Abstract
Background
The hybridizing field crickets, Gryllus firmus and Gryllus pennsylvanicus have several barriers that prevent gene flow between species. The behavioral pre-zygotic mating barrier, where males court conspecifics more intensely than heterospecifics, is important because by acting earlier in the life cycle it has the potential to prevent a larger fraction of hybridization. The mechanism behind such male mate preference is unknown. Here we investigate if the female cuticular hydrocarbon (CHC) profile could be the signal behind male courtship.
Results
While males of the two species display nearly identical CHC profiles, females have different, albeit overlapping profiles and some females (between 15 and 45%) of both species display a male-like profile distinct from profiles of typical females. We classified CHC females profile into three categories: G. firmus-like (F; including mainly G. firmus females), G. pennsylvanicus-like (P; including mainly G. pennsylvanicus females), and male-like (ML; including females of both species). Gryllus firmus males courted ML and F females more often and faster than they courted P females (p < 0.05). Gryllus pennsylvanicus males were slower to court than G. firmus males, but courted ML females more often (p < 0.05) than their own conspecific P females (no difference between P and F). Both males courted heterospecific ML females more often than other heterospecific females (p < 0.05, significant only for G. firmus males).
Conclusions
Our results suggest that male mate preference is at least partially informed by female CHC profile and that ML females elicit high courtship behavior in both species. Since ML females exist in both species and are preferred over other heterospecific females, it is likely that this female type is responsible for most hybrid offspring production.
Keywords: Mate choice, Hybrid zone, Pre-mating barrier, Speciation, Introgression
Background
To fully understand mate choice and its influence on speciation, we need to understand the mechanisms behind this choice. Mate choice in the form of preference for conspecifics is a pre-zygotic barrier that prevents gene flow between species and is important because by acting early in the life cycle, it can prevent more gene flow than other later acting barriers [10]. While mate choice has traditionally been almost synonymous with female mate choice [28], male mate choice, or preference, has now been reported even in species with little male parental care [15]. Males exhibit differential courtship behavior to females based on various traits such as size [4, 16, 22], relatedness [6, 35, 56] and species membership [25, 38, 44].
The morphologically and behaviorally similar hybridizing field crickets, Gryllus firmus [53] and Gryllus pennsylvanicus [19], provide an opportunity to better understand the role of male mate preferences in reproductive isolation. These two species form a well-described mosaic hybrid zone [20, 30, 48] and have several barriers that limit gene exchange [14, 19, 31, 37]. Their pre-mating barrier is largely explained by differential male courtship; males court conspecific females more readily and intensely than they court heterospecifics [38]. Therefore, while female crickets ultimately decide whether or not to mate (as they have to mount the male), male courtship intensity plays a significant role in their decision and females often mate with intensely courting males and will never mate a non-courting male [38].
While barriers to gene exchange are well described in these crickets, the mechanism behind this male mate preference is not understood. While morphological differences are often used in mate recognition, this is unlike to be the case between the morphologically similar G. firmus and G. pennsylvanicus which might instead use chemical signals such as cuticular hydrocarbons (CHCs). These compounds serve as contact pheromones to a wide variety of insects [2, 11, 12, 17, 33, 34, 50], are sexually dimorphic in many species [8, 9, 24, 59], including field crickets [38, 42, 61–64, 67], and are used for mate choice in various insect species [23, 45, 54, 59, 60]. In G. firmus and G. pennsylvanicus CHC composition is different between sexes however, while males of both species share the same composition, females of the two species are different but overlapping [38]. Furthermore, a subset of females from both species (between 15 and 45% based on this and previous data) exhibits a pattern that is typical of that of a male (male-like females, ML), the relevance of this pattern is unknown. This female CHC profile variability, with both unique and shared patterns between species, suggests that it could be the mechanism behind male mate recognition and could thus explain why males sometimes, but not always, court heterospecifics.
Our goal is to test the hypothesis that female CHC profile informs male mate preferences in the hybridizing field crickets G. firmus and G. pennsylvanicus. We show that males of both species mate with heterospecific ML (male-like) females at a higher rate than other heterospecifics, suggesting that males can indeed detect and use CHC information for courtship decisions. Therefore, it is possible that ML (male-like) females produce most of the hybrid offspring in the hybrid zone.
Results
Cuticular hydrocarbon analysis
For the gas chromatography analysis, we used the similar methods of Maroja et al. [38], we scored 17 peaks (Table 1) in 259 individuals (GP♂: n = 67, GP♀: n = 65, GF♂: n = 68, GF♀: n = 59). Males typically have fewer peaks than females with less variation between individuals (Table 1). Size (GP♂ 5.67 ± 0.47 cm, GP♀:5.90 ± 0.30 cm, GF♂: 5.42 ± 0.42 cm, GF♀: 5.76 ± 0.34 cm) was significantly different between sexes (F1, 249 = 35.0, p < 0.001) and between species (F1, 249 = 16.6, p < 0.001), but there is no significant interaction between sex and species (F1, 249 = 1.4, p = 0.24) in a two-way ANOVA, however, unlike previous studies, G. pennsylvanicus were the larger species in our sample [30].
