Abstract
Humans’ symbolic counting skills are built on a primordial ability to approximately estimate the number of items, or numerosity. To date it is debated whether numerosities presented in categorically different formats, that is as temporal sequences versus spatial arrays, are represented abstractly in the brain. To address this issue, we identified the behavioral characteristics and neuronal codes for sequential and simultaneous number formats in crows. We find a format-dependent representation by distinct groups of selective neurons during the sensory encoding stage. However, an abstract and format-independent numerosity code emerges once the encoding phase is completed and numerosities needed to be memorized. These results suggest a successive two-stage code for categorically different number formats and help to reconcile conflicting findings observed in psychophysics and brain imaging.
Subject terms: Cognitive neuroscience, Intelligence
Numbers are processed as abstract categories, despite considerable variations in presentation formats. By recording single-neuron activity in behaving crows, the authors show successive format-dependent and format-independent numerosity codes in the avian endbrain.
Introduction
Counting is a sophisticated cognitive capability inextricably tied to symbolic competence and therefore unique to humans. However, our advanced numerical skills are built on a primordial and evolutionarily ancient ability to approximately estimate the abstract number of items in a collection, its numerosity1,2. Comparative research, both in the wild and in the laboratory, has shown that different species of animals from diverse zoological groups, such as mammals3,4, birds5,6, reptiles7,8, amphibians9,10, fish11,12, and insects13,14, can discriminate numerosity.
The neurobiological foundations of numerical competence have best been studied in nonhuman and human primates. Here, the endbrain, most notably the six-layered cerebral cortex, contains a dedicated brain network for processing numerical information15. Neurons in these brain areas selectively respond to the number of objects in arrays and show tuned activity to preferred numbers16,17. Birds similarly exhibit sophisticated numerical abilities18–22, even though a radically different architecture emerged in their endbrains over 320 Mio years of parallel evolution23–25. Neurons in the crow endbrain area “nidopallium caudolaterale (NCL)”, a proposed equivalent of the primate prefrontal cortex (PFC)26,27, are activated by several cognitive factors28–30. NCL neurons also respond to the number of items that are displayed in spatial arrays and thus can be perceived from this specific, simultaneous number format31–33.
However, numerosity as an abstract quantity must also be represented when items are presented one-by-one in temporal succession, but its underlying neuronal substrate is unclear. Both psychophysical34–37 and brain imaging studies in humans38 report conflicting results and, depending on the respective findings, argue that sequential and simultaneous number formats access the same approximate number system, or different systems, respectively. However, because psychophysics only measures the output of subjects, and brain imaging explores metabolic brain changes at relatively low temporal and spatial resolution39, neither of these approaches alone is able to give a definitive answer. The only neuronal data available so far stem from the monkey parietal cortex and suggest an involvement of both format-dependent and -independent populations of neurons40. We therefore address the question of format-dependency in crows and set out to identify the neuronal codes for sequentially and simultaneously enumeration processes.
Results
Behavioral performance
We trained two crows on a delayed match-to-sample task in which they had to assess the number of (one to four) dots in a sample phase, memorize it over a brief delay period, and match it to the number of dots shown in the test phase (Fig. 1). The sample numerosities were displayed either as sequentially presented single dots (sequential format; Fig. 1a) or as simultaneous-presented dot arrays (simultaneous format; Fig. 1d). Both presentation formats were alternated in a pseudo-random fashion from trial to trial. To ensure that the crows discriminated the number of dots rather than non-numerical stimulus features, we applied standard and control conditions for both number formats (see Methods for details). Controls in the sequential presentation format eliminated temporal factors that may co-vary with increasing numbers of sequential items, such as the total duration of the sample period, the duration of individual items and pauses in between, the total visual energy (or total area across time, respectively), and the regularity (rhythm) of the item sequence (Fig. 1b). For the simultaneous format, the overall area and density of the dots of variable size that appeared at random locations on the gray background circle was equalized in control conditions (Fig. 1e).
We first determined and compared the crows’ performance to the sequential and simultaneous number formats, respectively, to explore behavioral evidence of a shared representational system. The crows’ average performance for the sequential (crow B: 70.9 ± 4.8%, crow J: 73.4 ± 4.0%) and simultaneous format (crow B: 77.7 ± 3.1%, crow J: 83.5 ± 2.3%) was significantly above chance for sample numerosities and presentation formats (Binomial test, p < 0.001). Performance to control conditions was better than to standard conditions for both formats (two-tailed t-test, p < 0.01), possibly a reflection of better familiarity with standard stimuli. Importantly, the percent correct performances for all numerosities, formats, and conditions were significantly better than chance (binomial test, p < 0.001). The crows extracted the numerical information from the stimuli and did not use co-varying non-numerical features.
