Abstract
Reduced food intake is common to many pathological conditions, such as infection and toxin exposure. However, cortical circuits that mediate feeding responses to these threats are less investigated. The anterior insular cortex (aIC) is a core region that integrates interoceptive states and emotional awareness and consequently guides behavioral responses. Here, we demonstrate that the right-side aIC CamKII+ (aICCamKII) neurons in mice are activated by aversive visceral signals. Hyperactivation of the right-side aICCamKII neurons attenuates food consumption, while inhibition of these neurons increases feeding and reverses aversive stimuli-induced anorexia and weight loss. Similar manipulation at the left-side aIC does not cause significant behavioral changes. Furthermore, virus tracing reveals that aICCamKII neurons project directly to the vGluT2+ neurons in the lateral hypothalamus (LH), and the right-side aICCamKII-to-LH pathway mediates feeding suppression. Our studies uncover a circuit from the cortex to the hypothalamus that senses aversive visceral signals and controls feeding behavior.
Subject terms: Neuroscience, Feeding behaviour, Neural circuits
Food intake can be attenuated by visceral aversive stimuli in pathological conditions. Here the authors identify a unilateral neural circuit from the CamKII-positive neurons in the anterior insular cortex to the vGluT2-positive neurons in the lateral hypothalamus that controls feeding responses to visceral aversive stimuli.
Introduction
Whether to eat and how much to eat in any one meal are critical decisions for the health of all metazoan organisms. Under normal conditions, food intake and energy expenditure are balanced by well-defined hypothalamic neural circuits that maintain stable body weight1–5. However, food intake can be attenuated in pathological conditions, such as food poisoning, inflammation, or chemotherapy treatment that may elicit visceral malaise, indicating that some emergency-response circuits hijack the energy homeostasis system. However, the neural circuits involved in mediating feeding responses under these non-homeostatic conditions are comparatively elusive.
Insular cortex, especially the anterior part of the insular cortex (aIC), is a critical region for processing salient stimuli and orchestrating appropriate behavioral responses. The aIC receives heavy sensory inputs from the thalamus and serves as the primary visceral and gustatory cortex6–14. Neuroimaging studies in humans have revealed that the aIC responds not only to food cues, but also to multiple noxious stimuli that trigger pain, anxiety, and disgust15. Studies in patients with eating disorders, such as bulimia nervosa16,17 and anorexia nervosa18–20, have shown altered insula activation. Animal studies confirmed the activation of the insular cortex in response to anorexigenic signals in vivo21–24. Moreover, direct insular stimulation in humans or rodents induces visceral sensations such as nausea25,26, whereas damage to the insular cortex blunts lithium-induced malaise21 and blocks the behavioral expression of a conditioned taste aversion27–29. Although the role of the insular cortex in perceiving multiple aversive signals has been extensively studied, the behavioral responses under these pathological conditions, especially feeding behaviors, upon insular activation are not well studied. By now, a direct role of the insular cortex in controlling food intake has not yet been demonstrated in patients with insular damage30 or in rodents with insular lesion or inactivation21,31.
In this study, we demonstrate that the CamKII-positive neurons in the caudal segment of the right-side, but not the left-side, aIC respond to aversive visceral stimuli and suppress feeding via projections to the vGluT2-positive neurons in the lateral hypothalamus (LH). Thus, we identify a role of the aIC that conveys aversive visceral information to the LH to regulate food consumption.
Results
The right-side aIC is activated by aversive visceral stimuli
To investigate the involvement of the aIC in processing aversive signals, we monitored Fos expression after intraperitoneal (i.p.) injection of anorexigenic signals, such as lithium chloride (LiCl), which induces nausea and visceral malaise, lipopolysaccharide (LPS), which triggers a wide range of inflammatory and sickness responses, and cis-diaminodichloroplatinum (Cisplatin)32, which is a chemotherapy medication used to treat cancers but induces anorexia and cancer anorexia-cachexia. Immunostaining for Fos revealed that injection (i.p.) of LiCl, LPS, or Cisplatin-induced substantial Fos expression bilaterally in several brain regions, including parabrachial nucleus (PBN), nucleus of solitary tract (NTS), basolateral amygdaloid nucleus (BLA), medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), LH, the parvicellular part of the ventroposteromedial nucleus of the thalamus (VPMpc), and central amygdala (CeA) (Two-way ANOVA, PBN, F2,15 = 0.3023, P = 0.0681; NTS, F2,15 = 0.1253, P = 0.8831; BLA, F2,15 = 0.562, P = 0.5816; mPFC, F2,15 = 0.1878, P = 0.8307, LH, F2,15 = 0.7986, P = 0.4682; VPMpc, F2,15 = 0.1315, P = 0.8778; CeA, F2,15 = 0.1166, P = 0.9493, Supplementary Fig. 1a, b). Of interest, LiCl, LPS, and Cisplatin produced a robust induction of Fos in the right, but not in the left aIC (Two-tailed unpaired t test, Saline, t10 = 0.6184, P = 0.5501; LiCl, t22 = 4.027, P = 0.0006; LPS, t18 = 6.946, P < 0.0001; Cisplatin, t20 = 2.886, P = 0.0091, Fig. 1a, b). Further analyses revealed that LiCl-induced or LPS-induced Fos-positive (Fos+) neurons were mainly localized within the caudal segment between 1.26 mm before Bregma (Bregma +1.26) and 0.02 mm after Bregma (Bregma −0.02) of the right aIC (Two-way ANOVA, Saline, F4,25 = 1.961, P = 0.1315; LiCl, F4,25 = 33.42, P < 0.0001; LPS, F4,25 = 6.957, P = 0.00061, Supplementary Fig. 2a–d). In contrast, injection (i.p.) of cholecysokinin (CCK), a potential satiation factor, or ghrelin, a hunger hormone, resulted in a much less Fos expression (Two-tailed unpaired t test, Ghrelin, t10 = 1.607, P = 0.1391; CCK, t10 = 0.4837, P = 0.639, Fig. 1a, b) and these Fos+ neurons were diffusely distributed within both sides of the aIC (Two-way ANOVA, Ghrelin, F4,25 = 1.641, P = 0.1612; CCK, F4,25 = 0.6909, P = 0.6052, Supplementary Fig. 2e, f). Additionally, LiCl-induced and LPS-induced Fos+ neurons were exclusively located in the agranular part of the caudal segment of the right aIC, whereas CCK-induced Fos+ neurons were mainly located in the granular part and Ghrelin-induced Fos+ neurons were diffusely distributed in this segment (one-way ANOVA, Saline, F3,20 = 0.6521, P = 0.5909; LiCl, F3,20 = 3.416, P = 0.0372; LPS, F3,20 = 0.3912, P = 0.0212; Ghrelin, F3,20 = 0.6696, P = 0.5806; CCK, F3,20 = 2.617, P = 0.0477, Supplementary Fig. 2g, h).
Moreover, no obvious Fos expression was observed in the left or right aIC in ad libitum-feeding mice, in 24-h fasted mice or in refeeding mice (two-tailed unpaired t test, home caged, t10 = 0.4953, P = 0.6311; fasted, t10 = 0.9209, P = 0.3788, re-fed, t10 = 0.5575, P = 0.5895, Fig. 1c, d). We also investigated the mice with electric shock, which exerts no direct effect on the gastrointestinal tract, and observed significant Fos staining in the BLA bilaterally, but not in the aIC (two-way ANOVA, aIC, F1,20 = 0.8639, P = 0.3637; BLA, F1,20 = 0.0216, P = 0.8846, Supplementary Fig. 3a, b). Additionally, the right aIC was significantly activated no matter whether LiCl was injected (i.p.) in the left side or right side of the mouse body, suggesting that the unilateral activation of the aIC is not determined by injection site (two-way ANOVA, left side (i.p.), F1,10 = 12.93, P = 0.0049; right side (i.p.), F1,10 = 47.22, P < 0.0001, Supplementary Fig. 3c). These results indicate that aIC, especially the caudal segment of the right aIC, responds to multiple aversive visceral stimuli.
Notably, double immunohistochemical staining revealed that almost 82.6% of Fos+ neurons activated by LiCl in the right aIC expressed CamKII, while only 4.1% of Fos+ neurons expressed GAD67 (Fig. 1e, f). To confirm this result, we injected a recombinant adreno-associated virus (AAV) that expressed Camk2a promoter-controlled mCherry (AAV9-CamKIIα-mCherry) into the caudal segment of the right aIC of the wild-type mice. Three weeks later, we observed that 87.8% of Fos+ neurons activated by LiCl were colocalized with mCherry-positive cells in this site (Supplementary Fig. 3d). Together, these results indicate that most of the activated neurons are excitatory neurons expressing CamKII.
To determine the dynamics of the CamKII-positive neurons in the caudal segment of the right or left aIC (the right or left aICCamKII neurons) in vivo during exposure to an aversive stimulus, we performed fiber photometry recording (Fig. 1g, h, Supplementary Fig. 4a). AAV containing GCaMP6f (AAV9-mCamKIIα-GCaMP6f) was injected unilaterally into the caudal segment of the right aIC or the left aIC of wild-type mice (Fig. 1g, h). Strikingly, i.p. injection of LiCl (One-way ANOVA, F3,12 = 8.409, P = 0.0028, Fig. 1i, Supplementary Fig. 4b, c) or Cisplatin (One-way ANOVA, F3,12 = 71.38, P < 0.0001, Fig. 1j, Supplementary Fig. 4d, e) evoked sustained increase in the activity of the right aICCamKII neuron, but not in the left aICCamKII neurons, confirming that aversive visceral stimuli enhance the excitatory neuronal activity in the right aIC.
Activating the right-side aICCamKII neurons impairs feeding
To explore whether the CamKII+ neurons in the caudal segment of the right aIC mediate feeding behavior, we injected Cre-dependent AAV containing a double floxed inverted orientation (DIO) excitatory optogenetic receptor construct (AAV8-DIO-hChR2(H134R)-eGFP) into the caudal segment of the right aIC of Camk2a-Cre transgenic mice (Fig. 2a, Supplementary Fig. 5a). Whole-cell patch-clamp recordings in the acute brain slices showed that action potentials of ChR2-expressing aICCamKII neurons were triggered by 473-nm light pulses (Fig. 2b). We next performed a time-resolved analysis of feeding in mice that were deprived of food for 24 h (Fig. 2c). Photoactivation of the right aICCamKII neurons at 20 Hz significantly decreased the amount of food intake (two-tailed unpaired t test, t17 = 2.385, P = 0.029) and the percentage of time spent feeding (feeding time was determined as the time the mice successfully accomplished touching and consuming a food pellet, Two-tailed unpaired t test, t17 = 4.259, P = 0.005) during a 20-min test (Fig. 2d, e). Moreover, photoactivation of these neurons decreased the number of approaches (the number of times at which the mice’s nose touched the food pellet, two-tailed unpaired t test, t17 = 4.775, P = 0.0067) and increased the latency of the first approach to food (two-tailed unpaired t test, t17 = 3.408, P = 0.0034) (Fig. 2d, e). Activation of these neurons also interrupted ongoing feeding within a few seconds following the onset of photoactivation in 24-h fasted mice when mice were in their home cages (Fig. 2f–h, Two-tailed unpaired t test, for latency to stop, t20 = 10.7, P < 0.0001; for the percentage of stimuli that inhibits feeding, t17 = 19.64, P < 0.0001). Thus, activation of the CamKII+ neurons in the caudal segment of the right aIC inhibited both the appetitive and consummatory phases of feeding behaviors.
We further tested whether activation of these neurons affects the valence of taste, cardiac function, or other classical consummatory behaviors and observed that optogenetic activation of these neurons has no effect on the mice bitter sensitivity (two-way ANOVA, F3,56 = 0.6245, P = 0.6022, Supplementary Fig. 5b), heart rate (Two-tailed unpaired t test, t6 = 0.9329, P = 0.3869, Supplementary Fig. 5c, d), drinking (two-tailed unpaired t test, Water intake, t12 = 0.1997, P = 0.8457; Number of licks, t12 = 0.2915, P = 0.7756; Time drinking, t12 = 0.1993, P = 0.8454, Supplementary Fig. 5e, f) or mating behaviors (two-tailed unpaired t test, t68 = 0.9092, P = 0.3702, Supplementary Fig. 5g–i).
In the following, we tested the approach or avoidance behavior using real-time place preference assay (RTPP) and observed that mice expressing hChR2 in right aICCamKII neurons spent significantly less time on the photostimulated side and made more escape attempts (two-tailed unpaired t test, Time in stimulation side, t12 = 2.915, P = 0.0095; Velocity, 1st light off step, t17 = 0.754, P = 0.4772, 1st light on step, t17 = 10.23, P < 0.0001, 2nd light off step, t17 = 0.262, P = 0.802, Fig. 2i–k). In contrast, optogenetic activation of these neurons induced no anxiety-like behaviors in two different assays of anxiety, including the elevated plus maze test (two-tailed unpaired t test, Open arm entries, t14 = 0.7477, P = 0.467; Time in open arm, t14 = 0.3227, P = 0.7517, Supplementary Fig. 5j, k) and open field test (OFT) (two-tailed unpaired t test, time in center, t15 = 0.3872, P = 0.7041; velocity, t15 = 0.777, P = 0.4492, Supplementary Fig. 5l, m).
To confirm the specificity of the caudal segment of the right aIC in mediating feeding behavior, we optogenetically activated the CamKII+ neurons in the rostral segment of the right aIC (two-tailed unpaired t test, t12 = 0.569, P = 0.5799, Supplementary Fig. 6a, b) or those in the right posterior IC (pIC) (two-tailed unpaired t test, t13 = 0.5801, P = 0.5718, Supplementary Fig. 6c–e) and observed that photoactivation of the CamKII+ neurons in these two neighboring sites has no effect on feeding behavior.
Silencing the right-side aICCamKII neurons promotes feeding
We next asked whether inhibition of the CamKII+ neurons in the caudal segment of the right aIC increases food consumption under conditions in which motivation to eat is low. We silenced the right aICCamKII neurons expressing Cre-dependent AAV9-DIO-eArch3.0-eGFP virus with 532-nm laser light (Fig. 3a upper, Supplementary Fig. 7a). Brain slice recordings confirmed that aICCamKII neurons expressing eArch3.0 could be strongly inhibited by 532-nm laser light (Fig. 3a lower). Indeed, optogenetic silencing of the CamKII+ neurons in the caudal segment of the right aIC increased the total amount of food intake (Two-tailed unpaired t test, t12 = 3.758, P = 0.049), the number of approach (Two-tailed unpaired t test, t12 = 7.167, P < 0.0001) and the percentage of time spent feeding (Two-tailed unpaired t test, t12 = 7.995, P < 0.0001) in fed mice expressing eArch3.0, but not in mice expressing eGFP (Fig. 3b, c). Moreover, silencing these neurons evoked feeding behavior in fed mice when the mice were in their home cages (Two-tailed unpaired t test, t12 = 20.14, P < 0.0001, Fig. 3d, e). These data suggest that inhibition of neural activity within the caudal segment of right aIC is sufficient to induce feeding behavior.
In the RTPP assay, the mice expressing eArch3.0 in these neurons exhibited a significant preference for the photoinhibition-paired chamber (Fig. 3f–g, Two-tailed unpaired t test, Time in stimulation side, t12 = 3.915, P = 0.0083; Velocity, 1st light off step, t12 = 0.3843, P = 0.7097, 1st light on step, t12 = 0.676, P = 0.515, 2nd light off step, t12 = 0.6583, P = 0.5268). In contrast, drinking (two-tailed unpaired t test, t test, water intake, t12 = 1.96, P = 0.0737; number of licks, t12 = 1.503, P = 0.1587; time drinking, t12 = 0.1523, P = 0.1523, Supplementary Fig. 7b), mating (two-tailed unpaired t test, t test, t34 = 1.335, P = 0.1908, Supplementary Fig. 7c), and anxiety-like behaviors (Supplementary Fig. 7d–g, Two-tailed unpaired t test, for Supplementary Fig. 7e, open arm entries, t12 = 0.6325, P = 0.539; time in open arm, t12 = 0.874, P = 0.3993, for Supplementary Fig. 7g, time in center, t12 = 1.037, P = 0.3202; velocity, t12 = 1.078, P = 0.3022) were unaffected. Taken together, these results indicate that the inhibition of the CamKII+ neurons in the caudal segment of the right aIC promotes food consumption in the absence of homeostatic deficit and at the same time induces approach behavior.
Activating the left aICCamKII neurons induces no feeding
We also explored whether the caudal segment of the left aICCamKII neuron activity is involved in mediating feeding or avoidance behavior. AAV virus carrying hChR2 was injected unilaterally in the caudal segment of the left aIC of Camk2a-Cre mice (Fig. 4a, Supplementary Fig. 8a). Optogenetic activation of these neurons in vivo at 20 Hz had no effect on feeding behavior in 24-h-fasted mice (two-tailed unpaired t test, food intake, t12 = 0.3688, P = 0.7187; number of approaches, t12 = 0.9881, P = 0.3426; time feeding, t12 = 0.2507, P = 0.8063, Fig. 4b, c). Moreover, activation of these neurons produced no conditioned place aversion (Two-tailed unpaired t test, time in stimulation side, t12 = 0.4276, P = 0.6765; velocity, 1st light off step, t12 = 0.493, P = 0.631, 1st light on step, t12 = 0.8914, P = 0.3902, 2nd light off step, t12 = 0.5257, P = 0.6087, Fig. 4d, e), induced no drinking (Two-tailed unpaired t test, water intake, t12 = 0.1104, P = 0.9139; number of licks, t12 = 0.5958, P = 0.5624; time drinking, t12 = 0.3115, P = 0.7607, Supplementary Fig. 8b) or mating behavior (two-tailed unpaired t test, t27 = 0.8559, P = 0.3996, Supplementary Fig. 8c), and exerted no effect on anxiety-like behavior (Supplementary Fig. 8d–g; two-tailed unpaired t test, for Supplementary Fig. 8d, t27 = 0.8559, P = 0.3996; for Supplementary Fig. 8e, open arm entries, t12 = 0.3174, P = 0.7564; time in open arm, t12 = 0.1824, P = 0.8583; for Supplementary Fig. 8g, time in center, t12 = 0.0082, P = 0.9936; velocity, t12 = 0.0059, P = 0.9953). These data suggest that the caudal segment of the left aIC is unnecessary for feeding or avoidance behavior.
Inhibiting the right aICCamKII neurons reverses anorexia
To test the long-term effect of the right aICCamKII neuron activity, we injected an excitatory virus AAV9-DIO-hM3Dq-mCherry or inhibitory virus AAV9-DIO-hM4Di-mCherry in the caudal segment of the right aIC of Camk2a-Cre mice (Supplementary Fig. 9a, b). Electrophysiological analysis in acute slices confirmed that Clozapine N-oxide (CNO, 5 μM) activated spiking in neurons expressing hM3Dq (Fig. 5a, b). CNO treatment rapidly and markedly inhibited food intake in 24 h-fasted mice expressing hM3Dq in the caudal segment of the right aICCamKII neurons during a 1-h test (Two-way ANOVA, food intake, F1,18 = 43.11, P < 0.0001; feeding time, F1,18 = 32.82, P < 0.0001, Fig. 5c). Moreover, long-term CNO treatment significantly reduced body weight in mice expressing hM3Dq in these neurons but not in mice expressing mCherry. Body weight returned to normal after cessation of CNO treatment (two-way RM ANOVA, interaction: F19,114 = 10.6, P < 0.0001, Fig. 5d).
Electrophysiological analysis in acute slices also confirmed that CNO (5 μM) inhibited spiking in neurons expressing hM4Di (Fig. 5e, f). Of interest, administration of CNO (i.p.) to inhibit the CamKII+ neurons in the caudal segment of the right aIC had no effect on CCK-induced reduction of food intake in fasted mice (two-way ANOVA, saline, F3,26 = 1.113, P = 0.3619; CCK, F3,26 = 2.238, P = 0.1076, Fig. 5g). However, chemogenetic inhibition of these neurons prevented LiCl-induced and LPS-induced reduction of food intake (two-way ANOVA, LiCl, F3,26 = 3.287, P = 0.0365; LPS, F3,26 = 9.614, P = 0.0002, Fig. 5g). Administration of saline (i.p.) has no effect on CCK-induced, LiCl-induced, or LPS-induced reduction of food intake (two-way ANOVA, Saline, F3,26 = 0.4549, P = 0.7161; CCK, F3,26 = 0.4727, P = 0.7039; LiCl, F3,26 = 0.7194, P = 0.5394; LPS, F3,26 = 0.6813, P = 0.5715, Supplementary Fig. 9c). Moreover, long-term CNO treatment significantly increased the body weight of mice expressing hM4Di in the CamKII+ neurons in the caudal segment of the right aIC, but not mice-expressing mCherry, and this effect was reversed post CNO injection (two-way RM ANOVA, interaction: F18,126 = 9.078, P < 0.0001, Bonferroni post hoc analysis mCherry with hM4Di on days 6–8 as indicated, Fig. 5h). Long-term Cisplatin treatment (every 2 days for 14 days) significantly reduced the body weight of the injected mice. Chronic chemogenetic inhibition of the right aICCamKII neurons significantly prevented further weight loss, while inhibition of the left aICCamKII neurons failed to rescue body weight loss (two-way RM ANOVA, interaction: F16,64 = 57.1, P < 0.0001, Bonferroni post hoc analysis mCherry with hM4Di on days 9–14 as indicated, Fig. 5i), implying that the caudal segment of the right aIC is critical in mediating feeding in response to aversive stimuli.
The aICCamKII neurons directly innervate LHvGluT2 neurons
To elucidate the neurocircuitry involved in the aIC-mediated feeding behavior, we anatomically mapped the long-range projections of CamKII+ neurons in the aIC. First, we selectively expressed a Cre-dependent fluorophore AAV8-DIO-eGFP into the right aIC of Camk2a-Cre mice. Imaging showed that the aICCamKII neurons projected heavily to the hypothalamus (Supplementary Fig. 10a, b). It is of interest to note that, in the hypothalamus, fiber terminals originating from the aIC were predominantly localized within the LH (Fig. 6a), a critical neuroanatomical substrate for feeding behavior33. Next, we designed a series of experiments to test whether the aICCamKII neurons have direct projections to the LH. Immunohistochemical staining showed that stimulation of the projection terminals with 473-nm laser induced significant Fos expression in the entire length of the anterior–posterior (A–P) axis of the LH, but a greater number between 1.06 and 1.58 mm posterior of Bregma (Two-way ANOVA, Fos expression at −0.7 mm, F2,28 = 1.364, P = 0.5503; Fos expression at −1.06 mm, F2,28 = 2.871, P = 0.073; Fos expression at −1.34 mm, F2,28 = 43.47, P < 0.0001; Fos expression at −1.58 mm, F2,28 = 5.581, P = 0.0031; Fos expression at −1.94 mm, F2,28 = 3.554, P = 0.041, Fig. 6b), indicating that terminals originating from the aIC form functional synaptic connections with the neurons in the LH. To characterize the aIC to LH connection, whole-cell electrophysiological recording was used to examine light-evoked inhibitory postsynaptic currents (IPSCs) and excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs) from LH neurons in brain slices. This was accomplished by alternation between the holding potential of 0 mV (IPSCs) and −70 mV (EPSCs) (Fig. 6c). Light activation resulted in eEPSCs and no eIPSCs in all cells tested. Moreover, evoked EPSCs were blocked by the glutamatergic receptor antagonist CNQX (Fig. 6d), indicating that the terminals originating from the aIC formed functional glutamatergic synaptic connections with the neurons in the LH. To further test the cell-type specificity of this projection, we performed Cre-dependent, rabies virus-based retrograde tracing from vGluT2 neurons of the LH. We injected synaptic retrograde ΔG-rabies viruses encoding mCh (RV) into the LH of Slc17a6 (encodes vesicular glutamate transporter, vGluT2)-ires-Cre mice. Positive signals were detected in the aIC and colocalized well with CamKII (Fig. 6e, f), which confirms that the aICCamKII neurons have direct projection to the vGluT2+ neurons in the LH.
To confirm that the aIC receives input from thalamus, we further injected retrograde cholera toxin subunit B (CTB, conjugated with Alexa-555) into the right aIC and observed significant signals in the area VPMpc (Supplementary Fig. 10c). Results from in situ hybridization of Slc17a6 mRNA showed that 94.5% of CTB555-labeled neurons overlapped with Slc17a6 mRNA, indicating that VPMpc-to-aIC projecting neurons are primarily glutamatergic (Supplementary Fig. 10c).
The right aIC-to-LH projections mediate feeding suppression
To assess the functional contribution of the right aICCamKII-to-LH projection to feeding behavior, we introduced hChR2-eGFP into the caudal segment of the right aIC of Camk2a-Cre mice (Fig. 7a and Supplementary Fig. 11a). Photoactivation of the right aICCamKII neuronal terminals in LH significantly reduced the amount of food intake, the number of approaches, and the percentage of time spent feeding in the fasted mice (two-tailed unpaired t test, food intake, t20 = 2.445, P = 0.0239; number of approaches, t20 = 5.451, P < 0.0001; time feeding, t20 = 5.814, P < 0.0001, Fig. 7b, c) and also inhibited ongoing feeding when mice were in their home cages (two-tailed unpaired t test, latency to stop, t20 = 15.33, P < 0.0001; feeding-inhibited stimulation, t20 = 14.31, P < 0.0001, Fig. 7d, e). In addition, activation of the right aICCamKII-to-LH pathway produced place avoidance (two-tailed unpaired t test, time in stimulation side, t20 = 3.493, P = 0.0023; velocity, 1st light off step, t20 = 0.5406, P = 0.5948, 1st light on step, t20 = 5.778, P < 0.0001, 2nd light off step, t20 = 0.7825, P = 0.3862, Fig. 7f, g), while it had no effects on drinking (two-tailed unpaired t test, water intake, t20 = 0.523, P = 0.6067; number of licks, t20 = 0.5141, P = 0.5942; time drinking, t20 = 0.1318, P = 0.8965, Supplementary Fig. 11b) or mating behavior (two-tailed unpaired t test, t26 = 0.1827, P = 0.8565, Supplementary Fig. 11c).
To confirm this observation, we used another labeling strategy. AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hChR2-eGFP virus was injected into the right aIC after a retrogradely transported virus canine adenovirus-2 encoding Cre-recombinase (Cav2-Cre) was injected into the LH (Supplementary Fig. 12a, b). This resulted in the specific hChR2 expression in the right aIC neurons that projected to LH. Photoactivation of these neurons inhibited food consumption in 24 h-fasted mice (Two-tailed unpaired t test, food intake, t8 = 2.803, P = 0.0230; number of approach, t8 = 3.153, P = 0.0275; time feeding, t8 = 4.786, P = 0.0014, Supplementary Fig. 12c) and induced place avoidance (Two-tailed unpaired t test, time in stimulation side, t8 = 4.329, P = 0.025; velocity, 1st light off step, t8 = 0.8011, P = 0.4462 1st light on step, t8 = 8.526, P < 0.0001, 2nd light off step, t8 = 1.824, P = 0.1056, Supplementary Fig. 12d, e), while having no effects on drinking behavior (Two-tailed unpaired t test, water intake, t8 = 0.7801, P = 0.4578; number of licks, t8 = 1.206, P = 0.2623; time drinking, t8 = 0.3376, P = 0.7443, Supplementary Fig. 12f), consistent with the aforementioned finding that the right aICCamKII-to-LH pathway is sufficient to inhibit feeding and induce aversive effects.
In contrast, specifically silencing the right aICCamKII-to-LH projection terminals via introducing eArch3.0 in the right aICCamKII neurons and stimulating projection terminals in the LH with 532-nm laser (Fig. 7h, Supplementary Fig. 13a) promoted food consumption in fed mice (Fig. 7i–l, Two-tailed unpaired t test, for Supplementary Fig. 7j, food intake, t10 = 4.341, P = 0.0075; number of approach, t10 = 5.593, P < 0.0001; time feeding, t10 = 4.082, P = 0.0022; for Supplementary Fig. 7l, t10 = 19.9, P < 0.0001), as well as place preference (Two-tailed unpaired t test, time in stimulation side, t20 = 3.493, P = 0.0023; velocity, 1st light off step, t20 = 0.5406, P = 0.5948, 1st light on step, t20 = 5.778, P < 0.0001, 2nd light off step, t20 = 0.7825, P = 0.3862, Fig. 7m, n), while having no effects on drinking behavior (Two-tailed unpaired t test, water intake, t10 = 1.253, P = 0.2386; number of licks, t10 = 1.949, P = 0.0798; time drinking, t10 = 0.2572, P = 0.8023, Supplementary Fig. 13b). Taken together, these results indicate that the right aICCamKII-to-LH pathway mediates feeding suppression and place avoidance.
Discussion
In this study, we investigated how aversive stimuli alter insular activity and how direct manipulation of neuronal activity in the aIC affects aversive stimuli-induced feeding responses. Combining virus tracing, optogenetics, pharmacogenetics, photometry, and behavioral studies, we demonstrated that the caudal segment of the right aIC responds to diverse aversive stimuli in vivo and mediates anorexia via innervation of the LH. Therefore, we have identified a neural circuit from the cortex to the hypothalamus that interrupts the homeostatic system and mediates feeding responses under emergency conditions.
The insular cortex is known to process polymodal information about bodily states, including visceral34, gustatory35, somatosensory36, and auditory modalities37. In this study, we identified a segment in the right aIC that gates feeding in response to aversive anorexigenic stimuli. Interestingly, when we compared it with the bitter center and sweet center identified by Zuker’s group38, we found that this segment is just located between these two taste centers. These data demonstrate topographic segregation in the functional architecture of the insula39–41. It is possible that inputs carrying information from outside and inside the body project to topographically organized insular sensory regions. More tracing work is required to carefully analyze the connection between the insula and the thalamus or the PBN, such as the neuronal types, subregion, and left–right difference that serves sensory function.
The LH is a critical neuroanatomical region that is tied to consummatory and motivational behaviors. vGAT and vGluT2-expressing LH neurons produce a bidirectional output signal, which is directly or indirectly conveyed to the ventral tegmental area (VTA) dopamine neurons to homeostatically invigorate behavioral output. Inhibitory GABAergic subcortical fibers that innervate LH may originate from the lateral septum42 and much of the basal forebrain and the extended amygdala, including the nucleus accumbens shell43,44, the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST)/preoptic area45, the ventral pallidum46 and nucleus basalis/substantia innominate47. However, the source of excitatory inputs to the LH is still unknown. In this study, we showed that the glutamatergic input from the aIC to the LH inhibits food intake, consistent with the previous finding that vGluT2-expressing LH neurons produce an output signal to inhibit feeding and induce place avoidance. Therefore, we propose a model that environmental representations, such as aversive visceral stimuli, are likely encoded in the right insular cortex, which in turn convey representational information to the LH neuronal circuits to regulate feeding and rewarding. Previous work has shown that the LH may exert its effect on feeding directly via projecting to the PBN48,49 to suppress feeding behavior or indirectly to the lateral habenula (LHb)50 or VTA51 by affecting the rewarding aspect of food consumption. More work is required to identify downstream brain regions that elicit the feeding suppression function of the aIC-to-LH circuit.
Craig6 has proposed laterality differences in interoceptive perception related to emotional processing for the insular cortex in humans. In this study, we found that the right but not the left aIC in mice is critical in sensing aversive visceral stimuli and controlling feeding behavior. Moreover, inactivation of the right but not the left aIC reverses chemotherapy-induced weight loss. Consistently, one previous study in rats has also reported lateralized CREB activation in conditioned taste aversion with the right IC being more obvious than the left IC52. These findings suggest that hemispheric functional lateralization of aIC is not unique to humans but may be present in rodents as well. Furthermore, our findings are reminiscent of the right hemispheric dominance in negative emotional processing and expression53,54 and of the right aIC hemispheric dominance in interoceptive attention in humans15,55–57. Recently, more and more evidence in rodents indicates left–right asymmetry for cognitive and emotional faculties. Several brain regions have been reported to be lateralized for corresponding behavioral functions, such as anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) for observational fear58,59, mPFC for anxiety60, amygdala for pain and fear61–64, hypothalamus for energy metabolism65,66, and hippocampus for learning and spatial navigation67–69, respectively. However, further evidence is required to understand brain left–right asymmetries at microscopic levels involving molecules, synapses, neurons, and neuronal circuits. Our findings here may provide an entry point for addressing basic neuronal underpinnings of insular functions and malfunctions in an accessible animal model.
To conclude, in this study, we have identified a pathway from the right insular cortex to the right LH that controls the intake of food. Additionally, to avoid potential confounding effects caused by lateralization, care should be taken in future behavioral studies when examining the role of a circuit.
Methods
Animals
Camk2a-Cre mice (B6.Cg-Tg(Camk2a-cre)T29-1Stl/J, strain 005359, RRID:IMSR_JAX:005359) and Slc17a6-ires-Cre mice (B6J.129S6(FVB)-Slc17a6tm2(cre)Lowl/MwarJ, strain 028863, RRID:IMSR_JAX:028863) were obtained from the Jackson Laboratory. The male mice were used for behavioral test, and both the male and female mice were used for tracing image. Camk2a-Cre, Slc17a6-ires-Cre and C57BL/6J mice at 2–4 months of age were housed on a 12-h light/dark cycle under standard conditions in the animal facility with food and water ad libitum unless otherwise noted.
All animal care and experimental procedures complied with all relevant ethical regulations, were strictly conducted in accordance with the Guidelines for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of Zhejiang University and were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at Zhejiang University.
Regents
CNO, LiCl, LPS, Cisplatin, CNQX, and DAPI dye were obtained from Sigma Aldrich. Rat Ghrelin and mouse cholecysokinin (CCK) were obtained from Tocris Bioscience. CTB, Alexa FluorTM 555 were obtained from Thermo Fisher.
Viral constructs
The following AAV viruses were provide by Shanghai Taitool Bioscience Co., Ltd: AAV9-hSyn-DIO-hM3Dq-mCherry, AAV9-hSyn-DIO-hM4Di-mCherry, AAV8-hSyn-DIO-mCherry, AAV8-hSyn-DIO-eGFP, AAV9-CamKIIα-mCherry, AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hChR2(H134R)-eGFP, AAV8-CamKIIα-hChR2(H134R)-eGFP, AAV9-hSyn-DIO-eArch3.0-eGFP, AAV9-mCamKIIα-GCaMP6f, AAV9-hSyn-DIO-mCherry, and Cav2-CMV-Cre. AAV9-CAG-DIO-TVA-EGFP, AAV9-CAG-DIO-RG and EnvA-pseudotyped, glycoprotein (RG)-deleted and DsRed-expressing rabies virus (RV-EvnA-DsRed, RV) were provided by Wuhan BrainVTA Co., Ltd.
Stereotaxic surgery
2–4 months old mice were deeply anesthetized with 5% isoflurane (vol per vol) in oxygen and then kept with 1.5% isoflurane. Surgery was performed with a stereotaxic frame (Stoelting). For caudal segment of aIC injection, 150 nl of virus was injected unilaterally on site (+0.5 mm antero-posterior (AP); ±3.85 mm medio-lateral (ML); −2.82 mm dorso-ventral (DV) relative to Bregma). For right posterior insular cortex (pIC), virus was injected unilaterally on site (−1.10 mm AP; −3.90 mm ML; −3.25 mm DV relative to Bregma). For rostral segment of the right aIC, virus was injected unilaterally on site (+1.6 mm AP; −3.1 mm ML; −1.8 mm DV relative to Bregma). Virus was injected into each location at 0.03 μl/min. The syringe was not removed until 15–20 min after the end of infusion to allow diffusion of the virus. After injection, mice were single housed and allowed 2–4 weeks for viral expression and recovery from surgery.
For soma photostimulation or photoinhibition experiments, recombinant AAVs expressing hChR2 (H134R), eArch3.0, or eGFP were unilaterally injected into the aIC of Camk2a-Cre mice or wild-type mice. Three weeks after surgery, a fibreoptic cannula with 0.22 NA or 0.37 NA was implanted above the injected aIC (−2.67 mm DV) and mice were allowed another 1 week for recovery from surgery before behavioral test.
For axon terminal photostimulation experiments, recombinant AAVs expressing hChR2(H134R), eArch3.0, or eGFP were unilaterally injected into the aIC of Camk2a-Cre mice. Three weeks after surgery, a fibreoptic cannula with 0.22 NA was implanted above the LH (−1.28 mm AP, ±1.23 mm ML, −5.28 mm DV). Mice were allowed another 1 week for recovery from surgery before behavioral test.
For chemogenetic stimulation or inhibition, recombinant AAVs expressing hM3Dq, hM4Di, or mCherry were injected into the caudal segment of the aIC of Camk2a-Cre mice unilaterally. Four weeks later, the mice were received an i.p. injection of CNO (1 mg/kg) or the equivalent volume of saline and were allowed to recover in their home cage for 30 min before behavioral test. The mice with virus distributed mainly from Bregma +1.34 mm to Bregma +0.02 mm were used to analyze the related behaviors.
For photometry experiments, recombinant AAVs expressing mCamKIIα-GCaMP6f were injected unilaterally into the aIC of wild-type mice. Two weeks later, a fibreoptic cannula with 0.37 NA was implanted above the injected aIC (−2.67 mm DV). Mice were allowed 1 week for recovery from surgery before behavioral test.
For projection-mapping experiments, recombinant AAVs expressing GFP were injected unilaterally into the aIC of Camk2a-Cre mice. Eight weeks later, mice were sacrificed, fixed, and sections were collected for imaging.
For monosynaptic-tracing experiments, Slc17a6-ires-Cre mice were microinjected unilaterally in the LH with 150 nl viral cocktail (1:1) with AAV-CAG-DIO-TVA-EGFP to allow the initial infection of LH starter neurons. AAV-CAG-DIO-RG coding for the rabies virus envelope glycoprotein was also injected into the LH at the same time to allow the trans-synaptic spread of virus. Three weeks later, 200 nl of the modified rabies virus was microinjected into the same location. Another one week later, mice were sacrificed and fixed. Brain sections were collected for imaging.
Immunohistochemistry
Mice were sacrificed and perfused with 4% PFA in PBS. Brains were fixed overnight in PFA, cryoprotected in 30% sucrose (wt per vol) at 4 °C for 24 h, and then coronally sectioned at 40 μm on a cryostat (Leica CM1900). Brain sections were blocked with 10% NGS and 0.3% Triton-X in PBS for 1 h at room temperature, and then incubated with primary antibody (rabbit anti-Fos, SYSY system, 226 003, 1:10,000, RRID:AB_2231974; mouse anti-CamKII, Abcam, ab22609, 1:500, RRID:AB_447192; mouse anti-GAD67, Millipore, MAB5406, 1:1,000, RRID:AB_2278725) at 4 °C for 48 h. Sections were then washed with PBS for three times and incubated with secondary antibody (Alexa Fluor 488 goat anti-rabbit, Life Technologies, a11008, 1:2000, RRID:AB_143165; Alexa Fluor 555 donkey anti-rabbit, Life Technologies, a10040, 1:2000, RRID:AB_2534016; Alexa Fluor 488 goat anti-mouse, Life Technologies, a11029, 1:2000, RRID:AB_138404) for 2 h at room temperature. Sections were mounted and imaged with an Olympus VS120 microscope. For detection of Fos in ChR2 or GFP control mice, animals received 10-min of 20-Hz, ~5 mW photostimulation (488 nm) and were perfused 30 min later. For detection of Fos in mice with different aversive stimuli, mice were perfused 1.5 h after injection (i.p.) with different regents or electric foot shock unless otherwise mentioned.
RNAscope in situ hybridization
Animals were anesthetized and perfused with DEPC-PBS followed by ice-cold 4% PFA in DEPC-PBS. Brains were dissected and post-fixed over night at 4 °C and dehydrated with 30% sucrose in DPEC-PBS. Afterward, brains were sectioned at 20 μm thickness and mounted directly onto glass slides. RNAscope assay were performed according to RNAscope® Multiplex Fluorescent Reagent Kit v2 User Manual (ACD Bio). Probe against Slc17a6 (vGluT2) mRNA was obtained from ACD Bio (Mm-Slc17a6−319171). Images were captured with an Olympus VS120 microscope.
Acute brain-slice preparation and electrophysiology
The mouse was deeply anesthetized with diethyl ether and the brain was rapidly removed and placed in ice-cold, high-sucrose cutting solution containing (in mM): 194 sucrose, 30 NaCl, 26 NaHCO3, 10 glucose, 4.5 KCl, 1.2 NaH2PO4, 7 MgSO4, 0.2 CaCl2, and 2 MgCl2. Slices were cut on a Leica vibratome (VT1200S) in the high-sucrose cutting solution and immediately transferred to an incubation chamber with artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) containing (in mM) 119 NaCl, 26.2 NaHCO3, 11 glucose, 2.5 KCl, 1 NaH2PO4, 1.3 MgCl2, and 2.5 CaCl2. The slices were allowed to recover at 34 °C for 30 min before being allowed to equilibrate at room temperature for another hour. During recordings, the slices were placed in a recording chamber constantly perfused with warmed ACSF (28–30 °C) and gassed continuously with 95% O2 and 5% CO2. For whole-cell voltage-clamp recording, the recording pipettes (3–5 MΩ) were filled with a Cs-based low Cl− internal solution containing (in mM): 135 CsMeSO3, 10 HEPES, 1 EGTA, 3.3 QX-314, 4Mg-ATP, 0.3 Na-GTP, 8 Na2-phosphocreatine, 290 mOsm kg−1, adjusted to pH 7.3 with CsOH. For whole-cell current-clamp recording, the recording pipettes were filled with (in mM) 110 K-gluconate, 40 KCl, 10 HEPES, 2 Mg-ATP, 0.5 Na2-GTP, and 0.2 EGTA 290 mOsm kg−1, adjusted to pH 7.3 with KOH. Data were digitized at 10 kHz, collected with a MultiClamp 700B amplifier, and analyzed by pClamp10 software (Molecular Devices, Sunnyvale, USA). Membrane potential was held at −70 mV to record AMPAR-mediated current and at 0 mV to record GABAA receptor-mediated IPSC. The following drugs were used diluted in ACSF as indicated: 100 μM Picrotoxin (PTX) and 20 μM CNQX. For confirmation of the efficiency of chemogenetic activation (hM3Dq) or silencing (hM4Di) of synaptic transmission, 5 μM of CNO diluted in ACSF was used. For ChR2 validation and circuit mapping, a blue light-emitting diode (NWEDOON) controlled by digital commands from the 1440 A was connected to the epifluorescence port of an Olympus BX51 microscope to deliver photostimulation. To record light-evoked EPSCs, 1–2 ms at λ = 473 nm and 1–10 mW/mm2 blue light was delivered through the objective to illuminate the entire field of view. For optogenetic silencing validation, cells were activated using currents ranging from 0 to 45 pA (ΔI = 5 pA) for 3 s in duration injected under current clamp mode and photosilenced using a 532-nm LED light source (NEWDOON) sending constant light at 10 mW/mm2.
In vivo fiber photometry
AAV-mCamKIIα-GCaMP6f was injected unilaterally into the aIC. Two weeks later, optic-fiber cannulas (fiber: core = 230 μm, NA = 0.37, NEWDOON) were implanted 0.15 mm above to the injection side. Behavioral test started one week later to allow for recovery from surgery. To record fluorescence signals, laser beam from a 488-nm laser (OBIS 488LS; Coherent) was reflected by a dichroic mirror (MD498; Thorlabs), focused by a ×10 objective lens (NA = 0.3; Olympus) and then coupled to an optical commutator (Doric Lenses). To record fluorescence signals, laser beam from a 488-nm laser (OBIS 488LS; Coherent) was reflected by a dichroic mirror (MD498; Thorlabs), focused by a ×10 objective lens (NA = 0.3; Olympus) and then coupled to an optical commutator (Doric Lenses). An optical fiber (230 μm O.D., NA = 0.37, 2-m long) guided the light between the commutator and the implanted optical fiber. The laser power was adjusted at the tip of optical fiber to the low level of 0.01–0.02 mW to minimize bleaching. The GCaMP fluorescence was bandpass filtered (MF525-39, Thorlabs) and collected by a photomultiplier tube (R3896, Hamamatsu). An amplifier (C7319, Hamamatsu) was used to convert the photomultiplier tube current output to voltage signals, which was further filtered through a low-pass filter (40 Hz cut-off; Brownlee 440). The analog voltage signals were digitalized at 500 Hz and recorded by a Power 1401 digitizer.
For chemogenetic assay, mice were acclimated to the behavioral chamber (30 cm × 15 cm × 55 cm) for 30 min, and then injected intraperitoneally with LiCl (150 mg/kg) or Cisplatin (4 mg/kg). Calcium signals were recorded for another 30 min. Photometry setting, including laser power and time constant, were the same for every mouse and every recording session. Photometry data were subjected to minimal processing consisting of only within-trial fluorescence normalization.
In vivo photostimulation
For all optical activation studies, mice received blue light laser stimulation (473 nm, NEWDOON) of ~5 mW with a 10 ms pulse width. For optical inhibition studies, mice received constant green light stimulation (532 nm, NEWDOON) of ~10 mW. The lasers were triggered and pulses were controlled with Intelligent light system software.
Animal behaviors
All mice used in behavioral assays were allowed to recover from surgery of AAV injection and Ferrule fibers implantation for at least 4 weeks.
Feeding behavior for optogenetic assay
Before the test, mice were transferred into an empty testing cage in the behavioral testing room to habituate for at least 1 h. For 24 h-fasted feeding test, mice were food-deprived the day before test, with water provided ad libitum. The mice were then briefly anesthetized with isoflurane and coupled with optic fibers. 20 min after recovery, mice were introduced into the test cage with a regular food pellet (Chow), or anorexigenic agents (CCK, LiCl, LPS) allowed for feeding for 20 min. The weight of the food pellet, including the food debris left in the cage floor after test, was measured to calculate the food intake. For the fed feeding test, mice were not food deprived before testing and allowed to feed for 20 min. For optogenetic experiments, the light was started just before the mice were introduced into the testing cage or home cage. The feeding behavior was videotaped and manually analyzed.
Feeding behavior for pharmacogenetic assay
For chemogenetic stimulation studies, mice were injected with recombination AAV expressing hM3Dq 4 weeks before experiment. The mice were then given ad libitum with Chow 1 week prior to, during, and after the behavioral test. CNO (1 mg/kg) was injected twice a day for 7 days intraperitoneally. The body weight of mice was measured every day between 1 p.m. and 5 p.m.
For chemogenetic inhibition assay, mice were injected unilaterally into the aIC with recombination AAV expressing hM4Di 4 weeks before experiment. Mice were given ad libitum access to food prior to, during, and after the assay. Cisplatin (4 mg/kg) was administered every 2 days intraperitoneally for 2 weeks. CNO (1 mg/kg) was injected intraperitoneally twice a day for 7 days after Cisplatin administered for 1 week. The body weight of mice was measured every day between 1 p.m. and 5 p.m.
Taste sensitivity for optogenetic assay
Taste sensitivity was measured with free-moving mice in behavioral chamber. Prior to behavior test, mice were water deprived for 24 h. Before test mice were briefly anesthetized with isoflurane and coupled with optic fibers. 20 min after recovery, mice were introduced into the test cage with water, 0.01 mM quinine, 0.1 mM quinine, and 1 mM quinine for 20-min drinking test. The total numbers of lick were videotaped and manually analyzed.
Water consumption assay
Mice were water-restricted for 24 h in their home cage, acclimated to the behavioral chamber for 15 min, and then provided with access to water for 20 min. The weight of the water, total number of licks, and percent of drinking time after test was measured to calculate.
Elevated plus-maze test
The elevated plus-maze consisted of a plus-shaped platform with four intersecting arms: two opposing open arms and two closed arms. Animals were placed in the center of the apparatus facing a closed arm and allowed to freely explore the maze for 5 min. The parameters such as time and distance traveled in the open arm were analyzed with the smart3.0 (Panlab) software. The area was cleaned with 75% ethanol between tests.
Open field test
OFT was performed in an open field arena (50 cm long, 50 cm wide, and 60 cm high). Experiments were conducted under low light conditions in order to minimize anxiety effects. Mice were allowed to freely explore for 5 min. The parameter center entries time was analyzed with the smart 3.0 software. The area was cleaned with 75% ethanol between tests.
Real-time place preference assay
Place preference training was performed in a custom-made two-compartment conditioned place preference (CPP) apparatus (30 cm × 25 cm × 20 cm). After connecting with optical fiber, mice infected with AAV-hChR2 or AAV-eGFP were placed in the CPP training apparatus for 20 min to assess their baseline place preference. During the test, we assigned the counterbalanced side of the chamber as the stimulation side, and placed the mice in the non-stimulated side to start the experiment. When the mouse crossed to the stimulated side of the chamber, it triggered 5 Hz laser stimulation (473-nm, 10 ms pulses) until the mouse crossed back to the non-stimulated side. Avoidance score was calculated by subtracting the time spent in stimulation side during baseline (without light) from the time spent in stimulation side during the test (with light).
Measurement of heart rate
Mice were anesthetized with 5% isoflurane (vol per vol) in oxygen and then kept with 1.5% isoflurane. Heart rate data were analyzed after band pass filtering (10–200 Hz, Butterworth three-order filter). Heart beats per minute (BPM) were determined by custom-written Matlab program70,71. At least, three calculations from each animal were averaged over a period of 3 s recording or longer.
Data analysis
For photometry assay, all data were analyzed with custom-written Matlab program. For photometry data, all responses were normalized to baseline using the function: ΔF/F = (F−F0)/F0, in which F0 is the median fluorescence of the baseline. The baseline period for full experiments was 30 min before time zero. To analyze the responses during feeding, the control time window was set 2 s before food touch onset to minimize potential false-positive effects. ΔF/F values are presented as average plots with a gray area indicating the ±SEM. Locomotor activity for mouse was analyzed using a video tracking software (Smart v3.0). We then averaged the value > 10 trials for food consumption. To analyze the responses during aversive stimulation, the control time window was set 60 s before IP injection and averaged the value for 3–4 mouse for aversive stimulation test.
For rabies tracing quantification, the rabies-labeled inputs into brain regions were quantified and normalized to the sum of starter cells in LH. Three brains were used for each experiment.
For axon projection quantification, images were obtained and quantified with three junction sections for each region and were averaged to obtain the final value. Three brains were used for each experiment.
Quantification of Fos staining was performed on every 6th slice in the LH from Bregma −0.7 to −1.92 mm (six sections per mice) and on every third slice in the following area: PBN from Bregma −5.02 to −5.40 mm (three sections per mice), NTS from Bregma −6.96 to −7.48 mm (five sections per mice), BLA from Bregma −0.58 to −1.34 mm (six sections per mice), mPFC from Bregma +1.98 to +1.54 mm (five sections per mice), and VPMpc from Bregma −1.82 to −1.94 mm (two sections per mice). Quantification of Fos staining in the aIC in Fig. 1a–d and Supplementary Fig. 3d, or quantification of colocalization of Fos with CamKII or GAD67 in Fig. 1e, f was performed on every 6th slice in the aIC from Bregma +1.18 to +0.02 mm (four sections per mice). Quantification of Fos staining in the whole IC in Supplementary Fig. 2a–f was performed on every third slice from Bregma +2.46 to −1.06 mm (28 sections per mice). Quantification of Fos staining in the aIC in Supplementary Fig. 2g, h was performed on every third slice in the IC from Bregma +1.18 to +0.5 mm (seven sections per mice). All images were subsequently overlaid with the corresponding atlas section to anatomically define the regions of interest. Positive cells lying on the boundary were excluded. A cell was considered positive only if it displayed an intensity value above the intensity threshold of the background. Six brains were used for each experiment. Quantification was performed using the cell counter tool in ImageJ.
Statistical analysis
Statistical analyses were performed using Matlab or Prism 7 (GraphPad). Throughout the paper, the level of significance is indicated as *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.005. No statistical methods were used to predetermine the sample sizes. Pairwise comparisons were calculated with unpaired two-tailed t tests, and multiple group data comparisons were calculated with one-way or two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc test. Normality was assessed with Shapiro–Wilk tests. If normality tests failed, Mann–Whitney or Wilcoxon rank-sum tests were used. Cell count, anatomical, and behavioral analysis were fully and/or partially conducted by experimenters blind to experimental conditions.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (91732102, 81471125, and 81671049 to S.Q., 31900722 to Y.W.), the Zhejiang Science Fund for Distinguished Young Scholars (LR16C090001 to S.Q.), the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities of China (2019XZZX001-01-14 to S.Q.), the Chinese Ministry of Education Project 111 Program (B13026 to S.Q.), and Certificate of China Postdoctoral Science Foundation Grant (2018M630665 to Y.W.). CIHR operating grant (MOP-124807) and project grant (PJT-148648), Azrieli Neurodevelopmental Research Program and Brain Canada (M.Z.). We thank Dr. Shumin Duan (Zhejiang University) for providing us with Slc17a6-ires-Cre mice and Dr. Jianhong Luo (Zhejiang University) for providing us with Camk2a-Cre mice. We thank Yudong Zhou (Zhejiang University), Hailan Hu (Zhejiang University), and Tatiana Korotkova (Max Planck Institute for Metabolism Research) for valuable discussion. We also thank the technical support by the Core Facilities, Zhejiang University School of Medicine.
Source data
Author contributions
Y.W., C.W.C., and M.C. performed the viral injection, animal behavior, and immunostaining experiments; collected, analyzed, and interpreted data; and participated in writing the paper. X.Y.L. performed the electrophysiological experiments; collected, analyzed, and interpreted data; and participated in writing the paper. K.Q. and H.T.W. performed immunohistochemical staining. Y.W. and L.F.J. performed the calcium-imaging experiments; and collected, analyzed, and interpreted data. L.Y. and M.Z. contributed intellectually and revised the manuscript. S.Q. was responsible for the overall supervision of the study; designed the experiments; analyzed and interpreted data; and revised the paper.
Data availability
The source data underlying Figs. 1b, d, f, 2e, g, h, k, 3c, e, g, 4c, e, 5h, i, 6b, 7c, e, g, j, l, and n and Supplementary Figs. 1b, 2b–f, h, 3b–d, 4c, e, 5b, d, f, i, k, m, 6b, d, 7b, c, e, g, 8b, c, e, g, 9c, 10b, d, 11b, c, 12c, e, f and 13b are provided as a Source Data file. All relevant data are available from the corresponding authors upon reasonable request.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Peer review information Nature Communications thanks Kobi Rosenblum, Richard Palmiter and Zhiping Pang for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Peer reviewer reports are available.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: Yu Wu, Changwan Chen, Ming Chen.
Supplementary information
Supplementary information is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41467-020-14281-5.
References
- 1.Sternson SM, Eiselt AK. Three pillars for the neural control of appetite. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 2017;79:401–423. doi: 10.1146/annurev-physiol-021115-104948. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Sternson SM. Hypothalamic survival circuits: blueprints for purposive behaviors. Neuron. 2013;77:810–824. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.02.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Cowley MA, et al. The distribution and mechanism of action of ghrelin in the CNS demonstrates a novel hypothalamic circuit regulating energy homeostasis. Neuron. 2003;37:649–661. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00063-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Waterson MJ, Horvath TL. Neuronal regulation of energy homeostasis: beyond the hypothalamus and feeding. Cell Metab. 2015;22:962–970. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2015.09.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Dietrich MO, Zimmer MR, Bober J, Horvath TL. Hypothalamic Agrp neurons drive stereotypic behaviors beyond feeding. Cell. 2015;160:1222–1232. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.02.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Craig AD. How do you feel-now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2009;10:59–70. doi: 10.1038/nrn2555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Craig AD. Emotional moments across time: a possible neural basis for time perception in the anterior insula. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. 2009;364:1933–1942. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Shura RD, Hurley RA, Taber KH. Insular cortex: structural and functional neuroanatomy. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2014;26:276–282. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.260401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Gogolla N. The insular cortex. Curr. Biol. 2017;27:R580–R586. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Bliss TV, Collingridge GL, Kaang BK, Zhuo M. Synaptic plasticity in the anterior cingulate cortex in acute and chronic pain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2016;17:485–496. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Berman DE, Dudai Y. Memory extinction, learning anew, and learning the new: dissociations in the molecular machinery of learning in cortex. Science. 2001;291:2417–2419. doi: 10.1126/science.1058165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Shema R, Hazvi S, Sacktor TC, Dudai Y. Boundary conditions for the maintenance of memory by PKMzeta in neocortex. Learn. Mem. 2009;16:122–128. doi: 10.1101/lm.1183309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Inberg S, et al. Fluid consumption and taste novelty determines transcription temporal dynamics in the gustatory cortex. Mol. Brain. 2016;9:13. doi: 10.1186/s13041-016-0188-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Blonde GD, Bales MB, Spector AC. Extensive lesions in rat insular cortex significantly disrupt taste sensitivity to NaCl and KCl and slow salt discrimination learning. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0117515. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0117515. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Critchley HD. Electrodermal responses: what happens in the brain. Neuroscientist. 2002;8:132–142. doi: 10.1177/107385840200800209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Schienle A, Schafer A, Hermann A, Vaitl D. Binge-eating disorder: reward sensitivity and brain activation to images of food. Biol. Psychiatry. 2009;65:654–661. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.09.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Frank GK, Shott ME, Riederer J, Pryor TL. Altered structural and effective connectivity in anorexia and bulimia nervosa in circuits that regulate energy and reward homeostasis. Transl. Psychiatry. 2016;6:e932. doi: 10.1038/tp.2016.199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Nunn K, Frampton I, Fuglset TS, Torzsok-Sonnevend M, Lask B. Anorexia nervosa and the insula. Med. Hypotheses. 2011;76:353–357. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2010.10.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Holsen LM, et al. Food motivation circuitry hypoactivation related to hedonic and nonhedonic aspects of hunger and satiety in women with active anorexia nervosa and weight-restored women with anorexia nervosa. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 2012;37:322–332. doi: 10.1503/jpn.110156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Livneh Y, et al. Homeostatic circuits selectively gate food cue responses in insular cortex. Nature. 2017;546:611–616. doi: 10.1038/nature22375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Contreras M, Ceric F, Torrealba F. Inactivation of the interoceptive insula disrupts drug craving and malaise induced by lithium. Science. 2007;318:655–658. doi: 10.1126/science.1145590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Cai H, Haubensak W, Anthony TE, Anderson DJ. Central amygdala PKC-delta(+) neurons mediate the influence of multiple anorexigenic signals. Nat. Neurosci. 2014;17:1240–1248. doi: 10.1038/nn.3767. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Qiu S, et al. An increase in synaptic NMDA receptors in the insular cortex contributes to neuropathic pain. Sci. Signal. 2013;6:ra34. doi: 10.1126/scisignal.2003778. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Adaikkan C, Rosenblum K. A molecular mechanism underlying gustatory memory trace for an association in the insular cortex. Elife. 2015;4:e07582. doi: 10.7554/eLife.07582. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Oppenheimer SM, Gelb A, Girvin JP, Hachinski VC. Cardiovascular effects of human insular cortex stimulation. Neurology. 1992;42:1727–1732. doi: 10.1212/WNL.42.9.1727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Critchley HD, Harrison NA. Visceral influences on brain and behavior. Neuron. 2013;77:624–638. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Zito KA, Bechara A, Greenwood C, van der Kooy D. The dopamine innervation of the visceral cortex mediates the aversive effects of opiates. Pharm. Biochem. Behav. 1988;30:693–699. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(88)90086-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Bermudez-Rattoni F, McGaugh JL. Insular cortex and amygdala lesions differentially affect acquisition on inhibitory avoidance and conditioned taste aversion. Brain Res. 1991;549:165–170. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(91)90616-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Ramirez-Amaya V, Alvarez-Borda B, Bermudez-Rattoni F. Differential effects of NMDA-induced lesions into the insular cortex and amygdala on the acquisition and evocation of conditioned immunosuppression. Brain Behav. Immun. 1998;12:149–160. doi: 10.1006/brbi.1998.0518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Naqvi NH, Rudrauf D, Damasio H, Bechara A. Damage to the insula disrupts addiction to cigarette smoking. Science. 2007;315:531–534. doi: 10.1126/science.1135926. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Baldo BA, Spencer RC, Sadeghian K, Mena JD. GABA-mediated inactivation of medial prefrontal and agranular insular cortex in the rat: contrasting effects on hunger- and palatability-driven feeding. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;41:960–970. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Alhadeff AL, et al. Excitatory hindbrain-forebrain communication is required for cisplatin-induced anorexia and weight loss. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:362–370. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2714-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Stuber GD, Wise RA. Lateral hypothalamic circuits for feeding and reward. Nat. Neurosci. 2016;19:198–205. doi: 10.1038/nn.4220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Cechetto DF. Central representation of visceral function. Fed. Proc. 1987;46:17–23. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Yamamoto T, Yuyama N, Kato T, Kawamura Y. Gustatory responses of cortical neurons in rats. I. Response characteristics. J. Neurophysiol. 1984;51:616–635. doi: 10.1152/jn.1984.51.4.616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Rodgers KM, Benison AM, Klein A, Barth DS. Auditory, somatosensory, and multisensory insular cortex in the rat. Cereb. Cortex. 2008;18:2941–2951. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Sawatari H, et al. Identification and characterization of an insular auditory field in mice. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2011;34:1944–1952. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07926.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Peng Y, et al. Sweet and bitter taste in the brain of awake behaving animals. Nature. 2015;527:512–515. doi: 10.1038/nature15763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Morris R, Frey S, Kasambira T, Petrides M. Ibotenic acid lesions of the basolateral, but not the central, amygdala interfere with conditioned taste aversion: evidence from a combined behavioral and anatomical tract-tracing investigation. Behav. Neurosci. 1999;113:291–302. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.113.2.291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Reilly S, Bornovalova MA. Conditioned taste aversion and amygdala lesions in the rat: a critical review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2005;29:1067–1088. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.03.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Roman C, Nebieridze N, Sastre A, Reilly S. Effects of lesions of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, lateral hypothalamus, or insular cortex on conditioned taste aversion and conditioned odor aversion. Behav. Neurosci. 2006;120:1257–1267. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.120.6.1257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Anthony TE, et al. Control of stress-induced persistent anxiety by an extra-amygdala septohypothalamic circuit. Cell. 2014;156:522–536. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.12.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Heimer L, Zahm DS, Churchill L, Kalivas PW, Wohltmann C. Specificity in the projection patterns of accumbal core and shell in the rat. Neuroscience. 1991;41:89–125. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(91)90202-Y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Zahm DS. An electron microscopic morphometric comparison of tyrosine hydroxylase immunoreactive innervation in the neostriatum and the nucleus accumbens core and shell. Brain Res. 1992;575:341–346. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)90102-F. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Jennings JH, Rizzi G, Stamatakis AM, Ung RL, Stuber GD. The inhibitory circuit architecture of the lateral hypothalamus orchestrates feeding. Science. 2013;341:1517–1521. doi: 10.1126/science.1241812. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Root DH, Melendez RI, Zaborszky L, Napier TC. The ventral pallidum: subregion-specific functional anatomy and roles in motivated behaviors. Prog. Neurobiol. 2015;130:29–70. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2015.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Grove EA. Neural associations of the substantia innominata in the rat: afferent connections. J. Comp. Neurol. 1988;277:315–346. doi: 10.1002/cne.902770302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Norgren R. Gustatory responses in the hypothalamus. Brain Res. 1970;21:63–77. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(70)90021-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Li CS, Cho YK, Smith DV. Modulation of parabrachial taste neurons by electrical and chemical stimulation of the lateral hypothalamus and amygdala. J. Neurophysiol. 2005;93:1183–1196. doi: 10.1152/jn.00828.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Matsumoto M, Hikosaka O. Lateral habenula as a source of negative reward signals in dopamine neurons. Nature. 2007;447:1111–1115. doi: 10.1038/nature05860. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Mangieri LR, et al. A neural basis for antagonistic control of feeding and compulsive behaviors. Nat. Commun. 2018;9:52. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-02534-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Duerden EG, Arsalidou M, Lee M, Taylor MJ. Lateralization of affective processing in the insula. Neuroimage. 2013;78:159–175. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Adolphs R, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR. Cortical systems for the recognition of emotion in facial expressions. J. Neurosci. 1996;16:7678–7687. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-23-07678.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Ahern GL, Schwartz GE. Differential lateralization for positive and negative emotion in the human brain: EEG spectral analysis. Neuropsychologia. 1985;23:745–755. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(85)90081-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Craig AD. How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002;3:655–666. doi: 10.1038/nrn894. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Critchley HD, Mathias CJ, Dolan RJ. Fear conditioning in humans: the influence of awareness and autonomic arousal on functional neuroanatomy. Neuron. 2002;33:653–663. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00588-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Critchley HD, Wiens S, Rotshtein P, Ohman A, Dolan RJ. Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat. Neurosci. 2004;7:189–195. doi: 10.1038/nn1176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Keum S, et al. A missense variant at the Nrxn3 locus enhances empathy fear in the mouse. Neuron. 2018;98:588–601 e585. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.03.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Kim S, Matyas F, Lee S, Acsady L, Shin HS. Lateralization of observational fear learning at the cortical but not thalamic level in mice. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2012;109:15497–15501. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1213903109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Costa NS, Vicente MA, Cipriano AC, Miguel TT, Nunes-de-Souza RL. Functional lateralization of the medial prefrontal cortex in the modulation of anxiety in mice: Left or right? Neuropharmacology. 2016;108:82–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2016.04.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Baker KB, Kim JJ. Amygdalar lateralization in fear conditioning: evidence for greater involvement of the right amygdala. Behav. Neurosci. 2004;118:15–23. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.1.15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Scicli AP, Petrovich GD, Swanson LW, Thompson RF. Contextual fear conditioning is associated with lateralized expression of the immediate early gene c-fos in the central and basolateral amygdalar nuclei. Behav. Neurosci. 2004;118:5–14. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.1.5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Kolber BJ, et al. Activation of metabotropic glutamate receptor 5 in the amygdala modulates pain-like behavior. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:8203–8213. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1216-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Sadler KE, et al. Divergent functions of the left and right central amygdala in visceral nociception. Pain. 2017;158:747–759. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000830. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Toth I, et al. Hypothalamic sidedness in mitochondrial metabolism: new perspectives. Reprod. Sci. 2014;21:1492–1498. doi: 10.1177/1933719114530188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Toth I, et al. Estrogen- and satiety state-dependent metabolic lateralization in the hypothalamus of female rats. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0137462. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0137462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Igloi K, Doeller CF, Berthoz A, Rondi-Reig L, Burgess N. Lateralized human hippocampal activity predicts navigation based on sequence or place memory. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2010;107:14466–14471. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1004243107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Denenberg VH, Garbanati J, Sherman DA, Yutzey DA, Kaplan R. Infantile stimulation induces brain lateralization in rats. Science. 1978;201:1150–1152. doi: 10.1126/science.684436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Klur S, et al. Hippocampal-dependent spatial memory functions might be lateralized in rats: an approach combining gene expression profiling and reversible inactivation. Hippocampus. 2009;19:800–816. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Zehendner CM, Luhmann HJ, Yang JW. A simple and novel method to monitor breathing and heart rate in awake and urethane-anesthetized newborn rodents. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e62628. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0062628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Salay LD, Ishiko N, Huberman AD. A midline thalamic circuit determines reactions to visual threat. Nature. 2018;557:183–189. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0078-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The source data underlying Figs. 1b, d, f, 2e, g, h, k, 3c, e, g, 4c, e, 5h, i, 6b, 7c, e, g, j, l, and n and Supplementary Figs. 1b, 2b–f, h, 3b–d, 4c, e, 5b, d, f, i, k, m, 6b, d, 7b, c, e, g, 8b, c, e, g, 9c, 10b, d, 11b, c, 12c, e, f and 13b are provided as a Source Data file. All relevant data are available from the corresponding authors upon reasonable request.