Abstract
Concern over loss of cognitive function, including descent into Alzheimer’s disease or dementia, grips a growing percentage of men and women worldwide as the global population ages. Many studies, though not all, suggest that maintaining cognitive health, as well as slowing and even preventing cognitive decline, dementia, and Alzheimer’s disease, can be achieved by consuming healthy diets over a long enough period of time. This appears to be the case even for those who initiated dietary changes later in life, as evidenced by an intervention study assessing consumption of a healthy diet among those who were >50 years of age. All such diets share the common traits of being rich in fruits, vegetables, whole grains, and fish or seafood, while also being low in red meat and sweets. A Mediterranean-style diet shares these characteristics and has been associated with an estimated 40% lower risk of cognitive impairment, including mild cognitive impairment, dementia, and Alzheimer’s disease in prospective studies, in addition to being associated with both a 65% lower risk of mild cognitive impairment and improved cognitive performance in a notable randomized controlled trial.
Keywords: cognition, cognitive health, cognitive function, mild cognitive impairment, dementia, Alzheimer’s disease, diet, dietary patterns, Mediterranean diet
‘The MD [Mediterranean-style diet] has been studied by many more investigators than any other healthy dietary pattern.’
The role of diet in maintaining cognitive health is currently of considerable research interest. Dietary patterns are often studied, rather than focusing on isolated nutrients or specific foods, for several reasons. People consume complete diets, rather than foods in isolation, thus in order to make dietary recommendations easier to understand and follow, elucidating beneficial dietary patterns is of great benefit.1 Also, when examining multiple components of a diet simultaneously, food and nutrient interactions can be revealed together for their harm or benefit.2,3 Dietary patterns with varying definitions including a Mediterranean-style diet (MD),4-23 the DASH (Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension) diet,13,24 and the French national nutrition and health program25 have been evaluated for their association with several measures of cognitive health.
Cognitive Health
Cognitive health can be studied in several different ways, for example, by assessing (a) the rate of cognitive decline,16-18,20,26,27 (b) the maintenance of cognitive performance,* or (c) by studying the incidence of cognitive diseases such as dementia, mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or Alzheimer’s disease (AD).4-10 Dementia is characterized by a debilitating loss of memory, cognitive function, and the ability to live independently.30,31 It currently affects approximately 47 million people worldwide, with prevalence expected to more than triple by 2050.31 With the escalating cost of dementia reaching 1% of the world’s gross domestic product in 2010, and as the prevalence of dementia increases over time concomitant with the rise in the aging population globally, the economic, societal, and personal burdens of disease become increasingly evident.31
Dementia is the umbrella under which MCI and AD fall.30,31 MCI is considered the first step along the path away from normal cognitive aging.32 Those with MCI fare worse on tests of cognitive health than those who are aging normally, but still perform better than those with AD.32 AD is typified by neuronal damage and a loss of connectivity between neurons in the brain, leading to an impairment of proper brain function.33 Among some diagnosed with AD, amyloid plaques (deposits of amyloid protein in the spaces between neurons in the brain) and tau tangles (tau protein deposited inside neurons of the brain that clump together forming neurofibrillary tangles causing the microtubule network within the neuron to collapse) develop.33 Early-onset AD is typically seen among those with genetic predisposition and can occur among those in their late 30s to those in their 50s. Late-onset AD, which accounts for the majority of cases, may be multifactorial in its causes, with genetics, environment, and lifestyle all potentially playing a role.33 Tests of memory and language can be used to assess cognitive status, for example, the Mini-Mental State Examination; alternatively, the DSM-IV criteria for dementia diagnosis can been used.34,35 Because AD develops over a long period of time, lacking AD diagnosis may not mean that brain pathology indicative of AD does not exist. In postmortem brain examinations from the Rush Memory and Aging Project, more than half of the cohort exhibited neuropathology fulfilling the criteria for AD; however, not all of those who fulfilled the criteria were diagnosed with dementia (as defined by clinical exam and the Mini-Mental State Exam); in fact, 43.9% of those without dementia were found to have AD brain pathology.36,37 And studies have shown that even among those who are largely asymptomatic, beta-amyloid accumulation, indicative of AD, leads to reduced brain cell connectivity and is a signal of preclinical AD.33 Therefore, it is plausible that undetected disease progression may affect many lifestyle factors, which means that reverse causation may be an important limitation when conducting epidemiologic studies.
Dietary Patterns
Several healthy dietary patterns have been associated with improved cognitive function, and these dietary patterns have several components in common: a high consumption of fruits, vegetables, and whole grains along with a low consumption of red meat and sweets (Table 1). Interestingly, several dietary components that have been shown to be beneficial for cognitive health are a part of many of these dietary patterns. Antioxidants, polyphenols, long-chain omega-3 fatty acids, and polyunsaturated and monounsaturated fats have been shown to be beneficial, while saturated fats, omega-6 fatty acids, and refined carbohydrates and sugars have been shown to be detrimental.15,21
Table 1.
Mediterranean Diet | DASH Diet | MIND Diet | Healthy Eating Index | French National Nutrition and Health Program | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
High consumption | Fruits | Fruits | Berries | Fruits | Fruits |
Vegetables | Vegetables | Vegetables (green leafy vegetables) | Vegetables | Vegetables | |
Legumes | Whole grains | Whole grains | Whole grains | Whole grains | |
Nuts | Poultry | Poultry | Low-fat dairy | Vegetable fats | |
Fish | Fish | Seafood | Fish | Seafood | |
Seafood | Nuts | Nuts | Lean meats | Water | |
Beans | |||||
Wine | |||||
Olive oil | |||||
Moderate consumption | Alcohol | Dairy | |||
Bread | |||||
Potatoes | |||||
Cereals | |||||
Legumes | |||||
Poultry | |||||
Eggs | |||||
Alcohol | |||||
Low consumption | Dairy products | Fats (low-fat diet) | Red meat | Soda | |
Meat (processed meats) | Red meat | Fast food (fried foods) | Sweets | ||
Sugar sweetened beverages | Cheese | Salt | |||
Desserts | Butter | ||||
Margarine (solid) | |||||
Sweets | |||||
Pastries |
The MD4-23 has been studied by many more investigators than any other healthy dietary pattern. The MD, the definition of which may vary between studies and populations, is typically defined as being higher in fruits, vegetables, legumes, nuts, fish and seafood while also being lower in dairy products and meat, particularly processed meats, and moderate alcohol (typically red wine) consumption20 and has been associated with beneficial* or null† results in several cross-sectional studies, cohort studies, and randomized controlled trials (see Table 2).
Table 2.
