Mental State Examination score, median (IQR) | NA | NA | 29 (29-30) | 23 (21-25) |
Spatial memory score, median (IQR) | 3 (0-5) | 2 (0-4)a | NA | NA |
Reaction time, median (IQR), ms | 566 (511-636) | 577 (520-655)b | NA | NA |
Fluid intelligence score, mean (SD) | 8.82 (2.09) | 8.64 (2.14)c | NA | NA |
College degree education | 3936 (47.4) | 564 (37.2)d | ||
Cerebral amyloid standardized uptake value, median (IQR) | NA | NA | 1.05 (1.00-1.18)e | 1.43 (1.25-1.54)f |
Abbreviations: AD, Alzheimer disease; ADNI, Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative; CH, cognitively healthy; IQR, interquartile range; NA, not available.
Includes data from 1550 participants.
Includes data from 1501 participants.
Includes data from 1135 participants.
Includes data from 1518 participants.
Includes data from 97 participants.
Includes data from 128 participants.
The Table also presents characteristics of patients with AD and age-, sex-, and race/ethnicity-matched individuals who were CH selected from the ADNI cohort to develop a gray matter image mask describing regions showing relative atrophy with AD. Lower total brain volume in individuals with AD compared with individuals who were CH (difference, 60.0 mL; 95% CI, 31.4-88.7 mL; P < .001) was explained by lower total GMV alone (difference, 48.5 mL; 95% CI, 30.3-66.7 mL; P < .001); the difference in total white matter volume between the groups was not significant (difference, 11.6 mL; 95% CI, –3.0 to 26.1 mL; P = .12). Consistent with their diagnoses, participants with AD, compared with participants who were CH, had a lower a mean Mini-Mental State Examination score (difference, 6.0; 95% CI, 5.0-7.0; P < .001) and higher mean cerebral amyloid standardized uptake value (difference, 0.31; 95% CI, 0.25-0.37; P < .001).
Overlap of Regions Associated With AD With GMV Differences Explained by Aging and MRFD
We generated an image mask describing the distributions of regional brain volume differences in people with AD compared with people who were CH in the ADNI cohort. A voxelwise analysis showed that the differences were predominantly in the gray matter of the temporal, prefrontal, and posterior cingulate cortices (FWER-corrected P < .05) (Figure 1A). Based on this observation, analyses of the associations of aging and MRFD with brain structure focused on analyses of GMV differences in the UKB cohort.
Before the regional analysis, we tested for the independent associations of aging and 6 MRFD with total GMV of participants in the UKB cohort. Greater age (b = –3.54 mL; SE = 0.06 mL; P < .001) and all MRFD, including hypertension (b = –5.01 mL; SE = 0.96 mL; P < .001), diabetes (b = –14.39 mL; SE = 2.12 mL; P < .001), obesity (b = –4.70 mL; SE = 1.19 mL; P < .001), frequent alcohol use (b = –4.90 mL; SE = 1.18 mL; P < .001), current smoking (b = –6.06 mL; SE = 2.46 mL; P = .01), and inadequate sleep (b = –2.91 mL; SE = 1.39 mL; P = .04), were associated with lower GMV.
We then conducted a voxelwise regression analysis to spatially map these independent associations of age and those of each MRFD. Age-associated differences were found across most of the gray matter (FWER-corrected P < .05) (Figure 1A). These differences extended beyond the gray matter regions in which volume differences associated with AD were found in the ADNI cohort, that is, only 84% of gray matter showing differences associated with age also showed differences associated with AD. Sex-associated differences in regional brain volume are shown in eFigure 2 in the Supplement (FWER-corrected P < .05). Gray matter regions in which volume was lower in men compared with women overlapped those associated with AD and with age. Individual brain regions in which volume was associated with hypertension, diabetes, obesity, or frequent alcohol use are presented in Figure 2. Lower GMV associated with these 4 MRFD (ie, hypertension, diabetes, obesity, and frequent alcohol use) (Figure 2) mapped within the larger volume showing GMV differences associated with age (FWER-corrected P < .05) (Figure 1B) and overlapped in the posterior cingulate cortex, the thalamus, the hippocampus, and the orbitofrontal cortex (Figure 1B). Neuroanatomically different gray matter regions showed negative volume associations with smoking or self-reported inadequate sleep. While the overall association of obesity with GMV was negative, smaller brain regions were found in which obesity was associated with higher GMV.
Interaction of Aging With MRFD
We tested for interactions among the associations of aging and MRFD with GMV. Introducing the product of age and the number of any of 4 MRFD (ie, hypertension, diabetes, obesity, and frequent alcohol use) for each individual improved the fit of the model (b = –0.39 mL; SE = 0.07 mL; P < .001), indicating that MRFD were associated with modifying the associations of age with total GMV. Figure 3 shows a steeper slope for the fit lines in the presence of a greater number of MRFD, suggesting that the association of age with change in total GMV was more pronounced when individuals were exposed to more MRFD (no MRFD, –0.45%/y; 1 MRFD, –0.51%/y; 2 MRFD, –0.56%/y; 3-4 MRFD, –0.60%/y).
Associations of Cognitive Test Scores With Brain Regions Showing Volume Differences With Aging and MRFD
To better understand the potential clinical relevance of the GMV differences found in the generally healthy UKB population, we tested for voxelwise correlations in GMV with cognitive test performance (FWER-corrected P < .05). Better scores of spatial memory performance were positively correlated with greater GMV in the posterior cingulate cortex. Lower fluid intelligence test performance was associated with lower volume in the posterior cingulate cortex, the thalamus, and the orbitofrontal cortex, and reaction time was negatively associated with GMV in the thalamus and the hippocampus (eFigure 3 in the Supplement).
Associations of Aging and MRFD With Cognitive Test Scores
We next tested for associations between aging and the 4 MRFD (ie, hypertension, diabetes, obesity, and frequent alcohol use) and differences in cognitive performance (eTable 2 in the Supplement). As expected, age was negatively associated with performance on tests of spatial memory (b = 0.06; SE = 0.004; P < .001), reaction time (b = 3.77; SE = 0.15; P < .001), and fluid intelligence (b = –0.03; SE = 0.003; P < .001). Participants reporting all 4 MRFD had lower spatial memory performance scores than those without any MRFD (b = 1.52; SE = 0.63; P = .048).
Mediation Associated With Age or MRFD on Cognitive Performance by Brain Volume
Based on the results of these brain volume and cognitive analyses, we used structural equation modeling to explore whether GMV in the posterior cingulate cortex could account for the associations of the 4 MRFD with lower spatial memory performance (Figure 4). There was no direct association of the 4 MRFD with spatial memory (β = –0.013; SE = 0.011; P = .23). However, the model showed that the negative associations of the 4 MRFD with spatial memory were associated with a partial mediation by lower GMV in the posterior cingulate cortex (β = 0.0014; SE = 0.0006; P = .02). There was a direct association of aging with spatial memory (β = 0.138; SE = 0.011; P < .001), and lower GMV mediated this association (β = 0.009; SE = 0.0004; P = .003).
Discussion
This cross-sectional study explored brain structural differences associated with aging and MRFD in a middle-aged UKB population who did not report a history of neurological or psychiatric diseases. Independent associations of 4 MRFD (ie, hypertension, diabetes, obesity, and frequent alcohol use) with differences in GMV associated with age were discovered. Aging and the 4 MRFD were associated with distinct regional differences in GMV, suggesting differences in sensitivity. We hypothesized that these differences reflect sensitivity to the early pathology leading to AD. As hypothesized, we found that regions of lower GMV associated with aging and exposures to the 4 MRFD individually overlapped with each other and with regions of expected GMV loss associated with AD in the posterior cingulate cortex, the thalamus, the hippocampus, and the orbitofrontal cortex. The associations of aging and MRFD with GMV were synergistic, indicating that aging-related GMV loss may be more severe with more MRFD. The clinical relevance of even these small GMV differences was supported by the observation that volume differences in the posterior cingulate cortex were associated with mediation of the associations of age and the 4 MRFD with spatial memory performance.