The behavioral performance functions to sequential (Fig. 1c) and simultaneous numerosities (Fig. 1f) illustrate two classic psychophysical phenomena in number discrimination: first, the numerical distance effect is evidenced by the finding that adjacent numerosities were more difficult to discriminate for the crows, which resulted in bell-shaped performance curves. Second, the numerical size effect is reflected by the finding that, at a given numerical distance, larger numerosities were more difficult to discriminate for the crows than smaller ones, which caused a progressive widening of the performance curves with an increase in sample numerosity. These classical signatures of numerosity discrimination were present for both number formats (Fig. 1g). The similarity of the performance curves may suggest that crows engage a format-independent approximate number system when judging sequential and simultaneous numerosity formats. We next explored the neuronal foundations underlying these behavioral numerosity representations.
Single-neuron responses during the sample phase
While the crows performed the task, we recorded the activity of 376 single neurons (crow B: 117 neurons, crow J: 259 neurons) in their endbrain area NCL41 (crow B: 50 sessions, crow J: 76 sessions). Many neurons modulated their firing rate as a function of the number of items presented in the sample phase. Figure 2 depicts the response of four different neurons to the sequential format. Each neuron showed highest firing rates to one of the tested sample numerosities, such as one (Fig. 2a), two (Fig. 2d), three (Fig. 2g), or four (Fig. 2j). Importantly, the tuning of the four neurons was unaffected whether the standard or control protocols (“equal item duration”) were presented (Fig. 2b, e, h, k), or how many items were shown in a row (Fig. 2c, f, i, l).
We tested the selectivity of neurons based on average firing rates in correct trials with a two-factor ANOVA, with main factors “number of items (one to four)” and “protocol (standard and control)”, separately for the sequential and simultaneous format (criterion p < 0.01). Of the randomly recorded (i.e., not pre-selected) neurons, 17% (64/376 neurons) were numerosity selective for sequential numerosities (four of which shown in Fig. 2). A smaller proportion of 12% (45/376) of the neurons were numerosity selective for simultaneous numerosities.
Different selective neurons were tuned to different preferred numerosities in the two formats (Fig. 3a, b). We constructed average tuning curves by normalizing the individual tuning curves and pooling them according to preferred numerosity. Just as the behavioral performance functions, the average neuronal tuning functions showed a peak at their preferred numerosity and a progressive decline of activity with numerical distance from the preferred numerosity for both formats (Fig. 3c, d).
We wondered whether the numerosity-selective neurons might abstract across time and space during stimulus presentation to encode the same numerosity in both formats; alternatively, two specialized populations of neurons could encode the input number formats separately. We found the latter scenario realized: only 3.5% (13/376) of the sequentially and simultaneously tuned neurons were numerosity selective to both number formats, which was close to the calculated chance probability of 2.04% (Eq. (1)) (binomial test, p = 0.047). In addition, the neurons’ preferred numerosities in the two number formats was not correlated (r = 0.37 (Spearman correlation, p = 0.21)), meaning that these 13 neurons did not prefer similar numerosities across formats. This suggests that two independent neuronal populations in the NCL were responsible to encode sequential and simultaneous numerosities during the sample phase.
These neurons were behaviorally meaningful, because their responses were distorted whenever the crows made judgment errors (Fig. 3e, f). The firing rates to the preferred numerosities, which was 100% by definition for correct trials, were significantly reduced in error trials (sequential format: 84%, p = 0.006, n = 62; simultaneous format: 78%, p = 0.024, n = 38, both Wilcoxon signed rank test). Thus, if the neurons failed to encode their preferred numerosities adequately, then the crows were at risk of making judgment errors.
Single-neuron responses during the delay phase
After sample presentation, the crows had to memorize the numerosity for 1 s until the test came up. We tested whether neurons were selective to numerosity during the delay irrespective of the number format. Indeed, 10% (37/376) of all recorded neurons were exclusively selective to numerosity during the delay (two-factor ANOVA, p < 0.01). Figure 4a–c displays three such exclusively numerosity-tuned delay neurons preferring numerosity 4, 2, and 1. The time course of neuronal firing shows that these neurons responded similarly to their respective numerosity independent of presentation format, and particularly towards the end of the delay phase (Fig. 4a–c). A negligible fraction of neurons (3.4%, 13/376 neurons) was selective to both numerosity and presentation format.
As can be seen from the frequency plot, we found only numerosity selective neurons preferring each of the four presented numerosity during the delay period (Fig. 5a). Average tuning functions of all numerosity-selective delay neurons show a characteristic peak at their preferred numerosity and a drop-off for more distant numerosities (Fig. 5b), again a reflection of a neuronal numerical distance effect.
The Venn diagram in Fig. 5d summarizes the counts of numerosity-selective neurons found in the sample and delay periods. The numbers in the blue and pink circles show the numbers of neurons that were exclusively selective to the sequential (n = 64) and simultaneous format (n = 45), respectively, during the sample period. Only 13 neurons were selective to both formats in the sample period, thus emphasizing the engagement of separate neuron populations during the sample phase. The cell counts in the green circle depict neurons that were exclusively numerosity selective to both formats during the delay period (n = 37). The overlap of the green circle with the sample circles means that some of these delay-selective neurons were also selective during the previous sample period. Thus, the blue and pink cell counts on the one hand, and the green cell count on the other hand, were derived from two different trial intervals.
A negligible proportion of the neuron tuned in the delay phase were also tuned to numerosity during the sample phases of the two number formats (Fig. 5d), thus emphasizing the engagement of separate neuron populations.
The histograms of the example neurons (Fig. 4a–c) suggest that delay-selective neurons switch between different types of information they encode between the start and end of the delay phase. To quantify how much information about the sample numerosity and the format was conveyed by delay-selective neurons at any moment in the delay period, we performed a sliding-window percent-explained-variance analysis (ω2 explained variance; see Methods for details)). At the beginning of the delay period, the neurons primarily encode the format (Fig. 5c). Towards the end of the delay phase, format information strongly decreased and was replaced by numerosity information (Fig. 5c). Thus, just before the crows require information about the memorized numerosity to make a decision in the following test phase, numerosity became the dominant information carried by the neurons.
Neuronal population coding
So far, our single-neuron data suggest that one population of neurons, namely those cells that were exclusively numerosity selective to both formats during the delay period (n = 37), maintains numerical information in working memory independently from presentation formats. To explore to which extent these neuronal representations were indeed similar across formats, we applied a machine-learning algorithm using a linear multi-class support vector machine (SVM) as a classifier42. We first trained the classifier on firing rates recorded during the sequential number format and cross-validated the model with firing rates which were not used for training from the same presentation format. Then, we tested the classifier’s accuracy in predicting numerosity with firing rates recorded during the simultaneous format, and vice versa. Cross-validation accuracies for both formats was comparable (Fig. 6a, b) (sequential: 43.1% ± 5.5, standard deviation; simultaneous: 41.9% ± 4.3). The classifier trained on the sequential format predicted the correct numerosity in the simultaneous format with an accuracy of 42.8% (±3.8) (Fig. 6c), and significantly above chance level of 25%. Similarly, the classifier trained on the simultaneous format predicted the correct numerosity in the sequential format with an accuracy of 44.5% (±3.9) (Fig. 6d) (the 5th percentile for both classifiers was above the 95th percentile of their respective shuffled distribution). Based on the similar accuracies of both cross-validations and both tests with the other presentation format, we conclude that the classifier’s performance was equally well transferred from the sequential to the simultaneous, as from the simultaneous to the sequential format. The average classification probability functions show that classification errors mostly occur for neighboring numerosities (Fig. 6a–d). However, misclassification probabilities decreased with increasing numerical distance, a neuronal reflection of the behavioral numerical distance effect.
As a sign of the behavioral relevance of the neuronal population, activity recorded during the crows’ errors resulted in more classification errors compared to correct trials (Fig. 6e). Average classification performance in correct trials was 44.8% (±2.9), whereas in error trials it was 37.1% (±3.7). This suggests that neuronal discharges were behaviorally relevant and predicted the crows’ performance errors.
Finally, we analyzed the coding capacity and dynamics of the population of numerosity-selective delay neurons (n = 37) by performing a multi-dimensional state-space analysis (see Methods for details)43. Here, trajectories reflect the instantaneous firing rates of the neuronal populations as they evolve over time. For each preferred numerosity one to four, Fig. 7a–d displays the trajectories for the sequential and simultaneous format. For all numerosities, the trajectories representing sequential and simultaneous responses are spaced apart during the last 500 ms of the sample phase. This indicates that, although numerosity is represented during the last part of the sample period, the population also discriminates the formats and represents numerosity in a format-dependent way. However, during the course of the delay period, the trajectories converge and overlap, signifying that the population of neurons represented numberosity during working memory irrespective of the presentation format. This was quantified by measuring the Euclidean distances between the trajectories of the two number formats which converged for each numerosity (Fig. 7e). This indicates that information about the formats vanished in the delay period. Instead, the neuronal population discriminated better between the four numerosities, as demonstrated by increased Euclidean distances for increasing numerical distances (Fig. 7f).
Discussion
Behavioral experiments in humans and monkeys have demonstrated that numerosity judgment show identical characteristics for sequential and simultaneous presentation formats34,35. This was taken as evidence that judgments of numerosity are abstract and based on the same approximate number system (but see also refs. 36,37 for contrasting findings). In the current study, we find equivalent performance signatures when crows assessed sequential and simultaneous numerosity formats, suggesting that crows also engage a format-independent approximate number system. The enumeration of sequentially presented items required the crows not only to enumerate (or add) numerosity but also to keep track of the serial position of the presented single item. A study in newborn chicks in which sequential and simultaneous numerosity discrimination during arithmetic tasks was compared similarly showed that these birds do not differentiate between the two formats44.