Study Design | Dietary Pattern | Outcomes | Associations Found |
---|---|---|---|
Cohort study | Mediterranean | Cognitive impairment | 7 beneficial4-6,8,10,16,18,23 |
Mediterranean | Dementia | 7 beneficial4-6,8,10,16,18,23 | |
Mediterranean | Alzheimer’s disease | 10 null4,5,7-10,17,22,23 | |
Mediterranean | Cognitive performance | 3 beneficial12,137 null11,14,15 | |
Cohort study | DASH | Cognitive performance | 4 beneficial13,241 null24 |
DASH | Alzheimer’s disease | 1 null23 | |
Cohort study | French National Nutrition and Health Program | Cognitive performance | 1 beneficial251 null25 |
Cohort study | Healthy and traditional dietary patterns | Cognitive performance | 2 beneficial281 null28 |
Healthy and traditional dietary patterns | Cognitive decline | 3 null16,27 | |
Cohort study | Canadian Healthy Eating Index | Cognitive performance | 1 null29 |
Cohort study | Recommended food score | Cognitive decline | 1 beneficial26 |
Cohort study | MIND diet | Cognitive decline | 1 beneficial39 |
MIND diet | Alzheimer’s disease | 1 beneficial23 | |
Randomized controlled trial | Mediterranean | Cognitive performance | 3 beneficial19,21 |
Mediterranean | Cognitive impairment | 1 beneficial201 null20 |
Other dietary patterns that have been shown to be beneficial include the DASH diet,13,24 and those that follow national dietary guidelines (such as those of the United States and France).25,26 The main components of these dietary patterns are outlined in Table 1. The Dietary Approached to Stop Hypertension (DASH) diet is a low-fat diet, high in fruits and vegetables, whole grains, poultry, fish, and nuts, with little red meat, sugar sweetened beverages or desserts.38 The most recently developed dietary pattern associated with cognitive health is the Mediterranean-Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension Diet Intervention for Neurodegenerative Delay (MIND) diet. Developed at Rush University, the MIND diet was studied in their Memory and Aging Project cohort. This diet, a combination of the MD and DASH diets, is characterized by brain healthy and unhealthy food groups as determined through literature review.39 The brain healthy food groups include vegetables, particularly green leafy vegetables, berries, whole grains, nuts, beans, seafood, poultry, and wine, with olive oil used as the main cooking oil. The unhealthy food groups include red meat, cheese, butter and margarine (solid), sweets and pastries, and fast food (particularly fried foods).39
The French National Nutrition and Health Program was established to improve health and created dietary guidelines promoting a high consumption of fruits and vegetables, whole grains, fats from vegetable rather than animal sources, seafood and water, moderate dairy, bread, potatoes, cereals, legumes, poultry, eggs, and alcohol intake, and a low consumption of soda, sweets, and salt.25 Healthy and traditional French dietary patterns were also studied in the same cohort. The healthy dietary pattern was high in fiber, calcium, beta-carotene, folic acid, vitamin E, vitamin C, and omega-3 and omega-6 long-chain polyunsaturated fats while also being low in alcohol and saturated fat, while the traditional pattern had similar components but was higher in total fat, including polyunsaturated and monounsaturated fat, alcohol, and protein, while being lower in carbohydrates and calcium.28
Other diets that have been studied include the Traditional Taiwanese diet, characterized by a high intakes of fruits, vegetables, and legumes, and a moderate consumption of meat, poultry, eggs, and fish, as well as a healthy dietary pattern in this same population, characterized by a higher consumption of fruits and vegetables, fish, and legumes, and a lower consumption of meat and eggs.27 Finally, diet quality, as quantified by a recommended food score based on foods included in the American Dietary Guidelines has also been studied in relation to cognitive health and is defined by a higher consumption of fruits, vegetables, whole grains, low-fat dairy, fish, and lean meats.26
A Posteriori Dietary Patterns
In addition to the dietary patterns mentioned above, all of which were established prior to running statistical analyses and are based on typical or traditional dietary patterns (eg, the Mediterranean diet) or recommended diets (eg, the DASH diet), dietary patterns can also been developed using statistical methods. The dietary or biomarker data particular to each cohort being studied is used to develop unique dietary patterns associated with disease outcomes. These are known as a posteriori dietary patterns.
A posteriori dietary pattern can be developed using techniques including reduced rank regression, factor analysis, or cluster analysis.1,40
Using reduced rank regression to explain the most variability in levels of omega-3 and omega-6 polyunsaturated fatty acids, saturated and monounsaturated fatty acids, and vitamins E, B, and folate, Gu et al identified a dietary pattern characterized by higher amounts of fruits, vegetables (cruciferous, dark, and green leafy), tomatoes, fish, and nuts and lower amounts of meat (red and organ), butter, and high-fat dairy products. High adherence to this patterns was associated with a 38% lower risk of AD.41 Ozawa et al determined a dietary pattern explaining the most variation in levels of saturated, monounsaturated, and polyunsaturated fatty acids, vitamin C, calcium, potassium, and magnesium. This pattern was higher in vegetables, soybeans (and soybean products), dairy products, and algae, and lower in rice, and associated with a 34% lower risk of developing dementia and vascular dementia.42
A majority of studies have used principal components analysis (PCA), or factor analysis, to determine dietary patterns associated with cognitive health, where correlated food groups are grouped together.43 For example, using PCA, foods (factors) were identified by Tomata et al that explained the greatest between-person variation in 39 foods consumed by cohort participants that were then used to create a dietary pattern score for each participant. They found that a Japanese dietary pattern, high in vegetables (green and yellow), seaweed, miso soup, rice, pickles, fish, and green tea, and low in coffee, beef, and pork, was associated with a 20% lower risk of developing dementia.1,44 PCA was also used by Akbaraly et al to determine 2 dietary patterns: a “whole food” pattern associated with lower cognitive deficit and a “processed food” pattern associated with greater cognitive deficit.45 The “whole food” pattern was high in leafy and cruciferous vegetables, fruits, tomatoes, fish, salad dressing, and legumes, while the “processed food” pattern was high in desserts, sweets, fried and processed foods, margarine, refined grains, and high-fat dairy.45 Similarly, factor analysis has been used to determine that eating a “Western” dietary pattern (high in red and processed meat, refined carbohydrates, sugar and alcohol, not including wine) led to hastened cognitive decline, while a “prudent” dietary pattern (high in vegetables, fruit, whole grains, fish, low-fat dairy, poultry, legumes, rice, pasta, and water with the use of oils for cooking and dressings) slowed cognitive decline, and those with a high adherence to a “Western” diet tempered the cognitive effects of such eating by nearly 50% when also following the “prudent” dietary pattern closely.46 Pearson et al found that a diet high in green leafy vegetables, tomatoes, salad dressing, and alcohol (wine and liquor) was associated with better cognitive performance and a lower odds of incident cognitive impairment, while a Southern diet, high in fried foods, eggs, organ and processed meats, and sugar sweetened beverages was associated with poorer cognitive performance and an increased odds of incident cognitive impairment.47 Consuming a diet high in vegetables and fruits (particularly tomatoes, dark green and leafy green vegetables, and cruciferous vegetables), as well as a diet high in coffee, nuts, and whole grains, both developed using factor analysis, has been shown to reduce the risk of cognitive impairment.48 Factor analysis was also used by Qin et al to determine a “wheat-based diverse” dietary pattern, similar in some aspects to an adapted Mediterranean diet, high in fruits, nuts, fish, dairy, and grains, while also being low in saturated fats for cooking, that was associated with slowed cognitive decline in a Chinese cohort among adults 65 years of age and older.18
Cluster analysis, where cohort participants are grouped into homogenous clusters,43 has been used to determine a healthy dietary pattern, associated with improved cognitive health, which was high in fruits, vegetables, cereals and bread, and dairy products, and among men this pattern was high in alcohol intake.49 Similarly, in a Korean cohort, Kim et al used cluster analysis and revealed that a diet high in fruits (and fruit juices), multigrain rice, dairy products, and fish was associated with a lower risk of cognitive impairment when compared to a diet higher in white rice, noodles, and coffee (the latter was used as the reference group).43
Overall, these studies lend further support to the claim that consuming a diet rich in fruits and vegetables may help preserve cognitive health.
Studies of Cognitive Health and Dietary Patterns
Randomized Clinical Trials
The use of randomized controlled trials (RCTs) can provide some of the strongest evidence for causation in the diet-disease relationship. RCTs, by design, eliminate the confounding that can skew results in cohort studies (see Table 3).
Table 3.
Author and Year | Cohort | Study Size | Study Period (Mean Years) | Age at Baseline (Mean Years) | Exposure | Outcome | Effect Estimate |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Scarmeas et al (2006) | WHICAP | 2258 | 4 | ≥65 | Mediterranean diet score (tertiles) | Incident Alzheimer’s disease | Continuous HR = 0.91 (95% CI: 0.83, 0.98), P-value 0.015T1 (low) = RefT2 HR = 0.85 (95% CI: 0.63, 1.16)T3 HR = 0.60 (95% CI: 0.42, 0.87), P-value trend = 0.007 |
Scarmeas et al (2009) | WHICAP | 1393 | 4.5 | ≥65 | Mediterranean diet score (tertiles) | Incident mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease | Incident MCI:Continuous HR = 0.92 (95% CI: 0.85, 0.99), P-value 0.04T1 (low) = Ref, P-value trend 0.05T2 HR = 0.83 (95% CI: 0.62, 1.12), P-value 0.24T3 HR = 0.72 (95% CI: 0.52, 1.00), P-value 0.05Incident AD:Continuous HR = 0.89 (95% CI: 0.78, 1.02), P-value 0.09T1 (low) = Ref, P-value trend 0.02T2 HR = 0.55 (95% CI: 0.34, 0.90), P-value 0.01T3 HR = 0.52 (95% CI: 0.30, 0.92), P-value 0.02 |
Scarmeas et al (2009) | WHICAP | 1880 | 5.4 | ≥65 | Mediterranean diet score (tertiles) and physical activity | Incident Alzheimer’s disease | T1 (low) = Ref, P-value trend 0.008T2 HR = 0.98 (95% CI: 0.72, 1.33), P-value 0.88T3 HR = 0.60 (95% CI: 0.42, 0.87), P-value 0.007Low diet score + low PA HR = RefHigh diet score + low PA HR = 0.77 (95% CI: 0.53, 1.13), P-value 0.18Low diet score + high PA HR = 0.81 (95% CI: 0.57, 1.16), P-value 0.26High diet score + high PA HR = 0.65 (95% CI: 0.44, 0.96), P-value 0.03P-value trend: 0.03 |
Feart et al (2009) | Three City | 1410 | 5 | 75.9 | Mediterranean diet score (tertiles) | Incident Alzheimer’s disease | T1 (low) = RefT2 HR = 0.99 (95% CI: 0.51, 1.94)T3 HR = 0.86 (95% CI: 0.39, 1.88), P-value trend: 0.72 |
Feart et al (2009) | Three City | 1410 | 5 | 75.9 | Mediterranean diet score (tertiles) | Incident dementia | T1 (low) = RefT2 HR = 1.11 (95% CI: 0.63, 1.94)T3 HR = 1.12 (95% CI: 0.60, 2.10), P-value trend: 0.72 |
Gu et al (2010) | WHICAP | 1219 | 4 | ≥65 | Mediterranean diet score (tertiles) | Incident Alzheimer’s disease | Continuous HR = 0.87 (95% CI: 0.78, 0.97), P-value 0.01T1 (low) = RefT2 HR = 0.56 (95% CI: 0.36, 0.86), P-value 0.01T3 HR = 0.68 (95% CI: 0.42, 1.08), P-value 0.10P-value trend: 0.06 |
Morris et al (2015) | Rush Memory and Aging Project | 923 | 4.5 | 58-98 (mean ~81 years) | Mediterranean, MIND, and DASH diets | Incident Alzheimer’s disease | Mediterranean diet:T1 (low) = RefT2 HR = 0.81 (95% CI: 0.53, 1.21)T3 HR = 0.49 (95% CI: 0.29, 0.85), P-value trend: 0.01MIND diet:T1 (low) = RefT2 HR = 0.64 (95% CI: 0.42, 0.97)T3 HR = 0.48 (95% CI: 0.29, 0.79), P-value trend:0.003DASH diet:T1 (low) = RefT2 HR = 0.98 (0.64, 1.46)T3 HR = 0.60 (0.37, 0.96), P-value trend: 0.06 |
Roberts et al (2010) | Rochester Epi. Project | 1141 | 2.2 (median) | 70-89 | Mediterranean diet score (tertiles) | Incident MCI or dementia | T1 (low) = RefT2 HR = 0.79 (95% CI: 0.51, 1.21), P-value: 0.28T3 HR = 0.75 (95% CI: 0.46, 1.21), P-value: 0.24 |
Tsivgoulis et al (2013) | REGARDS | 17 478 | 4 | 64.4 | Mediterranean diet score (dichotomous) | Incident cognitive impairment | Low adherence = RefHigh OR = 0.87 (95% CI: 0.76, 1.00), P-value 0.0460Nondiabetics:High OR = 0.81 (95% CI: 0.70, 0.94), P-value 0.0066Diabetics:High OR = 1.27 (95% CI: 0.95, 1.71), P-value 0.1063 |
Samieri et al (2013) | NHS | 16,058 | 13 | 74.3 | Alternate Mediterranean diet (quintiles) | Cognitive status, global cognition, and verbal memory | Cognitive status (mean difference in score):Q1 = Ref, P-value trend: 0.004Q2 = 0.02 (95% CI: −0.02, 0.07), Q3 = 0.03 (95% CI: −0.01, 0.08)Q4 = 0.06 (95% CI: 0.02, 0.11), Q5 = 0.06 (95% CI: 0.01, 0.11)Global cognitive function (mean difference in score):Q1 = Ref, P-value trend: 0.002Q2 = 0.02 (95% CI: −0.01, 0.05), Q3 = 0.03 (95% CI: −0.00, 0.06)Q4 = 0.04 (95% CI: 0.01, 0.07), Q5 = 0.05 (95% CI: 0.01, 0.08)Verbal memory (mean difference in score):Q1 = Ref, P-value trend <0.001Q2 = 0.01 (95% CI: −0.03, 0.04), Q3 = 0.03 (95% CI: −0.01, 0.06)Q4 = 0.04 (95% CI: 0.01, 0.08), Q5 = 0.06 (95% CI: 0.03, 0.10) |
Samieri et al (2013) | WHS | 6174 | 5.6 | 71.9 | Alternate Mediterranean diet (quintiles) | Global cognition and verbal memory | Global cognitive function (mean difference in score):Q1 = Ref, P-value trend 0.63Q2 = 0.03 (95% CI: −0.02, 0.08), Q3 = 0.02 (95% CI: −0.03, 0.06)Q4 = 0.02 (95% CI: −0.03, 0.07), Q5 = 0.02 (95% CI: −0.03, 0.06)Verbal memory (mean difference in score):Q1 = Ref, P-value trend 0.44Q2 = 0.04 (95% CI: −0.02, 0.09), Q3 = 0.01 (95% CI: −0.04, 0.06)Q4 = 0.03 (95% CI: −0.02, 0.08), Q5 = 0.03 (95% CI: −0.02, 0.07) |
Wengreen et al (2013) | Cache County Memory | 3580 | 10.6 | ≥65 | Mediterranean diet score (quintiles) | Cognitive performance (difference −10.6 years) | Q1 = RefQ2 = 0.68 ±- 0.29, Q3 = 0.62 ± 0.29Q4 = 0.83 ± 0.29, Q5 = 0.94 ± 0.29P-value trend: 0.022, P-value Q5 vs Q1: 0.0014 |
Kesse et al (2013) | SU.VI.MAX | 3083 | 13 | 52 | Mediterranean diet and Mediterranean style dietary pattern score (tertiles) | Cognitive performance (mean difference over 13 years) | Mediterranean diet adherence:T1 = −0.18 (−1.09, 0.73)T2 = 0.58 (−0.21, 1.37)T3 (high) = Ref, P-value trend = 0.27Mediterranean style dietary pattern adherence:T1 = −0.41 (−1.23, 0.40)T2 = −0.17 (−0.96, 0.63)T3 (high) = Ref, P-value trend 0.12 |
Psaltopoulou et al (2008) | EPIC-Greece | 732 | 6.4-12.6 | ≥60 | Mediterranean diet score | Cognitive function | Mean MMSE score:β (per unit change in MeDi score) = 0.05(95% CI: −0.09, 0.19), P-value = 0.485 |
Cherubin et al (2012) | PATH Through Life | 1528 | 4 | 60-64 | Mediterranean diet score | Cognitive impairment or dementia | Mild cognitive impairment: OR = 1.41 (95% CI: 0.95, 2.10), P-value 0.087Clinical dementia rating: OR = 1.18 (95% CI: 0.88, 1.57), P-value 0.266Any mild cognitive disorder: OR = 1.20 (95% CI: 0.98, 1.47), P-value 0.079 |
Tangney et al (2013) | CHAP | 3790 | 7.6 | 75.4 | Mediterranean diet score | Cognitive decline | Global cognitive score:MedDiet score β = 0.0014,P-value 0.0004MedDiet wine score β = 0.0014, P-value 0.0009 |
Vercambre et al (2012) | WACS | 2504 | 5.4 | ≥65 | Mediterranean diet score (tertiles) | Cognitive decline | Difference in annual rate of global cognitive decline:T1 (low) = RefT2 = 0.01 (95% CI: −0.01, 0.02)T3 = 0.00 (95% CI: −0.02, 0.01), P-value trend: 0.88 |
Qin et al (2015) | CHNS | 1650 | 5.3 | ≥55 | Adapted Mediterranean diet score (tertiles) | Cognitive decline/change | Difference in mean rate of change in cognitive function:T1 β = RefT2 β = 0.018 (−0.019, 0.056)T3 β = 0.042 (0.001, 0.027) |
Martínez-Lapiscina et al (2013) | PREDIMED (RCT) | 522 | 6.5 | 74.6 | Mediterranean diet + nuts or EVOO | Cognitive performance | Mean difference in MMSE score:Low fat = RefMedDiet + EVOO: +0.62 (95% CI: 0.18, 1.05), P-value 0.005MedDiet + Nuts: +0.57 (95% CI: 0.11, 1.03), P-value 0.015Mean difference in CDT score:Low fat = RefMedDiet + EVOO: +0.51 (95% CI: 0.20, 0.82), P-value 0.001MedDiet + Nuts: +0.33 (95% CI: 0.003, 0.67), P-value 0.048 |
Valls-Pedret et al (2015) | PREDIMED (RCT) | 447 | 4.1 (median) | 66.9 | Mediterranean diet + nuts or EVOO | Cognitive performance change | Change in cognitive composites score:Low-fat = −0.38 (−0.57, −0.18)MedDiet + EVOO OR = 0.05 (−0.11, 0.21), P-value 0.005MedDiet + Nuts OR = −0.05 (−0.27, 0.18), P-value <0.05 |
Martínez-Lapiscina et al (2013) | PREDIMED (RCT) | 285 | 6.5 | 74.1 | Mediterranean diet + nuts or EVOO | Cognitive status (mild cognitive impairment) | Low-fat = RefMedDiet + EVOO OR = 0.341 (95% CI: 0.120, 0.969), P-value 0.044MedDiet + Nuts OR = 0.563 (95% CI: 0.222, 1.427), P-value 0.226 |
Kesse et al (2011) | SU.VI.MAX | 2135 | 13 | 52.6 men; 51.6 women | French National Nutrition and Health Program (quartiles) | Cognitive performance | Verbal Memory:β = 0.41 (95% CI: 0.17, 0.64) P-value <.05Q1 = 48.81 ± 0.82, Q2 = 49.59 ± 0.83Q3 = 50.48 ± 0.83, Q4 = 50.43 ± 0.85, P-value trend = 0.003Executive functioning:β = −0.09 (95% CI: −0.33, 0.14), P-value >.05Q1 = 49.46 ± 0.84, Q2 = 49.66 ± 0.85Q3 = 49.90 ± 0.85, Q4 = 49.04 ± 0.86, P-value trend = 0.60 |
Kesse et al (2012) | SU.VI.MAX | 3054 | 13 | 52.1 | Healthy and traditional dietary patterns (quartiles) | Cognitive performance (mean test score) | Global cognitive function:Q1 = 48.9 ± 0.7, Q2 = 49.4 ± 0.7Q3 = 50.8 ± 0.7, Q4 = 50.1 ± 0.7, P-value trend: 0.001Verbal memory:Q1 = 49.1 ± 0.7, Q2 = 49.5 ± 0.7Q3 = 50.6 ± 0.7, Q4 = 50.3 ± 0.7, P-value trend: 0.01Executive functioning:Q1 = 49.3 ± 0.7, Q2 = 49.6 ± 0.7Q3 = 50.6 ± 0.7, Q4 = 49.8 ± 0.7, P-value trend 0.13 |
Shatenstein et al (2012) | NuAge | 1488 | 3 | 74.1 men; 74.4 women | Canadian Healthy Eating Index | Cognitive performance | β = −0.0008 ± 0.00403, P-value = 0.852 |
Wengreen et al (2013) | Cache County Memory | 3580 | 10.6 | ≥65 | DASH diet score (quintiles) | Cognitive performance (difference over 10.6 years) | Q1 = RefQ2 = 0.35 ± 0.29, Q3 = 0.68 ± 0.29Q4 = 0.96 ± 0.29, Q5 = 0.97 ± 0.29, P-value trend: 0.0001, P-value Q5 vs Q1: 0.0009 |
Smith et al (2010) | ENCORE (RCT) | 124 | 4 months | 52.3 | DASH diet score | Cognitive function | Psychomotor speed test:DASH P-value 0.36, DASH + WM P-value 0.023Executive function-memory-learning tests:DASH: no significant P-valuesDASH + WM P-values: 0.026, 0.045, and 0.024 |
Wengreen et al (2013) | Cache County Memory | 3580 | ~11 | ≥65 | Recommended food score (quartiles) | Cognitive decline (decrease in score over 11 years) | Q1 = −5.15 ± 0.69 Least Varied DietQ2 = −3.98 ± 1.0, Q3 = −4.91 ± 1.08Q4 = −3.41 ± 0.79 Most Varied DietP-value Q5 vs Q1: 0.013 |
Tangney et al (2013) | CHAP | 3790 | 7.6 | 75.4 | Healthy eating index score | Cognitive decline | Global cognitive score: β = 0.0002, P-value = 0.214 |
Tsai et al (2015) | TLSA | 23578 | 8 | 73 | Traditional dietHealthy diet | Cognitive decline | Traditional diet: OR = 1.37 (0.85, 2.21), P-value 0.20Healthy diet: OR = 1.13 (0.53, 2.41), P-value 0.75 |
Morris et al (2015) | Rush Memory and Aging Project | 960 | 4.7 | 81.4 | MIND diet | Cognitive decline | Global cognitive score: β = 0.0092, P-value <0.0001 |
Abbreviations: HR, hazard ratio; CI, confidence interval; MCI, mild cognitive impairment; PA, physical activity; OR, odds ratio; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; RCT, randomized controlled trial.
RCTs of the MD have provided strong evidence that adhering to this diet can help maintain cognitive health. The Spanish PREDIMED RCT has shown that higher consumption of a MD, supplemented daily with either olive oil or nuts over 6.5 years, among those who are on average 74 years of age, can increase cognitive performance and decrease the risk of MCI by up to 65% when compared to a low-fat diet (mean difference in Mini-Mental State Exam score for MD + extra virgin olive oil [EVOO]: +0.62, 95% confidence interval [CI]: 0.18, 1.05, P-value = 0.005, MD + Nuts: +0.57, 95% CI: 0.11, 1.03, P-value = 0.015; incidence of MCI for MD + EVOO: odds ratio [OR] = 0.341, 95% CI: 0.120, 0.969, P-value = 0.044, MD + Nuts: OR = 0.563, 95% CI: 0.222, 1.427, P-value 0.226).19
Evidence for an association between the MIND diet and both cognitive decline and brain neurodegeneration will be examined in a recently funded phase III, 3-year, RCT in the United States. Among 600 individuals, 65 years of age and older, those without cognitive impairment will be studied. The participants are not currently eating healthy diets and all are overweight.50
Prospective Studies
Prospective studies have investigated several endpoints related to cognitive health, and while the aforementioned RCT has provided the strongest evidence for the importance of consuming a healthy diet for the maintenance of cognitive health, it is important to note that proper adjustment for the correct confounders can lead investigators using observational data to obtain results comparable to those found in RCTs.51
We will begin by reviewing the associations found between diet and incident MCI, AD, and dementia. Scarmeas et al found, in an elderly cohort of participants ≥65 years of age, that over 4.5 years those people in the highest tertile of MD consumption had the lowest risk of MCI compared to those in the lowest tertile (hazard ratio [HR] = 0.72, 95% CI: 0.52, 1.00, P-value = 0.05).5 And, among those who developed MCI the risk of progression to AD was 45% lower among those with moderate adherence (HR = 0.55, 95% CI: 0.34, 0.90, P-value = 0.01) and 48% lower among those with the highest adherence to the MD (HR = 0.52, 95% CI: 0.30, 0.92, P-value = 0.02).5 In addition, Tsivgoulis et al found that over 4 years those who consumed a MD had lower odds of incident cognitive impairment (high OR = 0.87, 95% CI: 0.76, 1.00, P-value = 0.0460) among participants who were, on average, 64 years of age at baseline.10 However, Roberts et al did not find a significant relationship between incidence of MCI and the MD (highest tertile vs lowest HR = 0.75, 95% CI: 0.46, 1.21, P-value = 0.24) in their shorter study of 2.2 years among relatively older participants 70-89 years of age.9
With the incidence of AD as the study endpoint, Morris et al recently found that those in the highest tertile of MD consumption had a significantly lower incidence of AD than those in the lowest tertile (HR = 0.46, 95% CI = 0.26, 0.79) over 4.5 years among participants between 58 and 98 years of age (average age was approximately 81 years).23 They also found that those in the highest 2 tertiles of MIND diet consumption had a lower rate of AD than those in the lowest tertile (tertile 3 vs 1 HR = 0.47, 95% CI = 0.26, 0.75; tertile 2 vs 1 HR = 0.65, 95% CI = 0.44, 0.98).23 Scarmeas et al found, in 3 separate studies where participants were ≥65 years of age, that those people in the highest tertile of MD consumption had a lower risk of AD compared to those in the lowest tertile (HR = 0.60, 95% CI: 0.42, 0.87, P-value trend = 0.008;6 HR = 0.52, 95% CI: 0.30, 0.92, P-value 0.02;5 HR = 0.60, 95% CI: 0.42, 0.87, P-value = 0.0074) over a period of up to 5.4 years. However, Gu et al found in the same cohort that there was no significant benefit to consuming such a diet over 4 years among those in the highest tertile compare to those in the lowest tertile of diet consumption (HR = 0.68, 95% CI: 0.42, 1.08, P-value = 0.10),8 but that those in the second tertile with a moderate consumption of MD had a 44% lower risk of developing AD (HR = 0.56, 95% CI: 0.36, 0.86, P-value = 0.01); it may be that adjustment for intermediate inflammatory biomarkers (hsCRP, insulin and adiponectin) attenuated the relationship between AD and the MD in this analysis (the trend of increasing risk of AD along increasing tertiles of MD was significant when there was no adjustment for biomarkers, P-value = 0.04).8 Feart et al similarly found null results among participants who were 75.9 years of age, on average, over 5 years when comparing those in the highest tertile of MD consumption to those in the lowest (HR = 0.86, 95% CI: 0.39, 1.88, P-value = 0.71).7
These data suggest that the MD may be beneficial in preventing the incidence of MCI and/or AD, and while some studies have found null results, there have not any been studies suggesting any harm in consuming a MD.
