Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2020 Feb 10;15(2):e0228756. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0228756

Effect of restricting bedtime mobile phone use on sleep, arousal, mood, and working memory: A randomized pilot trial

Jing-wen He 1,#, Zhi-hao Tu 2,#, Lei Xiao 1, Tong Su 1, Yun-xiang Tang 1,*
Editor: Sergio Garbarino3
PMCID: PMC7010281  PMID: 32040492

Abstract

Background

This study aimed to assess the effects of restricting mobile phone use before bedtime on sleep, pre-sleep arousal, mood, and working memory.

Methods

Thirty-eight participants were randomized to either an intervention group (n = 19), where members were instructed to avoid using their mobile phone 30 minutes before bedtime, or a control group (n = 19), where the participants were given no such instructions. Sleep habit, sleep quality, pre-sleep arousal and mood were measured using the sleep diary, the Pittsburgh sleep quality index, the Pre-sleep Arousal Scale and the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule respectively. Working memory was tested by using the 0-,1-,2-back task (n-back task).

Results

Restricting mobile phone use before bedtime for four weeks was effective in reducing sleep latency, increasing sleep duration, improving sleep quality, reducing pre-sleep arousal, and improving positive affect and working memory.

Conclusions

Restricting mobile phone use close to bedtime reduced sleep latency and pre-sleep arousal and increased sleep duration and working memory. This simple change to moderate usage was recommended to individuals with sleep disturbances.

Introduction

Mobile phones have become an essential tool in our daily life in the past decade. The number of mobile phone users in China has increased from 0.39 billion in 2012 to 0.72 billion in 2017, accounting for 72.2% in 2012 and 96.3% of the netizens in 2017. Students are the largest group of Chinese Internet users [1]. Mobile phone use prior to bedtime or even after lights-out is a common habit among many young adults. However, this unhealthy habit may lead to delayed bedtime, sleep loss, irregular sleep-wake patterns, poor sleep quality, and increased tiredness during the day [24]. In addition, media content or games might induce pre-sleep hyperarousal [5].

Sleep plays an important role in mood cognitive function. Sleep loss, sleep restriction, and sleep disorders might have a negative effect on mood and cognitive functions. Previous studies found that sleep disorders were associated with mood deficits [6], and sleep loss led to anger, confusion, anxiety, and depression [78]. In addition, sleep loss might impair working memory [9] and vigilant attention [10]. Furthermore, one meta-analysis found that individuals with insomnia exhibit performance impairments in several cognitive functions, including working memory and executive functioning [11].

Many cross-sectional studies have investigated the relationship between mobile phone use and sleep [1215]. Nevertheless, regarding the effects of restricting mobile phone use around bedtime on sleep, the number of existing randomized-controlled trials is limited. Furthermore, these studies report contradictory results. To our knowledge, only Harris et al investigated the effect of restricting the use of electronic media after 22:00 in high school athletes, and they did not find any improvement in sleep habits, athletic performance, cognitive performance, or mood after four weeks of this intervention [16]. Meanwhile, another study assessed the impact of restricting phone use for one hour before bedtime and found that this restriction could advance lights-out time and increase total sleep-time [17].

Thus, in our study, we aimed to investigate the effects of restricting mobile phone use before bedtime in college students who have a habit of using a mobile phone before sleep and thus have poor sleep quality. We hypothesized that the restriction of mobile phone use will help reduce pre-sleep arousal, enable early sleep, and regulate a healthier sleep with good sleep quality. Additionally, we presumed that sleep will mediate mood and cognitive performances.

Methods

2.1. Participants

Study participants were recruited from our university through social media from April 1st to April 8th 2019. The inclusion criteria were poor sleep quality (Pittsburgh sleep quality index [PSQI]>5) and a habit of using a mobile phone during bedtime (>30 mins). The exclusion criteria included the following: (1) self-reported medical conditions such as depression, schizophrenia, metabolic diseases, cardiovascular diseases, chronic or recurrent respiratory conditions, active cancer, or neurological disorders; (2) other self-reported sleep disorders such as obstructive sleep apnea-hypopnea syndrome, restless leg syndrome, or rapid eye movement sleep disorder; (3) use of medications, devices, or hypnotics to assist sleep; (4) excessive caffeine use (>500 mg/day) or excessive alcohol use (>4 cups/day); (5) shift workers or those with work requiring long-distance driving or operating heavy equipment; (6) self-reported total nap duration of >90 min/day or excessive daytime sleepiness; and (7) pregnancy, lactation, or perimenopausal state with irregular menses.

A total of 72 individuals were screened, and 32 were found to be ineligible. Out of these 32 individuals, 23 were ineligible because of the presence of other sleep disturbances, five volunteers were ineligible because of their mobile phone use habit during bedtime or for use for less than 30 minutes prior to time of sleep. Two participants were ineligible because of medication use to help with sleep and another two were ineligible because of a history of depression. After screening, two participants were dropped because they were being deployed in the South China sea for medical treatment. (Fig 1)

Fig 1. Flow chart.

Fig 1

This study was approved by the Ethical Committee of the Second Military Medical University. A complete survey description was first presented to the participants. Written informed consent and oral approval were obtained before the baseline measurements were taken.

2.2. Intervention

All participants entered baseline data before intervention and data after a week’s trial of intervention into an online sleep diary. Then, the participants of the intervention group were instructed to refrain from using their mobile phone for 30 minutes prior to their average bedtime. In the intervention group, 12 participants used Huawei, five used Apple, and two used Mi phones. For participants whose mobile phones were Huawei or Apple, we could set the screen time. Using the “manage screen time” function in the “settings,” we set the mode as follows: "this is my child's phone" and then “bedtime” and then a password was added. Thus, during the set “bedtime,” the screen would turn grey and no apps could be opened; the phone goes off service after this set time. As for participants whose mobile phones were of other brands, they were instructed to turn them off at the appointed time every night. One researcher would send a message to remind them to turn off their mobile phones 10 minutes before their appointed time, and two researchers would call them at any time after turn-off time every night to ensure that everyone had turned off their mobile phones.

The participants in the control group did not receive any instructions regarding their sleep or mobile phone use.

2.3. Measures

Assessments were conducted at baseline and post-intervention.

2.3.1. Sociodemographic variables

The sociodemographic variables included age, sex, body mass index, marital status, exercise habits, smoking, and the frequency of drinking coffee, tea, or alcohol.

2.3.2. Sleep quality measure

The PSQI is a 19-item self-reported questionnaire that assesses sleep quality and disturbances over the past month. The total score comprises seven component scores: subjective sleep quality, sleep latency, sleep duration, habitual sleep efficiency, sleep disturbances, use of sleep medication, and daytime dysfunction. Scores range from 0 to 21 and a high score indicates poor sleep [18].

2.3.3. Sleep diary

Sleep diary data one week before and after the intervention were collected. An online sleep diary was sent to every individual by “Wechat” in the morning. The participants were instructed to complete the sleep diary as soon as they woke up. The sleep diary items included bedtime, sleep latency, wake time, rise time, sleep duration, mobile phone usage time per day, and mobile phone usage time prior to bedtime. Mobile phone usage time prior to bedtime was defined as phone use from 9:00 pm to the time of sleep. Sleep diaries are viewed as inexpensive and valid measures of sleep in healthy participants and are known to be highly correlated with data from actigraphy.

2.3.4. Arousal measure

Pre-sleep arousal was evaluated with the Pre-sleep Arousal Scale (PSAS) [19]. PSAS is a 16-item, self-reported questionnaire comprising both cognitive and somatic manifestations of arousal, with eight items in each subscale. Two subscale scores ranging from 8 to 40 were computed separately: Pre-sleep Arousal Scale Cognitive Arousal (PSAS-C) with sum items 1–8, and Pre-sleep Arousal Scale Somatic Arousal (PSAS-S) with sum items 8–16. When responding to the PSAS, subjects were asked to describe how intensely they generally experienced each component as they attempted to fall asleep in their own bedroom by selecting an appropriate rating of “1: do not at all,” “2: slightly,” “3: moderately,” “4: a lot,” or “5: extremely,” during the past week.

2.3.5. Mood measure

The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) is a 20-item scale for measuring the positive and negative effects based on a 5-point Likert scale. Ten questions are related to the positive effects, including enthusiasm, pleasure, and engagement. A high score reflects a high degree of positive effect. Ten questions are related to the negative effects such as feeling distressed, upset, or guilty. A high score indicates a high degree of negative effect [20].

2.3.6. Working memory

In the n-back task, participants were shown a series of numbers presented one by one for a duration of 1000 milliseconds each and an intertrial interval of 700 milliseconds. This task involves three different memory load conditions: 0-, 1-, and 2-back. Participants respond by clicking the mouse button. In the 0-back condition, participants are instructed to click the right button every time a “five” is displayed and the left button when other numbers were presented. In the 1-back condition, they are instructed to click the right button when the number presented is identical to the previous one (e.g., a 5 followed by a 5) and to click the left button if not identical (e.g., a 5 followed by a 4). In the 2-back condition, they are instructed to click the right button when the current number is identical to the one before the last (e.g., a 5 followed by a 1 and then another 5) and click the left mouse button when they are not identical. There were six blocks (two for each condition), each block consisting of 30 trials. Each block lasted for approximately two minutes, leading to a total testing time of 12 minutes. The practice consisted of 10 trials for all conditions, and once all the practice trials were correctly completed, participants were allowed to start the task. Outcome measures were the reaction time and accuracy.

2.4. Statistical analysis

Before performing statistical analyses, trials with reaction times (RTs) exceeding ±3 standard deviations (SD) from the mean were excluded as outliers and removed from the RT and accuracy analyses. Outliers comprised less than 1% of the trials for each group and condition.

No formal sample size calculations were made as this was a pilot study. Statistical analysis was performed with SPSS version 22.0. Descriptive statistics such as mean, range, SD, and percentages were calculated. We used the unpaired t-test to test group differences in baseline statistics. Between-subject analyses of variance, with one between-group factor (intervention vs control group) and one within-subjects/repeated measures factor (baseline vs follow-up), were conducted to assess the effect of the intervention on participants’ scores on sleep, arousal, mood, and cognitive performance. The main effects on time and group, and the interaction effects (group × time) are reported. When the interaction effects were significant, a simple effect analysis was conducted.

Results

The demographic information and characteristics of participants are reported in Table 1. No significant differences between the groups at baseline were observed.

Table 1. Baseline demographics and clinical characteristics.

Intervention group
N = 19
Control group
N = 19
P
Sex 0.728
Male 12(63.2%) 14(73.7%)
Female 7(36.8%) 5(26.3%)
Age 20.95±2.068 21.37±2.63 0.587
Marital status 0.486
Single 19(100%) 17(89.5%)
Married 0(0%) 2(10.5%)
Coffee consumption 0.115
Never or seldom 9(47.4%) 12(63.2%)
Sometimes 10(52.6%) 5(26.3%)
Often or always 0(0%) 2(10.5%)
Tea consumption 0.743
Never or seldom 7(36.8%) 9(47.4%)
Sometimes 12(63.2%) 10(52.6%)
Often or always 0 0
Smoking status 0.486
Never or seldom 19(100%) 17(89.5%)
Sometimes 0 0
Often or always 0 2(10.5%)
Alcohol consumption 0.295
Never or seldom 15(78.9%) 11(57.9%)
Sometimes 4(21.1%) 8(42.1%)
Often or always 0 0
Duration of mobile phone use in one day (hours) 5.47±1.87 5.82±3.31 0.698
Duration of mobile phone use during bedtime (hours) 1.47±1.08 1.39±0.68 0.809

3.1. Sleep

There was a significant interaction between groups and time regarding sleep latency, sleep duration, and PSQI scores (Table 2). Analysis of the simple effect test results for sleep latency showed a significant difference in the between-group post-test (p = 0.049) and a significant difference (p<0.01) over time in the intervention group (Fig 2A). Analysis of the simple effect test results for sleep duration showed a significant difference (p = 0.035) over time in the intervention group (Fig 2B). Analysis of the simple effect test results for PSQI scores showed a significant difference in the between-groups post-test (p<0.01) and a significant difference (p<0.01) over time in the intervention group (Fig 2C).

Table 2. Sleep, arousal, mood, and working memory by group over time.

Intervention Control Time effect Group effect Time x group
Pretest Posttest Pretest Posttest
Sleep latency 30.98(14.22) 18.38(11.85) 29.32(10.13) 26.54(12.78) 16.34** 0.85 6.67*
Sleep duration 6.20(0.58) 6.56(0.71) 6.48(0.45) 6.27(0.68) 0.43 0.003 5.91*
PSQI 10.05(2.51) 5.63(1.64) 9.63(1.64) 8.58(1.57) 31.47** 7.71** 11.92**
PSAS-S 14.58(5.07) 10.79(3.03) 14.68(4.37) 14.32(4.56) 9.42** 2.19 6.38*
PSAS-C 23.32(7.23) 16.37(6.07) 24.21(4.72) 22.05(3.55) 23.52** 4.55* 6.51*
Positive affect 26.53(7.13) 31.53(6.39) 26.58(5.48) 26.00(6.12) 4.82* 2.36 7.68**
Negative affect 18.26(5.80) 14.84(4.68) 21.58(5.19) 20.74(7.50) 3.632 8.84** 1.33
0-back ACC 0.99(0.016) 0.98(0.017) 0.98(0.016) 0.98(0.031) 1.635 1.41 0.093
1-back ACC 0.94(0.055) 0.97(0.018) 0.94(0.070) 0.95(0.038) 5.017* 0.94 0.604
2-back ACC 0.93(0.072) 0.96(0.042) 0.91(0.066) 0.93(0.046) 7.174* 2.85 0.257
0-back RT 571.96(114.18) 499.63(85.74) 570.82(89.52) 555.62(83.56) 6.079* 1.22 2.590
1-back RT 651.23(118.84) 581.69(98.36) 634.34(93.08) 634.64(82.76) 4.810* 0.41 4.892*
2-back RT 774.87(197.92) 670.50(171.35) 815.35(186.53) 730.79(139.34) 18.537** 0.92 0.204

PSQI, the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; PSAS-S, Pre-sleep Arousal Scale Somatic Arousal; PSAS-C, Pre-sleep Arousal Scale Cognitive Arousal; ACC, accuracy; RT, reaction time.

*p<0.05.

**p<0.01.

Fig 2. The change in sleep after the intervention.

Fig 2

A, sleep latency at baseline and at the end of intervention. B, sleep duration at baseline and at the end of intervention. C, PSQI scores at baseline and at the end of intervention. Note: PSQI, the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index. Intervention group is designated with a white square; control group is designated with a black square. *p<0.05. **p<0.01.

3.2. Pre-sleep hyperarousal

There was a significant interaction between groups and time regarding somatic and cognitive arousal (Table 2). Analysis of the simple main effect test results for somatic arousal showed a significant difference in the between-groups post-test (p = 0.008) and a significant difference (p<0.01) over time in the intervention group (Fig 3A). Analysis of the simple main effect test results for cognitive arousal showed a significant difference in the between-groups post-test (p = 0.001) and a significant difference (p<0.01) over time in the intervention group (Fig 2B).

Fig 3. The change in pre-sleep hyperarousal and mood after the intervention.

Fig 3

A, PSAS-S at baseline and at the end of intervention. B, PSAS-C at baseline and at the end of intervention. C, positive affection at baseline and at the end of intervention. D, negative affection at baseline and at the end of intervention. Note: PSAS-S, Pre-sleep Arousal Scale Somatic Arousal; PSAS-C, Pre-sleep Arousal Scale Cognitive Arousal. Intervention group is designated with a white square; control group is designated with a black square. *p<0.05. **p<0.01.

3.3. Mood

There was a significant interaction between groups and time for positive affect (Table 2). Analysis of the simple main effect test results showed a significant difference in the between-groups post-test (p = 0.01), and a significant difference (p = 0.001) over time in the intervention group (Fig 3C). There was an important effect on the group regarding the negative affect (Table 2). The results indicated that there was a significant difference in the between-group post-test (p = 0.06) and a significant difference (p = 0.037) over time in the intervention group (Fig 3D).

3.4. Working memory

No significant main effects of time and group, and interaction (group × time) was found in 0-back accuracy (Table 2 and Fig 4A). There was a significant main effect of time in 1-back accuracy (Table 2). The result indicated that there was a significant difference in the between-groups post-test (p = 0.046) and a significant difference (p = 0.04) over time in 1-back accuracy in the intervention group (Fig 4C). A significant main effect of time in 2-back accuracy was found (Table 2). The result indicated that there was a significant difference in between-group post-test (p = 0.036) and a significant difference (p = 0.03) over time in 2-back accuracy in the intervention group (Fig 4E).

Fig 4. The change in n-back after the intervention.

Fig 4

A, 0-back ACC at baseline and at the end of intervention. B, 0-back RT at baseline and at the end of intervention. C, 1-back ACC at baseline and at the end of intervention. D, 1-back RT at baseline and at the end of intervention. E, 2-back ACC at baseline and at the end of intervention. F, 2-back RT at baseline and at the end of intervention. ACC, accuracy; RT, reaction time. Intervention group is designated with a white square; control group is designated with a black square. *p<0.05. **p<0.01.

A significant main effect of time in 0-back RT was detected (Table 2). Analysis of the simple main effect test results showed a significant difference in the between-groups post-test (p = 0.049) and a significant difference (p = 0.07) over time in 0-back RT in the intervention group (Fig 4B). For 1-back RT, there was a significant interaction between groups and with time (Table 2). Analysis of the simple main effect test results showed a significant difference (p = 0.004) over time in 1-back RT in the intervention group (Fig 4D). A significant main effect of time was found in 2-back RT (Table 2). Analysis of the simple main effect test results showed a significant difference (p = 0.002) over time in the intervention group and a significant difference (p = 0.01) over time in 2-back RT in the control group (Fig 4F).

Discussion

In this study, the effects of restricting mobile phone use before bedtime on sleep, pre-sleep arousal, mood, and working memory were investigated. After a four-weeks of intervention, effective reduction of sleep latency, increased sleep duration, improved sleep quality, reduced pre-sleep arousal, reduced negative affect, and improved positive affect and working memory were achieved.

Sleep latency in the intervention group was reduced by around 12 minutes and sleep duration was increased by around 18 minutes. Sleep quality was improved significantly. This was consistent with the results of a previous study which found that adolescents who stopped using their phones 80 min earlier and turned their lights off 17 min earlier, slept 21 min longer after one week of phone usage restriction [17]. This implies that limiting the use of mobile phones before bedtime could effectively improve sleep. First, it could reduce the impact of light emitted by mobile phones on sleep. Second, reducing the arousal induced by mobile phone contents may also help sleep. This simple change to moderate use was recommended by media scholars.

Pre-sleep arousal was decreased significantly in the intervention group, suggesting that it could be reduced by restricting mobile phone usage around bedtime. Mobile phone content could induce pre-sleep arousal, which might lead to difficulty in falling asleep and poor sleep quality. To our knowledge, no published report has shown the effect of restricting nocturnal mobile phone use on pre-sleep arousal. Pre-sleep arousal was common among individuals with sleep disorders, and it was an important etiological factor of insomnia disorders [21]. It has been reported that insomnia patients have somatic, cognitive, and cortical arousal [2225].

Sleep plays a very important role in mediating multiple domains of affective functioning including emotion, emotion memory, and emotion regulation. Previous studies have found the impact of sleep deprivation and sleep restriction on mood [2629], and the influence of sleep extension on the reduction of the tendency to fall into a depressed mood [30]. Furthermore, all participants in the research agreed that using their mobile phones during bedtime affected sleep, and they had a high level of motivation to reduce use. When their goals are achieved, positive affect could increase and negative affect could decrease.

The participants of the intervention group had higher accuracy in the 1- and 2-back tasks and faster reaction times in the 0- and 1-back tasks. These results suggest that restricting mobile phone usage around bedtime could improve working memory. Previous studies on children and adolescents have demonstrated that sleep restriction and deprivation could affect cognitive functions [31]. Increasing sleep duration could enhance cognitive performance [32]. In our study, sleep quality of individuals in the intervention group was shown to have improved significantly, having a positive effect on cognitive function.

Several limitations of the present study must be addressed. First, the present study used a sleep diary and PSQI to measure the participants’ sleep, which may not be very objective. Further studies using objective measures such as actigraphy are needed for a more objective evaluation of sleep. Second, the participants of both groups agreed that excessive mobile phone use around bedtime could disturb their sleep and were all willing to reduce the amount of use around bedtime. However, these results might not be generally applicable to participants who did not agree that phone use around bedtime affects sleep and who lacked motivation to restrict mobile phone use around bedtime. Third, the sample size was small, thus limiting the interpretation of the results. These findings might set the basis for future studies.

In summary, restricting mobile phone use during bedtime for four weeks reduced sleep latency, pre-sleep arousal, and negative affect, and increased sleep duration, positive affect, and working memory. This intervention was effective in improving sleep quality and could be used as an adjunctive treatment for individuals with poor sleep quality and who are highly motivated to reduce bedtime mobile phone use.

Supporting information

S1 File. Data sets of the study.

(SAV)

S2 File. Questionnaire.

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We sincerely thank the students for their participation and all the medical staff involved in the testing. We would like to thank Editage (www.editage.cn) for English language editing.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by the Mental Health Application Research of PLA (12XLZ109) and the National Natural Science Foundation of China (81372122) to YT. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.China Internet Network Information Center (CNNIC). The 40th China statistical report on internet development. Office of the Central Leading Group for Cyberspace Affairs. 2017. 10.1089/cyber.2012.0482 [DOI]
  • 2.Bartel KA, Gradisar M, Williamson P,. Protective and risk factors for adolescent sleep: a meta-analytic review. Sleep Med Rev. 2014;21: 72–85. 10.1016/j.smrv.2014.08.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Kubiszewski V., Fontaine R., Rusch E., Hazouard E. Association between electronic media use and sleep habits: an eight-day follow-up study. Int J Adolesc. Youth. 2013;19 (3): 395–407. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Exelmans L. Bedtime Mobile Phone Use and Sleep in Adults. Social Science & Medicine. 2016;148: 93–101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Dill KE, Gentile DA, Richter WA, Dill JC. Violence, sex, race, and age in popular video games: a content analysis In: Cole E., Henderson D.J. (Eds.), Featuring Females: Feminist Analyses of the Media. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Palagini L, Moretto U, Dell'Osso L, Carney C. Sleep-related cognitive processes, arousal, and emotion dysregulation in insomnia disorder: the role of insomnia-specific rumination. Sleep Medicine. 2017;30: 97–104. 10.1016/j.sleep.2016.11.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Babson KA, Trainor CD, Feldner MT, Blumenthal H. A test of the effects of acute sleep deprivation on general and specific self-reported anxiety and depressive symptoms: an experimental extension. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2010;41(3): 297–303. 10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.02.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Short M A, Louca M. Sleep deprivation leads to mood deficits in healthy adolescents. Sleep Medicine, 2015, 16(8):987–993. 10.1016/j.sleep.2015.03.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Rangtell F H, Karamchedu S, Andersson P, Liethof L, Olaya Bucaro Marcela, Lampola L, et al. A single night of sleep loss impairs objective but not subjective working memory performance in a sex-dependent manner[J]. Journal of Sleep Research, 2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Lim J, Dinges D F. Sleep deprivation and vigilant attention[J]. Ann N Y Acad Sci, 2010, 1129(1):305–322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.émilie Fortier-Brochu, Beaulieu-Bonneau S, Ivers H, Ivers H, Morin C M. Insomnia and daytime cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Sleep Medicine Reviews. 2012;16(1): 0–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Amra B, Shahsavari A, Shayan-Moghadam R, Mirheli O, Moradi-khaniabadi, Bazukar M, et al. The association of sleep and late-night cell phone use among adolescents. Jornal de Pediatria. 2017:S0021755717301511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Munezawa T, Kaneita Y, Osaki Y, Kanda H, Minowa M, Suzuki K, et al. The association between use of mobile phones after lights out and sleep disturbances among Japanese adolescents: a nationwide cross-sectional survey. Sleep. 2011;34: 1013–20. 10.5665/SLEEP.1152 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hysing M, Pallesen S, Stormark K M, Jakobsen R, Lundervold A J, Sivertsen B. Sleep and use of electronic devices in adolescence: results from a large population-based study. BMJ Open. 2015;5(1):e006748–e006748. 10.1136/bmjopen-2014-006748 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Thomée S. Mobile Phone Use and Mental Health. A Review of the Research That Takes a Psychological Perspective on Exposure. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2018;15(12): 2692. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Harris Anette, Gunderson Hilde, Mørk-Andreassen Pia, Thun Eirunn, Bjorvatn Bjørn, Pallesen Ståle. Restricted use of electronic media, sleep, performance, and mood in high school athletes—a randomized trial. Sleep Health. 2015; 1(4):S2352721815001564. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Bartel K, Scheeren R, Gradisar M. Altering Adolescents’ Pre-Bedtime Phone Use to Achieve Better Sleep Health. Health Commu. 2018:1–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Buysse D J, Reynolds C F, Monk T H, Hoch C C, Yeager A L, Kupfer D J. Quantification of subjective sleep quality in healthy elderly men and women using the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI). Sleep. 1991;14(4): 331–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Nicassio PM, Mendlowitz DR, Fussell JJ, Petras L. The phenomenology of the presleep state: the development of the pre-sleep arousal scale. Behav Res Ther. 1985;23(3): 263–71. 10.1016/0005-7967(85)90004-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures ofpositive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54(6):1063–1070. 10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Riemann D; Spiegelhalder K; Feige B; Voderholzer U; Berger M; Perlis M; Nissen C. The hyperarousal model of insomnia: A review of the concept and its evidence. Sleep Med Rev. 2010;14(1): 19–31. 10.1016/j.smrv.2009.04.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Dodds K L, Miller C B, Kyle S D, Marshall N S, Gordon C J. Heart rate variability in insomnia patients: a critical review of the literature. Sleep Med Rev. 2016:S1087079216300533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Varkevisser M, Van Dongen H P, Kerkhof G A. Physiologic indexes in chronic insomnia during a constant routine: evidence for general hyperarousal?. Sleep. 2005; 28(12):1588–96. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Hantsoo L, Khou C S, White C N, along J C. Gender and cognitive–emotional factors as predictors of pre-sleep arousal and trait hyperarousal in insomnia. Journal of Psychosom Res, 2013;74(4):283–289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Fernandez-Mendoza J, Li Y, Vgontzas A N, Eang J, Gaines J, Calhoun S L, et al. Insomnia is Associated with Cortical Hyperarousal as Early as Adolescence. Sleep. 2016;39(5): 1029–1036. 10.5665/sleep.5746 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Dagys N, McGlinchey E L, Talbot L S, Kaplan K A, Dahl RE, Harvey A G. Double trouble? The effects of sleep deprivation and chronotype on adolescent affect. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012;53: 660–667. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02502.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Baum KT, Desai A, Field J, Miller LE, Rausch J, Beebe DW. Sleep restriction worsens mood and emotion regulation in adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2014;55(2):180–90. 10.1111/jcpp.12125 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Franzen P L, Siegle G J, Buysse D J. Relationships between affect, vigilance, and sleepiness following sleep deprivation. J Sleep Res. 2010;17(1): 34–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Kahn M, Fridenson S, Lerer R, Bar-Haim Y, Sadeh A. Effects of one night of induced night-wakings versus sleep restriction on sustained attention and mood: a pilot study. Sleep Med. 2014;15(7): 825–832. 10.1016/j.sleep.2014.03.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Dewald-Kaufmann JF, Oort FJ, Meijer AM. The effects of sleep extension and sleep hygiene advice on sleep and depressive symptoms in adolescents: a randomized controlled trial. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2014;55(3): 273–83. 10.1111/jcpp.12157 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Astill RG, van der Heijden KB, Van IJzendoorn MH, van Someren EJW. Sleep, cognition, and behavioral problems in school-age children: a century of research meta-analyzed. Psychol Bull. 2012;138: 1109–38. 10.1037/a0028204 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Ritland BM, Simonelli G, Gentili RJ, Smith JC, He X, Mantua J, et al. Effects of sleep extension on cognitive/motor performance and motivation in military tactical athletes. Sleep Med. 2019; 58: 48–55. 10.1016/j.sleep.2019.03.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Sergio Garbarino

11 Oct 2019

PONE-D-19-19506

Effect of Restricting Bedtime Mobile Phone Use on Sleep, Arousal, Mood and Working memory

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr Tang,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

We would appreciate receiving your revised manuscript by Nov 25 2019 11:59PM. When you are ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter.

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). This letter should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Manuscript'.

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Sergio Garbarino

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

1. When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

http://www.journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and http://www.journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please note that there are still two instances (line 30, line 138) where 'diary' has been spelt 'dairy'. Please ensure these errors are corrected as part of your revisions.

3. Thank you for stating that “The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript” in your financial disclosure.

Please also provide the name of the funders of this study (as well as grant numbers if available) in your financial disclosure statement.

Please include your amended statements within your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

4. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

Reviewer #2: No

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: In this study, He et al reported how reduction use of mobile phone at bedtime affected sleep through Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI), Pre-sleep Arousal Scale (PSAS), Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) and n-back task, and concluded that reduction use of mobile phone at bedtime was very helpful to sleep, although the number of participant was not big enough (n=19).

Majors:

It is confused that several significance marks (* or **) were missing in Table 2 and Figs 2 and 3 according to results (Line 202-246). The writing is poor and tables are ugly.

Minors:

I suggest authors make all the statistical raw data available in spreadsheet format (like Microsoft excel) and describe statistical analysis in text with more details.

Figure legends are missing (only abbreviations).

Line 8 and 10: 1 and 2 should be superscript.

Line 30, 138: “dairy”?

Line 30-34: rephrase with ‘respectively’.

Line 82-83: rephrase

Line 94: screen should be screened

Line 94-99: confused, need rephrase

Line 135: missing original reference of PSQI

Line 137-138: Sleep diary data of the four weeks between that were collected or not?

Line 142: rephrase

Line 158: reference should be after period.

Line 161-162: “There are 10 questions for positive and 10 for negative affects”, redundant.

Line 172-173: “when the number presented is not identical to the previous one” replaced with “if not”.

Line 176-179: confused. Need rephrase.

Table 1 and 2: ugly, need revise.

Line 202-246 of results: Significance for some tests cannot be found in Table 2, need correct for each. For example:

Table 2: A star (*) is missing in the group effect of sleep latency and time effect of sleep duration according to Line 205 and 207-208, respectively.

Fig 2 and 3: Significance (* and **) should be noted with corresponding p-value for each test.

Line 212-219: correct Table 2 and Figs for significance

Line 222-225: correct positive affect for significance in Table 2 and Figs.

Line 227: it is not significant for p=0.06. Correct Table 2 and Figs.

Line 233-234, Line 236-237: Don’t know it is for accuracy or reaction time, or for 2-back accuracy?

Line 235: redundant with Line 232

Line 238-246: not corresponding to Table 2.

Line 262-264: “In this” study, “the effects” …. Was investigated.

Line 268: Sleep quality “was” improved significantly.

Line 269: One research “group”

Line 270-271: adolescents “who” stopped, delete “and”

Line 276-277: arousal “was” decreased, suggesting that it…

Line 278: no published report has shown

Line 281: It was reported that insomnia patients have somatic, cognitive and cortical arousal [21-24].

Line 282-283: “Content in the mobile….sleep quality” should be in Line 278.

Line 284-285: redundant. Sleep “plays”

Line 288-289: rephrase

Line 294: suggesting

Line 296-300: rephrase

Line 304-308: confused

Line 309: remove “four weeks of”, add “for four weeks” after “bedtime”.

Others:

1. The study presents the results of original research.

Yes.

2. Results reported have not been published elsewhere.

Yes.

3. Experiments, statistics, and other analyses are performed to a high technical standard and are described in sufficient detail.

Ok.

4. Conclusions are presented in an appropriate fashion and are supported by the data.

Ok.

5. The article is presented in an intelligible fashion and is written in standard English.

Ugly tables. Poor writing.

6. The research meets all applicable standards for the ethics of experimentation and research integrity.

Yes.

7. The article adheres to appropriate reporting guidelines and community standards for data availability.

Yes.

Reviewer #2: 1) Authors should briefly provide a description/synonym of netizens, since not all readers may be familiar with this term.

2) There are some typos. Some sentences begin with "And". English should be revised for the sake of clarity.

3) Only after reading methods, I come to know that the study is a randomized trial. However, authors did not follow reporting guidelines for randomized trials (for example, the title does not contain informative keywords). Did authors register the randomized trial in a database of RTs?

4) Sample size is very small (because was small already the recruited sample size at the commencement of the trial, after a lot of individuals being excluded). However, authors performed a lot of statistical analyses. Seems (to my opinion) that the study is underpowered to make sufficiently strong statements and conclusions. Is it a pilot study? How much is the statistical power found (in tems of effect sizes)? And the post-hoc power Did authors perform a sample power analysis?

5) Some important studies are not mentioned (one for all: Mobile Phone Use and Mental Health. A Review of the Research That Takes a Psychological Perspective on Exposure. Thomée S. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2018 Nov 29;15(12)).

6) Graphs miss bars of standard deviations.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Guo Luo

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files to be viewed.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2020 Feb 10;15(2):e0228756. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0228756.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


8 Nov 2019

Thank you for your comments concerning our manuscript entitled “Effect of Restricting Bedtime Mobile Phone Use on Sleep, Arousal, Mood and Working memory” (ID: PONE-D-19-19506). Those comments are valuable and very helpful for revising and improving our paper, as well as the important guiding significance to our researches. We have modified our manuscript according to the comments. Besides, we have carefully checked through the whole manuscript and corrected some grammar mistakes. We hope the revised paper would meet the requirement from you.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Sergio Garbarino

23 Jan 2020

Effect of restricting bedtime mobile phone use on sleep, arousal, mood, and working memory: A randomized pilot trial

PONE-D-19-19506R1

Dear Dr. Tang,

We are pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it complies with all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you will receive an e-mail containing information on the amendments required prior to publication. When all required modifications have been addressed, you will receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will proceed to our production department and be scheduled for publication.

Shortly after the formal acceptance letter is sent, an invoice for payment will follow. To ensure an efficient production and billing process, please log into Editorial Manager at https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the "Update My Information" link at the top of the page, and update your user information. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to enable them to help maximize its impact. If they will be preparing press materials for this manuscript, you must inform our press team as soon as possible and no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

With kind regards,

Sergio Garbarino

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The authors should carefully check grammar/typos again before print.

Reviewer #2: All comments and concerns of reviewers have been properly addressed and the manuscript has been considerably improved.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes: Nicola Luigi Bragazzi

Acceptance letter

Sergio Garbarino

3 Feb 2020

PONE-D-19-19506R1

Effect of restricting bedtime mobile phone use on sleep, arousal, mood, and working memory: A randomized pilot trial

Dear Dr. Tang:

I am pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper at this point, to enable them to help maximize its impact. If they will be preparing press materials for this manuscript, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

For any other questions or concerns, please email plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE.

With kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Sergio Garbarino

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 File. Data sets of the study.

    (SAV)

    S2 File. Questionnaire.

    (DOCX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES