Abstract
Background
Altered brain activation during response inhibition has been linked to a greater risk for alcohol and other substance use behaviors in late adolescence. However, the ability of neural markers of response inhibition, acquired during adolescence, to temporally predict the transition from less frequent and lower quantity alcohol use to high-risk, frequent (≥weekly) binge drinking behavior remains unclear.
Methods
Adolescents (N=29; 9 females) were selected from a larger ongoing longitudinal study to include those who transitioned to at least weekly binge drinking (≥5/4 alcoholic drinks for males/females per occasion) over a 15-year follow-up period. Prior to the onset of weekly binge drinking (mean age=18.0), participants underwent a functional MRI including a Go/No-go task. Whole-brain activation from the no-go correct rejection vs. no-go false alarm contrast was used to predict time to transition to frequent binge drinking.
Results
Less no-go correct rejection vs. no-go false alarm activation in a cluster including the precentral gyri, insula, and inferior frontal gyri predicted a more rapid transition into frequent binge drinking (voxel-wise alpha < 0.001, cluster-wise alpha < 0.05, cluster threshold ≥ 18 voxels).
Conclusions
Results from this study are supported by literature suggesting that fronto-insular involvement is important for successful inhibition and cognitive control. Altered brain activation during response inhibition may thus represent neural antecedents of impulse regulation difficulties related to alcohol consumption. The magnitude of this activation provides temporal information that may be used to inform and optimize timing of interventions aimed at preventing the escalation and transition to problematic drinking for youth who have already begun to engage in drinking behaviors.
Keywords: Binge drinking, adolescence, longitudinal, response inhibition, fMRI
INTRODUCTION
Alcohol remains the most commonly used substance of abuse during adolescence and young adulthood. The act of binge drinking, often defined as the consumption of greater than either 4 or 5 drinks in a given drinking episode, is of particular concern in youth given the host of associated negative consequences (for a review see Courtney and Polich, 2009) and potential for neurological alterations to the developing adolescent brain (Ruan et al., 2019). Approximately 17% of 12th graders and 33% of college-aged young adults (modal ages 19–22) reported recent binge drinking, defined as the consumption of 5 or more drinks in a row at least once in the two weeks prior to assessment (Miech et al., 2017, Schulenberg et al., 2018). Notably, almost 1% of adolescents aged 12 to 17 and 10% of young adults aged 18 to 25 engage in binge drinking episodes frequently, averaging more than once per week over the previous 30 days (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2018). Frequent binge drinking (binging ≥ once per week in the previous year) during adolescence is associated with elevations in multiple risk factors, including adolescent drug use, antisociality, and parent alcoholism (Chassin et al., 2002), as well as a number of negative consequences in adulthood such as alcohol use disorder diagnosis, drug use, psychiatric morbidity, homelessness, legal problems, accidents, and lower social class (Viner and Taylor, 2007). Importantly, many of these elevated risks are greater for those who frequently binge drink during adolescence, as opposed to those who are infrequent/moderate binge drinkers (Chassin et al., 2002), suggesting that the frequency with which one binges during adolescence is an important factor in future alcohol-related outcomes. Thus, given the known neurotoxicity of alcohol at higher doses (for reviews see Oscar-Berman and Marinkovic, 2007, Sullivan and Pfefferbaum, 2005), efforts to predict who is at risk of drinking at these frequent high levels during the critical period of neurodevelopment are warranted.
A hallmark characteristic of the binge drinking episode is the apparent loss of control over ones’ alcohol intake. In line with this, diminished inhibitory control (i.e., the capacity to voluntarily regulate or inhibit prepotent behavioral or attentional responses) during adolescence is consistently implicated as a risk factor for future alcohol and substance use (for a review see Casey, 2015). Successful inhibitory control likely involves the ventral attention, fronto-parietal and fronto-striatal networks, including regions such as the inferior frontal gyrus extending to the insula, cingulate and paracingulate gyri, superior parietal gyrus, and basal ganglia structures (Zhang et al., 2017, Morein-Zamir and Robbins, 2015), suggesting deficiencies in these networks may serve as correlates of alcohol-related risk prior to binge drinking onset (Whelan et al., 2014).
Longitudinal functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies of adolescents have identified several neural aberrations during inhibition, as measured on the Go/No-go task, as significant predictors of greater alcohol and substance use, even in the absence of behavioral differences on the tasks (Norman et al., 2011, Mahmood et al., 2013, Wetherill et al., 2013). Specifically, greater left angular gyrus and less ventromedial prefrontal blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) activation during no-go correct rejection vs. go trials in 16 to 19 year-olds was found to predict higher levels of alcohol and substance use and dependence symptoms over an 18-month follow-up. This effect was especially pronounced for adolescents who were high frequency substance users at baseline (Mahmood et al., 2013). In an analysis of 12–14 year-olds scanned prior to the onset of alcohol use and followed up about 4.2 years later, less BOLD response in regions including the right inferior frontal gyrus, left dorsal and medial frontal areas during no-go correct rejection vs. baseline trials was found to differentiate between those who transitioned to alcohol use from those who remained continuous controls (Norman et al., 2011); however, the activation in those regions was found to be associated with attention problems at follow-up, and not substance use outcomes per se, suggesting the groups may have differed on multiple related factors. In an additional longitudinal analysis of 11–16 year-olds, with follow-up approximately 3 years later, adolescents who transitioned into drinking by follow-up exhibited less BOLD response during no-go correct rejection vs. go trials at baseline in bilateral middle frontal gyri, left putamen, right inferior parietal lobule, and left cerebellar regions. Yet increased activation was observed after the onset of heavy drinking in all regions except the putamen, as compared to matched continuous non-drinkers who displayed decreased activation in these regions at follow-up (Wetherill et al., 2013). These results suggest alcohol-exposure may increase engagement of these neural networks in order to successfully inhibit prepotent responses; however, the degree of alcohol exposure required to produce this change has yet to be investigated.
Taken together, the current literature implies the presence of a pre-existing neural inhibition risk profile for future alcohol and substance use, along with a potential for additional alcohol and substance-related disturbances in normal neural inhibitory maturation processes. However, the neural underpinnings subserving the transition from moderate, arguably even “normative”, alcohol use behavior in adolescence to the extremely high-risk pattern of frequent binge drinking have not been determined. Thus, the present study seeks to prospectively predict the time to transition to high-risk frequent binge drinking (averaging ≥ once per week for at least one year) from the neural patterns of successful inhibitory control in a single sample of adolescents who were already engaged in moderate alcohol use. Given the broad set of inhibitory-related regions identified in the earlier literature, a whole-brain exploratory approach was used for the present analysis, with a general hypothesis of alcohol risk-related activation to fall within the fronto-parietal and fronto-striatal networks. No directionality was hypothesized for the present analyses given the mixed results of the literature and the novelty of the current inquiry.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Participants & Procedures
Current study data was culled from a larger, ongoing longitudinal substance use and neuroimaging project (NIAAA R01 AA013419). Participants at baseline were healthy 12–14 year-olds, recruited through schools in the San Diego area, with very minimal to no experience with alcohol or drugs. Exclusionary criteria for the parent study at baseline included: premature birth prior to the 35th gestational week; report of prenatal alcohol (>2 drinks during a given week) or illicit drug exposure; history of any DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) Axis I or neurological disorder; psychoactive medication use; loss of consciousness (>2 minutes) or head trauma; learning disability or mental retardation; chronic medical illness; history of alcohol use (≥10 total drinking days, or > 2 drinks per week in lifetime); history of drug use (≥5 lifetime cigarette uses, ≥3 experiences with cannabis in lifetime or use in the past 3 months, or any other intoxicant use); non-correctable sensory problems; and inadequate English comprehension. Exclusionary criteria at each follow-up time-point consisted of endorsement of an emergent Axis I disorder as measured by a structured diagnostic interview (Shaffer et al., 2000). Participants were asked to refrain from alcohol and substance use for at least 24 hours prior to all baseline or follow-up assessments, verified via breath alcohol concentration and urine drug screen. The University of California San Diego Human Research Protections Program approved the study protocol and procedures (for additional methods see: Squeglia et al., 2009, Nguyen-Louie et al., 2015, Courtney et al., 2018).
Data for the current project was selected among the first 15 years of annual follow-up assessments. Participants were included in the present analysis if they: (1) transitioned to frequent binge drinking, averaging ≥ one binge episode per week for at least a one-year period, at any point during the 15-year follow-up period, and (2) provided usable neuroimaging data within 3 years of their 18th birthday. fMRI Go/No-go data was selected from available scan time points to be closest to their 18th birthday and prior to their transition to frequent binge drinking (mean age at scan = 18.0, SD = 1.3; N = 29; see Table 1). This time point was selected because all participants had begun moderate drinking by this time and it represented a proximal time point to the average age of transition (mean = 19.6 years old; SD = 1.6), thus reducing the potential for influence from extraneous developmental factors. Time to transition to frequent binge drinking was calculated as the difference in time (months) between age of transition onset and age at scan. The neuroimaging data of 8 participants in this sample are also included in the report by Wetherill et al., 2013.
Table 1.
Sample demographics.
Time Point | ||
---|---|---|
Frequency or Mean (SD)/[Range] | ||
Variable | Scan | Follow-Up |
Total N | 29 | 29 |
Age (years) | 18.0 (1.3) | 19.6 (1.6) |
[16.2–20.9] | [17–23] | |
Time to transition (years) | 1.6 (0.9) | |
[0.1–4.2] | ||
Sex | ||
- Male | 20 | |
- Female | 9 | |
Race/Ethnicity | ||
- White | 22 | |
- Asian | 1 | |
- Mixed | 6 | |
Education (years completed) | 11.3 (1.2) | 13.6 (1.4) |
[9.0–14.0] | [11.0–16.0] | |
Hollingshead Index - | 24.1 (14.9) | 26.5 (18.0) |
Socioeconomic Status | [11.0–65.0] | [11.0–65.0] |
Alcohol use (previous year) | 4.5 (3.0) | 6.4 (2.5) |
- Drinks per drinking day | 4.5 (3.0) | 6.4 (2.5) |
[1.0–12.0] | [3.0–12.0] | |
- Drinking days per month | 3.8 (4.6) | 13.7 (9.1) |
[<1.0–20.0] | [1.7–30.4] | |
- Binge days per month | 1.4 (2.3) [<1.0–11.6] | 5.6 (4.4) [4.0–19.5] |
Other substance use | ||
(≤1x per week, 3+ months) | ||
- Tobacco | 4 | 9 |
- Cannabis | 3 | 14 |
Measures
Youths were administered comprehensive interviews at baseline, including the assessment of demographics, living situations, and alcohol and drug use. The Hollingshead Index of Social Position score (Hollingshead, 1965), an index of socioeconomic status (SES), was calculated for each subject using parental socioeconomic background information (i.e., educational attainment, occupation, and salary of each parent) to characterize the youth’s rearing environment. Higher values indicate lower SES (possible range 11–77). Corroborative information from an informant (a biological parent in the majority of cases) was used to support youth report on demographic background and family history topics. The annual follow-up assessments were similarly structured.
The Customary Drinking and Drug Use Record structured interview (Brown et al., 1998) was used to assess history of alcohol consumption and alcohol-related problems, as well as additional substance use information. For the purposes of this study, a binge drinking episode was defined as the consumption of ≥5 alcoholic drinks for males, or ≥4 drinks for females, in a single occasion.
Imaging
Consistent with previous analysis on the parent data set (Wetherill et al., 2013), imaging data were collected using a 3.0 Tesla General Electric short bore Excite-2 system with an eight-channel phase-array head coil. A high-resolution T1-weighted sequence including a sagittally acquired spoiled gradient recalled sequence (256 × 256 × 192 matrix, .94 × .94 × 1mm voxels, field of view [FOV] 24cm, 176 slices, repetition time=8ms, inversion time=450ms, echo time=25ms, flip angle 12°, 7:26 minutes) was acquired. BOLD signal was measured with T2*-weighted axially acquired echo-planar images (64 × 64 matrix, 3.75 × 3.75 × 3.8mm voxels, FOV=24cm, 32 slices, echo time=30ms, repetition time=1,500ms, flip angle 90°, task time: 6:24 minutes). Field maps with two different echo times were used to measure signal dropout and field inhomogeneities. Field maps were applied to the BOLD signal to minimize signal dropouts and warping. Stimuli for the task were back-projected from a laptop to a screen at the foot of the scanner bed and were visible via an angled mirror attached to the head coil. Task performance and behavior was recorded using a fiber-optic response box compatible with MRI (Current Designs, Pittsburgh, PA).
Response inhibition was assessed during scanning via an event-related Go/No-Go paradigm (see Norman et al., 2011, Wetherill et al., 2013 for additional task details). The task consisted of a serial presentation of blue shapes, which included 64 large circles, 16 small circles, 43 large squares, and 57 small squares. The duration of each stimulus was 200ms and the intertrial interval was 1,500ms. Participants were asked to press a button each time a large circle, small circle, or large square shape was presented (go stimuli) but to withhold their response when a small square was presented (no-go stimulus, 32.0% of trials). Baseline constituted ~114 seconds scattered throughout the task. Primary analyses contrasted BOLD response during no-go correct rejection (successful response inhibition) trials (86.9% of no-go trials; SD=10.0%) relative to no-go false alarm (unsuccessful response inhibition) trials (13.1% of no-go trials). The no-go correct rejection versus go contrast was also evaluated. Correct rejections were determined by the absence of a motor response during no-go trials. False alarms were defined as a button press following a no-go stimulus.
Image Processing
Processing of imaging data was conducted using the bug-corrected version of the Analysis of Functional NeuroImages software (AFNI) (Cox, 1996). Abnormal signals and artifacts were removed from the data, and the time series data were aligned temporally and co-registered to a maximally stable base volume using an iterated least squares algorithm (Cox and Jesmanowicz, 1999). AFNI’s 3dSkullStrip was used to skull strip each participant’s high-resolution T1-weighted image. Participants’ anatomical and functional data sets were co-registered and warped to Talairach space (Talairach and Tournoux, 1988). Functional data were resampled to 3mm2 voxels, and activations maps were spatially smoothed using a 5mm full-width half maximum Gaussian filter. Motion was estimated for each participant (i.e., three rotational and three linear displacement parameters) and used as a control in task analyses (Bandettini et al., 1993). 3dDespike was used to detect outliers in the motion parameters. Significant outliers in the time-series data were censored or despiked.
Analysis of time series data utilized multiple regression (3dDeconvolve) controlling for linear drift, baseline signal, and motion from 6 motion parameters calculated above. Regressors of interest (go, no-go correct rejection, no-go false alarm) and no interest (go error) convolved with a modified gamma variate function (Boynton et al., 1996) that modeled anticipated hemodynamic response. Beta weights were converted to percent signal change which were used for further analysis.
Statistical analysis
Performance measures evaluated in the Go/No-Go task included percent correct on inhibitory trials, β (a marker of response bias), and d′ (a marker of accuracy in discriminating between go and no-go stimuli) (Green and Swets, 1966).
Similar to methods previously used by the authors (Courtney et al., 2019), activation was masked by an average skull-stripped anatomical image from all participants. Whole brain, voxel-level analysis on the masked data was conducted using a paired t-test (AFNI 3dttest++) to contrast no-go correct rejection vs. no-go false alarm trials. To control for variability in age at scan acquisition, time to transition to frequent binge drinking (age of transition onset – age at scan) was chosen for the correlation analysis and entered as the covariate of interest in the model. The Clustsim nonparametric randomization/permutation option of 3dttest++ was used with a conservative voxel-wise alpha of 0.001 and cluster-wise alpha of 0.05, resulting in an estimated a cluster size threshold of 18 contiguous voxels. This method of Type I error control has been shown to produce false positive rates compatible with the nominal 95% confidence interval (Cox et al., 2017, Eklund et al., 2016). A second model not containing the covariate of interest was run to validate the task by showing task-relevant activation in this sample using the same Type 1 error correction as above.
RESULTS
Participants
At the time of the scan, the sample drank alcohol approximately 4 days a month and binge drank once per month. After transitioning, the sample drank approximately 14 days per month and binged 6 days per month. Roughly 10% of the sample endorsed regular use (≥1x per week, 3+ months) of tobacco and/or cannabis, in addition to alcohol, at the time of the scan (see Table 1 for participant characteristics). Consistent with previous Go/No-Go reports (Ahmadi et al., 2013, Smith et al., 2014, Norman et al., 2011, Wetherill et al., 2013, Mahmood et al., 2013), false alarm rates were not associated with age at scan, time to transition to frequent binge drinking, drinking behavior at scan, or drinking behavior at follow-up (ps>.34) (see Table 2 for task performance).
Table 2.
Go/No-go task performance.
Variable | Mean (SD) |
---|---|
% Correct rejection – inhibitory trials | 86.9% (0.1) |
% False alarm – inhibitory trials | 13.1 % (0.1) |
Trial discrimination accuracy - d’ | 3.5 (0.6) |
Response bias - β | 0.33 (0.4) |
BOLD Response
The no-go correct rejection vs. no-go false alarm contrast revealed multiple regions related to response inhibition that surpassed thresholding: bilateral putamen, left cingulate, left postcentral gyrus, bilateral anterior cingulate (negative), and left middle temporal gyrus (see Table 3 and Figure 1).
Table 3.
Clusters of significant BOLD activation to no-go correct rejection vs. no-go false alarm trials from the whole-brain analysis (thresholded at a voxel-wise alpha of 0.001, cluster-wise alpha of 0.05; ≥ 17 contiguous voxels).
Talairach Coordinates | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Region (peak) | Cluster Voxels | X | Y | Z | Peak Z |
L putamen | 80 | −19.5 | 4.5 | −12.5 | 4.86 |
R putamen | 71 | 22.5 | 4.5 | −9.5 | 4.68 |
L fusiform gyrus, extending into parahippocampal gyrus | 41 | −22.5 | −37.5 | −15.5 | 4.09 |
L postcentral gyrus, extending into inferior parietal lobule | 40 | −52.5 | −28.5 | 41.5 | 4.15 |
R fusiform gyrus, extending into inferior temporal gyrus | 31 | 43.5 | −55.5 | −9.5 | 4.29 |
R parahippocampal gyrus, extending into culmen | 31 | 4.5 | −43.5 | −3.5 | 4.06 |
L/R posterior cingulate extending into culmen | 23 | −1.5 | −55.5 | 5.5 | 4.17 |
L middle temporal gyrus, extending into middle occipital gyrus | 22 | −40.5 | −67.5 | 20.5 | 3.99 |
L caudate extending into putamen | 20 | −16.5 | 7.5 | 14.5 | 4.71 |
R/L anterior cingulate | 20 | 1.5 | 16.5 | 20.5 | −3.95 |
Figure 1.
Percent signal change in blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) activation to no-go correct rejection vs. no-go false alarm trials (thresholded at a voxel-wise alpha of 0.001, cluster-wise alpha of 0.05; ≥ 18 contiguous voxels).
BOLD Response and Drinking
BOLD response contrast of no-go correct rejections vs. false alarms in the precentral gyrus/insula/inferior frontal gyrus cluster correlated with time to transition to frequent binge drinking (cluster size = 24 voxels, peak Z = 4.54; see Table 4 and Figure 2). In no region did no-go correct rejection relative to go activation significantly correlate with time to transition.
Table 4.
Cluster of significant BOLD response from the whole-brain analysis (no-go correct rejection vs. no-go false alarm) that correlated with time to transition to frequent binge drinking (thresholded at a voxel-wise alpha of 0.001 and cluster-wise alpha of 0.05; ≥ 18 contiguous voxels).
Talairach Coordinates | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Region (peak) | Cluster Voxels | X | Y | Z | Peak Z |
L precentral gyrus, including insula and inferior frontal gyrus | 24 | −34.5 | 1.5 | 23.5 | 4.54 |
Figure 2.
a) Percent signal change in blood-oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) activation to no-go correct rejection vs. no-go false alarm trials which correlated with time to transition to frequent binge drinking (thresholded at a voxel-wise alpha of 0.001, cluster-wise alpha of 0.05; ≥ 18 contiguous voxels); b) Average percent signal change extracted from the cluster identified in the whole-brain analysis and plotted against time to transition. Cluster activation estimates were extracted for visualization purposes only - no statistics were calculated on the extracted datapoints to avoid potential inflation of the correlation estimate.
DISCUSSION
As expected, successful inhibition in this sample of frequent binge drinkers was associated with activation in a number of regions previously implicated in the literature, including the fronto-striatal system (Zhang et al., 2017, Morein-Zamir and Robbins, 2015), validating the use of this paradigm in our high risk sample. In the correlation analysis, a single cluster of activation, including portions of the left insula, inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), and precentral gyrus, elicited during successful inhibitory control, was found to predict time to transition to high-risk frequent binge drinking in adolescents who were already engaged in moderate alcohol use. Specifically, greater BOLD response in this cluster predicted longer time to transition, implying that lower magnitude of activation during successful inhibition could serve as a temporal warning of future high-risk impulsive behavior.
The IFG, precentral gyrus, and insula have been consistently implicated as critical regions involved in response inhibition (Zhang et al., 2017). Although the right IFG/insula are most commonly implicated (for a review see Bari and Robbins, 2013), a number of studies have also implicated key roles for the left IFG/insula in this process (Swick et al., 2008, Meffert et al., 2016, Zhang and Li, 2012). This study provides further support for the involvement of the left IFG/insula in inhibitory control by demonstrating the predictive utility of activation in these top-down executive control regions for the onset of impulsive binge drinking behavior.
The correlation with time to transition to frequent binge drinking also supports the notion that this pattern of alcohol consumption is likely driven, at least in part, by deficiencies in inhibitory control and suggests opportunities for intervention prior to the onset of this very high-risk behavior. Inhibitory control interventions, particularly those utilizing Go/No-go paradigms, have demonstrated effectiveness for short-term health behavior change (Jones et al., 2016, Allom et al., 2016), which may be all that is needed to delay onset of this high-risk drinking pattern beyond the critical neurodevelopmental stage of adolescence.
The results of this study should be interpreted within the context of its strengths and limitations. The prospective correlational design is a strength of the study, as it avoids issues related to the selection of comparable controls and addresses the question of whether the magnitude of the BOLD signal during inhibitory control contains clinically relevant predictive information. Another strength is the well-characterized sample of frequent binge drinkers who were already engaged in moderate alcohol use at scan acquisition. Few attempts have been made to identify unique risk factors for adolescents that are already engaged in moderate alcohol use, despite the exceptionally high prevalence of adolescent alcohol users. Limitations of the study include the relatively small sample size for fMRI studies and small number of no-go false alarm trials included in the analysis. The use of conservative statistical thresholding and the consistence of implicated regions with the extent literature on inhibitory control provides support for the validity of the results; however, additional studies using more difficult tasks (and thus greater trial numbers) within larger samples are needed to confirm these within-subjects effects. Furthermore, the sample is comprised predominately of White adolescents with high educational attainment, potentially limiting the generalizability of the results. Thus, replication within a more diverse sample of adolescents is warranted.
In conclusion, this study suggests that BOLD response in portions of the IFG, insula, and precentral gyrus during successful inhibitory control could prove valuable as a temporally specific risk marker for future frequent binge drinking behavior. Early identification of adolescents at-risk for this pattern of alcohol use is of great importance given the potential for neural consequences associated with alcohol use during neurodevelopment (Squeglia et al., 2014). The increased study of risk factors for youth already engaged in moderate alcohol use could provide additional insights into meaningful pathways for intervention that were previously overlooked.
Acknowledgments
Support: Research supported by NIAAA grant R01 AA013419. KEC was supported by training grants from NIAAA (T32 AA013525) and NIMH (T32 MH018399). The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.
REFERENCES
- Allom V, Mullan B, Hagger M (2016) Does inhibitory control training improve health behaviour? A meta-analysis. Health Psychol Rev 10:168–186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association; (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC, Author. [Google Scholar]
- Bandettini PA, Jesmanowicz A, Wong EC, Hyde JS (1993) Processing strategies for time-course data sets in functional MRI of the human brain. Magn Reson Med 30:161–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bari A, Robbins TW (2013) Inhibition and impulsivity: behavioral and neural basis of response control. Prog Neurobiol 108:44–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boynton GM, Engel SA, Glover GH, Heeger DJ (1996) Linear systems analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging in human V1. J Neurosci 16:4207–4221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown SA, Myers MG, Lippke L, Tapert SF, Stewart DG, Vik PW (1998) Psychometric evaluation of the Customary Drinking and Drug Use Record (CDDR): A measure of adolescent alcohol and drug involvement. J Stud Alcohol 59:427–438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casey BJ (2015) Beyond simple models of self-control to circuit-based accounts of adolescent behavior. Annu Rev Psychol 66:295–319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chassin L, Pitts SC, Prost J (2002) Binge drinking trajectories from adolescence to emerging adulthood in a high-risk sample: predictors and substance abuse outcomes. J Consult Clin Psychol 70:67–78. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Courtney KE, Infante MA, Brown GG, Tapert SF, Simmons AN, Smith TL, Schuckit MA (2019) The relationship between regional cerebral blood flow estimates and alcohol problems at 5-year follow-up: the role of level of response. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 43:812–821. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Courtney KE, Polich J (2009) Binge Drinking in Young Adults: Data, Definitions, and Determinants. Psychol Bull 135:142–156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Courtney KE, Worley M, Castro N, Tapert SF (2018) The effects of alcohol hangover on future drinking behavior and the development of alcohol problems. Addict Behav 78:209–215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cox RW (1996) AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput Biomed Res 29:162–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cox RW, Chen G, Glen DR, Reynolds RC, Taylor PA (2017) FMRI Clustering in AFNI: False-Positive Rates Redux. Brain Connect 7:152–171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cox RW, Jesmanowicz A (1999) Real-time 3D image registration for functional MRI. Magn Reson Med 42:1014–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eklund A, Nichols TE, Knutsson H (2016) Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 113:7900–7905. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green DM, Swets JA (1966). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York, Wiley & Sons, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Hollingshead AB (1965). Two-factor index of social position. New Haven, CT, Yale University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Jones A, Di Lemma LC, Robinson E, Christiansen P, Nolan S, Tudur-Smith C, Field M (2016) Inhibitory control training for appetitive behaviour change: A meta-analytic investigation of mechanisms of action and moderators of effectiveness. Appetite 97:16–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mahmood OM, Goldenberg D, Thayer R, Migliorini R, Simmons AN, Tapert SF (2013) Adolescents’ fMRI activation to a response inhibition task predicts future substance use. Addict Behav 38:1435–1441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meffert H, Hwang S, Nolan ZT, Chen G, Blair JR (2016) Segregating attention from response control when performing a motor inhibition task: Segregating attention from response control. Neuroimage 126:27–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miech RA, Schulenberg JE, Johnston LD, Bachman JG, O’malley PM, Patrick ME (2017). National Adolescent Drug Trends in 2017: Findings Released. Ann Arbor, Institute for Social Research, The University of Michigan. [Google Scholar]
- Morein-Zamir S, Robbins TW (2015) Fronto-striatal circuits in response-inhibition: Relevance to addiction. Brain Res 1628:117–129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nguyen-Louie TT, Castro N, Matt GE, Squeglia LM, Brumback T, Tapert SF (2015) Effects of emerging alcohol and marijuana use behaviors on adolescents’ neuropsychological functioning over four years. J Stud Alcohol Drugs 76:738–748. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norman AL, Pulido C, Squeglia LM, Spadoni AD, Paulus MP, Tapert SF (2011) Neural activation during inhibition predicts initiation of substance use in adolescence. Drug Alcohol Depend 119:216–223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oscar-Berman M, Marinkovic K (2007) Alcohol: effects on neurobehavioral functions and the brain. Neuropsychol Rev 17:239–257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruan H, Zhou Y, Luo Q, Robert GH, Desrivières S, Quinlan EB, Liu Z, Banaschewski T, Bokde AL, Bromberg U, Büchel C, Flor H, Frouin V, Garavan H, Gowland P, Heinz A, Ittermann B, Martinot JL, Martinot MLP, Nees F, Orfanos DP, Poustka L, Hohmann S, Fröhner JH, Smolka MN, Walter H, Whelan R, Li F, Schumann G, Feng J (2019) Adolescent binge drinking disrupts normal trajectories of brain functional organization and personality maturation. Neuroimage Clin 22:101804. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schulenberg JE, Johnston LD, O’malley PM, Bachman JG, Miech RA, Patrick ME (2018). Monitoring the Future national survey results on drug use, 1975–2017: Volume II, College students and adults ages 19–55. Ann Arbor, Institute for Social Research, The University of Michigan. [Google Scholar]
- Shaffer D, Fisher P, Lucas CP, Dulcan MK, Schwab-Stone ME (2000) NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children Version IV (NIMH DISC-IV): Description, differences from previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 39:28–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Squeglia LM, Jacobus J, Tapert SF (2014) The effect of alcohol use on human adolescent brain structures and systems. Handb Clin Neurol 125:501–510. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Squeglia LM, Spadoni AD, Infante MA, Myers MG, Tapert SF (2009) Initiating moderate to heavy alcohol use predicts changes in neuropsychological functioning for adolescent girls and boys. Psychol Addict Behav 23:715–722. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration 2018. Key substance use and mental health indicators in the United States: Results from the 2017 National Survey on Drug Use and Health. HHS Publication No. SMA 18–5068, NSDUH Series H-53. Rockville, MD: Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. [Google Scholar]
- Sullivan EV, Pfefferbaum A (2005) Neurocircuitry in alcoholism: a substrate of disruption and repair. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 180:583–594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swick D, Ashley V, Turken AU (2008) Left inferior frontal gyrus is critical for response inhibition. BMC Neurosci 9:102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Talairach J, Tournoux P (1988). Three-dimensional Coplanar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain proportional system: an approach to cerebral imaging. New York, Thieme. [Google Scholar]
- Viner RM, Taylor B (2007) Adult outcomes of binge drinking in adolescence: findings from a UK national birth cohort. J Epidemiol Community Health 61:902–907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wetherill RR, Squeglia LM, Yang TT, Tapert SF (2013) A longitudinal examination of adolescent response inhibition: Neural differences before and after the initiation of heavy drinking. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 230:663–671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whelan R, Watts R, Orr CA, Althoff RR, Artiges E, Banaschewski T, Barker GJ, Bokde AL, Buchel C, Carvalho FM, Conrod PJ, Flor H, Fauth-Buhler M, Frouin V, Gallinat J, Gan G, Gowland P, Heinz A, Ittermann B, Lawrence C, Mann K, Martinot JL, Nees F, Ortiz N, Paillere-Martinot ML, Paus T, Pausova Z, Rietschel M, Robbins TW, Smolka MN, Strohle A, Schumann G, Garavan H (2014) Neuropsychosocial profiles of current and future adolescent alcohol misusers. Nature 512:185–189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang R, Geng X, Lee TMC (2017) Large-scale functional neural network correlates of response inhibition: an fMRI meta-analysis. Brain Struct Funct 222:3973–3990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang S, Li CS (2012) Functional networks for cognitive control in a stop signal task: independent component analysis. Hum Brain Mapp 33:89–104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]