Two previous population-based studies have mapped brain regions associated with risk factors for dementia. A 2004 study by Taki et al13 included 769 Japanese participants with no known cognitive disorders with information of systolic blood pressure, alcohol use, and smoking. In that cohort, age was negatively correlated with overall brain volume; associations of focal GMV with systolic blood pressure and alcohol use were found, but they did not overlap, possibly because of limitations of power with the smaller sample size.13
A 2019 study by Cox et al20 explored associations of multiple vascular risk factors with brain structure in a UKB population different from ours. Smoking pack-years, hypertension, and higher aggregate vascular risk were associated with lower volumes of several gray matter regions, including the frontal and temporal cortex, and in subcortical gray matter structures. However, despite exploring risk factors chosen for their association with vascular disease, a striking observation in that study was the relatively weak association with direct imaging evidence of cerebrovascular disease in the population (white matter hyperintense volumes on T2-weighted images or an anatomical distribution of gray matter volume differences in regions of brain characteristically associated with large or small vessel disease).20,55,56 The greatest cortical differences associated with the risk factors were found in the anterior temporal cortex. Differences in white matter diffusivity, a measure associated with axon or myelin pathologies, were found throughout the brain. In our study, we used voxelwise rather than atlas region–based analyses and more stringent statistical criteria57 to achieve greater neuroanatomical precision in mapping structural associations. We also investigated the more general hypothesis that risk factors for late-life cognitive impairment may be associated with AD independent of any associations with vascular disease. While our conclusions agree in suggesting regional associations with some risk factors, most of which are also found in the studies by Taki et al13 and Cox et al,20 we do not interpret this regional association as reflecting brain structural changes associated with hypoperfusion or stroke.
We made 3 novel observations. First, we observed associations of aging and 4 MRFD exposures with GMV differences in common regions of the posterior cingulate cortex, the thalamus, the hippocampus, and the orbitofrontal cortex that correspond well with regions of early AD neuropathology.58,59,60,61 Reduced metabolism in the posterior cingulate cortex may precede clinical expression of AD.58 The hippocampus and thalami both show early neurofibrillary changes and can develop pathology associated with core amnestic symptoms.59,60 Widespread neurofibrillary tangle pathology in the orbitofrontal cortex also is found in patients with AD.61
Second, associations of lower GMV with the MRFD were circumscribed within the larger brain regions showing a negative association of GMV with age. The additive contributions to AD risk from aging and these 4 MRFD6,7 may be explained by their anatomically overlapping independent associations with lower GMV. The GMV identified may have common associations with the apparently disparate risk factors through multiple mechanisms, such as involving local cell autonomous factor,62 properties of regional glial, dendritic, and synaptic networks or activity,63,64 or secondary associations mediated by longer range connectivities.65
Third, we provided indirect evidence for the relevance of these observations for late-life cognitive impairments in a relatively healthy middle-aged population. We showed the jointly overlapping associations of lower GMV with aging and the 4 MRFD were found within the larger volume of relative atrophy seen in an independent population with AD. Consistent with earlier studies, we found independent associations of age and the 4 MRFD with lower cognitive performance.32,66,67,68,69 We also found that variation in an overlapping posterior cingulate cortex GMV associated with aging and the 4 MRFD was associated with mediating the negative associations of these risk factors with spatial memory. This association of the relative volume of the posterior cingulate cortex with spatial memory is consistent with a previous report.58
Limitations
This study had limitations, including that it is based on cross-sectional analyses. Effect sizes may be lower than some previous aging studies10,15,16 that included participants 80 years or older because our population was relatively young for the expression of late-life cognitive impairment. The UKB population also is generally healthier than the wider UK population.35 We limited our analyses to regional GMV because this is more directly interpretable than diffusion tensor-based measures of white matter connectivities or functional MRI measures, for example. There also is a larger literature in which to contextualize the results.70,71 Limitations of the data available meant that MRFD were treated categorically rather than by severity and years of exposure to risk; analyses based on the latter would be expected to enhance power to detect differences. Additionally, because of limitations of study power, we only tested for linear associations with aging or MRFD.
Conclusions
This cross-sectional study found that aging and 4 MRFD, including hypertension, diabetes, obesity, and frequent alcohol use, were independently negatively associated with focal GMV in common regions of the posterior cingulate gyrus, the thalamus, the hippocampus, and the orbitofrontal cortex, which are also associated with the progression of AD. The associations observed in this generally healthy population highlight common regions of potential associations of risk factors with dementia and the association of these risk factors with mediation of their associations with reduced cognitive performance. We hypothesize that molecular pathology associated with the early biochemical phase of AD72 may be found in these regions. If the underlying mechanisms of association were identified and able to be targeted therapeutically, early stages of progression to AD could potentially be slowed or reversed.
References
- 1.World Health Organization Dementia: A Public Health Priority. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 2.World Health Organization A Global Brief on Hypertension: Silent Killer, Global Public Health Crisis. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 3.World Health Organization Global Report on Diabetes. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Afshin A, Forouzanfar MH, Reitsma MB, et al. ; GBD 2015 Obesity Collaborators . Health effects of overweight and obesity in 195 countries over 25 Years. N Engl J Med. 2017;377(1):-. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1614362 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Harwood DG, Kalechstein A, Barker WW, et al. . The effect of alcohol and tobacco consumption, and apolipoprotein E genotype, on the age of onset in Alzheimer’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010;25(5):511-518. doi: 10.1002/gps.2372 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Norton S, Matthews FE, Barnes DE, Yaffe K, Brayne C. Potential for primary prevention of Alzheimer’s disease: an analysis of population-based data. Lancet Neurol. 2014;13(8):788-794. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70136-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Livingston G, Sommerlad A, Orgeta V, et al. . Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet. 2017;390(10113):2673-2734. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)31363-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Dickerson BC, Stoub TR, Shah RC, et al. . Alzheimer-signature MRI biomarker predicts AD dementia in cognitively normal adults. Neurology. 2011;76(16):1395-1402. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182166e96 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Dickerson BC, Wolk DA; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative . MRI cortical thickness biomarker predicts AD-like CSF and cognitive decline in normal adults. Neurology. 2012;78(2):84-90. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31823efc6c [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Svennerholm L, Boström K, Jungbjer B. Changes in weight and compositions of major membrane components of human brain during the span of adult human life of Swedes. Acta Neuropathol. 1997;94(4):345-352. doi: 10.1007/s004010050717 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Raz N, Gunning FM, Head D, et al. . Selective aging of the human cerebral cortex observed in vivo: differential vulnerability of the prefrontal gray matter. Cereb Cortex. 1997;7(3):268-282. doi: 10.1093/cercor/7.3.268 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Good CD, Johnsrude IS, Ashburner J, Henson RN, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RS. A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage. 2001;14(1)(pt 1):21-36. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0786 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Taki Y, Goto R, Evans A, et al. . Voxel-based morphometry of human brain with age and cerebrovascular risk factors. Neurobiol Aging. 2004;25(4):455-463. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2003.09.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Taki Y, Kinomura S, Sato K, Goto R, Kawashima R, Fukuda H. A longitudinal study of gray matter volume decline with age and modifying factors. Neurobiol Aging. 2011;32(5):907-915. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.05.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Fotenos AF, Snyder AZ, Girton LE, Morris JC, Buckner RL. Normative estimates of cross-sectional and longitudinal brain volume decline in aging and AD. Neurology. 2005;64(6):1032-1039. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000154530.72969.11 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Cook AH, Sridhar J, Ohm D, et al. . Rates of cortical atrophy in adults 80 years and older with superior vs average episodic memory. JAMA. 2017;317(13):1373-1375. doi: 10.1001/jama.2017.0627 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Strassburger TL, Lee HC, Daly EM, et al. . Interactive effects of age and hypertension on volumes of brain structures. Stroke. 1997;28(7):1410-1417. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.28.7.1410 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Korf ES, White LR, Scheltens P, Launer LJ. Midlife blood pressure and the risk of hippocampal atrophy: the Honolulu Asia Aging Study. Hypertension. 2004;44(1):29-34. doi: 10.1161/01.HYP.0000132475.32317.bb [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Gianaros PJ, Greer PJ, Ryan CM, Jennings JR. Higher blood pressure predicts lower regional grey matter volume: consequences on short-term information processing. Neuroimage. 2006;31(2):754-765. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Cox SR, Lyall DM, Ritchie SJ, et al. . Associations between vascular risk factors and brain MRI indices in UK Biobank. Eur Heart J. 2019;40(28):2290-2300. doi: 10.1093/eurheartj/ehz100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Debette S, Seshadri S, Beiser A, et al. . Midlife vascular risk factor exposure accelerates structural brain aging and cognitive decline. Neurology. 2011;77(5):461-468. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318227b227 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Moran C, Phan TG, Chen J, et al. . Brain atrophy in type 2 diabetes: regional distribution and influence on cognition. Diabetes Care. 2013;36(12):4036-4042. doi: 10.2337/dc13-0143 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Biessels GJ, Reijmer YD. Brain changes underlying cognitive dysfunction in diabetes: what can we learn from MRI? Diabetes. 2014;63(7):2244-2252. doi: 10.2337/db14-0348 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Srinivasa RN, Rossetti HC, Gupta MK, et al. . Cardiovascular risk factors associated with smaller brain volumes in regions identified as early predictors of cognitive decline. Radiology. 2016;278(1):198-204. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2015142488 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Pannacciulli N, Del Parigi A, Chen K, Le DS, Reiman EM, Tataranni PA. Brain abnormalities in human obesity: a voxel-based morphometric study. Neuroimage. 2006;31(4):1419-1425. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.047 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Taki Y, Kinomura S, Sato K, et al. . Relationship between body mass index and gray matter volume in 1,428 healthy individuals. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2008;16(1):119-124. doi: 10.1038/oby.2007.4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Franklin TR, Wetherill RR, Jagannathan K, et al. . The effects of chronic cigarette smoking on gray matter volume: influence of sex. PLoS One. 2014;9(8):e104102. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0104102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Fritz HC, Wittfeld K, Schmidt CO, et al. . Current smoking and reduced gray matter volume: a voxel-based morphometry study. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39(11):2594-2600. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Foubert-Samier A, Catheline G, Amieva H, et al. . Education, occupation, leisure activities, and brain reserve: a population-based study. Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33(2):423.e15-423.e25. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.09.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Liu Y, Julkunen V, Paajanen T, et al. ; AddNeuroMed Consortium . Education increases reserve against Alzheimer’s disease—evidence from structural MRI analysis. Neuroradiology. 2012;54(9):929-938. doi: 10.1007/s00234-012-1005-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Taki Y, Kinomura S, Sato K, et al. . Both global gray matter volume and regional gray matter volume negatively correlate with lifetime alcohol intake in non-alcohol-dependent Japanese men: a volumetric analysis and a voxel-based morphometry. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2006;30(6):1045-1050. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00118.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Topiwala A, Allan CL, Valkanova V, et al. . Moderate alcohol consumption as risk factor for adverse brain outcomes and cognitive decline: longitudinal cohort study. BMJ. 2017;357:j2353. doi: 10.1136/bmj.j2353 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Riemann D, Voderholzer U, Spiegelhalder K, et al. . Chronic insomnia and MRI-measured hippocampal volumes: a pilot study. Sleep. 2007;30(8):955-958. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.8.955 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Spira AP, Gonzalez CE, Venkatraman VK, et al. . Sleep duration and subsequent cortical thinning in cognitively normal older adults. Sleep. 2016;39(5):1121-1128. doi: 10.5665/sleep.5768 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Sudlow C, Gallacher J, Allen N, et al. . UK Biobank: an open access resource for identifying the causes of a wide range of complex diseases of middle and old age. PLoS Med. 2015;12(3):e1001779. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001779 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Miller KL, Alfaro-Almagro F, Bangerter NK, et al. . Multimodal population brain imaging in the UK Biobank prospective epidemiological study. Nat Neurosci. 2016;19(11):1523-1536. doi: 10.1038/nn.4393 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Petersen RC, Aisen PS, Beckett LA, et al. . Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI): clinical characterization. Neurology. 2010;74(3):201-209. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181cb3e25 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Biobank UK Blood pressure: version 1.0. https://biobank.ctsu.ox.ac.uk/crystal/crystal/docs/Bloodpressure.pdf. Accessed November 4, 2019.
- 39.Suzuki H, Gao H, Bai W, et al. . Abnormal brain white matter microstructure is associated with both pre-hypertension and hypertension. PLoS One. 2017;12(11):e0187600. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0187600 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Smith DJ, Nicholl BI, Cullen B, et al. . Prevalence and characteristics of probable major depression and bipolar disorder within UK biobank: cross-sectional study of 172,751 participants. PLoS One. 2013;8(11):e75362. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0075362 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Tsai TC, Wu JS, Yang YC, Huang YH, Lu FH, Chang CJ. Long sleep duration associated with a higher risk of increased arterial stiffness in males. Sleep. 2014;37(8):1315-1320. doi: 10.5665/sleep.3920 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.World Health Organization International Statistical Classification of Diseases, Tenth Revision (ICD-10). Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Biobank UK Touch-screen pairs memory test: version 1.2. http://biobank.ctsu.ox.ac.uk/crystal/crystal/docs/Pairs.pdf. Accessed November 4, 2019.
- 44.Biobank UK Touch-screen test of reaction time (Snap): version 1.1. https://biobank.ndph.ox.ac.uk/showcase/showcase/docs/Snap.pdf. Accessed October 30, 2019.
- 45.Biobank UK Touch-screen fluid intelligence test: version 1.1. http://biobank.ndph.ox.ac.uk/crystal/crystal/docs/Fluidintelligence.pdf. Accessed October 30, 2019.
- 46.Lyall DM, Cullen B, Allerhand M, et al. . Cognitive Test Scores in UK Biobank: data reduction in 480,416 participants and longitudinal stability in 20,346 participants. PLoS One. 2016;11(4):e0154222. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0154222 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Biobank UK Brain imaging documentation: version 1.2. https://biobank.ctsu.ox.ac.uk/crystal/crystal/docs/brain_mri.pdf. Accessed October 30, 2019.
- 48.Wyman BT, Harvey DJ, Crawford K, et al. ; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative . Standardization of analysis sets for reporting results from ADNI MRI data. Alzheimers Dement. 2013;9(3):332-337. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2012.06.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Jack CR Jr, Barnes J, Bernstein MA, et al. . Magnetic resonance imaging in Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative 2. Alzheimers Dement. 2015;11(7):740-756. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2015.05.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Ashburner J. A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage. 2007;38(1):95-113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Suzuki H, Sumiyoshi A, Matsumoto Y, et al. . Structural abnormality of the hippocampus associated with depressive symptoms in heart failure rats. Neuroimage. 2015;105:84-92. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.10.040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Suzuki H, Matsumoto Y, Ota H, et al. . Hippocampal blood flow abnormality associated with depressive symptoms and cognitive impairment in patients with chronic heart failure. Circ J. 2016;80(8):1773-1780. doi: 10.1253/circj.CJ-16-0367 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Nichols T, Hayasaka S. Controlling the familywise error rate in functional neuroimaging: a comparative review. Stat Methods Med Res. 2003;12(5):419-446. doi: 10.1191/0962280203sm341ra [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Suzuki H, Matsumoto Y, Ota H, et al. . Structural brain abnormalities and cardiac dysfunction in patients with chronic heart failure. Eur J Heart Fail. 2018;20(5):936-938. doi: 10.1002/ejhf.1104 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Payabvash S, Souza LC, Wang Y, et al. . Regional ischemic vulnerability of the brain to hypoperfusion: the need for location specific computed tomography perfusion thresholds in acute stroke patients. Stroke. 2011;42(5):1255-1260. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.110.600940 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Enzinger C, Smith S, Fazekas F, et al. . Lesion probability maps of white matter hyperintensities in elderly individuals: results of the Austrian stroke prevention study. J Neurol. 2006;253(8):1064-1070. doi: 10.1007/s00415-006-0164-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Lindquist MA, Mejia A. Zen and the art of multiple comparisons. Psychosom Med. 2015;77(2):114-125. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000148 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Leech R, Sharp DJ. The role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease. Brain. 2014;137(Pt 1):12-32. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt162 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Braak H, Braak E. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol. 1991;82(4):239-259. doi: 10.1007/BF00308809 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Aggleton JP, Pralus A, Nelson AJ, Hornberger M. Thalamic pathology and memory loss in early Alzheimer’s disease: moving the focus from the medial temporal lobe to Papez circuit. Brain. 2016;139(Pt 7):1877-1890. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww083 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Van Hoesen GW, Parvizi J, Chu CC. Orbitofrontal cortex pathology in Alzheimer’s disease. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10(3):243-251. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.243 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Fu H, Possenti A, Freer R, et al. . A tau homeostasis signature is linked with the cellular and regional vulnerability of excitatory neurons to tau pathology. Nat Neurosci. 2019;22(1):47-56. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0298-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Lesuis SL, Hoeijmakers L, Korosi A, et al. . Vulnerability and resilience to Alzheimer’s disease: early life conditions modulate neuropathology and determine cognitive reserve. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2018;10(1):95. doi: 10.1186/s13195-018-0422-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Bero AW, Yan P, Roh JH, et al. . Neuronal activity regulates the regional vulnerability to amyloid-β deposition. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14(6):750-756. doi: 10.1038/nn.2801 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Small SA, Swanson LW. A network explanation of Alzheimer’s regional vulnerability. Cold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol. 2018;83:193-200. doi: 10.1101/sqb.2018.83.036889 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Harada CN, Natelson Love MC, Triebel KL. Normal cognitive aging. Clin Geriatr Med. 2013;29(4):737-752. doi: 10.1016/j.cger.2013.07.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Iadecola C, Yaffe K, Biller J, et al. ; American Heart Association Council on Hypertension; Council on Clinical Cardiology; Council on Cardiovascular Disease in the Young; Council on Cardiovascular and Stroke Nursing; Council on Quality of Care and Outcomes Research; Stroke Council . Impact of hypertension on cognitive function: a scientific statement from the American Heart Association. Hypertension. 2016;68(6):e67-e94. doi: 10.1161/HYP.0000000000000053 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Moheet A, Mangia S, Seaquist ER. Impact of diabetes on cognitive function and brain structure. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2015;1353:60-71. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12807 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Nguyen JC, Killcross AS, Jenkins TA. Obesity and cognitive decline: role of inflammation and vascular changes. Front Neurosci. 2014;8:375. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00375 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.May A. Experience-dependent structural plasticity in the adult human brain. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15(10):475-482. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Zatorre RJ, Fields RD, Johansen-Berg H. Plasticity in gray and white: neuroimaging changes in brain structure during learning. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15(4):528-536. doi: 10.1038/nn.3045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Sala Frigerio C, De Strooper B. Alzheimer’s Disease mechanisms and emerging roads to novel therapeutics. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2016;39:57-79. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-070815-014015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.