With incident dementia as the study endpoint, which encompasses many more specific diseases including AD, the relationship with the MD is less clear, with studies finding no significant benefit for those consuming the highest amounts of these foods when compared to those consuming the least over 5 years among those who were 75.9 years on average (HR = 1.12, 95% CI: 0.60, 2.10, P-value = 0.72),7 and over 2.2 years among those 70 to 89 years of age (HR = 0.75, 95% CI: 0.46, 1.21, P-value = 0.24).9 These were, however, studies of relatively short duration among participants who were relatively old, thus, significant results might not have been found as a longer period of cumulative MD consumption earlier in life may be required to alter dementia outcomes. However, a recent meta-analysis found, when considering dementia, AD, and MCI together in their analysis, that consumption of the MD was associated with a significantly decreased risk of these outcomes (HR = 0.69, 95% CI: 0.57, 0.84).52
Results on dietary patterns and the rate of cognitive decline, as a separate and distinct endpoint, have been mixed. When considering cognitive decline and increased dietary variety as a measure of diet health, those who consumed the most varied diet, as based on the recommended food score following the American Dietary Guidelines, had test scores that did not decrease as quickly as those with the least varied diet over 11 years (Q1 [lease varied diet] = −5.15 ± 0.69, Q4 [most varied diet] = −3.41 ± 0.79, P-value Q4 vs Q1 = 0.0013) among those who were ≥65 years of age.26 While improved diet quality may be associated with slowed cognitive decline, this finding has not been replicated in other studies of healthy dietary patterns, aside from the MD. Tangney et al found that those with a higher adherence to a Healthy Eating Index, based on the American Dietary Guidelines, did not have a significantly slower rate of cognitive decline, among those 75.4 years of age on average, over a 7.6-year period (for each 1-point increase in the Health Eating Index score, with a score range from 0 to 55 and the cognitive score was higher by β = 0.0002, P-value = 0.214).16 Similarly, Tsai et al found no association between cognitive decline and (a) a traditional Taiwanese diet (OR = 1.37, 95% CI = 0.85, 2.21, P-value 0.20) or (b) a healthy dietary pattern (OR = 1.13, 95% CI = 0.53, 2.41, P-value 0.75) over 8 years among those who were 73 years of age on average.27
Taking a closer look at cognitive decline and the MD, Tangney et al found, among participants who were an average of 75.4 years of age, that those with a higher adherence to the MD had a slower rate of cognitive decline over a 7.6-year period, where cognitive function was assessed every 3 years (for each 1-point increase in MD score, with a score range from 0 to 55, the cognitive score was higher by β = 0.0014, P-value = 0.0004).16 Similarly, Qin et al found that those in the highest tertile of MD consumption had a slower rate of cognitive decline over an average of 5.3 years when compared to those in the lowest tertile among participants who were ≥55 years of age (difference in mean standardized unit change per year β = 0.042, 95% CI: 0.001, 0.027).18 These differences in cognitive score are modest and other studies have not found a significant association between the MD and a slowed rate of cognitive decline; however, it is notable that the null studies presented below were conducted over shorter periods of time that may not adequately capture the cumulative effect of the MD on cognitive health.
Vercambre et al found no significant difference in the annual rate of cognitive decline over 5 years among women ≥65 years of age when comparing those in the highest tertile of MD to those in the lowest (rate of change in global cognitive score = 0.00, 95% CI: −0.02, 0.01, P-value = 0.88).17 Additionally, Cherubin et al found no association between the MD and cognitive change over 4 years among participants 60 to 64 years of age (MCI: OR = 1.41, 95% CI: 0.95, 2.10, P-value 0.087; Clinical dementia rating: OR = 1.18, 95% CI: 0.88, 1.57, P-value 0.266; Any mild cognitive disorder: OR = 1.20, 95% CI: 0.98, 1.47, P-value 0.079).22 Thus, it appears that consumption of a MD may have only a moderate impact on preventing cognitive decline over the shorter periods of time used in these studies, but such limited effects should not be extrapolated to infer that there is little benefit to consuming a MD over one’s lifetime as benefits have been shown in studies of longer duration.
Interestingly, Morris et al found over approximately 4.5 years that among participants who were 81.4 years of age, on average, that those with a higher adherence to the MIND diet experienced slowed cognitive decline (β = 0.0092; P-value <.0001), effectively meaning that they were the equivalent of 7.5 years younger than study participants with a low adherence to the MIND diet.39
Results were also mixed when cognitive performance was measured as an endpoint, where measures of cognitive function were studied (instead of the rate of cognitive decline) to determine if those consuming a healthy diet were better at maintaining their cognitive health. Study period duration may again have a bearing on the results, as can be seen in the analyses of 2 separate cohorts of women by Samieri et al. In the first study, conducted in the Nurses’ Health Study over an average of 13 years among women who were on average 74.3 years of age, they found that those consuming the highest quintile of an Alternate MD which uniquely emphasizes the importance of increased monounsaturated fat consumption along with lower saturated fat consumption, in addition to giving importance to the staples of a typically defined MD that is rich in fruits, vegetables, whole grains, nuts, and fish12 had higher scores for global cognitive function (mean difference in Z-score Q5 vs Q1 = 0.05, 95% CI: 0.01, 0.08, P-value trend along quintiles = 0.002), verbal memory (mean difference in Z-score Q5 vs Q1 = 0.06, 95% CI: 0.03, 0.10, P-value trend <.001), and cognitive status, as measured by the Telephone Interview of Cognitive Status, adapted from the Mini-Mental State Exam (mean difference in Z-score Q5 vs Q1 = 0.06, 95% CI: 0.01, 0.11, P-value trend = 0.004) when compared to those in the lowest quintile. In the second study, conducted in the Women’s Health Initiative over 5.6 years among women who were an average of 71.9 years of age, they found no significant associations between the Alternate MD and cognitive function (global cognitive function mean difference in Z-score Q5 vs Q1 = 0.02, 95% CI: −0.03, 0.06, P-value trend along quintiles = 0.63, verbal memory mean difference in Z-score Q5 vs Q1 = 0.03, 95% CI: −0.02, 0.07, P-value trend = 0.44).11 We see from these 2 studies that significant results were found when the study period was longer, perhaps meaning that cumulative diet over a longer period of time may be needed to effect cognitive health. This is further evidenced by a study from Wengreen et al who found that those in the highest quintile of MD consumption scored significantly higher on a test of cognitive function than those in the lowest quintile over an 11-year period among participants ≥65 years of age (difference in Modified Mini-Mental State Exam score, Q5 vs Q1 = 0.94 ± 0.29, P-value = 0.001).13
Two additional studies found no significant relationship between the MD and cognitive function, even though they were conducted over more than a decade. Kesse-Guyot et al investigated the association between a MD computed in a fashion similar to the above studies as well as a MD pattern including the same dietary components, but weighting the dietary pattern score by the quantities of foods eaten; for example, if 50% of the foods eaten by a participant are included in the MD score, their total diet score is weighted by 0.5. Over a 13-year study period they found no significant association between either MD and most tests of cognitive function, including a composite cognitive score among participants who were an average of 52 years of age. And Psaltopoulou also found no significant difference in cognitive function with increasing adherence to a MD over a 6- to 13-year period among those who were ≥60 years of age (β = 0.05, 95% CI: −0.09, 0.19, P-value = 0.485).15 However, Kesse-Guyot et al did find that those consuming a MD had higher scores of short-term and working memory (difference in backward digit span score comparing low to high dietary adherence = −0.64, 95% CI: −1.60, 0.32, and medium to high = 0.03, 95% CI: −0.81, 0.86, P-value trend = 0.03) and those consuming the weighted MD had higher scores of lexical-semantic memory, where the test involves listing all known words beginning with the letter “p” in 2 minutes (difference in phonemic fluency score comparing low to high dietary adherence = −1.00, 95% CI: −1.85, −0.15, and medium to high = −0.61, 95% CI: −1.45, 0.22, P-value trend = 0.05).14
Higher consumption of the DASH diet has been associated with improved cognitive performance over an 11-year period among those who were ≥65 years of age (difference in Modified Mini-Mental State Exam score, Q5 vs Q1 = 0.97 ± 0.29, P-value = 0.0000).13 Another study found that both the DASH diet alone or the DASH diet plus 30 minutes of aerobic exercise 3 times per week and weight loss counseling lead to improved cognitive test results after only 4 months of intervention among those who were an average of 52.3 years of age (Psychomotor speed test: DASH P-value = 0.036, DASH + exercise/counseling P-value = 0.023, Executive function-memory-learning tests: DASH + exercise/counseling P-value <.05).24
Higher adherence to the French National Nutrition and Health Program dietary pattern was associated with higher scores on tests of verbal memory (β = 0.41, 95% CI: 0.17, 0.64, P-value < 0.05, P-value trend across continuous quartiles = 0.003) but not with tests of executive function (β = −0.09, 95% CI: −0.33, 0.14, P-value > 0.05, P-value trend across continuous quartiles = 0.60) over 13 years among men and women who were on average 52 years of age.25 Healthy and traditional French dietary patterns were also studied in the same cohort. They found that those with a higher adherence to a healthy diet had higher test scores of global cognitive function and verbal memory, but no significant difference in executive functioning (global cognitive function: Q1 [low dietary adherence] = 48.9 ± 0.7, Q4 [high dietary adherence] = 50.1 ± 0.7, P-value trend = 0.001; verbal memory: Q1 = 49.1 ± 0.7, Q4 = 50.3 ± 0.7, P-value trend = 0.01; executive functioning: Q1 = 49.3 ± 0.7, Q4 = 49.8 ± 0.7, P-value trend = 0.13) and there was no significant difference in test scores comparing those in the highest to lowest quartiles of adherence to the traditional French diet (global cognitive function: P-value trend = 0.68; verbal memory: P-value trend = 0.32; executive functioning: P-value trend = 0.60).28 Similarly, the Canadian Eating Index, based on the Canadian dietary guidelines, has also been studied, but no significant association was found with cognitive performance over 3 years among those who were an average of 74 years of age (β = −0.00008 ± 0.000403, P-value = 0.852).29
Meta-Analyses
Meta-analysis can be a useful way to summarize the literature, when between-study heterogeneity is not pronounced and when methodology is otherwise consistent. The meta-analysis by Psaltopoulou included 8 studies of mixed design (including Scarmeas,4,5 Roberts,9 and Feart,7 discussed above) and found that high adherence to a MD was associated with a lower risk of cognitive impairment, including MCI, dementia and AD, by 40% (RR = 0.60, 95% CI = 0.43, 0.83).15 When they considered only longitudinal cohort studies, apart from cross-sectional or case-control studies, this relationship remained (RR = 0.72, 95% CI = 0.58, 0.88),15 which lends greater support to their conclusion that consuming a MD may prevent cognitive impairment, as reverse causation is potentially minimized by the cohort study design when dietary exposures during the early stages of disease development are not included in the analysis. While this meta-analysis provides evidence of the benefits of consuming a MD, meta-analyses are often limited by the studies they can include—for example, diet needs to be similarly treated in order to make coherent comparisons. In this meta-analysis only studies where diet was treated as a categorical variable, where high or moderate adherence to a MD was compared to low adherence; this, by definition, excludes studies where diet was treated as a continuous variable and, as we have already noted, the findings of these studies were mixed. Furthermore, each study was performed in different populations (some older, some younger) and included different confounders, such as age, sex, or total calories, which might have led to either the attenuation or amplification of some results; the decision to include only certain studies on MD may have affected the overall results of the meta-analysis, leading to the significant protective effect found here. However, a more recent systematic review of prospective studies of the MD and cognition similarly concluded that such a diet was beneficial in regard to all measures of cognitive health.53
Overall, evidence from RCTs and cohort studies support the assertion that the consumption of a healthy dietary pattern can help in maintaining cognitive health. Although some studies found no significant results, only beneficial results were found otherwise. As such, consumption of these diets over time appears to be either beneficial or neutral (at worst) in maintaining cognitive health, particularly if certain MD staples are consumed over an extended period of time. For example, 2 studies found that consuming more whole grains, a main component of the MD that includes foods such as dark bread, oatmeal, and brown rice, tested higher on measures of cognitive health.11,13
Conclusion
Consumption of healthy dietary patterns, including a variety of fruits and vegetables, whole grains, and fish/seafood while also avoiding red and processed meats and sweets may go far in maintaining cognitive health. An intervention study and RCT have shown this to be the case even among those initiating such dietary changes in older age. Of a variety of healthy dietary patterns, the MD has been heavily studied and has been shown to be beneficial in maintaining cognitive health and in preventing cognitive decline, including MCI and AD. It also appears that consumption of a health dietary pattern over a long period of time, perhaps for more than approximately 5 years, may be required to obtain the full benefits of such diets on cognition.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests: The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Funding: The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Ethical Approval: Not applicable, because this article does not contain any studies with human or animal subjects.
Informed Consent: Not applicable, because this article does not contain any studies with human or animal subjects.
Trial Registration: Not applicable, because this article does not contain any clinical trials.
References
- 1. Willett W. Nutritional Epidemiology. 3rd ed. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 2. Fung TT, Feskanich D. Dietary patterns and risk of hip fractures in postmenopausal women and men over 50 years. Osteoporos Int. 2015;26:1825-1830. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Satija A, Yu E, Willett WC, Hu FB. Understanding nutritional epidemiology and its role in policy. Adv Nutr. 2015;6:5-18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Scarmeas N, Luchsinger JA, Schupf N, et al. Physical activity, diet, and risk of Alzheimer disease. JAMA. 2009;302:627-637. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Scarmeas N, Stern Y, Mayeux R, Manly JJ, Schupf N, Luchsinger JA. Mediterranean diet and mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol. 2009;66:216-225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Scarmeas N, Stern Y, Tang MX, Mayeux R, Luchsinger JA. Mediterranean diet and risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol. 2006;59:912-921. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Feart C, Samieri C, Rondeau V, et al. Adherence to a Mediterranean diet, cognitive decline, and risk of dementia. JAMA. 2009;302:638-648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Gu Y, Luchsinger JA, Stern Y, Scarmeas N. Mediterranean diet, inflammatory and metabolic biomarkers, and risk of Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2010;22:483-492. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Roberts RO, Geda YE, Cerhan JR, et al. Vegetables, unsaturated fats, moderate alcohol intake, and mild cognitive impairment. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2010;29:413-423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Tsivgoulis G, Judd S, Letter AJ, et al. Adherence to a Mediterranean diet and risk of incident cognitive impairment. Neurology. 2013;80:1684-1692. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Samieri C, Grodstein F, Rosner BA, et al. Mediterranean diet and cognitive function in older age. Epidemiology. 2013;24:490-499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Samieri C, Okereke OI, Devore E, Grodstein F. Long-term adherence to the Mediterranean diet is associated with overall cognitive status, but not cognitive decline, in women. J Nutr. 2013;143:493-499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Wengreen H, Munger RG, Cutler A, et al. Prospective study of dietary approaches to stop hypertension- and Mediterranean-style dietary patterns and age-related cognitive change: the Cache County Study on Memory, Health and Aging. Am J Clin Nutr. 2013;98:1263-1271. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Kesse-Guyot E, Andreeva VA, Lassale C, et al. Mediterranean diet and cognitive function: a French study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2013;97:369-376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Psaltopoulou T, Kyrozis A, Stathopoulos P, Trichopoulos D, Vassilopoulos D, Trichopoulou A. Diet, physical activity and cognitive impairment among elders: the EPIC-Greece cohort (European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition). Public Health Nutr. 2008;11:1054-1062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Tangney CC, Kwasny MJ, Li H, Wilson RS, Evans DA, Morris MC. Adherence to a Mediterranean-type dietary pattern and cognitive decline in a community population. Am J Clin Nutr. 2011;93:601-607. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Vercambre MN, Grodstein F, Berr C, Kang JH. Mediterranean diet and cognitive decline in women with cardiovascular disease or risk factors. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2012;112:816-823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Qin B, Adair LS, Plassman BL, et al. Dietary patterns and cognitive decline among Chinese older adults. Epidemiology. 2015;26:758-768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Martinez-Lapiscina EH, Clavero P, Toledo E, et al. Mediterranean diet improves cognition: the PREDIMED-NAVARRA randomised trial. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2013;84:1318-1325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Martinez-Lapiscina EH, Clavero P, Toledo E, et al. Virgin olive oil supplementation and long-term cognition: the PREDIMED-NAVARRA randomized, trial. J Nutr Health Aging. 2013;17:544-552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Valls-Pedret C, Sala-Vila A, Serra-Mir M, et al. Mediterranean diet and age-related cognitive decline: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Intern Med. 2015;175:1094-1103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Cherbuin N, Anstey KJ. The Mediterranean diet is not related to cognitive change in a large prospective investigation: the PATH Through Life Study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2012;20:635-639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Morris MC, Tangney CC, Wang Y, Sacks FM, Bennett DA, Aggarwal NT. MIND diet associated with reduced incidence of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2015;11:1007-1014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Smith PJ, Blumenthal JA, Babyak MA, et al. Effects of the dietary approaches to stop hypertension diet, exercise, and caloric restriction on neurocognition in overweight adults with high blood pressure. Hypertension. 2010;55:1331-1338. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Kesse-Guyot E, Amieva H, Castetbon K, et al. Adherence to nutritional recommendations and subsequent cognitive performance: findings from the prospective Supplementation with Antioxidant Vitamins and Minerals 2 (SU.VI.MAX 2) study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2011;93:200-210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Wengreen HJ, Neilson C, Munger R, Corcoran C. Diet quality is associated with better cognitive test performance among aging men and women. J Nutr. 2009;139:1944-1949. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Tsai HJ. Dietary patterns and cognitive decline in Taiwanese aged 65 years and older. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2015;30:523-530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Kesse-Guyot E, Andreeva VA, Jeandel C, Ferry M, Hercberg S, Galan P. A healthy dietary pattern at midlife is associated with subsequent cognitive performance. J Nutr. 2012;142:909-915. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Shatenstein B, Ferland G, Belleville S, et al. Diet quality and cognition among older adults from the NuAge study. Exp Gerontol. 2012;47:353-360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Prince M, Bryce R, Albanese E, Wimo A, Ribeiro W, Ferri CP. The global prevalence of dementia: a systematic review and metaanalysis. Alzheimers Dement. 2013;9:63-75.e62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. World Health Organization. The epidemiology and impact of dementia: current state and future trends. http://www.who.int/mental_health/neurology/dementia/dementia_thematicbrief_epidemiology.pdf. Accessed January 14, 2016.
- 32. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol. 1999;56:303-308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. US Department of Health and Human Services. Progress Report on Alzheimer’s Disease: A Deeper Understanding. Washington, DC: US Department of Health and Human Services; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 34. Annweiler C, Rolland Y, Schott AM, et al. Higher vitamin D dietary intake is associated with lower risk of Alzheimer’s disease: a 7-year follow-up. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2012;67:1205-1211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Annweiler C, Rolland Y, Schott AM, Blain H, Vellas B, Beauchet O. Serum vitamin D deficiency as a predictor of incident non-Alzheimer dementias: a 7-year longitudinal study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2011;32:273-278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Schneider JA, Arvanitakis Z, Bang W, Bennett DA. Mixed brain pathologies account for most dementia cases in community-dwelling older persons. Neurology. 2007;69:2197-2204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Breteler MMB. Epidemiology of Alzheimer Disease, Other Dementing Disorders and Related Pathologies. Boston, MA: Harvard School of Public Health; February 17, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 38. Sacks FM, Svetkey LP, Vollmer WM, et al. Effects on blood pressure of reduced dietary sodium and the Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension (DASH) diet. DASH-Sodium Collaborative Research Group. N Engl J Med. 2001;344:3-10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Morris MC, Tangney CC, Wang Y, et al. MIND diet slows cognitive decline with aging. Alzheimers Dement. 2015;11:1015-1022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Gu Y, Scarmeas N. Dietary patterns in Alzheimer’s disease and cognitive aging. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2011;8:510-519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Gu Y, Nieves JW, Stern Y, Luchsinger JA, Scarmeas N. Food combination and Alzheimer disease risk: a protective diet. Arch Neurol. 2010;67:699-706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Ozawa M, Ninomiya T, Ohara T, et al. Dietary patterns and risk of dementia in an elderly Japanese population: the Hisayama Study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2013;97:1076-1082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Kim J, Yu A, Choi BY, et al. Dietary patterns derived by cluster analysis are associated with cognitive function among Korean older adults. Nutrients. 2015;7:4154-4169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Tomata Y, Sugiyama K, Kaiho Y, et al. Dietary patterns and incident dementia in elderly Japanese: the Ohsaki Cohort 2006 Study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2016;71:1322-1328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Akbaraly TN, Singh-Manoux A, Marmot MG, Brunner EJ. Education attenuates the association between dietary patterns and cognition. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2009;27:147-154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Shakersain B, Santoni G, Larsson SC, et al. Prudent diet may attenuate the adverse effects of Western diet on cognitive decline. Alzheimers Dement. 2016;12:100-109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Pearson KE, Wadley VG, McClure LA, Shikany JM, Unverzagt FW, Judd SE. Dietary patterns are associated with cognitive function in the REasons for Geographic And Racial Differences in Stroke (REGARDS) cohort. J Nutr Sci. 2016;5:e38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Chan R, Chan D, Woo J. A cross sectional study to examine the association between dietary patterns and cognitive impairment in older Chinese people in Hong Kong. J Nutr Health Aging. 2013;17:757-765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Samieri C, Jutand MA, Feart C, Capuron L, Letenneur L, Barberger-Gateau P. Dietary patterns derived by hybrid clustering method in older people: association with cognition, mood, and self-rated health. J Am Diet Assoc. 2008;108:1461-1471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. NIH Research Portfolio Online Reporting Tools. Project 1R01AG052583-01. https://projectreporter.nih.gov/project_info_description.cfm?aid=9081135&icde=31045287&ddparam=&ddvalue=&ddsub=&cr=53&csb=default&cs=ASC. Accessed September 16, 2016.
- 51. Hernan MA, Alonso A, Logan R, et al. Observational studies analyzed like randomized experiments: an application to postmenopausal hormone therapy and coronary heart disease. Epidemiology. 2008;19:766-779. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Cao L, Tan L, Wang HF, et al. Dietary patterns and risk of dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis of cohort studies. Mol Neurobiol. 2016;53:6144-6154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Hardman RJ, Kennedy G, Macpherson H, Scholey AB, Pipingas A. Adherence to a Mediterranean-style diet and effects on cognition in adults: a qualitative evaluation and systematic review of longitudinal and prospective trials. Front Nutr. 2016;3:22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]