Skip to main content
Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences logoLink to Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences
. 2014 Jan 31;24(2):158–165. doi: 10.1017/S2045796014000018

Retrospective and prospectively assessed childhood adversity in association with major depression, alcohol consumption and painful conditions

S B Patten 1,*, T C R Wilkes 1, J V A Williams 1, D H Lavorato 1, N el-Guebaly 1, D Schopflocher 1, C Wild 1, I Colman 1, A G M Bulloch 1
PMCID: PMC6998119  PMID: 24480045

Abstract

Background.

Considerable evidence now links childhood adversity to a variety of adult health problems. Unfortunately, almost all of these studies have relied upon retrospective assessment of childhood events, creating a vulnerability to bias. In this study, we sought to examine three associations using data sources that allowed for both prospective and retrospective assessment of childhood events.

Methods.

Methods. A 1994 national survey of children between the ages of 0 and 11 collected data from a ‘person most knowledgeable’ (usually the mother) about a child. It was possible to link data for n = 1977 of these respondents to data collected from the same people in a subsequent adult study. The latter survey included retrospective reports of childhood adversity. We examined three adult health outcomes in relation to prospectively and retrospectively assessed childhood adversity: major depressive episodes, excessive alcohol consumption and painful conditions.

Results.

Results. A strong association between childhood adversities (as assessed by both retrospective and prospective methods) and major depression was identified although the association with retrospective assessment was stronger. Weaker associations were found for painful conditions, but these did not depend on the method of assessment. Associations were not found for excessive alcohol consumption irrespective of the method of assessment.

Conclusions.

These findings help to allay concerns that associations between childhood adversities and health outcomes during adulthood are merely artefacts of recall bias. In this study, retrospective and prospective assessment strategies produced similar results.

Key words: Alcohol consumption, back pain, epidemiological studies, longitudinal studies, major depressive episodes, migraine, painful conditions

Introduction

Adverse childhood experiences (ACE) are believed to have a negative impact on health status later in life. Mental health outcomes, particularly depression, may occur more often in people with adverse childhood experiences. The ACE study, for example, retrospectively linked ACE to lower levels of adult mental health (Felitti et al. 1998; Edwards et al. 2003) and to probable depressive disorders (Chapman et al. 2004). McLaughlin et al. (2010) used data from the US National Epidemiological Survey of Alcohol and Related Conditions to document stress sensitisation, defined as a greater than additive effect of childhood adversity and more recent stressful events, in the aetiology of major depressive episode (MDE), post-traumatic stress disorder and anxiety disorders. A similar result for MDE was reported in two analyses arising from a Canadian longitudinal survey (Colman et al. 2013; Patten, 2013). In the World Mental Health (WMH) Surveys, an association between childhood adversity and almost all evaluated mental disorders, including major depression, was found (Kessler et al. 2010). The association between childhood adversity and adult MDE has been consistently reported in the literature.

Pain and depression may be linked at a pathophysiological level (Miller et al. 2009), but the association of painful conditions with childhood adversity is more tenuous than that of MDE. Epidemiologically, painful medical conditions are among those most strongly associated with MDE (Patten et al. 2005). Pain itself is a complex phenomenon that can be assessed along an affective dimension (pain unpleasantness) as well as a more conventional dimension of pain intensity, with pain sensitivity being affected by depression (Strigo et al. 2008). Here, pain sensitivity is defined as a qualitatively altered negative emotional response to normally non-aversive thermal stimuli (Strigo et al. 2008). Childhood adversity has been suspected of increasing the risk of painful conditions. A review by Raphael et al. (2004), however, noted that most of the evidence for this idea derives from cross-sectional studies that relied on retrospective reports of childhood adversity, whereas most prospective investigations have failed to identify an association. These authors suggested that an association may not exist, or that if it does exist it may be weak. A prospective study, however, reported that self-reported arthritis was associated with reports of childhood adversity in a community study (Keyes et al. 2013).

The association between self-reported alcoholism (‘have you ever considered yourself an alcoholic?’) and childhood adversity was reported by the ACE study (Felitti et al. 1998) and an association of childhood adversity with substance-use disorders was seen in the WMH surveys (Kessler et al. 2010). However, this literature has been inconsistent. Colman et al. (2013) reported an association between heavy drinking and childhood adversity in some but not all cycles of a Canadian longitudinal survey. Scott et al. (2012) reported that associations of alcohol use disorders with childhood maltreatment were no longer significant after adjustment for sociodemographic variables.

Most studies have assessed childhood adversity retrospectively. For example, in the ACE study a questionnaire about adverse childhood events was administered to adults attending a health care organisation (Felitti et al. 1998). Measurement errors resulting from inaccurate recall may inflate associations if respondents having poor health in adulthood are more likely to recall childhood adversities (differential misclassification bias) or may dilute those associations if inaccuracies in recall occur at the same rate irrespective of adult health status (Kleinbaum et al. 1982). Misclassification may arise through several mechanisms, one of which is retrospective amplification. The perceived level of threat associated with a traumatic experience, for example, was found in one study to be subject to retrospective amplification among those with more persistent post-traumatic symptoms (Heir et al. 2009). One review on the other hand emphasised the frequent occurrence of false negative reports in the literature concerned with accuracy of recall of adverse childhood events (Hardt & Rutter, 2004).

Interestingly, Raphael et al. reported that court-documented child abuse or neglect failed to predict pain in young adulthood but that young adults with pain complaints were more likely to retrospectively report childhood victimisation. These authors suggested that recall of such events may actually be a more salient factor than their actual historical occurrence (Raphael et al. 2001).

In the present study, we sought to evaluate epidemiological associations between childhood adversities, as assessed in 1994 in a study called the National Longitudinal Study of Children and Youth (NLSCY) and adult health outcomes. Outcomes were assessed later during a partially linked study called the National Population Health Survey (NPHS). In the NLSCY, the adversities were reported by the person most knowledgeable about the child (PMK), usually the mother. The NPHS assessed childhood adversity using a series of retrospective interview items. The goal was to examine whether these two types of assessment similarly associated with the three outcomes, or not. We hypothesised that stronger associations would specifically occur with retrospective assessment in situations where differential recall bias might increase the recollection of past adversities, specifically MDE, and that in other circumstances retrospective assessment would cause non-differential misclassification and tend to dilute the associations, such that weaker associations would be observed with retrospective assessment. We focused on three health outcomes discussed above: MDE, painful medical conditions and alcohol consumption.

Method

Data sources

The NLSCY (Statistics Canada, 2010) and the NPHS (Statistics Canada, 2012) were both population-based studies conducted by Statistics Canada. The NLSCY targeted children and the NPHS targeted adults. Both were longitudinal in their design and both began data collection in 1994. The NPHS collected follow-up data for 16 years. The target population in each study was the national household-dwelling population. A feature of these studies is that they employed a linked sampling methodology. Some children between the ages of 0 and 12 surveyed for the 1994 NLSCY study were subsequently followed-up by the NPHS. These respondents were interviewed as part of the NPHS cohort when they reached age of 12 but were not included in the NLSCY longitudinal cohort. The NPHS interviewed its cohort every 2 years until 2010 (Swain et al. 1999). An 11-year-old NLSCY respondent sampled in 1994 with complete data collection would have been followed approximately to age 27 in the NPHS, whereas an infant in the 1994 NLSCY with complete follow-up would have been interviewed with the adult NPHS interview in the two final NPHS cycles at the ages of 14 and 16. The shared component of the sample was a random sample representative of the target population.

Measures

The NLSCY included a series of questions directed towards the PMK about each child. We selected a set of 14 adversities that most closely resembled those asked later in the NPHS. These 14 adversities were: death of parent, death of family member, parents separated, number of moves, a stay in foster care, other separation from parents, change in household membership, alcohol/mental health issues, injury/illness of family member, abuse, hospital stay, injury/illness of child, conflict between parents and other trauma.

The NPHS included a series of retrospective questions inquiring about childhood adversities, all of which were asked directly of the study participants. The questions were preceded by: ‘The next few questions ask about some things that may have happened to you while you were a child or teenager, before you moved out of the house. Please tell me if any of these things have happened.’ This was followed by individual items referring to the following events: spending 2 weeks in hospital, parental divorce, long-term parental unemployment, ‘something that scared you so much you thought about it for years after’, being sent away from home for doing something wrong, parental drinking or drug use that ‘caused problems for the family’, ‘physical abuse by someone close to you.’ Childhood stressful experiences were again classified as occurring when there were one or more positive responses to these items.

The NPHS interview included the Composite International Diagnostic Interview Short Form for Major Depression (CIDI-SF) (Kessler et al. 1998) to assess past year MDE. The CIDI-SF was developed using data from the National Comorbidity Survey (Kessler et al. 1994), which used the DSM-IIIR classification. The CIDI-SF is scored using an algorithm that produces a predictive probability of past year MDE. For the current analysis, the 90% predictive cut-point was used in scoring. This scoring procedure requires endorsement of five symptom-based criteria (at least one of which must be depressed mood or loss of interest), providing face validity in relation both to the DSM-IIIR and DSM-IV definitions of MDE.

The NPHS also included a variety of self-report items on alcohol consumption. These included a 7-day consumption diary. Canadian ‘low risk’ drinking guidelines in place at the time of the study (14 per week and men and 9 per week in women) were used to identify excess consumption this analysis. Due to limitations of the data source we were unable to assess alcohol dependence – this was assessed in only one NPHS cycle by the relevant CIDI-SF module.

A set of painful chronic conditions were identified based on a series of items asking about ‘long-term’ (defined as ‘expected to last’ longer than 6 months) medical conditions. The items employed in this part of the survey resembled those of the US Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance system questionnaires (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2013) and asked about professionally diagnosed long-term conditions. Three conditions were classified as painful conditions in these analyses: migraine, back pain and ‘arthritis or rheumatism.’ A self-reported professional diagnosis of any one of these conditions was taken to mean that the respondent had a painful condition. These conditions were also examined individually when the sample size was adequate.

Data linkage

The linkage of the two survey datasets was carried out by Statistics Canada and the linked data were forwarded to the Prairie Regional Data Centre at the University of Calgary, where it was analysed using Stata (Stata Corporation, 2012).

Analysis

In order to maximise the use of the follow-up data, we selected a method of analysis that could incorporate repeated observations from each respondent: generalised estimating equations (GEE). As the outcome variables were binary, logistic regression was used. We employed an unstructured correlation structure after determining that this produced better goodness of fit statistics than two alternatives: independent and exchangeable. Robust standard errors were used in all analyses. The three outcomes were modelled in the same way in relation to adversities in the linked sample, and from the self-reported adversities in the entire NPHS longitudinal cohort. We produced sex-adjusted models within the linked data file (all respondents entered into the NPHS follow-up at age 12 such that, barring a cohort effect, age adjustment was not relevant) and age and sex adjusted estimates for the NPHS longitudinal file. Whereas the painful conditions category consisted of respondents with any one of the three specified painful conditions, we also examined each of those conditions individually. This resulted in 12 tests of significance, leading to a concern about Type I error. To address this, the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure was used to control the false discovery rate in this part of the analysis. The procedure was implemented using spreadsheet-based method (Thiessen et al. 2002). The datasets from these population surveys are each associated with a set of replicate bootstrap weights designed to account for design effects and to ensure representation of the target population. In analyses where one dataset was used the estimates were weighted. Most of the analyses, however, were not weighted since they were based on a linked sample from two different surveys.

Results

There were n = 1977 respondents in the linked sample. Of these subjects 95.9% provided outcome data during at least one follow-up assessment and could therefore be included in the GEE analysis. Complete follow-up was achieved for 66.0% of the sample over the entire interval (up to 16 years, for some of the participants), 0.7% left the sampling frame because of death or institutionalisation during follow-up and 0.9% provided partial responses. The proportion lost to follow-up during NPHS follow-up of the linked sample was 32.5%. Table 1 presents demographic features of the sample and Table 2 reports the frequency of experiencing any one or more childhood adversity in the NLSCY cross-tabulated against the NPHS data. In the NLSCY, one or more adversity was reported by 25.1% (95% confidence intervals (CI) 22.6–27.6) of the sample whereas in the NPHS 54.3% (95% CI 50.1–58.5) reported one or more of the targeted adversities. Respondents with missing data on one or both of the adversity measures could not be included in the cross-tabulation, so the row totals are not exactly the same as the frequencies reported above. There was approximately 60% agreement between the two sets of reports, which was significantly greater than chance (z = 7.26, p < 0.001) but (not unexpectedly since different adversities were assessed) the Kappa was only 0.21, indicating a low degree of chance-corrected agreement. When this comparison was restricted to a subset of adversities that were somewhat consistently assessed: parental divorce, physical abuse and being sent away from home/placed in foster care (examples of where the two surveys assessed similar concepts, although different items were employed), the Kappa was somewhat higher at 0.43.

Table 1.

Features of the linked sample in 1994 (NLSCY) and 2010 (NPHS)

Characteristics Categories Weighted percentage* 95% CI
Characteristics of the linked sample as reported by the PMK in 1994 (age 0–11)
Sex Male 51.7 48.8–54.7
Female 48.3 45.3–51.2
Raised by lone mother Yes 16.3 14.2–18.5
No 83.7 81.5–85.8
PMK depressed† Yes 3.0 1.9–4.2
No 97.0 95.8–98.1
Demographic context Urban 84.3 82.5–86.1
Rural 15.7 13.9–17.5
Immigrated to Canada PMK 21.3 18.6–23.9
Spouse 19.6 17.0–22.2
Both 27.3 24.5–30.1
Characteristics of the linked sample in the final NPHS Cycle, 2010 (age 16–24)
Marital status Married/common law 5.3 4.0–6.7
Not married 94.7 93.3–96.0
Education No post-secondary 37.4 33.9–40.9
At least some post-secondary 62.6 59.1–66.1
Employment Currently working 67.7 64.2–71.1
Not currently working 32.3 28.9–35.8

*Analyses based on the linked sample are not weighted in this paper, but the frequencies in table are weighted using NLSCY (top section, childhood demographics) or NPHS (bottom section, adult demographics) weights to help ensure representativeness.

†Score of 21 or greater on a modified version of the CES-D scale (Poulin et al. 2005).

Table 2.

Frequency of any reported childhood adversities in the NLSCY and NPHS cohorts

NLSCY
Yes No
NPHS Yes 22.8% 30.0%
No 10.0% 37.2%

Table 3 presents sex adjusted odds ratios (ORs) and associated 95% CI for each of the three outcomes evaluated in the linked sample. In the right-hand column, the estimates for NLSCY adversities are recalculated with exclusion of those subjects reporting adversities in the NPHS. This is a group of respondents who apparently had adversities but did not report them in the later survey. Major depression was consistently associated with reports of adversity, with the stronger association being seen in the NPHS. Weaker, but still significant associations were seen for painful conditions, again, these were consistently seen in both NLSCY-linked and NPHS-based analyses. No association specifically of adversity with ‘arthritis or rheumatism’ was observed. Excessive alcohol consumption was not associated with adversity irrespective of method of assessment. Table 3 reports p-values arising from the sex-adjusted models. Identification of critical values for assessing statistical significance in each test with the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure did not alter the pattern of statistical significance. All of the p-values <0.05 remained significant when assessed against Benjamini–Hochberg critical values while all of the p-values >0.05 remained non-significant. The largest of the p-values that was <0.05 was that for arthritis in the NLSCY (p = 0.008) which was the eighth ranked p-value and was judged against a critical value of 0.01.

Table 3.

Sex-adjusted ORs for childhood adversity assessed during childhood (NLSCY) or adulthood (NPHS)

Outcomes (NPHS) NLSCY NPHS NLSCY, excluding those reporting adversities in the NPHS
OR OR OR
95% CI 95% CI 95% CI
p-value p-value p-value
MDE 2.0 5.5 2.7
1.5–2.5 3.2–9.4 2.0–3.6
<0.001 <0.001 <0.001
Migraine 1.6 1.4 1.7
1.2–2.1 0.9–2.1 1.2–2.2
0.004 0.152 0.001
Arthritis 2.3 1.0 –*
1.2–4.3 0.7–1.6
0.008 0.842
Back pain 1.5 1.9 1.4
1.1–2.0 1.3–2.8 1.1–1.9
0.006 <0.001 0.022
Painful conditions 1.9 1.5 1.8
1.5–2.4 1.1–2.0 1.4–2.2
<0.001 0.007 <0.001
Alcohol above weekly guidelines 1.2 1.3 1.1
0.9–1.5 0.9–1.7 0.9–1.4
0.132 0.107 <0.001

*Due to small cell sizes, the estimate could not be released.

Discussion

These results support the idea that childhood adversity is associated with MDE and, less strongly, with painful conditions, a pattern consistent with prior literature. No association with excessive drinking was observed. A prior analysis of NPHS data (Colman et al. 2013) did report an association between childhood adversity and excessive drinking, but only when ≥2 adversities were reported and, as noted above, negative results for this association have also been reported by other studies (Scott et al. 2012). Identification of a strong association of childhood adversity with MDE, a weaker one with painful conditions and none for alcohol consumption seems broadly consistent with prior literature. The ORs for alcohol consumption (see Table 3) were slightly elevated, a situation that could lead to the pattern of weak and inconsistent association being observed in prior studies since only large studies would have sufficient power to reliably detect weak effects. Notably, the ACE study used ‘alcoholism’ as an outcome, which is a different concept than that of excessive consumption that that used here. Also, the ACE study had a very large sample size.

The literature linking childhood adversity to adult pain has been inconsistent. As noted above, based on a literature review, Raphael et al. reported that the association may be weak, or may not exist at all. In this study, we observed a significantly elevated risk of painful conditions in association with childhood adversity but the association was indeed weak compared to that for MDE, consistent with the prior report (Raphael et al. 2004). Most importantly for the goals of this study, the pattern was comparable irrespective of the method of assessment employed.

Unlike a prior study (Scott et al. 2012), we were unable to directly compare different methods of assessment of adversity. There are many explanations for the differing responses. It is likely that some children experienced adversities at a time after the time of reporting in the NLSCY, such that the record of exposure to adversity in the NPHS, which only began at age 18 might pick up problems not identified at the earlier survey. A major limitation of this investigation is that the specific adversities assessed in two surveys were not the same, such that differential reporting cannot be assumed to be due to recall issues at all. Studies having an opportunity to compare identical sets of adversities with prospective and retrospective recall are needed to decisively determine the impact of recall on estimated associations. Despite these methodological uncertainties, the results reported here are in line with those reported previously (Scott et al. 2012): there was a considerable degree of consistency of estimated associations across the different assessment methods. While there is a tendency to consider self-report as merely representing an error prone proxy assessment of actual historical events, the likelihood of recollecting and reporting of such events may be related to their personal salience and health-impact. In turn, this may mean that retrospective assessments are more useful than expected despite their likely vulnerability to error. However, the hypothesis put forward by Raphael et al. (2001) that recall of adversities may actually be a more salient factor than their actual historical occurrence was not confirmed by these results, since a similar pattern of association with prospectively reported adversities was seen even when adversities were not reported retrospectively, see the right-hand column in Table 3.

Since misclassification of risk factor exposures tends to be a source of bias in epidemiologic research (Kleinbaum et al. 1982), expectation holds that systematic differences should be introduced when misclassification is substantial. For example, in a hypothetical population with 1100 people that are non-depressed and 120 depressed members one may imagine a scenario in which 20 of the 120 depressed and 100 of the 1100 non-depressed have experienced an adverse event during their childhood, leading to an OR of 2. However, if there is a 35% false negative rate for the reporting of adversity in the non-depressed respondents an OR of 3.2 would be expected. Similar inflation of the association would be seen if false positives occurred selectively in the depressed subjects. On the other hand, if there was an equivalent 35% false negative rate in both depressed and non-depressed subjects (non-differential misclassification), a slightly reduced OR of 1.9 would be expected. Most of the existing literature would suggest that false negative classification among respondents whose mental health is good is likely to be the main problem with retrospective reports (Fergusson et al. 2000; Hardt & Rutter, 2004; Heir et al. 2009) and one may therefore expect to see an overestimation of the association when retrospective reports are used. This is a potential explanation for the very strong association (OR = 5.5) seen for MDE when retrospective reports were used in the classification of exposure, but the same effect was apparently not seen for the other outcomes examined. By way of emphasising this point, it may be noted that the OR for MDE when adversity was defined in terms of the retrospective NPHS reports was approximately twice as large as that seen when adversity was defined using the NLSCY data and the lower bounds of the 95% CI for the NPHS-based estimate exceeded the upper bound of that for NLSCY-defined adversity. A tendency for differential misclassification to inflate estimates might be offset by aspects of inaccuracy that are not related to health status at the time of measurement (non-differential misclassification), such that the theoretical direction of bias is difficult to anticipate. Empirical observations are therefore important and the ones reported here are reassuring. However, it is worth noting that in the case of painful conditions, which might not be associated with differential recall in the same way that depression may be, did show a slightly stronger association with childhood adversity in the NLSCY data, which is consistent with the hypothesis that retrospective misclassification of adversity may be non-differential with respect to adult pain.

Important limitations of this study include attrition, which could have distorted the associations observed, and the limited sample size. The cross-tabulation required to estimate the ORs could not generally be further stratified to detect effect modification or explore issues of confounding. Also, the analysis would have been more decisive if the assessment of adversity were more consistent across the studies. The employment of validated instruments to assess adversity and its severity would be superior to the sorts of items included, which allow only a rough determination that an adversity occurred.

In totality, these results concur with the conclusions of prior studies (Hardt & Rutter, 2004; Scott et al. 2012) that retrospective assessment of adversity, despite an obvious vulnerability to inaccuracy, cannot be assumed de facto to invalidate epidemiological estimates. Such measures appear to perform more favourably than, based on epidemiologic theory, one might expect.

Acknowledgements

This work was conducted using data collected by Statistics Canada. However, the analysis presented here was conducted by the authors and the interpretations presented in this paper do not the interpretations or opinions of Statistics Canada.

Financial Support

This project was supported by a grant from the Alberta Centre for Child, Family and Community Research. Dr Patten is a Senior Health Scholar with Alberta Innovates, Health Solutions.

Conflict of Interest

None.

Ethical Standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

  1. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2013). Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System. Retrieved 14 August 2013 from http://www.cdc.gov/brfss/questionnaires/index.htm.
  2. Chapman DP, Whitfield CL, Felitti VJ, Dube SR, Edwards VJ, Anda RF (2004). Adverse childhood experiences and the risk of depressive disorders in adulthood. Journal of Affective Disorders 82, 217–225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Colman I, Garad Y, Zeng Y, Naicker K, Weeks M, Patten SB, Jones PB, Thompson AH, Wild TC (2013). Stress and development of depression and heavy drinking in adulthood: moderating effects of childhood trauma. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 48, 265–274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Edwards VJ, Holden GW, Felitti VJ, Anda RF (2003). Relationship between multiple forms of childhood maltreatment and adult mental health in community respondents: results from the adverse childhood experiences study. American Journal of Psychiatry 160, 1453–1460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM, Edwards V, Koss MP, Marks JS (1998). Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. American Journal of Preventive Medicine 14, 245–258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Woodward LJ (2000). The stability of child abuse reports: a longitudinal study of the reporting behaviour of young adults. Psychological Medicine 30, 529–544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Hardt J, Rutter M (2004). Validity of adult retrospective reports of adverse childhood experiences: review of the evidence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 45, 260–273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Heir T, Piatigorsky A, Weisaeth L (2009). Longitudinal changes in recalled perceived life threat after a natural disaster. British Journal of Psychiatry 194, 510–514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Kessler RC, McGonagle KA, Zhao S, Nelson CB, Hughes M, Eshleman S, Wittchen HU, Kendler KS (1994). Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States. Results from the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry 51, 8–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Kessler RC, Andrews G, Mroczek D, Ustun B, Wittchen HU (1998). The World Health Organization Composite International Diagnostic Interview Short-Form (CIDI-SF). International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 7, 171–185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Kessler RC, McLaughlin KA, Green JG, Gruber MJ, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, Aguilar-Gaxiola S, Alhamzawi AO, Alonso J, Angermeyer M, Benjet C, Bromet E, Chatterji S, De GG, Demyttenaere K, Fayyad J, Florescu S, Gal G, Gureje O, Haro JM, Hu CY, Karam EG, Kawakami N, Lee S, Lepine JP, Ormel J, Posada-Villa J, Sagar R, Tsang A, Ustun TB, Vassilev S, Viana MC, Williams DR (2010). Childhood adversities and adult psychopathology in the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. British Journal of Psychiatry 197, 378–385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Keyes KM, McLaughlin KA, Demmer RT, Cerda M, Koenen KC, Uddin M, Galea S (2013). Potentially traumatic events and the risk of six physical health conditions in a population-based sample. Depression and Anxiety 30, 451–460. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Kleinbaum DG, Kupper LL, Morgenstern H (1982). Information bias In Epidemiologic Research, pp. 220–241. Van Nostrand Reinhold: New York. [Google Scholar]
  14. McLaughlin KA, Conron KJ, Koenen KC, Gilman SE (2010). Childhood adversity, adult stressful life events, and risk of past-year psychiatric disorder: a test of the stress sensitization hypothesis in a population-based sample of adults. Psychological Medicine 40, 1647–1658. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Miller AH, Maletic V, Raison CL (2009). Inflammation and its discontents: the role of cytokines in the pathophysiology of major depression. Biological Psychiatry 65, 732–741. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. National Longitudinal Survey of Children and Youth (NLSCY). 2010. Statistics Canada. Retrieved 14 August 2013 from http://www23.statcan.gc.ca/imdb/p2SV.pl?Function=getSurvey&SDDS=4450&Item_Id=44678.
  17. Patten SB (2013). Childhood and adult stressors and major depression risk: interpreting interactions with the sufficient-component cause model. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 48, 927–933. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Patten SB, Beck CA, Kassam A, Williams JVA, Barbui C, Metz LM (2005). Long-term medical conditions and major depression: strength of association for specific conditions in the general population. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 50, 195–202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Poulin C, Hand D, Boudreau B (2005). Validity of a 12-item version of the CES-D used in the National Longitudinal Study of Children and Youth. Chronic Diseases in Canada 26, 65–72. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Raphael KG, Widom CS, Lange G (2001). Childhood victimization and pain in adulthood: a prospective investigation. Pain 92, 283–293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Raphael KG, Chandler HK, Ciccone DS (2004). Is childhood abuse a risk factor for chronic pain in adulthood? Current Pain and Headache Reports 8, 99–110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Scott KM, McLaughlin KA, Smith DA, Ellis PM (2012). Childhood maltreatment and DSM-IV adult mental disorders: comparison of prospective and retrospective findings. British Journal of Psychiatry 200, 469–475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Stata Corporation (2012). Stata, Version 12.1. Stata Corporation: College Station, TX.
  24. Statistics Canada (2012). National Population Health Survey – Household Component – Longitudinal (NPHS). Retrieved 14 August 2013 from http://www23.statcan.gc.ca/imdb/p2SV.pl?Function=getSurvey&SDDS=3225&Item_Id=1213.
  25. Strigo IA, Simmons AN, Matthews SC, Craig AD, Paulus MP (2008). Increased affective bias revealed using experimental graded heat stimuli in young depressed adults: evidence of “emotional allodynia”. Psychosomatic Medicine 70, 338–344. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Swain L, Catlin G, Beaudet MP (1999). The National Population Health Survey – its longitudinal nature. Health Reports 10, 69–82. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Thiessen D, Steinberg L, Kuang D (2002). Quick and easy implementation of the Benjamini-Hochberg Procedure for controlling the false positive rate in multiple comparisons. Journal of Educational and Behavioural Statistics 27, 77–83. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences are provided here courtesy of Cambridge University Press

RESOURCES