Despite the same overall performance outcome, different neuronal mechanisms can give rise to behavioral equivalence. Whether number representations are format-dependent, format-independent, or a mixture of both, cannot be resolved from behavior alone. In fact, the neuronal data from crows argue for a neuronal two-stage process when these two fundamentally different number formats need to be represented. In each of these stages, different neuronal populations became engaged. First, during the sensory presentation stage, the number of sequentially presented items was extracted by one population of numerosity-tuned neurons, whereas the numerosity in dot arrays was represented by another population of numerosity-tuned neurons. These two numerosity-tuned populations showed strikingly similar neuronal coding characteristics but responded independently from each other to encode numerosity from the two formats. Both populations of neurons were behaviorally relevant and were predictive of erroneous trials for the respective number formats. At this sensory stage of number processing, neurons therefore responded format dependently.
However, once the encoding process was completed, a third population of numerosity-selective neurons became engaged. These delay-selective neurons represented numerosity irrespective of the presentation format. This third, format-independent population of neurons maintained numerical information in working memory and also predicted the crows’ performance success. In summary, sequential and simultaneous number formats engage different and temporally succeeding populations of format-dependent and format-independent numerosity-selective neurons. These neuronal results are in agreement with previous findings in the convergently and anatomically distinctly evolved neocortex of primates. In macaque monkeys trained to perform almost identical sequential and simultaneous enumeration tasks, single-neuron recordings in the ventral intraparietal area (VIP) of the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) revealed the same two-step processing stages and coding mechanisms40.
These findings in crows and monkeys help to explain seemingly equivocal results reported in human functional imaging. Several fMRI studies showed that arrays of dots, the most commonly used simultaneous number format, bilaterally activates areas of the IPS and prefrontal cortex in children45,46 and adults47–49. However, when simultaneous and sequential number formats were directly compared in the same participants, a different activation pattern emerged for the sequential presentation: in contrast to activation patterns for dot arrays, activations were restricted to the right hemispheric IPS and the inferior frontal gyrus during processing of flashed dot sequences38. These findings argue that simultaneous and sequential formats are partly segregated in the brain because they engage different cortical networks. Importantly, however, these cortical networks overlapped in two brain areas, namely the right IPS and the right precentral/frontal gyrus. It seems that the format-dependent sensory encoding stage engages segregated format-dependent neural networks, whereas the coding stage following sensory enumeration activates the same format-independent neurons in IPS, thus giving rise to an abstract number representation later in the task. The brain engages both format-dependent and format-independent neuron populations to deal with simultaneous and sequential set sizes, but each during different and successive processing stages of a number task.
Methods
Subjects
Two male hand-raised crows (Corvus corone corone) were trained on a match-to-numerosity task. Both crows were experienced with cognitive tasks from previous studies. Before the introduction of sequential numerosities, both crows proficiently judged simultaneous numerosity displays. The crows were housed in indoor aviaries in social groups. The crows were on a controlled feeding protocol during the training and recording period. Body weight was measured daily. The daily amount of food was given as reward during, or if necessary, after the sessions. Water was ad libitum available in the aviaries and during the experiments. All procedures were carried out according to the guidelines for animal experimentation and approved by the responsible national authorities, the Regierungspräsidium Tübingen, Germany.
Experimental setup
The experiment was conducted in a darkened operant conditioning chamber. The birds were perched in front of a touchscreen monitor (ART-Development PS-150, 15″, 60 Hz) that served for stimulus presentation. An automated feeder below the touchscreen delivered reward. The food reward consisted of food pellets and mealworms. A lamp on top of the feeder and speakers located behind the touchscreen provided additional feedback. An infrared light barrier activated by a reflector attached to the bird’s head ensured a stable head position in front of the screen throughout the trial. We used the CORTEX system (National Institute of Mental Health) to carry out the experiment and collect behavioral data. Neuronal data were recorded using a PLEXON system (Plexon Inc., Dallas, Texas).
Stimuli
The crows had to assess whether displays in the sample and test periods contained the same number of one to four items. The displays consisted of black dots (diameter range: 2–4 mm) on a gray background circle (diameter: 3 cm). The sample numerosity was either presented sequentially by a sequence of centered single dots interleaved by pauses (sequential format) or simultaneously as dot arrays in which the dots were shown at random positions and with varying sizes (2–4 mm diameter) on the gray background circle (simultaneous format). Twelve different dot displays per numerosity were used, and all displays were replaced after each session with displays newly generated by computer software (custom written MATLAB script, MathWorks, R2016a). Furthermore, sample and test images, the latter always dot arrays, were never identical. Both measures ensured that the crows were paying attention to the number of dots, and prevented them from performing visual pattern matching.
One standard and two controls conditions in the sequential presentation format ensured that the crows could not use co-varying temporal cues to solve the task. The three conditions were applied to vary/control for (a) sample period duration, (b) individual item or pause duration, (c) rhythm, and (b) intensity over time. In the standard sequential format, numerosities were shown with randomly generated timings, but with the following constraints: the entire sample duration (overall time of dot presentation and pauses) had to add up to 2625 ms and the minimum time for an item or pause had to be 300 ms in duration. The item timings were generated anew for each trial. Thus, in the standard condition, sample period duration was constant, individual item or pause duration decreased with numerosity, rhythm was irregular, and intensity over time was variable. In addition, we implemented two temporal control conditions that were alternatingly shown with the standard conditions: In the “equal variance” control conditions, sample period duration was constant (2625 ms), individual item or pause duration decreased with numerosity, rhythm was regular, and intensity over time decreased with numerosity. In the “equal item duration” control condition, sample period duration increased with numerosity, individual item or pause duration was constant across numerosities, rhythm was regular, and intensity over time increased with numerosity.
Similarly, standard and control conditions were applied in the simultaneous presentation format that prevented the crows from using co-varying visuo-spatial cues. In the standard conditions, non-overlapping dots of varying size were presented at varying locations on the gray background circle. In the control condition, the overall area of blackness by the dots as well as the density between the dots was equalized across numerosities. Density was calculated as the distances between the centers of the items. In contrast to our previous study31, we did not include a control for total contour length, or circumference, because this visual parameter only had a negligible effect on NCL neurons. In addition, the large number of stimulus conditions forced us to focus on the most important non-numerical parameters in order to record enough stimulus and condition repetitions proper neuronal statistics. Standard and control simultaneous conditions were shown with equal proportions in each session.
The presentation formats (sequential and simultaneous) were displayed pseudo-randomly and balanced in each session. In addition, the sample numerosities (1–4) were shown in a balanced and pseudo-randomized fashion.
Task
The birds were trained on a delayed match-to-sample task with the numbers of dots as discriminanda. The crow initiated a trial by positioning its head facing the monitor whenever a go-stimulus (white circle, 2° visual angle) was shown, thus closing an infrared light barrier, and maintaining this position throughout the trial. To indicate that the light barrier had been entered, the bird heard a click sound and the go-stimulus vanished. Whenever a crow made premature head movements and thereby left the light barrier during an ongoing trial, this trial was terminated.
After the go-stimulus, the main task started with an empty background circle for 500 ms. Next, the sample numerosity was displayed. The sample numerosity was pseudo-randomly either presented sequentially or simultaneously. To signify the end of sample presentation, the subsequent delay period was indicated by a green background circle shown for 1000 ms. A test stimulus followed the delay. The dots of the test numerosity were always presented simultaneously (dot array) and the appearance was never identical to the sample. The first test was either the same or a different numerosity as the sample. If the test was the same numerosity (match), the crow had to answer by moving the head, thus leaving the light barrier. If the first test showed a different numerosity smaller or larger than the sample numerosity (non-match), the crow was not allowed to move its head but remain in the light barrier for 800 ms longer until a second test display appeared that was always a match, and the crow needed to respond. Match and non-match trials were shown pseudo-randomized with equal proportions. Correct trials yielded a food reward; incorrect trials yielded a time-out (8 s). Aborted trials were repeated at a later, pseudo-random time point.
To sum up, two sequential (standard and control) and two simultaneous presentation formats (standard and control) were shown in match and non-match trials in a balanced and pseudo-random fashion per session. In the sequential format, the two control conditions (“equal variance” and “equal item duration”) alternated between sessions.
Surgery and recordings
The surgery was performed while the crow was under general anesthesia with a mixture of ketamine (50 mg/kg) and Rompun (5 mg/kg xylazine). The crow was placed in a stereotaxic holder. We targeted the medial part of NCL by performing a craniotomy at 5 mm anteriorposterior and 13 mm mediolateral on the right hemisphere. This part of NCL, termed mNCL, is known to contain highly associative neurons. Two manual microdrives containing four electrodes each (2 MΩ; Alpha Omega Co.) were implanted at the craniotomy. In addition, a miniature connector for the headstage and a small holder for attaching the reflector were implanted. Each recording session started with adjusting the electrodes until a proper neuronal signal was detected on at least one channel. The neurons were never pre-selected for any involvement in the task. Single-cell separation was done offline (Plexon Offline Sorter, version 2.8.8). No obvious anatomical organization of location preferences was detected.
Data analysis
All data analysis was carried out with MATLAB (MathWorks, R2016a). The neuronal population was defined as all single units that express a firing rate of at least 0.5 Hz and were present for at least three correct trials per condition (numerosity 1–4 × sequential/simultaneous × standard/control). A total of 376 neurons fulfilled these criteria and constitute the data base of this paper.
Numerosity selectivity in the sample phase was calculated separately for both presentation formats (sequential and simultaneous). A two-factor ANOVA with numerosity and standard/control as factors was used. Only neurons with a significant numerosity effect (p < 0.01) and no condition (standard/control) or interaction effect (p > 0.01) were called numerosity selective. The analysis windows for the ANOVA were shifted by 100 ms after stimulus onset (simultaneous presentation format) or item onset (sequential presentation format) to account for neuronal response latencies and lasted for the entire stimulus or item presentation duration, but maximally up to 800 ms. In the delay phase, numerosity selectivity was determined by an ANOVA as well; the factors were numerosity and presentation format. The analysis window was set to 600 ms after delay onset and lasted for 500 ms. Standard numerosity 1 trials were omitted for the analysis, since these trials had a prolonged pre-sample phase to introduce some variance (only the firing rates of numerosities 2, 3, and 4 during the first dot presentation were used).
Tuning functions of numerosity-selective neurons were normalized to be between 0 (minimum firing rate; 0%) and 1 (maximum firing rate; 100%). All normalized tuning functions of individual neurons that had the same preferred numerosity were then averaged. To evaluate whether tuning properties (i.e. their preferred numerosity) were similar for neurons that were numerosity selective in both sample presentation formats, we tested the correlation of their preferred numerosity in both presentation formats with a Spearman’s correlation coefficient.
The probability of how many neurons are expected to be numerosity selective by chance during both presentation formats in the sample phase was calculated with:
1 |
with denoting the probability that a neuron is numerosity selective in the sample phase in both presentation formats, P(seq) the probability of being numerosity selective in the sequential presentation format and P(sim) for the simultaneous format.
Error analysis for the sample phase included all sample phase selective numerosity neurons that were present for at least three error trials for their preferred numerosity. In the sequential presentation format, this was the case for 62/64 neurons, in the simultaneous format for 38/45. Differences in firing rates between correct and error trials were tested with a Wilcoxon signed rank test.
The percent-explained variance (PEV) analysis served to quantify the information content about numerosity and presentation format over the time course of the delay. For each numerosity-selective delay neuron (n = 37), we used a sliding-window analysis (300 ms duration, 20 ms steps) and started 200 ms prior to delay onset. For each time window, we calculated ω² with a two-factorial ANOVA to test the relationship between firing rates and stimulus category, and repeated this process with shuffled labels 1000 times (permutation test) to obtain a null distribution. The null distribution was obtained for each factor, namely numerosity, presentation format, and their interaction. To illustrate the explained variance for the entire population, we averaged the obtained ω² values from each neuron and time window as well as the ω² values from the null distributions. A given ω² value was plotted at the time point from the middle of the sliding window. We determined the populations’ information content to be significant, when the PEV value (ω²) was above the 95th percentile of the null distribution.
We also used multi-class SVMs. The aim of building and testing a decoder was to see how similar numerosities are represented across presentation formats. We included neurons that were numerosity selective in the delay phase, and were recorded for at least 32 correct trials and 1 error trial per numerosity and presentation format (n = 20 neurons). We excluded erroneous numerosity 1 trials from testing, since only few neurons had any error trials for this condition.
The multi-class SVM (LIBSVM toolbox for MATLAB, version 3.23) was trained with firing rates from one of the presentation formats (only correct trials). Default parameters from the toolbox delivered the best results, except that we used a polynomial kernel with one degree—which is linear—and used probability estimates for classification. Firing rates were averages taken 700 ms after delay onset for 400 ms. The model was cross-validated with the “leave-one-out” method, where one trial of every condition is left out for later testing and the other 31 trials are used to build the model. Before building, the firing rates were neuron-wise z-scored; cross-validation and test sets were normalized with µ and σ obtained from z-scoring the training set. This process repeats 32 times, so that every trial serves as test trial once. After the cross-validation process, we build a model with all 32 trials per condition. This model is then tested with firing rates from the other presentation format in correct trials. The procedure was executed for both presentation formats; the model was built with sequential and tested with simultaneous trials, and vice versa. The depicted classification performances in confusion matrices show how likely the classifier predicts the true numerosity or confuses it with neighboring ones. Classification performance curves are averaged over iterations and presentation formats. This whole process of building, cross-validating, and testing repeats 100 times. In every iteration, new random trials for building and testing are drawn from each neuron per condition.
Finally, we wanted to see how the classifier performed on error trials, i.e. trials in which the crows responded wrongly. Therefore, we build a classifier with data from both presentation formats. Hence, for each numerosity, we used 32 sequential and 32 simultaneous trials from the 20 neurons to train the classifier. We applied a cross-validation just as before and then tested the model with error trials. For this testing, we randomly drew two error trials per numerosity (one error trial per presentation format) and had the model predict the shown numerosity. Since we only take two error trials per numerosity, but many neurons for which more than one error trial per numerosity and presentation format was recorded, we repeated the process of drawing two random error trial per condition 32 times. This served to get a more reliable error decoding performance. This process was repeated 100 times to account for the randomness of trials that were selected.
Differences in classification performances between correct (from cross-validation) and error trials were quantified with an AUC (area under the receiver operating curve). This measure is independent of the number of data points used. It gives a measure of how well two distributions are separable. A value of 0.5 means that the distributions are completely overlapping and therefore not separable; a 1 means that the distributions perfectly separable.
A population state-space analysis served to reveal population dynamics that might be hidden at the single unit level. At each point in time, the activity of n recorded neurons is defined by a point in n-dimensional space, with each dimension representing the activity of a single neuron. Dimensionality reduction to the first three dimensions (that explain 48% of the neuronal covariance) using a principal component analysis results in trajectories that are traversed for different neuronal states, i.e. the four numerosities and the two number formats. These trajectories reflect the instantaneous firing rates of the respective neuronal population as they evolve over time.
For the population state-space analysis, we used all numerosity-selective delay neurons (n = 37). For each neuron, spike trains were averaged across trials over of the same condition (numerosity × presentation format), then smoothed with 150 ms Gaussian kernel, binned (300 ms, in 25 ms steps), and finally neuron-wise z-scored before calculating the principal components. Z-scoring prevents the state-space dynamics to be monopolized by only a few highly discriminative single neurons. The trajectories start 600 ms before delay onset and end 200 ms after delay offset. In the plots are the first three dimensions shown. To evaluate how population dynamics change over time in respect to presentation format, we calculated the Euclidean distances over the entire n-dimensional space (37-dimensional space) from the trajectories that represent the same numerosity in different presentation formats. Furthermore, to see how numerical information evolves during the course of the delay, we calculated the Euclidean distances between each pair of numerosities within the presentation formats (SEQ: 1v2, 1v3, 1v4, 2v3, 2v4, 3v4; SIM: 1v2, 1v3, 1v4, 2v3, 2v4, 3v4). Then we pooled the Euclidean distances from pairs with the same numerical distance across presentation format, e.g. the Δ2 trajectory is an average of the distances SEQ: 1v3 and 2v4, and SIM: 1v3 and 2v4.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by a DFG grant NI 618/3-1 to A.N.
Author contributions
A.N. and H.M.D. designed the study, interpreted the data, and wrote the manuscript. H.M.D. performed experiments and analyzed the data. A.N. supervised the study.
Data availability
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
Code availability
The code that supports the findings of this study is available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Peer review information Nature Communications thanks Michael Colombo and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Peer reviewer reports are available.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Supplementary information is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41467-020-14519-2.
References
- 1.Cantlon JF, Brannon EM. Shared system for ordering small and large numbers in monkeys and humans. Psychol. Sci. 2006;17:401–406. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01719.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Merten K, Nieder A. Compressed scaling of abstract numerosity representations in adult humans and monkeys. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2009;21:333–346. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.McComb K, Packer C, Pusey A. Roaring and numerical assessment in contests between groups of female lions, Panthera leo. Anim. Behav. 1994;47:379–387. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1994.1052. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Brannon EM, Terrace HS. Ordering of the numerosities 1 to 9 by monkeys. Science. 1998;282:46–49. doi: 10.1126/science.282.5389.746. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Scarf D, Hayne H, Colombo M. Pigeons on par with primates in numerical competence. Science. 2011;334:1664. doi: 10.1126/science.1213357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Ditz HM, Nieder A. Numerosity representations in crows obey the Weber-Fechner law. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2016;283:20160083. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2016.0083. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Miletto Petrazzini ME, Bertolucci C, Foà A. Quantity discrimination in trained lizards (Podarcis sicula) Front. Psychol. 2018;9:274. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Gazzola A, Vallortigara G, Pellitteri-Rosa D. Continuous and discrete quantity discrimination in tortoises. Biol. Lett. 2018;14:20180649. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2018.0649. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Uller C, Jaeger R, Guidry G, Martin C. Salamanders (Plethodon cinereus) go for more: rudiments of number in an amphibian. Anim. Cogn. 2003;6:105–112. doi: 10.1007/s10071-003-0167-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Stancher G, Rugani R, Regolin L, Vallortigara G. Numerical discrimination by frogs (Bombina orientalis) Anim. Cogn. 2015;18:219–229. doi: 10.1007/s10071-014-0791-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Agrillo C, Piffer L, Bisazza A, Butterworth B. Evidence for two numerical systems that are similar in humans and guppies. PLoS One. 2012;7:e31923. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0031923. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Potrich D, Sovrano VA, Stancher G, Vallortigara G. Quantity discrimination by zebrafish (Danio rerio) J. Comp. Psychol. 2015;29:388–393. doi: 10.1037/com0000012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Dacke M, Srinivasan MV. Evidence for counting in insects. Anim. Cogn. 2008;11:683–689. doi: 10.1007/s10071-008-0159-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Howard SR, Avarguès-Weber A, Garcia JE, Greentree AD, Dyer AG. Numerical ordering of zero in honey bees. Science. 2018;360:1124–1126. doi: 10.1126/science.aar4975. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Nieder A. A Brain for Numbers: The Biology of the Number Instinct. Cambridge, USA: MIT Press; 2019. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Nieder A. The neuronal code for number. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2016;17:366–382. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kutter EF, Bostroem J, Elger CE, Mormann F, Nieder A. Single neurons in the human brain encode numbers. Neuron. 2018;100:753–761. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.08.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Xia L, Emmerton J, Siemann M, Delius JD. Pigeons (Columba livia) learn to link numerosities with symbols. J. Comp. Psychol. 2001;115:83. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.115.1.83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Hunt S, Low J, Burns KC. Adaptive numerical competency in a food-hoarding songbird. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2008;275:2373–2379. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.0702. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Rugani R, Regolin L, Vallortigara G. Discrimination of small numerosities in young chicks. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 2008;34:388–399. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.34.3.388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Ujfalussy D, Miklósi A, Bugnyar T, Kotrschal K. Role of mental representations in quantity judgments by jackdaws (Corvus monedula) J. Comp. Psychol. 2013;128:11–20. doi: 10.1037/a0034063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Bogale BA, Aoyama M, Sugita S. Spontaneous discrimination of food quantities in the jungle crow, Corvus macrorhynchos. Anim. Behav. 2014;94:73–78. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.05.012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kumar S, Hedges SB. A molecular timescale for vertebrate evolution. Nature. 1998;392:917–920. doi: 10.1038/31927. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Hedges SB. The origin and evolution of model organisms. Nat. Rev. Genet. 2002;3:838–849. doi: 10.1038/nrg929. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Wang Z, et al. The draft genomes of soft-shell turtle and green sea turtle yield insights into the development and evolution of the turtle-specific body plan. Nat. Genet. 2013;45:701–706. doi: 10.1038/ng.2615. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Güntürkün O. The avian ‘prefrontal cortex’ and cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2005;15:686–693. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2005.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Nieder A. Inside the corvid brain—probing the physiology of cognition in crows. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2017;2017:8–14. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.02.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Moll FW, Nieder A. Cross-modal associative mnemonic signals in crow endbrain neurons. Curr. Biol. 2015;25:2196–2201. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.07.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Veit L, Pidpruzhnykova G, Nieder A. Associative learning rapidly establishes neuronal representations of upcoming behavioral choices in crows. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2015;112:15208–15213. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1509760112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Rinnert P, Kirschhock ME, Nieder A. Neuronal correlates of spatial working memory in the endbrain of crows. Curr. Biol. 2019;29:2616–2624. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.06.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Ditz HM, Nieder A. Neurons selective to the number of visual items in the corvid songbird endbrain. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2015;112:7827–7832. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1504245112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Ditz HM, Nieder A. Sensory and working memory representations of small and large numerosities in the crow endbrain. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:12044–12052. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1521-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Wagener L, Loconsole M, Ditz HM, Nieder A. Neurons in the endbrain of numerically naive crows spontaneously encode visual numerosity. Curr. Biol. 2018;28:1090–1094. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.02.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Barth H, Kanwisher N, Spelke E. The construction of large number representations in adults. Cognition. 2003;86:201–221. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00178-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Barth H, La Mont K, Lipton J, Spelke ES. Abstract number and arithmetic in preschool children. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2005;102:14116–14121. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0505512102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Castronovo J, Seron X. Numerical estimation in blind subjects: evidence of the impact of blindness and its following experience. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2007;33:1089–1106. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.5.1089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Castronovo J, Delvenne JF. Superior numerical abilities following early visual deprivation. Cortex. 2013;49:1435–1440. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.12.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Dormal V, Andres M, Dormal G, Pesenti M. Mode-dependent and mode-independent representations of numerosity in the right intraparietal sulcus. NeuroImage. 2010;52:1677–1686. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Nieder A. The number domain—can we count on parietal cortex? Neuron. 2004;44:407–409. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.10.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Nieder A, Diester I, Tudusciuc O. Temporal and spatial enumeration processes in the primate parietal cortex. Science. 2006;313:1431–1435. doi: 10.1126/science.1130308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Sen S, Parishar P, Pundir AS, Reiner A, Iyengar S. The expression of tyrosine hydroxylase and DARPP-32 in the house crow (Corvus splendens) brain. J. Comp. Neurol. 2019;527:1801–1836. doi: 10.1002/cne.24649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Chang C-C, Lin C-J. LIBSVM: a library for support vector machines. ACM Trans. Intell. Syst. Technol. 2011;2:27. doi: 10.1145/1961189.1961199. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Cunningham JP, Yu BM. Dimensionality reduction for large-scale neural recordings. Nat. Neurosci. 2014;17:1500–1509. doi: 10.1038/nn.3776. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Rugani R, Fontanari L, Simoni E, Regolin L, Vallortigara G. Arithmetic in newborn chicks. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2009;276:2451–2460. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.0044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Cantlon JF, Brannon EM, Carter EJ, Pelphrey KA. Functional imaging of numerical processing in adults and 4-y-old children. PLoS Biol. 2006;4:e125. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Kersey AJ, Cantlon JF. Neural tuning to numerosity relates to perceptual tuning in 3-6-year-old children. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:512–522. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0065-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Piazza M, Izard V, Pinel P, Le Bihan D, Dehaene S. Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the human intraparietal sulcus. Neuron. 2004;44:547–555. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.10.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Eger E, et al. Deciphering cortical number coding from human brain activity patterns. Curr. Biol. 2009;19:1608–1615. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.08.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Harvey BM, Klein BP, Petridou N, Dumoulin SO. Topographic representation of numerosity in the human parietal cortex. Science. 2013;341:1123–1126. doi: 10.1126/science.1239052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
The code that supports the findings of this study is available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.