Abstract
As ketamine is increasingly used as an effective antidepressant with rapid action, sustaining its short-lived efficacy over a longer period of time using a schedule of repeated injections appears as an option. An open question is whether repeated and single administrations would affect convergent neurocircuits. We used a combination of one of the most robust animal models of depression with high-field neuroimaging to perform a whole-brain delineation of functional mechanisms underlying ketamine’s effects. Rats from two genetic strains, depressive-like and resilient, received seven treatments of 10 mg/kg S-ketamine (N = 14 depressive-like, N = 11 resilient) or placebo (N = 12 depressive-like, N = 10 resilient) and underwent resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging. Using graph theoretical models of brain networks, we compared effects of repeated ketamine with those of single administration from a separate dataset of our previous study. Compared to single treatment, repeated ketamine evoked strain-specific brain network randomization, resembling characteristics of the depressive-like strain and patients. Several affected regions belonged to the auditory, visual, and motor circuitry, hinting at possible cumulative side effects. Finally, when compared to saline, repeated ketamine affected only a few local topological properties and had no effects on global properties. In combination with the lack of clear differences compared to placebo, our findings point toward an inefficacy of ketamine’s long-term administration on brain topology, making questionable the postulated effect of repeated administration and being consistent with the recently reported absence of repeated ketamine’s antidepressant efficacy in several placebo-controlled studies.
Subject terms: Depression, Molecular neuroscience
Introduction
Ketamine has a rapid antidepressant action, which is, however, relatively short-lived, typically lasting 7 days1–3. To prevent a post-ketamine depressive relapse, several studies tested a repeated administration as a possible maintenance strategy and demonstrated sustained antidepressant efficacy at least throughout the duration of the infusion period4–12. In rodents repeated treatment with ketamine reverses depressive-like behavior in chronic unpredictable stress model13,14 and chronic mild stress models of depression15.
In our previous study we investigated the long-term effects of a single administration of ketamine16 using the negative cognitive (NC) model17, one of the most robust and well-validated animal models of treatment-resistant depression (TRD)18. It comprises breeding two genetic strains of rats, based on their susceptibility to develop stress-escape behavior, when exposed to electrical footshocks17. The NC strain failing to escape represents cognitive aspects of depression, in which events are considered negative and uncontrollable, and displays the pessimistic response bias in cognitive tasks19. The resilient strain displays escape behavior (positive cognitive (PC) state) and represents a non-depressed phenotype17. In this study ketamine and placebo were administered to both strains. In contrast to the PC group, the NC strain exhibited strain-specific effects of ketamine on brain topology mainly localized within the habenula-midthalamic-hippocampal circuit16. This neurocircuit mediates cognitive flexibility, an ability to adaptively switch behavior depending on situation or situational context16, profoundly impaired in depression20,21. We suggested that ketamine mediates its pro-cognitive effects by normalizing the disrupted functional wiring within the habenula-thalamic-hippocampal cognitive circuitry, which might be a key imaging correlate of its long-term effect.
However, whether ketamine’s repeated administration would affect convergent neurocircuits, implying neural mechanisms similar to a single treatment, remains an open question. To address this question, we compared the effects of repeated and single types of ketamine treatment on brain circuitry. Depressive-like and resilient rats from the same genetic generation, as in our previous study, received seven daily injections of ketamine or placebo (saline). Unlike most human studies on repeated ketamine lacking placebo group4–8,10–12, both rat strains had parallel control groups receiving saline. After conducting a series of daily ketamine/saline injections, the animals underwent resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI). Focusing on finding the neural basis of repeated ketamine’s action in alignment with the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) principles22 and to gain insights into the fundamental brain network alterations induced by this treatment, as compared with the single administration from our previous study, we used graph theoretical models of brain networks to perform a whole-brain delineation of functional mechanisms underlying ketamine effects.
Materials and methods
Experimental design
Four parallel groups of the depressive-like NC and resilient PC male rats (N = 47; Sprague–Dawley; 83rd genetical generations; 8 weeks old; 246–344 g) underwent an escape test (for description see Supplement) and starting from this day received seven daily injections of either S-ketamine at 10 mg/kg (Ketanest, Pfizer Pharma GmBH, Berlin, Germany) (14 NC, 11 PC rats) or saline (12 NC, 10 PC rats) (subcutaneously (s.c.), 1 ml/kg) (Fig. 1). We chose the dose of 10 mg/kg as it corresponds to 0.5 mg/kg dose used in humans23,24, resulting in a similar range of peak concentrations25, and is an optimum antidepressant dose in the animal models13,15. In humans a regimen for ketamine’s repeated treatment mainly comprises six to eight intermittent injections; however, in rats a schedule is often consecutive due to faster metabolic rates in rodents26. Similar to an intermittent regimen, in a rat model of depression it demonstrates robust antidepressant-like effects13. We used a consecutive regimen in order to fit the injection schedule into a week instead of prolonging it to 2 weeks by intermittent treatments, thus keeping the ages of rats in alignment with our previous study and avoiding brain aging effects.
