Abstract
Rationale
Social functioning is modulated by the endogenous opioid system. In opioid use disorder, social functioning appears disrupted, but little research has delineated the nature of these deficits and their relationship to acute opioid use.
Objectives
The current study aimed to assess both emotional and cognitive empathy, along with subjective and physiological responses to social exclusion in opioid users who were either acutely intoxicated or non-intoxicated from using opioids.
Methods
Individuals on an opioid substitution medication (OSM) were divided into ‘intoxicated users’ (had taken their OSM the same day as testing, n = 20) and ‘non-intoxicated users’ (had taken their OSM > 12 h ago, n = 20) and compared with opioid-naïve controls (n = 24). Empathy was assessed using the multifaceted empathy test and self-report questionnaire. Participants also underwent a period of social exclusion (Cyberball Game) and completed measures of mood and physiological responses (salivary cortisol and heart rate).
Results
Non-intoxicated users had significantly lower emotional empathy (the ability to experience others’ emotions), as well as greater anger after social exclusion when compared with the intoxicated users and controls. Anger did not change with social exclusion in the intoxicated user group and cortisol levels were lower overall.
Conclusions
Reduced ability to spontaneously share the emotions of others was reported in non-intoxicated users, particularly regarding positive emotions. There was some support for the idea of hyperalgesia to social pain, but this was restricted to an enhanced anger response in non-intoxicated users. Equivalent rates of empathy between the intoxicated users and controls could indicate some remediating effects of acute opioids.
Electronic supplementary material
The online version of this article (10.1007/s00213-019-05378-x) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Keywords: Opioids, Addiction, Empathy, Social pain, Social cognition, Cortisol
Introduction
The misuse of opioids is a growing global concern, with approximately 34 million users worldwide and recent reports of a dramatic increase in overdose rates (United Nations of Office on Drugs and Crime 2018). As well as high rates of mortality, opioid misuse has other health-related consequences, such as increased rates of HIV, hepatitis C and neonatal abstinence syndrome (National Institute on Drug Abuse 2018). Understanding the factors that initiate and maintain opioid use disorder is thus imperative from a public health perspective. Much work has focused on the biological and behavioural mechanisms of opioid addiction; however, research into the role of psychosocial factors is comparatively sparse (Heilig et al. 2016). It is well understood that social factors including social deprivation and interpersonal trauma can predict and maintain addiction (Gerra et al. 2014; Heffernan et al. 2000; Kendler et al. 2014; Lake et al. 2015; MacGregor and Thickett 2011; Naqavi et al. 2011). Opioids may be used in part to compensate for difficulties in emotion regulation (Moustafa et al. 2018; Wolff et al. 2016). Additionally, high rates of social marginalisation, ostracism and discrimination towards addicted individuals (Barry et al. 2014) may perpetuate deficits in social functioning and could contribute to the maintenance of opioid use.
Neurobiologically, the endogenous opioid system plays a role in social functioning (see Machin and Dunbar 2011, for a review) and is involved in empathy (Rutgen et al. 2015), which has a uniquely social purpose (Panksepp and Panksepp 2013; Pearce et al. 2017). Empathy is crucial for interpersonal relationships and bonding: impairments in the ability to empathise are observed in disorders such as autism spectrum disorder (Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright 2004) and schizophrenia (Green et al. 2015), and are related to difficulties in social functioning (Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright 2004). Impaired empathy in people with substance use disorders has also been reported (Ferrari et al. 2014). Two pivotal aspects of empathy are ‘emotional empathy’, referring to the ability to vicariously feel the emotional state of others, and ‘cognitive empathy’, which refers to the ability to identify and understand the emotional state of others (sometimes referred to as ‘theory of mind’) (Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright 2004; Blair 2005). Impairments in emotional empathy have been observed in drug users generally (Ferrari et al. 2014), alcohol users (Maurage et al. 2011) and stimulant users (Kroll et al. 2018a, 2018b; Preller et al. 2014). Two studies with chronic opioid users have similarly reported impairments in emotional empathy using a subjective questionnaire among methadone- and diacetylmorphine-maintained individuals (Stange et al. 2017; Tomei et al. 2017) but a further study failed to replicate these findings (Kroll et al. 2018a). The ability to empathise can be affected by situational factors including psychosocial stress, affective state, and socioeconomic status (Kanske et al. 2017), where acute opioid intoxication state may also be important to understand impairments in empathy within the context of wider social stress.
Opioid drugs may also affect social functioning by altering responses to difficult social events. Acutely, exogenous opioids have shown to alleviate the experience of both physical and emotional pain (Bershad et al. 2016; Inturrisi 2002; Stein et al. 2007). The latter is termed ‘emotional analgesia’ and is thought to be a protective mechanism from emotional pain, and is associated with reductions in subjective distress and cortisol following social exclusion (Bass et al. 2014). ‘Social’ pain is used to refer to a specific form of emotional pain, such as the painful feelings following an unpleasant social event like bullying, social rejection or exclusion (Eisenberger 2015). Both social and physical pain have overlapping neural mechanisms (however see Iannetti et al. 2013, for a review of the differences). Similar to physical pain, the brain responds to social pain (exclusion) by releasing endorphins to buffer against the unpleasant emotional experience (Hsu et al. 2013).
Pain perception is altered following chronic use of opioid drugs. Studies have consistently reported a heightened sensitivity to physical pain in chronic opioid users (Compton et al. 2001; Higgins et al. 2018; Mao 2002; Marion Lee et al. 2011; Pud et al. 2006). Increased opioid tolerance via the downregulation of endogenous opioid receptors has been suggested to underpin opioid-induced hyperalgesia (Higgins et al. 2018; Mao 2002). As physical and social pain share some similar neural mechanisms (Eisenberger 2015; Hsu et al. 2013), it is plausible to suggest that alterations in opioid receptor function could similarly cause a heightened sensitivity to social, as well as physical, pain. To our knowledge, only one study has investigated the link between chronic opioid use and the experience of social pain in non-intoxicated opioid users, and found a heightened cortisol response to social exclusion (Kroll et al. 2019). We do not yet know how being acutely intoxicated affects response to social exclusion and empathy in opioid users, which may be a powerful factor in maintaining problematic substance use.
Therefore, the current study aimed to investigate alterations in social functioning by measuring empathy and responses to social exclusion among individuals with histories of chronic opioid use. We aimed to examine both the acute and long-term effects of opioids in people prescribed an opioid substitution medication (OSM), by testing people intoxicated with OSM at the time of testing, and people who had not taken their medication for at least 12 hours. Based on previous research showing deficits in empathy in opioid users, we hypothesised that both of the opioid user groups would show impairments in empathy; however, given evidence that opioid intoxication is associated with impaired emotional empathy (Stange et al. 2017; Tomei et al. 2017), we predicted that emotional empathy would be most impaired in the intoxicated group. Secondly, we hypothesised that the intoxicated user group would have a dampened response to social exclusion—based on the analgesic effects of opioids and the assertion that physical and social pain are related. We further predicted that the non-intoxicated user group would be more subjectively affected by social exclusion given the hyperalgesia to physical pain seen in non-intoxicated opioid users.
Method
Design and participants
Sixty-four participants (39 males; 24 females; 1 non-binary) aged 22–67 (M = 42.69, SD = 11.54) were recruited into the study. Forty were opioid users currently stabilised on OSM (methadone or buprenorphine), and all had histories of illicit heroin use. Of these, 20 individuals took their opioid prescription in the morning of the study (intoxicated group), and 20 individuals had taken their prescription > 12 h ago (non-intoxicated group). Group membership was validated with tests of salivary opioid levels. The remaining 24 individuals were opioid-naïve controls with no history of opioid use. Groups were matched in age, gender and verbal IQ. Participants were recruited via word of mouth and advertisements in drug services and employment/training agencies.
The study was a mixed design. Inclusion criteria for the opioid groups were a prolonged history of opioid use and currently taking daily OSM. General inclusion criteria were being a minimum of 18 years old and a fluent English speaker. Exclusion criteria were neurological conditions, history of severe mental health issues, diagnosis of a physical illness that directly influences cortisol activity (i.e. Cushing or Addison disease), taking oral steroid medication and pregnancy. Individuals were excluded from the control group if they had any history of opioid use. Participants were asked to abstain from alcohol and drugs 24 h prior to their study session and abstain from smoking or eating for 45 min prior to their session. The study was reviewed by the institutional ethics committee and was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, all participants gave written, witnessed, informed consent.
Measures
Multifaceted empathy test (MET, see Fig 1) (Dziobek et al. 2008)
This computerised task indexes cognitive and emotional empathy. Forty photographs of people with emotionally charged expressions are given in eight blocks consisting of ten pictures each. In half of these blocks, participants are asked to identify the correct emotion of the subject in each scene (cognitive empathy). In the other half, participants were asked to rate how much they empathise with the individual in each scene (emotional empathy). Each image was presented until the participant gave a response, and participants were asked to respond as quickly as possible. The task lasted approximately 15 min. Responses for cognitive empathy were the total count of correctly identified emotions, while responses for emotional empathy were the mean empathy score.
The Cyberball Game (Williams et al. 2012)
This is a computerised ball-tossing game shown to simulate social exclusion. Participants are told that they are playing real people on a virtual network in a mental visualisation experiment, yet unbeknown to them the other players are fictitious and were set up to socially exclude them. In the present study, the Cyberball Game contained four players and had two conditions that simulated either social inclusion or exclusion. There were two games: inclusion followed by exclusion, and each game lasted between 2 and 4 min. Each condition had approximately 60 ball throws between the four players. In the social inclusion condition, participants were over-included and received 20 ± 1 (~ 33.3%) of 60 ball throws. In the exclusion game, participants received exactly 6 ± 1 (~ 10%) of 60 ball throws.
Affective and physiological responses to social inclusion and exclusion were recorded after each game with the Post-ostracism Cyberball Questionnaire (Williams et al. 2002), which assessed mood and basic psychological needs (see supplementary material (SM1) for more details)
Physiological measures
Seven saliva samples were collected by passive drool method. Participants were required to provide approximately 2 ml of saliva, which was immediately stored at − 80 °C until analysis using enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA) kits to assess cortisol levels, as well as levels of methadone, buprenorphine and opiates (baseline sample only). Heart rate was also assessed alongside each saliva sample (see SM1 for details).
Questionnaires
Trait empathy was assessed using the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI; Davis 1980), which consists of four different subscales: two characterise emotional empathy (empathic concern; personal distress), and two characterise cognitive empathy (perspective taking; fantasy scale). The Life Events Checklist Version 5 (LEC-5; Weathers et al. 2013) was used to assess trauma by measuring past exposure to any stressful or traumatic life events, and how proximal these events were to the participant (adapted to include age). Loneliness was measured using the UCLA Loneliness Scale (Russell 1996) which assesses feelings of social isolation and loneliness. Craving was assessed using three single items of drug liking, wanting and motivation to obtain opioid drugs (Pool et al. 2016). Verbal IQ was assessed using the Spot-the-Word Test (Baddeley et al. 1993). Mood at baseline was also assessed using the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (Watson et al. 1988). See SM1 for further details of each psychometric measure.
Procedure
Participants arrived in the afternoon between the times of 1 and 1.30 pm to control for diurnal variation in cortisol, and testing lasted for approximately 2 h. All procedures and approximate timings are depicted in Fig. 2.
Upon completion of all procedures, participants were fully debriefed on the true nature of the study, and given an opportunity to ask any questions. Participants were remunerated for their participation with a voucher.
Statistical analysis
Data were analysed using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) version 23 and Mplus version 8. Assumptions of normality were checked, extreme outliers were winsorized (Wilcox 2005) and random missing values were imputed by group mean substitution.
A series of one-way, between-subject ANOVAs were used to assess the effect of group on both emotional and cognitive empathy. For the Cyberball Games, subjective responses to social exclusion were analysed using a series of 3 × 2 mixed measures ANOVAs assessed the effects of group and inclusion status on subjective measures (the PCQ and craving). For the cortisol and heart rate, latent growth curve models (LGCM) were used to understand the between-person difference in the trajectory of responses over time in respect to the average trend (Muthén and Curran 1997; Willett and Sayer 1994) and encompass features of both structural equation modelling and repeated measures ANOVA (Duncan et al. 2013) (described in SM2).
Any significant interactions were investigated further using post hoc t tests, which were adjusted using the Holm-Bonferroni correction. Differences between groups in demographic information was analysed using t tests, Chi-square tests where data was categorical and the Kruskal-Wallis test (groups > 3) or Mann-Whitney U (groups < 2) test where data was non-parametric. Pearson’s correlations were used to assess statistical relationships, and Spearman’s correlations were used when normality was violated.
Latent growth curve modelling (LGCM)
To investigate if the levels of opioid exposure (‘Group’) were associated with different physiological response trajectories throughout the tasks, we applied LGCM using Mplus (Muthén and Muthén 2000) (the growth model procedure is described in more detail in SM2). Model fit was assessed using the comparative fit index (CFI), the Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), the root mean squared error of approximation (RMSEA) and the standardised root mean square residual (SRMR). Improvements in the model were assessed using both the Bayesian information criterion (aBIC) and Akaike information criterion (AIC). The robust maximum likelihood estimation (MLR) was used for each model.
Results
Demographics and drug use (Table 1)
Table 1.
Intoxicated (n = 20) | Non-intoxicated (n = 20) | Controls (n = 24) | Test statistic | P value | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 44.45 (11.51) | 40.40 (10.04) | 43.13 (12.83) | F = 0.64 | .533 | |
Gender (male, female, other) | 12, 8, 0 | 14, 6, 0 | 13, 10, 1 | χ2 = 2.56 | .663 | |
Ethnicity (Caucasian, Hispanic, mixed) | 20, 0, 0 | 18, 1, 1 | 21, 0, 3 | χ2 = 5.20 | .267 | |
Age left education | 16.25 (1.55) | 15.32 (3.79) | 17.65 (3.25) | F = 3.26 | .045* | |
Verbal IQ | 47.35 (10.82) | 44.89 (8.91) | 48.83 (5.76) | F = 1.09 | .342 | |
Mental health problems (n = yes) | 11 | 16 | 8 | χ2 = 11.12 | .004** | |
Diagnosis (n) | Depression | 10 | 14 | 6 | ||
Anxiety | 5 | 2 | 1 | |||
Other | 0 | 2 | 1 | |||
Physical health problems (n = yes) | 6 | 4 | 3 | χ2 = 2.04 | .360 | |
Antidepressants (n = yes) | 7 | 10 | 5 | χ2 = 4.72 | .095 | |
Oral contraceptives (n = yes) | 1 | 1 | 0 | χ2 = 1.71 | .426 | |
Familial mental health problems (n = yes) | 4 | 6 | 9 | χ2 = 1.61 | .447 | |
Familial substance use disorder (n = yes) | 7 | 4 | 6 | χ2 = 1.04 | .595 | |
Baseline positive affect | 28.33 (7.50) | 29.72 (8.17) | 29.92 (7.13) | F = 0.25 | .779 | |
Baseline negative affect | 14.16 (5.56) | 15.45 (6.20) | 11.71 (2.94) | F = 3.23 | .046* | |
Opioid substitution medications (OSM) | ||||||
Medication, n (methadone, buprenorphine, other) | 16, 1, 3 | 12, 6, 2 | χ2 = 4.34 | .114 | ||
Dose (standardised to oral morphineb, mg) | 28.78 (17.24) | 36.43 (19.32) | F = 1.75 | .194 | ||
Months taken OSM | 60.00 (173.25)a | 12.00 (31.00)a | U = 106.0 | .011* | ||
Hours since taken OSM | 3.92 (2.01) | 23.41 (7.65) | F = 114.19 | < .001*** | ||
Current regular drug use (n) | ||||||
Illicit opioids | 9 | 9 | 0 | χ2 = 15.03 | .001 | |
Alcohol | 11 | 12 | 13 | χ2 = 0.17 | .919 | |
Tobacco | 14 | 17 | 7 | χ2 = 15.46 | < .001*** | |
Cannabis | 8 | 8 | 2 | χ2 = 7.44 | .024* | |
Benzodiazepines | 3 | 3 | 0 | χ2 = 3.97 | .137 | |
Cocaine | 7 | 6 | 0 | χ2 = 9.94 | .007** | |
Salivary opioid screens | n = 20 | n = 20 | ||||
Methadone, n = positive, % due to opioid prescription | 16, 100% | 13, 83.3% | ||||
Buprenorphine, n = positive | 0 | 1, 100% | ||||
Opiates, n = positive | 6, 33.3% | 1, 0% | ||||
Urine drug screens | n = 20 | n = 15 | n = 24 | |||
Methadone, n = positive | 14 | 10 | 0 | |||
Opiates, n = positive | 9 | 8 | 0 | |||
Cannabis/THC, n = positive | 6 | 5 | 3 | |||
Cocaine, n = positive | 5 | 5 | 2 | |||
Amphetamine, n = positive | 1 | 1 | 2 | |||
Benzodiazepines, n = positive | 3 | 7 | 0 | |||
MDMA, n = positive | 0 | 1 | 0 |
Note: aNon-parametric data: median and IQR are reported
bThe equivalent doses are an approximation and calculated from the following sources (Foley 1985; Royal College of Anaesthetists 2018)
Current regular use of MDMA, amphetamines, and hallucinogens were excluded from the table due to minimal numbers
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001
Groups were matched in age, gender, ethnicity, alcohol use, verbal IQ, baseline positive affect and familial history of substance abuse problems and mental health problems. There were differences in the number of diagnosed mental health problems, with increased incidence of mental health problems in the non-intoxicated opioid users compared to controls (χ2 = 11.13, p = .004), but no other differences (Holm-Bonferroni corrected). Although there was an overall group difference in age individuals left education and baseline negative affect, after correction for multiple comparisons, there were no group differences.
There was a significant difference in the number of months taking an OSM between the two opioid user groups, with a greater number of months on OSM in the intoxicated (see Table 1). There were significant group differences in substance use for opioids, tobacco, cannabis and cocaine users comparing both the opioid groups to the controls (χ2 = 15.02, p = .012; χ2 = 14.53, p = .012; χ2 = 7.44, p = .042; and χ2 = 9.79, p = .016, respectively); however, there were no significant differences in illicit substance use between the two opioid user groups (χ2 = 0.00, p > .999, χ2 = 1.29, p > .999; χ2 = 0.00, p > .999; and χ2 = 0.11, p > .999, respectively) (Holm-Bonferroni corrected). Further details on drug use history can be found in SM3.
Hypothesis 1: impairment in emotional empathy
The multifaceted empathy test (MET)
For emotional empathy, there was a significant difference in group (F(2,61) = 3.52, p = .036, η2 = .10). Holm-Bonferroni t tests indicated the non-intoxicated user group scored significantly lower than the controls (t(42) = 2.64, p = .048, η2 = .14) (Fig. 3); however, there were no differences between the non-intoxicated users and the intoxicated users (t(38) = 1.91, p = .128, η2 = .09) or the intoxicated users and controls (t(42) = 0.40, p = .688, η2 < .01). Emotional empathy to either positive or negative affect was also explored (all analyses were Holm-Bonferroni corrected). For positive affect, there was an effect of group (F(2,61) = 6.39, p = .024, η2 = .17), where the non-intoxicated group rated significantly lower than controls (t(42) = 4.03, p = .002, η2 = .28). There were no differences between the intoxicated and non-intoxicated users (t(38) = 1.53, p = .512, η2 = .06) or intoxicated users and controls (t(42) = 1.78, p = .415, η2 = .07) (Fig. 3). For negative affect, there were no differences between groups (F(2,61) = 1.99, p = .512, η2 = .06).
When assessing cognitive empathy, there were no significant differences between the three groups (F(2,56) = 1.76, p = .182, η2 = .04). Number of words known in the MET was included as a covariate in this analysis due to being correlated with cognitive empathy (r = .55, n = 60, p < .001). There were no group differences in cognitive empathy for positive or negative affect (F(2,61) = 1.07, p = .696, η2 = .03, and F(2,61) = 1.03, p = .696, η2 = .03, respectively) (Holm-Bonferroni corrected). For M and SDs for MET subscales, see SM4.
The Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI)
For emotional empathy subscales, there were no significant group differences in ‘empathic concern’ (F(2,61) = 0.14, p = .871, η2 = .01) or ‘personal distress’ (F(2,61) = 0.05, p = .950, η2 < .01). For cognitive empathy subscales, there were no significant group differences in ‘perspective taking’ (F(2,61) = 0.95, p = .394, η2 = .03) or ‘fantasy’ (F(2,61) = 1.62, p = .206, η2 = .05) (for M and SDs, see SM4).
Hypothesis 2: chronic opioid users show dampened responses to social pain
The Cyberball Task
There were significant main effects of inclusion status which reflected decreases in mood, self-esteem, control, meaningful existence and sense of belonging following exclusion, as well as increases in hurt feelings. However, there were no significant effects of group, or interaction between inclusion status and group (Table 2).
Table 2.
Inclusion status | Intoxicated | Non-intoxicated | Control | F-statistic | p value | η2 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
∆Mood | Inclusion | 2.49 (1.04) | 2.27 (1.35) | 2.62 (0.83) | Group | 1.95 | .151 | .03 |
Exclusion | 1.65 (1.92) | 0.36 (2.03) | 0.95 (1.74) | Inclusion status | 39.00 | < .001*** | .18 | |
Group*inclusion status | 1.79 | .176 | .02 | |||||
Self-esteem | Inclusion | 3.02 (1.33) | 2.80 (1.23) | 3.44 (0.95) | Group | 1.63 | .205 | .09 |
Exclusion | 2.41 (1.31) | 1.87 (0.85) | 2.25 (0.89) | Inclusion status | 47.28 | < .001*** | .39 | |
Group*inclusion status | 1.68 | .196 | .03 | |||||
Sense of belonginga | Inclusion | 1.35 (0.48) | 1.32 (0.71) | 1.14 (0.28) | Group | 0.77 | .466 | .02 |
Exclusion | 2.45 (1.43) | 2.92 (1.48) | 2.53 (1.13) | Inclusion status | 69.46 | < .001*** | .52 | |
Group*inclusion status | 0.74 | .480 | .01 | |||||
Meaningful existencea | Inclusion | 0.09 (1.78) | 0.11 (0.16) | 0.03 (0.08) | Group | 2.23 | .116 | .03 |
Exclusion | 0.34 (0.22) | 0.35 (0.26) | 0.26 (0.21) | Inclusion status | 52.13 | < .001*** | .28 | |
Group*inclusion status | < .01 | .996 | < .01 | |||||
Controla | Inclusion | 0.30 (0.20) | 0.31 (0.20) | 0.38 (0.17) | Group | 1.06 | .352 | .02 |
Exclusion | 0.18 (0.22) | 0.09 (0.16) | 0.16 (0.20) | Inclusion status | 68.12 | < .001*** | .20 | |
Group*inclusion status | 1.97 | .148 | .01 | |||||
Anger | Inclusion | 1.15 (0.67) | 1.40 (0.88) | 1.04 (0.20) | Group | 12.12 | < .001*** | .24 |
Exclusion | 1.15 (0.37) | 2.50 (1.32) | 1.46 (0.78) | Inclusion status | 14.22 | < .001*** | .13 | |
Group*inclusion status | 5.42 | .007 | .10 | |||||
Hurt feelingsa | Inclusion | 0.02 (0.07) | 0.02 (0.11) | 0.02 (0.08) | Group | 0.20 | .822 | < .01 |
Exclusion | 0.19 (0.27) | 0.23 (0.27) | 0.19 (0.25) | Inclusion status | 32.25 | < .001*** | .19 | |
Group*inclusion status | 0.09 | .910 | < .01 | |||||
% of perceived ball throws | Inclusion | 32.93 (10.75) | 38.62 (23.12) | 41.47 (19.81) | Group | 0.91 | .409 | .01 |
Exclusion | 15.27 (9.87) | 11.14 (6.24) | 16.03 (11.18) | Inclusion status | 62.61 | < .001*** | .39 | |
Group*inclusion status | 0.97 | .386 | .01 | |||||
∆Moodb (baseline to exclusion) | Baseline | 14.35 (8.91) | 13.83 (11.05) | 18.21 (9.52) | Group | 0.68 | .508 | .02 |
Exclusion | 7.22 (14.06) | 6.44 (12.09) | 8.58 (12.41) | Inclusion status | 32.06 | < .001*** | .11 | |
Group*inclusion status | 0.43 | .652 | < .01 |
Note: ∆Mood was calculated by subtracting negative affect scores from overall positive affect scores. The adjectives used to compute positive mood in the PCQ were good, happy, friendly, relaxed, whilst negative mood were bad, sad, unfriendly and tense
aLog transformation was applied. Mean values are adjusted for the log transformation
b∆Mood (baseline to exclusion) is a manipulation check that Cyberball exclusion condition caused reductions in mood from baseline (using responses on the PANAS rather than mood assessed by the PCQ)
For Anger, there was a significant interaction between inclusion status and group (F(2,61) = 5.42, p = .007, η2 = .10). Holm-Bonferroni corrected pairwise comparisons indicated that there was a significant difference in anger between the non-intoxicated user group with the intoxicated group (p < .001) and controls (p < .001); however, there were no differences between the intoxicated group with controls (p = .561) (Fig. 4). There was also a main effect of inclusion status (F(1,61) = 14.11, p < .001, η2 = .13), alongside a main effect of group (F(2,61) = 12.12, p < .001, η2 = .24). There were no effects of group or inclusion status on opioid craving (SM5).
Physiological responses
Salivary cortisol
The LGCM with continuous latent variables of intercept for cortisol at minute 0 (baseline) and a quadratic slope as outcome between minutes 0 and 119 including dummy-coded group as the covariate revealed a good fit χ2(22) = 34.54, p = .043, CFI = .94; TLI = .93; SRMR = .07; RMSEA = .09, 90%CI [0.02,0.15]; AIC = − 1064.34; aBIC = − 1027.66. Being intoxicated was negatively related with the intercept at 0 minutes (b = − 0.07, SE = 0.03, p = .016), suggesting they had lower cortisol levels at baseline compared to the controls, but there were no effects for the non-intoxicated group (b = − 0.01, SE = 0.04, p = .759) who showed similar cortisol levels as the controls (Fig. 5a). In addition, there were significant effects of the intoxicated group when the intercept was set at minutes 46 (post-inclusion), 60 (post-exclusion), 85 (recovery period) and 101 (recovery period) (see SM6 for the data) indicating that intoxicated users had lower cortisol responses throughout social exclusion and recovery in comparison to the non-intoxicated and controls. Neither being intoxicated (b = 0.01, SE = 0.01, p = .326) nor being non-intoxicated (b < 0.01, SE = 0.01, p = .690) was associated with the slope, suggesting that the trajectory of cortisol over time was not associated to acute opioid state.
Heart rate
A piecewise LGCM with continuous latent variables of intercept, with one linear slope from 46 to 68 min (the Cyberball paradigm) and the second linear slope from 85 to 119 min (post-exclusion recovery period) in heart rate change, including group and interpersonal trauma as a covariate revealed the best and an overall acceptable fit χ2(21) = 36.73, p = .018, CFI = .95; TLI = .92; SRMR = .04; RMSEA = .12, 90%CI [0.05, 0.18]; AIC = 2008.64; aBIC = 2056.81. Being intoxicated had a significant negative effect on the intercept at 46 min (b = − 4.77, SE = 2.17, p = .028) (Fig. 5b), suggesting lower heart rate at baseline. In addition, there were significant effects of the intoxicated group when the intercept was set at minutes 60 and 101 and minutes 68 and 119 (see SM6) indicating that intoxicated users had less change in heart rate throughout social exclusion and recovery in comparison to the non-intoxicated and controls. There were no significant slope effects but the intoxicated user group had a near-significant effect on the linear slope between 46 and 68 min (b = 1.04, SE = 0.55, p = .057), and a similar trend was observed in the non-intoxicated user group (b = 1.26, SE = 0.71, p = .075) suggesting a gentler downward slope compared with the control condition. There was also a trend to suggest the effect of the intoxicated user group on the linear slope between 85 and 119 min (b = − 1.46, SE = 0.84, p = .081), suggesting smaller change during the recovery period compared with the controls. Rates of interpersonal trauma did not exert any significant effects on the intercept or slopes although adding it improved overall model fit.
Exploratory analyses
Emotional empathy was not correlated with the total months taking an OSM (rs = − .372, n = 20, p = .424) or hours since the OSM was taken (r = − .159, n = 15, p > .999) within the non-intoxicated group, nor was it correlated with rates of mental health problems over the sample (r = .03 n = 63, p < .999). There was a medium effect size for the association between emotional empathy and OSM dose within the non-intoxicated user group; however, it failed to reach significance (r = − .49, n = 20, p = .203).
Negative affect at baseline was not significantly related to emotional empathy for positive emotions (r = − 0.04, n = 63, p = .773) or change in anger from inclusion to exclusion (r = − 0.07, n = 63, p = .606).
Discussion
The current study aimed to assess empathy and responses to social exclusion among individuals with opioid use disorder. We found lower emotional empathy (i.e., the ability to vicariously experience the emotional state of others) among non-intoxicated opioid users compared with opioid-naïve controls, particularly for positive emotions. Non-intoxicated opioid users also expressed significantly greater anger after being socially excluded compared to the intoxicated user group and controls. On the other hand, intoxicated opioid users showed lower salivary cortisol and heart rate across the testing session; however, they did not differ in the level at which cortisol and heart rate particularly increased or decreased in response to social exclusion.
The finding of lower emotional empathy in the non-intoxicated users partially replicates previous research suggesting impaired empathy among drug users (Ferrari et al. 2014; Kroll et al. 2018a, 2018b; Maurage et al. 2011; Preller et al. 2014) and opioid users specifically (Kroll et al. 2018a; Stange et al. 2017; Tomei et al. 2017), but crucially highlighted that acutely intoxicated opioid users show intact emotional empathy compared to controls. This was contrary to our initial prediction that empathy would be lowest within the intoxicated user group. Previous work in healthy participants has connected higher levels of endogenous opioids with decreased empathy for pain, possibly due to a decreased sensitivity in the ability to feel pain in oneself (Rutgen et al. 2015); therefore, it has been suggested that the use of analgesic drugs like opioids could also reduce empathy. Our results suggest that this is not the case in this group of chronic opioid users and, in fact, the on-board opioids appear to repair their empathy to the level of controls, whereas non-intoxicated users showed impairments—specifically for positive emotions. There was a medium to large effect size for the correlation between opioid substitution medication (OSM) dose and emotional empathy, potentially indicating a dose-dependent reduction in emotional empathy within the non-intoxicated group that was not driven by outliers. However, this should be interpreted with caution, as this relationship was using a small sample size and was non-significant after adjusting the α-criterion for multiple comparisons. Further work should investigate whether there is a dose dependent relationship between opioid use and empathy.
The specific impairment in empathy for positive emotions was demonstrated previously in a similar study with opioid users (Kroll et al. 2018a). This suggests a possible negative bias where relating to positive emotions is more difficult for opioid users not currently experiencing the acute effects of opioids. Prior research has suggested that abstinent opioid addicts are biased when attending to negative emotions, where they show enhanced detection of negative expressions during a visual search paradigm (Zhou et al. 2012). This bias is potentially due to greater exposure to negative expressions and reactions from society in everyday life, as well as impaired emotion processing that could predate addiction (Zhou et al. 2012). Additional to this, distress intolerance—the inability to endure difficult emotional states—is associated with greater attentional bias towards negative emotions and decreased attention toward positive emotions (Macatee et al. 2018). Opioid users on an OSM show greater distress intolerance (Kathryn McHugh and Otto 2012), where opioids may heighten the threshold to cope with difficult emotional states. The empathy deficit for positive emotions in the non-intoxicated users may therefore be due to reduced exposure to positive emotions in everyday life and reduced attention towards them. Opioid intoxication may serve to remediate emotion difficulties by increasing distress tolerance and enhancing their ability to relate to positive emotions.
The study also reported a novel finding of increased rates of anger following social exclusion in the non-intoxicated opioid users, compared to the intoxicated user group and controls. Past research has linked anger expression with endogenous opioid functioning, suggesting that increased anger expression may be related to an impaired endogenous opioid response to stress (Bruehl et al. 2007). Preclinical evidence supports this assumption, finding that opioid blockade using naltrexone has shown to increase rates of anger and pain (Bruehl et al. 2008; Burns et al. 2009). As the non-intoxicated user group in the current study may have a dampened endogenous opioid response, this could possibly account for the large increase in anger after being socially excluded. The intoxicated user group may experience no change in anger due to the acute effects of opioids buffering from this unpleasant emotional state. Higher rates of hostility and anger are related to poor emotion regulation in drug users (Handelsman et al. 2000; Shabanloo et al. 2018). This finding of greater anger, together with impaired empathy, potentially suggests an overall impairment in both understanding and expressing one’s own emotions in individuals who have chronically used opioids but are not acutely under the influence of them. It could suggest that opioids are used to alleviate difficult emotional states such as anger, and heighten users’ ability to tolerate social exclusion.
Cortisol and heart rate change were lower in the intoxicated user group, which is to be expected given cardiac depression following opioids (Vargish et al. 1987) and evidence that opioids can reduce cortisol responses to psychosocial stress (Bershad et al. 2015). Heart rate did not recover (reduce) over the duration of the experiment for the non-intoxicated user group; prior work has indicated the role of the endogenous opioid system in the recovery of the cardiovascular response to stress by reducing heart rate and cortisol (Morris et al. 1990). Heart rate has been linked with emotional and cognitive functions, where lower heart rate variability is related with poorer emotion regulation, higher alcohol craving (Ingjaldsson et al. 2003), and lower empathy (Lischke et al. 2018). Moreover, the groups did not differ in physiological responses to social exclusion and over the recovery period as we expected. A psychosocial stressor such as the Trier Social Stress Test (Kirschbaum et al. 1993) may produce more robust changes in cortisol and heartrate.
The current findings on empathy broadly concur with impairments observed in a previous study of opioid users by Kroll and colleagues (2018a) who also implemented the Multifaceted Empathy Test (MET); however they reported impairments in cognitive empathy (i.e. the ability to understand and identify the emotional states of others) among non-medically prescribed opioid users. The discrepancy between the two studies could be due to various differences between our samples: the sample tested by Kroll et al. excluded those with history of heroin abuse, and consequently may have experienced much lower levels of deprivation, poly-drug use and social adversity than our sample. One similarity between the two studies is specific impairment to positive emotions, which could suggest an overall negativity bias across the samples irrespective of socioeconomic or drug use background.
The study had limitations. Firstly, the intoxicated group had been prescribed OSM for more months than the non-intoxicated group; however, the months on OSM were not correlated with empathy, and the impairment in empathy was within the non-intoxicated group which suggests that this does not account for the key findings of the study. Secondly, the study did not measure symptoms of opioid withdrawal; however, this could have been linked with increased distress and anger following rejection in the non-intoxicated group. In addition, high rates of polysubstance and antidepressant use were reported in the opioid user groups, which could have biased the results. Nonetheless, the two opioid groups are well matched in drug use history which indicates a specific effect of opioid intoxication on emotional empathy and post-exclusion anger. The three groups were well matched in other variables, including loneliness and history of childhood adversity.
In summary, the current study provides both novel findings and supporting evidence for altered social functioning among opioid users. Blunted subjective anger in response to stress and lower cortisol and heart rate was observed in intoxicated users, which partially supports the notion that opioids could cause hyperalgesia to social pain. Impaired emotional empathy and increased rates of anger among opioid users who are not currently intoxicated with opioids may be due to an attentional bias toward negative expressions and poorer ability to tolerate difficult emotions, which is repaired by the use of opioids. With this knowledge, potential treatments for opioid use disorder should focus on heightening one’s ability to tolerate difficult social situations in a wider attempt to improve social skills, alongside emotion regulation training specifically aimed at reducing anger.
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Jamie Blackwell for assisting with the analysis of biological samples using the enzyme-linked immunosorbent assays.
Funding information
This study was funded by the University of Exeter.
Compliance with ethical standards
Conflicts of interest
CJA Morgan has consulted for Janssen, GlaxoSmithKline and Beckley canopy therapeutics. All other authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- Baddeley A, Emslie H, Nimmo-Smith I. The Spot-the-Word test: a robust estimate of verbal intelligence based on lexical decision. Br J Clin Psychol. 1993;32(Pt 1):55–65. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1993.tb01027.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S. The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. J Autism Dev Disord. 2004;34(2):163–175. doi: 10.1023/B:JADD.0000022607.19833.00. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barry CL, McGinty EE, Pescosolido BA, Goldman HH. Stigma, discrimination, treatment effectiveness, and policy: public views about drug addiction and mental illness. Psychiatr Serv. 2014;65(10):1269–1272. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.201400140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bass EC, Stednitz SJ, Simonson K, Shen T, Gahtan E. Physiological stress reactivity and empathy following social exclusion: a test of the defensive emotional analgesia hypothesis. Soc Neurosci. 2014;9(5):504–513. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2014.929533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bershad AK, Jaffe JH, Childs E, de Wit H. Opioid partial agonist buprenorphine dampens responses to psychosocial stress in humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2015;52:281–288. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bershad AK, Seiden JA, de Wit H. Effects of buprenorphine on responses to social stimuli in healthy adults. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2016;63:43–49. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blair RJR. Responding to the emotions of others: Dissociating forms of empathy through the study of typical and psychiatric populations. Conscious Cogn. 2005;14(4):698–718. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2005.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruehl S, Chung OY, Burns JW, Diedrich L. Trait anger expressiveness and pain-induced beta-endorphin release: support for the opioid dysfunction hypothesis. Pain. 2007;130(3):208–215. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2006.11.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruehl S, Burns JW, Chung OY, Quartana P. Anger management style and emotional reactivity to noxious stimuli among chronic pain patients and healthy controls: The role of endogenous opioids. Health Psychol. 2008;27(2):204–214. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.27.2.204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burns JW, Bruehl S, Chung OY, Magid E, Chont M, Goodlad JK, Gilliam W, Matsuura J, Somar K. Endogenous opioids may buffer effects of anger arousal on sensitivity to subsequent pain. Pain. 2009;146(3):276–282. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.07.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Compton P, Charuvastra V, Ling W. Pain intolerance in opioid-maintained former opiate addicts: effect of long-acting maintenance agent. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2001;63(2):139–146. doi: 10.1016/S0376-8716(00)00200-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy.
- Duncan, T. E., Duncan, S. C., & Strycker, L. A. (2013). An introduction to latent variable growth curve modeling: Concepts, issues, and application: Routledge.
- Dziobek I, Rogers K, Fleck S, Bahnemann M, Heekeren HR, Wolf OT, Convit A. Dissociation of cognitive and emotional empathy in adults with asperger syndrome using the multifaceted empathy test (MET) J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38(3):464–473. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0486-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eisenberger NI. Social pain and the brain: controversies, questions, and where to go from here. Annu Rev Psychol. 2015;66:601–629. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferrari V, Smeraldi E, Bottero G, Politi E. Addiction and empathy: a preliminary analysis. Neurol Sci. 2014;35(6):855–859. doi: 10.1007/s10072-013-1611-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foley KM. The treatment of cancer pain. N Engl J Med. 1985;313(2):84–95. doi: 10.1056/NEJM198507113130205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerra G, Somaini L, Manfredini M, Raggi M, Saracino M, Amore M, et al. Dysregulated responses to emotions among abstinent heroin users: Correlation with childhood neglect and addiction severity. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2014;48:220–228. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green MF, Horan WP, Lee J. Social cognition in schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16(10):620–631. doi: 10.1038/nrn4005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Handelsman L, Stein JA, Bernstein DP, Oppenheim SE, Rosenblum A, Magura S. A latent variable analysis of coexisting emotional deficits in substance abusers: alexithymia, hostility, and PTSD. Addict Behav. 2000;25(3):423–428. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4603(99)00010-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heffernan K, Cloitre M, Tardiff K, Marzuk PM, Portera L, Leon AC. Childhood trauma as a correlate of lifetime opiate use in psychiatric patients. Addict Behav. 2000;25(5):797–803. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4603(00)00066-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heilig M, Epstein DH, Nader MA, Shaham Y. Time to connect: bringing social context into addiction neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2016;17(9):592. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higgins, C., Smith, B., & Matthews, K. (2018). Evidence of opioid-induced hyperalgesia in clinical populations after chronic opioid exposure: a systematic review and meta-analysis. British Journal of Anaesthesia. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Hsu DT, Sanford BJ, Meyers KK, Love TM, Hazlett KE, Wang H, et al. Response of the mu-opioid system to social rejection and acceptance. Mol Psychiatry. 2013;18(11):1211–1217. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iannetti GD, Salomons TV, Moayedi M, Mouraux A, Davis KD. Beyond metaphor: contrasting mechanisms of social and physical pain. Trends Cogn Sci. 2013;17(8):371–378. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ingjaldsson JT, Laberg JC, Thayer JF. Reduced heart rate variability in chronic alcohol abuse: relationship with negative mood, chronic thought suppression, and compulsive drinking. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54(12):1427–1436. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01926-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Inturrisi CE. Clinical pharmacology of opioids for pain. Clin J Pain. 2002;18(4):S3–S13. doi: 10.1097/00002508-200207001-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanske P, Böckler A, Singer T. Models, Mechanisms and Moderators Dissociating Empathy and Theory of Mind. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2017;30:193. doi: 10.1007/7854_2015_412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kathryn McHugh R, Otto MW. Profiles of Distress Intolerance in a Substance-Dependent Sample. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2012;38(2):161–165. doi: 10.3109/00952990.2011.643990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kendler K, Ohlsson H, Sundquist K, Sundquist J. The causal nature of the association between neighborhood deprivation and drug abuse: a prospective national Swedish co-relative control study. Psychol Med. 2014;44(12):2537–2546. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713003048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirschbaum C, Pirke K-M, Hellhammer DH. The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’–a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology. 1993;28(1-2):76–81. doi: 10.1159/000119004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kroll S, Nikolic E, Bieri F, Soyka M, Baumgartner M, Quednow B (2018a) Cognitive and socio-cognitive functioning of chronic non-medical prescription opioid users. Psychopharmacology:1–14 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Kroll SL, Wunderli MD, Vonmoos M, Hulka LM, Preller KH, Bosch OG, Baumgartner MR, Quednow BB. Socio-cognitive functioning in stimulant polysubstance users. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2018;190:94–103. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kroll, S. L., Williams, D. P., Thoma, M., Staib, M., Binz, T. M., Baumgartner, M. R., … Quednow, B. B. (2019). Non-medical prescription opioid users exhibit dysfunctional physiological stress responses to social rejection. Psychoneuroendocrinology. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Lake S, Hayashi K, Milloy M-J, Wood E, Dong H, Montaner J, Kerr T. Associations between childhood trauma and non-fatal overdose among people who inject drugs. Addict Behav. 2015;43:83–88. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.12.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lischke A, Pahnke R, Mau-Moeller A, Behrens M, Grabe HJ, Freyberger HJ, et al. Inter-individual differences in heart rate variability are associated with inter-individual differences in empathy and alexithymia. Front Psychol. 2018;9:229. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macatee RJ, McDermott KA, Albanese BJ, Schmidt NB, Cougle JR. Distress Intolerance Moderation of Attention to Emotion: An Eye-Tracking Study. Cogn Ther Res. 2018;42(1):48–62. doi: 10.1007/s10608-017-9869-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- MacGregor S, Thickett A. Partnerships and communities in English drug policy: The challenge of deprivation. Int J Drug Policy. 2011;22(6):478–490. doi: 10.1016/j.drugpo.2011.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Machin AJ, Dunbar RI. The brain opioid theory of social attachment: a review of the evidence. Behaviour. 2011;148(9-10):985–1025. doi: 10.1163/000579511X596624. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mao J. Opioid-induced abnormal pain sensitivity: implications in clinical opioid therapy. Pain. 2002;100(3):213–217. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00422-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marion Lee M, Sanford Silverman M, Hans Hansen M, Vikram Patel M. A comprehensive review of opioid-induced hyperalgesia. Pain Physician. 2011;14:145–161. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maurage P, Grynberg D, Noel X, Joassin F, Philippot P, Hanak C, et al. Dissociation between affective and cognitive empathy in alcoholism: a specific deficit for the emotional dimension. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2011;35(9):1662–1668. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01512.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris M, Salmon P, Steinberg H, Sykes E, Bouloux P, Newbould E, McLoughlin L, Besser GM, Grossman A. Endogenous opioids modulate the cardiovascular response to mental stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1990;15(3):185–192. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(90)90029-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moustafa AA, Parkes D, Fitzgerald L, Underhill D, Garami J, Levy-Gigi E et al (2018) The relationship between childhood trauma, early-life stress, and alcohol and drug use, abuse, and addiction: An integrative review. Curr Psychol:1–6
- Muthén BO, Curran PJ. General longitudinal modeling of individual differences in experimental designs: A latent variable framework for analysis and power estimation. Psychol Methods. 1997;2(4):371. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.2.4.371. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Muthén B, Muthén LK. Integrating person-centered and variable-centered analyses: Growth mixture modeling with latent trajectory classes. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2000;24(6):882–891. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2000.tb02070.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naqavi MR, Mohammadi M, Salari V, Nakhaee N. The Relationship between Childhood Maltreatment and Opiate Dependency in Adolescence and Middle Age. Addict Health. 2011;3(3-4):92–98. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- National Institute on Drug Abuse. (2018). Opioid Overdose Crisis. Retrieved from https://www.drugabuse.gov/drugs-abuse/opioids/opioid-overdose-crisis
- Panksepp J, Panksepp JB. Toward a cross-species understanding of empathy. Trends Neurosci. 2013;36(8):489–496. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2013.04.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pearce E, Wlodarski R, Machin A, Dunbar RIM. Variation in the β-endorphin, oxytocin, and dopamine receptor genes is associated with different dimensions of human sociality. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2017;114(20):5300–5305. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1700712114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pool E, Sennwald V, Delplanque S, Brosch T, Sander D. Measuring wanting and liking from animals to humans: A systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;63:124–142. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preller KH, Hulka LM, Vonmoos M, Jenni D, Baumgartner MR, Seifritz E, Dziobek I, Quednow BB. Impaired emotional empathy and related social network deficits in cocaine users. Addict Biol. 2014;19(3):452–466. doi: 10.1111/adb.12070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pud D, Cohen D, Lawental E, Eisenberg E. Opioids and abnormal pain perception: New evidence from a study of chronic opioid addicts and healthy subjects. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2006;82(3):218–223. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Royal College of Anaesthetists. (2018). Dose Equivalent and Changing Opioids. Retrieved from https://www.rcoa.ac.uk/faculty-of-pain-medicine/opioids-aware/structured-approach-to-prescribing/dose-equivalents-and-changing-opioids
- Russell DW. UCLA Loneliness Scale (Version 3): reliability, validity, and factor structure. J Pers Assess. 1996;66(1):20–40. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6601_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rutgen M, Seidel EM, Silani G, Riecansky I, Hummer A, Windischberger C, et al. Placebo analgesia and its opioidergic regulation suggest that empathy for pain is grounded in self pain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2015;112(41):E5638–E5646. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1511269112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shabanloo S, Alimoradi M, Moazedian A (2018) Comparative study of alexithymia and anger dimensions in two groups with dependency to opioids and stimulants with the healthy counterparts. Journal of Fundamentals of Mental Health 20(5)
- Stange, K., Krüger, M., Janke, E., Lichtinghagen, R., Bleich, S., Hillemacher, T., & Heberlein, A. (2017). Positive association of personal distress with testosterone in opiate-addicted patients. Journal of Addictive Diseases, 1-8. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Stein DJ, van Honk J, Ipser J, Solms M, Panksepp J. Opioids: from physical pain to the pain of social isolation. CNS Spectrums. 2007;12(9):669–674. doi: 10.1017/S1092852900021490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomei A, Besson J, Reber N, Rougemont-Bücking A, Grivel J. Personal distress and empathic concern in methadone-maintained patients. J Subst Abus. 2017;22(1):37–41. [Google Scholar]
- United Nations of Office on Drugs and Crime. (2018). World Drug Report 2018. Retrieved from https://www.unodc.org/wdr2018/prelaunch/WDR18_Booklet_1_EXSUM.pdf
- Vargish T, Beamer K, Daly T, Riggs T. Morphine sulfate depression of cardiac function is attenuated by opiate receptor antagonism with naloxone. Circ Shock. 1987;23(3):189. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54(6):1063. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weathers, F., Blake, D., Schnurr, P., Kaloupek, D., Marx, B., & Keane, T. (2013). The life events checklist for DSM-5 (LEC-5). Instrument available from the National Center for PTSD at www. ptsd. va. gov.
- Wilcox, R. (2005). Trimming and winsorization. Encyclopedia of Biostatistics, 8.
- Willett, J., & Sayer, A. (1994). Using Covariance Structure Analysis to Detect Correlates and Predictors of Individual Change Over Time (Vol. 116).
- Williams KD, Govan CL, Croker V, Tynan D, Cruickshank M, Lam A. Investigations into differences between social-and cyberostracism. Group Dyn Theory Res Pract. 2002;6(1):65. doi: 10.1037/1089-2699.6.1.65. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Williams, K. S., Yeager, D. S., Cheung, C. K. T., & Choi, W. (2012). Cyberball (version 4.0) [Software]. Available from https://cyberball.wikispaces.com.
- Wolff S, Holl J, Stopsack M, Arens EA, Höcker A, Staben KA, et al. Does emotion dysregulation mediate the relationship between early maltreatment and later substance dependence? Findings of the CANSAS Study. Eur Addict Res. 2016;22(6):292–300. doi: 10.1159/000447397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou Y, Zhu H, Jin X, Li X, Zhang M, Zhang F, Shen M. Biased attention towards negative schematic expression in abstinent heroin abusers. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2012;43(2):705–710. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.