Summary
Background
Frontotemporal dementia is a heterogenous neurodegenerative disorder, with about a third of cases being genetic. Most of this genetic component is accounted for by mutations in GRN, MAPT, and C9orf72. In this study, we aimed to complement previous phenotypic studies by doing an international study of age at symptom onset, age at death, and disease duration in individuals with mutations in GRN, MAPT, and C9orf72.
Methods
In this international, retrospective cohort study, we collected data on age at symptom onset, age at death, and disease duration for patients with pathogenic mutations in the GRN and MAPT genes and pathological expansions in the C9orf72 gene through the Frontotemporal Dementia Prevention Initiative and from published papers. We used mixed effects models to explore differences in age at onset, age at death, and disease duration between genetic groups and individual mutations. We also assessed correlations between the age at onset and at death of each individual and the age at onset and at death of their parents and the mean age at onset and at death of their family members. Lastly, we used mixed effects models to investigate the extent to which variability in age at onset and at death could be accounted for by family membership and the specific mutation carried.
Findings
Data were available from 3403 individuals from 1492 families: 1433 with C9orf72 expansions (755 families), 1179 with GRN mutations (483 families, 130 different mutations), and 791 with MAPT mutations (254 families, 67 different mutations). Mean age at symptom onset and at death was 49·5 years (SD 10·0; onset) and 58·5 years (11·3; death) in the MAPT group, 58·2 years (9·8; onset) and 65·3 years (10·9; death) in the C9orf72 group, and 61·3 years (8·8; onset) and 68·8 years (9·7; death) in the GRN group. Mean disease duration was 6·4 years (SD 4·9) in the C9orf72 group, 7·1 years (3·9) in the GRN group, and 9·3 years (6·4) in the MAPT group. Individual age at onset and at death was significantly correlated with both parental age at onset and at death and with mean family age at onset and at death in all three groups, with a stronger correlation observed in the MAPT group (r=0·45 between individual and parental age at onset, r=0·63 between individual and mean family age at onset, r=0·58 between individual and parental age at death, and r=0·69 between individual and mean family age at death) than in either the C9orf72 group (r=0·32 individual and parental age at onset, r=0·36 individual and mean family age at onset, r=0·38 individual and parental age at death, and r=0·40 individual and mean family age at death) or the GRN group (r=0·22 individual and parental age at onset, r=0·18 individual and mean family age at onset, r=0·22 individual and parental age at death, and r=0·32 individual and mean family age at death). Modelling showed that the variability in age at onset and at death in the MAPT group was explained partly by the specific mutation (48%, 95% CI 35–62, for age at onset; 61%, 47–73, for age at death), and even more by family membership (66%, 56–75, for age at onset; 74%, 65–82, for age at death). In the GRN group, only 2% (0–10) of the variability of age at onset and 9% (3–21) of that of age of death was explained by the specific mutation, whereas 14% (9–22) of the variability of age at onset and 20% (12–30) of that of age at death was explained by family membership. In the C9orf72 group, family membership explained 17% (11–26) of the variability of age at onset and 19% (12–29) of that of age at death.
Interpretation
Our study showed that age at symptom onset and at death of people with genetic frontotemporal dementia is influenced by genetic group and, particularly for MAPT mutations, by the specific mutation carried and by family membership. Although estimation of age at onset will be an important factor in future pre-symptomatic therapeutic trials for all three genetic groups, our study suggests that data from other members of the family will be particularly helpful only for individuals with MAPT mutations. Further work in identifying both genetic and environmental factors that modify phenotype in all groups will be important to improve such estimates.
Funding
UK Medical Research Council, National Institute for Health Research, and Alzheimer’s Society.
Introduction
Frontotemporal dementia is a genetically and pathologically heterogeneous neurodegenerative disease.1 The most common clinical subtypes of this disease are behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia, presenting with changes in personality and executive dysfunction, and primary progressive aphasia, in which individuals develop impairment of language processing. Three forms of primary progressive aphasia have been described: semantic, non-fluent or agrammatic, and logopenic; however, up to 20% of people do not fit the criteria for any of these variants and are categorised as having primary progressive aphasia not otherwise specified.2 Both behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia and primary progressive aphasia overlap with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis and with the atypical parkinsonian syndromes corticobasal syndrome and progressive supranuclear palsy.1
About a third of frontotemporal dementia cases are genetic,3 with mutations in multiple genes shown to be causative of this disease. However, most of the heritability of frontotemporal dementia is accounted for by mutations in three genes: progranulin (GRN), microtubule-associated protein tau (MAPT), and chromosome 9 open reading frame 72 (C9orf72; also known as C9orf72-SMCR8 complex subunit). Although much has been learned over the past decade about the clinical features of these genetic forms of frontotemporal dementia, most studies exploring age at symptom onset and disease duration have been small and geographically restricted.4–6 In particular, although individual case series have suggested that such phenotypic characteristics can be quite variable, no studies have systematically investigated these factors across all the different genetic groups and the different mutations found within these groups.
Therefore, in this large international study, we aimed to analyse phenotypic characteristics of the main three forms of genetic frontotemporal dementia, including ages at symptom onset and death and disease duration, as well as examining the effect of mutation type and family membership on these factors.
Methods
Study design and participants
In this international retrospective cohort study, we collected data from centres that are part of the Frontotemporal Dementia Prevention Initiative (FPI) and through a literature review of publications. The FPI is a group connecting natural history cohort studies of genetic frontotemporal dementia: the Genetic Frontotemporal Dementia Initiative (GENFI),7 Advancing Research and Treatment for Frontotemporal Lobar Degeneration (ARTFL), Longitudinal Evaluation of Familial Frontotemporal Dementia Subjects (LEFFTDS), and the Dominantly Inherited Non-Alzheimer’s Dementias (DINAD) studies. These research studies include most of the centres investigating genetic frontotemporal dementia in Europe and eastern Canada (GENFI), USA and western Canada (ARTFL and LEFFTDS), and Australia (DINAD). In total, 33 centres across the world (12 countries; appendix p 20) provided participant data for our study. We included all known pathogenic mutations in the GRN, MAPT, and C9orf72 genes in our study. Families with intermediate length expansions of C9orf72 were not included in the study. All mutations were reviewed by two geneticists (RG and JB) to examine pathogenicity and were only included if both agreed on their probable pathogenic nature (full inclusion and exclusion criteria are in the appendix, p 2). Local ethics committees at each of the sites approved the study and data from participants was provided through informed written consent.
Procedures
Participant data collected from FPI centres included genetic group, individual mutation (for participants with mutations in GRN and MAPT), sex, clinical phenotype, age at symptom onset (defined by the onset of progressive behavioural, cognitive, or motor symptoms reported either by an informant [usually a family member] or, for non-behavioural symptoms, by the patient themselves), age at death, and relationship to other affected family members.
For the literature review, we assessed publications cited in the Alzheimer Disease & Frontotemporal Dementia Mutation database, and supplemented this by a detailed search of PubMed (done between Jan 1, 2015, and July 1, 2017) for other publications with data for age at symptom onset, age at death, or disease duration in people with genetic frontotemporal dementia: this identified 308 journal articles. To avoid potential double reporting, centres were asked to provide a list of publications relevant to their dataset. These lists were then manually examined for possible duplicates, which were removed when identified.
Statistical analysis
We grouped participants into a GRN, MAPT, or C9orf72 group according to the mutation present. We calculated the numbers and percentages of participants within each genetic group by geographic location and clinical phenotype. We used a χ2 test to compare sex distribution in each of the genetic groups. We calculated means and SDs for age at symptom onset, age at death, and disease duration in each genetic group and in the most common mutations in the MAPT and GRN groups (defined as those identified in the greatest number of individuals in the study). We used mixed effects models to examine differences in age at symptom onset, age at death, and disease duration between genetic groups (GRN, MAPT, and C9orf72), between the most common mutations in the GRN and MAPT groups, between an earlier (first) and later (second) generation of family members in all genetic groups, between men and women within each genetic group, and between the main clinical phenotypes within each genetic group. Analyses accounted for relatedness by including family membership as a random effect. We calculated Pearson correlation coefficients to explore the relationship between an individual’s age at symptom onset (or death) and the age at symptom onset (or death) of their affected parent and the association between an individual’s age at symptom onset (or death) and the average age at symptom onset (or death) of other members of the same family. Lastly, we also used mixed effects models to explore the extent to which variability in age at symptom onset and at death were explained by family membership (exploring variability both within and between families) and the specific mutation carried (in GRN and MAPT groups). Detailed statistical methods are shown in the appendix (pp 15–19). All statistical analyses were done with Stata (v.14 or later).
Role of the funding source
The funders of the study had no role in study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, or writing of the report. The corresponding author had full access to all the data in the study and had final responsibility for the decision to submit for publication.
Results
Our combined dataset comprised 3403 symptomatic individuals from 1492 families who had data available for one or more of age at symptom onset, age at death, disease duration, and clinical phenotype (table 1): 1433 individuals with C9orf72 expansions (from 755 families), 1179 with GRN mutations (483 families), and 791 with MAPT mutations (254 families).
Table 1:
GRN (n=1179) |
MAPT (n=791) |
C9orf72 (n=1433) |
Difference MAPT vs GRN |
Difference C9orf72 vs GRN |
Difference C9orf72 vs MAPT |
p value MAPT vs GRN |
p value C9orf72 vs GRN |
p value C9orf72 vs MAPT |
|
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sex (n [%]) | .. | .. | .. | .. | .. | .. | 0·0015 | <0·0001 | 0·18 |
Men | 490 (41·6%) | 386 (48·8%) | 742 (51·8%) | .. | .. | .. | .. | .. | .. |
Women | 689 (58·4%) | 405 (51·2%) | 691 (48·2%) | .. | .. | .. | .. | .. | .. |
Number of families | 483 | 254 | 755 | .. | .. | .. | .. | .. | .. |
Age at symptom onset (years) | .. | .. | .. | −11·8 (−13·0 to −10·6 | −2·8 (−3·8 to −1·9) | 9·0 (7·8 to 10·1) | <0·0001 | <0·0001 | <0·0001 |
Mean (SD; n) | 61·3 (8·8; n=967) | 49·5 (10·0; n=609) | 58·2 (9·8; n=1076) | .. | .. | .. | .. | .. | .. |
Range | 25–90 | 17–82 | 20–91 | .. | .. | .. | .. | .. | .. |
Age at death (years) | .. | .. | .. | −10·7 (−12·3 to −9·1) | −3·5 (−4·9 to −2·2) | 7·2 (5·7 to 8·6) | <0·0001 | <0·0001 | <0·0001 |
Mean (SD; n) | 68·8 (9·7; n=656) | 58·5 (11·3; n=485) | 65·3 (10·9; n=839) | .. | .. | .. | .. | .. | .. |
Range | 42–98 | 24–93 | 26–97 | .. | .. | .. | .. | .. | .. |
Disease duration (years) | .. | .. | .. | 0·18 (0·08 to 0·29) | −0·26 (−0·35 to −0·17) | −0·44 (−0·54 to −0·34) | 0·0005 | <0·0001 | <0·0001 |
Mean (SD; n) | 7·1 (3·9; n=548) | 9·3 (6·4; n=394) | 6·4 (4·9; n=618) | .. | .. | .. | .. | .. | .. |
Range | 0–27 | 0–45 | 0–36 | .. | .. | .. | .. | .. | .. |
For age at symptom onset, age at death, and disease duration, differences are adjusted mean differences (natural log values for disease duration) with 95% CIs. For sex differences, p values calculated with a χ2 test. For age at symptom onset, age at death, and disease duration, p values calculated with mixed effects models.
In total, 130 GRN mutations and 67 MAPT mutations were identified and all were included in the study (appendix pp 5–10). We found 78 GRN and 45 MAPT pathogenic mutations through the Alzheimer Disease & Frontotemporal Dementia Mutation database, 35 GRN and 18 MAPT variants, which were not included in the database, through a PubMed search, and the FPI centres provided data on an additional 17 GRN and four MAPT variants not previously described in the literature (appendix p 2). The most common GRN mutations were Thr272fs (rs63749877; 201 individuals, 95 families), Arg493X (rs63751294; 55 individuals, 22 families), IVS7–1G→A (50 individuals, 18 families), Cys31fs (rs63751057, 47 individuals, 10 families), Gly35fs (rs63751073, 42 individuals, 10 families), and Ala9Asp (rs63751243, 37 individuals, four families). The most common MAPT mutations were Pro301Leu (rs63751273, 234 individuals, 59 families), IVS10 + 16→T (rs63751011; 149 individuals, 48 families), Arg406Trp (rs63750424, 67 individuals, nine families), and Asn279Lys (rs63750756, 44 individuals, 17 families).
Overall, the most prevalent genetic group was that comprising individuals carrying a C9orf72 expansion (1433 [42·1%] of 3403 individuals), followed by individuals with GRN mutations (1179 [34·6%]), with individuals carrying MAPT mutations comprising the least common group (791 [23·2%]; figure 1). However, we observed geographical variability in the distribution of these mutations, with a different spread of frequencies among the three genetic groups in some countries and regions: individuals with GRN mutations were more common than those of other groups in Italy (289 [66%] of 438 individuals) and, to a lesser extent, in Spain (76 [49%] of 155); whereas individuals with MAPT mutations were found more frequently in the Netherlands (81 [40%] of 204) and the US west coast (71 [47%] of 150; appendix pp 20–23).
Although behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia was the most common diagnosis in each genetic group, we observed phenotypic variability across the different mutations (table 2; appendix pp 24–32). Both C9orf72 and MAPT groups contained approximately equal numbers of men and women (table 1, appendix pp 33–34). However, the GRN group had a significant overrepresentation of women compared with both the C9orf72 group and the MAPT group.
Table 2:
GRN (n=1179) |
MAPT (n=791) |
C9orf72 (n=1433) |
|
---|---|---|---|
Diagnoses within the frontotemporal dementia spectrum | |||
Behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia | 446 (37·8%) | 354 (44·8%) | 450 (31·4%) |
Non-fluent variant primary progressive aphasia | 107 (9·1%) | 14 (1·8%) | 26 (1·8%) |
Semantic variant primary progressive aphasia | 13 (1·1%) | 14 (1·8%) | 13 (0·9%) |
Logopenic variant primary progressive aphasia | 4 (0·3%) | 0 (0%) | 3 (0·2%) |
Primary progressive aphasia not otherwise specified* | 36 (3·1%) | 2 (0·3%) | 4 (0·3%) |
Frontotemporal dementia with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis | 7 (0·6%) | 2 (0·3%) | 157 (11·0%) |
Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis | 7 (0·6%) | 1 (0·1%) | 276 (19·3%) |
Corticobasal syndrome | 47 (4·0%) | 14 (1·8%) | 2 (0·1%) |
Progressive supranuclear palsy† | 0 (0%) | 33 (4·2%) | 1 (0·1%) |
Diagnoses outside the frontotemporal dementia spectrum | |||
Alzheimer’s disease | 97 (8·2%) | 24 (3·0%) | 84 (5·9%) |
Huntington’s disease | 0 (0%) | 1 (0·1%) | 4 (0·3%) |
Parkinson’s disease | 16 (1·4%) | 39 (4·9%) | 15 (1·0%) |
Dementia with Lewy Bodies | 4 (0·3%) | 1 (0·1%) | 5 (0·3%) |
Vascular dementia | 9 (0·8%) | 1 (0·1%) | 7 (0·5%) |
Dementia not otherwise specified* | 361 (30·6%) | 274 (34·6%) | 362 (25·3%) |
Other | 25 (2·1%) | 17 (2·1%) | 24 (1·7%) |
Does not meet criteria for a specific subtype.
Richardson’s syndrome.
The mean age at symptom onset was lowest for the MAPT group, which was significantly lower than those of the GRN and C9orf72 groups (p<0·0001 for each comparison). The C9orf72 group had the second lowest age, which was significantly lower than that of the GRN group (p<0·0001; table 1, appendix pp 35–36). However, we observed a wide range of age at symptom onset within each of the genetic groups, from the 20s to the 90s in the GRN and C9orf72 groups and from age 17 years to the 80s in the MAPT group (figure 2, appendix pp 36). Cumulative probability curves for age at symptom onset in each of the genetic groups are shown in figure 3A (appendix p 39).
We also observed a wide range of age at symptom onset among GRN and MAPT mutations (appendix pp 6–10). We plotted cumulative probability curves for age at symptom onset for the most common GRN (figure 3B) and MAPT (figure 3C) mutations (appendix p 39). These curves largely overlapped for the GRN mutations, without any significant difference between groups. By contrast, we observed a significant difference between MAPT mutations, with the Asn279Lys mutation group having a lower age at symptom onset (mean 43·8 years, SD 6·7) than that of the other groups (p≤0·0026 for all comparisons; appendix p 40). The generational analysis showed a significantly lower age at symptom onset in the second (later) generation than the first (earlier generation) in all three groups: mean age was 65·5 years (SD 9·1) in GRN first generation and 60·7 years (8·9) in GRN second generation (p<0·0001); 62·3 years (10·9) in C9orf72 first generation and 56·7 years (11·0) in C9orf72 second generation (p<0·0001); and 51·4 years (9·5) in MAPT first generation and 49· 6 years (10·0) in MAPT second generation (p=0·011; appendix pp 41–43).
We found no significant differences in age at symptom onset between men and women in the MAPT group (appendix p 44). However, in the GRN group, women were significantly older at symptom onset (61·8 years, SD 9·2) than men (60·5 years, 8·3; p=0·019), with the same observed in the C9orf72 group (58·9 years, SD 9·6, in women, compared with 57·7 years, 10·0, in men [p=0·041]).
In the C9orf72 group, we found no significant differences in mean age at symptom onset between individuals with different clinical phenotypes, except for those with a diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease (appendix pp 45–46). These individuals were significantly older at symptom onset than individuals with other phenotypes (mean 65·1 years, SD 10·6; p<0·0001 for all comparisons except for that with primary progressive aphasia [p=0·010]). Similarly, we found no significant differences in age at symptom onset between individuals with different clinical phenotypes in the GRN group, except for those with a diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease, who were significantly older at symptom onset (mean 66·4 years, SD 8·1) than individuals with other phenotypes (p<0·0001 for all comparisons). In the MAPT group, we found no significant differences in age at symptom onset between individuals with behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia and those with primary progressive aphasia; however, individuals with Alzheimer’s disease were significantly older at symptom onset (mean 56·7 years, SD 11·1) than those with behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia (p=0·0006), primary progressive aphasia (p=0·013), and a combined corticobasal syndrome and progressive supranuclear palsy (atypical parkinsonism; p<0·0001). Furthermore, individuals in the MAPT group with atypical parkinsonism were significantly younger at symptom onset (mean 44·9 years, SD 7·8) than those with other phenotypes (individuals with atypical parkinsonism vs those with behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia p=0·013; vs individuals with primary progressive aphasia p=0·037).
The mean age at death was lowest in individuals with MAPT mutations and highest in those with GRN mutations (p<0·0001 for all comparisons between groups; table 1). Age at death was variable within genetic groups (table 1, figure 2) and within individual mutations (appendix pp 6–10, 37). As with age at symptom onset, we found no significant differences in age at death between men and women in the MAPT group, but found significant differences between men and women in the GRN group (mean 69·4 years, SD 10·2, in women vs 67·8 years, 8·8, in men; p=0·029) and C9orf72 group (66·1 years, 11·0, in women vs 64·6 years, 10·8, in men; p=0·034; appendix p 44).
As with age at symptom onset, individuals with a diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease in all three genetic groups were significantly older at death than individuals with other phenotypes (appendix pp 45·46). In the C9orf72 group, individuals with a diagnosis of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis were significantly younger (mean 59·2 years, SD 9·7) at death compared with those with a diagnosis of frontotemporal dementia with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (62·1 years, 8·9; p=0·014) and those with a diagnosis of behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia (64·6 years, 9·0; p<0·0001). In turn, individuals with a diagnosis of frontotemporal dementia with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis were younger at death than those with a diagnosis of behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia (p=0·014). In the MAPT group, individuals with a diagnosis of atypical parkinsonism were significantly younger at death (mean 52·8 years, SD 8·9) compared with those with a diagnosis of behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia (60·6 years, 9·9; p=0·030) and those with a diagnosis of primary progressive aphasia (60·9 years, 15·2; p=0·036).
The mean disease duration was lowest for individuals with C9orf72 expansions, followed by those with GRN mutations, and those with MAPT mutations (p≤0·0005 for each comparison; table 1). However, within each genetic group, several individuals survived for many decades; the longest surviving individuals lived 27 years from symptom onset in the GRN group, 36 years in the C9orf72 group, and 45 years in the MAPT group (table 1, figure 2, appendix p 38). Although variability within individual mutations existed (appendix pp 6–10, 38), mean disease duration was similar across the GRN group, except for a significantly longer disease duration in individuals with an Ala9Asp mutation compared with those with Gly35fs, Thr272fs, and Arg493X mutations (appendix p 40). We found a greater variability in the mean disease duration in the MAPT group than in the GRN group. Individuals with an Arg406Trp mutation had a significantly longer disease duration than those with Pro301Leu and Asn279Lys mutations, and individuals with an IVS10+16→T mutation had a significantly longer disease duration than those with Pro301Leu mutations (appendix pp 40, 47–48).
We found no significant differences in disease duration between men and women in any of the groups (appendix p 44). We observed no significant differences in disease duration between clinical phenotypes in the GRN and MAPT groups (appendix pp 45–46). However, individuals with C9orf72 expansions and a diagnosis of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis had a significantly lower disease duration (mean 2·9 years, SD 2·8) than those of the other groups (p<0·0001 for all comparisons); individuals with C9orf72 expansions and a diagnosis of frontotemporal dementia with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis also had a lower disease duration (5·0 years, 4·2) than that of those with C9orf72 expansions and behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia (7·8 years, 4·4; p<0·0001), primary progressive aphasia (7·5 years, 4·8; p=0·0016), and Alzheimer’s disease (10·4 years, 4·9; p<0·0001; appendix pp 45–46).
Individual age at symptom onset significantly correlated with both parental and mean family age at symptom onset in all three genetic groups (p<0·0001; figure 4); in each group, individual age at symptom onset either had a similar correlation with parental and mean family age or a stronger correlation with mean family age than with parental age. The strength of these correlations varied across the genetic groups, being strongest in the MAPT group and weakest in the GRN group (figure 4). As with age at symptom onset, individual age at death significantly correlated with both parental age at death and mean family age at death in all three genetic groups (p<0·0001). We observed a similar pattern to that of age at symptom onset in the three genetic groups: the MAPT group had the strongest correlation (r=0·69 for mean family age at death, r=0·58 for parental age at death), followed by the C9orf72 group (r=0·40, r=0·38) and the GRN group (r=0·32, r=0·22).
We found significant differences between the three mutation groups in the inter-family and intra-family variability of age at symptom onset (both p<0·0001; appendix p 49). Family membership explained 66% (95% CI 56–75) of this variability in individuals with MAPT mutations, but only 14% (9–22) in those with GRN mutations and 17% (11–26) in those with C9orf72 expansions. We observed a significant difference between the GRN and MAPT groups in the between-mutation variability in age at symptom onset (p<0·0001): in the GRN group, only 2% (95% CI 0–10) of the variability was explained by the specific mutation, whereas in the MAPT group, 48% (35–62) of the variability was explained by the specific mutation.
Significant differences were also found between the three genetic groups in the variability of inter-family and intra-family age at death (both p<0·0001; appendix p 49). Family membership explained 74% (95% CI 65·82) of this variability in individuals with MAPT mutations, but only 20% (12·30) in those with GRN mutations and 19% (12–29) in those with C9orf72 expansions. We also found a significant difference between the GRN and MAPT groups in the between-mutation variability in age at death (p<0·0001): in the GRN group, only 9% (95% CI 3–21) of the variability was explained by the specific mutation, whereas in the MAPT group, 61% (47–73) of the variability was explained by the specific mutation.
Discussion
To our knowledge, we report in this study the largest dataset of age at onset, age at death, and disease duration in individuals with genetic frontotemporal dementia to date, incorporating data from across the world for the three main genetic groups and for all reported mutations in the GRN and MAPT groups. Our study provides evidence that an individual’s age at symptom onset and death in genetic frontotemporal dementia is modulated by both the individual mutation carried and family membership and varies by clinical phenotype and sex, with the strongest effect of these factors seen in individuals with MAPT mutations.
Our study provides further evidence that genetic frontotemporal dementia is a disorder that can occur throughout adult life, with symptom onset occurring from as early as the late teens to age 90 years or older. Although we did not account for individuals with mutations who were unaffected in the analysis, our findings are consistent with previous studies showing age-related penetrance in the GRN8 and C9orf729 groups, with individuals developing symptoms at age 90 years and older. A leftwards shift towards younger ages is evident in the penetrance curve in individuals with MAPT mutations (figure 3A) but nonetheless, the oldest age at symptom onset in this group was 82 years. Although usually considered a fully penetrant disorder, occasional incomplete penetrance might exist in some families with MAPT mutations (eg, Leu315Arg10, Val363Ile11,12, and Gly389Arg13), which might be age-related.
The investigation of individual mutations within GRN revealed little difference between them in terms of age at symptom onset, age at death, or disease duration. These results are consistent with the underlying pathophysiological mechanism of progranulin haplo-insufficiency being the same in most GRN mutations.14,15 By contrast, we found significant differences between individual MAPT mutations, with the mean symptom onset in the Asn279Lys mutation group occurring 12 years earlier than in the Arg406Trp mutation group. Along with the Val337Met mutation, the Arg406Trp mutation has a distinct pathological form compared with that of the other MAPT mutations, with the presence of tau pathology with paired helical filaments similar to that seen in Alzheimer’s disease; this group had a significantly longer disease duration than that of the other mutations, as previously described in single case reports.16
The generational analysis revealed significant differences in all three genetic groups, consistent with previous studies,4,17 with earlier age at symptom onset occurring in later (second) generations. These findings have been variably interpreted previously. One study has suggested that, in individuals with C9orf72 expansions, this finding was evidence of genetic anticipation.17 However, another research group interpreted this data as likely to be related to later generations recognising the disease earlier because of increased familiarity with symptoms and being more likely to be alert to the presence of such symptoms because of their awareness of being at-risk.4 At a molecular level, studies have shown that although C9orf72 expansions might be dynamic, they can both expand and contract across generations.17 Furthermore, no clear evidence exists for a relationship between age at symptom onset and expansion length, with contradictory evidence of both a positive correlation in some studies18–20 and an inverse correlation in another.21 Evidence against anticipation being an explanation for the earlier age at symptom onset in later generations also comes from the similar results observed in the GRN (corroborated by another study4) and MAPT groups: these mutations are stable and do not change molecularly across generations, therefore no plausible mechanism exists for anticipation in GRN or MAPT mutations.
Few studies have compared whether age at symptom onset, age at death, or disease duration vary by clinical phenotype within genetic groups. In our study, individuals with a diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease within each group were significantly older at symptom onset than those with other diagnoses. Although it is possible that individuals with a true amnestic presentation of genetic frontotemporal dementia do present at an older age (and that an underlying biological explanation for this exists), this is more likely to be related to the misdiagnosis of individuals with late-onset dementia as having Alzheimer’s disease. In the MAPT group, individuals with an atypical parkinsonian syndrome were significantly younger at symptom onset and at death and had a shorter disease duration than those of the other groups—this was not entirely driven by the presence of a specific mutation because the phenotype was seen across multiple mutations (eg, only 13% of this group had an Asn279Lys mutation, which has an earlier mean age at symptom onset than that of other mutations). In the C9orf72 group, the presence of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis was associated with a shorter disease duration than that of other phenotypes (with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis alone having a significantly shorter disease duration than the combined phenotype of frontotemporal dementia with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis), as previously reported.22 A previous study compared a combined frontotemporal dementia group with an amyotrophic lateral sclerosis group in individuals with C9orf72 expansions and found an earlier onset in the amyotrophic lateral sclerosis group.9 In our C9orf72 cohort, a significantly earlier onset was found in the amyotrophic lateral sclerosis group compared with that of a combined group of individuals with a cognitive presentation (appendix p 46), but this is partly driven by the Alzheimer’s disease group and no differences were found between the amyotrophic lateral sclerosis group and either the behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia or primary progressive aphasia groups individually (appendix pp 45–46).
Individual age at symptom onset was significantly correlated with both parental age at symptom onset and mean family age at symptom onset in all three genetic groups. Similarly, individual age at death was significantly correlated with both parental age at death and mean family age at death in all three genetic groups. However, we found stronger correlations in the MAPT group than in the other two groups, similar to the results found in patients with familial Alzheimer’s disease.23 The variability in age at symptom onset and at death for individuals with MAPT mutations was partly explained by the specific mutation and more so by family membership. Unlike the other genetic groups, in the MAPT group, prediction of probable age at symptom onset and at death is therefore highly related to the presence of the MAPT mutation itself. Other genetic or environmental factors affecting age at symptom onset and at death in individuals with MAPT mutations have not yet been well studied.24
Despite being statistically significant, correlation coefficients were low in the GRN group for the comparisons between individual age at symptom onset and parental and mean family age at symptom onset. The variability in age at symptom onset and age at death for individuals with GRN mutations was not accounted for particularly by either the individual mutation or family membership. This finding is consistent with previous reports of large variability within families (and specific mutations), even within the same generation.25–27 Genetic factors affecting age at symptom onset include polymorphisms in TMEM106B28,29 and potentially also in PRNP,30 but several recent studies suggest that environmental factors related to an altered neuroinflammatory response might also be important.31–34
The C9orf72 group sits between the GRN and MAPT groups in of the terms strength of correlation of individual age at symptom onset and at death with parental and mean family age at symptom onset and at death. However, similar to GRN mutations, the variability in age at symptom onset and at death was not accounted for particularly by family membership. Although conflicting evidence exists about whether expansion length is relevant,18–21 several studies have identified DNA methylation21, 35,36 and a locus on chromosome 637 as important factors in age at symptom onset, age at death, and disease duration in individuals with C9orf72 expansions (see appendix for further discussion of potential modifiers of age at symptom onset and age at death [pp 49–50]).
Our study has several limitations. One such limitation was its focus on mainly retrospective data collection, with age at symptom onset recorded as the age at which an individual was determined to have progressive cognitive, behavioural, or motor symptoms. As such, our data might be confounded by factors such as individual differences in interpreting symptom onset. This is a major issue in the study of frontotemporal dementia, for which objective measures of symptom onset are needed. A grey zone in proximity to symptom onset exists, in which subtle cognitive and behavioural deficits are present,7 but have not yet been identified as symptoms by the patient themselves or family members. Work within the FPI aims to identify such proximity markers, which will be important for future stratification in clinical trials, particularly, as identified in this study, for individuals with GRN and C9orf72 mutations, in whom prediction by age itself is poor.
Another limitation of the study is that we did not collect data on individuals with known mutations who did not develop symptoms of frontotemporal dementia. This is particularly important when assessing age-related penetrance in the GRN and C9orf72 groups, although we did identify people older than 90 years developing symptoms of frontotemporal dementia in both these groups. Attainment of data from long-living individuals with mutations will be important to better understand the modifiers of age at symptom onset and this will require large, well characterised longitudinal cohort studies, such as those in the FPI.
Although many of the centres in our study saw patients and families with all phenotypes of frontotemporal dementia, amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, and movement disorders within their clinics, the focus on genetic frontotemporal dementia within our study might have led to an underrepresentation of patients with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis or parkinsonian disorders. However, many of the families had members with multiple different phenotypes (including cognitive, behavioural, and motor), and few families had only a single phenotype, suggesting that the data in our study is unlikely to lead to a major discrepancy in phenotypic frequency.
Lastly, we did not have any data on the TMEM106B genotype (which is a known modifier in individuals with GRN mutations) nor on other genetic modifiers, such as APOE genotype, to further investigate their effect. However, such data, along with various environmental and lifestyle factors, are now being collected within the FPI and will be investigated in future studies.
In summary, we showed that individuals with MAPT mutations are younger at symptom onset and at death than those of the other groups, with the observed variance largely accounted for by family membership and the specific mutation carried. Individuals with GRN mutations had the weakest association of age at symptom onset and at death with other family members, and most of the observed variance in age at symptom onset and at death was accounted for by neither family membership nor the specific mutation. However, we found a sex effect, with increased prevalence of symptomatic individuals and older age at onset in women than in men, probably driven by age-related penetrance seen in those with GRN mutations. C9orf72 expansions were the overall most common cause of genetic frontotemporal dementia in our study. Phenotypical differences in disease duration exist, with the presence of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis leading to a shortened disease duration. As in the GRN group, little of the variance in age at symptom onset or at death was accounted for by family membership, with other genetic and environmental factors likely to be involved.
Our study highlights the strength of collaborative studies in rare diseases, bringing together data from across the world to better understand genetic frontotemporal dementia and to provide important data relevant to future trial designs. The prospective cohort studies within the FPI will hopefully provide solutions to some of the unanswered questions over the forthcoming years.
Supplementary Material
Research in context.
Evidence before this study
We searched PubMed for articles on genetic frontotemporal dementia with no language restrictions from database inception up to July 1, 2017, using the following terms: “frontotemporal dementia AND genetics”, “progranulin OR GRN”, “tau OR MAPT”, and “chromosome 9 open reading frame 72 OR C9orf72”, focusing on studies that reported age at symptom onset, age at death or disease duration of individuals with symptoms. No studies were found that had systematically investigated age at symptom onset, age at death, or disease duration across all the different genetic groups and the different mutations found within the groups. However, evidence from cohort studies and individual case series suggested that the age at symptom onset, age at death, and disease duration were highly variable across the genes implicated in frontotemporal dementia. Age-related penetrance was described in individuals with GRN and C9orf72 mutations, with MAPT mutations usually being fully penetrant. We found a generational difference in age at symptom onset, with an earlier onset in later generations occurring in individuals with GRN or C9orf72 mutations. Phenotypic differences in age at symptom onset have not been studied in detail yet, but one study showed a shorter disease duration in individuals with a diagnosis of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis in the C9orf72 group compared with those with other diagnoses, and another study showed an earlier age at symptom onset in this group compared with that of other diagnoses.
Added value of this study
To our knowledge, this is the largest international study to date investigating individual age at symptom onset, age at death, and disease duration in patients with genetic frontotemporal dementia, across all the three main genetic groups (C9orf72, GRN, and MAPT), and all known mutations within the GRN and MAPT groups. Our study provides important evidence about the factors underlying age at symptom onset, age at death, and disease duration in the different groups. We showed that only in the MAPT mutation group were age at symptom onset and at death highly correlated with both parental and mean family ages at symptom onset and at death, with variability in these ages explained partly by the specific mutation and more so by family membership. Such correlations were weaker in the other two groups, with the variability in age at symptom onset and age at death for individuals with GRN mutations and C9orf72 expansions not accounted for particularly by family membership or, for individuals with GRN mutations, by the specific mutation. This is the first time that such key differences between genetic frontotemporal dementia groups have been shown.
Implications of all the available evidence
Optimal therapeutic trial design will be important in genetic frontotemporal dementia, and particularly because many trials will aim to include presymptomatic individuals who are expected to be in proximity to symptom onset. Our study suggests that in individuals with MAPT mutations data from other family members will be particularly helpful in estimating time from symptom onset. Further work is needed to understand the variability in the other genetic groups, and other proximity markers, either individually or in combination, are likely to be required to refine the estimation of time to symptom onset in individuals with GRN or C9orf72 mutations. In the meantime, the available data will provide clinicians and family members with a better understanding of the individual risk of probable symptom onset and time to death in each genetic group and within individual mutations.
Acknowledgments
We particularly extend our appreciation to the patients and their families who have been active participants in genetic frontotemporal dementia research for over three decades.
Appendix
*FTD Prevention Initiative collaborators
Carolin Heller, Rhian Convery, Ione O C Woollacott, Rachelle Shafei, Jonathan Graff-Radford, David T Jones, Christina M Dheel, Rodolfo Savica, Maria I Lapid, Matt Baker, Julie A Fields, Ralitza Gavrilova, Kimiko Domoto-Reilly, Jackie M Poos, Emma L van der Ende, Jessica L Panman, Laura Donker Kaat, Harro Seelaar, Anna Richardson, Giovanni Frisoni, Anna Mega, Silvia Fostinelli, Huei-Hsin Chiang, Antonella Alberici, Andrea Arighi, Chiara Fenoglio, Hilary Heuer, Bruce Miller, Anna Karydas, Jamie Fong, Maria João Leitão, Beatriz Santiago, Diana Duro, Carlos Ferreira, Alazne Gabilondo, María de Arriba, Mikel Tainta, Miren Zulaica, Catarina B Ferreira, Elisa Semler, Albert Ludolph, Bernhard Landwehrmeyer, Alexander E Volk, Gabriel Miltenberger, Ana Verdelho, Sónia Afonso, Maria Carmela Tartaglia, Morris Freedman, Ekaterina Rogaeva, Camilla Ferrari, Irene Piaceri, Valentina Bessi, Gemma Lombardi, Frédéric St-Onge, Marie-Claire Doré, Rose Bruffaerts, Mathieu Vandenbulcke, Jan Van den Stock, M Marsel Mesulam, Eileen Bigio, Christos Koros, John Papatriantafyllou, Christos Kroupis, Leonidas Stefanis, Christen Shoesmith, Erik Roberson, Giovanni Coppola, Eliana Marisa Da Silva Ramos, Daniel Geschwind
Affiliations
Dementia Research Centre (C Heller MSc, R Convery MSc, I O C Woollacott MRCP, R Shafei MRCP) and UK Dementia Research Institute (C Heller), Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, Institute of Neurology, University College London, London, UK; Department of Neurology (J Graff-Radford MD, D T Jones MD, C M Dheel BSc, R Savica PhD), Department of Psychiatry (M I Lapid MD), Department of Psychology (M I Lapid, J A Fields PhD), and Department of Clinical Genomics (R Gavrilova MD), Mayo Clinic, Rochester, MN, USA; Department of Neuroscience, Mayo Clinic, Jacksonville, FL, USA (M Baker BSc); University of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA (K Domoto-Reilly MD); Department of Neurology, Erasmus Medical Centre, Rotterdam, Netherlands (J M Poos MSc, E L van der Ende MSc, J L Panman MSc, L D Kaat PhD, H Seelaar PhD); Cerebral Function Unit, Salford Royal NHS Foundation Trust and Division of Neuroscience and Experimental Psychology, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK (A Richardson MD); IRCCS Istituto Centro San Giovanni di Dio Fatebenefratelli, Brescia, Italy (G Frisoni PhD, A Mega MSc, S Fostinelli MSc); Department NVS, Center for Alzheimer Research, Division of Neurogenetics, Bioclinicum, Karolinska Institutet, Solna, Sweden (H-H Chiang MD); Unit for Hereditary Dementias, Theme Aging, Karolinska University Hospital, Solna, Sweden (H-H Chiang); Centre for Neurodegenerative Disorders, Department of Clinical and Experimental Sciences, University of Brescia, Brescia, Italy (A Alberici PhD); Fondazione IRCCS Ca’ Granda, Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Milan, Italy (A Arighi MD); Deptartment of Pathophysiology and Transplantation, University of Milan, Centro Dino Ferrari, Milan, Italy (C Fenoglio PhD); Department of Neurology, Memory and Aging Center, University of California at San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA (H Heuer PhD, B Miller MD, A Karydas, J Fong MSc); Neurology Department, Centro Hospitalar e Universitário de Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal (M J Leitão BSc); Faculty of Medicine, University of Coimbra, Portugal (B Santiago MD, D Duro BSc); Institute of Nuclear Sciences Applied to Health (ICNAS), Coimbra, Portugal (C Ferreira PhD); Department of Neurology, Hospital de Bidasoa, Irun, Spain (A Gabilondo MD); Neuroscience Area, Biodonostia Health Research Institute, San Sebastian, Spain (A Gabilondo, M de Arriba BSc, M Tainta MD, M Zulaica BSc); Center for Networked Biomedical Research on Neurodegenerative Disease, Carlos III Health Institute, Madrid, Spain (A Gabilondo, M de Arriba, M Tainta, M Zulaica); CITA Alzheimer, San Sebastian, Spain (M de Arriba, M Tainta); Laboratory of Neurosciences, Institute of Molecular Medicine (C B Ferreira MSc), Faculty of Medicine (G Miltenberger PhD), University of Lisbon, Lisbon, Portugal; Department of Neurology, University of Ulm, Germany (E Semler MSc, A Ludolph MD, B Landwehrmeyer MD); Institute of Human Genetics, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany (A E Volk MD); Department of Neurosciences and Mental Health, Centro Hospitalar Lisboa Norte, Hospital de Santa Maria and Faculty of Medicine, University of Lisbon, Lisbon, Portugal (A Verdelho PhD, S Afonso BSc); Tanz Centre for Research in Neurodegenerative Diseases, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada (M C Tartaglia PhD, E Rogaeva PhD); Baycrest Health Sciences, Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada (M Freedman MD); Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health, University of Florence, Florence, Italy (C Ferrari, I Piaceri, V Bessi, G Lombardi); Clinique Interdisciplinaire de Mémoire, Département des Sciences Neurologiques, Hôpital de l’Enfant-Jésus, Université Laval, Quebec, QC, Canada (F St-Onge MSc, M-C Doré PhD); Department of Neurology, University Hospitals Leuven, Leuven, Belgium (R Bruffaerts PhD, M Vandenbulcke PhD, J V den Stock PhD); Mesulam Center for Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer’s Disease, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL, USA (M M Mesulam MD, E Bigio PhD); Cognitive Disorders/Dementia Unit, 2nd Department of Neurology (C Koros MD, J Papatriantafyllou MD) and Department of Clinical Biochemistry (C Kroupis PhD), National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Attikon University General Hospital, Athens, Greece; 1st Department of Neurology, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Eginition Hospital, Athens, Greece (L Stefanis MD); Department of Clinical Neurological Sciences, University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada (C Shoesmith MD); Departments of Neurology and Neurobiology, Center for Neurodegeneration and Experimental Therapeutics, Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Evelyn F McKnight Brain Institute, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL, USA (E Roberson MD); Department of Psychiatry and Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior, David Geffen School of Medicine, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA (G Coppola PhD, E M S Ramos, D Geschwind MD)
Declaration of interests
KMM reports grants from the Alzheimer’s Society, during the conduct of the study. JN reports grants from the UK Medical Research Council, during the conduct of the study. MG, CTM, and DJI report grants from the National Institutes of Health (NIH), during the conduct of the study. JDW reports grants from the Alzheimer’s Society and the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) University College London Hospitals Biomedical Research Centre, during the conduct of the study. NCF reports grants from the Leonard Wolfson Experimental Neurology Centre, and the UK Dementia Research Institute, during the conduct of the study; and personal fees from Biogen, GE Healthcare, Eli Lilly, and Roche, outside the submitted work. MNR reports fees paid to the institution from Servier and Merck, outside the submitted work. BFB reports grants from the Mayo Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center and the NIH, during the conduct of the study; and personal fees from the scientific advisory board of the Tau Consortium, grants from Biogen, Mangurian Foundation, Alector, and Little Family Foundation, outside the submitted work. DSK reports personal fees from the DIAN study and grants from Eli Lilly and Biogen, outside the submitted work. NRG-R reports grants from Novartis, AbbVie, Eli Lilly, and Biogen, outside the submitted work. JCvS reports grants from the Dioraphte Foundation, the Bluefield Project to Cure Frontotemporal Dementia, Alzheimer Nederland, ZonMw Memorabel, The Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research, and The Association for Frontotemporal Dementias Research, during the conduct of the study. LHM reports grants from Deltaplan Dementie, Alzheimer Nederland, European Joint Programme—Neurodegenerative Disease Research, the Netherlands Organisation for Health Research and Development, and the Bluefield Project, during the conduct of the study; and grants from Alector, outside the submitted work. ABri reports grants from France Parkinson and Fédération pour la Recherche sur le Cerveau, Agence Nationale de Recherche, Epigenomics of Common and Age-related Diseases, EU Joint Programme—Neurodegenerative Disease Research (JPND), Roger de Spoelberch Foundation, France Alzheimer, Ecole des Neurosciences Paris, Institut de France, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire de Nimes, ERA-NET, and Assistance Publique Hôpitaux de Paris, outside the submitted work. RGh reports grants from the Italian Ministry of Health, during the conduct of the study. BCD reports grants from the NIH National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NIH-NINDS) during the conduct of the study. CG and LÖ report grants from the JPND Prefrontals Swedish Research council, Swedish Frontotemporal Dementia Initiative Schörling Foundation, Swedish Alzheimer foundation, Swedish Brain Foundation, and Stockholm County Council ALF, during the conduct of the study. LÖ also reports grants from Karolinska Institutet Doctoral Funding, StratNeuro, and Swedish Demensfonden, during the conduct of the study. NG reports grants from Tau Consortium and Rainwater Foundation, during the conduct of the study. JCM reports grants from the NIH, during the conduct of the study. DG and ES report grants from JPND and Italian Ministry of Health, during the conduct of the study. GGF reports grants from Associazione Italiana Ricerca Alzheimer ONLUS-COOP Italia, during the conduct of the study. IRM reports personal fees from Prevail Therapeutics, grants from Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR), NIH National Institute on Aging (NIH-NIA), Brain Canada, and Weston Brain Institute, outside the submitted work; and has a patent for “Detecting and treating dementia” issued. G-YRH reports grants from CIHR, Brain Canada, and NIH-NIA. ALB reports grants from NIH-NIA and NIH-NINDS, during the conduct of the study; reports stock or options from Aeton Therapeutics and Alector; reports personal fees from Abbvie, Amgen, Arkuda, Arvinas, Denali, Ionis, Janssen, Lundbeck, Merck, Pinteon, Passage BIO, Neurogenetics, Samumed, Toyama, and UCB; and grants from C2N, Cortice, Eli Lilly, Forum, Genentech, Roche, TauRx, NIH, AFTD, Bluefield Project, Tau Consortium, outside the submitted work. GH reports grants from the National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia, during the conduct of the study. RS-V reports grants from Fundació Marató de TV3, Spain, during the conduct of the study. SB-E reports grants from the Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Spain, during the conduct of the study. FM reports grants from the Tau Consortium, during the conduct of the study. JBR reports grants from the NIHR, Wellcome Trust, and Medical Research Council, during the conduct of the study; travel funds from Guarantors of Brain; personal fees from Asceneuron, Astex, and Biogen; and grants from Janssen, AZ Medimmune, and Eli Lilly, outside the submitted work. MO and SA-S report grants from the Federal Ministry of Education and Research, Germany, during the conduct of the study. MM reports grants from the CIHR, during the conduct of the study; is part of the editorial board of Current Pharmacogenomics and Personalized Medicine; personal fees from Arkuda Therapeutics and Ionis; and grants from the CIHR, Early Researcher Award, Ministry of Economic Development and Innovation of Ontario, Ontario Brain Institute, Sunnybrook AFP Innovation Fund, Alzheimer’s Drug Discovery Foundation, Brain Canada, Heart and Stroke Foundation Centre for Stroke Recovery, Weston Brain Institute, Roche, Washington University, Axovant, Novartis, and Alector; and reports royalties from Henry Stewart Talks, outside the submitted work. SEB reports grants and personal fees from Eli Lilly, Novartis, Biogen Idec, and Roche; and grants from GE Healthcare, Genentech, and Optina, outside the submitted work. EDH reports grants from the NIH-NINDS, outside the submitted work. RV reports grants from the Mady Browaeys Fund for Research into Frontotemporal Dementia, during the conduct of the study. PVD reports membership of advisory board for Pfizer, Cytokinetics, and Alexion Pharmaceuticals, outside the submitted work. EJR reports grants from the NIH, during the conduct of the study. SW reports grants from the NIH, during the conduct of the study, and consulting fees from Bracket Global, outside the submitted work. CUO reports grants from the NIH, and support for research from the Jane Tanger Black Scholarship, the Nancy H Hall Memorial Fund, and the Joseph Trovato Fund, during the conduct of the study; and reports a grant from Biogen, outside the submitted work. ASLN reports grants from the National Medical Research Council Singapore, during the conduct of the study. EF reports grants from the CIHR, during the conduct of the study. JDR reports grants from the UK Medical Research Council, the NIHR Rare Diseases Translational Research Collaboration, the Bluefield Project, and the Association for Frontotemporal Degeneration, during the conduct of the study; and was on the medical advisory boards for Ionis, Alector, and Prevail, outside the submitted work. MF reports an International Patent Application (PCT/CA2009/000346) and an Israeli Patent Application (208134) pending. All other authors declare no competing interests.
Contributor Information
Katrina M Moore, Dementia Research Centre, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, Institute of Neurology, University College London, London, UK.
Jennifer Nicholas, Department of Medical Statistics, London School of Hygiene & Tropical Medicine, London, UK.
Murray Grossman, Department of Neurology, Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA.
Corey T McMillan, Department of Neurology, Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA.
David J Irwin, Department of Neurology, Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA.
Lauren Massimo, Department of Neurology, Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA.
Vivianna M Van Deerlin, Department of Neurology, Penn Frontotemporal Degeneration Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA.
Jason D Warren, Dementia Research Centre, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, Institute of Neurology, University College London, London, UK.
Nick C Fox, Dementia Research Centre, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, Institute of Neurology, University College London, London, UK.
Martin N Rossor, Dementia Research Centre, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, Institute of Neurology, University College London, London, UK.
Simon Mead, Institute of Prion Diseases, University College London, London, UK.
Martina Bocchetta, Dementia Research Centre, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, Institute of Neurology, University College London, London, UK.
Bradley F Boeve, Department of Neurology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, MN, USA.
David S Knopman, Department of Neurology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, MN, USA.
Neill R Graff-Radford, Department of Neurology, Mayo Clinic, Jacksonville, FL, USA.
Leah K Forsberg, Department of Neurology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, MN, USA.
Rosa Rademakers, Department of Neuroscience, Mayo Clinic, Jacksonville, FL, USA.
Zbigniew K Wszolek, Department of Neurology, Mayo Clinic, Jacksonville, FL, USA.
John C van Swieten, Department of Neurology, Erasmus Medical Centre, Rotterdam, Netherlands.
Lize C Jiskoot, Department of Neurology, Erasmus Medical Centre, Rotterdam, Netherlands.
Lieke H Meeter, Department of Neurology, Erasmus Medical Centre, Rotterdam, Netherlands.
Elise GP Dopper, Department of Neurology, Erasmus Medical Centre, Rotterdam, Netherlands.
Janne M Papma, Department of Neurology, Erasmus Medical Centre, Rotterdam, Netherlands.
Julie S Snowden, Cerebral Function Unit, Salford Royal NHS Foundation Trust and Division of Neuroscience and Experimental Psychology, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK.
Jennifer Saxon, Cerebral Function Unit, Salford Royal NHS Foundation Trust and Division of Neuroscience and Experimental Psychology, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK.
Matthew Jones, Cerebral Function Unit, Salford Royal NHS Foundation Trust and Division of Neuroscience and Experimental Psychology, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK.
Stuart Pickering-Brown, Cerebral Function Unit, Salford Royal NHS Foundation Trust and Division of Neuroscience and Experimental Psychology, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK.
Isabelle Le Ber, Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle épinière & Centre de Référence des Démences Rares ou précoces, Institut de la Mémoire et de la Maladie d’Alzheimer, Assistance Publique-Hôpitaux de Paris, Hôpital de la Pitié-Salpêtrière, Paris, France.
Agnès Camuzat, Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle épinière & Centre de Référence des Démences Rares ou précoces, Institut de la Mémoire et de la Maladie d’Alzheimer, Assistance Publique-Hôpitaux de Paris, Hôpital de la Pitié-Salpêtrière, Paris, France.
Alexis Brice, Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle épinière & Centre de Référence des Démences Rares ou précoces, Institut de la Mémoire et de la Maladie d’Alzheimer, Assistance Publique-Hôpitaux de Paris, Hôpital de la Pitié-Salpêtrière, Paris, France.
Paola Caroppo, Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle épinière & Centre de Référence des Démences Rares ou précoces, Institut de la Mémoire et de la Maladie d’Alzheimer, Assistance Publique-Hôpitaux de Paris, Hôpital de la Pitié-Salpêtrière, Paris, France.
Roberta Ghidoni, Molecular Markers Laboratory, Istituto di Ricovero e Cura a Carattere Scientifico Centro San Giovanni di Dio Fatebenefratelli, Brescia, Italy.
Michela Pievani, Alzheimer’s Neuroimaging & Epidemiology Laboratory, Istituto di Ricovero e Cura a Carattere Scientifico Centro San Giovanni di Dio Fatebenefratelli, Brescia, Italy.
Luisa Benussi, Molecular Markers Laboratory, Istituto di Ricovero e Cura a Carattere Scientifico Centro San Giovanni di Dio Fatebenefratelli, Brescia, Italy.
Giuliano Binetti, Molecular Markers Laboratory, Istituto di Ricovero e Cura a Carattere Scientifico Centro San Giovanni di Dio Fatebenefratelli, Brescia, Italy.
Bradford C Dickerson, Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA.
Diane Lucente, Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA.
Samantha Krivensky, Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA.
Caroline Graff, Center for Alzheimer Research, Division of Neurogenetics, Department of Neurobiology, Care Sciences and Society, Bioclinicum, Karolinska Institutet, Solna, Sweden; Unit for Hereditary Dementias, Theme Aging, Karolinska University Hospital, Solna, Sweden.
Linn Öijerstedt, Center for Alzheimer Research, Division of Neurogenetics, Department of Neurobiology, Care Sciences and Society, Bioclinicum, Karolinska Institutet, Solna, Sweden; Unit for Hereditary Dementias, Theme Aging, Karolinska University Hospital, Solna, Sweden.
Marie Fallström, Center for Alzheimer Research, Division of Neurogenetics, Department of Neurobiology, Care Sciences and Society, Bioclinicum, Karolinska Institutet, Solna, Sweden; Unit for Hereditary Dementias, Theme Aging, Karolinska University Hospital, Solna, Sweden.
Håkan Thonberg, Center for Alzheimer Research, Division of Neurogenetics, Department of Neurobiology, Care Sciences and Society, Bioclinicum, Karolinska Institutet, Solna, Sweden; Unit for Hereditary Dementias, Theme Aging, Karolinska University Hospital, Solna, Sweden.
Nupur Ghoshal, Department of Neurology, Knight Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, Washington University School of Medicine, St Louis, MO, USA.
John C Morris, Department of Neurology, Knight Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, Washington University School of Medicine, St Louis, MO, USA.
Barbara Borroni, Centre for Neurodegenerative Disorders, Department of Clinical and Experimental Sciences, University of Brescia, Brescia, Italy.
Alberto Benussi, Centre for Neurodegenerative Disorders, Department of Clinical and Experimental Sciences, University of Brescia, Brescia, Italy.
Alessandro Padovani, Centre for Neurodegenerative Disorders, Department of Clinical and Experimental Sciences, University of Brescia, Brescia, Italy.
Daniela Galimberti, Department of Biomedical, Surgical and Dental Sciences, Centro Dino Ferrari, University of Milan, Milan, Italy; Istituto di Ricovero e Cura a Carattere Scientifico Fondazione Ca’ Granda, Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Milan, Italy.
Elio Scarpini, Department of Pathophysiology and Transplantation, Centro Dino Ferrari, University of Milan, Milan, Italy; Istituto di Ricovero e Cura a Carattere Scientifico Fondazione Ca’ Granda, Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Milan, Italy.
Giorgio G Fumagalli, Department of Pathophysiology and Transplantation, Centro Dino Ferrari, University of Milan, Milan, Italy; Istituto di Ricovero e Cura a Carattere Scientifico Fondazione Ca’ Granda, Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Milan, Italy; Department of Neurosciences, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health, University of Florence, Florence, Italy.
Ian R Mackenzie, Division of Neurology, Department of Medicine, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada.
Ging-Yuek R Hsiung, Division of Neurology, Department of Medicine, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada.
Pheth Sengdy, Division of Neurology, Department of Medicine, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada.
Adam L Boxer, Department of Neurology, Memory and Aging Center, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA.
Howie Rosen, Department of Neurology, Memory and Aging Center, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA.
Joanne B Taylor, Department of Neurology, Memory and Aging Center, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA.
Matthis Synofzik, Department of Neurodegenerative Diseases, Center for Neurology and Hertie-Institute for Clinical Brain Research, Tübingen, Germany; German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases, Tübingen, Germany.
Carlo Wilke, Department of Neurodegenerative Diseases, Center for Neurology and Hertie-Institute for Clinical Brain Research, Tübingen, Germany; German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases, Tübingen, Germany.
Patricia Sulzer, Department of Neurodegenerative Diseases, Center for Neurology and Hertie-Institute for Clinical Brain Research, Tübingen, Germany; German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases, Tübingen, Germany.
John R Hodges, Brain and Mind Centre & Central Clinical School, Faculty of Medicine and Health, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia.
Glenda Halliday, Brain and Mind Centre & Central Clinical School, Faculty of Medicine and Health, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia.
John Kwok, Brain and Mind Centre & Central Clinical School, Faculty of Medicine and Health, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia.
Raquel Sanchez-Valle, Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Cognitive Disorders Unit, Hospital Clínic, Institut d’Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer, Barcelona, Spain.
Albert Lladó, Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Cognitive Disorders Unit, Hospital Clínic, Institut d’Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer, Barcelona, Spain.
Sergi Borrego-Ecija, Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Cognitive Disorders Unit, Hospital Clínic, Institut d’Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer, Barcelona, Spain.
Isabel Santana, Center for Neuroscience and Cell Biology, University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal; Faculty of Medicine, University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal; Neurology Department, Centro Hospitalar e Universitário de Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal.
Maria Rosário Almeida, Center for Neuroscience and Cell Biology, University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal.
Miguel Tábuas-Pereira, Neurology Department, Centro Hospitalar e Universitário de Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal.
Fermin Moreno, Cognitive Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology, Donostia Universitary Hospital, San Sebastian, Spain; Neuroscience Area, Biodonostia Health Research Institute, San Sebastian, Spain; Center for Networked Biomedical Research on Neurodegenerative Disease, Carlos III Health Institute, Madrid, Spain.
Myriam Barandiaran, Cognitive Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology, Donostia Universitary Hospital, San Sebastian, Spain; Neuroscience Area, Biodonostia Health Research Institute, San Sebastian, Spain; Center for Networked Biomedical Research on Neurodegenerative Disease, Carlos III Health Institute, Madrid, Spain.
Begoña Indakoetxea, Cognitive Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology, Donostia Universitary Hospital, San Sebastian, Spain; Neuroscience Area, Biodonostia Health Research Institute, San Sebastian, Spain; Center for Networked Biomedical Research on Neurodegenerative Disease, Carlos III Health Institute, Madrid, Spain.
Johannes Levin, Department of Neurology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Munich, Germany; German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases, Munich, Germany; Munich Cluster for Systems Neurology, Munich, Germany.
Adrian Danek, Department of Neurology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Munich, Germany.
James B Rowe, Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK.
Thomas E Cope, Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK.
Markus Otto, Department of Neurology, University of Ulm, Ulm, Germany.
Sarah Anderl-Straub, Department of Neurology, University of Ulm, Ulm, Germany.
Alexandre de Mendonça, Faculty of Medicine, University of Lisbon, Lisbon, Portugal.
Carolina Maruta, Faculty of Medicine, University of Lisbon, Lisbon, Portugal.
Mario Masellis, Division of Neurology, Department of Medicine, Hurvitz Brain Sciences Program, Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada.
Sandra E Black, Division of Neurology, Department of Medicine, Hurvitz Brain Sciences Program, Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada.
Philippe Couratier, Centre de Compétence Démences Rares, Centre Hospitalier et Universitaire Limoges, Limoges, France.
Geraldine Lautrette, Centre de Compétence Démences Rares, Centre Hospitalier et Universitaire Limoges, Limoges, France.
Edward D Huey, Departments of Psychiatry and Neurology, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA.
Sandro Sorbi, Department of Neurosciences, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health, University of Florence, Florence, Italy; Istituto di Ricovero e Cura a Carattere Scientifico Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi, Florence, Italy.
Benedetta Nacmias, Department of Neurosciences, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health, University of Florence, Florence, Italy.
Robert Laforce, Jr, Clinique Interdisciplinaire de Mémoire, Département des Sciences Neurologiques, Hôpital de l’Enfant-Jésus, and Faculté de Médecine, Université Laval, Québec, QC, Canada.
Marie-Pier L Tremblay, Clinique Interdisciplinaire de Mémoire, Département des Sciences Neurologiques, Hôpital de l’Enfant-Jésus, and Faculté de Médecine, Université Laval, Québec, QC, Canada.
Rik Vandenberghe, Department of Neurology, University Hospitals Leuven, Leuven, Belgium.
Philip Van Damme, Department of Neurology, University Hospitals Leuven, Leuven, Belgium; Center for Brain & Disease Research, VIB, Leuven, Belgium.
Emily J Rogalski, Mesulam Center for Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer’s Disease, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL, USA.
Sandra Weintraub, Mesulam Center for Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer’s Disease, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL, USA.
Alexander Gerhard, Wolfson Molecular Imaging Centre, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK; Departments of Nuclear Medicine and Geriatric Medicine, University Hospital Essen, Essen, Germany.
Chiadi U Onyike, Division of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neuropsychiatry, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA.
Simon Ducharme, Montreal Neurological Institute, McConnell Brain Imaging Centre, McGill University Health Centre, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada.
Sokratis G Papageorgiou, Cognitive Disorders/Dementia Unit, 2nd Department of Neurology, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Attikon University General Hospital, Athens, Greece.
Adeline Su Lyn, Department of Neurology, National Neuroscience Institute, Singapore, Singapore.
Amy Brodtmann, Florey Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health, Melbourne, VIC, Australia.
Elizabeth Finger, Department of Clinical Neurological Sciences, University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada.
Rita Guerreiro, Center for Neurodegenerative Science, Van Andel Research Institute, Grand Rapids, MI, USA.
Jose Bras, Center for Neurodegenerative Science, Van Andel Research Institute, Grand Rapids, MI, USA.
Jonathan D Rohrer, Dementia Research Centre, Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, Institute of Neurology, University College London, London, UK.
References
- 1.Warren JD, Rohrer JD, Rossor MN. Clinical review. Frontotemporal dementia. BMJ 2013; 347: f4827. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Harris JM, Gall C, Thompson JC, et al. Classification and pathology of primary progressive aphasia. Neurology 2013; 81: 1832–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Rohrer JD, Guerreiro R, Vandrovcova J, et al. The heritability and genetics of frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Neurology 2009; 73: 1451–56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Barbier M, Camuzat A, Houot M, et al. Factors influencing the age at onset in familial frontotemporal lobar dementia: important weight of genetics. Neurol Genet 2017; 3: e203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Cosseddu M, Benussi A, Gazzina S, et al. Mendelian forms of disease and age at onset affect survival in frontotemporal dementia. Amyotroph Lateral Scler Frontotemporal Degener 2018; 19: 87–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Ferrari R, Grassi M, Graziano F, et al. Effects of multiple genetic loci on age at onset in frontotemporal dementia. J Alzheimers Dis 2017; 56: 1271–78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Rohrer JD, Nicholas JM, Cash DM, et al. Presymptomatic cognitive and neuroanatomical changes in genetic frontotemporal dementia in the Genetic Frontotemporal dementia Initiative (GENFI) study: a cross-sectional analysis. Lancet Neurol 2015; 14: 253–62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Gass J, Cannon A, Mackenzie IR, et al. Mutations in progranulin are a major cause of ubiquitin-positive frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Hum Mol Genet 2006; 15: 2988–3001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Murphy NA, Arthur KC, Tienari PJ, Houlden H, Chiò A, Traynor BJ. Age-related penetrance of the C9orf72 repeat expansion. Sci Rep 2017; 7: 2116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.van Herpen E, Rosso SM, Serverijnen LA, et al. Variable phenotypic expression and extensive tau pathology in two families with the novel tau mutation L315R. Ann Neurol 2003; 54: 573–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Anfossi M, Bernardi L, Gallo M, et al. MAPT V363I variation in a sporadic case of frontotemporal dementia: variable penetrant mutation or rare polymorphism? Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2011; 25: 96–99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Munoz DG, Ros R, Fatas M, Bermejo F, de Yebenes JG. Progressive nonfluent aphasia associated with a new mutation V363I in tau gene. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen 2007; 22: 294–99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Rossi G, Marelli C, Farina L, et al. The G389R mutation in the MAPT gene presenting as sporadic corticobasal syndrome. Mov Disord 2008; 23: 892–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Baker M, Mackenzie IR, Pickering-Brown SM, et al. Mutations in progranulin cause tau-negative frontotemporal dementia linked to chromosome 17. Nature 2006; 442: 916–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Cruts M, Gijselinck I, van der Zee J, et al. Null mutations in progranulin cause ubiquitin-positive frontotemporal dementia linked to chromosome 17q21. Nature 2006; 442: 920–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Ygland E, van Westen D, Englund E, et al. Slowly progressive dementia caused by MAPT R406W mutations: longitudinal report on a new kindred and systematic review. Alzheimers Res Ther 2018; 10: 2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Van Mossevelde S, van der Zee J, Gijselinck I, et al. Clinical evidence of disease anticipation in families segregating a C9orf72 repeat expansion. JAMA Neurol 2017; 74: 445–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.van Blitterswijk M, DeJesus-Hernandez M, Niemantsverdriet E, et al. Association between repeat sizes and clinical and pathological characteristics in carriers of C9ORF72 repeat expansions (Xpansize-72): a cross-sectional cohort study. Lancet Neurol 2013; 12: 978–88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Nordin A, Akimoto C, Wuolikainen A, et al. Extensive size variability of the GGGGCC expansion in C9orf72 in both neuronal and non-neuronal tissues in 18 patients with ALS or FTD. Hum Mol Genet 2015; 24: 3133–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Fournier C, Barbier M, Camuzat A, et al. Relations between C9orf72 expansion size in blood, age at onset, age at collection and transmission across generations in patients and presymptomatic carriers. Neurobiol Aging 2019; 74: 234.e1–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Gijselinck I, Van Mossevelde S, van der Zee J, et al. The C9orf72 repeat size correlates with onset age of disease, DNA methylation and transcriptional downregulation of the promoter. Mol Psychiatry 2016; 21: 1112–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Irwin DJ, McMillan CT, Brettschneider J, et al. Cognitive decline and reduced survival in C9orf72 expansion frontotemporal degeneration and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2013; 84: 163–69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Ryman DC, Acosta-Baena N, Aisen PS, et al. Symptom onset in autosomal dominant Alzheimer disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurology 2014; 83: 253–60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Koriath C, Lashley T, Taylor W, et al. ApoE4 lowers age at onset in patients with frontotemporal dementia and tauopathy independent of amyloid-β copathology. Alzheimers Dement 2019; 11: 277–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Kelley BJ, Haidar W, Boeve BF, et al. Prominent phenotypic variability associated with mutations in Progranulin. Neurobiol Aging 2009; 30: 739–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Beck J, Rohrer JD, Campbell T, et al. A distinct clinical, neuropsychological and radiological phenotype is associated with progranulin gene mutations in a large UK series. Brain 2008; 131: 706–20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Le Ber I, Camuzat A, Hannequin D, et al. Phenotype variability in progranulin mutation carriers: a clinical, neuropsychological, imaging and genetic study. Brain 2008; 131: 732–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Nicholson AM, Rademakers R. What we know about TMEM106B in neurodegeneration. Acta Neuropathol 2016; 132: 639–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Pottier C, Zhou X, Perkerson RB 3rd, et al. Potential genetic modifiers of disease risk and age at onset in patients with frontotemporal lobar degeneration and GRN mutations: a genome-wide association study. Lancet Neurol 2018; 17: 548–58. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Moreno F, Alzualde A, Camblor PM, et al. Prion protein codon 129 polymorphism modifies age at onset of frontotemporal dementia with the C.709-1G>A progranulin mutation. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2011; 25: 93–95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Bossu P, Salani F, Alberici A, et al. Loss of function mutations in the progranulin gene are related to pro-inflammatory cytokine dysregulation in frontotemporal lobar degeneration patients. J Neuroinflammation 2011; 8: 65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Martens LH, Zhang J, Barmada SJ, et al. Progranulin deficiency promotes neuroinflammation and neuron loss following toxin-induced injury. J Clin Invest 2012; 122: 3955–59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Miller ZA, Rankin KP, Graff-Radford NR, et al. TDP-43 frontotemporal lobar degeneration and autoimmune disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2013; 84: 956–62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Menzel L, Kleber L, Friedrich C, et al. Progranulin protects against exaggerated axonal injury and astrogliosis following traumatic brain injury. Glia 2017; 65: 278–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Russ J, Liu EY, Wu K, et al. Hypermethylation of repeat expanded C9orf72 is a clinical and molecular disease modifier. Acta Neuropathol 2015; 129: 39–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Zhang M, Tartaglia MC, Moreno D, et al. DNA methylation age-acceleration is associated with disease duration and age at onset in C9orf72 patients. Acta Neuropathol 2017; 134: 271–79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Zhang M, Ferrari R, Tartaglia MC, et al. A C6orf10/LOC101929163 locus is associated with age of onset in C9orf72 carriers. Brain 2018; 141: 2895–907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.