On the day following the last injection the animals underwent fMRI scanning. Group (PC/NC), treatment (ketamine/saline), and time of day were randomized in the fMRI measurements. During the experiments the investigator was blinded to the assignment of groups.
For the single injection, we used a separate previously acquired fMRI dataset consisting of NC and PC rats belonging to the same genetic generation as rats in the current dataset and treated with 10 mg/kg S-ketamine (12 NC, 13 PC rats) or saline (12 NC, 12 PC rats)16.
The experimental procedures (behavioral test, fMRI scanning, anesthesia protocol) and pre-processing pipeline for both datasets were identical.
Due to the exploratory nature of this study, no formal power or sample size estimation was carried out; however, the group sizes (N = 10–14 per group) were toward the high end of the range typically used in animal fMRI experiments.
The rats were housed in plastic cages (two rats per cage) at a constant temperature of 22 °C and 12-h light–dark cycle (lights on at 07:00 a.m.) and with food and water available ad libitum. At the end of the experiments the rats were sacrificed. The experiments were performed according to the regulations covering animal experimentation within the European Union (European Communities Council Directive 86/609/EEC) and within the German Animal Welfare Act, and were approved by the German animal welfare authorities (Regierungspräsidium Karlsruhe).
MRI acquisition and pre-processing
The rs-fMRI experiments were carried out at 9.4 T MRI scanner (Bruker BioSpec, Ettlingen, Germany) with Avance III hardware, BGA12S gradient system (maximum strength 705 mT/m) and Paravision 6 software. We used a linear whole-body volume transmitter coil combined with an anatomically shaped four-channel receive-only coil array. The rats were initially anesthetized with 4% isoflurane (Baxter Deutschland GmbH, Unterschleissheim, Germany) in a mixture of 70% N2 and 30% O2; then, after positioning in the scanner, isoflurane level was reduced to 2.5% and medetomidine (Domitor, Janssen-Cilag, Neuss) was injected as a bolus (0.5 ml, 0.07 mg/kg, s.c.). Isoflurane administration was slowly discontinued during 10 min (reduction by 0.5% every 2 min), and after switching it off, the animals continuously received medetomidine at 0.28 mg/kg/h. We monitored sedation depth via recording the physiological (respiratory and cardiac) parameters throughout the experiment at 10-ms resolution using the signal breakout module (Small Animal Instruments Inc., NY, USA) and a 4-channel recorder (Velleman® N.V., Gavere, Belgium). The physiological parameters stabilized at 15 min after the start of continuous medetomidine and remained stable during the whole experiment.
The MRI acquisition protocol included a T2*-weighted echo-planar imaging (EPI) free induction decay sequence (repetition time/echo time (TR/TE) 1500/17.5 ms, flip angle 60°, field of view 35 × 35 mm2, voxel dimension 0.365 mm, 30 coronal slices, slice thickness 0.5 mm, 340 acquisitions over 8.5 min, 8 dummy scans), a 3D double gradient echo FieldMap sequence for correction of geometric distortions (TR = 20 ms, short TE = 1.7 ms, long TE = 5.7 ms), and a T2-weighted rapid acquisition with refocused echoes (RARE) sequence (RARE factor 16, TR/TE 1200/50 ms, flip angle 180°, the voxel dimension 0.15 × 0.15 × 0.3 mm3, acquisition time 23 min).
Image pre-processing was performed as in our previous studies27,28 and comprised the following steps: (1) correction of each EPI time-series for magnetic field (B0) inhomogeneities and movement using “realign & unwarp” SPM function (SPM8: http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm8); (2) regressing out the estimated movement parameter vectors from each voxel (FSL, version 4.1. http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl); (3) filtering out respiratory and cardiac signals using Aztec software;29 (4) slice-timing correction (SPM8); (5) spatial normalization to a rat brain template in Paxinos space (SPM8);30 (6) filtering out the cerebrospinal fluid signal from the normalized images (FSL); (7) scrubbing data frames with a frame-wise displacement >0.03 mm (linear interpolation) in order to capture the remaining motion-related artifacts;31 (8) global signal regression; (9) band-pass filtering (0.01–0.1 Hz) (Analysis of Functional NeuroImages version 2)32.
Graph theoretical analysis
Graph generation
Fully connected weighted graphs covering the whole brain33 were created by calculating pairwise Pearson’s correlation coefficients between time-courses of 43 bilateral regions defined by Schwarz atlas30. The graph of each subject and measurement was normalized, that is, divided by the maximum value within its correlation matrix, in order to get rid of inter-individual differences and make the values in the connectivity matrices comparable across datasets. The freely available Brain Connectivity Toolbox (2015-01-25 version) was used for analyses of the matrices in Matlab34.
Integration of network metrics over a range of sparsity thresholds
By retaining a fixed percentage of edges, the graphs were converted into equi-sparse networks. A fixed percentage of edges has to be retained in order to have the same density between the individual networks to make them comparable. To determine systematic effects on topological organization that would be independent of the choice of a single arbitrary density threshold, we selected a range of thresholds. The lower threshold of 25% corresponded to the value at which networks for each subject and measurement remained connected, meaning there were no infinite path lengths for any node35. The upper threshold of 45% was the highest density with no negatively weighted edges present27,33. These thresholds were identical to the ones used for ketamine single injection datasets16. After calculating graph metrics for each threshold, we computed the area under the curve (AUC) for each network metric by averaging the metric values within the selected range of thresholds (25–45%). The AUC method is sensitive to topological alterations in psychiatric disorders, and thus has been extensively used in brain network studies36,37. The groups were then compared based on the AUC parameters.
Global and local metrics
For each network and sparsity level, we calculated five global metrics: characteristic path length (λ)—the average shortest path length, global clustering coefficient (γ)—the average of all nodal clustering coefficients, small-world index (σ)—the ratio of global clustering coefficient and characteristic path length, global efficiency—the average inverse shortest path length, local efficiency—the average of local efficiency values for all nodes, and five local network metrics: degree—the number of edges connecting a given node to other nodes, strength—the sum of nodal edges weights, betweenness centrality—the percentage of all shortest paths in the network containing the given node, clustering coefficient—the number of edges between the neighbors of a given node normalized to the maximum number of possible edges, local efficiency—global efficiency computed on a node’s neighborhood. To control for possible differences in the overall connectivity strength, a resampling algorithm created 100 reference random networks for each network with preserved degree and strength distributions38.
Statistical analysis I. Strain-specific effects of ketamine’s repeated versus single types of administration
Since our aim was to compare the effect of ketamine’s repeated treatment to single administration, we included fMRI scans from our previous study in which the animals received only a single ketamine injection16. As an effect of saline was of no interest per se and if used as an additional factor would have hindered an interpretation of analysis of variance (ANOVA) results, we subtracted mean saline values from ketamine scans for each of the network properties, treating them as a reference:
Δ-Ketaminerepeated = Ketaminerepeated – mean(Salinerepeated),
Δ-Ketaminesingle = Ketaminesingle – mean(Salinesingle).
Then for each delta value, we calculated a two-way ANOVA with factors “group” (NC/PC) and “type of administration” (repeated/single) (p < 0.05, false discovery rate (FDR) correction for number of regions N = 43, q < 0.05).
Statistical analysis II. Strain-specific effects of repeated ketamine versus repeated saline administration
To test, whether ketamine’s effect differed from placebo (saline), when injected repeatedly, we used an ANOVA with factors “group” (NC/PC) and “treatment” (ketamine/saline) (p < 0.05, FDR correction, q < 0.05).
Results
Comparison of repeated to single ketamine administration
Global metrics
Repeated ketamine administration elicited a strain-specific change in characteristic path length (interaction, F1,46 = 6.5379, p = 0.0139) and global efficiency (interaction, F1,46 = 7.2677, p = 0.0098). A comparison of these parameters between the two types of administration (repeated vs. single) in the NC group revealed a reduction of the characteristic path length (p = 0.0043, FDR < 0.05) (lower λ in the NC-repeated group (light green), as compared to the NC-single (green) and no significant difference in the PC rats (orange vs. light orange in Fig. 2)) and an increase in global efficiency (p = 0.0029, FDR < 0.05) for repeated treatment (higher Eg in the NC-repeated group (light green), as compared to the NC-single (green) and no significant difference in the PC rats (orange vs. light orange in Fig. 2)). The reference values of each of the global metrics before subtraction and creating Δ-values are presented in Table S1.
Local metrics
Several graph metrics expressed interaction effects between rat strain and type of ketamine administration in the prefrontal and frontal cortical regions, such as the infralimbic (clustering, F1,46 = 5.0973, p = 0.0288), orbitofrontal (degree, F1,46 = 12.1477, p = 0.0011; strength, F1,46 = 5.4572, p = 0.0239; betweenness centrality, F1,46 = 4.8688, p = 0.0324; local efficiency, F1,46 = 4.8944, p = 0.0319), dorsal peduncular (clustering, F1,46 = 6.3568, p = 0.0152), and cingulate cortices (degree, F1,46 = 4.6352, p = 0.0366) (Figs. 3 and 4). The pronounced interaction effects were also present in the posterior cortical (auditory cortex: strength, F1,46 = 6.6431, p = 0.0132; betweenness centrality, F1,46 = 10.8406, p = 0.0019; local efficiency, F1,46 = 18.0898, p = 0.0001; parietal association cortex: clustering, F1,46 = 14.4347, p = 0.0004; local efficiency, F1,46 = 14.8902, p = 0.0004) and somato-motor areas (secondary motor cortex: degree, F1,46 = 5.0404, p = 0.0296; somatosensory cortex: strength, F1,46 = 5.0466, p = 0.0295; betweenness centrality, F1,46 = 5.6097, p = 0.0221).