Table 1.
PEAKS | GP ♂ (n = 67) | GP ♀ (n = 65) | GF ♂(n = 68) | GF ♀ (n = 59) | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | % zero | Mean | SD | % zero | Mean | SD | % zero | Mean | SD | % zero | |
F0 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 100.00 | 10.28 | 12.61 | 8.96 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 100.00 | 0.17 | 0.38 | 77.97 |
F4 | 0.02 | 0.06 | 89.55 | 1.16 | 1.16 | 14.93 | 0.24 | 0.42 | 48.53 | 1.47 | 1.40 | 22.03 |
M0 | 2.78 | 1.37 | 2.99 | 17.36 | 13.22 | 1.49 | 2.38 | 1.38 | 0.00 | 4.62 | 3.79 | 10.17 |
F5 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 100.00 | 7.84 | 9.55 | 23.88 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 100.00 | 2.84 | 3.54 | 25.42 |
M1 | 8.53 | 4.10 | 2.99 | 7.69 | 4.66 | 0.00 | 8.33 | 5.17 | 0.00 | 6.79 | 3.68 | 1.69 |
F8 | 1.37 | 0.85 | 4.48 | 3.37 | 2.94 | 1.49 | 1.10 | 0.73 | 4.41 | 3.02 | 1.89 | 5.08 |
M3 | 4.25 | 3.36 | 14.93 | 2.77 | 2.04 | 13.43 | 6.48 | 4.18 | 14.71 | 5.03 | 2.82 | 3.39 |
M4 | 17.81 | 17.07 | 38.81 | 14.30 | 14.98 | 31.34 | 24.01 | 15.63 | 19.12 | 6.44 | 9.96 | 37.29 |
M5 | 33.86 | 16.50 | 10.45 | 10.65 | 11.06 | 4.48 | 25.87 | 12.26 | 7.35 | 6.93 | 8.89 | 16.95 |
M6 | 16.66 | 9.26 | 7.46 | 10.85 | 4.14 | 0.00 | 15.38 | 7.35 | 1.47 | 19.12 | 7.73 | 3.39 |
F10 | 0.02 | 0.08 | 92.54 | 3.44 | 4.15 | 16.42 | 0.04 | 0.14 | 91.18 | 8.93 | 6.46 | 3.39 |
M7 | 8.03 | 8.12 | 38.81 | 4.42 | 4.56 | 34.33 | 9.08 | 6.49 | 22.06 | 11.37 | 5.31 | 1.69 |
F11 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 100.00 | 0.20 | 0.59 | 70.15 | 0.03 | 0.09 | 91.18 | 1.21 | 0.93 | 20.34 |
F12 | 0.35 | 1.95 | 74.63 | 0.85 | 1.37 | 35.82 | 1.55 | 1.48 | 13.24 | 8.18 | 9.22 | 3.39 |
M8 | 6.28 | 8.14 | 38.81 | 3.20 | 4.49 | 40.30 | 5.28 | 5.76 | 42.65 | 2.61 | 3.64 | 49.15 |
F13 | 0.04 | 0.26 | 97.01 | 1.47 | 3.51 | 44.78 | 0.01 | 0.07 | 95.59 | 6.73 | 6.30 | 18.64 |
F14 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 100.00 | 0.16 | 0.49 | 85.07 | 0.22 | 0.81 | 86.76 | 4.54 | 3.52 | 10.17 |
As reported before [38], the first two principal components of relative CHC peak proportion (percent of each peak) and composition (presence or absence of peak) were less varied within males of both species with the area of the convex hull for males equal to 4.8 and 25.3 as compared to 46.6 and 38.8 for females, for peak proportion and composition respectively (Fig. 1). Moreover, while females had significantly different CHC profiles between species [38], some of the profiles were overlapping between species and some females exhibited male-like profiles (Figure 4 in Appendix and Fig. 1). Male or female size were not associated with CHC profile and therefore were not included in the analyses.
Using the results from a cluster analysis of female CHC profile, we classified females into three distinct CHC categories (Fig. 1): “F” (G. firmus females, all but 2 individuals in this category are G. firmus, n = 29), “P” (G. pennsylvanicus only, all individuals in this category are G. pennsylvanicus, n = 23), and “ML” (females that have male-like CHC profiles – both species are in this category, n = 32 for G. firmus and n = 40 for G. pennsylvanicus). Since these two of the clusters correlate well with female species, we focus on the third cluster of females that have male-like profiles and consider female species in further analyses.
Courtship success and female profile types
Among pairings with G. firmus males, the proportion of courtship success was greatest with conspecific females (85%), followed by male-like (ML) heterospecific females (79%) and then the lowest success rate (66%) was with heterospecific females that do not have a male-like CHC profile (Fig. 2). The highest rate of courtship initiation with G. pennsylvanicus males happened with heterospecific ML females (68%) and conspecific females (66%).
A binomial logistic mixed effects model was fitted to the data to test whether the courtship success of G. firmus or G. pennsylvanicus males was impacted by female species and CHC categories while accounting for the baseline variation between individual crickets. Figure 2 shows estimated probabilities of courtship success with associated 95% confidence intervals. In general, among conspecific pairs, courtship success is similar between CHC profiles (GF: p = 0.31, GP: p = 0.68), but in heterospecific pairings, success was more likely for male-like CHC profiles (GF: p < 0.001, GP: p = 0.80) (Fig. 2). Table 2 gives the estimated odds ratios of courtship success between male-like and not male-like CHC profile for each male species and type of pair crossing. For conspecific pairings, we estimated that courtship was less likely for male-like females than those with species-specific CHC profiles, but we do not have enough evidence to claim a statistical discernable difference. For heterospecific pairings, the odds of male G. firmus crickets to engage in courting behavior with a female bearing the male-like profile were 2.58 times more that of a female with a species-specific profile (95% CI: 1.314, 5.079). For male G. pennsylvanicus crickets, we estimate the odds of courtship success with a female bearing a male-like profile was 1.1 times that of a female with a species-specific CHC profile, but we do not have the power to claim a statistical discernable difference (95% CI: 0.604, 2.247).
Table 2.
Female CHC Category | Courtship Success Odds Ratio (95% CI) | Courtship Time Hazard Ratio (95% CI) | |
---|---|---|---|
Conspecific Pairs | |||
Male Species: GF | ML v. Not ML | 0.781 (0.337, 1.673) | 1.176 (0.858, 1.613) |
Male Species: GP | ML v. Not ML | 0.957 (0.496, 1.846) | 0.910 (0.645, 1.283) |
Heterospecific Pairs | |||
Male Species: GF | ML v. Not ML | 2.583 (1.314, 5.079) | 1.610 (1.166, 2.222) |
Male Species: GP | ML v. Not ML | 1.165 (0.604, 2.247) | 1.039 (0.731, 1.478) |
Random Intercepts | Binomial Logistic Mixed Effects Estimated SD | Cox Proportional Hazard Mixed Effects Estimated SD | |
Male ID | 1.435 | 0.714 |
Binomial logistic mixed effects regression analysis of courtship success odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals and the estimated standard deviations of random intercepts are reported for males. Cox proportional hazard mixed effects hazard ratios and 95% confidence intervals and the estimated standard deviations of random intercepts for males. Hazard ratios are interpreted as the relative courtship rate at a time t of one group as compared to another. For example, at any point in the trial, G. firmus males are 1.610 more likely to start courting with a heterospecific females with a male-like (ML) CHC profile then females with any other CHC profile
Time to initiate courtship
To analyze the time males took to court females, we measured how long males took to initiate the courtship call. A survival analysis was fitted to this call time data for both G. firmus and G. pennsylvanicus males to estimate the probability that a male has not yet courted at a given time for each female CHC category. Survival analysis models time to event data, and in this context, the event is successful courtship. For G. firmus males, conspecific females were courted significantly more quickly than heterospecific females (Fig. 3). We also note that G. firmus males were significantly more like to quickly court a male-like heterospecific female than another heterospecific female. For G. pennsylvanicus males, the estimated Kaplan-Meier curves are not significantly different across the female species and male-like CHC categories.
The Cox proportional hazards mixed effect models the hazard function, which is the instantaneous rate of successfully courtship at a given time, as a function of male species, type of pair crossing, male-like or not male-like CHC category, and all of their interactions. At any point in the trial, we estimated G. firmus males were 1.61 (95% CI: 1.166, 2.22) times more likely to start courting ML profile females than females with other profiles in heterospecific pairings and while not significant, the estimated chance of courtship initiation with ML profile females was 1.176 (95% CI: 0.858, 1.613) times that of other profile females in conspecific pairings (Table 2). No significant differences in the time to courtship events were detected among female CHC categories for G. pennsylvanicus males.
Discussion
Our study suggests that female CHC profile can inform male mate preferences at least in one of the hybridizing field cricket species: G. firmus. Gryllus firmus males treated females with distinct CHC profiles differently and, even after controlling for female species (heterospecific vs. conspecific females), male G. firmus had a higher courtship success with heterospecific ML (male-like) than other heterospecific females. While differences were not significant for G. pennsylvanicus males, the direction of the preference was also towards ML females.
To our knowledge, this is the first report pointing to the importance of female CHC profile in male mate choice. In species that lack parental care, male mate choice is rare [15], but can evolve in species where females are found simultaneously [3] and/or courtship is expensive [5] or exposes males to predators [15]. Crickets fulfill these requirements; in our species, populations are locally abundant (especially for G. firmus) with dozens of individuals often living under a single rock or wooden plank (LSM personal observation). Furthermore, courtship can be costly, not only because the calling male exposes himself to predators and parasitoids, but also because the spermatophore is nutrient rich and often consumed by females. We need further data to fully understand the evolution of male mate choice. For instance, it is unclear if female CHC profile serves as an indicator of female fitness or if males are more able to detect certain chemicals. This information will be essential in understanding the selective basis behind the evolution of male mate choice in this system.
Here we confirmed previous results [38] showing that G. firmus and G. pennsylvanicus males exhibited similar CHC profiles and that females varied within and between species and had profiles that were distinct from that of males (although some females displayed a male-like profile). Sexual dimorphism of CHC profile is common and has been observed in several insect species such as burrowing bees [57], fireflies [41], and the Australian field cricket Teleogryllus oceanicus [60]. Importantly sexually dimorphic CHC profiles have been shown to mediate sex recognition in males, leading to aggression towards other males and courtship towards females [43, 66]. In closely related species, interspecific differences in CHC profile might be important as a reproductive barrier. Interspecific differences are known to exist in closely related Drosophila species (Etges & Ahrens, 2001 [7, 39, 54];) as well as between potentially hybridizing crickets such as the Laupala complex in Hawaii [42] and other field crickets [32, 65]. These interspecific differences in CHC profile have been implicated in influencing mate choice and therefore reproductive isolation.
Implications to the hybrid zone and speciation
Gryllus firmus and G. pennsylvanicus have various prezygotic barriers in all life stages that reduce heterospecific matings [21, 37, 51]. Male mate preference seems to be a significant prezygotic behavioral barrier leading to a longer time or failure of courtship when the female is heterospecific [37, 38]. Furthermore, these crickets also have a unidirectional post-mating barrier to fertilization: only G. pennsylvanicus females can produce hybrid offspring [20, 29, 37]. It is thus important to understand the basis of male mate preference especially in G. firmus males, the only species capable of siring hybrid offspring.
To examine the influence of CHC composition on male mate preference, we measured courtship success as well as time to mate. We categorized females into species specific CHC compositions (G. firmus and G. pennsylvanicus) as well as a third category composed of females of both species which exhibit a CHC composition similar to that of males (male like; ML). When accounting for female species, male G. firmus courted heterospecific ML females more often than other heterospecific females (Fig. 2). While CHC category alone was not entirely responsible for G. firmus male mate preference, as conspecific ML females were courted more quickly than heterospecific ML females (Fig. 3), it still played a role in the male mate preference. In agreement to previous studies, we also found that G. pennsylvanicus males court less often and did not discriminate between conspecifics and heterospecifics [37]; however, they too were more successful at courting ML females but not significantly so (Fig. 2).
Given that courtship in this system is initiated by males and thus mediated by male mate preference and that a female will only mate a male that is intensely courting [38] this preference for ML females, the only CHC category common in both species, is relevant to hybridization. Male-like females are common in both species constituting between 15 and 45% of the female population (based on this and other population surveys). Since G. firmus males court ML heterospecific females more successfully than other heterospecifics, this suggests that most of the hybridization happens through this female type.
Male-like females is not an strategy to evade courtship
Initially female male-like profiles were hypothesized as a strategy to evade male harassment [38]. In studies with other Gryllus crickets, males are known to aggressively compete for acquisition of females [13, 55], and in other species, such as damselflies, male mimicry is often used to evade male harassment [18, 47]. In polyandrous Ischnura damselflies, females exhibit three different color morphs, one of which resembles the male coloring and aids in male courtship evasion [52]. In our sample a significant fraction of the females had male-like CHCs (GF: 54.2%, GP: 61.5%), however these females seemed to be favored by males, who courted them more or as intensely as they courted other conspecifics. We thus refute the hypothesis of courtship evasion previously proposed. Further studies are needed to elucidate why such phenotype is maintained in these species.
Conclusions
We showed that G. firmus males courted heterospecific females with a male-like profile more successfully than other types of heterospecific females and, since G. firmus is the only male capable to siring hybrid offspring, these ML females might be responsible for most of the hybridization. We also showed that conspecific ML females seem to be either preferred (G. pennsylvanicus) or not discriminated against (G. firmus) males, thus refuting the hypothesis that females with a male-like profile could be evading courtship.
Methods
Collection
In August 2013, we collected penultimate instar G. firmus crickets from Guilford, CT (41°.13′,-72°40′) and G. pennsylvanicus crickets from Ithaca, NY (42°25′,-76°.29′), allopatric pure species populations. Individuals were separated by sex and species, and raised at room temperature (25 °C) in plastic cages (33 × 20 × 13 cm, with a maximum of 12 individuals) with ad libitum food (a mixture of Purina Cat Chow®, LM Bonanza Rabbit Food®, and Fluker’s Cricket Feed®) and water.
Courtship trials
To measure male courtship intensity as a function of speed, we placed males in a petri dish with either a heterospecific or conspecific female and recorded the time to the start of courtship. We conducted four sets of crosses each day (10 AM and 2 PM). In each cross set (am and pm), a male was paired to a conspecific female followed by a heterospecific female an hour later (or reverse, heterospecific then conspecific). In total each male was placed with four conspecific and four heterospecific females over a two day period (alternating the order of conspecific and heterospecific females). Females were also tested to eight males, but were kept virgin throughout the experiment. Based on previous work, [38] we limited time to a maximum of 25 min (95% of males either already initiated or will not initiate courtship past this time), if the male did not initiate courtship within that time, the trial was considered unsuccessful. Pairs were never allowed to mate; after the male initiated courtship the pair was immediately separated. All male and female crickets used in the experiment were of approximately 12 days old (within 1–4 days apart); the adult lifespan in captivity is 30 ± 8 days (personal observation). We measured pronotum size as a proxy for body size in both males and females. and then compared species with a two-way ANOVA analysis.
Cuticular hydrocarbon analysis
We extracted CHC from all individuals used in the courtship experiment by placing whole crickets into glass vials containing 3 mL (females) or 2 mL (males) of HPLC grade hexane for 5–7 min. For the analysis we transferred the CHC samples into 2 mL clear glass surestop vials with 300 μL glass inserts and analyzed with Agilent Technologies (AT) 7890A GC system with an (AT) HP-5 ms (325 °C 30 m × 250 μm x .250 μm) column attached to an AT 5975C inert XL EI/CI MSD with triple-Axis Detector MS system, that obtains chromatograms and both electron and chemical ionization mass spectra. The GCMS method consisted of a 2 μL of each sample injected in a split mode with a split ratio of 100:1. The column was held at an initial temperature of 100 °C for 1 min followed by 15 °C/min increase to 180 °C, then a 3 °C/min increase to 260 °C, and finally a 1 °C/min increase to a final temperature of 280 °C held for 10 min.
For GCMS data analysis, we scored a total of 17 peaks for each individual. Ten of these peaks were previously used as representative of common compounds in males and females [38] and we also scored seven new peaks. To score the peaks as a relative proportion of the total, we took the percent area contributed by each of the scored peaks and scaled the scored peaks to add up to 100% for each individual. To account for the dependence in the relative proportions, we used the centered log ratio (CLR) transformation prior to further analyzing the relative proportions of the peaks [1].
Cuticular hydrocarbon data was visualized via principal component analysis (Figure 4 in Appendix). Then the female CHC data were clustered into homogenous groups, or CHC categories, based on the Euclidean distance of CLR transformed relative proportions of the 17 peaks using the partitioning around medoids algorithm [27]. We chose the number of groups that maximized the average silhouette, a cluster validity measure which measures the cohesion and separation of the clusters [49]. This clustering process of female CHC profiles resulted in three clusters, two that correlated with species and a third that was the most similar to an average male CHC profile.
Behavioral and CHC integration analysis
We used a binomial logistic mixed effects model to predict courtship success of each pair as a function of the species and female CHC profile category. Based on visualizations of courtship success rates, we included all main effects and pairwise interactions with male species, type of pair crossing (conspecific or heterospecific), and an indicator for “male-like” female CHC category and a three-way interaction between these variables. This model parameterization retained full information on the crickets since two of the female CHC categories correspond to female species. With the model, we then estimated the probability for courtship success and then the odds ratios for courtship success comparing females with “male-like” CHC profiles to females with other CHC profiles by male species for both conspecific and heterospecific pairs. To control for variability in individual courtship behavior in males across the repeated pairings, we included random intercepts for individual male crickets in the binomial logistic analysis.
We performed time to event analyses (more commonly known as survival analyses) to determine how the rate at which males successfully court a female depends on the species and the female CHC categories. Using the Kaplan-Meier curve, we estimated the probability that a courting event has not yet occurred at every point in time during the courtship trial (0–25 min) for each CHC category, stratifying by male species, and then by pair crossing and whether or not the female has a “male-like” CHC category [26]. Statistical differences between estimated curves were measured by a log-rank test [36]. A Cox proportional-hazards regression model was used to investigate the association of species and the female CHC profiles with the time until courtship success [40, 58]. Using the model, we estimated the hazard ratio for courtship initiation, the ratio of courting rate at any fixed point in time, comparing females with “male-like” CHC profiles to females with other CHC profiles by male species for both conspecific and heterospecific pairings. Similarly, we used a random intercept for individual male crickets to account for variability in individual courtship behavior. We completed the analysis with R 3.6.1 [46].
Acknowledgements
We thank Dr. David Richardson and Gisela Demant for the help with the GCMS procedure.
Abbreviations
- CHC
Cuticular hydrocarbon
- GCMS
Gas chromatograph mass spectrometry (GCMS)
- ML
Male-like females, that is, females that have a typical male CHC profile
Appendix
Authors’ contributions
BH analyzed data and wrote the manuscript. Undergraduate students DS and LP collected CHC data, participated in data analyses and wrote the first draft of parts of the manuscript. LSM conceived and designed the experiments, analyzed the data and wrote the manuscript. All authors read, made comments on and approved the final manuscript.
Funding
This work was partially funded by NSF grant 1650887 to L. S. Maroja and by Williams College. The funding bodies played no role in the design of the study and collection, analysis, and interpretation of data and in writing the manuscript.
Availability of data and materials
The dataset generated and analyzed here is available in the dryad repository under DOI: 10.5061/dryad.cz8w9gj0g
Ethics approval
We studied two common cricket species, G. firmus and G. pennsylvanicus. This study does not involve regulated invertebrates and neither of the two cricket species are endangered or in extinction.
Consent for publication
Not applicable
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Aitchison J. The statistical analysis of compositional data. J Royal Stat Soc Ser B. 1982;44(2):139–177. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Bagine RKN, Brandl R, Kaib M. Species delimitation in macrotermes (isoptera, macrotermitidae) - evidence from epicuticular hydrocarbons, morphology, and ecology. Ann Entomol Soc Am. 1994;87:498–506. doi: 10.1093/aesa/87.5.498. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Barry KL, Kokko H. Male mate choice: why sequential choice can make its evolution difficult. Anim Behav. 2010;80:163–169. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.04.020. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Bateman PW, Fleming PA. Males are selective too: mating, but not courtship, with sequential females influences choosiness in male field crickets (Gryllus bimaculatus) Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2006;59:577–581. doi: 10.1007/s00265-005-0083-y. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Byrne PG, Rice WR. Evidence for adaptive male mate choice in the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster. Proc Biol Sci. 2006;273:917–922. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Chuine A, Sauzet S, Debias F, Desouhant E. Consequences of genetic incompatibility on fitness and mate choice: the male point of view. Biol J Linnean Soc. 2015;114(2):279–286. doi: 10.1111/bij.12421. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Chung H, Loehlin DW, Dufour HD, Vaccarro K, Millar JG, Carroll SB. A single gene affects both ecological divergence and mate choice in Drosophila. Science. 2014;343:1148–1151. doi: 10.1126/science.1249998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Cobb M, Jallon JM. Pheromones, mate recognition and courtship stimulation in the Drosophila-melanogaster species subgroup. Anim Behav. 1990;39:1058–1067. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80778-X. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Coyne JA, Crittenden AP, Mah K. Genetics of a Pheromonal difference contributing to reproductive isolation in Drosophila. Science. 1994;265:1461–1464. doi: 10.1126/science.8073292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Coyne JA, Orr HA. Speciation. Sunderland: Sinauer associates, Inc. Publishers; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Curtis S, Sztepanacz JL, White BE, Dyer KA, Rundle HD, Mayer P. Epicuticular compounds of Drosophila subquinaria and D. recens: identification, quantification, and their role in female mate choice. J Chem Ecol. 2013;39:579–590. doi: 10.1007/s10886-013-0284-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Dapporto L, Theodora P, Spacchini C, Pieraccini G, Turillazzi S. Rank and epicuticular hydrocarbons in different populations of the paper wasp Polistes dominulus (Christ) (Hymenoptera, Vespidae) Insect Soc. 2004;51:279–286. doi: 10.1007/s00040-004-0738-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Dixon KA, Cade WH. Some factors influencing male male-aggression in the field cricket Gryllus integer (time of day, age, weight and sexual maturity) Anim Behav. 1986;34:340–346. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(86)80102-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Doherty JA, Storz MM. Calling song and selective phonotaxis in the field crickets, Gryllus firmus and G. pennsylvanicus (orthoptera, gryllidae) J Insect Behav. 1992;5:555–569. doi: 10.1007/BF01048004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Edward DA, Chapman T. The evolution and significance of male mate choice. Trends in Ecology & Evolution26:647-654.Etges, W.J. & Ahrens, M.a. 2001. Premating isolation is determined by larval-rearing substrates in cactophilic Drosophila mojavensis. V. Deep geographic variation in epicuticular hydrocarbons among isolated populations. Am Nat. 2011;158:585–598. doi: 10.1086/323587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Gage AR, Barnard CJ. Male crickets increase sperm number in relation to competition and female size. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1996;38:349–353. doi: 10.1007/s002650050251. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Giglio EM, Dyer KA. Divergence of premating behaviors in the closely related species Drosophila subquinaria and D. recens. Ecol Evol. 2013;3:365–374. doi: 10.1002/ece3.477. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Gosden TP, Svensson EI. Density-dependent male mating harassment, female resistance, and male mimicry. Am Nat. 2009;173:709–721. doi: 10.1086/598491. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Harrison RG, Arnold J. A narrow hybrid zone between closely related cricket species. Evolution. 1982;36:535–552. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1982.tb05075.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Harrison RG. Barriers to gene exchange between closely related cricket species .1. laboratory hybridization studies. Evolution. 1983;37:245–251. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1983.tb05534.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Harrison RG. Barriers to gene exchange between closely related cricket species .2. Life-cycle variation and temporal isolation. Evolution. 1985;39:244–259. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1985.tb05664.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Herdman EJE, Kelly CD, Godin JGJ. Male mate choice in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata): do males prefer larger females as mates? Ethology. 2004;110:97–111. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2003.00960.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Howard RW, Jackson LL, Banse H, Blows MW. Cuticular hydrocarbons of Drosophila birchii and D. serrata: identification and role in mate choice in D. serrata. J Chem Ecol. 2003;29:961–976. doi: 10.1023/A:1022992002239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Ingleby Fiona C., Hunt John, Hosken David J. Genotype-by-Environment Interactions for Female Mate Choice of Male Cuticular Hydrocarbons in Drosophila simulans. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(6):e67623. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0067623. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Johannesson K, Havenhand JN, Jonsson PR, Lindegarth M, Sundin A, Hollander J. Male discrimination of female mucous trails permits assortative mating in a marine snail species. Evolution. 2008;62:3178–3184. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00510.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Kaplan EL, Meier P. Nonparametric-estimation from incomplete observations. J Am Stat Assoc. 1958;53:457–481. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1958.10501452. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kaufman L, Rousseeuw PJ. Finding groups in data : an introduction to cluster analysis. New York: Wiley; 1990. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kirkpatrick M. Sexual selection and the evolution of female choice. Evolution. 1982;36:1–12. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1982.tb05003.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Larson Erica L., Andrés Jose A., Harrison Richard G. Influence of the Male Ejaculate on Post-Mating Prezygotic Barriers in Field Crickets. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(10):e46202. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Larson EL, Becker CG, Bondra ER, Harrison RG. Structure of a mosaic hybrid zone between the field crickets Gryllus firmus and G. pennsylvanicus. Ecol Evol. 2013;3:985–1002. doi: 10.1002/ece3.514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Larson EL, White TA, Ross CL, Harrison RG. Gene flow and the maintenance of species boundaries. Mol Ecol. 2014;23:1668–1678. doi: 10.1111/mec.12601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Leonard AS, Hedrick AV. Single versus multiple cues in mate discrimination by males and females. Anim Behav. 2009;77:151–159. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.09.029. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Lihoreau M, Rivault C. Kin recognition via cuticular hydrocarbons shapes cockroach social life. Behav Ecol. 2009;20:46–53. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arn113. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Lorenzi MC, Bagneres AG, Clement JL, Turillazzi S. Polistes biglumis bimaculatus epicuticular hydrocarbons and nestmate recognition (Hymenoptera, Vespidae) Insect Soc. 1997;44:123–138. doi: 10.1007/s000400050035. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Mack PD, Hammock BA, Promislow DEL. Sperm competitive ability and genetic relatedness in Drosophila melanogaster: similarity breeds contempt. Evolution. 2002;56:1789–1795. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2002.tb00192.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Mantel N. Evaluation of survival data and two new rank order statistics arising in its consideration. Cancer Chemother Rep. 1966;50:163–170. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Maroja LS, Andres JA, Walters JR, Harrison RG. Multiple barriers to gene exchange in a field cricket hybrid zone. Biol J Linn Soc. 2009;97:390–402. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2009.01201.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Maroja LS, McKenzie ZM, Hart E, Jing J, Larson EL, Richardson DP. Barriers to gene exchange in hybridizing field crickets: the role of male courtship effort and cuticular hydrocarbons. BMC Evol Biol. 2014;14:65–75. doi: 10.1186/1471-2148-14-65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Mas F, Jallon JM. Sexual isolation and cuticular hydrocarbon differences between Drosophila santomea and Drosophila yakuba. J Chem Ecol. 2005;31:2747–2752. doi: 10.1007/s10886-005-7570-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.May W. Kaplan-Meier Survival Analysis. In: Schwab M, editor. Encyclopedia of Cancer. Berlin: Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Ming QL, Lewis SM. Mate recognition and sex differences in cuticular hydrocarbons of the diurnal firefly Ellychnia corrusca (Coleoptera: Lampyridae) Ann Entomol Soc Am. 2010;103:128–133. doi: 10.1603/008.103.0116. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Mullen SP, Mendelson TC, Schal C, Shaw KL. Rapid evolution of cuticular hydrocarbons in a species radiation of acoustically diverse Hawaiian crickets (Gryllidae : Trigonidiinae : Laupala) Evolution. 2007;61:223–231. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00019.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Peschke K, Metzler M. Cuticular hydrocarbons and female sex-pheromones of the rove beetle, Aleochara-Curtula (Goeze) (Coleoptera, Staphylinidae) Insect Biochemistry. 1987;17:167–178. doi: 10.1016/0020-1790(87)90157-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Peterson MA, Honchak BM, Locke SE, Beeman TE, Mendoza J, Green J, Buckingham KJ, White MA, Monsen KJ. Relative abundance and the species-specific reinforcement of male mating preference in the Chrysochus (Coleoptera : Chrysomelidae) hybrid zone. Evolution. 2005;59:2639–2655. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2005.tb00976.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Peterson MA, Dobler S, Larson EL, Juarez D, Schlarbaum T, Monsen KJ, Francke W. Profiles of cuticular hydrocarbons mediate male mate choice and sexual isolation between hybridising Chrysochus (Coleoptera : Chrysomelidae) Chemoecology. 2007;17:87–96. doi: 10.1007/s00049-007-0366-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.R Core Team . R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. 2019. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Robertson HM. Female dimorphism and mating-behavior in a damselfly, Ischnura-Ramburi - females mimicking males. Anim Behav. 1985;33:805–809. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(85)80013-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Ross CL, Harrison RG. A fine-scale spatial analysis of the mosaic hybrid zone between Gryllus firmus and Gryllus pennsylvanicus. Evolution. 2002;56:2296–2312. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2002.tb00153.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Rousseeuw PJ. Silhouettes: a graphical aid to the interpretation and validation of cluster analysis. J Comput Appl Math. 1987;20:53–65. doi: 10.1016/0377-0427(87)90125-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Ruther J, Sieben S, Schricker B. Nestmate recognition in social wasps: manipulation of hydrocarbon profiles induces aggression in the European hornet. Naturwissenschaften. 2002;89:111–114. doi: 10.1007/s00114-001-0292-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Saleh NW, Larson EL, Harrison RG. Reproductive success and body size in the cricket Gryllus firmus. J Insect Behav. 2014;27:346–356. doi: 10.1007/s10905-013-9425-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Sánchez-Guillén RA, Cordero-Rivera A, Rivas-Torres A, Wellenreuther M, Bybee S, Hansson B, Velasquez-Vélez MI, Realpe E, Chávez-Ríos JR, Villalobos F, Dumont H. The evolutionary history of colour polymorphism in Ischnura damselflies. J Evol Biol. 2018. 10.1111/jeb.13289. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 53.Scudder SH. The species of Gryllus found in the United States east of the sierra Nevadas. Psyche. 1902;9:291–296. doi: 10.1155/1902/35962. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Seeholzer LF, Seppo M, Stern DL, Ruta V. Evolution of a central neural circuit underlies Drosophila mate preferences. Nature. 2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 55.Simmons LW. Intermale competition and mating success in the field cricket, Gryllus-Bimaculatus (Degeer) Anim Behav. 1986;34:567–579. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(86)80126-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Simmons LW. Kin recognition and its influence on mating preferences of the field cricket, gryllus-bimaculatus (degeer) Anim Behav. 1989;38:68–77. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(89)80066-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Simmons LW, Alcock J, Reeder A. The role of cuticular hydrocarbons in male attraction and repulsion by female Dawson's burrowing bee, Amegilla dawsoni. Anim Behav. 2003;66:677–685. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2003.2240. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Therneau TM, Grambsch PM, Pankratz VS. Penalized survival models and frailty. J Comput Graph Stat. 2003;12:156–175. doi: 10.1198/1061860031365. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Thistle R, Cameron P, Ghorayshi A, Dennison L, Scott K. Contact chemoreceptors mediate male-male repulsion and male-female attraction during Drosophila courtship. Cell. 2012;149:1140–1151. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2012.03.045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Thomas ML, Simmons LW. Sexual dimorphism in cuticular hydrocarbons of the Australian field cricket Teleogryllus oceanicus (Orthoptera : Gryllidae) J Insect Physiol. 2008;54:1081–1089. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2008.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Thomas ML, Simmons LW. Male-derived cuticular hydrocarbons signal sperm competition intensity and affect ejaculate expenditure in crickets. Proc Biol Sci. 2009;276:383–388. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.1206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Thomas Melissa L, Simmons Leigh W. Sexual selection on cuticular hydrocarbons in the Australian field cricket, Teleogryllus oceanicus. BMC Evolutionary Biology. 2009;9(1):162. doi: 10.1186/1471-2148-9-162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Thomas ML, Simmons LW. Cuticular hydrocarbons influence female attractiveness to males in the Australian field cricket, Teleogryllus oceanicus. J Evol Biol. 2010;23:707–714. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2010.01943.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Thomas ML, Gray B, Simmons LW. Male crickets alter the relative expression of cuticular hydrocarbons when exposed to different acoustic environments. Anim Behav. 2011;82:49–53. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.03.023. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Tyler F, Fisher D, d'Ettorre P, Rodriguez-Munoz R, Tregenza T. Chemical cues mediate species recognition in field crickets. Front Ecol Evol. 2015;3.
- 66.Wang XM, Chong SL. A hierarchical model for language maintenance and language shift: focus on the Malaysian Chinese community. J Multiling Multicult Dev. 2011;32:577–591. doi: 10.1080/01434632.2011.617820. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Weddle CB, Steiger S, Hamaker CG, Ower GD, Mitchell C, Sakaluk SK, Hunt J. Cuticular hydrocarbons as a basis for chemosensory self-referencing in crickets: a potentially universal mechanism facilitating polyandry in insects. Ecol Lett. 2013;16:346–353. doi: 10.1111/ele.12046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The dataset generated and analyzed here is available in the dryad repository under DOI: 10.5061/dryad.cz8w9gj0g