Skip to main content
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America logoLink to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
letter
. 2020 Jan 24;117(5):2251–2252. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1918402117

An ancestral anatomical and spatial bias for visually guided behavior

Patrick Friedrich a,b,1, Michel Thiebaut de Schotten a,b, Stephanie J Forkel c,d, Martin Stacho e, Henrietta Howells f
PMCID: PMC7007561  PMID: 31980539

Human behavioral asymmetries are commonly studied in the context of structural cortical and connectional asymmetries. Within this framework, Sreenivasan and Sridharan (1) provide intriguing evidence of a relationship between visual asymmetries and the lateralization of superior colliculi connections—a phylogenetically older mesencephalic structure. Specifically, response facilitation for cued locations (i.e., choice bias) in the contralateral hemifield was associated with differences in the connectivity of the superior colliculus. Given that the superior colliculus has a structural homolog—the optic tectum—which can be traced across all Vertebrata, these results may have meaningful evolutionary ramifications.

Even the oldest vertebrates display asymmetries in visually guided responses. Lungfish, the closest extant predecessor of the first land-dwelling vertebrates, show biases in their sidewise movements depending on their percept: When fleeing from oncoming predators, they deviate left, whereas during foraging behavior they commonly turn rightward (2). Avian species also show asymmetries in visually guided behavior that closely resemble human visual choice bias. Due to the full crossing of the optic chiasm, visual input entering one hemisphere can be hindered by occluding the contralateral eye. When doing this, pigeons show higher success rates in grain–grit discrimination tasks only when they have access to the right eye. Moreover, pigeons with higher degrees of visual asymmetry display more successful foraging behavior under regular, binocular circumstances (3). Concordantly, both chicks with normal cerebral asymmetries (4) and peociliid fish selectively bred for stronger visual lateralization (5) show an advantage in simultaneous predator monitoring and foraging. Hence, behavioral asymmetries for visually guided responses are not unique to humans and may constitute an important advantage for predator avoidance and foraging that has been conserved throughout evolution.

In line with Sreenivasan and Sridharan (1), species that display asymmetries in visually guided behavior also show asymmetries in mesencephalic connections. For instance, zebrafish show leftward asymmetries between mesencephalic and epithalamic structures, in particular between the left dorsal nucleus interpeduncularis and the left lateral habenula which responds mostly to visual input (6, 7). In pigeons, the tectofugal pathway which is involved in visual processing is left-lateralized. Precisely the left visual thalamic nucleus receives stronger input from the contralateral optic tectum than the right nucleus (8). Hence, mesencephalic asymmetries are evolutionary conserved, although their appearance differs across species.

The study by Sreenivasan and Sridharan (1) reveals that asymmetries in the phylogenetically older mesencephalon affect visually guided responses in humans. The cause of mesencephalic asymmetries, for example in pigeons, has been revealed to be driven by environmental factors: During embryogenesis there is stronger photostimulation of the right eye (9). Whether these mesencephalic connectional asymmetries are shaped by environmental factors or are genetically determined in humans remains to be explored and could provide useful insight into the mechanisms of human behavior. This may allow for a more complete perspective regarding the circumstances under which evolutionary adaptations in cognition and brain anatomy arose.

Footnotes

The authors declare no competing interest.

References

  • 1.Sreenivasan V., Sridharan D., Subcortical connectivity correlates selectively with attention’s effects on spatial choice bias. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116, 19711–19716 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Lippolis G., Joss J. M. P., Rogers L. J., Australian lungfish (Neoceratodus forsteri): A missing link in the evolution of complementary side biases for predator avoidance and prey capture. Brain Behav. Evol. 73, 295–303 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Güntürkün O., et al. , Asymmetry pays: Visual lateralization improves discrimination success in pigeons. Curr. Biol. 10, 1079–1081 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Rogers L. J., Zucca P., Vallortigara G., Advantages of having a lateralized brain. Proc. Biol. Sci. 271 (suppl. 6), S420–S422 (2004). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Dadda M., Bisazza A., Does brain asymmetry allow efficient performance of simultaneous tasks? Anim. Behav. 72, 523–529 (2006). [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Dreosti E., Vendrell Llopis N., Carl M., Yaksi E., Wilson S. W., Left-right asymmetry is required for the habenulae to respond to both visual and olfactory stimuli. Curr. Biol. 24, 440–445 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Aizawa H., et al. , Laterotopic representation of left-right information onto the dorso-ventral axis of a zebrafish midbrain target nucleus. Curr. Biol. 15, 238–243 (2005). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Güntürkün O., Morphological asymmetries of the tectum opticum in the pigeon. Exp. Brain Res. 116, 561–566 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Manns M., Güntürkün O., Heumann R., Blöchl A., Photic inhibition of TrkB/Ras activity in the pigeon’s tectum during development: Impact on brain asymmetry formation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 22, 2180–2186 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America are provided here courtesy of National Academy of Sciences

RESOURCES