The subcortical regions manifested interaction effects in the zona incerta (strength, F1,46 = 6.8532, p = 0.0119; clustering, F1,46 = 9.4532, p = 0.0035; local efficiency, F1,46 = 8.1497, p = 0.0064) and superior colliculus (degree, F1,46 = 5.5051, p = 0.0233; strength, F1,46 = 8.9966, p = 0.0044; local efficiency, F1,46 = 5.5032, p = 0.0233).
Most of these interaction effects arose from the differences between repeated and single administration within the NC group, as demonstrated by the post hoc pairwise comparisons (Fig. S1). Importantly, the direction of changes after repeated ketamine in the prefrontal areas qualitatively corresponded to the direction of the local topological metrics in the NC strain baseline pre-ketamine state, as compared to the PC strain, in our previous study16, with orbitofrontal, infralimbic, and cingulate cortices displaying reduced degree, clustering, and efficiency (Figs. 3, 4, and S1 and Table 1). On the contrary, the somato-motor, parietal association, and auditory cortices had enhanced values of almost all graph parameters for repeated treatment (Figs. 3, 4, and S1 and Table 1). Similarly, the zona incerta and superior colliculus exhibited increased values of degree, strength, betweenness centrality, or local efficiency (Figs. 3, 4, and S1 and Table 1).
Table 1.
Brain regions | p Value | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Degree | Strength | Betweenness centrality |
Clustering coefficient |
Local efficiency | |
Amyg | ↓ 0.0230 | ||||
Au | ↑ 0.0169 | ↑ 0.0016∇ | ↑ 0.0001∇ | ||
BNST | ↑ 0.0060∇ | ↑ 0.0384 | ↑ 0.0244 | ||
Cg1 | ↓ 0.0046∇ | ↓ 0.0097 | |||
Cg2 | ↓ 0.0031∇ | ||||
DLT | ↑ 0.0086∇ | ↑ 0.0005∇ | ↑ 0.0005∇ | ↑ 0.0307 | ↑ 0.0134 |
DRN | ↓ 0.0107∇ | ||||
HcPD | ↓ 0.0028∇ | ↓ 0.0390 | ↓ 0.0375 | ||
HcV | ↓ 0.0349 | ||||
I | ↑ 0.0307 | ↑ 0.0056∇ | ↑ 0.0237 | ||
IL | ↓ 0.0003∇ | ↓ 0.0140 | |||
M1 | ↑ 0.0130∇ | ↑ 0.0012∇ | |||
M2 | ↑ 0.0200 | ||||
OF | ↓ 0.0039∇ | ↓ 0.0086∇ | |||
PtA | ↑ 0.0191 | ↑ 0.0040∇ | |||
S2 | ↑ 0.0016∇ | ↑ 0.0002∇ | ↑ 0.0001∇ | ||
SC | ↑ 0.0047∇ | ↑ 0.0066∇ | ↑ 0.0318 | ||
Sept | ↓ 0.0333 | ↓ 0.0206 | ↓ 0.0008∇ | ||
SN | ↑ 0.0136 | ||||
TeA | ↓ 0.0011∇ | ||||
VTA | ↑ 0.0159 | ||||
ZI | ↑ 0.0377∇ | ↑ 0.0035∇ | ↑ 0.0445 |
Comparison of repeated ketamine to saline
Global metrics displayed no differences between the groups or the treatment. For the local metrics repeated ketamine, as compared to saline, had a very modest effect. Strain-specific effects appeared only in a few of the cortical regions, such as the retrosplenial (degree, F1,43 = 4.54, p = 0.0388), entorhinal (degree, F1,43 = 5.98, p = 0.0186), dorsal peduncular (clustering coefficient, F1,43 = 4.52, p = 0.0393), auditory (local efficiency, F1,43 = 11.14, p = 0.0018), and parietal association cortices (clustering coefficient, F1,43 = 8.98, p = 0.0045; local efficiency, F1,43 = 8.99, p = 0.0045) (Table S2).
The post hoc pairwise comparisons demonstrated some of these changes to result from effects of ketamine either within the NC group, for example, in the entorhinal cortex (degree, NC-ketamine < NC-saline, p = 0.0408) and auditory cortex (local efficiency, NC-ketamine > NC-saline, p = 0.0001, survived FDR), or between NC and PC groups, as in dorsal peduncular cortex (clustering coefficient, NC-ketamine < PC-ketamine, p = 0.0171) and parietal association cortex (local efficiency, NC-ketamine > PC-ketamine, p = 0.0391).
Discussion
Repeated ketamine, as compared to single injection, evoked a strain-specific brain network randomization effect, expressed as enhanced global efficiency and reduced path length in the depressive-like NC rats. This effect strikingly resembles the depressive-like strain’s pre-ketamine baseline network characteristics16 and those of depressed patients37,39,40. Similarly at the nodal level, the strain-specific reduced prefrontal connectivity in the NC rats mirrored the depressive pattern. Finally, the scarcity of differences between the repeated ketamine and placebo (saline) stresses out an inefficacy of repeated treatment at least at the network level.
Strain-specific effects of repeated vs. single ketamine
Repeated application of ketamine induced prefrontal topological changes in the NC rats similar to their baseline state16 and common network phenotypes of clinical depressive state41–43, expressed as reduced degree for the cingulate cortex and reduced local efficiency for the orbitofrontal cortex. Conversely, the somato-motor cortical regions displayed strain-specific increased degree, strength and betweenness centrality. As degree and strength signify number and weights of connections, this change may rather reflect an increased dendritic spine formation in the somatosensory cortex, reported in mice treated for 5 days with 10 mg/kg ketamine44.
Several other areas involved in sensory and motor functions manifested strain-specific effects: these included the auditory cortex and superior colliculus mediating auditory and visual perception, the parietal association cortex providing sensory-to-motor transformation45, and the zona incerta participating in limbic-motor integration46. While it is difficult to speculate about the functional meaning of these findings, these effects might possibly explain the long-lasting perceptual and motor-related cumulative side effects. For example, an increase in strength, betweenness centrality, and local efficiency in the auditory cortex resembles a general strain-independent acute effect of ketamine in both NC and PC rats16, as well as in healthy Sprague–Dawley rats from our previous study27. This increase might reflect a lasting effect on auditory perception, as chronic ketamine elicits reductions in several parameters of the auditory event-related potentials in humans47 and in rodents produces chronic reductions in auditory-evoked potentials48. In parallel, the visual function could also be affected, as repeated ketamine enhanced degree, strength, and efficiency of the superior colliculus, a midbrain structure receiving connections from the retinal ganglion cells and involved in the generation of saccades and in visual orienting behavior toward novel, unexpected or salient stimuli49. This function is directly linked to distractibility, an inability to sustain attention50,51, manifested in clinical depression52 and induced by ketamine53. Collicular lesions reduce distractibility54, whereas hyper-responsive superior colliculus could enhance this condition51. Thus, in addition to the modulation of the visual perception47,55, repeated ketamine’s effect on collicular topological properties might underlie attentional deficits.
Effects of repeated ketamine vs. placebo
It is important to note that, in contrast to most of the investigations on repeated ketamine, our study included a placebo (control) group. Only five recent human studies included a control group9,55–58, and, strikingly, they showed contradictory results. Ketamine either maintained antidepressant efficacy9,56,57 or failed to outperform placebo58 and was ineffective55, additionally causing side effects55,58. The study of Canuso et al. 56 demonstrated that although the depressive symptoms diminish after ketamine at all time points, the suicidal ideation reduces only at 4 h, and displays no improvement at 24 h or at end-point day 25. The reasons for these mixed outcomes are unclear and might include higher treatment resistance levels58, chronic history of depression58, ineffective dosing, and possible tolerance-like phenomena55,58,59. For example, Sprague–Dawley rats develop tolerance to repeated administration of anesthetic doses of ketamine, possibly arising as a result of receptor desensitization, reduction of receptor density, or increased induction of metabolizing enzymes60. Also, a recent animal study comparing the effects of a single and repeated treatment of ketamine demonstrated the lack of antidepressant-like effectiveness of long-term administration61, hypothesizing to result from the detrimental effects of the parvalbumin interneurons loss in the prefrontal cortex after repeated treatment62 and/or possible metabolic side effects.
On top of that, most studies, except a few56,58, excluded patients with high suicide risk. The study demonstrating an antidepressant efficacy of repeated ketamine9 had exclusion criteria of suicidal ideation and history of non-response to ketamine. On the contrary, the study including patients with both severe TRD and current chronic suicidal ideation showed a failure of ketamine to surpass placebo58. The absence of difference to placebo may reflect the fact that severely depressed patients have decreased responsiveness to placebo, rather than increased responsiveness to medication63. In agreement with this suggestion, our additional analysis comparing repeated ketamine to saline detected very few strain-specific differences. This is consistent with the results of the human placebo-controlled studies55,58 and might similarly result from an inability of repeated ketamine to surpass placebo effects in this TRD model. Out of 18 assessed rodent models of depression, the genetic model used in the current study is one of the most robust and reliable18. It models depression resistant to antidepressant (desipramine) and electroconvulsive therapy64. Ketamine’s single injection is effective in this model23,65,66; however, repeated treatment was tested only in the chronic unpredictable stress and chronic mild stress models of depression13–15, and it remains to be tested in this cognitive model.
The possibility of a differential neural mechanism underlying ketamine’s antidepressant effect, which would not be reflected in network topology, also cannot be excluded. This might be represented by neuroplastic changes, levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor and corticosterone, gene expression, and molecular pathway changes, among many possible reasons. Therefore, we would like to stress that we show a lack of long-term efficacy of repeated treatment only from a network point of view.
Conclusions
The strain-specific effect of repeated ketamine treatment on brain topology resembled the depressive-like imaging phenotype and showed an effect highly dissimilar to single application. Several of the affected regions belonging to the auditory and visual circuitry hint at possible cumulative side effects at the sensory level. The scarcity of differences between the repeated ketamine and placebo (saline) indicates an inefficacy of repeated treatment, at least on the network topology, and is supported by the lack of antidepressant efficacy of repeated treatment reported in recent placebo-controlled studies. However, differential mechanisms for an antidepressant effect of repeated ketamine not reflected in network topology cannot be excluded. Further behavioral validation and correlation with imaging data would be necessary to ascertain the functional significance of our findings. Nevertheless, this empirical evidence is an advance in understanding the underlying neurobiological basis of both types of treatment.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
We thank Felix Hörner, Helene Schamber, and Claudia Falfan-Melgoza for their excellent technical assistance. This work was supported by the grants from the German Research Foundation (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft): DFG GA 2109/2-1 to N.G., DFG SA 1869/15-1 to N.G. and A.S., DFG SA 1869/11-2 within the priority program (SPP1629) “Thyroid Trans Act” to A.S. and J.R.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Supplementary Information accompanies this paper at (10.1038/s41398-020-0727-8).
References
- 1.Zarate CA, Jr., et al. A randomized trial of an N-methyl-d-aspartate antagonist in treatment-resistant major depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2006;63:856–864. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.8.856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Coyle CM, Laws KR. The use of ketamine as an antidepressant: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Hum. Psychopharmacol. 2015;30:152–163. doi: 10.1002/hup.2475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Romeo B, Choucha W, Fossati P, Rotge JY. Meta-analysis of short- and mid-term efficacy of ketamine in unipolar and bipolar depression. Psychiatry Res. 2015;230:682–688. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.10.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.aan het Rot M, et al. Safety and efficacy of repeated-dose intravenous ketamine for treatment-resistant depression. Biol. Psychiatry. 2010;67:139–145. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.08.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Murrough JW, et al. Rapid and longer-term antidepressant effects of repeated ketamine infusions in treatment-resistant major depression. Biol. Psychiatry. 2013;74:250–256. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.06.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Rasmussen KG, et al. Serial infusions of low-dose ketamine for major depression. J. Psychopharmacol. 2013;27:444–450. doi: 10.1177/0269881113478283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Segmiller F, et al. Repeated S-ketamine infusions in therapy resistant depression: a case series. J. Clin. Pharm. 2013;53:996–998. doi: 10.1002/jcph.122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Shiroma PR, et al. Augmentation of response and remission to serial intravenous subanesthetic ketamine in treatment resistant depression. J. Affect. Disord. 2014;155:123–129. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.10.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Singh JB, et al. A double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled, dose-frequency study of intravenous ketamine in patients with treatment-resistant depression. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2016;173:816–826. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16010037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Albott, C. S. et al. Efficacy, safety, and durability of repeated ketamine infusions for comorbid posttraumatic stress disorder and treatment-resistant depression. J. Clin. Psychiatry79, 10.4088/JCP.17m11634 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 11.Zheng W, et al. Rapid and longer-term antidepressant effects of repeated-dose intravenous ketamine for patients with unipolar and bipolar depression. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2018;106:61–68. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2018.09.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Zhou Y, et al. Neurocognitive effects of six ketamine infusions and the association with antidepressant response in patients with unipolar and bipolar depression. J. Psychopharmacol. 2018;32:1118–1126. doi: 10.1177/0269881118798614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Zhang GF, et al. Repeated ketamine administration redeems the time lag for citalopram’s antidepressant-like effects. Eur. Psychiatry. 2015;30:504–510. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2014.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Clarke M, et al. Ketamine modulates hippocampal neurogenesis and pro-inflammatory cytokines but not stressor induced neurochemical changes. Neuropharmacology. 2017;112:210–220. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2016.04.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Papp M, Gruca P, Lason-Tyburkiewicz M, Willner P. Antidepressant, anxiolytic and procognitive effects of subacute and chronic ketamine in the chronic mild stress model of depression. Behav. Pharm. 2017;28:1–8. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0000000000000259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Gass, N. et al. Differences between ketamine’s short-term and long-term effects on brain circuitry in depression. Transl. Psychiatry9, 172 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 17.Vollmayr B, Gass P. Learned helplessness: unique features and translational value of a cognitive depression model. Cell Tissue Res. 2013;354:171–178. doi: 10.1007/s00441-013-1654-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Willner P, Belzung C. Treatment-resistant depression: are animal models of depression fit for purpose? Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 2015;232:3473–3495. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-4034-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Enkel T, et al. Ambiguous-cue interpretation is biased under stress- and depression-like states in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:1008–1015. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Pehrson AL, et al. Treatment of cognitive dysfunction in major depressive disorder—a review of the preclinical evidence for efficacy of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, serotonin-norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors and the multimodal-acting antidepressant vortioxetine. Eur. J. Pharm. 2015;753:19–31. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2014.07.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.van Rooij, S. J. H. et al. The role of the hippocampus in predicting future posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in recently traumatized civilians. Biol. Psychiatry84, 106–115 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 22.Insel T, et al. Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2010;167:748–751. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Koike H, Iijima M, Chaki S. Involvement of AMPA receptor in both the rapid and sustained antidepressant-like effects of ketamine in animal models of depression. Behav. Brain Res. 2011;224:107–111. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.05.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Fukumoto K, et al. Antidepressant potential of (R)-ketamine in rodent models: comparison with (S)-ketamine. J. Pharm. Exp. Ther. 2017;361:9–16. doi: 10.1124/jpet.116.239228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Chowdhury GM, et al. Transiently increased glutamate cycling in rat PFC is associated with rapid onset of antidepressant-like effects. Mol. Psychiatry. 2017;22:120–126. doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Demetrius, L.: Of mice and men. When it comes to studying ageing and the means to slow it down, mice are not just small humans. EMBO Rep.6 (2005). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 27.Gass N, et al. Antagonism at the NR2B subunit of NMDA receptors induces increased connectivity of the prefrontal and subcortical regions regulating reward behavior. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 2018;235:1055–1068. doi: 10.1007/s00213-017-4823-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Reinwald, J. R. et al. Neural mechanisms of early-life social stress as a developmental risk factor for severe psychiatric disorders. Biol Psychiatry84, 116–128 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 29.van Buuren M, et al. Cardiorespiratory effects on default-mode network activity as measured with fMRI. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009;30:3031–3042. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20729. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Schwarz AJ, et al. A stereotaxic MRI template set for the rat brain with tissue class distribution maps and co-registered anatomical atlas: application to pharmacological MRI. Neuroimage. 2006;32:538–550. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.04.214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Power JD, et al. Methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRI. Neuroimage. 2014;84:320–341. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Cox RW. AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 1996;29:162–173. doi: 10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Gass N, et al. Brain network reorganization differs in response to stress in rats genetically predisposed to depression and stress-resilient rats. Transl. Psychiatry. 2016;6:e970. doi: 10.1038/tp.2016.233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Rubinov M, Sporns O. Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. Neuroimage. 2010;52:1059–1069. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Bordier C, Nicolini C, Bifone A. Graph analysis and modularity of brain functional connectivity networks: searching for the optimal threshold. Front. Neurosci. 2017;11:441. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Wang J, et al. Parcellation-dependent small-world brain functional networks: a resting-state fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009;30:1511–1523. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20623. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Zhang J, et al. Disrupted brain connectivity networks in drug-naive, first-episode major depressive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry. 2011;70:334–342. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.05.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Rubinov M, Sporns O. Weight-conserving characterization of complex functional brain networks. Neuroimage. 2011;56:2068–2079. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.03.069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Hou Z, et al. Divergent topological architecture of the default mode network as a pretreatment predictor of early antidepressant response in major depressive disorder. Sci. Rep. 2016;6:39243. doi: 10.1038/srep39243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Li H, Zhou H, Yang Y, Wang H, Zhong N. More randomized and resilient in the topological properties of functional brain networks in patients with major depressive disorder. J. Clin. Neurosci. 2017;44:274–278. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2017.06.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Sripada RK, et al. Neural dysregulation in posttraumatic stress disorder: evidence for disrupted equilibrium between salience and default mode brain networks. Psychosom. Med. 2012;74:904–911. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318273bf33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Sawaya, H. et al. Resting-state functional connectivity of antero-medial prefrontal cortex sub-regions in major depression and relationship to emotional intelligence. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 18, 10.1093/ijnp/pyu112 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 43.Scheinost D, et al. Multimodal investigation of network level effects using intrinsic functional connectivity, anatomical covariance, and structure-to-function correlations in unmedicated major depressive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2018;43:1119–1127. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Pryazhnikov, E. et al. Longitudinal two-photon imaging in somatosensory cortex of behaving mice reveals dendritic spine formation enhancement by subchronic administration of low-dose ketamine. Sci. Rep. 8, 6464 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 45.Whitlock JR, Sutherland RJ, Witter MP, Moser MB, Moser EI. Navigating from hippocampus to parietal cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2008;105:14755–14762. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0804216105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Edwards DA, Isaacs S. Zona incerta lesions: effects on copulation, partner-preference and other socio-sexual behaviors. Behav. Brain Res. 1991;44:145–150. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(05)80019-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Schwertner A, Zortea M, Torres FV, Caumo W. Effects of subanesthetic ketamine administration on visual and auditory event-related potentials (ERP) in humans: a systematic review. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2018;12:70. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Amann LC, et al. Chronic ketamine impairs fear conditioning and produces long-lasting reductions in auditory evoked potentials. Neurobiol. Dis. 2009;35:311–317. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2009.05.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Felsen G, Mainen ZF. Neural substrates of sensory-guided locomotor decisions in the rat superior colliculus. Neuron. 2008;60:137–148. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Clements KM, Devonshire IM, Reynolds JN, Overton PG. Enhanced visual responses in the superior colliculus in an animal model of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and their suppression by d-amphetamine. Neuroscience. 2014;274:289–298. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.05.054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Brace LR, et al. Altered visual processing in a rodent model of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuroscience. 2015;303:364–377. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Lemelin S, Baruch P, Vincent A, Everett J, Vincent P. Distractibility and processing resource deficit in major depression. Evidence for two deficient attentional processing models. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 1997;185:542–548. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199709000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Condy C, Wattiez N, Rivaud-Pechoux S, Gaymard B. Ketamine-induced distractibility: an oculomotor study in monkeys. Biol. Psychiatry. 2005;57:366–372. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Goodale MA, Foreman NP, Milner AD. Visual orientation in the rat: a dissociation of deficits following cortical and collicular lesions. Exp. Brain Res. 1978;31:445–457. doi: 10.1007/BF00237301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Galvez V, et al. Repeated intranasal ketamine for treatment-resistant depression—the way to go? Results from a pilot randomised controlled trial. J. Psychopharmacol. 2018;32:397–407. doi: 10.1177/0269881118760660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Canuso CM, et al. Efficacy and safety of intranasal esketamine for the rapid reduction of symptoms of depression and suicidality in patients at imminent risk for suicide: results of a double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled study. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2018;175:620–630. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.17060720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Domany Y, et al. Repeated oral ketamine for out-patient treatment of resistant depression: randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled, proof-of-concept study. Br. J. Psychiatry. 2019;214:20–26. doi: 10.1192/bjp.2018.196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Ionescu DF, et al. Repeat-dose ketamine augmentation for treatment-resistant depression with chronic suicidal ideation: a randomized, double blind, placebo controlled trial. J. Affect. Disord. 2019;243:516–524. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.09.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Pouget P, Wattiez N, Rivaud-Pechoux S, Gaymard B. Rapid development of tolerance to sub-anaesthetic dose of ketamine: an oculomotor study in Macaque monkeys. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 2010;209:313–318. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-1797-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Gerb, S. A. et al. Ketamine tolerance in Sprague–Dawley rats after chronic administration of ketamine, morphine, or cocaine. Comp. Med. 69, 29–34 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 61.Jiang Y, et al. Short- and long-term antidepressant effects of ketamine in a rat chronic unpredictable stress model. Brain Behav. 2017;7:e00749. doi: 10.1002/brb3.749. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Yang C, Han M, Zhang JC, Ren Q, Hashimoto K. Loss of parvalbumin-immunoreactivity in mouse brain regions after repeated intermittent administration of esketamine, but not R-ketamine. Psychiatry Res. 2016;239:281–283. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.03.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Kirsch I, et al. Initial severity and antidepressant benefits: a meta-analysis of data submitted to the Food and Drug Administration. PLoS Med. 2008;5:e45. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0050045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Sartorius A, Mahlstedt MM, Vollmayr B, Henn FA, Ende G. Elevated spectroscopic glutamate/gamma-amino butyric acid in rats bred for learned helplessness. NeuroReport. 2007;18:1469–1473. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282742153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Belujon P, Grace AA. Restoring mood balance in depression: ketamine reverses deficit in dopamine-dependent synaptic plasticity. Biol. Psychiatry. 2014;76:927–936. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.04.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Brachman RA, et al. Ketamine as a prophylactic against stress-induced depressive-like behavior. Biol. Psychiatry. 2016;79:776–786. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.04.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.