Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: Soc Sci Res. 2019 Nov 23;86:102396. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2019.102396

IS GEOGRAPHY DESTINY? DISRUPTING THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN SEGREGATION AND NEIGHBOHROOD OUTCOMES

Christine Leibbrand 1, Ryan Gabriel 2, Chris Hess 3, Kyle Crowder 4
PMCID: PMC7024700  NIHMSID: NIHMS1545681  PMID: 32056562

Abstract

Considerable research has shown that, in the cross-section, segregation is associated with detrimental neighborhood outcomes for blacks and improved neighborhood outcomes for whites. However, it is unclear whether early-life experiences of segregation shape later-life neighborhood outcomes, whether this association persists for those who migrate out of the metropolitan areas in which they grew up, and how these relationships differ for blacks and whites. Using the Panel Study of Income Dynamics from 1979-2013, we find that the level of segregation experienced during adolescence is associated with significantly worse neighborhood outcomes in adulthood for blacks. However, migrating out of the metropolitan area an individual grew up in substantially moderates these relationships. In contrast, adolescent segregation is associated with improved, or not significantly different, neighborhood outcomes in adulthood for whites. These findings have important implications for theorizing about the mechanisms linking segregation and neighborhood outcomes and for considering potential means of assuaging racial disparities in harmful neighborhood exposures.

Keywords: Segregation, neighborhood attainment, life course, racial stratification, gender

Introduction

Since the publication of Massey and Denton’s American Apartheid in 1993, there has been growing attention to, and awareness of, the detrimental effects of racial residential segregation on blacks’ wellbeing. Metropolitan-level segregation tends to concentrate poverty and its resultant disadvantages, including higher levels of crime, single motherhood, welfare receipt, and high school dropout in particular neighborhoods and clusters of neighborhoods, frequently those that contain high proportions of blacks (Bruch 2014; Massey and Denton 1993; Quillian 2012). In doing so, segregation creates swaths of disadvantage and inequality within metropolitan areas and is associated with deleterious individual and neighborhood outcomes for blacks (Bruch 2014; Charles 2003; Massey and Denton 1993; Mouw 2000; Quillian 2012; Sharkey 2013; Sharkey and Faber 2014; Squires and Kubrin 2005; Subramanian et al. 2005; Wagmiller 2007; Williams and Collins 2001). By concentrating disadvantage in the areas in which blacks live, segregation can also improve whites’ outcomes by concentrating advantage in predominantly white residential spaces (Flippen 2010; South and Crowder 1997; South et al. 2011; St. John 2002). As such, segregation frequently corresponds with higher levels of inequality across racial groups in terms of both residential context and individual wellbeing.

However, very few studies have explored whether early-life experiences of metropolitan-level segregation follow individuals throughout their life course and impact their long-term outcomes, especially their long-term neighborhood outcomes. The growing literature on the ways in which early-life experiences influence later-life outcomes suggests theoretical reasons for expecting that segregation is a strong determinant of residential outcomes later in life (Chetty et al. 2016; Dannefer 2003; Elder et al. 2003; Sharkey and Elwert 2011; Sharkey 2013; Wodtke et al. 2011). Moreover, one of the few studies that has explored the relationship between childhood exposure to segregation and later-life outcomes, found that childhood segregation was associated with worse neighborhood outcomes in adulthood for blacks (Pais 2017). Yet, no research has investigated these relationships for migrants, so it is unknown whether early-life exposure to metropolitan-level segregation continues to shape neighborhood outcomes for blacks who move out of the segregated metropolitan areas of their youth or whether early-life experiences of segregation are primarily detrimental for blacks who stay in the metropolitan areas they grew up in.

This is an important investigation given recent findings by Leibbrand and Crowder (2018) that moving out of the metropolitan areas, states, and/or regions in which individuals grew up corresponds to significant reductions in blacks’ exposure to neighborhood poverty in adulthood, relative to what they experienced in adolescence. These findings indicate that migration substantially disrupts early-life, local neighborhood contexts and their associations with blacks’ later-life outcomes. However, the Leibbrand and Crowder study solely focused on neighborhood poverty, rather than segregation, we therefore have no knowledge of whether migration would disrupt segregation’s broader and, as noted above, pervasive associations with blacks’ outcomes. Indeed, segregation influences individuals’ access to opportunities and disadvantage in ways that extend beyond one’s local neighborhood and that can structure individuals’ lives and outcomes in uniquely comprehensive ways. As a result, the influence of segregation may endure across time and place, though we do not know whether this is the case. Conversely, it is unclear whether any benefits associated with segregation for whites follow them throughout their life course, or whether these benefits are interrupted by the decision to move to a different metropolitan area.

To explore these gaps in our understanding, we use data from the Panel Study of Income Dynamics (PSID) linked to U.S. Census data to examine the associations between metropolitan-level segregation experienced during adolescence and two primary neighborhood outcomes in adulthood: poverty rates and the percentage of black residents. Investigating these two neighborhood outcomes has the benefit of aligning our study with research that assesses racial disparities in the association between segregation and neighborhood socioeconomic and racial composition outcomes, while providing a holistic picture of how segregation shapes neighborhood outcomes in diverse ways (Charles 2003; Massey and Denton 1993; Quillian 2012; South and Crowder 1997, 1998; South, Pais, and Crowder 2011). Additionally, we assess whether migrating out of the metropolitan area an individual grew up in moderates these relationships. We conduct separate models by race because solely attending to these relationships for blacks may underestimate the extent to which adolescent exposure to segregation increases racial disparities in neighborhood outcomes in adulthood if segregation simultaneously improves whites’ outcomes. Lastly, we examine the robustness of our results to other neighborhood outcomes and to moves of varying distances.

Our investigation contributes to the literature on racial stratification by highlighting the persistent and enduring ways segregation is associated with racial disparities in neighborhood socioeconomic status and racial and ethnic composition. As such, it demonstrates that the value of combatting segregation may be underestimated without accounting for the cumulative impact segregation has on individuals’ outcomes throughout their life course. Examining these relationships across migration status is also important because it indicates possible mechanisms through which segregation could harm the life chances of blacks and help those of whites, while shedding light on potential means for ameliorating racial disparities in outcomes.

Background

The Potential Influence of Early-Life Segregation on Later-Life Outcomes

Socioeconomic Mechanisms

While there is considerable evidence demonstrating that living in segregated metropolitan areas is harmful for blacks, few studies have explored whether the effects of segregation are persistent over the life course.1 The large body of research on segregation strongly suggests it may have long-term impacts on black households and especially on black adolescents. For instance, researchers have demonstrated that the isolated black neighborhoods that characterize segregated metropolitan areas exhibit higher levels of high school dropout and lower standardized test scores and school quality (Krivo and Peterson 1996; Massey and Denton 1993; Orfield and Lee 2005; Rumberger and Palardy 2005; Saporito 2003; Sharkey 2013; Sharkey and Faber 2014; Squires and Kubrin 2005). Likewise, disadvantaged neighborhood contexts have important, deleterious effects on socioeconomic status, particularly influencing adolescents’ patterns of educational attainment (Galster and Sharkey 2017).

Additionally, segregation is associated with considerably fewer grocery stores in black residential areas relative to white residential areas (Morland et al. 2002), greater exposure to pollution (Crowder and Downey 2010; Houston et al. 2004; Kravitz-Wirtz et al. 2016; Woo et al 2018), and lower access to medical care facilities and doctors (Hayanga et al. 2009), all of which may have particularly negative effects for developing adolescents. In sum, the findings from these studies suggest that segregation has detrimental associations with young blacks’ socioeconomic and health outcomes, which may, in turn, limit their formation of human capital and their future abilities to move into and/or stay in advantaged residential areas.

Conversely, early-life exposure to segregation may improve whites’ later-life outcomes by increasing the probability that whites live in non-poor and higher-income neighborhoods (South and Crowder 1997). Because higher-income neighborhoods frequently claim higher-quality schools and lower levels of crime, among other advantages, whites in segregated metropolitan areas may experience better long-term socioeconomic outcomes, resulting in improved abilities to move into and stay in advantaged neighborhoods in adulthood (Kane et al. 2006; Leventhal and Brooks-Gunn 2000; Neckerman et al. 2009).

The Social Structural Sorting Perspective

Early-life experiences of segregation may also be associated with later-life neighborhood outcomes by shaping individuals’ knowledge of, and information about, potential neighborhoods to move to in adulthood. This possibility is outlined in the social structural sorting perspective, which suggests that neighborhood outcomes are shaped, not only by economics, discrimination, and preferences—explanations frequently given for racial disparities in neighborhood attainment—but also by racial/ethnic differences in the housing search process which is, itself, a product of social processes (Krysan and Crowder 2017). Indeed, individuals engage in routine activities such as grocery shopping and going to work, church, or the doctor in similarly disadvantaged neighborhoods to the ones they live in, illustrating that even when people leave their home neighborhoods, they tend to travel in similar and likely proximate environmental contexts (Krivo et al. 2013). Segregation can exacerbate the inequitable effects of this process by grouping black communities in space so that segregated, disadvantaged communities are frequently surrounded by other segregated, disadvantaged communities, making it less likely that blacks living in these communities are exposed to advantaged neighborhoods (Massey and Denton 1993). In contrast, by concentrating whites within clusters of relatively advantaged neighborhoods, segregation may increase whites’ knowledge about, and access to, residential opportunities in advantaged areas. Thus, the social structural sorting perspective anticipates that segregation will be associated with persistently better neighborhood outcomes for whites and persistently worse neighborhood outcomes for blacks because it will shape the neighborhood knowledge both groups draw on when they choose places to live.

This process is likely to be especially important during adolescence because this is the period of time when individuals develop coherent life stories that inform their expectations about their future (Bohn and Berntsen 2013; Habermas and Bluck 2000; Johnson et al. 2011). As a result, the lack of exposure to different communities may constrain individuals’ knowledge of, and abilities to envision and aspire to, different types of neighborhoods than the ones in which they grew up, even if they possess sufficient economic resources to move elsewhere. This lack of information would tend to hurt blacks’ neighborhood outcomes, because they are more likely to originate in disadvantaged areas relative to whites, while enhancing whites’ knowledge of advantaged residential spaces.

Further, early-life segregation may be associated with later-life neighborhood outcomes by geographically constraining kith and kin networks. Spring, Ackert, and Crowder (2017) observed that low-income adult children are particularly likely to stay in and move to neighborhoods in which their kin live. Because segregation often concentrates blacks in relatively small geographic areas, it may limit the distance black migrants can move without finding themselves distant from family and friends. Hence, even blacks who have the means to move to more advantaged areas might forego the opportunity so they can live near kith and kin (Massey and Denton 1993). Conversely, because whites are more likely to originate in advantaged areas, they will be more likely to remain near kith and kin and reside in advantaged neighborhoods. Adolescents’ residential contexts are expected to be more closely tied to their kith and kin networks than the residential contexts of adults, given that adolescents exhibit high likelihoods of living with family and given the local, school-based nature of adolescents’ peer networks (Arnett 2000). Thus, it is valuable to study the role of adolescents’ residential contexts for shaping their adulthood outcomes because it is these early-life contexts that are likely to be especially impactful for shaping the social-based factors that inform where individuals will move to and live in adulthood.

Does Migration Moderate the Influence of Segregation?

The influence of segregation on human capital formation, individuals’ information about potential neighborhoods, and the interaction of segregation with individuals’ preferences to live near kith and kin indicate that it is likely that early-life experiences of segregation correspond to later-life neighborhood outcomes. Yet, it is unclear whether this would be the case both for individuals who stay in the metropolitan areas they grew up in and for those who leave. While the research by Leibbrand and Crowder (2018) suggests that migration can disrupt the persistence of harmful neighborhood environments across the life course, segregation’s uniquely comprehensive associations with individuals’ outcomes and access to opportunities suggests that it is by no means a foregone conclusion that moving could disrupt the long-term disadvantaging influence of growing up in a highly segregated metropolitan area.

Examining the extent to which migration moderates the association between adolescent exposure to segregation and adulthood neighborhood outcomes offers numerous insights into the plausibility of potential mechanisms linking segregation and life-course outcomes. The mechanisms explored in the previous section suggest somewhat different expectations about whether and why migrating out of one’s adolescent metropolitan area of residence might moderate the relationship between adolescent segregation and adulthood neighborhood outcomes. For example, adolescent experiences of segregation may be associated with adulthood neighborhood attainment outcomes by influencing human capital formation and individuals’ resulting abilities to move to, and stay in, more advantaged neighborhoods later in life (Charles 2003; Massey and Denton 1993; Massey et al. 1987; Wagmiller 2007; Williams and Collins 2001). However, internal migration tends to be associated with improvements in individuals’ economic wellbeing, in part, by exposing migrants to new social and economic opportunities (Clark and Withers 2002; Cooke et al. 2009; Knapp et al. 2013). Inter-metropolitan migration may therefore moderate the relationship between adolescent exposure to segregation and adulthood neighborhood outcomes by facilitating upward socioeconomic mobility. If this is the case, accounting for adulthood socioeconomic status will partially or entirely explain away the relationship between adolescent segregation and adulthood neighborhood outcomes and the moderating effect of migration.

If factors emphasized in the social structural sorting perspective are important for explaining the relationship between adolescent segregation and adulthood neighborhood outcomes, then accounting for individuals’ socioeconomic status will not explain away this relationship. Further, migrating may substantially moderate the relationship between adolescent segregation and adulthood neighborhood outcomes because individuals will not be able to rely on their past residential experiences to decide where to live. As such, migrants may be more likely to explore areas that differ from those they grew up in. This could correspond to mobility into whiter and wealthier neighborhoods for blacks and mobility into more economically and racially diverse neighborhoods for whites. Finally, migrating out of the metropolitan area an individual grew up in could disrupt peer networks and kinship ties that would otherwise keep blacks in segregated, disadvantaged neighborhoods and keep whites in advantaged neighborhoods.

We have little knowledge of which of these mechanisms may predominate or the role migration might play in moderating the relationship between adolescent exposure to segregation and adulthood neighborhood outcomes. It is important to examine these relationships because if migrating out of the metropolitan area an individual grew up in does not moderate the association between adolescent exposure to segregation and adulthood neighborhood outcomes, then it would indicate that early-life experiences with segregation have enduring negative influences on blacks’ outcomes and durable positive influences on whites’ outcomes that are persistent across time and space. In contrast, if migrating out of the metropolitan area an individual grew up in does moderate the association between adolescent segregation and neighborhood attainment in adulthood, it would suggest that migration may be an important means of disrupting the pernicious and enduring influence of segregation on blacks’ outcomes. Investigating these possibilities also allows us to provide important, suggestive evidence for which mechanisms might be predominating in the relationship between segregation and neighborhood attainment for blacks and whites.

Hypotheses

Following these theoretical arguments, our study seeks to test the following hypotheses:

  1. Metropolitan-level segregation in adolescence will be associated with residence in more disadvantaged and racially isolated neighborhoods in adulthood for blacks, even after accounting for adulthood socioeconomic status.

  2. Migrating out of the metropolitan area an individual grew up in will moderate the negative association of adolescent segregation with adulthood neighborhood outcomes for blacks.
    1. Adulthood socioeconomic status will partially explain the moderating effect of inter-metropolitan migration. However, given our expectation that the social mechanisms outlined by the social structural sorting perspective are also at work, this moderating effect will remain robust to the inclusion of adulthood socioeconomic characteristics.
  3. Adolescent segregation will be associated with residence in more advantaged, predominantly white neighborhoods for whites, even after accounting for adulthood socioeconomic status.

  4. Once adulthood socioeconomic status is accounted for, migrating out of the metropolitan area an individual grew up in may moderate the relationship between adolescent exposure to segregation and adulthood neighborhood outcomes for whites. This could be the case if migrating substantially disrupts the localized nature of individuals’ neighborhood knowledge, perhaps prompting them to explore neighborhoods that are more economically and racially diverse.

Data and Methods

Sample Selection

For this analysis, we use the PSID from 1979 to 2013. The PSID is a nationally-representative, longitudinal survey that began surveying 18,000 members of approximately 4,800 families in 1968. Since that time, it has continued to survey the majority of those original family members and has followed the children of those original families as they break off to form their own households. The PSID is an advantageous survey to use for this analysis because it is one of the few data sources that allows us to examine adolescent experiences of segregation before individuals move out to form their own households. As such, we can explore the association between early-life experiences of segregation and adulthood outcomes in a longitudinal framework. Moreover, the PSID provides information about the geographic location of households, including the metropolitan areas and census tracts they live in. Because of this, we are able to study inter-metropolitan segregation and migration and their relationships with the characteristics of individuals’ census tracts.

We restrict our analysis to interview years after 1979 because these are the years in which key variables, including employment status, are consistently available for single and partnered males and females. Because members of households tend to move together, we focus on one family member per household: either the household head or, in the case of partnered women (who are rarely considered household heads), the female spouse of the household head. The inclusion of multiple household members would lead to multiple moves being counted when only one household move is actually occurring.

We segment our data into a series of person-period observations that represent either the one-year or two-year intervals between PSID interviews (the PSID conducted annual interviews until 1997, after which it switched to biennial interviews). In total, our analysis includes 18,846 person-periods for blacks and 24,438 person-periods for whites. Utilizing person-periods allows us to examine residential outcomes at multiple points in time during adulthood and to explore how these adulthood residential outcomes may be dependent on exposure to segregation in adolescence as well as on contemporary, adulthood characteristics.

Outcomes

The outcomes of our analysis are represented by (1) the poverty rate of the neighborhood that the respondent resides in at the end of the observation period and (2) the percentage of black residents in the neighborhood that the respondent resides in at the end of the observation period. Both of our focal outcomes capture important elements of neighborhood quality. The neighborhood poverty rate reflects the level of socioeconomic disadvantage individuals are exposed to. As Massey and Denton (1993) illustrate, neighborhood poverty is associated with numerous deleterious outcomes and other components of neighborhood disadvantage, such as high school dropout rates, the prevalence of single-motherhood, and crime rates. The percentage of black residents in a neighborhood is frequently related to a neighborhood’s socioeconomic status, but it is distinct from it as well, capturing the level of racial isolation blacks experience in their residential contexts.2 This isolation may limit individuals’ knowledge of more integrated neighborhoods to move to and tends to correspond to fewer community resources such as grocery stores and worse individual educational, health, and economic outcomes (Chang et al. 2009; Johnson 2010; Leventhal and Brooks-Gunn 2000; Mason et al. 2009; Mendenhall et al. 2006; Zenk et al. 2005). We also examined the percentage of white residents in the neighborhood that the respondent resides in at the end of the observation period, but the results were almost the exact inverse of the results for the percentage of black residents and yielded no additional substantive information (results available upon request). Given that, we focus on the percentage of black residents because this is an important outcome in the neighborhood attainment literature and one frequently used to capture neighborhood-level racial isolation (Clark 2007; Clark 2008; Crowder et al. 2012; South and Crowder 1998). We also examine the robustness of our results to other outcomes including the percentage of Hispanic residents in the neighborhood and the logged average household income of the neighborhood.

The PSID’s restricted-access Geospatial Match Files allow us to identify each respondent’s census tract of residence, which we use to define an individual’s neighborhood. Although the census tract is an imperfect definition of a neighborhood, census tracts approximate the concept of neighborhoods (Jargowsky 1997; Lee et al. 2008; White 1987). Utilizing this definition also allows us to be consistent with previous research (e.g. Quillian 2002) and to capitalize on tract-level information, such as poverty rates and racial composition provided by the U.S. Census. Because tract boundaries often change between censuses, we utilize the Neighborhood Change Database (NCDB) to normalize tract boundaries between 1980 and 2010 to 2010 census boundaries (GeoLytics 2013). Additionally, we employ linear interpolation/extrapolation to estimate tract-level and metropolitan-level characteristics for noncensus years (see Bruch 2014).

Focal Independent Variables

Our focal independent variables include the dissimilarity index for the metropolitan area the respondent lived in at age 17. The dissimilarity index measures the evenness of the racial distribution across census tracts in a metropolitan area. Specifically, dissimilarity measures the percentage of blacks that would need to migrate to different neighborhoods for each neighborhood to have the same percentage of blacks as the metropolitan area overall, where a value of 0 represents no segregation and a value of 1 signifies complete segregation.

We measure metropolitan-level segregation at age 17 because residential location at this age is relevant to a number of important developmental experiences. For example, in comparison to subsequent ages, individuals at age 17 are more likely to live with family and near childhood friends. Because individuals often live near kith and kin (Spring et al. 2017) and segregation tends to contain kin networks within circumscribed locations, the metropolitan area of residence at 17-years-old is likely to have a particularly important influence on individuals’ future decisions of where to live. Moreover, individuals’ residential locations at age 17, directly before they formally enter adulthood, may be particularly informative for informing their adulthood decisions of where to live and start their own households than residential locations at earlier ages. Finally, Ihrke and Faber (2012) find that only 10.1 percent of 10-17-year-olds moved out of their counties of residence during a five-year period, and this percentage would be lower for generally longer-distance inter-metropolitan moves. Thus, metropolitan residence at age 17 captures the segregation levels the vast majority of individuals experience during much of their adolescence and childhood.

Our other focal independent variable is a dummy variable representing whether the respondent had, by the beginning of the observation period, moved out of the metropolitan area they resided in at age 17. This variable takes a value of 1 if the respondent is living in a different metropolitan area from the one they resided in at age 17 during the observation period and takes a value of 0 if the respondent is living in the same metropolitan area as the one they resided in at age 17 during the observation period. These inter-metropolitan moves can occur at any time period and age when we observe individuals after they have formed their own households. We also examine the robustness of our results to moves of other types and distances, as described in the supplementary analysis section below.

Covariates

Numerous individual-, household-, and metropolitan-level characteristics are likely to influence our outcomes and/or our focal independent variables and, as a result, confound the relationships between them. Consequently, we control for a variety of characteristics in our multivariate regressions, including whether the head of the household is employed, the educational attainment of the respondent (in years), and the taxable family income of the household (measured in constant 2010 dollars) in order to account for the possibility that the respondent’s adulthood socioeconomic status explains the association between adolescent segregation and adulthood neighborhood outcomes.3 Including measures of adulthood socioeconomic status further allows us to assess whether migration moderates the relationship between adolescent segregation and adulthood neighborhood outcomes because of its association with upward socioeconomic mobility in adulthood, or whether the moderating effect of migration is persistent across models and therefore potentially reflective of its role in lifting the restrictions segregation places on individuals’ neighborhood knowledge and in disrupting kith and kin networks.

We control for the age of the respondent, marital status, and the number of children in the household because these characteristics are all negatively associated with migration probabilities, though older and married individuals may possess greater levels of socioeconomic resources that can be utilized to move to advantaged neighborhoods. Additionally, we hold constant housing characteristics including, persons per room (to account for crowding), whether the respondent is a homeowner, and whether the respondent has lived in the same house for 3 or more years. The former housing characteristic implies that families’ housing resources are strained and that families may be more predisposed to move, whereas the latter two characteristics suggest that families may be satisfied with where they are living or may, at least, be more constrained in their ability to move to better neighborhoods. Moreover, we include controls for the year of observation and the number of years that have elapsed between surveys (1 or 2 years to account for the PSID’s switch to a biennial survey in 1997).

Housing availability, the population composition of the metropolitan area, and the overall poverty level of the metropolitan area may shape individuals’ neighborhood options, the opportunity structures to which they are exposed, and resultant neighborhood location. Thus, we control for the proportion of housing in the metropolitan area that was built in the last 10 years, the proportion of vacant housing in the metropolitan area, the proportion of the metropolitan population that is non-Hispanic black, the proportion of the population that is Hispanic, and the proportion of the metropolitan area that is in poverty. Finally, we control for the region the respondent lives in (Northeast (ref.), North Central, West, or South) to account for regional variations in opportunity structures.

With the exception of the number of years that have elapsed between surveys, all control variables are measured at the beginning of the observation period in order to ensure that the covariates are not, themselves, influenced by moves made by the respondent and all are measured as time-varying at level-1 in our multilevel models.

Analytic Strategy

For this analysis, we use linear mixed-effects models (using Stata 14’s mixed command) with individual- and metropolitan-level random intercepts. Including individual- and metropolitan-level random intercepts allows us to address the non-independence of including multiple observations for a given individual. This means that person-period observations are nested within individuals and individuals are nested within metropolitan areas. To account for heteroskedasticity in model errors, we also utilize robust standard errors.

To examine the potentially moderating influence of inter-metropolitan migration, we conduct interactions between the level of dissimilarity a respondent experiences at age 17 and a dummy variable indicating whether the respondent had moved out of the metropolitan area they grew up in by the observation year in question. To facilitate the interpretability of the results, we present tables of the results along with graphs of each of the predicted outcomes by the level of dissimilarity experienced at age 17. Utilizing these graphs provides a clearer illustration of how segregation experienced during adolescence is associated with neighborhood attainment in adulthood as well as how inter-metropolitan migration moderates these relationships. We do not present corresponding figures for whites because inter-metropolitan migration does not moderate these relationships for whites; hence, the graphs are not substantively informative.

Results

We first highlight the descriptive results in Table 1, which are disaggregated by race and migration status. These results provide us with an initial indication of whether we observe race and migration-related disparities in neighborhood outcomes and metropolitan-level segregation.

Table 1:

Descriptive characteristics by migration status and gender: Panel Study of Income Dynamics, 1979-2013

Black Non-Migrants
Mean / SD
Black Migrants
Mean / SD
White Non-Migrants
Mean / SD
White Migrants
Mean / SD
Outcome and Focal Ind. Variable
  Neighborhood Poverty Rate 0.258 (0.146) 0.173 (0.127) 0.098 (0.082) 0.088 (0.081)
  Neighborhood Percent Black 0.658 (0.309) 0.363 (0.312) 0.064 (0.117) 0.069 (0.105)
  Neighborhood Percent Hispanic 0.064 (0.123) 0.132 (0.170) 0.076 (0.138) 0.084 (0.110)
  Logged Avg. Neighborhood Income 10.213 (0.545) 10.495 (0.550) 10.641 (0.551) 10.847 (0.554)
  Dissimilarity at age 17 0.719 (0.110) 0.708 (0.117) 0.699 (0.119) 0.700 (0.123)
Covariates
  Female 0.611 (0.488) 0.512 (0.500) 0.520 (0.500) 0.500 (0.500)
  Married 0.375 (0.484) 0.445 (0.497) 0.654 (0.476) 0.674 (0.469
  Number of children in family, origin 1.328 (1.278) 1.113 (1.254) 0.995 (1.137) 0.884 (1.098)
  Age of respondent, origin 30.765 (7.730) 31.835 (8.076) 30.564 (7.569) 32.326 (7.818)
  Years of education, origin 12.169 (1.920) 13.160 (2.349) 13.120 (2.332) 14.805 (2.705)
  Family income, origin 29.196 (26.123) 40.552 (35.420) 56.576 (56.730) 72.243 (83.989)
  HH head employed, origin 0.686 (0.464) 0.745 (0.436) 0.820 (0.384) 0.841 (0.366)
  Homeowner, origin 0.221 (0.415) 0.238 (0.426) 0.523 (0.499) 0.524 (0.499)
  Persons per room, origin 0.708 (0.423) 0.653 (0.423) 0.568 (0.311) 0.509 (0.257)
  Lived in same house 3+ years, origin 0.335 (0.472) 0.258 (0.438) 0.398 (0.489) 0.339 (0.473)
  Observation length 1.314 (0.464) 1.398 (0.490) 1.333 (0.471) 1.413 (0.492)
  Year of observation 1993.340 (9.316) 1995.287 (9.583) 1993.878 (9.139) 1995.695 (9.043)
  Prop. new housing, origin metro 0.215 (0.087) 0.215 (0.093) 0.182 (0.088) 0.198 (0.095)
  Prop. vacant housing, origin metro 0.078 (0.023) 0.078 (0.029) 0.072 (0.025) 0.078 (0.042)
  Prop. in poverty, origin metro 0.124 (0.032) 0.123 (0.036) 0.115 (0.030) 0.115 (0.028)
  Prop. NH black, origin metro 0.224 (0.095) 0.175 (0.099) 0.117 (0.090) 0.115 (0.078)
  Prop. Hispanic, origin metro 0.067 (0.088) 0.116 (0.124) 0.084 (0.107) 0.120 (0.109)

N 16,395 2,451 16,768 7,671

Indeed, blacks live in substantially more impoverished, racially isolated neighborhoods than whites, though these gaps are smaller for migrants than for non-migrants. Moreover, blacks and whites exhibit similar average exposures to segregation in adolescence (as measured by the dissimilarity index), though blacks are considerably less likely than whites to live in different metropolitan areas from the ones they grew up in. In fact, while 31.39 percent of whites live in different metropolitan areas from the ones they grew up in, only 13.01 percent of blacks do so, indicating that blacks may be particularly stuck and/or tied to the residential contexts of their youth. This may be a result of blacks’ lower average socioeconomic status. Table 1 illustrates that black respondents have lower average levels of education and family income and are less likely to be employed, suggesting they may be less able to afford an inter-metropolitan move. This possibility is further supported by the fact that both black and white migrants in Table 1 are more socioeconomically advantaged, on average, than black and white non-migrants.

These initial descriptive results suggest that blacks tend to live in more disadvantaged neighborhoods than whites and are less likely to move from the residential contexts of their youth. Both of these descriptive findings may be due to the lower average socioeconomic status claimed by black respondents. It is unclear whether inter-metropolitan migration moderates these racial disparities in neighborhood attainment, particularly after individual and household socioeconomic characteristics are accounted for. Accordingly, we turn to Table 2, which illustrates the multivariate results from the linear mixed effects models examining the relationships between inter-metropolitan migration, adolescent segregation (as measured using the dissimilarity index4), and neighborhood poverty rates by race.

Table 2:

The relationship between adolescent segregation, migration, and neighborhood poverty rates by gender: Panel Study of Income Dynamics, 1979-2013

Model 1 (Black) Model 2 (Black) Model 3 (Black) Model 4 (Black) Model 5 (White) Model 6 (White) Model 7 (White) Model 8 (White)
Focal Independent Variables
 Dissimilarity at 17, origin 0.153*** (0.033) 0.120*** (0.035) 0.095** (0.035) 0.181*** (0.037) −0.076*** (0.015) −0.043** (0.015) −0.025+ (0.013) −0.035+ (0.021)
Covariates
 Female 0.004 (0.005) 0.005 (0.005) 0.003 (0.005) −0.004+ (0.002) −0.003+ (0.002) −0.003+ (0.002)
 Married, origin −0.016*** (0.004) −0.017*** (0.004) −0.016*** (0.004) −0.009*** (0.002) −0.009*** (0.002) −0.009*** (0.002)
 Number of children in family, origin 0.002 (0.002) 0.002 (0.002) 0.002 (0.002) −0.001 (0.001) −0.001 (0.001) −0.001 (0.001)
 Age of respondent, origin −0.001 (0.000) −0.000 (0.000) −0.000 (0.000) −0.001** (0.000) −0.001*** (0.000) −0.001*** (0.000)
 Years of education, origin −0.008*** (0.001) −0.008*** (0.001) −0.007*** (0.001) −0.002*** (0.000) −0.002*** (0.000) −0.002*** (0.000)
 Family income, origin −0.000*** (0.000) −0.000*** (0.000) −0.000*** (0.000) −0.000** (0.000) −0.000** (0.000) −0.000** (0.000)
 HH head employed, origin −0.012*** (0.002) −0.012*** (0.002) −0.012*** (0.002) −0.001 (0.001) −0.001 (0.001) −0.001 (0.001)
 Homeowner, origin −0.014** (0.005) −0.013** (0.005) −0.014** (0.005) −0.008*** (0.002) −0.007*** (0.002) −0.007*** (0.002)
 Persons per room, origin 0.005+ (0.002) 0.005* (0.002) 0.005* (0.002) −0.000 (0.002) −0.000 (0.002) −0.000 (0.002)
 Lived in same house 3+ years, origin 0.005* (0.002) 0.007** (0.002) 0.006* (0.002) 0.003** (0.001) 0.004*** (0.001) 0.004*** (0.001)
 Observation length −0.021*** (0.006) −0.010+ (0.005) −0.011* (0.005) −0.005* (0.002) −0.000 (0.002) −0.000 (0.002)
 Year of observation, destination 0.000 (0.001) −0.001 (0.001) −0.000 (0.001) 0.001*** (0.000) 0.000* (0.000) 0.000+ (0.000)
 Prop. new housing, origin metro −0.154 (0.118) −0.145 (0.111) −0.021 (0.022) −0.022 (0.022)
 Prop. vacant housing, origin metro 0.260+ (0.158) 0.210 (0.152) 0.108+ (0.059) 0.108+ (0.059)
 Prop. in poverty, origin metro 1.271*** (0.199) 1.284*** (0.182) 0.749*** (0.084) 0.747*** (0.084)
 Prop. NH black, origin metro −0.145 (0.092) −0.167* (0.078) −0.110*** (0.026) −0.109*** (0.026)
 Prop. Hispanic, origin metro −0.137 (0.085) −0.135+ (0.070) −0.053* (0.026) −0.053* (0.026)
 Northeast Ref. Ref. Ref. Ref.
 Midwest 0.004 (0.016) −0.015 (0.013) 0.009 (0.008) 0.009 (0.008)
 South −0.034** (0.012) −0.031** (0.011) 0.013+ (0.008) 0.013+ (0.008)
 West −0.045* (0.018) −0.030* (0.014) 0.016* (0.007) 0.016* (0.007)
Mobility Interactions
 Living in different metro from age 17 0.061+ (0.035) −0.013 (0.018)
 Different metro*dissimilarity at 17 −0.176*** (0.052) 0.018 (0.026)
 Constant 0.105*** (0.021) −0.577 (1.093) 1.730 (1.571) 1.003 (1.524) 0.162*** (0.011) −1.634*** (0.379) −0.873+ (0.476) −0.823+ (0.483)
Variance Components
 Between MSAs 0.045*** (0.005) 0.041*** (0.005) 0.037*** (0.006) 0.026*** (0.006) 0.036*** (0.005) 0.035*** (0.005) 0.014*** (0.002) 0.014*** (0.002)
 Between Individuals 0.100*** (0.004) 0.090*** (0.004) 0.090*** (0.004) 0.088*** (0.003) 0.064*** (0.002) 0.061*** (0.002) 0.061*** (0.002) 0.061*** (0.002)
 Residual 0.097*** (0.004) 0.096*** (0.004) 0.095*** (0.003) 0.094*** (0.003) 0.048*** (0.001) 0.047*** (0.001) 0.047*** (0.001) 0.047*** (0.001)

Observations 18846 18846 18846 18846 24439 24439 24439 24439
BIC −29718.216 −30350.846 −30580.032 −30808.710 −71299.006 −71626.603 −71954.513 −71936.182

Standard errors in parentheses

*

p < 0.05

**

p < 0.01

***

p < 0.001

In Model 1 of Table 2, which does not include covariates, higher levels of adolescent segregation are associated with significantly higher adulthood neighborhood poverty rates for blacks. Moreover, Models 2 and 3 indicate that these relationships are robust to the inclusion of covariates that help capture individual and household socioeconomic and sociodemographic characteristics along with metropolitan opportunity structures. Individual and familial socioeconomic status in adulthood explain about 20 percent of the relationship between adolescent segregation and adulthood neighborhood poverty rates, and metropolitan characteristics explain 20 percent of the remaining relationship.

The continuing and meaningful significance of metropolitan-level segregation after the inclusion of a wide host of covariates suggests that adolescent exposure to segregation is associated with adulthood neighborhood poverty not merely or even largely through its association with human capital or through the greater proportion of impoverished neighborhoods in segregated areas. Rather, adolescent segregation may be associated with adulthood neighborhood poverty through alternative mechanisms such as its association with kith and kin networks and individuals’ information about communities to move to. To further explore this possibility, Model 4 includes our inter-metropolitan migration dummy variable, which allows us to determine whether internal migration moderates the relationships we observe, potentially by disrupting kith and kin networks and the limitations segregation places on individuals’ neighborhood knowledge. Indeed, Model 4 demonstrates that migrating out of the metropolitan area an individual grew up in significantly and substantially moderates the relationship between adolescent exposure to segregation and adulthood neighborhood poverty rates for blacks.5

To illustrate these relationships more clearly, Figure 1 graphs the predicted neighborhood poverty rate exhibited by black respondents who have and have not moved out of the metropolitan areas they resided in at age 17 as the level of segregation experienced at age 17 increases. The level of segregation is graphed so that only individuals who live in metropolitan areas that have dissimilarity scores that are between 2 standard deviations above or below the mean dissimilarity score for blacks are included (mean = 0.718; 1 SD = 0.111). The predicted values for these graphs are calculated from the fully-specified multivariate results presented in Table 2, Model 4, with all covariates held at their means and with 95 percent confidence intervals included.6

Figure 1:

Figure 1:

The relationship between segregation experienced at age 17 and neighborhood poverty rates for inter-metropolitan migrant and non-migrant blacks: Panel Study of Income Dynamics, 1979-2013

Figure 1 confirms the relationships exhibited in Table 1—increases in adolescent segregation levels are associated with significantly higher neighborhood poverty rates in adulthood for black respondents who remain in the metropolitan areas they grew up in. For those who move, these relationships are significantly and markedly more modest. Specifically, living in a metropolitan area with a dissimilarity score 1 standard deviation above the mean level of dissimilarity for blacks (D = 0.829) during adolescence is associated with an increase in adulthood neighborhood poverty rates of only 0.13 percentage points for black migrants relative to living in a metropolitan area with a dissimilarity score 1 standard deviation below the mean (D = 0.607). For non-migrants, this difference is 3.99 percentage points, which translates into a 18.43 percent increase in the neighborhood poverty rate for non-migrants. Consequently, migrating out of the metropolitan area blacks grew up in does moderate the relationship between adolescent segregation and adulthood neighborhood poverty rates.

In marked contrast, adolescent segregation is associated with significant or marginally significant decreases in neighborhood poverty rates for whites in both bivariate and multivariate models (Table 2, Models 5-8), though these associations are partially explained by the age of the respondent, homeownership status, individual and household socioeconomic charactersitics, the observation length, and metropolitan characteristics. Moreover, migrating out of the metropolitan area an individual grew up in is not associated with significant changes in neighborhood poverty rates, nor does inter-metropolitan migration moderate the association between adolescent segregation and adulthood neighborhood poverty. Consequently, adolescent segregation exhibits a protective association with whites’ adulthood neighborhood outcomes and migration does not significantly moderate this relationship.

Neighborhood Percent Black

Examining neighborhood racial composition provides an additional lens with which to understand the long-term relationship between segregation and neighborhood disadvantage. For black respondents, the results for neighborhood percent black (Table 3, Models 1-4) are very similar to the results for neighborhood poverty rates. In Model 1, increases in metropolitan-level segregation in adolescence are associated with significant increases in neighborhood percent black in adulthood. These relationships remain robust to the inclusion of covariates, with a quarter of the relationship between adolescent segregation and neighborhood percent black in adulthood being attenuated as a result of the inclusion of the variables representing socioeconomic status and year of observation. The inclusion of contextual characteristics (specifically the proportion of blacks in the metropolitan area) modestly strengthens the relationship between segregation and neighborhood outcomes. This could be because some blacks live in areas with lower levels of segregation because their metropolitan areas contain few blacks, though they may still be relatively racially isolated within those metropolitan areas. While the proportion of Hispanics in the metropolitan area does not strengthen or attenuate the focal relationships we observe, it is associated with significantly lower neighborhood racial isolation among blacks because blacks are more likely to share neighborhoods with Hispanics than they are with Asians or whites (Brown and Chung 2006; Iceland 2004; Lee et al. 2008).

Table 3:

The relationship between adolescent segregation, migration, and neighborhood percent black by gender: Panel Study of Income Dynamics, 1979-2013

Model 1 (Black) Model 2 (Black) Model 3 (Black) Model 4 (Black) Model 5 (White) Model 6 (White) Model 7 (White) Model 8 (White)
Focal Independent Variable
 Dissimilarity at 17, origin 0.418*** (0.086) 0.316*** (0.080) 0.348*** (0.080) 0.739*** (0.089) −0.042* (0.018) 0.010 (0.018) −0.014 (0.017) −0.012 (0.024)
Covariates
 Female 0.018+ (0.010) 0.019* (0.009) 0.013 (0.010) 0.001 (0.003) 0.001 (0.003) 0.001 (0.003)
 Married, origin −0.021** (0.008) −0.022** (0.008) −0.021** (0.008) −0.009*** (0.002) −0.009*** (0.003) −0.009*** (0.003)
 Number of children in family, origin −0.003 (0.004) −0.003 (0.004) −0.003 (0.004) −0.002+ (0.001) −0.002+ (0.001) −0.002+ (0.001)
 Age of respondent, origin −0.001 (0.001) −0.001 (0.001) −0.002* (0.001) −0.001* (0.000) −0.001* (0.000) −0.001* (0.000)
 Years of education, origin −0.014*** (0.002) −0.015*** (0.002) −0.012*** (0.002) −0.001 (0.001) −0.001 (0.001) −0.001 (0.001)
 Family income, origin −0.001*** (0.000) −0.001*** (0.000) −0.001*** (0.000) −0.000* (0.000) −0.000* (0.000) −0.000* (0.000)
 HH head employed, origin −0.016** (0.005) −0.017** (0.005) −0.016** (0.005) −0.001 (0.002) −0.001 (0.002) −0.001 (0.002)
 Homeowner, origin 0.028* (0.012) 0.027* (0.012) 0.025* (0.012) 0.000 (0.003) 0.000 (0.003) 0.000 (0.003)
 Persons per room, origin 0.006 (0.006) 0.007 (0.006) 0.007 (0.006) −0.000 (0.003) −0.000 (0.003) −0.000 (0.003)
 Lived in same house 3+ years, origin 0.021** (0.007) 0.021** (0.006) 0.018** (0.007) 0.005** (0.002) 0.005** (0.002) 0.005*** (0.002)
 Observation length 0.007 (0.009) 0.008 (0.010) 0.005 (0.009) 0.003 (0.004) 0.003 (0.004) 0.003 (0.004)
 Year of observation, destination −0.002* (0.001) −0.002 (0.001) −0.001 (0.001) 0.001*** (0.000) 0.001** (0.000) 0.001** (0.000)
 Prop. new housing, origin metro 0.048 (0.161) 0.006 (0.158) 0.020 (0.035) 0.020 (0.035)
 Prop. vacant housing, origin metro 0.287 (0.355) 0.120 (0.337) 0.018 (0.063) 0.016 (0.063)
 Prop. in poverty, origin metro −0.253 (0.419) −0.191 (0.383) 0.061 (0.085) 0.065 (0.085)
 Prop. NH black, origin metro 0.620*** (0.162) 0.563*** (0.138) 0.303*** (0.042) 0.306*** (0.042)
 Prop. Hispanic, origin metro −0.496** (0.156) −0.450*** (0.120) −0.040 (0.031) −0.040 (0.031)
 Northeast Ref. Ref. Ref. Ref.
 Midwest 0.074 (0.064) 0.004 (0.054) 0.013* (0.006) 0.012* (0.006)
 South 0.056 (0.062) 0.067 (0.057) 0.057*** (0.007) 0.055*** (0.007)
 West −0.175** (0.066) −0.103+ (0.055) −0.008 (0.007) −0.009 (0.007)
Mobility Interactions
 Living in different metro from age 17 0.334*** (0.098) 0.011 (0.016)
 Different metro*dissimilarity at 17 −0.757*** (0.139) −0.007 (0.022)
 Constant 0.121* (0.058) 4.569* (1.821) 3.507 (2.548) 1.417 (2.402) 0.103*** (0.014) −2.412*** (0.621) −2.022** (0.654) −2.064** (0.665)
Variance Components
  Between MSAs 0.170*** (0.011) 0.165*** (0.010) 0.119*** (0.017) 0.075*** (0.013) 0.052*** (0.005) 0.052*** (0.005) 0.024*** (0.005) 0.024*** (0.005)
  Between Individuals 0.211*** (0.005) 0.201*** (0.005) 0.200*** (0.005) 0.192*** (0.005) 0.076*** (0.004) 0.075*** (0.004) 0.075*** (0.004) 0.075*** (0.004)
  Residual 0.197*** (0.006) 0.196*** (0.006) 0.195*** (0.006) 0.192*** (0.006) 0.071*** (0.004) 0.071*** (0.004) 0.071*** (0.004) 0.071*** (0.004)

Observations 18846 18846 18846 18846 24438 24438 24438 24438
BIC −2610.864 −2899.774 −3114.451 −3705.518 −52745.499 −52834.356 −53186.454 −53172.452

Standard errors in parentheses

*

p < 0.05

**

p < 0.01

***

p < 0.001

Thus, as was the case for neighborhood poverty, individual and household socioeconomic and sociodemographic characteristics and metropolitan opportunity structures explain some of the deleterious relationship between adolescent segregation and adulthood neighborhood outcomes, but not the majority of it. Moreover, in Model 4, this relationship is significantly and substantially moderated for black migrants. Hence, adolescent exposure to segregation may harm adulthood neighborhood outcomes through mechanisms that inter-metropolitan migration fundamentally disrupts.

The differences across migrants and non-migrants are quite dramatic, as illustrated in Figure 2. In the fully-specified multivariate models, black non-migrants who lived in metropolitan areas with dissimilarity scores 1 standard deviation above the mean during adolescence (D = 0.829) live in neighborhoods with 16.25 percentage points more blacks in adulthood than black non-migrants who lived in metropolitan areas with dissimilarity scores 1 standard deviation below the mean (D = 0.607), respectively. These differences are considerably more modest for migrants, with black migrants experiencing a non-significant −0.42 percentage point decrease in their neighborhood percent black in adulthood when they lived in a metropolitan area with a dissimilarity index 1 standard deviation above the mean during adolescence relative to migrants who lived in a metropolitan area with a dissimilarity score 1 standard deviation below the mean during adolescence.

Figure 2:

Figure 2:

The relationship between segregation experienced at age 17 and neighborhood percent black for inter-metropolitan migrant and non-migrant blacks: Panel Study of Income Dynamics, 1979-2013

For whites (Table 3, Models 5-8), increases in adolescent segregation are associated with significant declines in neighborhood percent black in bivariate models. This relationship becomes non-significant once covariates are included. Model 8 adds the interactions with inter-metropolitan migration. However, inter-metropolitan migration does not significantly moderate the relationship between segregation and neighborhood percent black. Thus, as was the case for neighborhood poverty, adolescent segregation is largely not associated with whites’ neighborhood racial composition outcomes once our theoretically-derived covariates are accounted for. This overall lack of significance could be a result of the historical legacy of segregation and the manner in which it has preserved and even enhanced advantaged, white spaces. Advantaged, predominantly white neighborhoods are far more readily available than advantaged, predominantly black neighborhoods. Hence, living in a white, advantaged neighborhood may be a default experience for most whites, leaving little room for variations in adolescent experiences of segregation to improve or harm neighborhood outcomes.

Supplementary Analyses

Other Outcomes

There are numerous other potential neighborhood outcomes that may be important for individuals’ access to opportunities and for their wellbeing. We therefore examine the robustness of our results to outcomes including the percentage of Hispanics in the neighborhood, as this measure is increasingly important for understanding the racial and ethnic composition of blacks’ neighborhoods and their racial isolation. We also explored whether our relationships hold when we examine logged average household income because this measure helps capture access to advantaged neighborhoods, rather than merely access to non-poor neighborhoods. For both outcomes, our conclusions remain the same. Among blacks, adolescent exposure to segregation is associated with higher percentages of Hispanics in one’s adulthood neighborhood and lower neighborhood income (Table 4, “Across-Metro Moves” section). However, moving out of the metropolitan area one grew up in significantly moderates these relationships such that, after moving, there is no relationship between adolescent exposure to segregation and adulthood neighborhood income and there is a negative association between adolescent exposure to segregation and neighborhood percent Hispanic in adulthood for blacks. Our results for whites indicate that segregation has very little association with neighborhood outcomes. Hence, our relationships are robust to other components of neighborhood advantage and disadvantage.

Table 4:

Focal regression coefficients from fully-specified multivariate models assessing the relationship between adolescent segregation and neighborhood outcomes by types of migration status. Panel Study of Income Dynamics, 1979-2013

Black White

% in Poverty % Black % Hispanic Avg. HH Income % in Poverty % Black % Hispanic Avg. HH Income
Within-Metro, Across-Tract Moves
 Dissimilarity at 17, origina 0.192*** (0.052) 0.668*** (0.104) −0.048 (0.036) −0.431** (0.150) −0.033 (0.026) 0.021 (0.035) −0.033 (0.046) 0.194 (0.119)
 Living in different tract from age 17 0.031 (0.027) 0.104 (0.067) −0.007 (0.015) −0.050 (0.094) 0.013 (0.019) −0.007 (0.019) −0.016 (0.026) 0.023 (0.085)
 Different tract*dissimilarity at 17 −0.062 (0.037) −0.212* (0.089) 0.008 (0.022) 0.105 (0.126) −0.018 (0.027) 0.015 (0.029) 0.018 (0.035) −0.038 (0.119)
 Constant −2.490+ (1.310) −6.876*** (2.036) −1.885* (0.851) −63.889*** (3.039) −0.366 (0.646) −2.638** (0.871) −2.519** (0.933) −65.104*** (2.719)
 Observations 15751 15751 15751 15740 15421 15421 15421 15405

Across-Metro Moves
 Dissimilarity at 17, origina 0.189*** (0.035) 0.668*** (0.104) 0.119* (0.047) −0.431** (0.150) −0.033 (0.020) 0.021 (0.035) 0.014 (0.051) 0.194 (0.119)
 Living in different metro from age 17 0.066+ (0.034) 0.277** (0.099) 0.143*** (0.037) −0.144 (0.116) 0.009 (0.017) −0.007 (0.019) 0.001 (0.037) 0.101 (0.089)
 Different metro*dissimilarity at 17 −0.181*** (0.050) −0.685*** (0.140) −0.181*** (0.050) 0.403* (0.164) −0.001 (0.023) 0.015 (0.029) −0.017 (0.052) −0.116 (0.131)
 Constant 1.034 (1.519) 1.660 (2.438) −0.076 (0.990) −72.767*** (4.192) −0.870+ (0.465) −2.184*** (0.643) −2.798** (0.919) −68.347*** (3.023)
 Observations 18846 18846 18846 18823 24439 24438 24438 24420

Across-State Moves
 Dissimilarity at 17, origin 0.189*** (0.047) 0.683*** (0.058) 0.039 (0.031) −0.454*** (0.105) −0.022 (0.020) −0.017 (0.024) −0.014 (0.042) 0.184* (0.072)
 Living in different state from age 17 0.051 (0.039) 0.222* (0.106) 0.105** (0.038) −0.052 (0.081) −0.003 (0.016) 0.017 (0.024) −0.009 (0.028) 0.121 (0.071)
 Different state*dissimilarity at 17 −0.163** (0.059) −0.585*** (0.147) −0.124** (0.047) 0.272* (0.126) −0.002 (0.022) −0.004 (0.033) 0.002 (0.041) −0.125 (0.099)
 Constant 1.387 (1.308) 3.704 (2.458) −0.192 (0.773) −72.759*** (4.985) −1.447*** (0.425) −2.175** (0.781) −3.383*** (0.912) −67.822*** (1.826)
 Observations 18971 18971 18971 18948 25040 25034 25034 25017

Across-Region Moves
 Dissimilarity at 17, origin 0.208*** (0.043) 0.897*** (0.036) 0.011 (0.017) −0.526*** (0.101) −0.017 (0.020) −0.003 (0.042) 0.045 (0.049) 0.253 (0.165)
 Living in different region from age 17 0.181*** (0.032) 0.657*** (0.031) 0.104** (0.033) −0.304*** (0.064) 0.010* (0.005) 0.037** (0.012) 0.018 (0.020) 0.110 (0.143)
 Different region*dissimilarity at 17 −0.331*** (0.055) −1.167*** (0.191) −0.116** (0.043) 0.576*** (0.124) −0.023 (0.013) −0.043 (0.022) −0.054 (0.043) −0.083 (0.225)
 Constant 0.974 (1.703) 4.498 (2.740) −2.216* (1.123) −72.399*** (6.162) −1.599*** (0.335) −1.727 (1.142) −4.522*** (1.345) −68.489*** (3.194)
 Observations 18971 18971 18971 18948 25040 25034 25034 25017
a

Results are from fully-specified multivariate models, covariates include: marital status, number of children, educational attainment, household income, employment and homeownership status, residence in home for 3+ years, persons per room, observation length, region of residence, proportion of respondents in poverty in the metropolitan area, proportion of new housing in the metropolitan area, proportion of vacant residences in the metropolitan area, proportion of population that is black in the metropolitan area, and proportion of the population that is Hispanic in the metropolitan area (full set of results available upon request).

Standard errors in parentheses

*

p < 0.05

**

p < 0.01

***

p < 0.001

Moves of Varying Distances

It is possible that our relationships could differ if we explore other types and distances of moves. Indeed, Leibbrand and Crowder (2018) illustrated that inter-regional moves correspond to particularly dramatic declines in exposure to neighborhood poverty relative to inter-metropolitan and inter-state moves. It is therefore possible that we would see stronger relationships for longer-distance moves and weaker relationships for shorter-distance moves. Moreover, if we find no moderating effect for within-metro moves, this would add additional support for our expectation that it is longer-distance moves that substantially disrupt the residential context and, as such, individuals’ neighborhood knowledge that moderate the relationship between segregation and neighborhood outcomes. In contrast, inter-metro, inter-state, and inter-regional moves all disrupt local residential knowledge. Thus, we would expect to find that all of these moves moderate the relationship between segregation and neighborhood outcomes and could do so to similar extents given the localized nature of daily rounds and residential knowledge (Krivo et al. 2013; Krysan and Crowder 2017).

As Table 4 demonstrates, localized moves that are within-metro areas but across tracts largely do not moderate the relationship between segregation and neighborhood outcomes. There is a significant moderating effect associated with these moves for blacks for neighborhood percent black, though this moderating effect is substantially more modest than the moderating effect found for other types of moves.7 In contrast, inter-state and inter-regional moves significantly and substantially moderate the relationship between segregation and each neighborhood outcome for blacks and are largely not associated with neighborhood outcomes for whites (though inter-regional moves are associated with a slight increase in neighborhood poverty rates and percent black for whites). The magnitudes of these moderating effects are extremely similar for inter-metro and inter-state moves, and modestly larger for inter-regional moves, as could be expected given that these moves tend to be especially dramatic. Consequently, inter-metropolitan, inter-state, and inter-regional moves moderate the relationship between segregation and neighborhood outcomes and do so to a largely similar extent.

The Issue of Selectivity

Although we have found compelling evidence that migration is an important means of ameliorating the associations between adolescent segregation and adulthood neighborhood outcomes for blacks, these relationships could be largely due to migrant selectivity. For example, migrants may be more prone to risk-taking and exploring new neighborhoods and it may be these characteristics that explain black migrants’ improved neighborhood outcomes, rather than the migration event itself. Our controls help to address some of this selectivity. However, we cannot fully account for migrant selectivity using these models. Given that, we examine the robustness of our results using fixed effects models that examine within-person change in the relationship between segregation and adulthood neighborhood outcomes, with the individual representing our panel variable and “year” representing our time variable within panels. We solely examine these relationships for blacks and we replace our focal independent variable, “dissimilarity exposure at age 17,” with a time-varying measure that differences current exposure to segregation in adulthood from exposure to segregation at age 17. In these models, we are examining whether worsening exposure to segregation in adulthood relative to adolescence is associated with worse neighborhood outcomes in adulthood. As such, it allows us to examine whether in situ changes in segregation are associated with neighborhood outcomes, as well as whether migration disrupts these relationships. Our models include the full suite of covariates used in Tables 2 and 3.

Our fully-specified multivariate results (Appendix Table 2) largely confirm the patterns we describe using the linear mixed-effects models. We find that those who experience higher levels of segregation in adulthood relative to adolescence experience higher neighborhood poverty rates, higher percentages of blacks, and lower incomes in their neighborhoods, relative to those whose exposure to segregation is stable or, especially, declining over time. However, for those who have left the metropolitan areas they grew up in, these relationships are significantly weaker for neighborhood poverty and income. For neighborhood percent black, migrants exhibit significantly lower levels of racial isolation in their neighborhoods regardless of the extent to which their exposure to segregation has changed, though this difference is more modest for individuals who have experienced increases in their exposure to segregation across their lifetimes. Inter-metropolitan migration does not moderate these relationships for exposure to Hispanics because there is no relationship to moderate in these models.

We also explored the extent to which inter-metropolitan migration moderates the relationship between adolescent exposure to segregation and adulthood neighborhood outcomes using marginal structural models with inverse probability weights. These models help account for migrant selectivity, though they are less intuitive to interpret than linear mixed-effects models. They do so by utilizing weights which incorporate baseline, lagged, and contemporaneous individual and contextual characteristics to predict the probability of selection into migration (see Wodtke et al. (2011) for a detailed example). Regression models are then weighted using these inverse probability weights such that the issue of (observed) selection into migration is attenuated, though the issue of unobserved selection does remain. We found that these models yielded substantively identical conclusions to our linear mixed-effects models (results available upon request). Consequently, our supplementary analyses largely confirm that the patterns we observe are robust.

Discussion

In this paper, we used data from the Panel Study of Income Dynamics and multiple censuses to examine whether the level of segregation experienced during adolescence is associated with worse neighborhood outcomes for blacks and better neighborhood outcomes for whites in their adulthood. We also explored whether migrating out of the metropolitan area an individual grew up in moderates these relationships.

Consistent with our first hypothesis, we found that the level of segregation experienced during adolescence is associated with significantly worse neighborhood outcomes in adulthood for blacks. These findings suggest that the effects of early-life exposure to segregation are pernicious, long-lasting, and may continue to influence black individuals’ neighborhoods into adulthood. Moreover, the relationship between early-life exposure to segregation and adult residential outcomes persists after accounting for a wide host of individual and contextual characteristics. Thus, segregation’s association with residential trajectories appears to extend beyond economic mechanisms emphasized in past research, pointing to the potential influence of the social dynamics of residential mobility emphasized in the social structural sorting perspective.

This support for the social structural sorting perspective is reinforced by the moderating effects of longer-distance migration. Consistent with our second hypothesis, our results indicate that, for blacks, migrating out of the metropolitan area an individual grew up in is associated with a significant reduction in the harmful relationship between segregation experienced during adolescence and neighborhood attainment outcomes in adulthood. In fact, for all our outcomes, the relationship between adolescent exposure to segregation and adulthood neighborhood outcomes is close to zero for black migrants, indicating that inter-metropolitan migration is a particularly meaningful moderator. Importantly, this moderating effect is highly robust to the inclusion of individual socioeconomic and contextual characteristics and various strategies to account for the non-random selection into migration. Thus, migration may be able to reduce the impact of early-life exposure not just by shaping economic and sociodemographic conditions later in life but, as suggested by the social structural sorting perspective, by lifting the effects of circumscribed neighborhood knowledge and social ties to areas formed during adolescence.

For whites, adolescent segregation is associated with modestly improved adulthood neighborhood poverty rates, supporting Hypothesis 3. None of the relationships between segregation and neighborhood outcomes were moderated by inter-metropolitan migration, however. This finding is unsurprising given the very modest associations between segregation and neighborhood outcomes for whites. Nevertheless, it does not provide support for Hypothesis 4 where we anticipated that migration might disrupt whites’ local neighborhood knowledge, perhaps prompting them to explore more economically and racially diverse neighborhoods. Thus, whites tend to live in whiter and wealthier neighborhoods regardless of the level of segregation they experience in adolescence and regardless of their migration status, highlighting the availability of white, low-poverty neighborhoods for whites.

Finally, we illustrate that our findings are robust to the examination of different outcomes, including the percentage of Hispanics in the neighborhood and neighborhood income, and our findings hold for all types of longer-distance moves. Specifically, inter-metropolitan, inter-state, and inter-regional moves all significantly and substantially moderate the relationship between segregation and neighborhood outcomes for blacks and they do so to a relatively similar extent. In contrast, intra-metropolitan moves largely do not moderate the relationships we observe. These findings offer suggestive support for our theoretical expectations that it is longer distance moves that disrupt local knowledge and social ties that moderate the relationship between segregation and neighborhood outcomes, as posited by the social structural sorting perspective.

Contributions

Our findings offer a number of contributions to the literature on segregation, neighborhood attainment, and migration. For blacks who remain in the metropolitan areas they grew up in, early-life segregation is associated with persistently worse neighborhood outcomes in adulthood, a finding that adds to the life course literature on the importance of early-life experiences. Individual socioeconomic status and metropolitan opportunity structures do not explain the majority of this relationship. Therefore, our results provide suggestive evidence that adolescent exposure to segregation may be harming outcomes through more socially-oriented mechanisms.

This possibility is reinforced by the finding that migrating out of the metropolitan area an individual grew up in offers substantial promise for improving blacks’ residential outcomes. Our study builds on Leibbrand and Crowder’s (2018) findings that migrating disrupts the association between early- and later-life neighborhood poverty by illustrating that migration also disrupts the broader, more comprehensive influences of segregation in important and substantial ways. We are also able to illustrate that the moderating effect of segregation holds for a wider range of outcomes than Leibbrand and Crowder investigated and is quite robust to numerous methodological techniques that help account for migrant selectivity. By illustrating that segregation does not tend to be associated with worse neighborhood outcomes for those who move out of the metropolitan areas they grew up in, our findings suggest that explanations for the relationship between segregation and neighborhood outcomes should focus on why individuals face difficulties leaving segregated, disadvantaged areas and thereby experience worse outcomes. In particular, our findings offer tentative support for the social structural sorting perspective as an explanation for the relationship between segregation and neighborhood outcomes, and for the influence of kith and kin networks on destination locations and choices.

Our findings are also highly informative for policymakers. The observation that migration substantially moderates the relationship between adolescent segregation and adulthood neighborhood outcomes indicates possible avenues for ameliorating the detrimental associations of segregation with life course outcomes. For instance, our results could illustrate the value of facilitating inter-metropolitan moves for blacks interested in migrating. The persistence of this moderating effect with the inclusion of individuals’ characteristics suggests that segregation may moderate these relationships through social mechanisms, such as by relaxing the constraints segregation puts on individuals’ knowledge of other neighborhoods. Thus, a less disruptive policy implied by our findings is to enhance individuals’ knowledge of local communities to move to that may differ in fundamental ways from their origin locations. The Baltimore Mobility Program and the Oak Park Regional Housing Center are two examples of programs and organizations that counsel potential movers about neighborhood options they may not be familiar with and, in doing so, enhance the probability that individuals will move into neighborhoods that do not closely resemble those they are moving from (Krysan and Crowder 2017).

Moreover, our findings demonstrate the privileged position of whites, attested by the fact that advantaged, white spaces are often widely available and whites are far more likely to start in advantaged neighborhoods than blacks (Sharkey 2013). Hence, whites rarely need to move or move far to find advantaged neighborhoods. In contrast, our results illustrate that blacks often need to engage in reasonably dramatic moves across metropolitan areas to experience improvements in their neighborhood outcomes. Consequently, although our findings offer hopeful possibilities for improving blacks’ outcomes, they also demonstrate the enduring, protective effect segregation has for whites’ outcomes and for maintaining racial disparities across the life course.

Limitations and Future Directions

A limitation of our analysis is its inability to fully explore potential mechanisms behind our relationships. The location of social networks and individuals’ information about neighborhoods may provide important explanations for the relationships between segregation and neighborhood outcomes and may explain the strong moderating effect of longer-distance migration in these relationships. Assuming the advent of data with direct measures of the geographic structure of social networks, patterns of neighborhood knowledge, and their relationship to residential decision making, future research should continue to explore the utility of these social dynamics for explaining why early-life experiences of segregation are associated with later-life outcomes.

Furthermore, there are limitations associated with measuring whether a respondent has moved out of the metropolitan area they resided in at age 17, a strategy that does not distinguish individuals who moved before they turned 17. However, the majority of individuals under age 17 have not moved, and the vast majority of individuals under age 17 have not moved beyond their county of residence (Ihrke and Faber 2012). Consequently, for most respondents, this measure captures individuals who have never moved out of their origin metropolitan area. Nevertheless, future research should examine the effects of a wider range of migration histories and early-life residential exposures on residential outcomes in adulthood.

Segregation has a powerful influence on individuals’ and families’ outcomes and wellbeing. “Geography is destiny,” Napoleon is reputed to have said. Segregation’s association with educational, occupational, physical and mental health, and a multitude of other outcomes, provides some support for this declaration. However, when individuals migrate to new metropolitan areas, they appear to disrupt the harmful relationship between segregation and neighborhood outcomes in important and fundamental ways and thereby improve their outcomes. Future scholars and policymakers should continue to investigate the ways in which migration can be harnessed and used to reduce racial disparities across the life course, in addition to focusing on ways in which the harmful influence of segregation can be mitigated for those who cannot or do not desire to move from their homes.

Acknowledgments

Partial support for this research came from a Shanahan Endowment Fellowship and a Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development training grant, T32 HD007543, as well as a Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development research infrastructure grant, P2C HD042828 to the Center for Studies in Demography & Ecology at the University of Washington. We would like to thank the two anonymous reviewers and the participants of the 2018 PAA conference for offering valuable feedback on this paper.

Appendix Table 1:

The correlations between the focal and supplementary outcome variables for blacks and whites Panel Study of Income Dynamics, 1979-2013

Poverty Rate Percent Black Percent Hispanic Avg. HH Income
Black Respondents
  Poverty Rate 1.000
  Percent Black 0.5364 1.000
  Percent Hispanic −0.0093 −0.4021 1.000
  Avg. HH Income −0.6593 −0.4043 0.1151 1.000
White Respondents
  Poverty Rate 1.000
  Percent Black 0.3780 1.000
  Percent Hispanic 0.3740 0.0549 1.000
  Avg. HH Income −0.4996 −0.1506 −0.0514 1.000

Appendix Table 2:

The relationship between differenced exposure to segregation (Dissimilarity in adulthood – dissimilarity in adolescence) and adulthood neighborhood outcomes for inter-metropolitan migrant and non-migrant blacks: Panel Study of Income Dynamics, 1979-2013

Neighb. Poverty Neighb. Percent Black Neighb. Percent Hispanic Logged Neighb. Income
Living in different metro from age 17 −0.059*** (0.009) −0.161*** (0.023) 0.002 (0.010) 0.177*** (0.030)
Differenced dissimilarityb 0.141* (0.055) 0.275* (0.107) −0.064 (0.044) −0.625*** (0.142)
Different metro*Differenced Dissim. −0.108* (0.053) 0.260* (0.119) −0.002 (0.059) 0.423** (0.154)
Constant 5.129 (5.141) −4.299 (9.936) −2.178 (2.777) −37.379** (13.195)

Observations 18846 18846 18846 18823
BIC −36886.008 −9759.974 −49577.216 59.161
a

Results are from fully-specified multivariate models, covariates include: marital status, number of children, educational attainment, household income, employment and homeownership status, residence in home for 3+ years, persons per room, observation length, region of residence, proportion of respondents in poverty in the metropolitan area, proportion of new housing in the metropolitan area, proportion of vacant residences in the metropolitan area, proportion of population that is black in the metropolitan area, and proportion of the population that is Hispanic in the metropolitan area.

b

Differenced exposure to segregation (Dissimilarity in adulthood – dissimilarity in adolescence)

Standard errors in parentheses

+

p < 0.10

*

p < 0.05

**

p < 0.01

***

p < 0.001

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

1

The important exception being Pais’ 2017 study.

2

The related, but still distinct nature of these two outcomes is illustrated in the correlation matrix in Appendix Table 1 which shows how the two focal outcome measures and the two supplementary outcome measures we examine are correlated with one another. The poverty rate of the neighborhood and percentage of black residents in the neighborhood are moderately, but not strongly correlated with one another for black and white respondents. None of the outcomes are strongly correlated (correlation > 0.7) with one another for either race, indicating that each captures a distinct element of the neighborhood environment.

3

Because we are interested in how adolescent exposure to segregation is associated with later-life neighborhood outcomes, we solely examine individuals who belonged to families who were interviewed when respondents were 17-years-old and who later formed their own households.

4

As a robustness check, we also examined these relationships using the isolation index, which measures individuals’ exposure to members of their same race. We found that all of the relationships for all of the outcomes were consistent using this measure of segregation.

5

We also included fathers’ and mothers’ educational attainment as covariates for each of the outcomes to help account for the possibility that the relationship between adolescent segregation and neighborhood outcomes in adulthood is confounded by parents’ socioeconomic status when respondents are young. Including parents’ education strengthened the relationships we observed for blacks and did not change the relationships for whites, our findings for blacks might be conservative without including parents’ socioeconomic status. However, the missingness for parents’ education was high enough to prohibit including it in this analysis.

6

These results remain the same when covariates are allowed to freely vary rather than to be held at their means.

7

The sample sizes for these moves are smaller because we restrict the sample to individuals who stay within their metropolitan area so that we do not include long-distance movers within our comparison group and thereby create artificially modest relationships where inter-tract movers appear disadvantaged relative to the comparison population.

Contributor Information

Christine Leibbrand, Department of Sociology, University of Washington, 211 Savery Hall, Seattle, WA 98195.

Ryan Gabriel, Department of Sociology, Brigham Young University, 2033 JFSB, Provo, UT 84062, Phone: 801-422-3461.

Chris Hess, Department of Sociology, University of Washington, 211 Savery Hall, Seattle, WA 98195.

Kyle Crowder, Department of Sociology, University of Washington, 225 Savery Hall, Seattle, WA 98195.

References

  1. Arnett Jeffrey Jensen. 2000. “Emerging Adulthood: A Theory of Development from the Late Teens through the Twenties.” American Psychologist 55(5): 469–480. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bianchi Suzanne and Milkie Melissa. 2010. “Work and Family Research in the First Decade of the 21st Century.” Journal of Marriage and Family 72(3): 705–725. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bohn Annette and Berntsen Dorthe. 2013. “The Future is Bright and Predictable: The Development of Prospective Life Stories across Childhood and Adolescence.” Developmental Psychology 49(7): 1232–1241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Brown Belinda, Mackett Roger, Gong Yi, Kitazawa Kay, and Paskins James. 2008. “Gender Differences in Children’s Pathways to Independent Mobility.” Children’s Geographies 6(4): 385–401. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bruch Elizabeth. 2014. “How Population Structure Shapes Neighborhood Segregation.” American Journal of Sociology 119(5): 1221–1278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Chang Virginia, Hillier Amy, and Mehta Neil. 2009. “Neighborhod Racial Isolation, Disorder, and Obesity.” Social Forces 87(4): 2063–2092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Charles Camille Zubrinsky. 2003. “The Dynamics of Racial Residential Segregation.” Annual Review of Sociology 29: 167–207. [Google Scholar]
  8. Charles Camille Z., Dinwiddie Gniesha, and Massey Douglas. 2004. “The Continuing Consequences of Segregation: Family Stress and College Academic Performance.” Social Science Quarterly 85(5): 1353–1373. [Google Scholar]
  9. Chetty Raj, Hendren Nathaniel, and Katz Lawrence F.. 2016. “The Effects of Exposure to Better Neighborhoods on Children: New Evidence from the Moving to Opportunity Experiment.” American Economic Review 106(4):855–902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Clark William. 2007. “Race, class, and place: Evaluating mobility outcomes for African Americans.” Urban Affairs Review 42 (3): 295–314. [Google Scholar]
  11. Clark William. 2008. “Reexamining the Moving to Opportunity Study and Its Contribution to Changing the Distribution of Poverty and Ethnic Concentration.” Demography 45(3): 515–535. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Clark William and Withers Suzanne Davies. 2002. “Disentangling the Interaction of Migration, Mobility, and Labor-Force Participation.” Environment and Planning 34(5): 923–945. [Google Scholar]
  13. Cooke Thomas, Boyle Paul, Couch Kenneth, and Feijten Peteke. 2009. “A Longitudinal Analysis of Family Migration and the Gender Gap in Earnings in the United States and Great Britain.” Demography 46(1): 147–167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Crowder Kyle and Downey Liam. 2010. “Interneighborhood Migration, Race, and Environmental Hazards: Modeling Microlevel Processes of Environmental Inequality.” American Journal of Sociology 115(4): 1110–1149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Crowder Kyle, Pais Jeremy, and South Scott. 2012. “Neighborhood Diversity, Metropolitan Constraints, and Household Migration.” American Sociological Review 77(3): 325–353. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cutler David M. and Glaeser Edward. 1997. “Are Ghettos Good or Bad?” The Quarterly Journal of Economics 112(3): 827–872. [Google Scholar]
  17. Dannefer Dale. 2003. “Cumulative Advantage/Disadvantage and the Life Course: Cross Fertilizing Age and Social Science Theory.” The Journals of Gerontology 58(6): S327–S337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Elder Glen, Johnson Monica Kirkpatrick, and Crosnoe Robert. 2003. “The Emergence and Development of Life Course Theory” in The Handbook of the Life Course edited by Mortimer JT and Shanahan Michael J.. New York, NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. [Google Scholar]
  19. Flippen Chenoa. 2010. “The Spatial Dynamics of Stratification: Metropolitan Context, Population Redistribution, and Black and Hispanic Homeownership.” Demography 47(4): 845–868. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Galster George, and Sharkey Patrick. 2017. “Spatial Foundations of Inequality: A Conceptual Model and Empirical Overview.” RSF: The Russell Sage Foundation Journal of the Social Sciences 3(2): 1–32.29780882 [Google Scholar]
  21. GeoLytics. 2013. Neighborhood Change Database [Database]. East Brunswick, NJ: GeoLytics, Inc. Retrieved from www.geolytics.com/USCensus,Neighborhood-Change-Database-1970-2000,Products.asp [Google Scholar]
  22. Habermas Tilmann and Bluck Susan. 2000. “Getting a Life: The Emergence of the Life Story in Adolescence.” Psychological Bulletin 126(5): 748–769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hayanga Awori Jeremiah, Kaiser Heather E., Sinha Rakhi, Berenholtz Sean, Makary Marty, and Chang David. 2009. “Residential Segregation and Access to Surgical Care by Minority Populations in US Counties.” Journal of the American College of Surgeons 208(6): 1017–1022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Houston Douglas, Wu Jun, Ong Paul, and Winer Arthur. 2004. “Structural Disparities of Urban Traffic in Southern California: Implications for Vehicle-Related Air Pollution Exposure in Minority and High-Poverty Neighborhoods.” Journal of Urban Affairs 26(5): 565–592. [Google Scholar]
  25. Ihrke David and Faber Carol. 2012. “Geographical Mobility: 2005 to 2010.” U.S. Department of Commerce. [Google Scholar]
  26. Jargowsky Paul. 1997. Poverty and Place: Ghettos, Barrios, and the American City. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation. [Google Scholar]
  27. Johnson Odis. 2010. “Assessing Neighborhood Racial Segregation and Macroeconomic Effects in the Education of African Americans.” Review of Educational Research 80(4): 527–575. [Google Scholar]
  28. Johnson Monica Kirkpatrick, Crosnoe Robert, and Elder Glen H. Jr. 2011. “Insights on Adolescence from a Life Course Perspective.” Journal of Research on Adolescence 21(1): 273–280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kahn Joan, McGill Brittany, and Bianchi Suzanne. 2011. “Help to Family and Friends: Are There Gender Differences at Older Ages?” Journal of Marriage and Family 73(1): 77–92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kane Thomas, Riegg Stephanie, and Staiger Douglas. 2006. “School Quality, Neighborhoods, and Housing Prices.” American Law and Economics Reivew 8(2): 183–212. [Google Scholar]
  31. Knapp Thomas, White Nancy, and Wolaver Amy. 2013. “The Returns to Migration: The Influence of Education and Migration Type.” Growth and Change 44(4): 589–607. [Google Scholar]
  32. Kravitz-Wirtz Nicole, Crowder Kyle, Hajat Anjum, and Sass Victoria. 2016. “The Long-Term Dynamics of Racial/Ethnic Inequality in Neighborhood Air Pollution Exposure, 1990-2009.” Du Bois Review 13(2): 237–259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Krieg Randall. 1997. “Occupational Change, Employer Change, Internal Migration, and Earnings.” Regional Science and Urban Economics 27: 1–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Krivo Lauren and Peterson Ruth. 1996. “Extremely Disadvantaged Neighborhoods and Urban Crime.” Social Forces 75(2): 619–648. [Google Scholar]
  35. Krivo J. Lauren, Washington Heather M., Peterson Ruth D., and Kwan Mei-Po. 2013. “Social Isolation of Disadvantage and Advantage: The Reproduction of Inequality in Urban Space.” Social Forces 92(1): 141–164. [Google Scholar]
  36. Krysan Maria and Bader Michael. 2009. “Racial Blind Spots: Black-White-Latino Differences in Community Knowledge.” Social Problems 56(4): 677–701. [Google Scholar]
  37. Krysan Maria and Crowder Kyle. 2017. Cycle of Segregation: Social Processes and Residential Stratification. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation. [Google Scholar]
  38. Kwan Mei-Po. 2000. “Gender Differences in Space-Time Constraints.” Area 32(2): 145–156. [Google Scholar]
  39. Lee Barrett. A., Reardon Sean, Firebaugh Glenn, Farrell Chad, Matthews Stephen, and O’Sullivan David. 2008. “Beyond the Census Tract: Patterns and Determinants of Racial Segregation at Multiple Geographic Scales.” American Sociological Review 73(5):766–91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Leventhal Tama and Brooks-Gunn Jeanne. 2000. “The Neighborhoods They Live In: The Effects of Neighborhood Residence on Child and Adolescent Outcomes. Psychological Bulletin 126(2): 309–337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Mason Susan, Messer Lynne, Laraia Barbara, and Mendola Pauline. 2009. “Segregation and Preterm Birth: The Effects of Neighborhood Racial Composition in North Carolina.” Health & Place 15(1): 1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Massey Douglas and Denton Nancy. 1993. American Apartheid: Segregation and the making of the underclass. Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  43. Massey Douglas and Fischer Mary. 2006. “The Effect of Childhood Segregation on Minority Academic Performance at Selective Colleges.” Ethnic and Racial Studies 29(1): 1–26. [Google Scholar]
  44. Massey Douglas, Condran Gretchen and Denton Nancy. 1987. “The Effect of Residential Segregation on Black Social and Economic Well-Being.” Social Forces 66(1): 29–56. [Google Scholar]
  45. Mendenhall Ruby, DeLuca Stefanie, and Duncan Greg. 2006. “Neighborhood Resources, Racial Segregation, and Economic Mobility: Results from the Gautreaux Program.” Social Science Research 35(4): 892–923. [Google Scholar]
  46. Misra Joya, Lundquist Jennifer Hickes, and Templer Abby. 2012. “Gender, Work Time, and Care Responsibilities among Faculty.” Sociological Forum 27(2): 300–323. [Google Scholar]
  47. Morland Kimberly, Wing Steve, Roux Ana Diez, and Poole Charles. 2002. “Neighborhood Characteristics associated with Location of Food Stores and Food Service Places.” American Journal of Preventative Medicine. 22(1): 23–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Mouw Ted. 2000. “Job Relocation and the Racial Gap in Unemployment in Detroit and Chicago, 1980 to 1990.” American Sociological Review 65(5): 730–753. [Google Scholar]
  49. Neckerman Kathryn, Lovasi Gina, Davies Stephen, Purciel Marnie, Quinn James, Feder Eric, Raghunath Nakita, Wasserman Benjamin, and Rundle Andrew. 2009. “Disparities in Urban Neighborhood Conditions: Evidence from GIS Measures and Field Observation in New York City.” Journal of Public Health 30: 264–285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Orfield Gary and Lee Chungmei. 2005. Why Segregation Matters: Poverty and Educational Inequality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University, The Civil Rights Project. [Google Scholar]
  51. Page Angie, Cooper Ashley, Griew Pippa, Davis Laura, and Hillsdon Melvyn. 2009. “Independent Mobility in Relation to Weekday and Weekend Physical Activity in Children Aged 10-11 Years: The PEACH Project.” International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity 6(2). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Pais Jeremy 2017. “Intergenerational Neighborhood Attainment and the Legacy of Racial Residential Segregation: A Causal Mediation Analysis.” Demography 54(4): 1221–1250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Quillian Lincoln. 2002. “Why Is Black-White Residential Segregation So Persistent?: Evidence on Three Theories from Migration Data.” Social Science Research 31(2):197–229. [Google Scholar]
  54. Quillian Lincoln. 2012. “Segregation and Poverty Concentration: The Role of Three Segregations.” American Sociological Review 77(3): 354–379. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Rugh Jacob and Massey Douglas. 2010. “Racial Segregation and the American Foreclosure Crisis” American Sociological Review 75(5): 629–651. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Rumberger Russell and Palardy Gregory. 2005. “Does Segregation Still Matter? The Impact of Student Composition on Academic Achievement in High School.” Teachers College Record 107(9): 1999–2045. [Google Scholar]
  57. Santiago Anne and Wilder Margaret. 1991. “Residential Segregation and Links to Minority Poverty: The Case of Latinos in the United States.” Social Problems 38(4): 492–515. [Google Scholar]
  58. Saporito Salvatore. 2003. “Private Choices, Public Consequences: Magnet School Choice and Segregation by Race and Poverty.” Social Problems 50(2): 181–203. [Google Scholar]
  59. Shanahan Michael and Flaherty Brian. 2001. “Dynamic Patterns of Time Use in Adolescence.” Child Development 72(2): 385–401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Sharkey Patrick. 2013. Stuck in Place: Urban neighborhoods and the end of progress toward racial equality. University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  61. Sharkey Patrick and Elwert Felix. 2011. “The Legacy of Disadvantage: Multigenerational Neighborhood Effects on Cognitive Ability.” American Journal of Sociology 116(6): 1934–1981. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Sharkey Patrick, and Faber Jacob W.. 2014. “Where, when, why, and for whom do residential contexts matter? Moving away from the dichotomous understanding of neighborhood effects.” Annual Review of Sociology 40: 559–579. [Google Scholar]
  63. South Scott and Crowder Kyle. 1997. “Escaping Distressed Neighborhoods: Individual Community, and Metropolitan Influences.” American Journal of Sociology 102(4): 1040–1084. [Google Scholar]
  64. South Scott J. and Crowder Kyle D.. 1998. “Leaving the ‘Hood: Residential Mobility between Black, White, and Integrated Neighborhoods.” American Sociological Review 63(1): 17–26. [Google Scholar]
  65. South Scott, Pais Jeremy, and Crowder Kyle. 2011. “Metropolitan Influences on Migration into Poor and Nonpoor Neighborhoods.” Social Science Research 40(3): 950–964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Spring Amy, Ackert Elizabeth, Crowder Kyle, and South Scott. 2017. “Influence of Proximity to Kin on Residential Mobility and Destination Choice: Examining Local Movers in Metropolitan Areas.” Demography 54(4): 1277–1304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Squires Gregory and Kubrin Charis. 2005. “Privileged Places: Race, Uneven Development and the Geography of Opportunity in Urban America.” Urban Studies 42(1): 47–68. [Google Scholar]
  68. St. John Craig. 2002. “The Concentration of Affluence in the United States, 1990.” Urban Affairs Review 37(4): 500–520. [Google Scholar]
  69. Subramanian SV, Acevedo-Garcia Dolores, and Osypuk Theresa. 2005. “Racial Residential Segregation and Geographic Heterogeneity in Black/White Disparity in Poor Self-Rated Health in the US: A Multilevel Statistical Analysis.” Social Science and Medicine 60(8): 1667–1679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Wagmiller Robert. 2007. “Race and the Spatial Segregation of Jobless Men in Urban America.” Demography 44(3): 539–562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. White Michael. 1987. American Neighborhoods and Residential Differentiation. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation. [Google Scholar]
  72. Williams David and Collins Chiquita. 2001. “Racial Residential Segregation: A Fundamental Cause of Racial Disparities in Health.” Public Health Rep 116: 404–416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Wodtke Geoffrey, Harding David, and Elwert Felix. 2011. “Neighborhood Effects in Temporal Perspective.” American Sociological Review 76(5): 713–736. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Woo Bongki, Kravitz-Wirtz Nicole, Sass Victoria, Crowder Kyle, Teixeira Samantha, and Takeuchi David T.. 2018. “Residential Segregation and Racial/Ethnic Disparities in Ambient Air Pollution.” Race and Social Problems 2018: 1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Zenk Shannon, Schulz Amy, Israel Barbara, James Sherman, Bao Shuming, and Wilson Mark. 2005. “Neighborhood Racial Composition, Neighborhood Poverty, and the Spatial Accessibility of Supermarkets in Metropolitan Detroit.” American Journal of Public Health 95(4): 660–667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES