Skip to main content
Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences logoLink to Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences
. 2013 May 20;23(2):155–165. doi: 10.1017/S2045796013000243

Discrimination against people with severe mental illness and their access to social capital: findings from the Viewpoint survey

M Webber 1,*, E Corker 2, S Hamilton 3, C Weeks 3, V Pinfold 3, D Rose 2, G Thornicroft 2, C Henderson 2
PMCID: PMC6998171  PMID: 23683403

Abstract

Aims.

Discrimination against people with severe mental illness is an international problem. It is associated with reduced social contact and hinders recovery. This paper aims to evaluate if experienced or anticipated discrimination is associated with social capital, a known correlate of mental health.

Methods.

Data from the annual viewpoint cross-sectional survey of people with severe mental illness (n = 1016) were analysed. Exploratory univariate analysis was used to identify correlates of social capital in the sample, which were then evaluated in linear regression models. Additional hypotheses were tested using t tests.

Results.

Experienced discrimination made a modest contribution to the explained variance of social capital. Experienced discrimination from friends and immediate family was associated with reduced access to social capital from these groups, but this was not found for wider family, neighbours or mental health staff. Experience of discrimination in finding or keeping a job was also associated with reduced access to social capital.

Conclusions.

Further longitudinal research is needed to determine how resources within people's networks can help to build resilience, which reduces the harmful effect of discrimination on mental health.

Key words: Discrimination, severe mental illness, social capital, stigma

Introduction

Discrimination against people with severe mental illness is a global problem (Thornicroft et al. 2009; Kapungwe et al. 2010; Aromaa et al. 2011b; Rose et al. 2011; Evans-Lacko et al. 2012a; Świtaj et al. 2012). Sources of discrimination include friends and family (Thornicroft, 2006; Henderson et al. 2012), the general public (Thornicroft, 2006), mental health professionals (Wahl & Aroesty-Cohen, 2010), emergency department clinicians (van Nieuwenhuizen et al. 2012), and public and private institutions (Corrigan et al. 2004). However, people with severe mental illness appear to have more social tolerance towards others with similar diagnoses, though self-stigma is common (Aromaa et al. 2011a).

Discrimination can be defined as the behavioural component of stigma (Link & Phelan, 2001), which also includes ignorance (a problem of knowledge) and prejudice (a problem of attitude) (Goffman, 1970; Thornicroft, 2006). People with severe mental illness who experience discrimination in their close environment are more likely to anticipate it (Thornicroft et al. 2009); this leads to avoidance of seeking work and relationships, hampering recovery (Link et al. 2001).

Supporting the recovery of people with severe mental illness is a priority for mental health services (Slade, 2009). This includes ‘social recovery’, which requires the development of social environments that are both accepting and enabling (Beresford, 2002). Anti-stigma campaigns such as Time to Change (Henderson & Thornicroft, 2009) support this by seeking to change public attitudes towards mental illness. Social contact interventions are included in these programmes as a means of reducing discrimination (Evans-Lacko et al. 2012b). However, social engagement itself plays an important role in recovery from severe mental illness because it helps to build social capital (Webber, 2005; Tew et al. 2012).

Social capital is increasingly being recognized as important for health and well-being (Kawachi et al. 2007). It is a multi-dimensional concept encompassing diverse aspects such as trust (Coleman, 1988), social norms and reciprocity (Putnam, 2000), features of social structures and networks (Burt, 1992; Lin, 2001), and the resources embedded within them (Bourdieu, 1997). Although epidemiological studies have largely drawn upon Putnam's (2000) conception of social capital (De Silva et al. 2005), social network approaches more clearly align the concept with recovery discourses (Webber, 2005).

Defined as the resources that are embedded within social networks, social capital can lead to greater occupational prestige, income and political influence when mobilized (Lin, 2001; Lin & Erickson, 2008). Health gains can be accrued by investing in relationships, which may promote positive health behaviours (Zambon et al. 2010), provide employment opportunities (Flap, 1999), and reinforce an individual's identification with a group and help to maintain subjective social status (Song, 2011).

Social capital is negatively correlated with depression (Webber & Huxley, 2007; Song, 2011; Webber et al. 2011) and severe mental illnesses such as schizophrenia or bi-polar disorder (Dutt & Webber, 2010). Longitudinally, it is associated with changes in quality of life in depression (Webber et al. 2011). However, an association between discrimination and reduced access to network resources has not yet been empirically demonstrated.

There is a strong theoretical rationale for an association of discrimination and social capital in people with severe mental illness. Bourdieu (1997), a key contributor to the development of the concept of social capital, argued that social capital accrued within durable networks of people with strong relationships and shared interests. Social network researchers identified the ‘homophily principle’ as being important to this process whereby people associate with others similar to themselves (Lin, 2001). People within networks predominantly composed of those with a high socio-economic status have improved life chances, such as occupational attainment (Lin et al. 1981), but they exclude those who are different from themselves as they are perceived as not being able to contribute to the group's social capital (Lin & Ao, 2008). People with severe mental illness are commonly characterized as transgressing social norms and distinguished from other members of society (Thornicroft, 2006). It follows that discrimination due to mental health stigma may restrict the access of people with severe mental illness to resourceful social networks and the social capital held within them. Therefore, it is important to explore the extent to which discrimination is associated with social capital in people with severe mental illness to empirically verify this assumption.

It is possible, though, that low access to social capital may increase perceptions of discrimination. People who do not have full access to resourceful networks within their community may attribute this to discrimination. Studies of perceived discrimination among ethnic minority groups in the US exemplify this possibility (Goto et al. 2002; Estrada et al. 2008; Pérez et al. 2008). Although perceptions of discrimination are different from actual experiences of discrimination, careful measurement of both social capital and discrimination is required to fully understand the direction of any cross-sectional association.

This paper aims to test the following hypotheses:

  • (1)

    that experienced and anticipated discrimination are associated with access to reduced social capital, while controlling for confounding variables;

  • (2)

    that experienced discrimination from specific social ties such as family, friends, neighbourhoods and mental health professionals are associated with reduced access to social capital from these social ties; and

  • (3)

    that experienced discrimination in different life domains (such as employment, social life and family) is associated with reduced access to corresponding domains of social capital.

Method

Viewpoint (Hamilton et al. 2011; Corker et al. 2013) is an annual survey of mental health service users in England, which aims to evaluate outcomes of the Time to Change programme (Henderson & Thornicroft, 2009, 2013), the largest ever programme to reduce discrimination against people with mental health problems in England. Phase 1 of this programme aimed to achieve a 5% reduction in discrimination over 4 years through a national social marketing campaign launched in 2009, and national and local interventions which engaged individuals, communities and professional groups to improve the attitudes and behaviour of the general public towards people with mental health problems. To evaluate the effect of Phase 2 (2011–2015) on social capital in addition to discrimination, a measure of social capital was added to the survey in 2011 to provide a baseline.

Data were collected via a cross-sectional telephone survey with separate samples of users of specialist National Health Service (NHS) mental health services each year. Participants were aged between 18 and 65 years, had used NHS mental health services in the previous 6 months, but were not hospital in-patients or those with a diagnosis of dementia. Participants were selected from five NHS mental health trusts, which were chosen on the basis of the level of socio-economic deprivation in their catchment area, determined from census data. Full details about the methodology of the Viewpoint surveys are available elsewhere (Henderson et al. 2012).

This paper uses data from the 2011 Viewpoint survey. 9120 invitation packs were posted to eligible participants, of whom 1016 undertook telephone interviews, representing a response rate of 11.1%. Although low, this response rate was an improvement on the three previous surveys which achieved response rates not higher than 8% (Henderson et al. 2012).

The 22-item version of the Discrimination and Stigma Scale (DISC-12) (Thornicroft et al. 2009; Brohan et al. in press) was used to measure participants' reports of experienced and anticipated discrimination. This interviewer-administered scale contains 22 items related to negative experiences of mental health-related discrimination over the prior 12 months and four items related to anticipated discrimination. A ‘not applicable’ option is available for items about situations that were not relevant to the participant in the previous 12 months. Additional questions on positive discrimination were asked in the interview, but were not analysed for this paper. The DISC-12 has robust psychometric properties in this population (Thornicroft et al. 2009; Brohan et al. in press).

The Resource Generator-UK (RG-UK) (Webber & Huxley, 2007) was used to measure participants' access to social capital. In the tradition of social network measures such as the Name Generator (McCallister & Fischer, 1978) and Position Generator (Lin & Dumin, 1986), this instrument measures participants' access to social resources within their own social network. The RG-UK was derived from a version developed in The Netherlands (van der Gaag & Snijders, 2005) and its items have been made culturally relevant and validated for use in the UK general population. It has good reliability and validity (Webber & Huxley, 2007), and has been used in samples of people with mental health problems (e.g., Murray et al. 2007; Webber & Huxley, 2007; Dutt & Webber, 2010; Webber et al. 2011) and produced valid findings.

The RG-UK asks participants whether or not they could obtain access to 27 skills and resources within their social network within 1 week if they needed it. If they respond ‘yes’ to an item, they are asked to indicate the nature of the social tie – i.e., close family, wider family, friends, colleague, acquaintance, mental health professional – through which they could access each skill or resource. The instrument has four sub-scales each representing a concrete domain of social capital to which an individual may have access: domestic resources, personal skills, expert advice and problem-solving resources. Participants were additionally asked if they personally possessed 14 of the items as it would be unlikely for them to ask anyone for these items if they personally possessed them. This formed a separate human capital sub-scale, which was entered as a potential confounding variable in multivariate analysis (Webber & Huxley, 2007). Socio-demographic and clinical characteristics were also obtained from the sample.

RG-UK total and sub-scale scores were calculated by scoring items accessible within a participant's network as 1 and those not as 0, and then summing to calculate scale totals. Experienced discrimination DISC scores were calculated by scoring any reported instance of negative discrimination as 1 and situations in which no discrimination was reported as 0. The overall score was calculated as the sum of reported discrimination items divided by the number of items answered, multiplied by 100 to give a percentage of items in which discrimination was reported. Items were excluded where the participant had not been in the situation asked about in the previous 12 months, and therefore could not have experienced discrimination, e.g., in relation to starting a family. The four anticipated discrimination items were analysed individually as binary variables (0 = no, 1 = yes).

We used t tests to compare mean RG-UK scores in the Viewpoint sample with a UK general population sample (Webber & Huxley, 2007) and a sample of people with severe mental illness (Murray et al. 2007). Univariate analysis using Pearson correlations, t tests, one-way analysis of variance and Spearman's rank correlations explored the relationship between anticipated and experienced discrimination, socio-demographic and clinical variables, and the RG-UK. To test hypothesis one, we entered variables with a significant association with RG-UK total scores (p < 0.05) in blocks into a linear regression model to explore the independent relationship between discrimination and access to social capital (adjusted for human capital). Hypotheses two and three were tested using t tests. All analyses were conducted using SPSS v.15.

The study received ethical approval from Riverside NHS Ethics Committee (ref. 07/H0706/72).

Results

The demographic characteristics of the Viewpoint sample are shown in Table 1. In comparison with national data (Her Majesty's Government, 2012), women (59.3% v. 51.8%) and people of white ethnicity (85.4% v. 80.0%) were oversampled in our population. Otherwise, it was representative of people using secondary mental health services in England.

Table 1.

Demographic characteristics of participants

Demographic characteristic Participants (n = 1016)
n (%)
Gender
Male 411 (40.5)
Female 602 (59.3)
Transgender 2 (0.2)
Age (years)* 45 (11.2)
Ethnicity
White British 868 (85.4)
Other White 36 (3.5)
Black or Mixed Black and White 40 (3.9)
Asian or Mixed Asian and White 52 (5.2)
Other Mixed 5 (0.5)
Other 7 (0.7)
Did not wish to disclose 7 (0.7)
Employment status
Unemployed 485 (47.7)
Part-time employed 90 (8.9)
Full-time employed 121 (11.9)
Self-employed 28 (2.8)
Retired 95 (9.4)
Volunteering 52 (5.1)
Training/education 20 (2.0)
Other 124 (12.2)
Did not wish to disclose 1 (0.1)
Main diagnosis
Depression 311 (30.6)
Bipolar disorder 184 (18.1)
Schizophrenia 116 (11.4)
Anxiety disorder 82 (8.1)
Personality disorder 55 (5.4)
Schizoaffective disorder 26 (2.6)
Eating disorder 6 (0.6)
Multiple diagnoses 4 (0.4)
Other 121 (11.9)
Missing 109 (10.7)
Received involuntary treatment
Yes 353 (34.7)
No 663 (65.3)

*Mean (s.d.).

88.2% (n = 896) of our sample reported experiencing discrimination in at least one life domain, with 58.0% (n = 589) reporting it in at least four life domains. Our sample had access to a mean of 13.9 (s.d. = 6.0) out of 27 social capital resources, which was fewer than a comparison general population sample (mean = 17.2, s.d. = 5.9) (Webber & Huxley, 2007), but more than a similar sample of people receiving specialist mental health care in London (mean = 10.8, s.d. = 5.8) (Murray et al. 2007).

The RG-UK was inversely correlated with experienced discrimination on the DISC (r = −0.219, p < 0.001), a pattern, which was repeated for all of the sub-scales (domestic resources r = −0.206, p < 0.001; expert advice r = −0.182, p < 0.001; personal skills r = −0.164, p < 0.001; problem-solving resources r = −0.154, p < 0.001). However, the human capital sub-scale was not correlated with DISC (r = 0.029, p = 0.353). The shared variance of RG-UK and DISC was low (4.8%) indicating that the two instruments measured distinct constructs.

Access to social capital resources as measured by the RG-UK was also lower for those who chose not to apply for a job (13.3 v. 14.5, t = −2.9, df = 899, p = 004); start adult education (12.9 v. 14.5, t = −3.7, df = 899, p < 0.001); or enter into a new relationship (13.1 v. 14.8, t = −4.4, df = 899, p < 0.001) because of anticipated discrimination than those who did not anticipate any discrimination in these life domains. However, people who concealed their diagnosis from others due to anticipated discrimination (14.0 v. 13.8, t = 0.3, df = 899, p = 0.74) had access to the same quantity of social capital as those who did not.

To test the hypothesized independent association of discrimination and access to social capital, we first explored the univariate associations of potential confounding variables with the RG-UK. The RG-UK social capital scale was positively correlated with the RG-UK human capital scale, but inversely correlated with increasing age and length of time in contact with mental health services (Table 2). In addition, women, university graduates, employed people, people with a primary diagnosis of an affective disorder and those with no prior history of being involuntary detained in hospital had increased access to social capital.

Table 2.

Univariate correlates of access to social capital

Variable Sample descriptives, n (%) RG-UK, mean (s.d.) Association with RG-UK total score
Human capital (RG-UK) 2.30 (1.70)* n/a r = 0.44, p < 0.001
Age (years) 45.00 (11.16)* n/a r = − 0.14, p < 0.001
Gender
Male 411 (40.5) 13.25 (6.09) t = − 2.79, df = 896, p = 0.005
Female 602 (59.3) 14.39 (5.93)
Ethnicity
White 904 (89.0) 13.98 (5.88) t = 0.473, df = 120, p = 0.637
Other 112 (11.0) 13.64 (6.95)
Education
Graduate 366 (36.0) 15.76 (5.83) t = 6.93, df = 895, p < 0.001
Non-graduate 642 (63.2) 12.94 (5.87)
Employment
Employed 239 (23.5) 17.24 (5.27) t = 10.50, df = 426, p < 0.001
Not employed 776 (76.4) 12.85 (5.85)
Primary diagnosis
Affective disorder 650 (64.0) 14.61 (5.82) F = 6.60, df = 3798, p < 0.001
Personality disorder 55 (5.4) 12.54 (6.01)
Psychotic disorder 170 (16.7) 12.45 (5.87)
Other disorder 30 (3.0) 14.91 (23)
Religious attendee
Yes 368 (36.2) 14.18 (6.20) t = 0.89, df = 896, p = 0.376
No 644 (63.4) 13.81 (5.90)
Contact with MH services (years) 11 (15.0)† n/a rho = −0.112, p = 0.001
Involuntary patient
Yes 353 (34.7) 12.61 (6.08) t = − 4.77, df = 899, p < 0.001
No 663 (65.3) 14.61 (5.86)

*Mean (s.d.).

†Median (IQR).

The human capital scale accounted for 18.9% of the variance of RG-UK social capital scale and, as it was a related concept, we adjusted for this in the multivariate linear regression analysis (Table 3). We sequentially entered the other covariates of the RG-UK social capital scale in the following blocks: socio-demographic variables (model 1); clinical variables (model 2); DISC score for experienced discrimination (model 3); DISC anticipated discrimination items (model 4). The entry of experienced discrimination (model 3) into the model created a modest increase in Radj2 from 0.26 to 0.31. The only anticipated discrimination item to remain significant in the model was ‘choosing not to start a new relationship’, though this made a negligible contribution to the variation explained. The only clinical variable correlated with increased social capital in the final model was not having been an involuntary patient. All the socio-demographic variables remained significant. However, the overall variance explained by all the variables in the multivariate model was somewhat low (31%).

Table 3.

Linear regression analysis of correlates of RG-UK*

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
Variable β s.e. P β s.e. p β s.e. p β s.e. p
Age −0.06 0.02 <0.001 −0.05 0.02 0.007 −0.07 0.02 <0.001 −0.07 0.02 <0.001
Employed 2.34 0.45 <0.001 2.07 0.45 <0.001 1.48 0.44 0.001 1.35 0.44 0.003
Female gender 1.19 0.37 0.001 1.09 0.37 0.004 1.26 0.36 <0.001 1.27 0.36 <0.001
University graduate 0.77 0.40 0.058 0.77 0.40 0.054 0.79 0.39 0.040 0.83 0.39 0.032
Not involuntary patient 1.05 0.41 0.010 0.91 0.39 0.021 0.96 0.39 0.015
Length of contact with MH services −0.04 0.02 0.051 −0.02 0.02 0.338 −0.02 0.02 0.318
Diagnosis −0.27 0.22 0.216 −0.272 0.21 0.198 −0.24 0.21 0.264
Experienced discrimination −0.06 0.01 <0.001 −0.06 0.01 <0.001
Chose not to start work −0.32 0.39 0.410
Chose not to start education −0.07 0.43 0.863
Chose not to start a relationship −0.77 0.38 0.046
Concealed diagnosis 0.12 0.42 0.783
Constant 15.83 1.36 <0.001 14.31 1.64 <0.001 15.61 1.58 <0.001 15.63 1.59 <0.001
Model summary Radj2 = 0. 24 F = 50.83, Radj2 = 0. 26, F = 34.48, Radj2 = 0.31 F = 40.53, Radj2 = 0.31, F = 28.54,
p < 0.001 p < 0.001 p < 0.001 p < 0.001
*

Adjusted for human capital.

Next, we tested the hypothesis that experience of discrimination from particular social groups was correlated with lower access to social capital from that group. Table 4 shows that the experience of discrimination from any individual social group was associated with reduced access to social capital overall. However, experience of discrimination from friends and immediate family was associated with lower access to social capital from these groups, but this was not found for wider family, neighbours or mental health staff. The mean number of RG-UK items accessible from immediate family and friends were 6.11 (s.d. = 4.56) and 4.57 (s.d. = 4.35), respectively, in contrast to the next highest which was a mean of only 1.41 (s.d. = 2.51) RG-UK items from wider family. It appears that discrimination from social groups had a larger effect on access to social capital when a larger proportion of RG-UK items was accessible via that group.

Table 4.

Tie-specific discrimination and access to social capital

DISC experienced discrimination from: RG-UK items accessible via group with experience of discrimination RG-UK items accessible via anyone
n Mean (s.d.) N Mean (s.d.)
Friends 400 3.96 (3.98)*** 356 13.01 (6.01)***
None from friends 536 5.20 (4.61) 470 14.87 (5.86)
Neighbours 231 0.49 (1.03) 199 12.11 (5.98)***
None from neighbours 560 0.65 (1.28) 490 14.59 (5.87)
Family 444 5.86a (4.54)* 400 13.42 (5.78)**
None from family 518 6.59a (4.47) 456 14.49 (6.15)
Family 444 1.34b (2.27) 400 13.42 (5.78)**
None from family 518 1.47b (2.65) 456 14.49 (6.15)
Mental health staff 309 0.78 (1.49) 281 12.84 (6.06)***
None from mental health staff 679 0.87 (1.67) 594 14.43 (5.93)

Difference in means (assessed using t test): *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.

aMean RG-UK items accessible via immediate family.

bMean RG-UK items accessible via wider family.

Finally, we tested the hypothesis that domain-specific discrimination had differential effects on specific RG-UK domains. Experiencing discrimination from friends or family; in finding or keeping a job; in one's social life or being shunned by other people were thought to be associated with reduced access to social capital, given the importance of friends and family to access to social capital for our sample; a substantial literature on the effect of employment on social capital (Moerbeek & Flap, 2008); and the resourcefulness of weak ties (Granovetter, 1973). Each of the six DISC items representing these areas of experienced discrimination was associated with access to less social capital on the main RG-UK scale and on at least two sub-scales (Table 5). Experiencing discrimination from friends or in finding or keeping a job had an effect on access to social capital across all RG-UK sub-scales. However, experiencing discrimination in broader social life primarily had an effect on the domestic resources sub-scale of the RG-UK.

Table 5.

Domain-specific discrimination and access to social capital

DISC experienced discrimination: RG-UK total scale Domestic resources Expert advice Personal skills Problem-solving resources
n Mean (s.d.) n Mean (s.d.) n Mean (s.d.) n Mean (s.d.) n Mean (s.d.)
From friends 356 13.01 (6.02)*** 388 3.85 (2.05)** 366 3.76 (2.35)*** 394 2.61 (1.59)** 386 2.82 (1.29)*
None from friends 470 14.87 (5.86) 516 4.22 (1.91) 492 4.63 (2.32) 525 2.91 (1.64) 515 3.04 (1.26)
From family 400 13.42 (5.78)** 433 3.88 (1.97)* 416 4.03 (2.29)* 441 2.73 (1.60) 431 2.85 (1.27)*
None from family 456 14.49 (6.15) 497 4.19 (1.99) 474 4.38 (2.42) 507 2.84 (1.66) 498 3.02 (1.26)
In finding a job 174 12.77 (6.09)*** 184 3.58 (2.07)*** 183 4.01 (2.27)** 187 2.41 (1.59)*** 183 2.70 (1.31)***
None in finding a job 268 15.19 (6.05) 279 4.40 (1.91) 274 4.62 (2.48) 283 3.07 (1.70) 286 3.15 (1.25)
In keeping a job 154 13.52 (5.92)*** 163 3.86 (1.99)*** 160 4.19 (2.25)** 168 2.52 (1.63)*** 165 2.90 (1.33)**
None in keeping a job 274 16.14 (5.76) 283 4.63 (1.86) 281 4.99 (2.37) 286 3.32 (1.61) 287 3.26 (1.20)
In social life 289 13.69 (6.23)* 312 3.82 (2.00)*** 297 4.21 (2.40) 319 2.69 (1.64)* 311 2.87 (1.34)
None in social life 480 14.76 (5.79) 524 4.34 (1.91) 503 4.45 (2.31) 532 2.94 (1.61) 523 3.05 (1.21)
Shunned by other people 448 13.39 (5.98)** 491 3.87 (1.98)** 468 4.06 (2.30)** 502 2.67 (1.62)* 491 2.83 (1.31)*
Not shunned 391 14.72 (5.99) 427 4.26 (1.98) 403 4.49 (2.44) 433 2.89 (1.61) 426 3.04 (1.21)

Difference in means (assessed using t test): *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.

Discussion

This is the first study to show a correlation between experienced discrimination and reduced access to social capital in people with severe mental illness. Experienced discrimination made a modest contribution to the explained variance of social capital. Age, employment status and gender are known correlates of access to social capital among people with mental illness (Webber & Huxley, 2007; Dutt & Webber, 2010; Webber, 2010; Webber et al. 2011) and these also made a modest contribution to the variance explained. Therefore, if interventions were designed to increase access to social capital, they need to consider both reducing discrimination and supporting people to obtain employment where possible. However, it is important to note that our final model explained about one-third of the variance in social capital suggesting that other unmeasured variables need to be considered in future studies.

This study additionally found that people with experience of a previous involuntary admission to hospital under the Mental Health Act 1983 had reduced access to social capital, while controlling for the potential confounding effect of diagnosis and length of time known to mental health services. A history of involuntary admission is likely to be a proxy of severity of mental disorder, suggesting that provision of continuous and assertive community-based care to this group, including using interventions such as joint crisis plans (Henderson et al. 2004), may be required in order to help them maintain their social networks and enhance their recovery.

Our finding that the anticipated discrimination item ‘chose not to start a relationship’ had an independent relationship with access to social capital in the regression model suggests that people with severe mental illness reduced their social contact when they experienced discrimination, which reduced their access to social capital. Although this item made a negligible contribution to the variance explained, and reverse causality cannot be ruled out, this may provide tentative support for the findings of previous studies (e.g., Link et al. 2001; Moriarty et al. 2012).

People who experienced discrimination from friends or family had reduced access to social capital from these social groups, which was the most common source of social capital for people in this sample and the UK general population (Webber & Huxley, 2007). Psycho-education with those closest to people with severe mental illness may help to reduce the discrimination experienced by people with severe mental illness and prevent a loss in their access to social capital.

A response rate of only 11% limits the generalizability of these findings. The sample was limited to people who had been in touch with mental health services within the previous 6 months. It is therefore possible that a significant number of those invited to take part were acutely unwell at the time which reduced the response rate. It is also possible that people who did not experience any discrimination chose not to participate as they may have thought that the survey was not relevant to them. Further, the possibility of reverse causality cannot be ruled out. People with access to less social capital possibly recounted more experiences of discrimination as they attempted to understand why their social networks were restricted or provided them with access to fewer resources. Longitudinal studies are required to establish the direction of causality and to examine the impact of changes in experienced or anticipated discrimination on access to social capital. They are also required to determine how resources within people's networks can help to build resilience, which reduces the harmful effect of discrimination on mental health.

This study provides tentative evidence of an association between experienced discrimination and social capital, which appears important for health and wellbeing (Kawachi et al. 2007). Mental health services could help to reduce discrimination by supporting clinicians to practice in anti-discriminatory ways and to work closely with individuals' friends and families to minimize their discriminatory behaviour towards them. Often this behaviour is unintentional, such as lowering of expectations, avoiding social situations and increased paternalism that can impact negatively on an individual's recovery. In addition, supporting people with severe mental illness to increase their access to social capital may empower them in the face of experienced or anticipated discrimination.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the participating NHS Trusts, the Viewpoint interviewers, and the Time to Change management team.

Financial Support

This work was supported by the Big Lottery Fund; Comic Relief; and SHiFT (Shifting Attitudes to Mental Illness), UK Government Department of Health. Dr Henderson, Ms Corker, Ms Hamilton, Dr Rose and Dr Pinfold were supported by a grant to TTC from the Big Lottery Fund and Comic Relief and a grant from SHiFT. Dr Henderson and Dr Thornicroft are funded by a National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) applied programme grant RP-PG-0606-1053 awarded to the South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust. Dr Rose and Dr Thornicroft are also supported by the NIHR Specialist Mental Health Biomedical Research Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, and the South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust. Dr Henderson is also funded by a grant from the Guy's and St Thomas' Charity and a grant from the Maudsley Charity. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, NIHR or the UK Government Department of Health.

Conflict of Interest

Professor Thornicroft has received research grants from Lundbeck UK. The other authors report no competing interests.

References

  1. Aromaa E, Tolvanen A, Tuulari J, Wahlbeck K (2011a). Personal stigma and use of mental health services among people with depression in a general population in Finland. BMC Psychiatry 11, 52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aromaa E, Tolvanen A, Tuulari J, Wahlbeck K (2011b). Predictors of stigmatizing attitudes towards people with mental disorders in a general population in Finland. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry 65, 125–132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Beresford P (2002). Thinking about ‘mental health’: towards a social model. Journal of Mental Health 11, 581–584. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bourdieu P (1997). The forms of capital In Education, Culture, Economy, Society (ed. Halsey AH, Lauder H, Brown P and Stuart Wells A). Oxford University Press: Oxford, 46–58. [Google Scholar]
  5. Brohan E, Slade M, Clement S, Rose D, Sartorius N, Thornicroft G (in press). Development and psychometric validation of the Discrimination and Stigma Scale (DISC). Psychiatry Research (http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0165178113001388). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Burt RS (1992). Structural Holes. The Social Structure of Competition. Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA. [Google Scholar]
  7. Coleman J (1988). Social capital in the creation of human capital. American Journal of Sociology 94, S95–S120. [Google Scholar]
  8. Corker E, Hamilton S, Henderson C, Weeks C, Pinfold V, Rose D, Williams P, Flach C, Gill V, Lewis-Holmes E, Thornicroft G (2013). Experiences of discrimination among people using mental health services in England 2008–11. British Journal of Psychiatry 202 (Suppl. 55), s58–s63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Corrigan P, Markowitz F, Watson A (2004). Structural levels of mental illness stigma and discrimination. Schizophrenia Bulletin 30, 481–491. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. De Silva MJ, Mckenzie K, Harpham T, Huttly SRA (2005). Social capital and mental illness: a systematic review. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 59, 619–627. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Dutt K, Webber M (2010). Access to social capital and social support amongst South East Asian women with severe mental health problems: a cross-sectional survey. International Journal of Social Psychiatry 56, 593–605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Estrada EP, Tsai Y-M, Chandler CR (2008). Assimilation and discriminatory perceptions and experiences: the case of Hispanics in the United States. Social Science Journal 45, 673–681. [Google Scholar]
  13. Evans-Lacko S, Brohan E, Mojtabai R, Thornicroft G (2012a). Association between public views of mental illness and self-stigma among individuals with mental illness in 14 European countries. Psychological Medicine 42, 1741–1752. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Evans-Lacko S, London J, Japhet S, Rüsch N, Flach C, Corker E, Henderson C, Thornicroft G (2012b). Mass social contact interventions and their effect on mental health related stigma and intended discrimination. BMC Public Health 12, 489. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Flap H (1999). Creation and returns of social capital. A new research program. La Revue Tocqueville 20, 5–26. [Google Scholar]
  16. Goffman E (1970). Stigma: Notes on the Management of Spoilt Identity. Penguin: Harmondsworth. [Google Scholar]
  17. Goto SG, Gee GC, Takeuchi DT (2002). Strangers still? The experience of discrimination among Chinese Americans. Journal of Community Psychology 30, 211–224. [Google Scholar]
  18. Granovetter MS (1973). The strength of weak ties. American Journal of Sociology 78, 1360–1380. [Google Scholar]
  19. Hamilton S, Pinfold V, Rose D, Henderson C, Lewis Holmes E, Flach C, Thornicroft G (2011). The effect of disclosure of mental illness by interviewers on reports of discrimination experienced by service users: a randomised study. International Review of Psychiatry 23, 47–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Henderson C, Thornicroft G (2009). Stigma and discrimination in mental illness: time to change. Lancet 373, 1928–1930. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Henderson C, Thornicroft G (2013). Evaluation of the time to change programme in England 2008–2011. British Journal of Psychiatry 202 (Suppl. 55), s45–s48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Henderson C, Flood C, Leese M, Thornicroft G, Sutherby K, Szmukler G (2004). Effect of joint crisis plans on use of compulsory treatment in psychiatry: single blind randomised controlled trial. British Medical Journal 329, 136–138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Henderson C, Corker E, Lewis-Holmes E, Hamilton S, Flach C, Rose D, Williams P, Pinfold V, Thornicroft G (2012). England's time to change antistigma campaign: one-year outcomes of service user–rated experiences of discrimination. Psychiatric Services 63, 451–457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Her Majesty's Government (2012). Neighbourhood statistics: mental health – adults accessing specialist NHS mental health services, England.
  25. Kapungwe A, Cooper S, Mwanza J, Mwape L, Sikwese A, Kakuma R, Lund C, Flisher AJ (2010). Mental illness – stigma and discrimination in Zambia. African Journal of Psychiatry (South Africa) 13, 192–203. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kawachi I, Subramanian SV, Kim D (ed.) (2007). Social Capital and Health. Springer-Verlag: New York. [Google Scholar]
  27. Lin N (2001). Social Capital. A Theory of Social Structure and Action. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge. [Google Scholar]
  28. Lin N, Ao D (2008). The invisible hand of social capital: an exploratory study In Social Capital. An International Research Program (ed. Lin N and Erickson BH). Oxford University Press: Oxford, 107–132. [Google Scholar]
  29. Lin N, Dumin M (1986). Access to occupations through social ties. Social Networks 8, 365–385. [Google Scholar]
  30. Lin N, Erickson B (ed.) (2008). Social Capital. An International Research Program. Oxford University Press: Oxford. [Google Scholar]
  31. Lin N, Ensel WM, Vaughn JC (1981). Social resources and strength of ties: structural factors in occupational attainment. American Sociological Review 46, 393–405. [Google Scholar]
  32. Link BG, Phelan JC (2001). Conceptualizing stigma. Annual Review of Sociology 27, 363–385. [Google Scholar]
  33. Link BG, Struening EL, Neese-Todd S, Asmussen S, Phelan JC (2001). Stigma as a barrier to recovery: the consequences of stigma for the self-esteem of people with mental illnesses. Psychiatric Services 52 1621–1626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Mccallister L, Fischer C (1978). A procedure for surveying personal networks. Sociological Methods and Research 7, 131–148. [Google Scholar]
  35. Moerbeek H, Flap HD (2008). Social resources and their effect on occupational attainment through the life course In Social Capital. An International Research Program (ed. Lin N and Erickson BH). Oxford University Press: Oxford, 133–156. [Google Scholar]
  36. Moriarty A, Jolley S, Callanan M, Garety P (2012). Understanding reduced activity in psychosis: the roles of stigma and illness appraisals. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 47, 1685–1693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Murray J, Easter A, Bellringer S (2007). Evaluation of Capital Volunteering. Fourth Interim eport: Outcomes at Twelve Months. Health Service & Population Research Department, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London: London. [Google Scholar]
  38. Pérez DJ, Fortuna L, Alegría M (2008). Prevalence and correlates of everyday discrimination among U.S. Latinos. Journal of Community Psychology 36, 421–433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Putnam R (2000). Bowling Alone. The Collapse and Revival of American Community. Simon & Schuster: New York. [Google Scholar]
  40. Rose D, Willis R, Brohan E, Sartorius N, Villares C, Wahlbeck K, Thornicroft G. (2011). Reported stigma and discrimination by people with a diagnosis of schizophrenia. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences 20, 193–204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Slade M (2009). Personal Recovery and Mental Illness: A Guide for Mental Health Professionals. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge. [Google Scholar]
  42. Song L (2011). Social capital and psychological distress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 52, 478–492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Świtaj P, Wciórka J, Grygiel P, Anczewska M, Schaeffer E, Tyczyński K, Wiśniewski A (2012). Experiences of stigma and discrimination among users of mental health services in Poland. Transcultural Psychiatry 49, 51–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Tew J, Ramon S, Slade M, Bird V, Melton J, Le Boutillier C (2012). Social factors and recovery from mental health difficulties: a review of the evidence. British Journal of Social Work 42, 443–460. [Google Scholar]
  45. Thornicroft G (2006). Shunned: Discrimination Against People with Mental Illness. Oxford University Press: Oxford. [Google Scholar]
  46. Thornicroft G, Brohan E, Rose D, Sartorius N, Leese M (2009). Global pattern of experienced and anticipated discrimination against people with schizophrenia: a cross-sectional survey. Lancet 373, 408–415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Van Der Gaag M, Snijders TAB (2005). The Resource Generator: social capital quantification with concrete items. Social Networks 27, 1–29. [Google Scholar]
  48. Van Nieuwenhuizen A, Henderson C, Kassam A, Graham T, Murray J, Howard LM, Thornicroft G (2012). Emergency department staff views and experiences on diagnostic overshadowing related to people with mental illness. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 1–8. doi: 10.1017/S2045796012000571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Wahl O, Aroesty-Cohen E (2010). Attitudes of mental health professionals about mental illness: a review of the recent literature. Journal of Community Psychology 38, 49–62. [Google Scholar]
  50. Webber M (2005). Social capital and mental health In Social Perspectives in Mental Health. Developing Social Models to Understand and Work with Mental Distress (ed. Tew J). Jessica Kingsley Publishers: London, 90–111. [Google Scholar]
  51. Webber M (2010). Helping women with depression to access social capital: A role for social workers In Women and Mental Health (ed. Sekar A, Rajaram P, Shekhar R and Mubarak AR). National Institute of Mental Health and Neuro Sciences: Bangalore, 19–27. [Google Scholar]
  52. Webber M, Huxley P (2007). Measuring access to social capital: the validity and reliability of the Resource Generator-UK and its association with common mental disorder. Social Science and Medicine 65, 481–492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Webber M, Huxley P, Harris T (2011). Social capital and the course of depression: six-month prospective cohort study. Journal of Affective Disorders 129, 149–157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Zambon A, Morgan A, Vereecken C, Colombini S, Boyce W, Mazur J, Lemma P, Cavallo F (2010). The contribution of club participation to adolescent health: evidence from six countries. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 64, 89–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences are provided here courtesy of Cambridge University Press

RESOURCES