Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2020 Feb 11;9:e51168. doi: 10.7554/eLife.51168

Body height in young adult men and risk of dementia later in adult life

Terese Sara Høj Jørgensen 1,2,, Gunhild Tidemann Okholm 1,2, Kaare Christensen 3, Thorkild IA Sørensen 2,4, Merere Osler 1,2
Editors: Belinda Nicolau5, Eduardo Franco6
PMCID: PMC7012597  PMID: 32041683

Abstract

This study examined the relationship between body height and dementia and explored the impact of intelligence level, educational attainment, early life environment and familial factors. A total of 666,333 men, 70,608 brothers, and 7388 twin brothers born 1939–1959 and examined at the conscript board were followed in Danish nationwide registers (1969–2016). Cox regression models were applied to analyze the association between body height and dementia. Within-brothers and within-twin pair analyses were conducted to explore the role of shared familial factors including partly shared genetics. In total, 10,599 men were diagnosed with dementia. The association between one z-score difference in body height and dementia (HR: 0.90, 95% CI: 0.89;0.90) was inverse and weakened slightly after adjustment for intelligence test scores and educational level. The associations persisted in within-brother analysis and revealed a stronger, but less precise, point estimate than the cohort analysis of brothers. The twin analysis showed similar, but imprecise estimates.

Research organism: Human

Introduction

Dementia poses substantial challenges for individuals and societies worldwide. (Livingston et al., 2017) This has motivated studies of potential predictors and risk factors for dementia in recent years. (Livingston et al., 2017) Development of dementia may be a result of both genetics and environmental exposures operating throughout the life course. (Borenstein et al., 2006; Bird, 2005) A newly published meta-analysis and systematic review showed that the risk of dementia may already be established early in life. (Wang et al., 2019) Body height has a strong genetic component and is at the same time influenced by environmental factors such as childhood diseases and nutrition. (Jelenkovic et al., 2016a; Jelenkovic et al., 2016b) Short height has been linked to development of dementia in a number of smaller studies (N = 203–3,734) and to dementia as cause of death in a large study pooling 18 prospective cohorts (N = 181,800). (Russ et al., 2014; Petot et al., 2007; Abbott et al., 1998; Gatz et al., 2006; Huang et al., 2008; Beeri et al., 2005) Body growth may be related to dementia as an indicator of brain and cognitive reserve and corresponding individual differences in the brain structure, which could imply differences in individual resilience toward development of dementia. (Wang et al., 2019) Another possible explanation of the height-dementia association is the correlation between body height and level of growth hormone that through hippocampal function and cognition has been linked to the risk of developing dementia. (Borenstein et al., 2006; Abbott et al., 1998; Beeri et al., 2005) Thus, rather than being a risk factor in itself, short body height is likely an indicator of harmful exposures early in life. (Russ et al., 2014) However, large scale high-quality longitudinal studies exploring the impact of early environmental factors and genetics to explain the link between body height and dementia are needed. (Borenstein et al., 2006; Wang et al., 2019; Russ et al., 2014) Previous studies question whether body height and dementia are solely linked by brain and cognitive reserve measured by intelligence and socioeconomic factors, or by shared underlying familial factors including genetics that may influence both body height and dementia. In this study, we examined whether body height in young adult men is associated with diagnosis of dementia while exploring whether intelligence test score, educational level, and underlying environmental and genetic factors shared by brothers explain the relationship.

Results

Figure 1 presents the selection of the three study populations; 1) men (N = 666,333), 2) brothers (N = 70,608), and 3) twins (N = 7,388). The total study population of 666,333 men were followed for an average of 41.4 years from a baseline mean age of 22.1 years (due to delayed entry for some men) to a maximum age of 57–77 years. The mean age at measurement of height was 19.73 years (95% confidence interval (CI):19.73;19.74). Supplementary file 1 provides body height measures and information on dementia diagnoses of the total population of men. In total, 10,599 (1.6%) men were registered with dementia diagnosis during follow-up. The mean height was 176.8 cm (SD: 6.6). The Table furthermore shows that the mean height was greater among men with higher intelligence level, higher level of education, younger birth cohorts, and those living in the Capital region. Similarly, these groups also experiences fewer cases of dementia.

Figure 1. Selection of the study population.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Time line of data collection from each of the registers and the total follow-up time.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

Foot note: *Anatomic Therapeutical Chemical (ATC) code for dementia (N06D) is also used for patients with Parkinson’s disease, but the treatment is only indicated if the patients have comorbid dementia. **Dementia was identified by international classification of disease (ICD) version 8 codes (290.00–290.99) until the end of 1994 and by ICD10 codes (F00.0-F03.9; G30.0-G30.9) from 1995 and thereafter.

In Model 1, the estimated hazard ratio (HR) was 0.86 (95%CI:0.85–0.87), which attenuated slightly after the additional adjustments for educational level and intelligence test scores in Model 2a and 2b (Table 1). The fully adjusted estimate of the birth cohort-specific z-score of body height (Model 3) showed that for each one unit taller z-score of body height, the HR of dementia were 0.90 (95% CI:0.89;0.90). This was the same estimate as found for the model adjusted for intelligence test scores without educational level (Model 2b). Supplementary file 2 shows that the mean heights range from 174.5 to 179.1 cm and the z-scores range from 6.34 to 6.6 cm for the birth cohorts in the total population without any clear patterns of change over time. The finding of 10% lower HR with one z-score higher body height suggests that men born in 1939 with a body height of 181.5 cm (equal to one z-score taller than the mean) can expect a 10% lower dementia risk compared with men with the mean body height (175.0 cm). The corresponding body height for the latest birth cohort (1959) is 185.6 cm compared with the mean body height of 179.1 cm. Figure 2 shows that the association between z-scores of body height with dementia diagnosis is curve-linear; the HRs showed a slightly stronger increase with lower z-scores of body height below the z-score reference of 0 than above the z-score reference of 0.

Table 1. Hazard ratios (HRs) and corresponding 95% confidence intervals (95% CIs) of the association between taller body height at entry to adulthood and dementia diagnoses among the total population of men.

Descriptive statistics HR (95% CI) for onset of dementia per one z-score* higher
Mean height (Standard diviation) Dementia cases
Number (%)
Model 1 Model 2a Model 2b§ Model 3
176.8
(6.6)
10,599
(1.6)
0.86
(0.85;0.87)
0.88
(0.87;0.89)
0.90
(0.89;0.90)
0.90
(0.89;0.90)

*Identify cohort-specific values of one z-score in Supplementary file 2.

Model 1: Stratified by birthcohort and conscript board district. Age included as underlying scale of the model.

Model 2a: Model 1 + adjusted for educational level.

§Model 2b: Model 1 + adjusted for intelligence test scores.

Model 3: Model 1 + adjusted for educational level and intelligence test scores.

Figure 2. Hazard Ratios (HRs) and corresponding 95% confidence intervals (95% CIs) for the association between z-score of body height at the entry to adulthood as a cubic spline with four knots and dementia.

A z-score of 0 is the reference value. Stratified by birth cohort and adjusted for conscript board district, educational level and intelligence test scores. Age included as underlying scale of the model. The analysis included the total population of men. Identify cohort-specific values of one z-score in Supplementary file 2.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Distribution of mean numeric difference in body height in cm between brothers (A) and twins (B).

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

Note: to hide micro data, the value 26 includes all values above for brother and the value 17 includes all values above for twins. In cases where there were more than two brothers with a shared mother, the difference in body height between all brothers were included. For example, for a groups of three brothers, the differences between brother one and two, brother one and three, and brother two and three, respectively, were included.

Analyses of Model 3 with the risk period divided at age less than 60 years and 60 years and older showed that the association between one z-score taller body height and dementia was slightly smaller among men below 60 years (HR:0.87, 95% CI: 0.84;0.90) than men 60 years and older (HR:0.91, 95% CI: 0.90;0.92) (Table 2).

Table 2. Fully adjusted* hazard ratios (HRs) and corresponding 95% confidence intervals (95% CIs) of the association between taller body height at entry to adulthood and dementia diagnosis among all men.

Dementia cases
Number (%)
HR (95% CI) for onset of dementia per one z-score* higher
<60 years
4191 (0.6) 0.87 (0.84;0.90)
≥60 years
6408 (1.3) 0.91 (0.90;0.92)

*Identify cohort-specific values of one z-score in Supplementary file 2.

Model 3: stratified by birth cohort and adjusted for conscript board district, educational level and intelligence test scores. Age included as underlying scale of the model.

Analyses of Model 3 with the follow-up period divided by 1) prior to 1995 and 2) 1995 and thereafter showed that the association between one z-score taller body height and dementia was comparable between the two periods (Supplementary file 3). The numeric mean (SE) body height difference was 5.1 (0.02) cm between brothers and 3.5 (0.06) cm between twins (See Figure 2—figure supplement 1 for distribution of the numeric difference in body height for brothers (A) and twins (B)). The analyses of Model 3 showed that the within-pair analysis of brothers revealed a stronger, but less precise, point estimate than the cohort analysis of brothers (Table 3). For twins, the comparison of the cohort analysis of twins and the within-pair analysis of twins revealed a pattern similar to that found for brothers. Yet, the pattern was less clear and the HRs were imprecisely estimated possible due to a lower number of individuals in these analyses (N = 7,388) (Table 3).

Table 3. Fully adjusted* hazard ratios (HRs) and corresponding 95% confidence intervals (95% CIs) of the association between taller body height at entry to adulthood and dementia diagnosis among brothers and twins.

Descriptive statistics HR (95% CI) for onset of dementia per one z-score* higher
Mean height
(standard deviation)
Dementia cases
Number (%)
Cohort analyses Within-brother/twin analyses
Brothers
177.8 (6.6) 597 (0.8) 0.90 (0.82;0.98) 0.78 (0.64;0.96)
Twins
175.8 (6.7) 107 (1.4) 0.91 (0.73;1.16) 0.86 (0.44;1.68)

*Identify cohort-specific values of one z-score in Supplementary file 2.

Model 3: Stratified by birth cohort and adjusted for conscript board district, educational level and intelligence test scores. Age included as underlying scale of the model.

Discussion

In this nationwide cohort of Danish men born from 1939 through 1959, we detected that taller body height at the entry to adulthood was associated with lower risk of dementia diagnosis later in life. The relationship attenuated, but persisted after adjustments for educational level and intelligence test scores. Within-brother analysis confirmed the findings of a relationship between taller body height and lower risk of dementia diagnosis, and even revealed a stronger, but less precise, point estimate of the association than the cohort analysis of brothers. The findings from the within-twin analyses were in the same direction, but with more uncertainty. These findings support that the relationship between body height and dementia is not explained by shared genetics and exposures to other familial factors shared between brothers.

Strengths and limitations

This study has a number of strengths. It is based on a very large unselected population followed from early adulthood until a maximum age of 57–77 years for dementia diagnoses. This study design is very important as previous findings may have underestimated the relationship due to selection of the healthiest part of the population, who are willing to participate, and have survived into this age. It is also a great advantage that we were able to investigate associations between body height and dementia adjusted for intelligence test scores and educational level. Intelligence together with educational level have been shown to be strong markers of cognitive reserve, which are hypothesized to be highly predictive of the age at which dementia is manifested and diagnosed. (Borenstein et al., 2006) We further included objective measures of body height rather than self-reported height with its implicit reporting errors. In the same line, this study included measures of body height from early adulthood, whereas previous studies have used measures from mid- and late-life. This could imply a bias because declining height since young adulthood (Cline et al., 1989) has been found to be more pronounced in socioeconomic dis-advantaged groups and those with lower later-life cognitive function. (Huang et al., 2013) Further, underdiagnoses of dementia has in some countries been shown to be greater in socioeconomically disadvantaged groups. (Lang et al., 2017) A final strength is that we were able to investigate the impact of familial factors, including various degrees of shared genetics and environmental factors, by conducting within-brother and twin-pair analyses.

We acknowledge the limitations of the study. A potential concern is that even though dementia diagnoses in the Danish National Patient Register is likely to be correct, (Phung et al., 2007; Salem et al., 2013) under-diagnosis is possible. The registers and identifications to define dementia change over time (Figure 1—figure supplement 1) and this difference in coverage could lead to differences in diagnostic validity and completeness at different time points during follow-up. However, we believe this is a minor issue as a previous study of data from the the Danish Conscription Database (DCD), which investigated the relationship between cognitive ability and dementia, only found small differences in risk estimates when using international classification of disease (ICD) version eight and ICD10 codes, respectively, to identify dementia. (Osler et al., 2017) Furthermore, the introduction of the Danish Prescription Registry on January 1st1995 has enabled identification of dementia patients who are not in contact with the secondary healthcare system for their dementia diagnosis, but who receive medication through their general practitioner. This has most likely improved the sensitivity of the dementia diagnoses from 1995. Our sensitivity analyses showed comparable findings of the relationship between body height and dementia before and after the introduction, yet the comparison of these results were limited by including men in different age-groups (<1995: maximum age of 36–56 and ≥1995: maximum age of 57–77). Another issue is that the recorded diagnosis of dementia has been shown to be less valid in younger populations (Phung et al., 2007; Salem et al., 2013) and our model tests indicated that the HRs were slightly less pronounced at higher ages. We therefore conducted analyses where follow-up time was split at age less than 60 years and 60 years and older. The analyses of both age-groups showed associations between taller body height and lower risk of dementia, but the point estimate was slightly stronger in the younger population. This may partly be explained by the less valid symptom profile in these patients and misdiagnoses of psychiatric or neurological conditions for dementia. (Salem et al., 2013) Also, the findings may not be generalizable to men aged above 77 years as the study population only reached a maximum age of 57–77 years during the follow-up period. Furthermore, the role of familial factors were not fully explored. The analyses of brothers were incomplete by 73.5% of the population being from the birth cohorts 1953–59 and by being restricted to brothers with known shared mothers, implying a mixture of twin brothers, full non-twin brothers and maternal half-brothers, but without paternal half-brothers. The specific contribution of genetic factors, independent of environmental factors, cannot be assessed by twin-pair analyses without distinction between mono- and dizygotic twins. Finally, the generalizability of the findings to women is questionable. Previous findings on potential gender differences in the relationship between body height and dementia or poor cognition in old age are inconclusive, (Petot et al., 2007; West et al., 2015) but the incidence and prevalence of dementia are found to be lower in men than in women. (Matthews et al., 2016).

Findings in light of previous studies

The findings of this current study provide substantial support to previous evidence of a link between body height and dementia. (Borenstein et al., 2006; Wang et al., 2019; Russ et al., 2014; Petot et al., 2007; Abbott et al., 1998; Gatz et al., 2006; Huang et al., 2008; Beeri et al., 2005) All previous studies had accounted for educational level and other socioeconomic indicators, yet none of these studies had adjusted for intelligence level earlier in life. Intelligence level has been suggested to be a stronger marker of brain and cognitive reserve than educational level. (Borenstein et al., 2006) Intelligence level is furthermore correlated with body height (Christensen et al., 2015; Beauchamp et al., 2011) and by itself associated with dementia. (Osler et al., 2017).

In contrast to previous studies, we also investigated the impact of other potential early-life familial factors including genetics and socioeconomic resources in the family that may influence both body height and later risk of dementia. (Wang et al., 2019) Body height has been shown to have a strong genetic component with around 80% of the variation in populations being explained by genetic differences between individuals. (Jelenkovic et al., 2016a; Lango Allen et al., 2010; McEvoy and Visscher, 2009) The genetic component of height has furthermore been found to be consistent across countries independent of living standards. (Jelenkovic et al., 2016a).

Interestingly, we found that the estimate for the relationship between body height and dementia was stronger in the within-pair analysis than in the cohort analysis of brothers. The genetic and environmental variation influencing body height, but not risk of dementia, is smaller within brothers than between men in general, which may weaken the association between body height and dementia in the latter compared to the former group. Through this mechanism, the finding of a stronger association within brothers may be explained by less dilution of the effects of different harmful exposures early in life influencing both body growth and risk of dementia. These findings furthermore suggests that genetics has a minor role in the association of body height and dementia. However, it is important to acknowledge that the within-brother analysis was subject to greater uncertainty shown by a wider confidence interval. One previous Swedish study applied a twin design including 106 monozygotic twin pairs discordant for dementia, which indicated that the twin with the shortest height also more often was the one who developed dementia, supporting our interpretation, but the association provided insignificant estimates. (Gatz et al., 2006) Two of the previous studies, which had adjusted for APOE genotype, being the strongest single genetic marker related to development of dementia, also supported that conclusion. In more detail, one of the studies detected an association between body height and dementia, (Petot et al., 2007) and the other study, examining length of extremities and dementia, found an association to arm length in both women and men, but only an association to knee height (i.e. distance from foot sole to the anterior surface of the thigh of the lower leg) in women. (Huang et al., 2008).

In conclusion, taller body height at the entry to adulthood, supposed to be a marker of early-life environment, is associated with lower risk of dementia diagnosis later in life. The association persisted when adjusted for educational level and intelligence test scores in young adulthood, suggesting that height is not just acting as an indicator of cognitive reserve. Within-brother analysis confirms the findings of a relationship between taller height and lower risk of dementia diagnosis, and suggest that the association may have common roots in early life environmental exposures that are not related to family factors shared among brothers. This is supported in within-twin analysis, although the finding was imprecisely estimated.

Materials and methods

The study was based on the DCD, which, in brief, holds information registered at mandatory Danish conscription examinations in the years 1957 to 1984 for men born from 1939 through 1959 (Christensen et al., 2015). All Danish men are requested by law to appear before the conscript board for a physical and mental examination between the ages of 18 and 26 years. We linked the data with the Danish Twin Register, the Danish Civil Registration System, the Danish National Patient Registry, the Danish Psychiatric Central Register, and the Danish National Prescription Register. (Mors et al., 2011; Skytthe et al., 2011; Pedersen, 2011; Kildemoes et al., 2011; Lynge et al., 2011Figure 1 presents the selection of the three study populations; 1) men (N = 666,333), 2) brothers (N = 70,608), and 3) twins (N = 7,388). Brothers and twins were identified by two different approaches. Brothers were identified from the population through maternal linkage in the Danish Civil Registration System and 73.5% were from the birth-cohorts 1953–1959, where the linkage between mothers and offspring is complete in the register. The coverage vary between birth cohorts because the linkage between parents and offspring was based on shared residency in 1968 where the Danish Civil Registration System was established. After 1968, the linkage was carried out at the time of birth. Please see Supplementary file 4 for number of brother included from each birth cohort (1939–1959). Brothers were defined broadly by including all men with the same identified mother that is full-brothers, half-brothers, adopted brothers, and twins, triplets etc. Thus, the sample of brothers also include twins from these birth cohorts as they are registered with the same mother. However, for the twin population, twin pairs in all the birth cohorts 1939–1959 were identified by linking the DCD to the Danish Twin Register. The project was evaluated and approved by the Danish Data Protection Agency: Jr nr 2014-41-2911. According to Danish law, ethical approval is not required for purely register-based studies.

Main variables

Body height at entry to adulthood was included as the main exposure in this study. Objective measures of body height without shoes at the time of conscript board examination were investigated as birth cohort specific z-scores (See Supplementary file 2). The z-scores were investigated as a continuous variable.

Dementia diagnosis was measured by in- and out-patient contacts with ICD8 and ICD10 codes (ICD8: 290.00–290.99 and ICD10: F00.0-F03.9; G30.0-G30.9) in the Danish Psychiatric Central Register, since 1969, and in the Danish National Patient Registry since 1977. (Mors et al., 2011; Lynge et al., 2011) Dementia diagnosis was furthermore identified by redeemed prescriptions of acetylcholinesterase inhibitor registered by Anatomic Therapeutical Chemical (ATC) codes (N06D) in the Danish National Prescription Registry since 1995. (Kildemoes et al., 2011) (See Figure 1—figure supplement 1 for a graphical illustration of the data collection time line for each of these registers). The dementia diagnoses in the Danish hospital registers have been shown to have a positive predictive value of 70% and 83% in two different randomly selected populations of 200 individual with dementia recorded in the register. The values vary between the two studies because of age differences in the study populations, the lowest positive predictive value was detected for the population with the youngest age group. (Phung et al., 2007; Salem et al., 2013).

Covariates

Intelligence test scores, educational level, age and district at conscript board examination, and birth cohorts were included as covariates in the analyses. Intelligence test scores were measured by the Børge Priens Prøve (BPP), which has been shown to be highly correlated with the full-scale Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale score (R = 0.82) (Reinish and Teasdale, 1989) The score ranged from 0 to 78 and was categorized in deciles by 1)≤21, 2) 22–27, 3) 28–31, 4) 32–35, 5) 36–38; 6) 39–42, 7) 43–45, 8) 46–49, 9) 50–53, 10)≥54, and missing. Educational level at the time of examination was categorized by 1) short educational level: primary school, 2) medium educational level: vocational education and training as reference, 3) long educational level: secondary school, medium length education, high school, and academic educations, and 4) missing. Age was included as a continuous variable. Information about the eight conscript board examination districts was included as a categorical covariate with the following categories; 1) Copenhagen greater area incl. northern part of Zealand, 2) the remaining parts of Zealand and adjacent islands, 3) Funen and adjacent islands, 4) Mid and South parts of Jutland, 5) North and West Jutland, 6) Bornholm, 7) Southernmost part of Jutland, and 8) missing. The birth cohorts were included as a continuous variable ranging from 1939 through 1959.

Statistical analyses

The association between body height at entry to adulthood and diagnosis of dementia was analyzed in three populations: 1) all men in the birth cohorts 1939–1959, 2) brothers mainly from the cohorts 1953–1959, and 3) twins in the birth cohorts 1939–1959.

We applied Cox proportional hazard regression models to estimate the associations between body height and subsequent dementia diagnosis (HR with 95% CI). For men with a conscript board examination date before January 1st 1969, age at this date was used as baseline. In contrast, men examined after January 1st 1969 had baseline at the age of their conscript board examination. The mean age was 22.1 years at baseline due to the delayed entry for some of the men. Men were included in the statistical analyses at baseline and followed until diagnosis of dementia, emigration, death, or end of follow-up (April 30th 2016) that is to a maximum age of 57–77 years. Age was the underlying time scale of the model. Model assumptions of time-constant HRs were tested by Schoenfeld residuals for continuous variables and log-minus-log curves for categorical variables. The model assumption was fulfilled for all variables. However, there was a tendency that the association between body height and dementia was stronger in the younger age groups, thus, a supplementary analyses with follow-up split at 1) age <60 years and 2) age ≥60 years were conducted to quantify this potential difference in the association between body height and dementia diagnosis before and after age 60 years. Sensitivity analysis of the association 1) before 1995 (dementia diagnosis identified by ICD8 codes) and 2) 1995 and thereafter (dementia diagnosis identified by ATC codes and ICD10 codes) was performed.

To investigate the impact of adjustments for intelligence test scores and educational level as important explanatory factors, the analyses of the total population of men were conducted in four steps by Model 1 adjusted for conscript board examination district, Model 2a-b further adjusted for either educational level or intelligence test scores and finally, Model 3 further adjusted for both educational level and intelligence test scores. All the analyses were stratified by birth cohort to account for potential birth cohort effects and included a cluster term to account for the interdependence of observations between brothers. Finally, all models were by default adjusted for age as the underlying scale of the statistical models. Body height was also explored as restricted cubic splines with four knots equally distributed over the range of the z-score variable. The knots were identified at z-scores of −1.63,–0.40, 0.36 and 1.65 with 0 as the reference.

To explore the role of shared familial factors including environmental factors and partly shared genetics for the association between body height and dementia diagnosis, sibling populations (brothers and twins) were analyzed in a Model 3 standard cohort analyses and within-brother/twin pair analyses. In the cohort analyses each brother and twin was treated as an individual and a cluster term was included to account for the interdependence of observations. The within-pair analyses were stratified by a variable identifying the brother/twin pair cluster to make comparisons within groups of brothers and twin pairs. These analyses will provide estimates that, in addition to the included co-variates, are adjusted for shared family factors.

Statistical analyses were conducted in the statistical software packages Stata version 15 (Research Resource Identifier: SCR_012763).

Acknowledgements

Authors thank D Molbo, and EL Mortensen, who together with K Christensen, M Osler and TIA Sørensen established the database. The work was supported by the Danish Medical Research Council [grant number 09–063599 and 09–069151], the Velux Foundation [grant number 31205], the Jascha Foundation, the Health Foundation (17-B-0033), Doctor Sofus Carl Emil Friis and Olga Doris Friis grant, and the Social Inequalities in Ageing (SIA) project, funded by NordForsk, project no. 74637.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Terese Sara Høj Jørgensen, Email: Tshj@sund.ku.dk.

Belinda Nicolau, McGill University, Canada.

Eduardo Franco, McGill University, Canada.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Danish Medical Research Council 09-063599 to Merere Osler.

  • Velux Foundation 31205 to Kaare Christensen.

  • Jascha Fonden to Gunhild Tidemann Okholm.

  • Læge Sophus Carl Emil Friis og hustru Olga Doris Friis' Legat to Merere Osler.

  • NordForsk The Social Inequality in Ageing project (74637) to Terese Sara Høj Jørgensen.

  • Danish Medical Research Council 09-069151 to Merere Osler.

  • Health Foundation 17-B-0033 to Merere Osler.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Methodology.

Supervision, Methodology.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Investigation, Methodology.

Conceptualization, Data curation, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology.

Additional files

Source code 1. Stata code used for the analyses.
elife-51168-code1.do (4.9KB, do)
Supplementary file 1. Distribution of covariates for body height and dementia diagnoses among the total population of men.
elife-51168-supp1.docx (17.2KB, docx)
Supplementary file 2. Mean body height (z-scores) in cm for each of the birth cohorts in the full study population, brothers and twins.
elife-51168-supp2.docx (16.4KB, docx)
Supplementary file 3. Fully adjusted* hazard ratios (HRs) of the association between taller body height at entry to adulthood and dementia diagnosis among all men before 1995 and from 1995 and onwards.
elife-51168-supp3.docx (14.8KB, docx)
Supplementary file 4. Frequency (%) of individuals included from each birth cohort in the brother analyses.
elife-51168-supp4.docx (15.4KB, docx)
Transparent reporting form

Data availability

We have uploaded a file with the code used to program the statistical analyses performed in Stata. To access the anonymised dataset used in this study, researchers need to apply the Danish Conscript board steering committee and Statistics Denmark. Only aggregated data, where no identification of persons is possible i.e. minimum five observations in each cell, can be removed from the server containing the data accessed through Statistics Denmark. Thus, we cannot provide an anonymised copy of the dataset as individuals may be identified based on the information in the data e.g. birthday, height, dementia status etc. Access to the data through Statistics Denmark is only granted for authorised research and analysis environments of a more permanent nature with a chief researcher and several researchers/analysts. Foreign researchers affiliated to a Danish authorised environment can also get access. Authorisation is granted by the Director General. Please find more information in the document 'Access to micro data at Statistics Denmark_2014' on https://www.dst.dk/en/TilSalg/Forskningsservice.

References

  1. Abbott RD, White LR, Ross GW, Petrovitch H, Masaki KH, Snowdon DA, Curb JD. Height as a marker of childhood development and late-life cognitive function: the Honolulu-Asia aging study. Pediatrics. 1998;102:602–609. doi: 10.1542/peds.102.3.602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Beauchamp JP, Cesarini D, Johannesson M, Lindqvist E, Apicella C. On the sources of the height-intelligence correlation: new insights from a bivariate ACE model with assortative mating. Behavior Genetics. 2011;41:242–252. doi: 10.1007/s10519-010-9376-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Beeri MS, Davidson M, Silverman JM, Noy S, Schmeidler J, Goldbourt U. Relationship between body height and dementia. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2005;13:116–123. doi: 10.1097/00019442-200502000-00005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bird TD. Genetic Factors in Alzheimer's Disease. New England Journal of Medicine. 2005;352:862–864. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp058027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Borenstein AR, Copenhaver CI, Mortimer JA. Early-life risk factors for alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders. 2006;20:63–72. doi: 10.1097/01.wad.0000201854.62116.d7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Christensen GT, Molbo D, Ängquist LH, Mortensen EL, Christensen K, Sørensen TI, Osler M. Cohort profile: the danish conscription database(DCD): A cohort of 728,160 men born from 1939 through 1959. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2015;44:432–440. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyu114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cline MG, Meredith KE, Boyer JT, Burrows B. Decline of height with age in adults in a general population sample: estimating maximum height and distinguishing birth cohort effects from actual loss of stature with aging. Human Biology. 1989;61:415–425. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Gatz M, Mortimer JA, Fratiglioni L, Johansson B, Berg S, Reynolds CA, Pedersen NL. Potentially modifiable risk factors for dementia in identical twins. Alzheimer's & Dementia. 2006;2:110–117. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2006.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Huang TL, Carlson MC, Fitzpatrick AL, Kuller LH, Fried LP, Zandi PP. Knee height and arm span: a reflection of early life environment and risk of dementia. Neurology. 2008;70:1818–1826. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000311444.20490.98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Huang W, Lei X, Ridder G, Strauss J, Zhao Y. Health, height, height shrinkage, and SES at older ages: evidence from China. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics. 2013;5:86–121. doi: 10.1257/app.5.2.86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Jelenkovic A, Hur YM, Sund R, Yokoyama Y, Siribaddana SH, Hotopf M, Sumathipala A, Rijsdijk F, Tan Q, Zhang D, Pang Z, Aaltonen S, Heikkilä K, Öncel SY, Aliev F, Rebato E, Tarnoki AD, Tarnoki DL, Christensen K, Skytthe A, Kyvik KO, Silberg JL, Eaves LJ, Maes HH, Cutler TL, Hopper JL, Ordoñana JR, Sánchez-Romera JF, Colodro-Conde L, Cozen W, Hwang AE, Mack TM, Sung J, Song YM, Yang S, Lee K, Franz CE, Kremen WS, Lyons MJ, Busjahn A, Nelson TL, Whitfield KE, Kandler C, Jang KL, Gatz M, Butler DA, Stazi MA, Fagnani C, D'Ippolito C, Duncan GE, Buchwald D, Derom CA, Vlietinck RF, Loos RJ, Martin NG, Medland SE, Montgomery GW, Jeong HU, Swan GE, Krasnow R, Magnusson PK, Pedersen NL, Dahl-Aslan AK, McAdams TA, Eley TC, Gregory AM, Tynelius P, Baker LA, Tuvblad C, Bayasgalan G, Narandalai D, Lichtenstein P, Spector TD, Mangino M, Lachance G, Bartels M, van Beijsterveldt TC, Willemsen G, Burt SA, Klump KL, Harris JR, Brandt I, Nilsen TS, Krueger RF, McGue M, Pahlen S, Corley RP, Hjelmborg JV, Goldberg JH, Iwatani Y, Watanabe M, Honda C, Inui F, Rasmussen F, Huibregtse BM, Boomsma DI, Sørensen TI, Kaprio J, Silventoinen K. Genetic and environmental influences on adult human height across birth cohorts from 1886 to 1994. eLife. 2016a;5:e20320. doi: 10.7554/eLife.20320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Jelenkovic A, Sund R, Hur YM, Yokoyama Y, Hjelmborg JV, Möller S, Honda C, Magnusson PK, Pedersen NL, Ooki S, Aaltonen S, Stazi MA, Fagnani C, D'Ippolito C, Freitas DL, Maia JA, Ji F, Ning F, Pang Z, Rebato E, Busjahn A, Kandler C, Saudino KJ, Jang KL, Cozen W, Hwang AE, Mack TM, Gao W, Yu C, Li L, Corley RP, Huibregtse BM, Derom CA, Vlietinck RF, Loos RJ, Heikkilä K, Wardle J, Llewellyn CH, Fisher A, McAdams TA, Eley TC, Gregory AM, He M, Ding X, Bjerregaard-Andersen M, Beck-Nielsen H, Sodemann M, Tarnoki AD, Tarnoki DL, Knafo-Noam A, Mankuta D, Abramson L, Burt SA, Klump KL, Silberg JL, Eaves LJ, Maes HH, Krueger RF, McGue M, Pahlen S, Gatz M, Butler DA, Bartels M, van Beijsterveldt TC, Craig JM, Saffery R, Dubois L, Boivin M, Brendgen M, Dionne G, Vitaro F, Martin NG, Medland SE, Montgomery GW, Swan GE, Krasnow R, Tynelius P, Lichtenstein P, Haworth CM, Plomin R, Bayasgalan G, Narandalai D, Harden KP, Tucker-Drob EM, Spector T, Mangino M, Lachance G, Baker LA, Tuvblad C, Duncan GE, Buchwald D, Willemsen G, Skytthe A, Kyvik KO, Christensen K, Öncel SY, Aliev F, Rasmussen F, Goldberg JH, Sørensen TI, Boomsma DI, Kaprio J, Silventoinen K. Genetic and environmental influences on height from infancy to early adulthood: an individual-based pooled analysis of 45 twin cohorts. Scientific Reports. 2016b;6:28496. doi: 10.1038/srep28496. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Kildemoes HW, Sørensen HT, Hallas J. The danish national prescription registry. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health. 2011;39:38–41. doi: 10.1177/1403494810394717. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Lang L, Clifford A, Wei L, Zhang D, Leung D, Augustine G, Danat IM, Zhou W, Copeland JR, Anstey KJ, Chen R. Prevalence and determinants of undetected dementia in the community: a systematic literature review and a meta-analysis. BMJ Open. 2017;7:e011146. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-011146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Lango Allen H, Estrada K, Lettre G, Berndt SI, Weedon MN, Rivadeneira F, Willer CJ, Jackson AU, Vedantam S, Raychaudhuri S, Ferreira T, Wood AR, Weyant RJ, Segrè AV, Speliotes EK, Wheeler E, Soranzo N, Park JH, Yang J, Gudbjartsson D, Heard-Costa NL, Randall JC, Qi L, Vernon Smith A, Mägi R, Pastinen T, Liang L, Heid IM, Luan J, Thorleifsson G, Winkler TW, Goddard ME, Sin Lo K, Palmer C, Workalemahu T, Aulchenko YS, Johansson A, Zillikens MC, Feitosa MF, Esko T, Johnson T, Ketkar S, Kraft P, Mangino M, Prokopenko I, Absher D, Albrecht E, Ernst F, Glazer NL, Hayward C, Hottenga JJ, Jacobs KB, Knowles JW, Kutalik Z, Monda KL, Polasek O, Preuss M, Rayner NW, Robertson NR, Steinthorsdottir V, Tyrer JP, Voight BF, Wiklund F, Xu J, Zhao JH, Nyholt DR, Pellikka N, Perola M, Perry JR, Surakka I, Tammesoo ML, Altmaier EL, Amin N, Aspelund T, Bhangale T, Boucher G, Chasman DI, Chen C, Coin L, Cooper MN, Dixon AL, Gibson Q, Grundberg E, Hao K, Juhani Junttila M, Kaplan LM, Kettunen J, König IR, Kwan T, Lawrence RW, Levinson DF, Lorentzon M, McKnight B, Morris AP, Müller M, Suh Ngwa J, Purcell S, Rafelt S, Salem RM, Salvi E, Sanna S, Shi J, Sovio U, Thompson JR, Turchin MC, Vandenput L, Verlaan DJ, Vitart V, White CC, Ziegler A, Almgren P, Balmforth AJ, Campbell H, Citterio L, De Grandi A, Dominiczak A, Duan J, Elliott P, Elosua R, Eriksson JG, Freimer NB, Geus EJ, Glorioso N, Haiqing S, Hartikainen AL, Havulinna AS, Hicks AA, Hui J, Igl W, Illig T, Jula A, Kajantie E, Kilpeläinen TO, Koiranen M, Kolcic I, Koskinen S, Kovacs P, Laitinen J, Liu J, Lokki ML, Marusic A, Maschio A, Meitinger T, Mulas A, Paré G, Parker AN, Peden JF, Petersmann A, Pichler I, Pietiläinen KH, Pouta A, Ridderstråle M, Rotter JI, Sambrook JG, Sanders AR, Schmidt CO, Sinisalo J, Smit JH, Stringham HM, Bragi Walters G, Widen E, Wild SH, Willemsen G, Zagato L, Zgaga L, Zitting P, Alavere H, Farrall M, McArdle WL, Nelis M, Peters MJ, Ripatti S, van Meurs JB, Aben KK, Ardlie KG, Beckmann JS, Beilby JP, Bergman RN, Bergmann S, Collins FS, Cusi D, den Heijer M, Eiriksdottir G, Gejman PV, Hall AS, Hamsten A, Huikuri HV, Iribarren C, Kähönen M, Kaprio J, Kathiresan S, Kiemeney L, Kocher T, Launer LJ, Lehtimäki T, Melander O, Mosley TH, Musk AW, Nieminen MS, O'Donnell CJ, Ohlsson C, Oostra B, Palmer LJ, Raitakari O, Ridker PM, Rioux JD, Rissanen A, Rivolta C, Schunkert H, Shuldiner AR, Siscovick DS, Stumvoll M, Tönjes A, Tuomilehto J, van Ommen GJ, Viikari J, Heath AC, Martin NG, Montgomery GW, Province MA, Kayser M, Arnold AM, Atwood LD, Boerwinkle E, Chanock SJ, Deloukas P, Gieger C, Grönberg H, Hall P, Hattersley AT, Hengstenberg C, Hoffman W, Lathrop GM, Salomaa V, Schreiber S, Uda M, Waterworth D, Wright AF, Assimes TL, Barroso I, Hofman A, Mohlke KL, Boomsma DI, Caulfield MJ, Cupples LA, Erdmann J, Fox CS, Gudnason V, Gyllensten U, Harris TB, Hayes RB, Jarvelin MR, Mooser V, Munroe PB, Ouwehand WH, Penninx BW, Pramstaller PP, Quertermous T, Rudan I, Samani NJ, Spector TD, Völzke H, Watkins H, Wilson JF, Groop LC, Haritunians T, Hu FB, Kaplan RC, Metspalu A, North KE, Schlessinger D, Wareham NJ, Hunter DJ, O'Connell JR, Strachan DP, Wichmann HE, Borecki IB, van Duijn CM, Schadt EE, Thorsteinsdottir U, Peltonen L, Uitterlinden AG, Visscher PM, Chatterjee N, Loos RJ, Boehnke M, McCarthy MI, Ingelsson E, Lindgren CM, Abecasis GR, Stefansson K, Frayling TM, Hirschhorn JN. Hundreds of variants clustered in genomic loci and biological pathways affect human height. Nature. 2010;467:832–838. doi: 10.1038/nature09410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Livingston G, Sommerlad A, Orgeta V, Costafreda SG, Huntley J, Ames D, Ballard C, Banerjee S, Burns A, Cohen-Mansfield J, Cooper C, Fox N, Gitlin LN, Howard R, Kales HC, Larson EB, Ritchie K, Rockwood K, Sampson EL, Samus Q, Schneider LS, Selbæk G, Teri L, Mukadam N. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. The Lancet. 2017;390:2673–2734. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)31363-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Lynge E, Sandegaard JL, Rebolj M. The danish national patient register. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health. 2011;39:30–33. doi: 10.1177/1403494811401482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Matthews FE, Stephan BCM, Robinson L, Jagger C, Barnes LE, Arthur A, Brayne C. A two decade dementia incidence comparison from the cognitive function and ageing studies I and II. Nature Communications. 2016;7:11398. doi: 10.1038/ncomms11398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. McEvoy BP, Visscher PM. Genetics of human height. Economics & Human Biology. 2009;7:294–306. doi: 10.1016/j.ehb.2009.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Mors O, Perto GP, Mortensen PB. The danish psychiatric central research register. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health. 2011;39:54–57. doi: 10.1177/1403494810395825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Osler M, Christensen GT, Garde E, Mortensen EL, Christensen K. Cognitive ability in young adulthood and risk of dementia in a cohort of danish men, brothers, and twins. Alzheimer's & Dementia. 2017;13:1355–1363. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2017.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Pedersen CB. The danish civil registration system. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health. 2011;39:22–25. doi: 10.1177/1403494810387965. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Petot GJ, Vega U, Traore F, Fritsch T, Debanne SM, Friedland RP, Lerner AJ. Height and Alzheimer's disease: findings from a case-control study. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease. 2007;11:337–341. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2007-11310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Phung TK, Andersen BB, Høgh P, Kessing LV, Mortensen PB, Waldemar G. Validity of dementia diagnoses in the danish hospital registers. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders. 2007;24:220–228. doi: 10.1159/000107084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Reinish JM, Teasdale TW. Intelligence as measured by the WAIS and a military draft board group test. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. 1989;30:315–318. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.1989.tb01094.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  26. Russ TC, Kivimäki M, Starr JM, Stamatakis E, Batty GD. Height in relation to dementia death: individual participant meta-analysis of 18 UK prospective cohort studies. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2014;205:348–354. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.113.142984. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Salem LC, Andersen BB, Nielsen TR, Stokholm J, Jørgensen MB, Rasmussen MH, Waldemar G. Overdiagnosis of dementia in young patients – A Nationwide Register-Based Study. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders. 2013;34:292–299. doi: 10.1159/000345485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Skytthe A, Kyvik KO, Holm NV, Christensen K. The danish twin registry. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health. 2011;39:75–78. doi: 10.1177/1403494810387966. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Wang XJ, Xu W, Li JQ, Cao XP, Tan L, Yu JT. Early-Life risk factors for dementia and cognitive impairment in later life: a systematic review and Meta-Analysis. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease. 2019;67:221–229. doi: 10.3233/JAD-180856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. West RK, Ravona-Springer R, Heymann A, Schmeidler J, Leroith D, Koifman K, Guerrero-Berroa E, Preiss R, Hoffman H, Silverman JM, Beeri MS. Shorter adult height is associated with poorer cognitive performance in elderly men with type II diabetes. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease. 2015;44:927–935. doi: 10.3233/JAD-142049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Belinda Nicolau1

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

Body height, a biomarker that reflects the interplay of genetic endowment and early-life experiences and exposures (e.g., nutrition, social and psychological circumstances), may provide insights into the development of a wide range of disease outcomes. In the current study, the authors explore the association between body height and dementia using information from the Danish national registers. Their work showed that people who were taller had significantly lower rates of dementia after taking into account the effect of several other factors. Further, to distinguish this protective effect of body height from genetic predisposition, which is a strong determinant of height, the authors conducted an analysis of siblings and twins and their findings remain the same.

This study represents a promising start to new research that could shed light on the potential influence of early life risk factors for dementia since, by the very nature of the growth process, height is determined in early life.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Body height in young adult men and risk of dementia later in adult life" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by Belinda Nicolau as Reviewing Editor and Eduardo Franco as Senior Editor. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

They discussed the reviews with one another and submitted them to the Reviewing Editor, who has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

Body height, a biomarker that reflects the interplay of genetic endowment and early-life experiences and exposures (e.g., nutrition, social and psychological circumstances), may provide insights into the aetiology of a wide range of disease outcomes. In the current study, the authors explore the association between body height and dementia. They use Danish national registers to obtain data from the conscript board, including height, intelligence, and educational attainment, matched with data from health registers to identify those with dementia diagnoses or who were taking medication typically prescribed for Alzheimer's disease. Their results showed that taller body height was associated with significantly lower rate of dementia after adjusting for a series of confounders. In addition, they attempted to distinguish this protective effect of body height from confounding by genetics – a strong determinant of height – through analysis of within siblings and within twin differences. This study represents a potentially valuable contribution to the literature on early life risk factors for dementia. However, there are a few substantive concerns.

Essential revisions:

The authors underplay the importance of the analyses of siblings and twins. If height is a risk factor in intrapair analyses of siblings and twins, and comparable in magnitude to a standard cohort analysis, then it is strong evidence that the explanation for the association between height and dementia is something other than shared genes or shared rearing environment. Indeed, one of the main concerns raised by the reviewers is the adjustment for genetics with the within-twin pair analyses. The point estimate for within-twin pairs is similar to the point estimate for unrelated individuals. In the larger sample of unrelated individuals (Table 1), the confidence intervals (CI) are below 1.0, whereas the CI include 1.0 in both the cohort analysis and the twin analysis in Table 3. This likely is a reflection of the smaller N. This may, moreover, suggest that adjusting for genetics does not explain the association between body height and dementia. The authors don't reach this conclusion. In the Discussion, they state "These findings support a relationship between body height and dementia independent of the genetic relationship and of exposures to other shared early-life familial factors." The authors should elaborate on this interpretation, revisiting their reasoning so as to clarify the argument.

A second issue of concern raised is the definition of what constitutes tall height. The reviewers were concerned with the meaningful clinical interpretation of the results. They should be wary of the ability of a large study such as this to detect differences, which may not be meaningful. Even without considering the role of genetics, and taking at face value the results of the 'fully adjusted' model (Model 3) that shows a 10% reduction in dementia risk for every one z-score increase in height, how clinically meaningful is this reduction? Since your z-score is defined as how far you are from the average in a group, in the real-world, how tall should a person be to start getting any 'meaningful' protective effect from what is being reported? Clinical pertinence should be considered.

The authors discuss the limitations of obtaining dementia diagnoses from patient registers. However, they do not address the introduction of the national prescription register in 1995 and how that might affect sensitivity, specificity, and accuracy in identifying dementia cases.

The authors include birth cohort in the analyses and compare results dividing at age 60. They do not, however, address the fact that participants were aged 57 to 77 at the latest date for register dementia diagnoses, so that a large proportion are younger than average age of onset for dementia. Prevalence of dementia is quite low, which likely reflects both cases missed by registers and the age structure of the study population.

Not until the detailed methods is it learned that brothers were from 1953-1959 birth cohorts, making them age 57 – 63 as of the latest date of matching to dementia diagnoses. (The text states that the brothers were "mainly" from 1953-1959, so it's a bit ambiguous.) If the brothers are from 1953-1959 birth cohorts, it is not surprising that rate of dementia cases is lower for brothers than for the full population or for the twins. It seems possible that the difference in age structure might also contribute to the observation that the point estimate for the association between body height and dementia is greater in the twin pair analyses than in the analyses of brothers. It would also make the observation less interesting. This difference in birth cohorts is essential to note because, otherwise, the assumption would be that the sample of brothers contains all twin pairs in the sample of twins, since twins are also siblings.

It would be of interest to describe the mean and distribution of intrapair difference in height in centimeters (cm).

eLife. 2020 Feb 11;9:e51168. doi: 10.7554/eLife.51168.sa2

Author response


Essential revisions:

The authors underplay the importance of the analyses of siblings and twins. If height is a risk factor in intrapair analyses of siblings and twins, and comparable in magnitude to a standard cohort analysis, then it is strong evidence that the explanation for the association between height and dementia is something other than shared genes or shared rearing environment. Indeed, one of the main concerns raised by the reviewers is the adjustment for genetics with the within-twin pair analyses. The point estimate for within-twin pairs is similar to the point estimate for unrelated individuals. In the larger sample of unrelated individuals (Table 1), the confidence intervals (CI) are below 1.0, whereas the CI include 1.0 in both the cohort analysis and the twin analysis in Table 3. This likely is a reflection of the smaller N. This may, moreover, suggest that adjusting for genetics does not explain the association between body height and dementia. The authors don't reach this conclusion. In the Discussion, they state "These findings support a relationship between body height and dementia independent of the genetic relationship and of exposures to other shared early-life familial factors." The authors should elaborate on this interpretation, revisiting their reasoning so as to clarify the argument.

We completely agree that the results from the brother and twin analyses are of great importance in this study. We furthermore agree that the brother analyses indicate that genetics and shared environment between brothers do not explain the relationship between body height and dementia and that the findings from the twin analyses may be subject to power issues causing imprecise estimates with wider confidence intervals including 1.0 (reference value).

In the following, we have included the paragraphs from the manuscript where we elaborate on the results from the brother and twin analyses. We have rewritten some of these paragraphs from the previous version of the manuscript to emphasize the importance of these findings:

In the Abstract: “The associations persisted in within-brother analysis and revealed a stronger, but less precise, point estimate than the cohort analysis of brothers. The twin analysis showed similar, but imprecise estimates.”

In the Results: “The analyses of Model 3 showed that the within-pair analysis of brothers revealed a stronger, but less precise, point estimate than the cohort analysis of brothers (Table 3). For twins, the comparison of the cohort analysis of twins and the within-pair analysis of twins revealed a pattern similar to that found for brothers. Yet, the pattern was less clear and the HRs were imprecisely estimated possibly due to a lower number of individuals in these analyses (N=7,388) (Table 3).”

In the Discussion:

· “Within-brother analysis confirmed the findings of a relationship between taller body height and lower risk of dementia diagnosis, and even revealed a stronger, but less precise, point estimate of the association than the cohort analyses of brothers. The findings from the within-twin analyses were in the same direction, but with more uncertainty. These findings support that the relationship between body height and dementia is not explained by shared genetics and exposures to other familial factors shared between brothers.”

· “A final strength is that we were able to investigate the impact of familial factors, including various degrees of shared genetic and environmental factors, by conducting within-brother and twin-pair analyses.”

· “Interestingly, we found that the estimate for the relationship between body height and dementia was stronger in the within-pair analysis than in the cohort analysis of brothers. The genetic and environmental variation influencing body height, but not risk of dementia, is smaller within brothers than between men in general, which may weaken the association between body height and dementia in the latter compared to the former group. Through this mechanism, the finding of a stronger association within brothers may be explained by less dilution of the effects of different harmful exposures early in life influencing both body growth and risk of dementia. These findings furthermore suggests that genetics has a minor role in the association of body height and dementia.”

· “Within-brother analysis confirms the findings of a relationship between taller height and lower risk of dementia diagnosis, and suggest that the association may have common roots in early life environmental exposures that are not related to family factors shared among brothers. This is supported in within-twin analysis, although the finding was imprecisely estimated.”

However, we believe it is important to acknowledge the limitations of the brother and twin analyses. We have therefor kept the following paragraphs in the manuscript:

In the Discussion:

· “Furthermore, the role of familial factors were not fully explored. The analyses of brothers were incomplete by 73.5% of the population being from the birth cohorts 1953-59 and by being restricted to men with known shared mothers, implying a mixture of twin brothers, full non-twin brothers and maternal half-brothers, but without paternal half-brothers. The specific contribution of genetic factors, independent of environmental factors, cannot be assessed by twin-pair analyses without distinction between mono- and dizygotic twins.”

· “…However, it is important to acknowledge that the within-brother analysis was subject to greater uncertainty shown by a wider confidence interval”

A second issue of concern raised is the definition of what constitutes tall height. The reviewers were concerned with the meaningful clinical interpretation of the results. They should be wary of the ability of a large study such as this to detect differences, which may not be meaningful. Even without considering the role of genetics, and taking at face value the results of the 'fully adjusted' model (Model 3) that shows a 10% reduction in dementia risk for every one z-score increase in height, how clinically meaningful is this reduction? Since your z-score is defined as how far you are from the average in a group, in the real-world, how tall should a person be to start getting any 'meaningful' protective effect from what is being reported? Clinical pertinence should be considered.

Thank you for this comment. We also believe it is important to highlight the clinical relevance of the findings. To be subject to a 10% lower dementia risk, a man born in 1939 should be 6.5 cm (one z-score) taller than the mean height of 175.0 cm, i.e. 181.5 cm. For the birth cohort 1959, the corresponding values are 6.5 cm (one z-score) taller than mean height of 179.1 cm, i.e. 185.6 cm.

We have provided these calculations in the Results section: “Supplementary file 2 shows that the mean heights range from 174.5 to 179.1 cm and the z-scores range from 6.4 to 6.6 cm for the birth cohorts in the total population without any clear patterns of change over time. The finding of 10% lower HR with one z-score higher body height suggests that men born in 1939 with a body height of 181.5 cm (equal to one z-score taller than the mean) can expect a 10% lower dementia risk compared with men with the mean body height (175.0 cm). The corresponding body height for the latest birth cohort (1959) is 185.6 cm compared to the mean body height of 179.1 cm.”

To make these calculations transparent, we have included the mean body heights in addition to the z-scores for each birth cohort in Supplementary file 2 for the full population, brothers and twins.

The authors discuss the limitations of obtaining dementia diagnoses from patient registers. However, they do not address the introduction of the national prescription register in 1995 and how that might affect sensitivity, specificity, and accuracy in identifying dementia cases.

Thank you for making us aware of this. We have now explored the relationship between body height and dementia before and after the introduction of the National Prescription Register that enabled identification of dementia diagnoses with ATC codes from January 1st 1995. This is described in the following paragraphs in the manuscript:

In the Materials and methods section: “Sensitivity analysis of the association 1) before 1995 (dementia diagnosis identified by ICD-8 codes) and 2) 1995 and thereafter (dementia diagnosis identified by ATC codes and ICD-10 codes) was performed.”

In the Results section: “Analyses of Model 3 with the follow-up period divided in 1) prior to 1995 and 2) 1995 and thereafter showed that the association between one z-score taller body height and dementia was comparable between the two periods (Supplementary file 4).”

In the Discussion: “Furthermore, the introduction of the Danish Prescription Registry on January 1st 1995 has enabled identification of dementia patients who are not in contact with the secondary healthcare system for their dementia diagnosis, but who receive medication through their general practitioner. This has most likely improved the sensitivity of the dementia diagnoses from 1995. Our sensitivity analyses showed comparable findings of the relationship between body height and dementia before and after the introduction, yet the comparison of these results were limited by including men in different age-groups (<1995: maximum age of 36-56 and ≥1995: maximum age of 57-77).”

Finally, we want to acknowledge that acetylcholinesterase inhibitors (ATC: N06D) may be used for patients with Parkinson’s disease, but the treatment is only indicated if the patients have comorbid dementia. Thus, this should not influence the specificity of the diagnosis. This information has been included as a foot note in Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

The authors include birth cohort in the analyses and compare results dividing at age 60. They do not, however, address the fact that participants were aged 57 to 77 at the latest date for register dementia diagnoses, so that a large proportion are younger than average age of onset for dementia. Prevalence of dementia is quite low, which likely reflects both cases missed by registers and the age structure of the study population.

We agree and have included the following statement in the strength and limitation section of the manuscript: “Also, the findings may not be generalizable to men aged above 77 years as the study population only reached a maximum age of 57-77 years during the follow-up period.”

We also agree that the low frequency of dementia in our cohort could be due to the particular age structure of the cohort, which implies that the data cannot be used to estimate prevalence, only incidence of dementia through this age range.

Not until the detailed methods is it learned that brothers were from 1953-1959 birth cohorts, making them age 57 – 63 as of the latest date of matching to dementia diagnoses. (The text states that the brothers were "mainly" from 1953-1959, so it's a bit ambiguous.) If the brothers are from 1953-1959 birth cohorts, it is not surprising that rate of dementia cases is lower for brothers than for the full population or for the twins. It seems possible that the difference in age structure might also contribute to the observation that the point estimate for the association between body height and dementia is greater in the twin pair analyses than in the analyses of brothers. It would also make the observation less interesting. This difference in birth cohorts is essential to note because, otherwise, the assumption would be that the sample of brothers contains all twin pairs in the sample of twins, since twins are also siblings.

We apologize for not being specific about the birth cohorts from which the brothers were included in the prior version of the manuscript. We have rewritten and provided more information of the birth cohorts where brothers were identified in the materials and methods section: “Brothers were identified from the population through maternal linkage in the Danish Civil Registration System and 73.5% were included from the birth-cohorts 1953-1959, where the linkage between mothers and offspring is complete in the register. The coverage vary between birth cohorts because the linkage between parents and offspring was based on shared residency in 1968 where the Danish Civil Registration System was established. After 1968, the linkage was carried out at the time of birth. Please see Supplementary file 1 for number of individuals included from each birth cohort (1939-1959).”

We want to highlight that even though 73.5% of brothers were included from the birth cohorts 1953-1959, 26.5% were included from the prior birth cohorts (1939-1952). As described in the paragraph above, this information is now available in the manuscript and Supplementary file 1.

Also, we want to clarify that the results from the within-brother and within-twin analyses, respectively, are compared to cohort analyses of the population of brothers and twins, respectively. Thus, the interpretation of these results is based on comparisons of results based on the same population: brothers and twins, respectively, and thereby identical age structures. It is thereby not possible that difference in age distribution can explain the difference identified in the within-brother/twin analyses from the cohort analyses of brothers/twins. We have clarified this in the Results section in the manuscript: “The analyses of Model 3 showed that the within-pair analysis of brothers revealed a stronger, but less precise, point estimate than the cohort analysis of brothers (Table 3). For twins, the comparison of the cohort analysis of twins and the within-pair analysis of twins revealed a pattern similar to that found for brothers. Yet, the pattern was less clear and the HRs were imprecisely estimated possible due to a lower number of individuals in these analyses (N=7,388) (Table 3)”

It would be of interest to describe the mean and distribution of intrapair difference in height in centimeters (cm).

The numeric mean and SE difference in body height between brothers and twins, respectively, as well as the distribution of these estimates are now provided in the Results section of the manuscript: “The numeric mean (SE) body height difference was 5.1 (0.02) cm between brothers and 3.5 (0.06) cm between twins (See Figure 2—figure supplement 1 for distribution of the numeric difference in body height for brothers (A) and twins (B)).”

Please note that brothers could include more than just two. In cases where there were more than two brothers with a shared mother, the difference in body height between all brothers were included e.g. for a groups of three brothers, the differences between brother one and two, brother one and three, and brother two and three, respectively, were included. This information is provided as a foot note in Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Source code 1. Stata code used for the analyses.
    elife-51168-code1.do (4.9KB, do)
    Supplementary file 1. Distribution of covariates for body height and dementia diagnoses among the total population of men.
    elife-51168-supp1.docx (17.2KB, docx)
    Supplementary file 2. Mean body height (z-scores) in cm for each of the birth cohorts in the full study population, brothers and twins.
    elife-51168-supp2.docx (16.4KB, docx)
    Supplementary file 3. Fully adjusted* hazard ratios (HRs) of the association between taller body height at entry to adulthood and dementia diagnosis among all men before 1995 and from 1995 and onwards.
    elife-51168-supp3.docx (14.8KB, docx)
    Supplementary file 4. Frequency (%) of individuals included from each birth cohort in the brother analyses.
    elife-51168-supp4.docx (15.4KB, docx)
    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    We have uploaded a file with the code used to program the statistical analyses performed in Stata. To access the anonymised dataset used in this study, researchers need to apply the Danish Conscript board steering committee and Statistics Denmark. Only aggregated data, where no identification of persons is possible i.e. minimum five observations in each cell, can be removed from the server containing the data accessed through Statistics Denmark. Thus, we cannot provide an anonymised copy of the dataset as individuals may be identified based on the information in the data e.g. birthday, height, dementia status etc. Access to the data through Statistics Denmark is only granted for authorised research and analysis environments of a more permanent nature with a chief researcher and several researchers/analysts. Foreign researchers affiliated to a Danish authorised environment can also get access. Authorisation is granted by the Director General. Please find more information in the document 'Access to micro data at Statistics Denmark_2014' on https://www.dst.dk/en/TilSalg/Forskningsservice.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES