Skip to main content
Frontiers in Psychiatry logoLink to Frontiers in Psychiatry
. 2020 Jan 28;10:1016. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.01016

Sensory Abnormalities in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Focus on the Tactile Domain, From Genetic Mouse Models to the Clinic

Luigi Balasco 1,*, Giovanni Provenzano 2,*, Yuri Bozzi 1,3,*
PMCID: PMC6997554  PMID: 32047448

Abstract

Sensory abnormalities are commonly recognized as diagnostic criteria in autism spectrum disorder (ASD), as reported in the last edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorder (DSM-V). About 90% of ASD individuals have atypical sensory experiences, described as both hyper- and hypo-reactivity, with abnormal responses to tactile stimulation representing a very frequent finding. In this review, we will address the neurobiological bases of sensory processing in ASD, with a specific focus of tactile sensitivity. In the first part, we will review the most relevant sensory abnormalities detected in ASD, and then focus on tactile processing deficits through the discussion of recent clinical and experimental studies. In the search for the neurobiological bases of ASD, several mouse models have been generated with knockout and humanized knockin mutations in many ASD-associated genes. Here, we will therefore give a brief overview of the anatomical structure of the mouse somatosensory system, and describe the somatosensory abnormalities so far reported in different mouse models of ASD. Understanding the neurobiological bases of sensory processing in ASD mouse models may represent an opportunity for a better comprehension of the mechanisms underlying sensory abnormalities, and for the development of novel effective therapeutic strategies.

Keywords: autism, somatosensory, touch, mouse, behavior

Introduction: Altered Sensory Reactivity in ASD

Individuals diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) show deficits in social interaction and communication (developing, understanding and maintaining relationships) and repetitive/stereotyped behaviors, with different degree of severity, and sensory issues (1). However, it is interesting how typical cognitive difficulties of ASD are often associated with alterations in the perception of the external world. It is estimated that about 90% of individuals diagnosed with ASD have atypical sensory experiences (2). Differences in the sensory profile of ASD subjects are confirmed across lifespan (3, 4) and cross-culturally (5). Indeed, this trait is nowadays recognized in the DSM-V as hyper/hypo reactivity to sensory stimuli, demonstrating its primary importance in the description of the syndrome.

The correlation among autism and sensory deficits is not new. Formerly, (6) one of the first to describe autism, included different atypical sensory behaviors in his analysis (including heightened sensitivity to noise and touch, attraction to visual patterns and spinning objects, finger-stimming in front of the eyes) although considering them as a secondary phenomenon that occurs in parallel to the primary phenomenon (6).

(7) were the first instead to describe, in a case report study, a group of children who were particularly reactive to “unusual sensitivities” (low intensities of stimulation) in several sensory modalities (7). They hypothesized that an early developmental onset of sensitivity to sensory stimuli would cause social withdrawal in childhood.

Based on the Freud’s “protective barrier against stimuli” (Reizschutz), they proposed that these children eventually succeed in building defending strategies to protect themselves from sensory overload, which would result in developmental distortions typical of autistic conditions. Eveloff described different behavioral difficulties faced by autistic children (8). He hypothesized that altered sensory processing is the effect of the lack of early experiences of environmental stimuli therefore interfering with the development of self-representations in autism. Psychologist Lorna Wing noted the “detail-oriented” behavior of autistic children, showing that they have significantly more sensory processing abnormalities than both typically developing (TD) children and children with Down’s syndrome (9). She was the first to suggest to include abnormal sensory perceptual features as a proper diagnostic tool into ‘basic impairments in autism’. However, this was not included in the first diagnostic criteria for autism by DSM in 1980 (10). Ornitz extended the concept of autism as a sensory and information processing disorder (11). He suggested that autism could be highlighted in young children looking at abnormal behaviors caused by perceptual differences (12). Nonetheless, there have been detractors of this theory and the strength of sensory features in autism has been put under scrutiny during the past decades. As an example, Richer strongly argued against the sensory theory of autism stating it was “incoherent and instable” (13). He, instead, stated that the autistic children’s incompetence in language and symbol use was mainly due to their avoidance behavior in communication interactions. Similarly, Rutter proposed language deficits as the base of autistic syndrome, distancing himself from the sensory theory (14). Another line of research came in parallel from the field of occupational therapy (OT). (15) formulated the theory of sensory integration (SI) dysfunction to describe several neurological disorders including autism. This theory tried to relate sensory processing deficits with behavioral abnormalities and had the merit to define SI in terms of behavioral responses identifying for example tactile defensiveness and fight-or-flight reactions. More recently, OT researchers have suggested to consider the use of the term “sensory processing disorder” as comprising three primary groups (sensory modulation disorder, sensory discrimination disorder and sensory-based motor disorder) and the subtypes found within each group (16). However, sensory processing disorder is not still considered as a disorder per se. With Rutter, the theory of “social-perception” took hold in 1983 (17). He concluded the sensory symptoms found in the autistic population were the result of deficits in social cognition. It is not the processing of a sensory stimulus per se that creates difficulties in the autistic subject, but rather the processing of stimuli of emotional nature (i.e., those that possess a social content). Finally, in 2013 sensory processing deficits were included for the first time among the international diagnostic criteria of autism in the revision of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-V) (1). From a clinical point of view, sensory deficits are documented already in the 6th month of life of infants later diagnosed with autism (18, 19). This gives us a dual information, firstly that sensory symptoms anticipate social and communication deficits (19), and secondly that abnormal sensory traits could be predictive of the autistic condition (20). This appears strikingly evident when considering that not only the vast majority of individual diagnosed with autism experience atypical reactivity to sensory stimuli (2, 21), but also that this affect every sensory modality: smell (22, 23), taste (24), audition (25), vision (26), and touch (27, 28). It seems clear that understanding the neurobiological bases underlying these sensory processing deficits represents a new challenge for ASD research, specifically aiming to identify early biomarkers and novel possible therapeutic strategies for these disorders.

Here, we will describe sensory defects in ASD, specifically focusing on altered tactile sensitivity. An in-depth analysis of somatosensory defects detected in mouse lines harboring mutations in ASD-relevant genes will also be presented. The aim of this review is to highlight the contribution of animal model studies in our understanding of the neurobiological bases of altered sensory sensitivity in ASD.

Smell, Taste, Auditory, and Visual Deficits in ASD

Smell

The sense of smell in ASD is poorly investigated. Nonetheless, a parent-report study pointed out how the most pronounced sensory symptom to dissociate ASD children from children with other developmental disorders are de facto taste and smell abnormal responses (29). Furthermore, it has been reported that almost 40% of ASD children with sensory abnormalities exhibit an altered smell and taste perception (30). Children and adolescents aged between 10 and 18 years showed impaired olfactory identification, but typical odor detection (31). Another study with ASD children aged from 5 to 9 years showed no differences in olfactory identification compared with controls, however older children were less accurate than younger ones at identifying odors (32). In the follow-up study (5 years later), the same ASD individuals had developed odor identification impairments (33). A more recent study confirmed that ASD children present impaired odor identification but normal odor detection compared to control participants (22). However, a clear picture of how and when altered olfaction occurs in the ASD cascade has not yet emerged (22). Considering the possible influence of language in the common odor task, Rosenkrantz and coworkers suggested to use olfactory sniffing as a language and task-free measure of autism and its severity. Since sniffs are automatically modulated (vigorous sniffs for pleasant and truncated sniffs for unpleasant odors), these authors found that children with autism had a profound altered sniff response, sniffing equally regardless of odor valence (for example taking vigorous sniffs of rotten fish odor) compared to typically developing controls (23). These authors also found that this difference persisted despite equal reported odor perception and allowed for 81% correct ASD classification based on the sniff response alone. Moreover, they found that increasingly aberrant sniffing was associated with increasingly severe ASD, proposing it as a novel ASD biomarker (23).

Taste

There are few studies that deal with taste processing in ASD. However, it has been reported that children with ASD eat a smaller variety of food (e.g., less vegetables, fruits, dairy) regardless of texture, and refuse more food than typical developing children (34, 35). Bennetto and colleagues found a lower accuracy in taste discrimination for sour and bitter tastes, but similar identification for sweet and salty tastes, in adolescents (10–18 years) with ASD compared with control peers (31). Similar results were found in adults, except for sweet taste which was also impaired in those adults with ASD (24). A possible explanation for these taste identification differences in ASD might stem from the restricted diets of ASD patients that could alternatively explain why adolescents and adults are less accurate in identifying tastes (24). Abnormalities at the level of peripheral receptors and their transduction cascades could lead to taste impairments (36). Another view focuses on central rather than peripheral mechanisms (31). Indeed, central areas such as the thalamus, insula and cingulate cortex are involved in taste discrimination (37), and areas including the thalamus have been shown to be reduced in size in individuals with ASD (38). Thus, the difference in taste processing might be the result of atypical activity in these areas. However, further investigation is needed to understand whether ASD is associated with taste sensory defects at a peripheral or central level.

Audition

Sensory processing abnormalities have also been observed in the auditory domain. Indeed, children with autism often show difficulty in discerning two occurring tones when presented closely (39). In addition, a delayed evoked neural response compared to TD children have also been documented (40, 41). This latency has been observed in response to pure tones as well as to complex social stimuli (for example sound produced by speech) (42) and has been proposed as predictive of autism symptom severity (43). Although evidences for sensory processing deficits are more and more abundant in ASD literature, there are several reports that highlight enhanced perceptual strengths in response to specific sensory stimuli. As an example, individuals with ASD show superior abilities in pitch discrimination and in categorization compared to controls (25). In an effort to bring together all these findings, it has been suggested that perceptual capabilities may be subject to the nature and complexity of the sensory stimuli, with impairments associated with more complex stimuli and enhancements seen more often with simple stimuli (44, 45).

Vision

Over the years, there have been many studies investigating different aspects of visual perception in ASD. Defective retinal function has been described in ASD patients (4649). Enhanced visual evoked potentials (VEP) in response to high spatial frequencies have been found in visual brain areas of ASD children, while unaffected control children generally responded to visual stimuli with low spatial frequency (50). Other studies showed that visual perception in ASD is more detail-oriented, suggesting that primary visual processing might contribute to social and communication deficits in ASD (5154). It is generally accepted that individuals with ASD “see” and process the world differently, having a strong detail oriented ability in expense to global processing (55). ASD individuals are faster in detecting single details in Embedded Figure Tasks (EFT- a task in which participants are asked to find a target figure hidden in a larger image), being less susceptible to distractors (5660). Moreover, gaze patterns from individuals with autism show a preference for scene regions of high pixel-level saliency compared with object-level saliency or semantic-level saliency scenes in passive viewing of naturalistic scenes (61). This means that they favor regions of the scene that are related with contrast, color, or orientation (pixel-level) rather than related with size, density, or contour complexity of objects (object-level) or related with text, tools, or faces (semantic-level). How individuals with ASD have this detail-oriented visual ability is still under debate. Moreover, further complexity is given from the fact that in autism, basic measures of visual sensitivity such as visual acuity (59, 62), contrast discrimination (63, 64), and orientation processing (65, 66) are all comparable with normal developing children. Conversely to the static stimuli, ASD individuals show atypical processing of dynamic visual stimuli (67, 68). Indeed, ASD subjects show an impaired global motion perception in discerning the direction of a cloud of moving dots (69, 70), even though the detection thresholds for local motion appear to be typical (71), or ever superior in ASD (72, 73).

Tactile Senitivity in ASD

The typical description of sensory processing abnormalities falls in the terminology of “over-responsiveness”, “under-responsiveness”, and “failure to habituate”. Over-responsiveness, also called hyper-sensitivity, refers to children being more “reactive” to sensory stimulation compared to controls (74, 75), often associated with negative emotion or active avoidance of stimulation. However, the terminology used in clinical reports and questionnaires often fails in separating “over-responsiveness” from “impaired habituation”. Moreover, it is unclear whether this refers to hyper-excitability of sensory cortex or the expression of negative emotions to tactile stimulation. Conversely, under-responsiveness, also called hypo-sensitivity, is characterized by reduced reactivity to sensory stimulation and sensory seeking (75). Both over- and under-responsiveness then fall under the general term of tactile defensiveness (76), which describes both abnormal emotional responses to tactile stimulation as well as withdrawal/avoidance of a stimulation.

The vast majority of studies investigating tactile dysfunction have traditionally focused on parent and teacher reports and questionnaires. These studies, although informative, lack objectivity in the strict sense since they are based on subjective assessments of both behavioral and emotional responses to touch (77). Only recent works addressed the study of tactile abnormalities through a psychophysics approach, aiming to reduce the degree of subjectivity and to highlight neurophysiological underpinnings of this phenomenon.

As reported in a recent review (77), a number of studies have described tactile abnormalities using sensory profiles and parents reports (7881). By using two parent-report measures, the Short Sensory Profile (SSP) and the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R), and a clinical observation with the Autism Diagnostic Observation Scale (ADOS), Rogers et al. compared sensory profiles in toddlers with ASD and typically developing controls and with other groups of developmental delay such as Fragile-X children and children with Down syndrome. They found significantly elevated levels of sensory symptoms in children with autism compared with both children with typical development and those with delayed development of the same mental age. In particular, children with autism obtained significantly higher scores of tactile sensitivity and auditory filtering than children in the developmental delay and controls. Moreover, they observed a correlation between abnormal sensitivity and adaptive behaviors. They also found no meaningful relationships between social-communicative scores and sensory scores in children with mixed developmental delays, or the typically developing children. The explanation of the authors is that since sensory symptoms are not in general a peculiarity of autism, they could represent an additional primary impairment rather than an autism-specific impairment. Moreover, they found that sensory scores (including tactile scores) did not correlate with either developmental levels or with ratio IQ scores for any group except the children with Fragile-X syndrome. Increased sensory scores were associated with clinical diagnosis rather than with IQ or immature developmental levels (78). Other tests including the Infant/Toddler Sensory Profile (ITSP), Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (ITSEA), Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R), and Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic (ADOS-G), revealed that toddlers with ASD show higher under responsiveness (described as low registration by the authors) and stimulus avoidance as well as low frequency of seeking behaviors compared to IQ-, age-matched controls (79). Foss-Feig and coworkers investigated both under- and over-responsiveness to tactile stimuli in children with ASD through three measures of sensory processing: Tactile Defensiveness and Discrimination Test-Revised (TDDT-R), the Sensory Experiences Questionnaire (SEQ), and the Sensory Profile (SP). They reported that heightened levels of tactile seeking behavior were associated with more severe levels of social and repetitive behaviors. Additionally, heightened levels of hypo-responsiveness to tactile stimuli were associated with more severe levels of social and non-verbal communication impairments as well as increased repetitive behaviors. Conversely, over-responsiveness was not correlated with any of core symptoms of ASD (80). Data extracted from experimenter-reports of over-responsiveness, parent-reports of tactile symptoms, and self-reports of pleasantness of texture, showed that children with ASD have superior over-responsiveness scores compared to controls. Moreover, they observed a positive correlation between over-responsiveness and parent-report of tactile symptoms and between over-responsiveness and social impairments. Conversely, pleasantness ratings were inversely related with impaired communication (81). However, the contribution of ASD comorbidities such as intellectual disability (ID) and/or language impairments might have a role in defining the responses to studies involving self-reports and have to be considered. An individual with ASD and ID may have difficulties in describing to the experimenter the sensations generated by the stimulus, adding complexity to the interpretation and replication of studies. A risk of an imbalanced picture of ASD may arise and a selection bias for intellectual disability has been reported as issue in ASD research (82, 83). Most of studies on tactile processing so far have focused on children, however there are also studies (4, 84) showing that abnormal sensory processing is also present in adults.

These studies, although informative indicators of the tactile abnormalities in ASD, appear to be inconsistent with respect to pattern of response, correlation among measures, and diagnostic terms. In addition, different types of reports were used in different studies. All such aspects render these studies difficult to be compared; moreover, they do not always correlate to clinical observation, nor they provide indicators of possible cortical dysfunctions.

More recently, researchers have preferred a psychophysics approach to study tactile functionality in ASD in a more objective modality. Some of these studies have shown how detection of tactile stimuli is impaired in both adults and children with ASD (for example in vibration detection; (85), so in line with previous reports. However, other studies showed that tactile detection is normal in autism (57, 86, 87). It is possible to speculate that these differences result from the different type of stimulation used (i.e., flutter, vibration, sinusoidal, or constant) as well as its location. Although these works have the merit of bringing a greater objectivity to the study of tactile abnormalities in ASD, it remains unclear whether underlying sensory mechanisms are altered, or it is the emotional response to sensory input that leads to issues in filtering of the signal resulting in hyper/hypo-responsiveness.

Imaging studies have also tried to investigate the underlying neural mechanism of abnormal tactile sensitivity in ASD. Since tactile stimuli are part of the somatosensory world and as such rely on subcortical and cortical brain regions, researchers focused on possible differences in these brain areas between ASD and TD control subjects. Coskun and colleagues were among the first to report abnormalities in the sensory map organization of ASD individuals. Using magnetoencephalography (MEG) recordings, these authors examined the cortical responses to passive stimulation of the thumb and index finger of dominant hand as well as the lip from ASD and TD controls. They found a different cortical representation of the thumb and the lip in ASD individuals compared to TD controls (88), namely the distance between the cortical representations of these two body parts was significantly larger in the autism group than in TD subjects. Moreover, in the cerebral cortex, the thumb is typically closer to the lip than the index finger; this was not observed in ASD individuals. However, as found in a successive study by the same group (89), the variability of the evoked potential as a response to passive stimulation of the thumb and index finger did not differ between controls and adults with ASD. Conversely, other authors showed a lower amplitude of contralateral cortical S1 response to tactile stimulation in children with ASD (27). Although these studies provide us with useful indications of cortical function in autism, discrepancies exist across studies. Moreover, the variability in neural responses appears to be higher in ASD (90, 91). A possible explanation could be sought in the type of stimulation involved (i.e., passive vs. active) as well as in the high heterogeneity of ASD (2). In addition, a limit of these studies lies in the complexity to compare findings in children with those obtained in adolescents and adults.

Several studies suggest that ASD pathogenesis might involve an imbalance between excitation and inhibition (E/I imbalance). This hypothesis is supported by several lines of evidence showing that the γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) system is altered in ASD, and that may relate to alterations in sensation and symptoms in both animal models and humans. A pivotal role of GABAergic dysfunction in ASD was first hypothesized in early 2000s by Hussman (92) and Rubenstein and Merzenich (93), even if the key role of GABA in shaping neural response to tactile stimulation (94, 95), as well as in brain development and cortical plasticity (96, 97), was known from many years. Several genetic, neuropathological, and neuroimaging studies showed that GABAergic dysfunctions occur in ASD (98), and defective GABAergic neurotransmission has been suggested as a potential candidate in sensory deficits in ASD (99). In the tactile domain, a study investigating tactile detection thresholds in TD children was the first to report that tactile sensitivity was associated with GABRB3 genetic variation in typically developing children (100), confirming findings from animal model studies. The GABRB3 gene, coding for the β3 subunit of the GABA receptor channel, is one of the many candidate genes to be associated with autism (101, 102). Moreover, GABA levels were shown to be reduced in the sensorimotor cortex and positively correlated with worsened detection thresholds in children with ASD; in addition, GABA levels were not correlated with adaptation or frequency discrimination as for TD children (103).

Taken together, these results suggest that altered inhibition could explain some of the behavioral features of tactile abnormalities in ASD. Studies performed in appropriate mouse models contributed to better understand the neurobiological bases of tactile abnormalities in ASD.

Understanding the Impact of Somatosensory Function in Shaping Social Behavior

Altered sensory processing has revealed to be an important feature for the clinical description of ASD. As discussed above in the review, sensory dysregulation encompasses multiple modalities (vision, hearing, touch, olfaction, gustation) and arises early in the progression of ASD. There is evidence that this could impact social functioning. It has been proposed that sensory stimuli and social behaviors may have a reciprocal influence on each other throughout development (104). This idea is reinforced form findings of early abnormal sensory sensitivity to stimuli predicting later joint attention and language development (18) and higher levels of social impairment in adults with ASD (105).

Touch is considered one of the most basic ways to sense the external world (106) and has been reported to have a significant role in role in several social aspects such as communication (107), developing social bonds (108), and overall physical development and connectivity of brain areas (109, 110). For this reason, skin has been proposed by some authors as “social organ” (111).

It has been suggested that irregularities in touch and tactile perception may be associated with broad levels of social dysfunction in ASD. For example, as described earlier in the paper, touch seeking behaviors have been found to predict levels of social impairment, and tactile hypo responsivity was associated with both poorer social functioning and nonverbal communication skills (80). Differences in tactile processing and tactile preference behaviors in ASD have also been reported in early infancy (112). Furthermore, lack of social touch can lead to higher levels of anxiety, stress, and depression (113), aspects which are commonly seen in ASD population (114, 115). Moreover, atypical touch during infancy can develop into critical deficits later in life, specifically in regards to attachment. While individuals with ASD are capable of forming a secure attachment to their caregivers, they tend to be less securely attached than their typically developing peers (116). In addition, individuals with ASD who have secure attachments tend to have less socially severe symptoms than individuals with ASD who are not securely attached, suggesting symptom severity and overall level of functioning could impact the strength of attachment (117).

Touch is also important in developing social bonding. Oxytocin, the neuropeptide primarily involved in social bonding, has long known to be released in response to positive tactile stimuli (touch, warmth, odors) (118). In individuals with ASD, oxytocin abnormalities have been found in plasma levels (119), in the gene that encodes for the oxytocin receptor, OXTR (120), as well as in oxytocin receptors (121). However, the behavioral and neural effects of oxytocin were negatively correlated with ASD-like traits, suggesting these effects to be diminished in individuals exhibiting low social and emotional abilities associated with autistic traits (122). Future research should look further into the importance of tactile perception in shaping social aspects, as well as its impact on other social domains not previously explored.

Theoretical models have been proposed to integrate sensory and social features of ASD. One model that tried to explain altered sensory functioning in ASD is the “temporal binding hypothesis” (123). This theory lays on the assumption that sensory stimuli that occur in close temporal proximity are more likely to be integrated and so to be perceived as a whole; thus, timing information is crucial to binding and integrating associated stimuli (124). The possibility of an extended “temporal binding window” in individuals with ASD which may give rise to alterations in sensory processing has been proposed (125). Indeed, a longer temporal binding window could create a blurred, unpredictable sensory environment, as unrelated stimuli become bound together. Ideally, throughout development important social cues may fail to become integrated or salient. Thus, according to this theory an extended temporal binding window could negatively impact social behavior in ASD through altered binding of social cues.

Another theory is the “intense world theory” which offers a neurological mechanism for how the sensory and social features of ASD may be related (126, 127). This theory proposes an excessive functioning of neural circuits as the base of sensory and social impairments. Thus, such neural circuits are hyper-reactive, hyper-plastic, and generally up-regulated. This would create an intense world, a fragmented world (with focus on individual components of the environment), and an aversive world. Low level sensory perception is enhanced (intense world) and coupled with deficits in sensory integration (fragmented world). Throughout development, this could lead to an over-specialization for perceiving primary sensory cues at the expense of the ability to navigate in a socially complex world (127). In this way, the intense world theory explains both the unique sensory and the social features of ASD and offers a mechanism for how an up-regulation in primary sensory perception results in social avoidance and withdrawal.

Another theory focus on “atypical hierarchical information processing” as base of sensory and social functioning defects in individuals with ASD. Since we live in a world buzzed with stimuli, in order to adequately perceive and operate in it, humans use both incoming sensory information (bottom-up processes) and inference from prior experience and context (top-down processes) (128). It has been suggested that under-utilization of top-down processes such as context or experience (129) or an over-reliance on bottom-up sensory perception (130) is characteristic of perception in ASD. At the neural level, this profile may reflect hyper-activation of primary sensory cortices, decreased prefrontal activity, and reduced neural habituation during sensory processing (131). According to this theory, this information processing profile may inhibit social functioning as the interpersonal world demands strong central coherences, integration of context, and utilization of prior knowledge. Thus, over-functioning of bottom-up sensory processing coupled with under-utilizing top-down perception in ASD could explain both enhanced sensory processing and inefficient social functioning in this population.

When discussing dysfunctions of the somatosensory system, it is important to consider the sensory processing cascade in its entirety. Starting from the periphery (i.e., the skin, where the mechanical stimuli are transduced in electrical signals), moving to the intermediate stations (i.e., spinal cord and/or brainstem, where the electrical signals are delivered by means of neuronal ascending pathways), reaching subcortical and cortical brain areas (i.e., primary somatosensory cortex and other higher function somatosensory processing areas, where integration/codification of the information occurs), sensory information can undergo more or less severe modifications. Indeed, abnormal development or interaction in any of these steps could ideally lead to abnormal sensory processing. Moreover, since proper tactile perception is of importance in early development as well as in forming social and physical relationships (132), a possible relation between tactile abnormalities and social behaviors could be a matter of fact. For this reason, when assessing the behavioral outcomes of relevant social/sensory task performed by mouse models of ASD, it is at least necessary, when possible, to correlate the behavioral response to a potential neurobiological defect. Indeed, even though humans and animals have evolved under different evolutionary pressures making social behaviors much harder to compare, molecular and cellular functions are strongly conserved and so appear to be mostly comparable.

However, what must be kept in mind is that social behaviors not a unitary behavior with a unique neurobiological basis, but rather different aspects of social behavior show different neural substrates. Moreover, the modulation of environmental cues, the type of sensory stimulation, and the role of conspecific actions in shaping the social response add complexity to our understanding of social behavior in animals (included humans) (133).

In recent years, social neuroscience has made great progress in identifying the neural substrates of social behaviors, and the brain processes linked to social interactions in disease have received considerable attention. In humans, social cognition differentiates between social perceptual processes (devoted to the detection and the analysis of social stimuli like a face), social attribution processes (involved in the inference of other’s and one’s mental states from behavior), and social categorization processes (involved in the process by which individuals are placed into groups based on common characteristics like gender and ethnicity). On the neural level, the social perceptual processes include the primary sensory areas as well as more specialized regions like the fusiform face area (FFA) and the temporo-occipital associative cortex (V5 and extra-striate body area, EBA). The social attribution processes include the premotor cortex, the superior temporal sulcus (STS) and the temporo-parietal junction (TPJ). The social categorization processes encompass the medial/dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the anterior cingulate cortex. Instead, the emotional content and the motivational appraisal of social stimuli appear to be mediated by the amygdala, the orbitofrontal cortex, and the hippocampus (134, 135). Although one should be always cautious in comparing social behaviors in humans and in mice, it interesting how brain regions such as the amygdala and the hippocampus have been also related to social circuits for behavioral decision in mice (133).

How does social behavior relate to sensory stimulation processing? This question is far from being fully answered, however what we can say is that the sensory inputs (coming from other individuals or from the environment), whether they are olfactory, visual, or somatosensory, are processed and integrated over time with social internal states and transformed into behavioral outputs that in turn provide sensory cues to the other individual forming a feedback loop (133). As an example, somatosensory stimulation is critical for both mating (during copulation) and parenting (tactile stimulation of pups affects the development of normal behaviors) (136, 137).

The hypothesis that peripheral nervous system dysfunctions (namely, dysfunctions of the sensory system) could contribute to ASD pathophysiology has a recent history. Although somatosensory abnormalities in humans and rodents have long been reported, still little is known about their role in ASD. Further efforts are necessary to unravel the neural correlates of social behaviors, and their relationship with sensory processing abnormalities could be of help in describing the social impairments found in ASD.

Studying the Neurobiological Basis of ASD Through Mouse Models

It has long been known that ASD has a high degree of heritability: studies on monozygotic twins revealed a peak of concordance of 90% compared to 10% of dizygotic twins and siblings (138, 139). However, only recent efforts and technological advancements in genetics made it possible to identify a plethora of gene variants associated with ASD. These variants have been found in several hundreds of different genes and cover the entire spectrum of mutations, from single-nucleotide variants (SNVs) to copy number variants (CNVs), including inherited as well as de novo mutations (140, 141). Several genetic mutations in ASD have been associated with genes coding for proteins involved in synaptic functions, such as SHANK (142), CNTNAP (143, 144), NLGN (145, 146), and NRXN (147). Some examples of CNVs associated with ASD include chromosomal loci 15q11-q13 (148), 16p11.2 (149), and the UBE3A (150), NRXN1 (147), and CNTN4 (151) genes. In adding complexity to the understanding of ASD pathophysiology, a subset of single gene mutations associated with ASD are also responsible for other neurodevelopmental disorders, including FMR1 in fragile X syndrome, TSC1 in tuberous sclerosis, and MECP2 in Rett syndrome. The tremendous progress made in identifying all these genes associated with ASD has subsequently resulted in the generation of several ASD mouse models, through which it is possible to infer the effect of single mutations, thus advancing our understanding of the biological bases underpinning this complex syndrome. A multitude of mouse models have been generated by knockout and knockin mutations in ASD candidate genes. In developing new mouse models it is important to consider different aspects such as face validity (i.e., resemblance to human symptoms), construct validity (i.e., similarity to the causes of the disease), and predictive validity (i.e., expected responses to treatments that are effective in the human disease), with the best animal model keeping together the three validity criteria (152).

Given the complex phenotypic and genetic heterogeneity of ASD, developing a mouse model keeping together all these aspects represents a challenge for every researcher. Nonetheless, according to the Simons Foundation Autism Research Initiative (SFARI) Gene database (http://gene.sfari.org/, as of October 29, 2019) there are up to now 264 genetic, 42 pharmacologically induced and 4 inbred mouse models of ASD.

Since the diagnosis of ASD is mainly given by the analysis of behavioral aspects rather than physiological criteria, and being the mice, like humans, a social species displaying an extensive variety of social behaviors, neuroscientists tried to develop and refine behavioral paradigms that could be relevant to the human condition. The symptoms however may be uniquely humans and are often highly variable among individuals, so it appears clear that designing mouse behavioral assays relevant to autistic symptoms represents a unique challenge. However, different behavioral paradigms have been developed considering the two core symptoms of human disorder (social/communication defects and repetitive behaviors) and revealed to be qualitatively efficient and reproducible. For a detailed discussion of these experimental approaches, the reader is referred to the comprehensive review by Silverman et al. (153).

Beyond the central vs peripheral dysfunction dichotomy in ASD, it is interesting how sensory impairments in ASD do not only correlate to tactile processing defects but rather represent a complex multifaceted sensory phenomenon that encompass also other sensory systems. A brilliant example comes from the recent work by Goel and colleagues (154) focused on the sensory processing of Fmr1-/- mice in the visual domain. These mice exhibit a delayed learning in a visual discrimination task, an impairment similar to the human deficit in visual perception in FXS individuals. The reduced number of orientation-selective pyramidal cells of the primary visual cortex (V1) might represent the neural correlate of this defect. In targeting the visual cortex, the authors also found a reduction in the functional output of parvalbumin (PV) neurons (a subclass of GABAergic interneurons) in Fmr1-/- mice, as compared to wild-type controls. Surprisingly, when a DREADD (Designer Receptors Exclusively Activated by Designer Drugs) strategy was used to restore PV activity and orientation tuning in V1, Fmr1-/- mice accelerated learning in the visual task. Other studies focusing on the visual domain has also been carried out on En2-/- (155), BTBR (156), and SERT-Ala56 knockin mice (157).

Organization of the Mouse and Human Somatosensory System

The somatosensory system in mammals conveys sensory information from receptors located in the skin, muscle, and joints to the brain. In mice, the somatosensory system is dominated by the input coming from the facial vibrissae: the neuronal representation of whiskers in the primary somatosensory cortex (the barrel field) occupies more than two thirds of its total area (158). The anatomical and functional organization of the somatosensory system is highly conserved and is based on two major ascending components: the dorsal column system and the trigeminal system. The first-order sensory neurons are the dorsal root ganglion cells and the trigeminal ganglion cells that collect information from the receptors located in the body and the face, respectively. The whisker pad of mice is highly innervated: a single whisker follicle is sheathed in a complex capsular structure which receives up to 200 axonal projections (159). Moreover, the nerves of a single whisker do not connect with the adjacent follicle (160). The dorsal root ganglion (DRG) neurons send their central processes to make synapse in the spinal cord while the trigeminal ganglion cells make synapse in the hindbrain. The main hindbrain nucleus receiving afferents from the whisker system is the spinal trigeminal nucleus (Sp). The Sp can be divided in the oral, interpolar and caudal part (Sp5O, Sp5I, Sp5C), forming the largest nucleus of the mouse hindbrain. The whisker macro representation starts to be appreciable at the level of the hindbrain in concrete structures called “barrelettes” (161). The spinal cord and hindbrain nuclei in turn project to specialized somatosensory nuclei of the thalamus: the ventral posterior group (VP). The initial anatomical separation of the two systems is interrupted at the level of the thalamus, which represents a relay station for all sensory stimuli. The VP region of the thalamus is subdivided into a large medial portion (VPM), which receives afferents from the trigeminal system, and a smaller lateral portion (VPL) which instead receives afferents from the limbs and the trunk. The size of each subdivision of VP is proportional to the number of afferents, so the VPM appears to be larger than the VPL. Moreover, even from the VPM it is possible to appreciate a representation of individual facial whiskers, the so called “barreloids” (162). Somatosensory processes also terminate in clusters of heterogeneous thalamic nuclei (the posterior group, Po) lying medial, dorsal, and caudal to VPM. The largest component of the Po forms the medial subdivision (PoM), which also receives inputs from the whisker pad providing a parallel source of information to the primary somatosensory (S1) cortex (163). In rodents, two further clusters of nuclei have been identified in this region of the thalamus: the reticular nucleus of the thalamus (Rt) and the zona incerta (ZI). These two clusters do not receive somatosensory input from the brainstem or spinal cord but being packed with GABAergic neurons and strongly projecting to the VP, they are thought to play an important role in modulating the output of VP (164). All somatosensory stimuli converge onto the primary (S1) and secondary (S2) somatosensory cortices. S1 is dorsolateral in the rostral part of the neocortex, whereas S2 is located laterally to S1. The primary somatosensory cortex in mice is dominated by the barrel field (S1BF), containing the representation of single facial whiskers. In 1970, Woolsey and Van Der Loos were the first to report these distinct anatomical structures named “barrels” (165). Further division of the S1 are the forelimb area (S1FL), the trunk area (S1Tr), and the hindlimb area (S1HL), with each of these areas characterized by a thick condensed layer IV. Figure 1 schematically reports the organization of the somatosensory pathways in mice.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

The mouse somatosensory system. Somatosensory stimuli coming from the head region of the mouse are conveyed to the brain through trigeminal ganglion neurons. Neuronal fibers are depicted in blue (for trigeminal ganglion pathway) and green (for anterior and lateral spinothalamic pathways). The ophthalmic (V1), maxillary (V2) and mandibular (V3) branches of trigeminal ganglion process region-specialized somatosensory information with the maxillary branch (V2) innervating the whiskers. Here whiskers are indicated and color-coded to best follow their brain representations (whisker pad). Trigeminal ganglion neurons project to brainstem nuclei (spinal trigeminal nuclei – Sp) where they form an inverted neuronal representation of single whiskers (barrelettes). Trigeminothalamic fibers in turn project to the ventral posteromedial nucleus in the thalamus (Vpm) where again single whiskers are represented and shifted in orientation (barreloids). Finally, thalamocortical axons from the Vpm reach the primary somatosensory cortex (S1) in the barrel field, forming the final neuronal representation of single whiskers (barrels). Somatosensory stimuli coming from the body of the mouse are instead conveyed to the brainstem through dorsal root ganglia (DRG) neurons. The main difference in this system is the fact that somatosensory stimuli are conveyed to the ventral posterolateral nucleus of the thalamus (Vpl) before reaching the sensory cortex. See text for references.

As compared to the mouse, the human somatosensory system presents important similarities and differences. Somatosensory receptors located in the skin are essentially the same, and the anatomy of the ascending pathway organization is maintained in both species. The organization of somatosensory cortex found in mice is comparable to that found in mammals with relatively little expansion of the neocortex (166). Much of somatosensory cortex in these mammals is represented by two distinct systematic representations of the contralateral body surface, named the first (primary) representation, or S-I, and the second representation, or S-II (167). The larger S-I represents the body from tail to mouth in a mediolateral cortical sequence, while the smaller S-II has a head-to-tail mediolateral (or dorsoventral) cortical sequence (168). Instead, somatosensory cortex in higher primates (including humans) contains more subdivisions than somatosensory cortex in non-primates. Experiments on the organization of anterior parietal cortex in macaque monkeys defined S-I as a broad region including cytoarchitectonic areas 3 (3a and 3b), 1, and 2 of Brodmann, though Kaas argues that only area 3b should be considered primary somatosensory cortex (168, 169). Area 3b, indeed, forms a complete representation of the body surface. In mice, two whiskers that are adjacent to each other on the animal’s face are represented in adjacent cortical barrels, and the barrel field constitutes a topographic map. Similarly, a topographical organization of the somatosensory cortex (the so called homunculus) is present in humans (170). As for the cortical representation of the whiskers in mouse and rat, the homunculus is a topographic map because neighboring sites on the skin are represented at neighboring sites in the cortex. The whiskers are the critical touch organ in rats and mice, whereas in humans and other primates the fingertips are their equivalent. Each fingertip is innervated by axons from 250–300 sensory neurons (a comparable number as the whisker) and because individual axons terminate in multiple receptor structures, the density of mechanoreceptors is remarkably high (over 1,000 per cm2). One important way in which fingerprint touch differs from whisker touch is that primates manipulate objects with our hands whereas rodents do not manipulate objects with their whiskers. This difference is evident when comparing the mechanism for sensing texture. For mice and rodents in general, the firing rate of neurons in barrel cortex differ from rough to smooth surface (171). In primates, the perception of coarse textures is based on the difference in firing rate between adjacent slowly adapting neurons(172); the perception of fine surfaces is based on vibrations in the skin, transduced by rapidly adapting Pacinian receptors (173). Finally, important differences have been found in the structure of supragranular layers 2 and 3 of the mouse and human somatosensory cortex (174). Figure 2 schematically reports the somatotopic representation of the mouse and human primary somatosensory cortex.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Comparison of cortical somatosensory representation in mice and humans. Distorted representation of body areas in the mouse (A) and human (B) primary somatosensory cortex (S1). In both species, S1 somatosensory maps reflect the extent of cortical areas devoted to the processing of sensory information from different parts of the body. In mice, the altered proportions of the head and whisker pad with respect to other body regions mirrors the extent of innervation from these areas. Similarly, in humans, the cortical somatosensory representation is enlarged for those regions, such as the hands and the lips, that are densely innervated by sensory fibers. Conversely, the structure of supragranular layers 2 and 3 markedly differs between the mouse (A) and human (B) somatosensory cortex. See text for references.

Somatosensory Abnormalities in Mouse Models of Autism-Risk Genes

The scope of this review is to focus on sensory abnormalities in genetic mouse models of ASD (that is, mice bearing mutations in ASD-relevant genes). Alternative, non-genetic models such as maternal immune activation and valproate exposure during pregnancy have revealed to be valuable tools to study ASD-like phenotypes in rodents (175, 176). These studies contributed to the neurobiological investigation of sensory abnormalities in ASD (177, 178), but will not be reported in detail in this review.

ASDs are generally thought to be caused by defective brain development, and most studies traditionally focused solely on brain alterations. However, emerging evidences from mouse studies suggest that at least some aspects of the disorder are linked to defects in the peripheral nervous system that communicates sensory information to the brain. Table 1 summarizes the most relevant mouse models of autism-risk genes, described in this section, which display somatosensory system defects at central or peripheral level. For an extensive summary ASD-relevant mouse strains showing somatosensory deficits, the reader is referred to the AutDB database (http://autism.mindspec.org/autdb). A brilliant example of central sensory processing defects comes from the work of Michaelson and colleagues (179). Starting from the finding of touch-related sensory processing defects (i.e., blunted responses to painful touch-related stimuli and/or tactile seeking behavior as well as tactile aversive behaviors) in SYNGAP1 heterozygosity in humans, they found that Syngap1 heterozygosity causes touch-related deficits in cortical circuit activation in mice, namely a reduced whisker related activation of receptive fields in the primary somatosensory cortex. Moreover, alteration of whisker evoked activation was found to be whisker-dependent. Particularly, neurons in the layer 2/3 of the somatosensory cortex were less active in heterozygous mice compared to WT in live calcium imaging experiments. Interestingly, these deficits in touch-related cortical circuits were associated with reduced whisker-evoked synaptic potentials in layer 2/3 and anatomical irregularity of layer 4 neurons. This is interesting because neurons of layer 2/3 of the somatosensory cortex integrate bottom-up sensory signals originating in the periphery with information arriving from higher cortical areas (186), whereas neurons in layer 4 receive the bulk of sensory-related information arriving from subcortical areas (187). So, ideally these defects could represent the neurological basis of tactile behavior abnormalities. Indeed, the authors found that Syngap1 heterozygous mice were unable to discriminate among objects that differ for the texture in the NORT test. Thus, a possible correlation among circuitry dysfunctions and tactile behavior deficits could be a matter of fact.

Table 1.

Somatosensory deficits in mouse models for autism-risk genes.

Mouse
model
Behavioral test Measure Phenotype Age at testing Sex Reference
Syngap1+/- Tactile novel object recognition test (NORT) Time spent exploring the novel object Increased in HET 6-8 weeks M/F 179
Go/no go task involving whisker deflection Correct answers reporting whisker deflection Decreased in HET
Fmr1-/- Tactile defensiveness assay in head-restrained mice Withdrawal/habituation to whisker stimulation Increased in KO PD 14-16 and 35-41 M/F 180
Shank2-/- Electronic von Frey Apparatus Mechanical withdrawal threshold Increased in KO >2 months n.s. 181
Hot plate test Latency to first reaction Increased in KO
Von Frey filaments for allodynia after neuropathic or inflammatory pain Positive response (licking, biting, withdrawal) Decreased in KO
Cntnap2-/- Calibrated von Frey hairs Mechanical withdrawal threshold Decreased in KO 8-16 weeks M/F 182
Hot plate test Latency to withdrawal Decreased in KO (53°C)
Pain sensitivity to capsaicin and formalin Duration of response (licking, biting, paw lifting) Increased in KO
Mecp2 KO,
Skank3B HET, and
Fmr1 KO
Texture specific novel object recognition Preference for the novel object Decreased in all models 6-8 weeks M/F 183
Mecp2 KO,
Skank3B HET, and
Fmr1 KO
Tactile prepulse inhibition assay
(T-PPI)
Air puff response Increased in all models 6-8 weeks M/F 183
Mecp2 cKO in DRG Texture specific novel object recognition Preference for the novel object Decreased in cKO 6-8 weeks M/F 183
T-PPI Air puff response Increased in cKO
Gabrb3 HET and
Gabrb3 cKO in DRG
Texture specific novel object recognition Preference for the novel object Decreased in both models 6-8 weeks M/F 183
T-PPI Air puff response Decreased in both models
Shank3B HET and
Shank3B cKO in DRG
T-PPI Air puff response Increased in both models 6-8 weeks M/F 184
Texture specific novel object recognition Preference for the novel object Decreased in Shank3B HET
En2-/- Whisker nuisance test Scoring of avoidance behaviors Increased in KO 3-6 months M/F 185

cKO, conditional knockout; DRG, dorsal root ganglia; F, female; HET, heterozygous; KO, knockout; M, male; NORT, tactile novel object recognition test; n.s., not specified; PD, postnatal day; T-PPI, tactile prepulse inhibition assay.

This possibility is supported by the research carried out by He and colleagues on the Fmr1 knockout mouse model of ASD (180). Fragile X syndrome (FXS), in which transcriptional silencing of the Fmr1 gene leads to loss of the fragile X mental retardation protein (FMRP), represents one of the most common single-gene cause of autism [from 1% to 6% of cases; (188)], and the vast majority of FXS individuals show tactile impairments (189). The Fmr1 knockout mouse model of FXS exhibits behavioral deficits analogous to human symptoms and, as reported by He and colleagues, also shows tactile defensiveness measured as avoidance motor response in a whisker stimulation test in both juvenile (P14-P16) and adult (P35-41) mice. Moreover, the authors reported that in young mice only a reduced fraction of neurons of layer 2/3 of the barrel field were responsive to whisker stimulation in a time-locked manner and showed impaired adaptation to repeated whisker stimulation, suggesting that this could represent the explanation for the observed behavioral over-reactivity (180).

Another study focused on behavioral aspects of sensory processing, showing reduced nociception and chronic pain in Shank2-/- mice (181), as an extension of tactile hyposensitivity found in ASD individuals. These authors reported basal tactile sensitivity impairment in Shank2-/- mice as compared to WT, namely a higher basal mechanical threshold (the force applied when the mouse withdraws its paw) by using an electronic Von Frey apparatus (used to assess withdrawal responses in rodents). Moreover, the authors found a reduced sensitivity in Shank2-/- mice to chronic neuropathic pain (i.e., induced by nerve ligation) as well as inflammatory pain (i.e. induced by antigens injection) suggesting that these alterations could be due to defect both at the brain level than at peripheral level. Indeed, peripheral synaptic dysfunctions in the spinal cord, as well as central somatosensory cortex defects could explain these impaired responses in Shank2-/- mice.

Recent studies indicate that peripheral alterations of tactile sensitivity in mouse models of autism-risk genes might contribute to social and sensory behavior defects relevant for ASD. One example comes from the work of Dawes and colleagues on the Cntnap2 -/- mouse (182). They found that loss of Cntnap2 resulted in pain related hypersensitivity (as tested through the Von Frey apparatus and the pinprick application) in mice. Since Cntnap2 was found to be expressed in dorsal root ganglion neurons (DRG), the authors measured primary sensory neuron activity in vivo through calcium imaging and in vitro through patch-clamp technique to assess if Cntnap2 could impact neuronal excitability. They showed that DRG neurons were significantly hyper-responsive to sensory stimulation showing larger increase in intracellular calcium concentration and significantly lower rheobases (defined as the smallest injected current that elicit an action potential) compared to WT. Moreover, they found from in vivo extracellular recording of DRG neurons that loss of Cntnap2 leads to dorsal horn neuron hyper excitability, in line with the behavioral assays.

In line with these findings, Orefice and colleagues showed that mice harboring mutations in Mecp2, Gabrb3, Shank3, and Fmr1 genes exhibit aberrant tactile sensitivity, as detected by abnormal behavioral responses to skin or whisker stimulation (183). When compared with control wild-type littermates, all these mutant mice failed to distinguish between smooth and rough-textured objects in the texture novel object recognition test (NORT), indicating impairments in skin-based texture discrimination. In addition, this study tested sensorimotor gating and skin sensitivity using the tactile prepulse inhibition test (PPI), which consists in delivering puffed air onto the back of mice and evaluating whether this prepulse could inhibit a subsequent startle response to a loud stimulus. Interestingly, all mutant mice tested showed an enhanced response compared to controls. Further testing additionally demonstrated that this exaggerated response was elicited by air puff alone, suggesting an abnormal hypersensitivity to tactile stimulation (183). In order to explore the neuronal basis of this tactile deficit in Mecp2 mutant mice, the authors deleted Mecp2 in different body areas, namely from forebrain excitatory neurons, from the neurons caudal to cervical level 2 (including the spinal cord and the peripheral sensory system), and from the sensory ganglia (including trigeminal ganglia). Sensory testing through NORT and air PPI revealed that somatosensory deletion of Mecp2 alone leads to aberrant tactile sensitivity. The authors then tested the hypothesis that GABA imbalance could have a role in impaired tactile sensitivity in Mecp2 and Gabrb3 mutant mice: deletion of Mecp2 or Gabrb3 in peripheral somatosensory (dorsal root ganglia, DRG) neurons caused mechanosensory dysfunction through loss of GABAA receptor-mediated presynaptic inhibition of inputs to the CNS (183). More recently, using a similar approach, the same authors found that acute treatment with GABAA receptor agonist selectively acting on mechanosensory neurons reduced tactile over-reactivity in six different ASD mouse models, both genetic and environmental (184). Moreover, chronic treatment of two genetic mouse lines, namely Mecp2 and Shank3 mutants, improved multiple ASD-associated behavioral phenotypes such as tactile over-reactivity, anxiety-like behaviors and social impairments. These results strongly support the hypothesis that peripheral somatosensory circuit dysfunctions could contribute to social deficits in ASD.

The idea of GABA imbalance in explaining the somatosensory defects reported in mouse models of ASD (see above, Studying the Neurobiological Basis of ASD Through Mouse Models) comes from the studies on the Gabrb3 gene, which encodes one subunit of GABA receptors on postsynaptic neurons and is associated with ASD (102). Mice heterozygous for this gene show a reduced startle response. In addition, an increased tactile sensitivity and a reduced sensorimotor processing were reported for Gabrb3 heterozygous male mice (190), and a reduced expression of Gabrb3 was found in Mecp2 deficient mice (191). A similar approach was used by our laboratory in describing the somatosensory defects of En2 mutant mice (185). Genetic studies (192194) and expression analyses on post-mortem brain tissues (195197) indicated that deregulated expression of the human EN2 gene is linked to ASD. Accordingly, En2-/- mice are considered a reliable model for investigating the neurodevelopmental basis of ASD. Indeed, En2-/- mice show ASD-like behaviors (198), and a lower expression of Fmr1 (199) accompanied by anatomical defects common to Fmr1 knockout mice (200). We reported that En2-/- mice have a significantly reduced synchronization in somatosensory-auditory/associative cortices and dorsal thalamus, suggesting the presence of aberrant somatosensory processing in these mutants. Indeed, when tested in the whisker nuisance test (201, 202) En2-/- mice showed hyper-responsiveness to repetitive whisker stimulation. In line with our findings of primary somatosensory cortex functional hypo connectivity, sensory hyper-responsivity in En2-/- mice was accompanied by a reduced activation of primary somatosensory cortex showed by a decreased c-Fos immunoreactivity in layer IV. Interestingly, whisker stimulation under anesthesia also resulted in reduced c-fos mRNA expression in the En2-/- mice primary somatosensory cortex, corroborating the data obtained following whisker stimulation in freely moving animals. Our hypothesis is that this disruption of sensory processing in En2-/- mice is likely due to impaired function of GABAergic signaling, since En2-/- mice present a reduced number of GABAergic interneurons in the hippocampus and somatosensory cortex (203). In addition, altered electrophysiological and behavioral markers of sensory processing can be rescued by pharmacologically enhancing GABAergic signaling in ASD mouse models (98). Further efforts are needed to reveal the anatomical networks by which GABAergic deficits impact somatosensory processing in mice models of ASD. Our current work focuses on exploring the potential somatosensory defects in different sensory areas such as the trigeminal ganglion, the thalamus and the somatosensory cortex trying to extend these findings to other mouse models of ASD.

Together, these findings reinforce the need of studying sensory features of ASD in mouse models and suggest that tactile impairment in mice, akin to human ASD tactile abnormalities, could be explained through sensory processing defects in the peripheral and central nervous system.

Conclusions

In this review, we have discussed evidences of sensory impairments found in ASD. Abnormal sensory processing in autism represents a common feature and is recognized as a diagnostic criterion. It encompasses many aspects of all sensory systems, leading to both central and peripheral defects. Mouse models of autism-risk genes recapitulate sensory impairments found in autistic individuals and represent a valuable tool to study the cellular and molecular mechanism underlying sensory behaviors. We have addressed the organization of mouse somatosensory system to introduce the most recent findings on tactile sensitivity in genetic mouse models of ASD as well as studies on aberrant sensory processing in somatosensory and other sensory domains. Further efforts are needed to effectively link the sensory abnormalities and social features of ASD to the intrinsic multifaceted nature of sensory dysfunctions in ASD.

Ethics Statement

Experiments performed in our laboratory and reported in this review were approved by the University of Trento Animal Welfare Committee and Italian Ministry of Health.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed to draft the manuscript. LB wrote the manuscript. GP and YB edited the manuscript. GP and YB provided funding.

Funding

GP and YB are currently supported by the University of Trento 2018–2020 Strategic Project “Trentino Autism Initiative – TRAIN”.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank the technical and administrative staff of CIMeC and CIBIO for the excellent assistance and Simona Correale for her precious help with figure preparation.

References

  • 1. American Psychiatric A. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association; (2013). 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Marco EJ, Hinkley LB, Hill SS, Nagarajan SS. Sensory processing in autism: a review of neurophysiologic findings. Pediatr Res (2011) 69:48R–54R. 10.1203/PDR.0b013e3182130c54 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Kern JK, Garver CR, Carmody T, Andrews AA, Mehta JA, Trivedi MH. Examining sensory modulation in individuals with autism as compared to community controls. Res In Autism Spectr Disord (2008) 2:85–94. 10.1016/j.rasd.2007.03.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Crane L, Goddard L, Pring L. Sensory processing in adults with autism spectrum disorders. Autism (2009) 13:215–28. 10.1177/1362361309103794 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Cheung PP, Siu AM. A comparison of patterns of sensory processing in children with and without developmental disabilities. Res Dev Disabil (2009) 30:1468–80. 10.1016/j.ridd.2009.07.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Kanner L. Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Acta Paedopsychiatr (1968) 35:100–36. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Bergman P, Escalona SK. Unusual sensitivities in very young children. Psychoanal Study Child (2001) 3(1):333–352. 10.1080/00797308.1947.11823091 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Eveloff HH. The autistic child. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1960) 3:66–81. 10.1001/archpsyc.1960.01710010068010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Wing L. The handicaps of autistic children–a comparative study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (1969) 10:1–40. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1969.tb02066.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. American Psychiatric A. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-3. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Association; (1980). [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Ornitz EM. Disorders of perception common to early infantile autism and schizophrenia. Compr Psychiatry (1969) 10:259–74. 10.1016/0010-440X(69)90002-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Ornitz EM, Guthrie D, Farley AH. The early development of autistic children. J Autism Child Schizophr (1977) 7:207–29. 10.1007/BF01538999 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Richer J. The partial noncommunication of culture to autistic children—An application of human ethology. In: Autism (New York: Plenum; ) (1978). p. 47–61. 10.1007/978-1-4684-0787-7_3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Rutter M. Language disorder and infantile autism. In: Schopler E, editor. Autism: A Reappraisal of Concepts and Treatment. Boston, MA: Springer US; (1978). p. 85–104. 10.1007/978-1-4684-0787-7_6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Ayres AJ, Robbins J. Sensory integration and the child. Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services; (1979). [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Miller LJ, Anzalone ME, Lane SJ, Cermak SA, Osten ET. Concept evolution in sensory integration: a proposed nosology for diagnosis. Am J Occup Ther (2007) 61:135–40. 10.5014/ajot.61.2.135 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Rutter M. Cognitive deficits in the pathogenesis of autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (1983) 24:513–31. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1983.tb00129.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Baranek GT, Watson LR, Boyd BA, Poe MD, David FJ, Mcguire L. Hyporesponsiveness to social and nonsocial sensory stimuli in children with autism, children with developmental delays, and typically developing children. Dev Psychopathol (2013) 25:307–20. 10.1017/S0954579412001071 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Estes A, Zwaigenbaum L, Gu H, St John T, Paterson S, Elison JT, et al. Behavioral, cognitive, and adaptive development in infants with autism spectrum disorder in the first 2 years of life. J Neurodev Disord (2015) 7:24. 10.1186/s11689-015-9117-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Turner-Brown LM, Baranek GT, Reznick JS, Watson LR, Crais ER. The first year inventory: a longitudinal follow-up of 12-month-old to 3-year-old children. Autism (2013) 17:527–40. 10.1177/1362361312439633 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Robertson CE, Baron-Cohen S. Sensory perception in autism. Nat Rev Neurosci (2017) 18:671–84. 10.1038/nrn.2017.112 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Galle SA, Courchesne V, Mottron L, Frasnelli J. Olfaction in the autism spectrum. Perception (2013) 42:341–55. 10.1068/p7337 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Rozenkrantz L, Zachor D, Heller I, Plotkin A, Weissbrod A, Snitz K, et al. A mechanistic link between olfaction and autism spectrum disorder. Curr Biol (2015) 25:1904–10. 10.1016/j.cub.2015.05.048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Tavassoli T, Baron-Cohen S. Taste identification in adults with autism spectrum conditions. J Autism Dev Disord (2012) 42:1419–24. 10.1007/s10803-011-1377-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Bonnel A, Mottron L, Peretz I, Trudel M, Gallun E, Bonnel AM. Enhanced pitch sensitivity in individuals with autism: a signal detection analysis. J Cognit Neurosci (2003) 15:226–35. 10.1162/089892903321208169 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Simmons DR, Robertson AE, Mckay LS, Toal E, Mcaleer P, Pollick FE. Vision in autism spectrum disorders. Vision Res (2009) 49:2705–39. 10.1016/j.visres.2009.08.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Marco EJ, Khatibi K, Hill SS, Siegel B, Arroyo MS, Dowling AF, et al. Children with autism show reduced somatosensory response: an MEG study. Autism Res (2012) 5:340–51. 10.1002/aur.1247 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Puts NA, Wodka EL, Tommerdahl M, Mostofsky SH, Edden RA. Impaired tactile processing in children with autism spectrum disorder. J Neurophysiol (2014) 111:1803–11. 10.1152/jn.00890.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Rogers SJ, Hepburn S, Wehner E. Parent reports of sensory symptoms in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord (2003. a) 33:631–42. 10.1023/B:JADD.0000006000.38991.a7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Leekam SR, Nieto C, Libby SJ, Wing L, Gould J. Describing the sensory abnormalities of children and adults with autism. J Autism Dev Disord (2007) 37:894–910. 10.1007/s10803-006-0218-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Bennetto L, Kuschner ES, Hyman SL. Olfaction and taste processing in autism. Biol Psychiatry (2007) 62:1015–21. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.04.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Brewer WJ, Brereton A, Tonge BJ. Dissociation of age and ability on a visual analogue of the University of Pennsylvania smell identification test in children with autism. Res Autism Spectr Disord (2008) 2:612–20. 10.1016/j.rasd.2008.01.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. May T, Brewer WJ, Rinehart NJ, Enticott PG, Brereton AV, Tonge BJ. Differential olfactory identification in children with autism and Asperger’s disorder: a comparative and longitudinal study. J Autism Dev Disord (2011) 41:837–47. 10.1007/s10803-010-1101-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Schreck KA, Williams K, Smith AF. A comparison of eating behaviors between children with and without autism. J Autism Dev Disord (2004) 34:433–8. 10.1023/B:JADD.0000037419.78531.86 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Bandini LG, Anderson SE, Curtin C, Cermak S, Evans EW, Scampini R, et al. Food selectivity in children with autism spectrum disorders and typically developing children. J Pediatr (2010) 157:259–64. 10.1016/j.jpeds.2010.02.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Barker RA, Barasi S, Neal MJ. Neuroscience at a Glance. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; (2003). [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Kinomura S, Kawashima R, Yamada K, Ono S, Itoh M, Yoshioka S, et al. Functional anatomy of taste perception in the human brain studied with positron emission tomography. Brain Res (1994) 659:263–6. 10.1016/0006-8993(94)90890-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Tsatsanis KD, Rourke BP, Klin A, Volkmar FR, Cicchetti D, Schultz RT. Reduced thalamic volume in high-functioning individuals with autism. Biol Psychiatry (2003) 53:121–9. 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01530-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Kwakye LD, Foss-Feig JH, Cascio CJ, Stone WL, Wallace MT. Altered auditory and multisensory temporal processing in autism spectrum disorders. Front Integr Neurosci (2011) 4:129. 10.3389/fnint.2010.00129 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Roberts TP, Khan SY, Rey M, Monroe JF, Cannon K, Blaskey L, et al. MEG detection of delayed auditory evoked responses in autism spectrum disorders: towards an imaging biomarker for autism. Autism Res (2010) 3:8–18. 10.1002/aur.111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Edgar JC, Khan SY, Blaskey L, Chow VY, Rey M, Gaetz W, et al. Neuromagnetic oscillations predict evoked-response latency delays and core language deficits in autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord (2015) 45:395–405. 10.1007/s10803-013-1904-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Roberts TP, Cannon KM, Tavabi K, Blaskey L, Khan SY, Monroe JF, et al. Auditory magnetic mismatch field latency: a biomarker for language impairment in autism. Biol Psychiatry (2011) 70:263–9. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.01.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Brandwein AB, Foxe JJ, Butler JS, Frey HP, Bates JC, Shulman LH, et al. Neurophysiological indices of atypical auditory processing and multisensory integration are associated with symptom severity in autism. J Autism Dev Disord (2015) 45:230–44. 10.1007/s10803-014-2212-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Samson F, Mottron L, Jemel B, Belin P, Ciocca V. Can spectro-temporal complexity explain the autistic pattern of performance on auditory tasks? J Autism Dev Disord (2006) 36:65–76. 10.1007/s10803-005-0043-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Mongillo EA, Irwin JR, Whalen DH, Klaiman C, Carter AS, Schultz RT. Audiovisual processing in children with and without autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:1349–58. 10.1007/s10803-007-0521-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Ritvo ER, Creel D, Realmuto G, Crandall AS, Freeman BJ, Bateman JB, et al. Electroretinograms in autism: a pilot study of b-wave amplitudes. Am J Psychiatry (1988) 145:229–32. 10.1176/ajp.145.2.229 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Realmuto G, Purple R, Knobloch W, Ritvo E. Electroretinograms (ERGs) in four autistic probands and six first-degree relatives. Can J Psychiatry (1989) 34:435–9. 10.1177/070674378903400513 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Lavoie J, Maziade M, Hebert M. The brain through the retina: the flash electroretinogram as a tool to investigate psychiatric disorders. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2014) 48:129–34. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.09.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Constable PA, Gaigg SB, Bowler DM, Jagle H, Thompson DA. Full-field electroretinogram in autism spectrum disorder. Doc Ophthalmol (2016) 132:83–99. 10.1007/s10633-016-9529-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Vlamings PHJM, Jonkman LM, Van Daalen E, Van Der Gaag RJ, Kemner C. Basic abnormalities in visual processing affect face processing at an early age in autism spectrum disorder. Biol Psychiatry (2010) 68:1107–1113. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.06.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Dakin S, Frith U. Vagaries of visual perception. Autism Neuron (2005) 48:497–507. 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.10.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Happé F, Frith U. Detail-focused cognitive style in Autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord (2006) 36:5–25. 10.1007/s10803-005-0039-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Mottron L, Dawson M, Soulieres I, Hubert B, Burack J. Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: an update, and eight principles of autistic perception. J Autism Dev Disord (2006) 36:27–43. 10.1007/s10803-005-0040-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Behrmann M, Avidan G, Leonard GL, Luna B, Humphreys K, Minshew N. Configural processing in autism and its relationship to face processing. Neuropsychologia (2006) 44:110–129. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.04.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Shah A, Frith U. An islet of ability in autistic children: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (1983) 24:613–20. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1983.tb00137.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Plaisted K, O’riordan M, Baron-Cohen S. Enhanced visual search for a conjunctive target in autism: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (1998) 39:777–83. 10.1017/S0021963098002613 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. O’riordan M, Passetti F. Discrimination in autism within different sensory modalities. J Autism Dev Disord (2006) 36:665–75. 10.1007/s10803-006-0106-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Joseph RM, Keehn B, Connolly C, Wolfe JM, Horowitz TS. Why is visual search superior in autism spectrum disorder? Dev Sci (2009) 12:1083–96. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00855.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Keita L, Mottron L, Bertone A. Far visual acuity is unremarkable in autism: do we need to focus on crowding? Autism Res (2010) 3:333–41. 10.1002/aur.164 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. O’riordan MA, Plaisted KC, Driver J, Baron-Cohen S. Superior visual search in autism. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform (2001) 27:719–30. 10.1037/0096-1523.27.3.719 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Wang S, Jiang M, Duchesne XM, Laugeson EA, Kennedy DP, Adolphs R, et al. Atypical visual saliency in autism spectrum disorder quantified through model-based eye tracking. Neuron (2015) 88:604–16. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Tavassoli T, Latham K, Bach M, Dakin SC, Baron-Cohen S. Psychophysical measures of visual acuity in autism spectrum conditions. Vision Res (2011) 51:1778–80. 10.1016/j.visres.2011.06.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. De Jonge MV, Kemner C, De Haan EH, Coppens JE, Van Den Berg TJ, Van Engeland H. Visual information processing in high-functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders and their parents. Neuropsychology (2007) 21:65–73. 10.1037/0894-4105.21.1.65 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Koh HC, Milne E, Dobkins K. Spatial contrast sensitivity in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord (2010) 40:978–87. 10.1007/s10803-010-0953-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Grubb MA, Behrmann M, Egan R, Minshew NJ, Carrasco M, Heeger DJ. Endogenous spatial attention: evidence for intact functioning in adults with autism. Autism Res (2013) 6:108–18. 10.1002/aur.1269 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Freyberg J, Robertson CE, Baron-Cohen S. Typical magnitude and spatial extent of crowding in autism. J Vis (2016) 16:17. 10.1167/16.5.17 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Spencer J, O’brien J, Riggs K, Braddick O, Atkinson J, Wattam-Bell J. Motion processing in autism: evidence for a dorsal stream deficiency. Neuroreport (2000) 11:2765–7. 10.1097/00001756-200008210-00031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Blake R, Turner LM, Smoski MJ, Pozdol SL, Stone WL. Visual recognition of biological motion is impaired in children with autism. Psychol Sci (2003) 14:151–7. 10.1111/1467-9280.01434 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Robertson CE, Martin A, Baker CI, Baron-Cohen S. Atypical integration of motion signals in autism spectrum conditions. PloS One (2012) 7:e48173. 10.1371/journal.pone.0048173 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Robertson CE, Thomas C, Kravitz DJ, Wallace GL, Baron-Cohen S, Martin A, et al. Global motion perception deficits in autism are reflected as early as primary visual cortex. Brain (2014) 137:2588–99. 10.1093/brain/awu189 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71. Koldewyn K, Whitney D, Rivera SM. The psychophysics of visual motion and global form processing in autism. Brain (2010) 133:599–610. 10.1093/brain/awp272 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72. Chen Y, Norton DJ, Mcbain R, Gold J, Frazier JA, Coyle JT. Enhanced local processing of dynamic visual information in autism: evidence from speed discrimination. Neuropsychologia (2012) 50:733–9. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.01.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73. Manning C, Tibber MS, Charman T, Dakin SC, Pellicano E. Enhanced integration of motion information in children with autism. J Neurosci (2015) 35:6979–86. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4645-14.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Grandin T. An inside View of Autism. In: Schopler E, Mesibov GB, editors. High-Functioning Individuals with Autism. Springers: Boston, MA: (1992). 10.1007/978-1-4899-2456-8_6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Baranek GT, Foster LG, Berkson G. Tactile defensiveness and stereotyped behaviors. Am J Occup Ther (1997) 51:91–5. 10.5014/ajot.51.2.91 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76. Baranek GT, Berkson G. Tactile defensiveness in children with developmental disabilities: responsiveness and habituation. J Autism Dev Disord (1994) 24:457–71. 10.1007/BF02172128 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77. Mikkelsen M, Wodka EL, Mostofsky SH, Puts NAJ. Autism spectrum disorder in the scope of tactile processing. Dev Cogn Neurosci (2018) 29:140–50. 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.12.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78. Rogers SJ, Hepburn S, Wehner E. Parent reports of sensory symptoms in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord (2003. b) 33:631–42. 10.1023/B:JADD.0000006000.38991.a7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79. Ben-Sasson A, Cermak SA, Orsmond GI, Tager-Flusberg H, Carter AS, Kadlec MB, et al. Extreme sensory modulation behaviors in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Am J Occup Ther (2007) 61:584–92. 10.5014/ajot.61.5.584 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80. Foss-Feig JH, Heacock JL, Cascio CJ. Tactile responsiveness patterns and their association with core features in autism spectrum disorders. Res Autism Spectr Disord (2012) 6:337–44. 10.1016/j.rasd.2011.06.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81. Cascio CJ, Lorenzi J, Baranek GT. Self-reported pleasantness ratings and examiner-coded defensiveness in response to touch in children with ASD: effects of stimulus material and bodily location. J Autism Dev Disord (2016) 46:1528–37. 10.1007/s10803-013-1961-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82. Russell G, Mandy W, Elliott D, White R, Pittwood T, Ford T. Selection bias on intellectual ability in autism research: a cross-sectional review and meta-analysis. Mol Autism (2019) 10:9. 10.1186/s13229-019-0260-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83. Stedman A, Taylor B, Erard M, Peura C, Siegel M. Are children severely affected by autism spectrum disorder underrepresented in treatment studies? An Analysis of the Literature. J Autism Dev Disord (2019) 49:1378–90. 10.1007/s10803-018-3844-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84. Tavassoli T, Hoekstra RA, Baron-Cohen S. The sensory perception quotient (SPQ): development and validation of a new sensory questionnaire for adults with and without autism. Mol Autism (2014) 5:29. 10.1186/2040-2392-5-29 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85. Blakemore S-J, Tavassoli T, Calò S, Thomas RM, Catmur C, Frith U, et al. Tactile sensitivity in Asperger syndrome. Brain Cogn (2006) 61:5–13. 10.1016/j.bandc.2005.12.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86. Guclu B, Tanidir C, Mukaddes NM, Unal F. Tactile sensitivity of normal and autistic children. Somatosens Mot Res (2007) 24:21–33. 10.1080/08990220601179418 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87. Cascio C, Mcglone F, Folger S, Tannan V, Baranek G, Pelphrey KA, et al. Tactile perception in adults with autism: a multidimensional psychophysical study. J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:127–37. 10.1007/s10803-007-0370-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88. Coskun MA, Varghese L, Reddoch S, Castillo EM, Pearson DA, Loveland KA, et al. How somatic cortical maps differ in autistic and typical brains. NeuroReport (2009. a) 20:175–9. 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32831f47d1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89. Coskun MA, Varghese L, Reddoch S, Castillo EM, Pearson DA, Loveland KA, et al. Increased response variability in autistic brains? NeuroReport (2009. b) 20:1543–8. 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32833246b5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90. Dinstein I, Heeger D, Lorenzi L, Minshew N, Malach R, Behrmann M. Unreliable evoked responses in autism. Neuron (2012) 75:981–91. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.07.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91. Haigh SM, Gupta A, Barb SM, Glass SAF, Minshew NJ, Dinstein I, et al. Differential sensory fMRI signatures in autism and schizophrenia: analysis of amplitude and trial-to-trial variability. Schizophr Res (2016) 175:12–9. 10.1016/j.schres.2016.03.036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92. Hussman JP. Suppressed GABAergic inhibition as a common factor in suspected etiologies of autism. J Autism Dev Disord (2001) 31:247–8. 10.1023/a:1010715619091 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93. Rubenstein JL, Merzenich MM. Model of autism: increased ratio of excitation/inhibition in key neural systems. Genes Brain Behav (2003) 2:255–67. 10.1034/j.1601-183X.2003.00037.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94. Dykes RW, Landry P, Metherate R, Hicks TP. Functional role of GABA in cat primary somatosensory cortex: shaping receptive fields of cortical neurons. J Neurophysiol (1984) 52:1066–93. 10.1152/jn.1984.52.6.1066 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95. Juliano SL, Whitsel BL, Tommerdahl M, Cheema SS. Determinants of patchy metabolic labeling in the somatosensory cortex of cats: a possible role for intrinsic inhibitory circuitry. J Neurosci (1989) 9:1–12. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-01-00001.1989 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96. Mccormick DA. GABA as an inhibitory neurotransmitter in human cerebral cortex. J Neurophysiol (1989) 62:1018–27. 10.1152/jn.1989.62.5.1018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97. Markram H, Toledo-Rodriguez M, Wang Y, Gupta A, Silberberg G, Wu C. Interneurons of the neocortical inhibitory system. Nat Rev Neurosci (2004) 5:793. 10.1038/nrn1519 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98. Bozzi Y, Provenzano G, Casarosa S. Neurobiological bases of autism-epilepsy comorbidity: a focus on excitation/inhibition imbalance. Eur J Neurosci (2018) 47:534–48. 10.1111/ejn.13595 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99. Leblanc JJ, Fagiolini M. Autism: a “critical period” disorder? Neural Plast (2011) 2011:921680. 10.1155/2011/921680 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100. Tavassoli T, Auyeung B, Murphy LC, Baron-Cohen S, Chakrabarti B. Variation in the autism candidate gene GABRB3 modulates tactile sensitivity in typically developing children. Mol Autism (2012) 3:6. 10.1186/2040-2392-3-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101. Delorey TM. GABRB3 gene deficient mice: a potential model of autism spectrum disorder. In: International Review of Neurobiology. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press; (2005). p. 359–82. 10.1016/S0074-7742(05)71015-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102. Abrahams BS, Geschwind DH. Advances in autism genetics: on the threshold of a new neurobiology. Nat Rev Genet (2008) 9:341–55. 10.1038/nrg2346 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103. Puts N.A.J., Wodka EL, Harris AD, Crocetti D, Tommerdahl M, Mostofsky SH. Reduced GABA and altered somatosensory function in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res (2017) 10:608–19. 10.1002/aur.1691 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104. Gliga T, Jones EJ, Bedford R, Charman T, Johnson MH. From early markers to neuro-developmental mechanisms of autism. Dev Rev (2014) 34:189–207. 10.1016/j.dr.2014.05.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105. Hilton CL, Harper JD, Kueker RH, Lang AR, Abbacchi AM, Todorov A, et al. Sensory responsiveness as a predictor of social severity in children with high functioning autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord (2010) 40:937–45. 10.1007/s10803-010-0944-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106. Barnett K. A theoretical construct of the concepts of touch as they relate to nursing. Nurs Res (1972) 21:102–10. 10.1097/00006199-197203000-00002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107. Hertenstein MJ, Verkamp JM, Kerestes AM, Holmes RM. The communicative functions of touch in humans, nonhuman primates, and rats: a review and synthesis of the empirical research. Genet Soc Gen Psychol Monogr (2006. b) 132:5–94. 10.3200/MONO.132.1.5-94 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108. Dunbar RIM. The social role of touch in humans and primates: behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms. Neurosci Biobehavioral Rev (2010) 34:260–8. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.07.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109. Bjornsdotter M, Gordon I, Pelphrey K, Olausson H, Kaiser M. Development of brain mechanisms for processing affective touch. Front In Behav Neurosci (2014) 8–24. 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110. Brauer J, Xiao Y, Poulain T, Friederici AD, Schirmer A. Frequency of maternal touch predicts resting activity and connectivity of the developing social brain. Cereb Cortex (2016) 26:3544–52. 10.1093/cercor/bhw137 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111. Morrison I, Loken LS, Olausson H. The skin as a social organ. Exp Brain Res (2010) 204:305–14. 10.1007/s00221-009-2007-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112. Mammen MA, Moore GA, Scaramella LV, Reiss D, Ganiban JM, Shaw DS, et al. Infant avoidance during a tactile task predicts autism spectrum behaviors in toddlerhood. Infant Ment Health J (2015) 36:575–87. 10.1002/imhj.21539 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113. Hertenstein MJ. Touch: its communicative functions in infancy. Hum Dev (2002) 45:70–94. 10.1159/000048154 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 114. Ghaziuddin M, Ghaziuddin N, Greden J. Depression in persons with autism: implications for research and clinical care. J Autism Dev Disord (2002) 32:299–306. 10.1023/A:1016330802348 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115. Kerns CM, Kendall PC. The presentation and classification of anxiety in autism spectrum disorder. Clin Psychol: Sci Pract (2012) 19:323–47. 10.1111/cpsp.12009 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 116. Teague SJ, Gray KM, Tonge BJ, Newman LK. Attachment in children with autism spectrum disorder: a systematic review. Res In Autism Spectr Disord (2017) 35:35–50. 10.1016/j.rasd.2016.12.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 117. Capps L, Sigman M, Mundy P. Attachment security in children with autism. Dev Psychopathol (1994) 6:249–61. 10.1017/S0954579400004569 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 118. Uvnäs-Moberg K. Oxytocin may mediate the benefits of positive social interaction and emotions. The purpose of this paper is to describe the neuroendocrine mechanisms of positive social interactions. Psychoneuroendocrinology (1998) 23:819–35. 10.1016/S0306-4530(98)00056-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119. Modahl C, Green LA, Fein D, Morris M, Waterhouse L, Feinstein C, et al. Plasma oxytocin levels in autistic children. Biol Psychiatry (1998) 43:270–7. 10.1016/S0006-3223(97)00439-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120. Ebstein RP, Israel S, Lerer E, Uzefovsky F, Shalev I, Gritsenko I, et al. Arginine vasopressin and oxytocin modulate human social behavior. Ann New York Acad Sci (2009) 1167:87–102. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04541.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121. Campbell DB, Datta D, Jones ST, Batey Lee E, Sutcliffe JS, Hammock EAD, et al. Association of oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene variants with multiple phenotype domains of autism spectrum disorder. J Neurodev Disord (2011) 3:101–12. 10.1007/s11689-010-9071-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122. Scheele D, Kendrick KM, Khouri C, Kretzer E, Schläpfer TE, Stoffel-Wagner B, et al. An oxytocin-induced facilitation of neural and emotional responses to social touch correlates inversely with autism traits. Neuropsychopharmacol: Off Publ Am Coll Neuropsychopharmacol (2014) 39:2078–85. 10.1038/npp.2014.78 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123. Brock JON, Brown CC, Boucher J, Rippon G. The temporal binding deficit hypothesis of autism. Dev Psychopathol (2002) 14:209–24. 10.1017/S0954579402002018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124. Stevenson RA, Zemtsov RK, Wallace MT. Individual differences in the multisensory temporal binding window predict susceptibility to audiovisual illusions. J Exp Psychol: Hum Perception Perform (2012) 38:1517–29. 10.1037/a0027339 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125. Wallace MT, Stevenson RA. The construct of the multisensory temporal binding window and its dysregulation in developmental disabilities. Neuropsychologia (2014) 64:105–23. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126. Markram H, Rinaldi T, Markram K. The intense world syndrome - an alternative hypothesis for autism. Front Neurosci (2007) 1:77–96. 10.3389/neuro.01.1.1.006.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127. Markram K, Markram H. The intense world theory – a unifying theory of the neurobiology of autism. Front Hum Neurosci (2010) 4. 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00224 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128. Knill DC, Pouget A. The bayesian brain: the role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation. Trends Neurosci (2004) 27:712–9. 10.1016/j.tins.2004.10.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129. Pellicano E, Burr D. When the world becomes ‘too real’: a Bayesian explanation of autistic perception. Trends Cogn Sci (2012) 16:504–10. 10.1016/j.tics.2012.08.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130. Mottron L, Burack JA. Enhanced perceptual functioning in the development of autism. In: The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers: Mahwah, NJ, US: (2001). p. 131–48. [Google Scholar]
  • 131. Green SA, Hernandez L, Tottenham N, Krasileva K, Bookheimer SY, Dapretto M. Neurobiology of sensory overresponsivity in youth with autism spectrum disorders. JAMA Psychiatry (2015) 72:778–86. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0737 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132. Hertenstein MJ, Verkamp JM, Kerestes AM, Holmes RM. The communicative functions of touch in humans, nonhuman primates, and rats: a review and synthesis of the empirical research. Genet Soc Gen Psychol Monogr (2006. a) 132:5–94. 10.3200/MONO.132.1.5-94 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133. Chen P, Hong W. Neural circuit mechanisms of social behavior. Neuron (2018) 98:16–30. 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.02.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134. Blakemore SJ. The social brain in adolescence. Nat Rev Neurosci (2008) 9:267–77. 10.1038/nrn2353 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135. Meyer-Lindenberg A, Tost H. Neural mechanisms of social risk for psychiatric disorders. Nat Neurosci (2012) 15:663–8. 10.1038/nn.3083 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136. Contreras JL, Agmo A. Sensory control of the male rat’s copulatory thrusting patterns. Behav Neural Biol (1993) 60:234–40. 10.1016/0163-1047(93)90447-P [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137. Champagne FA, Curley JP. How social experiences influence the brain. Curr Opin Neurobiol (2005) 15:704–9. 10.1016/j.conb.2005.10.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138. Bailey A, Le Couteur A, Gottesman I, Bolton P, Simonoff E, Yuzda E, et al. Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: evidence from a British twin study. Psychol Med (1995) 25:63–77. 10.1017/S0033291700028099 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139. Le Couteur A, Bailey A, Goode S, Pickles A, Robertson S, Gottesman I, et al. A broader phenotype of autism: the clinical spectrum in twins. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (1996) 37:785–801. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01475.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140. Huguet G, Ey E, Bourgeron T. The Genetic Landscapes of Autism Spectrum Disorders. Annu Rev Genomics Hum Genet (2013) 14:191–213. 10.1146/annurev-genom-091212-153431 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141. De La Torre-Ubieta L, Won H, Stein JL, Geschwind DH. Advancing the understanding of autism disease mechanisms through genetics. Nat Med (2016) 22:345. 10.1038/nm.4071 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 142. Durand CM, Betancur C, Boeckers TM, Bockmann J, Chaste P, Fauchereau F, et al. Mutations in the gene encoding the synaptic scaffolding protein SHANK3 are associated with autism spectrum disorders. Nat Genet (2007) 39:25–7. 10.1038/ng1933 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 143. Alarcon M, Abrahams BS, Stone JL, Duvall JA, Perederiy JV, Bomar JM, et al. Linkage, association, and gene-expression analyses identify CNTNAP2 as an autism-susceptibility gene. Am J Hum Genet (2008) 82:150–9. 10.1016/j.ajhg.2007.09.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 144. Arking DE, Cutler DJ, Brune CW, Teslovich TM, West K, Ikeda M, et al. A common genetic variant in the neurexin superfamily member CNTNAP2 increases familial risk of autism. Am J Hum Genet (2008) 82:160–4. 10.1016/j.ajhg.2007.09.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 145. Jamain S, Quach H, Betancur C, Rastam M, Colineaux C, Gillberg IC, et al. Mutations of the X-linked genes encoding neuroligins NLGN3 and NLGN4 are associated with autism. Nat Genet (2003) 34:27–9. 10.1038/ng1136 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 146. Lawson-Yuen A, Saldivar JS, Sommer S, Picker J. Familial deletion within NLGN4 associated with autism and Tourette syndrome. Eur J Hum Genet (2008) 16:614–8. 10.1038/sj.ejhg.5202006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 147. Kim HG, Kishikawa S, Higgins AW, Seong IS, Donovan DJ, Shen Y, et al. Disruption of neurexin 1 associated with autism spectrum disorder. Am J Hum Genet (2008) 82:199–207. 10.1016/j.ajhg.2007.09.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 148. Christian SL, Brune CW, Sudi J, Kumar RA, Liu S, Karamohamed S, et al. Novel submicroscopic chromosomal abnormalities detected in autism spectrum disorder. Biol Psychiatry (2008) 63:1111–7. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.01.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 149. Fernandez BA, Roberts W, Chung B, Weksberg R, Meyn S, Szatmari P, et al. Phenotypic spectrum associated with de novo and inherited deletions and duplications at 16p11.2 in individuals ascertained for diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder. J Med Genet (2010) 47:195–203. 10.1136/jmg.2009.069369 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 150. Glessner JT, Wang K, Cai G, Korvatska O, Kim CE, Wood S, et al. Autism genome-wide copy number variation reveals ubiquitin and neuronal genes. Nature (2009) 459:569–73. 10.1038/nature07953 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 151. Fernandez T, Morgan T, Davis N, Klin A, Morris A, Farhi A, et al. Disruption of contactin 4 (CNTN4) results in developmental delay and other features of 3p deletion syndrome. Am J Hum Genet (2004) 74:1286–93. 10.1086/421474 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 152. Crawley JN. Designing mouse behavioral tasks relevant to autistic-like behaviors. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev (2004) 10:248–58. 10.1002/mrdd.20039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 153. Silverman JL, Yang M, Lord C, Crawley JN. Behavioural phenotyping assays for mouse models of autism. Nat Rev Neurosci (2010) 11:490–502. 10.1038/nrn2851 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 154. Goel A, Cantu DA, Guilfoyle J, Chaudhari GR, Newadkar A, Todisco B, et al. Impaired perceptual learning in a mouse model of Fragile X syndrome is mediated by parvalbumin neuron dysfunction and is reversible. Nat Neurosci (2018) 21:1404–11. 10.1038/s41593-018-0231-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 155. Zhang XP, Messina I, D’antongiovanni A, Crò V, Provenzano F, Bozzi G, et al. Retinal defects in mice lacking the autism-associated gene Engrailed-2. In: Neuroscience, submitted (2019). 408:177–190. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2019.03.061 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 156. Cheng N, Khanbabaei M, Murari K, Rho JM. Disruption of visual circuit formation and refinement in a mouse model of autism. Autism Res (2017) 10:212–23. 10.1002/aur.1687 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 157. Siemann JK, Muller CL, Forsberg CG, Blakely RD, Veenstra-Vanderweele J, Wallace MT. An autism-associated serotonin transporter variant disrupts multisensory processing. Transl Psychiatry (2017) 7e1067. 10.1038/tp.2017.17 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 158. Paxinos G. Paxinos and Franklin’s the mouse brain in stereotaxic coordinates. 4th ed Franklin KBJ, editor. Boston: Amsterdam: (2013). [Google Scholar]
  • 159. Lee KJ, Woolsey TA. A proportional relationship between peripheral innervation density and cortical neuron number in the somatosensory system of the mouse. Brain Res (1975) 99:349–53. 10.1016/0006-8993(75)90035-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 160. Zucker E, Welker WI. Coding of somatic sensory input by vibrissae neurons in the rat’s trigeminal ganglion. Brain Res (1969) 12:138–56. 10.1016/0006-8993(69)90061-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 161. Ma PM. The barrelettes–architectonic vibrissal representations in the brainstem trigeminal complex of the mouse. I. Normal structural organization. J Comp Neurol (1991) 309:161–99. 10.1002/cne.903090202 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 162. Van Der Loos H. Barreloids in mouse somatosensory thalamus. Neurosci Lett (1976) 2:1–6. 10.1016/0304-3940(76)90036-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 163. Diamond ME, Armstrong-James M, Budway MJ, Ebner FF. Somatic sensory responses in the rostral sector of the posterior group (POm) and in the ventral posterior medial nucleus (VPM) of the rat thalamus: dependence on the barrel field cortex. J Comp Neurol (1992) 319:66–84. 10.1002/cne.903190108 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 164. Lavallee P, Deschenes M. Dendroarchitecture and lateral inhibition in thalamic barreloids. J Neurosci (2004) 24:6098–105. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0973-04.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 165. Woolsey TA, Van Der Loos H. The structural organization of layer IV in the somatosensory region (SI) of mouse cerebral cortex. The description of a cortical field composed of discrete cytoarchitectonic units. Brain Res (1970) 17:205–42. 10.1016/0006-8993(70)90079-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 166. Hill RS, Walsh CA. Molecular insights into human brain evolution. Nature (2005) 437:64–7. 10.1038/nature04103 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 167. Nicholls JG, Martin AR, Fuchs PA, Brown DA, Diamond ME, Weisblat DA. From Neuron to Brain. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer; (2012). [Google Scholar]
  • 168. Kaas JH. Somatosensory Cortex. In: Squire LR, editor. Encyclopedia of Neuroscience. Oxford: Academic Press: (2004). p. 73–7. 10.1016/B978-008045046-9.02028-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 169. Nelson RJ, Sur M, Felleman DJ, Kaas JH. Representations of the body surface in postcentral parietal cortex of Macaca fascicularis. J Comp Neurol (1980) 192:611–43. 10.1002/cne.901920402 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 170. Penfield W, Boldrey E. Somatic motor and sensory representation in the cerebral cortex of man as studied by electrical stimulation. Brain (1937) 60:389–443. 10.1093/brain/60.4.389 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 171. Lottem E, Azouz R. Mechanisms of tactile information transmission through whisker vibrations. J Neurosci (2009) 29:11686–97. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0705-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 172. Connor C, Johnson K. Neural coding of tactile texture: comparison of spatial and temporal mechanisms for roughness perception. J Neurosci (1992) 12:3414–26. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-09-03414.1992 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 173. Hollins M, Bensmaia SJ. The coding of roughness. Can J Exp Psychol (2007) 61:184–95. 10.1037/cjep2007020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 174. Jabaudon D. Fate and freedom in developing neocortical circuits. Nat Commun (2017) 8:16042. 10.1038/ncomms16042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 175. Schneider T, Przewlocki R. Behavioral alterations in rats prenatally exposed to valproic acid: animal model of autism. Neuropsychopharmacology (2005) 30:80–9. 10.1038/sj.npp.1300518 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 176. Lins BR, Marks WN, Zabder NK, Greba Q, Howland JG. Maternal immune activation during pregnancy alters the behavior profile of female offspring of sprague dawley rats. eNeuro (2019) 6:1–14. 10.1523/ENEURO.0437-18.2019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 177. Boksa P. Effects of prenatal infection on brain development and behavior: a review of findings from animal models. Brain Behav Immun (2010) 24:881–97. 10.1016/j.bbi.2010.03.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 178. Reynolds S, Millette A, Devine DP. Sensory and motor characterization in the postnatal valproate rat model of autism. Dev Neurosci (2012) 34:258–67. 10.1159/000336646 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 179. Michaelson SD, Ozkan ED, Aceti M, Maity S, Llamosas N, Weldon M, et al. SYNGAP1 heterozygosity disrupts sensory processing by reducing touch-related activity within somatosensory cortex circuits. Nat Neurosci (2018) 21:1–13. 10.1038/s41593-018-0268-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 180. He CX, Cantu DA, Mantri SS, Zeiger WA, Goel A, Portera-Cailliau C. Tactile defensiveness and impaired adaptation of neuronal activity in the Fmr1 knock-out mouse model of autism. J Neurosci (2017) 37:6475–87. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0651-17.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 181. Ko HG, Oh SB, Zhuo M, Kaang BK. Reduced acute nociception and chronic pain in Shank2-/- mice. Mol Pain (2016) 12:1–5. 10.1177/1744806916647056 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 182. Dawes JM, Weir GA, Middleton SJ, Patel R, Chisholm KI, Pettingill P, et al. Immune or genetic-mediated disruption of CASPR2 causes pain hypersensitivity due to enhanced primary afferent excitability. Neuron (2018) 97:806–822 e810. 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.01.033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 183. Orefice LL, Zimmerman AL, Chirila AM, Sleboda SJ, Head JP, Ginty DD. Peripheral mechanosensory neuron dysfunction underlies tactile and behavioral deficits in mouse models of ASDs. Cell (2016) 166:299–313. 10.1016/j.cell.2016.05.033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 184. Orefice LL, Mosko JR, Morency DT, Wells MF, Tasnim A, Mozeika SM, et al. Targeting peripheral somatosensory neurons to improve tactile-related phenotypes in ASD models. Cell (2019) 178:867–886.e824. 10.1016/j.cell.2019.07.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 185. Chelini G, Zerbi V, Cimino L, Grigoli A, Markicevic M, Libera F, et al. Aberrant somatosensory processing and connectivity in mice lacking Engrailed-2. J Neurosci (2019) 39:1525–38. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0612-18.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 186. Peron SP, Freeman J, Iyer V, Guo C, Svoboda K. A Cellular resolution map of barrel cortex activity during tactile behavior. Neuron (2015) 86:783–99. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 187. Feldmeyer D, Brecht M, Helmchen F, Petersen CCH, Poulet JFA, Staiger JF, et al. Barrel cortex function. Prog Neurobiol (2013) 103:3–27. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2012.11.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 188. Muhle R, Trentacoste SV, Rapin I. The Genetics of Autism. Pediatrics (2004) 113:e472–86. 10.1542/peds.113.5.e472 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 189. Butler MG, Mangrum T, Gupta R, Singh DN. A 15-item checklist for screening mentally retarded males for the fragile X syndrome. Clin Genet (1991) 39:347–54. 10.1111/j.1399-0004.1991.tb03041.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 190. Delorey TM, Sahbaie P, Hashemi E, Li W-W, Salehi A, Clark DJ. Somatosensory and sensorimotor consequences associated with the heterozygous disruption of the autism candidate gene, Gabrb3. Behav Brain Res (2011) 216:36–45. 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.06.032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 191. Samaco RC, Hogart A, Lasalle JM. Epigenetic overlap in autism-spectrum neurodevelopmental disorders: MECP2 deficiency causes reduced expression of UBE3A and GABRB3. Hum Mol Genet (2005) 14:483–92. 10.1093/hmg/ddi045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 192. Gharani N, Benayed R, Mancuso V, Brzustowicz LM, Millonig JH. Association of the homeobox transcription factor, ENGRAILED 2, 3, with autism spectrum disorder. Mol Psychiatry (2004) 9:474–84. 10.1038/sj.mp.4001498 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 193. Benayed R, Gharani N, Rossman I, Mancuso V, Lazar G, Kamdar S, et al. Support for the homeobox transcription factor gene ENGRAILED 2 as an autism spectrum disorder susceptibility locus. Am J Hum Genet (2005) 77:851–68. 10.1086/497705 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 194. Hnoonual A, Sripo T, Limprasert P. Whole-exome sequencing identifies a novel heterozygous missense variant of the EN2 gene in two unrelated patients with autism spectrum disorder. Psychiatr Genet (2016) 26:297–301. 10.1097/YPG.0000000000000153 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 195. James SJ, Shpyleva S, Melnyk S, Pavliv O, Pogribny IP. Complex epigenetic regulation of engrailed-2 (EN-2) homeobox gene in the autism cerebellum. Transl Psychiatry (2013) 3:e232. 10.1038/tp.2013.8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 196. Choi J, Ababon MR, Soliman M, Lin Y, Brzustowicz LM, Matteson PG, et al. Autism associated gene, engrailed2, and flanking gene levels are altered in post-mortem cerebellum. PloS One (2014) 9:e87208. 10.1371/journal.pone.0087208 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 197. James SJ, Shpyleva S, Melnyk S, Pavliv O, Pogribny IP. Elevated 5-hydroxymethylcytosine in the Engrailed-2 (EN-2) promoter is associated with increased gene expression and decreased MeCP2 binding in autism cerebellum. Transl Psychiatry (2014) 4:e460. 10.1038/tp.2014.87 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 198. Brielmaier J, Matteson PG, Silverman JL, Senerth JM, Kelly S, Millonig JH, et al. Autism-relevant social abnormalities and cognitive deficits in engrailed-2 knockout mice. PLoS One (2012) 7(7):e40914. 10.1371/journal.pone.0040914 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 199. Provenzano G, Sgado P, Genovesi S, Zunino G, Casarosa S, Bozzi Y. Hippocampal dysregulation of FMRP/mGluR5 signaling in engrailed-2 knockout mice: a model of autism spectrum disorders. Neuroreport (2015) 26:1101–5. 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000477 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 200. Ellegood J, Anagnostou E, Babineau BA, Crawley JN, Lin L, Genestine M, et al. Clustering autism: using neuroanatomical differences in 26 mouse models to gain insight into the heterogeneity. Mol Psychiatry (2015) 20:118–25. 10.1038/mp.2014.98 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 201. Mcnamara KC, Lisembee AM, Lifshitz J. The whisker nuisance task identifies a late-onset, persistent sensory sensitivity in diffuse brain-injured rats. J Neurotrauma (2010) 27:695–706. 10.1089/neu.2009.1237 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 202. Balasco L, Chelini G, Bozzi Y, Provenzano G. Whisker Nuisance Test: a valuable tool to assess tactile hypersensitivity in mice. Bio-protocol (2019) 9:e3331. 10.21769/BioProtoc.3331 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 203. Sgado P, Genovesi S, Kalinovsky A, Zunino G, Macchi F, Allegra M, et al. Loss of GABAergic neurons in the hippocampus and cerebral cortex of Engrailed-2 null mutant mice: implications for autism spectrum disorders. Exp Neurol (2013) 247:496–505. 10.1016/j.expneurol.2013.01.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES