Abstract
Women are approximately two times as likely to be diagnosed with major depressive disorder (MDD) compared to men. While sex differences in MDD might be driven by circulating gonadal hormones, we hypothesized that developmental hormone exposure and/or genetic sex might play a role. Mice were gonadectomized in adulthood to isolate the role of developmental hormones. We examined the effects of developmental gonadal and genetic sex on anhedonia-/depressive-like behaviors under non-stress and chronic stress conditions and performed RNA-sequencing in three mood-relevant brain regions. We used an integrative network approach to identify transcriptional modules and stress-specific hub genes regulating stress susceptibility, with a focus on whether these differed by sex. After identifying sex differences in anhedonia-/depressive-like behaviors (female > male), we show that both developmental hormone exposure (gonadal female > gonadal male) and genetic sex (XX > XY) contribute to the sex difference. The top biological pathways represented by differentially expressed genes were related to immune function; we identify which differentially expressed genes are driven by developmental gonadal or genetic sex. There was very little overlap in genes affected by chronic stress in males and females. We also identified highly co-expressed gene modules affected by stress, some of which were affected in opposite directions in males and females. Since all mice had equivalent hormone exposure in adulthood, these results suggest that sex differences in gonadal hormone exposure during sensitive developmental periods program adult sex differences in mood, and that these sex differences are independent of adult circulating gonadal hormones.
Subject terms: Psychiatric disorders, Molecular neuroscience
Introduction
Major depressive disorder (MDD) is a debilitating disease affecting ~6.7% of the US population and a leading cause of disability worldwide1. Symptoms of MDD include emotional dysregulation, cognitive deficits, and anhedonia. Anhedonia, the reduced ability to experience pleasure and lack of interest in once “rewarding” activities, is present in ~37% of MDD patients, and often predicts poor treatment response2. The mesocorticolimbic system is a primary circuit of emotion and mood regulation, comprised of the prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices, hippocampus, amygdala, dorsal striatum, and nucleus accumbens (NAc), along with other regions3. Evidence suggests dysfunction of the mesocorticolimbic system in MDD [e.g., refs. 4–6]. For example, neuroimaging studies reveal that individuals with MDD have sustained amygdala hyperactivity, altered prefrontal cortex (PFC) functionality, and reduced activity of the NAc in response to rewarding stimuli7–9.
Women are twice as likely to be diagnosed with MDD and often report higher frequency of symptoms and increased symptom severity10–12. These sex differences in MDD might be driven by gonadal hormone differences between men and women (i.e., transient activational hormone effects). Indeed, both female and male gonadal hormones influence mood in humans (e.g., premenstrual dysphoric disorder in women13,14 and low testosterone in men15,16) and in animals (e.g., refs. 17,18). However, MDD prevalence remains higher in women across life stages and hormonal states19,20, suggesting that other sex-related factors contribute to higher rates of MDD in women. These sex differences could be driven by (1) permanent organizational effects of gonadal hormones during sensitive developmental periods and/or (2) effects of genetic sex21,22. Independent investigation of these factors is not possible using wild-type mice, as genetic sex (XX vs. XY) determines gonadal sex (ovaries vs. testes). However, by using the Four Core genotypes (FCG) mice, the contribution of genetic sex (XX/XY) and gonadal sex (ovaries/testes) can be independently investigated23. In FCG mice, the testes-determining Sry gene was spontaneously deleted from the Y chromosome and placed back onto an autosome. Thus, in these mice, the contribution of these sex-related factors to behavior can be independently investigated. Since we were particularly focused on the effects of developmental gonadal sex, we gonadectomized (GDX) mice to remove the potential confounding activational effects of adult circulating gonadal hormones. Using FCG mice, we previously found an effect of genetic sex on anxiety-like behavior17.
To understand the contribution of gonadal sex and genetic sex to depressive-/anhedonia-like behavior and related transcriptional changes, we utilized a chronic stress exposure, behavior testing, and RNA-sequencing (RNA-seq) of several major nodes in the mesocorticolimbic circuit. We found sex differences in depressive-/anhedonia-like behavior were driven by both developmental hormone exposure and genetic sex. We also identified potential candidate genes underlying these behavioral differences and discovered gene modules affected by stress, some of which are altered in opposite directions in males and females. Network analysis predicted potential upstream regulators of these sex-specific effects on gene expression patterns. Together, these data provide new insights into how chronic stress differentially affects males and females.
Materials and methods
Mice
Mice were group-housed (3–5 mice/cage) and maintained under standard conditions (12:12 h light/dark cycle; lights on 7 a.m.; 22 ± 1 °C, food and water ad libitum), in accordance with University of Pittsburgh Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. FCG mice (C57BL/6J background) from our in-house colony were used for all studies; these mice were originally obtained from The Jackson Laboratory (Bar Harbor, ME). We indicate both genetic and gonadal sex for each genotype: (1) XX = XX Female; (2) XY− = XY Female; (3) XXSry = XX Male; (4) XY−Sry = XY Male.
Experimental design
We tested the effects of genetic sex and gonadal sex on adult anhedonia-/depressive-like behaviors and on gene expression in mood-relevant brain regions (Fig. 1a). At ~12-weeks old, mice were GDX to remove endogenous sources of gonadal hormones and to ensure equivalent gonadal hormone exposure during testing. While this approach does not allow us to assess contributions of circulating hormones, it is necessary to isolate potential effects of developmental gonadal sex, which was our focus here. In behavior cohorts, we assessed baseline anhedonia-/depressive-like behaviors [3 weeks after GDX; novelty-suppressed feeding (NSF; n = 23 XX Females; n = 20 XY Males; 22 XY Female; 16 XX Males), and social conditioned place preference (social CPP; n = 21 XX Females; n = 19 XY Males; 20 XY Females; 15 XX Males); 2 days rest between behavioral tests], exposed mice to 8 weeks of unpredictable chronic mild stress (UCMS), and then assessed stress-induced anhedonia-/depressive-like behaviors. Behaviors were examined ~3–6 h after lights on, with genotypes evenly distributed across test days. For the gene expression cohort, half the mice were exposed to 8 weeks UCMS and half were left unstressed (n = 9 per FCG genotype per stress group, n = 72 mice total); we sacrificed the mice, isolated tissue punches of the PFC, BLA, and NAc, and performed RNA-seq. Sample sizes were chosen based on previous publications17,24. Experimenters were blinded to genotype and stress group throughout the experiments and testing order was counterbalanced across cohorts. Animal use was conducted in accordance with the National Institute of Health guidelines for the care and use of laboratory animals and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Pittsburgh.
Unpredictable chronic mild stress
We used UCMS to induce an elevated anxiety-/depressive-like state in our mice, as performed previously in our lab17,25,26. UCMS elicits homologous features associated with human depression. Mice were exposed to a random schedule of mild psychosocial stressors over the course of 8 weeks. Stressors included reduced space, predator odor, 45° tilted cage, social stress (exposure to dirty bedding from unfamiliar mice of the same sex), wet or no bedding, mild restraint in 50 ml conical tube with air holes, forced bath (~4 cm of 25° water), and light cycle disruption. We assessed weight and fur weekly to track progression of the UCMS syndrome. A detailed schedule of UCMS is provided in Table S1.
Novelty-suppressed feeding
Mice were food restricted overnight and allowed to habituate to the testing room for 1 h prior to testing. Mice were then placed into the corner of a 50.8 cm × 50.8 cm novel arena, and the latency to eat a food pellet at the brightly lit (750 lux) center of the arena was recorded (12-min maximum). After testing, we recorded homecage food consumption, which did not differ between groups under non-stress (p > 0.15) or stress conditions (p > 0.35).
Social conditioned place preference
Social CPP was performed similar to27. The premise is that mice prefer bedding in which they were previously socially housed versus bedding in which they were singly housed. On day 1, mice were allowed to freely explore an arena with two equally sized chambers (40 cm × 20 cm × 20 cm) for 30 min. Each chamber contained a novel bedding and time in each chamber was recorded using EthoVision (Noldus). On day 2, mice were socially housed with their cage-mates with the bedding that they spent the least amount of time in during day 1 (biased design). On day 3, mice were singly housed with the bedding they spent the most amount of time in during day 1. On day 4, mice were allowed to freely explore the testing arena containing each novel bedding and amount of time in each chamber was recorded. The social CPP score was calculated as the time spent in the social bedding post-conditioning minus the time spent in social bedding pre-conditioning. Six mice were excluded because they were socially housed in their preferred bedding during pre- or post-stress testing (pre-stress: 2 XX Females, 1 XY Female, 1 XY Male excluded; post-stress: 2 XX Females excluded). The pre-stress novel beddings were ALPHA-Dri® (Shepherd Specialty Papers) and Bed O’Cobs (Andersons Lab Bedding). The post-stress novel beddings were Aspen Shavings (Northeastern Products) and Paperchip® Soft Texture (Shepherd Specialty Papers); these are distinct from the pre-stress beddings to maintain novelty27.
RNA-seq
Brains were flash frozen on dry ice at sacrifice and stored at −80 °C. 160-µm-thick rostro-caudal sections were obtained using a cryostat and a 1 mm bore tissue punch was used to isolate the prefrontal cortex (PFC; includes cingulate and prelimbic cortices; Bregma +2.34 mm to +0.50 mm), NAc (Bregma +0.74 mm to +0.38 mm), and BLA (Bregma −0.94 mm to −1.82 mm)28. RNA was extracted using RNeasy Plus Micro kits with Qiashredders (Qiagen). RNA integrity was assessed using a Bioanalyzer (Agilent) and concentration was determined using a Qubit fluorometer (Invitrogen). We pooled RNA from three mice for each RNA-seq run, with three biological replicates per group (3 replicates × 4 genotypes × 3 brain regions × 2 stress conditions = 72 samples). Libraries were constructed using TruSeq Stranded mRNA (PolyA+) and sequenced by Illumina NextSeq 500. All samples were sequenced across all lanes to avoid lane bias by a technician blind to treatment groups (single-end). The read length was 75 base pairs. Average sample coverage was 30.72 million reads before alignment. FastQC (v0.11.3) was performed to assess the quality of the data. Per base sequence quality was high (Phred score generally >30), indicating good quality of the experimental data. TopHat2 (ref. 29) (TopHat v2.0.9) was used to align the read to the reference (Mus musculus UCSC mm10, downloaded at https://support.illumina.com/sequencing/sequencing_software/igenome.html) using default parameters. The resulting bam files after alignment were converted to expression count data using HTseq30 (HTseq v0.6.1) with default union mode. A total of 72 samples (24 BLA, 24 FC, and 24 NAc) with 24,421 genes were preprocessed together. Low expression genes (25% of lower expressed genes with smallest sum of counts for all 72 samples) were filtered out and 18,337 genes remained. The RNA-seq count data were transformed to log2 count data using voom function of the Bioconductor limma package31,32.
qPCR
We validated a subset of genes identified with RNA-seq using qPCR. Fifteen microliters of RNA per sample was converted into cDNA using the iScript cDNA Synthesis kit (Biorad, Hercules, CA) and quantities of cDNA were normalized across samples. qPCR was run on a CFX 96 Real-Time PCR (Biorad, Hercules, CA) using SsoAdvanced Universal SYBR Green Supermix (Biorad, Hercules, CA). We ran each gene and sample combination in triplicate and derived the mean of the replicates. Results were then calculated as the geometric mean of the relative intensities compared to two housekeeping genes, cyclophilin and actin. Importantly these housekeeping genes did not differ by sex or stress. We then transformed this value to the arbitrary signal using the formula (2−dCT × 10,000). Primer efficiency and specificity were verified; primer sequences are included in Table S2.
Data analysis
For behavioral analyses, we used two-way repeated measures (pre- vs. post-UCMS) ANOVA with 4 groups (XX Female, XY Female, XX Male, XY Male); significant group effects were followed by planned two-group comparisons (Fisher’s LSD for effects of genotype based on pairwise comparisons between group means—e.g., XX Female vs. XY Male, XX Female vs. XY Female, XX Male vs. XY Male, etc.; and Sidak’s multiple comparison test for within genotype effect of stress—e.g., Pre-UCMS vs. Post-UCMS XX Female, XY Male, etc.). All behavioral data are expressed as mean ± SEM and statistical significance was set at p < 0.05. Trend level was set at p < 0.1. Genes with p < 0.05 and fold change >1.3 were considered differentially expressed (DE). INGENUITY® Pathway Analysis (Qiagen) was used to determine canonical pathways represented by DE genes as follows: (1) 18,321 annotated genes remaining after filtering were used as background data set; (2) IPA pathways with <20 genes were not included. We used rank–rank hypergeometric overlap (RRHO) as a threshold-free method to evaluate the overlap of differential expression patterns across pairs of brain regions33,34. RRHO identifies overlap between two ranked lists of differential gene expression. The genes are ranked by the −log10(p value) multiplied by the effect size direction. Each brain region from all stressed mice were analyzed together through weighted gene co-expression network analysis (WGCNA)35,36. Module preservation analysis was performed to determine whether the modules identified were robust37. Analysis was based on the module size and compared to an independent network, where Z-scores measure whether a given module is significantly robust relative to a random set of genes. Z-scores were calculated for each module and ranked accordingly; summary Z-scores more than 10 are considered highly preserved, and these are the only modules included in downstream analyses. The module differential connectivity (MDC) metric was used to quantify differences in co-expression between non-stressed and stressed samples; MDC is the ratio of the connectivity of all gene pairs in a module from stressed mice to that of the same gene pairs from non-stressed mice38. To compare module connectivity between stressed and non-stressed mice, subsets of adjacent matrices were generated for sex and stress or non-stress mice, then directly compared using the ratio of summed lower triangular values. These summed lower triangular values represent the MDC metric. Connectivity is permuted to derive p values by randomizing values across the lower triangular matrix. Genes within each module are also permuted, and the p value for the MDC is compared to the null distribution to determine modules which were gained or lost between stressed and non-stressed mice. MDC > 1 indicates gain of connectivity, while MDC < 1 indicates loss of connectivity. To statistically test the significance of MDC, we estimated the p value based on two types of shuffling schemes: (1) shuffled samples—adjacent matrix with non-random nodes but random connections; (2) shuffled genes—adjacent matrix with random nodes but non-random connections. Data were permuted 1000 times. P values were converted to q values following Benjamini Hochberg procedure, and MDC q < 0.05 was considered significant. ARACNe was used to identify hub and stress-specific hub genes for network analysis39. Briefly, a gene is considered a hub if the N-hob neighborhood nodes (NHNN) for that gene is significantly higher than the average NHNN. A hub gene is considered a stress-specific hub if it attains hub status only in the chronic stress condition. Cytoscape 3.5.0 was used to generate networks, with correlations greater than 0.95 plotted in the networks.
Results
Sex differences in behavior are driven by both developmental gonadal sex and genetic sex
FCG mice were run through anhedonia-/depressive-like behavioral assays under non-stress conditions, and then exposed to UCMS and re-tested on the same behavioral assays. Since mice were GDX in adulthood, any gonadal sex differences are considered the organizational effects of hormones acting during sensitive developmental periods. In the NSF, the two-way repeated measures ANOVA indicated a main effect of genotype (F(3, 77) = 3.806; p < 0.05) and an effect of time (pre- vs. post-stress; F(1, 77) = 13.83; p < 0.001; Fig. 1b). We first established that we could detect a sex difference in latency to eat by comparing “pure” females (XX Females; genetic and gonadal females) to “pure” males (XY Male; genetic and gonadal males). Indeed, XX Females had a longer latency to eat compared to XY Males (XX Females vs. XY Males; t(77) = 2.207; p < 0.05). We next asked whether this sex difference was due to an effect of gonadal or genetic sex. We find that the sex difference in latency to eat was recapitulated in both XX Males (XX Males vs. XX Females; t(77) = 2.97; p < 0.01) and XY Females (XY Females vs. XX Females; t(77) = 2.207; p < 0.01); this suggests that both developmental gonadal sex and genetic sex contribute to the sex difference in latency to eat in NSF. In the social CPP test of social reward, the two-way repeated measures ANOVA indicated an effect of genotype (F(3, 72) = 7.511); p < 0.001) and an interaction of genotype and time (pre- vs. post-stress; F(3, 72) = 4.543; p < 0.01; Fig.1c). We confirmed that there was a sex difference in social CPP score (XX Females vs. XY Males; t(72) = 3.294; p < 0.01) by comparing pure females (XX Females) to pure males (XY Males). This sex difference in social CPP score was recapitulated in XX Males (XX Males vs. XX Females; t(4.17) = 4.17; p < 0.0001), but not in XY Females (p > 0.25 vs XX Females). Thus, the sex difference in social CPP is driven by developmental gonadal sex. Post hoc analyses for the significant interaction of genotype and time (pre- vs. post-stress) indicated an effect of stress only in XX Males (t(72) = 3.046; p < 0.01).
Gonadal and genetic sex differences in gene expression under non-stress conditions
To identify potential molecular drivers related to sex differences in behavior, we performed RNA-seq in a separate cohort of mice. First, we identified genes with overall sex differences in expression by comparing XX Females and XY Males; there were 505 DE genes in BLA, 516 DE genes in PFC, and 610 DE genes in NAc (Fig. 2a–c; Table S3). We next investigated whether these sex differences were driven by either gonadal sex or genetic sex. For all three brain regions, roughly 25% of sex differences in gene expression were driven by genetic sex (Table S4) and 23–31% (BLA-NAc; Table S5) were driven by developmental gonadal sex. As expected, we observed that sex differences in X and Y chromosome genes were driven by genetic sex (e.g., Ddx3y, Eif2s3x, Kdm5d, Xist). We also found sex differences in expression of many activity-dependent genes (e.g., Arc, Fos, Egr2, Npas4); these sex differences were driven by developmental gonadal sex and more highly expressed in gonadal females. Although the exact genes driven by genetic or gonadal sex differ, pathway analysis revealed that the genes driven by genetic sex and gonadal sex typically involve immune function and inflammation (Fig. 2a–c). We validated a subset of genetic and gonadal sex differences in gene expression under non-stress conditions using qPCR (Fig. S1A; Table S12); 63% of genes were significantly different by qPCR and all showed the same direction of effect as RNA-seq.
Gonadal and genetic sex differences in gene expression under chronic stress conditions
We also investigated sex differences in gene expression under chronic stress conditions. We first identified genes with overall sex differences by comparing XX Females and XY Males; there were 732 DE genes in BLA, 1464 DE genes in PFC, and 338 DE genes in NAc (Fig. 2d–f; Table S6). We next asked if gonadal sex or genetic sex drives any of these observed sex differences in gene expression. Overall, the contribution of genetic sex to these differences were lower relative to non-stress conditions: 14% and 16% for BLA and PFC, respectively, with 35% in the NAc (Fig. 2d–f; Table S7). Gonadal sex accounted for 28% in BLA, 16% in PFC, and 29% in NAc (Fig. 2d–f; Table S8). As predicted, across brain regions, genetic sex drives sex differences in X and Y chromosome gene expression. Notably, none of the activity-dependent genes that were DE under non-stress conditions were DE in any brain region under chronic stress. Pathway analysis revealed that the genes driven by genetic sex and gonadal sex typically involve immune function and inflammation (Fig. 2a–c). We validated a subset of genetic and gonadal sex differences in gene expression under stress conditions using qPCR (Fig. S1B); 50% of genes were significantly different by qPCR and all showed the same direction of effect as RNA-seq.
Sex-specific transcriptional effects of chronic stress
We examined stress-induced transcriptional changes to investigate whether there are sex-specific effects of stress on genes in each brain region. Notably, our behavioral analyses suggest that the chronic stress exposure impacted males and females similarly (i.e., increased anhedonia-/depressive-like behavior after chronic stress). While males and females arrive at the same behavioral endpoint, we asked whether similar molecular changes underlie these stress-induced effects in males and females. A direct comparison of stress DE genes in XX Females and XY Males revealed just 1–10% overlap across the three brain regions (Fig. 3a–c; Table S9). The biological pathways associated with stress DE genes are also distinct in females and males (Fig. 3d–f). We next used the FCG mice to determine whether any stress-sensitive pathways are recapitulated when we examine XX mice versus XY mice (Table S10) and gonadal males versus gonadal females (Table S11). The strongest effect of stress was in the BLA of females (Fig. 3a); pathway analysis determined that both gonadal and genetic sex strongly contribute to the female effect of stress in the BLA (Fig. 3g). For males, the strongest effect of stress was in the PFC (Fig. 3b), although the contribution of gonadal sex or genetic sex to the effect of stress in the PFC was less pronounced and indistinguishable between factors (Fig. 3h). Stress-induced DE overlap and biological pathways for gonadal females versus males and XX versus XY mice are provided for all brain regions in Figs. S2 and S3; these also show overlap of stress-induced DE genes with those showing sex differences under non-stress conditions. We validated a subset of genes exhibiting effects of stress using qPCR (Fig. S1C); 64% of genes were significantly different by qPCR and all but three showed the same direction of effect as RNA-seq.
Sex-specific transcriptional synchrony across mood-relevant brain regions
Since mood disorders often involve circuit-level changes across multiple brain regions, we next examined transcriptional coherence across brain regions in response to stress. We used the RRHO test to identify patterns of gene expression changes in response to stress33,34. RRHO identifies patterns of upregulated and downregulated genes without the restriction of significance thresholds. We used RRHO to investigate sex-specific patterns of cross-brain region transcriptional synchrony in response to stress. RRHO identifies four possible patterns: (1) overlap in genes that are increased by stress in both regions (coherence); (2) overlap in genes that are decreased by stress in both regions (coherence); (3) overlap in genes that are increased in region 1, but decreased in region 2 (anti-coherence); (4) overlap in genes that are decreased in region 1, but increased in region 2 (anti-coherence; Fig. 4a). RRHO was used previously to investigate transcriptional coherence in mood-related brain regions associated with stress susceptibility and resilience in mice, which revealed molecular mechanisms critical for depression-like behaviors40. We first examined stress-induced transcriptional coherence between brain regions in XX Females and XY Males. In XX Females, we found strong transcriptional anti-coherence between the NAc and BLA. In other words, in XX Females, stress induced opposite changes in expression of the same genes in the BLA and NAc (Fig. 4b); this pattern did not exist in XY Males. Rather, XY Males showed a weak pattern of coherence between the NAc and BLA (Fig. 4c). Additionally, in XY Males, there was coherence between the PFC and NAc; this pattern did not exist in XX Females. We next asked whether these cross-brain region transcriptional coherence patterns are recapitulated by gonadal or genetic sex. For the anti-coherence between the BLA and NAc observed for XX Females, this pattern is recapitulated in both gonadal females and XX mice (Fig. 4d). For the coherence between the BLA and NAc observed in XY Males, the pattern is weakly recapitulated in both gonadal males and in XY mice (Fig. 4d). For the coherence between the NAc and PFC in XY Males, the pattern is recapitulated in only XY mice.
Co-expression analysis identifies gene modules that are affected by stress in a sex-specific manner
We next identified gene modules differentially affected by stress in males and females. First, we generated a gene co-expression network using all stressed samples from all brain regions; this stressed network consisted of 30 gene modules, with 26 modules surviving preservation analysis (Fig. 5a). Given our focus on potential sex-specific effects of stress, we next used module differential connectivity (MDC) to ask whether module connectivity increased or decreased as a function of stress; this analysis was performed separately in XX Females and XY Males. If a module gains connectivity as a function of stress, it exhibits more coordinated expression in stressed compared to non-stressed mice. On the other hand, a module that loses connectivity as a function of stress is more highly coordinated in non-stressed than in stressed mice. In XX Females, 11 modules (42%) gained connectivity, 9 modules (35%) lost connectivity, and 6 modules (23%) did not change as a function of stress. In XY Males, 22 modules (85%) gained connectivity, 1 module lost connectivity (4%), and 3 modules (12%) did not change with stress exposure (Fig. 5b).
We recently found that depressed men and women exhibit opposite patterns of transcriptional changes across corticolimbic brain regions41. Thus, we were particularly interested in gene modules which were lost or gained in opposing directions based on sex in chronically stressed FCG mice. We asked if stress exposure induced gain of module connectivity in one sex, but loss of connectivity in the opposite sex. Indeed, there were five modules that gained connectivity in XY Males, but lost connectivity in XX Females (yellow, salmon, pink, magenta, and lightcyan). The yellow module was particularly interesting, as it showed enrichment for differentially expressed genes (Fig. 5c). This module was enriched in genes related to Caveolar-mediated endocytosis, Ephrin receptor signaling, and Fcγ receptor mediated phagocytosis (Fig. 5d). We decided to examine the yellow module in more detail using network analysis, depicting only “hub genes” for XX Females or XY Males. Based on their high level of co-expression within the module, hub genes are predicted to control expression of other genes within the module. “Stress-specific hub genes” are predicted to control expression of other genes within the module in stressed mice, but not under non-stress conditions; these stress-specific hubs might be key upstream regulators driving divergent sex-dependent stress effects. The composition and topography of the yellow hub network is markedly different in XX Females and XY Males (Fig. 5e). Not only is the network much more correlated in XX Females than XY Males, but many of the hub genes are distinct (overlap of ~20% of hub genes). This is consistent with the yellow module gaining connectivity in stressed XX Females, but losing connectivity in stressed XY Males (see MDC results). Examination of the stress-specific hub genes in this network provides key leads for potential upstream drivers of the sex-specific response to stress. For instance, the yellow module has 11 stress-specific hub genes in XX Females and 6 stress-specific hub genes in XY Males; Papl is the only gene that is a stress-specific hub in both XX Females and XY Males. Interestingly, the Papl gene is located within an anxiety quantitative trait loci (QTL) as well as a neuroinflammation QTL42,43. Two of the stress-specific hub genes in XX Females are related to synapse development (Thbs1 and Cadps2), which is notable given previous evidence for synapse dysfunction in MDD, with potential sex specificity41,44.
Discussion
Recent studies in the human brain suggest the molecular pathology of depression is distinct in men and women41,45, although the contribution of gonadal and/or genetic sex to these overall sex differences is less clear. We found that sex differences in anhedonia-/depressive-like behavior are driven by both developmental gonadal hormone exposure (i.e., gonadal sex) and genetic sex. Our transcriptome analysis revealed gonadal and genetic sex differences in mood-relevant brain regions, providing molecular leads that may drive sex differences in behavior. Further, we found distinct brain molecular profiles in males and females exposed to chronic stress. This finding is consistent with the human literature of distinct molecular pathology in depressed men and women and suggests contributions of both genetic and gonadal sex.
Since depression is known to involve circuit-level disruption, examining the global impact of stress on molecular pathways within these circuits in the brain is informative. Indeed, our results suggest that stress impacts circuit-level mesocorticolimbic function in a sex-specific fashion, with strong anti-coherence of stress-induced transcriptional changes between the NAc and BLA in XX Females, a pattern not found in XY Males. Rather, XY Males showed synchrony between the NAc and PFC. The source of these alterations is unclear. For example, these may be due to stress causing sex-dependent changes in the functional connections between or within these regions (e.g., neural activity, cellular and synaptic plasticity), or alterations in the activity of upstream molecular regulators of overlapping molecular signaling pathways40,46–50. Another possibility is that transcriptional coherence between brain regions reflects resting state activity patterns, although this has not been experimentally tested. Indeed, depressed subjects exhibit changes in resting state connectivity. Patients with an anxious subtype of MDD have decreased frontoamygdala connectivity, while patients with an anhedonia subtype have frontostriatal hyperconnectivity51. Interestingly, we see that females have reduced coherence between the PFC and BLA compared to males (possibly similar to reduced frontoamygdala connectivity in MDD), and that XY Males have increased coherence between the PFC and NAc (possibly similar to hyperactivity in frontostriatal network in MDD). Together, these findings suggest that stress produces sex-specific changes in mesocorticolimbic circuitry and provide novel circuit-based hypotheses to test in future studies.
The identified sex differences in gene expression provide molecular leads for pathways which may underlie observed sex differences in behavior. Many biological pathways represented by these genes are related to immune function; this is true for sex differences within non-stress or stress conditions, effects of stress on gene expression, as well as for the identified yellow module that shows a sex-specific effect of stress. This is especially interesting given the immune/inflammatory hypothesis of MDD, which posits that elevated peripheral inflammation contributes to the pathophysiology of depression. Reports show that MDD subjects have altered peripheral cytokine profiles (e.g. refs. 52,53), and there is high co-morbidity between immune disorders and MDD54. The story, however, is complicated, with evidence for both pro-inflammatory55 and immunosuppressive profiles in MDD52,53. One factor that might contribute to this heterogeneity is sex. For instance, male MDD seems to be characterized by inflammation-related immune profile not seen in depressed females (e.g., ref. 56). There are also reported sex differences in peripheral immune markers57,58. Investigation of peripheral pro-inflammatory cytokines in depressed subjects showed sex differences in correlation of these markers and MDD severity58. Similar sex-specific findings were reported for healthy subjects given a peripheral immune challenge; females experienced more depression-related symptoms, and there was a positive correlation of pro-inflammatory interleukin-6 (IL-6) levels and depression-related symptoms only in females59,60. Together, this evidence suggests that intrinsic sex-related factors might interact with immune function to influence mood.
Under non-stress conditions, there were consistent and robust gonadal sex differences in expression of immediate early genes (IEGs), including Arc, fos, Egr1, and Npas4, with gonadal females having higher expression than gonadal males. Prior to tissue collection, these mice were handled similarly; notably, there was no acute stress exposure, except for the short transport of their cages from holding to procedure rooms. Since IEG expression is considered a marker of neuronal activation, one possibility is there is an inherent sex difference in neuronal activity, which is dependent on developmental gonadal hormones. Basal patterns of neuronal activity are linked to differential waves of activity-dependent gene transcription involving IEGs61. For example, Egr1 has relatively high basal expression across the brain, which is thought to be in response to normal neural activity62–65. Further, many of these IEGs act as transcription factors to activate molecular pathways involved in neural plasticity in response to various stimuli66,67, including drugs of abuse and stress68–70, and previously linked to anhedonia, a core feature of MDD and addiction disorders. Notably, altered EGR1 has been found in the PFC of MDD patients71 and in other brain regions in animal models of stress-induced depressive-like behavior72–76. Npas4 is another IEG of interest and may be a key driver of excitatory and inhibitory balance77, potentially dependent on sex and also affected by stress78,79. Another intriguing possibility is the upregulation of these IEGs in gonadal females may be a result of a more permissive, stable chromatin state. Indeed, sex differences in chromatin accessibility and transcription are found in other tissues80,81.
The WGCNA and MDC analyses suggest that several gene modules are oppositely affected by stress in XX Females and XY Males. This result was particularly intriguing to us given our recent finding of opposite changes in gene expression in the brains of depressed men and women41. Thus, we decided to focus on these oppositely affected modules for downstream analyses. When examining enrichment for sex differences within these oppositely affected modules, the yellow module stood out as being enriched for sex differences across mesocorticolimbic brain regions. The top pathway represented by the genes in the yellow module was related to caveolar-mediated endocytosis. This is notable given that caveolin proteins traffic and cluster receptors to the cell membrane, including D1 dopamine receptors, M1 muscarinic receptors, and estrogen receptors82–84. Interestingly, a recent study identified estrogen receptor alpha (ERα) in the NAc as a top transcriptional regulator of stress resilience, with higher ERα conferring resilience85. This study also found that ERα overexpression in the NAc reproduced the transcriptional signature consistent with resilience in male, but not female mice. Whether these stress-resilience properties are mediated via intracellular or membrane-bound ERα (which would rely on caveolar-mediated processes) remain to be determined.
The gene co-expression network analysis of the yellow module identified several genes that might drive sex differences in response to stress. For instance, within the yellow module, there are two synaptic-related genes (Thbs1 and Cadps2) that are stress-specific hubs in XX Females, but not XY Males; these genes are also differentially expressed by sex. Notably, synaptic dysfunction has been implicated in MDD. There are reduced dendritic processes/spine synapses in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in MDD44. Chronic stress in male rodents recapitulates this dendritic pathology86,87, and it is hypothesized that dendritic spine alterations contribute to MDD symptoms88,89. However, we found opposite gene expression changes in MDD females, with increased expression of synapse-related genes90. Our results in males and females with MDD are consistent with rodent studies in which stressed males have decreased dendritic spine complexity, but exactly the opposite occurs in stressed females86,91. Together, our network analysis points towards potential upstream regulators of these sex-specific disease/stress effects on synapses.
It is important to note that these behavioral tests were developed in male mice; thus, it is possible that these specific tests do not tap into mood-related behavior in females. For instance, it is possible that females exhibit either different motivation for food in the NSF or differences in learning in the social CPP. However, we note that XY Females and XX Males respond similarly to XY Males, suggesting that these behavior tests “work” in both sexes. Additionally, it is possible that more than one pairing in each social context may be necessary for social CPP. We based our social CPP design on previously published papers27,92,93. For instance, using a similar design, Nardou et al.93 reported robust social conditioning in mice which required no additional social pairings. Here, we observed social conditioning prior to stress exposure that was largely lost following stress. However, we acknowledge that including additional conditioning days could have improved the level of social conditioning post-stress. In any case, sex differences in the behavioral tests assessed here exist, and they are driven by both developmental gonadal sex and genetic sex. Notably, all mice used in these studies underwent GDX several weeks prior to behavioral testing, stress exposure, and/or gene expression analyses. GDX may impact the peripheral stress response (i.e., corticosterone), which may have downstream effects on molecular/behavioral outcomes. Future studies will investigate whether the sex-specific effects on behavior and gene expression are mediated by changes in corticosterone. Interestingly, Hodes et al.94 found sex differences in mood-related behavior after 6 days of subchronic variable stress (SCVS). It would be interesting to use FCG mice to determine whether the sex differences after SCVS are due to genetic or gonadal sex.
To conclude, our study points towards a role for both developmental hormones and genetic sex in programming adult sex differences in mood. The cross-brain region coherence analysis suggests that stress induces transcriptional synchrony between the PFC and NAc in males, but synchrony between the BLA and NAc in females; this suggests that stress affects mesocorticolimbic circuitry differently in males and females. The impact of these circuitry changes is yet to be determined. We also identified potential upstream regulators of observed sex differences, and future studies will determine their impact on stress susceptibility.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) MH103473 (to M.L.S.) and MH120066 (to M.L.S.), by the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) DA038654 (to R.W.L.) and by the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (NHLBI) HL150432 (to R.W.L.). M.L.S. and R.W.L. were also supported by NARSAD Young Investigator Awards from the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation. The funding agencies had no role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish and preparation of the manuscript. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIMH or the National Institutes of Health.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Ryan W. Logan, Email: loganrw@upmc.edu
Marianne L. Seney, Email: seneyml@upmc.edu
Supplementary information
Supplementary Information accompanies this paper at (10.1038/s41398-020-0742-9).
References
- 1.Murray CJ, Lopez AD. Evidence-based health policy-lessons from the Global Burden of Disease Study. Science. 1996;274:740–743. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5288.740. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Spijker J, Bijl RV, de Graaf R, Nolen WA. Determinants of poor 1-year outcome of DSM-III-R major depression in the general population: results of the Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study (NEMESIS) Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2001;103:122–130. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2001.103002122.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Seminowicz DA, et al. Limbic-frontal circuitry in major depression: a path modeling metanalysis. Neuroimage. 2004;22:409–418. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Horger BA, Roth RH. The role of mesoprefrontal dopamine neurons in stress. Crit. Rev. Neurobiol. 1996;10:395–418. doi: 10.1615/critrevneurobiol.v10.i3-4.60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Di Chiara G, Loddo P, Tanda G. Reciprocal changes in prefrontal and limbic dopamine responsiveness to aversive and rewarding stimuli after chronic mild stress: implications for the psychobiology of depression. Biol. Psychiatry. 1999;46:1624–1633. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00236-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Nestler EJ. Role of the brain’s reward circuitry in depression: transcriptional mechanisms. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 2015;124:151–170. doi: 10.1016/bs.irn.2015.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Siegle GJ, Thompson W, Carter CS, Steinhauer SR, Thase ME. Increased amygdala and decreased dorsolateral prefrontal BOLD responses in unipolar depression: related and independent features. Biol. Psychiatry. 2007;61:198–209. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.05.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Robinson OJ, Cools R, Carlisi CO, Sahakian BJ, Drevets WC. Ventral striatum response during reward and punishment reversal learning in unmedicated major depressive disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2012;169:152–159. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11010137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Tremblay LK, et al. Functional neuroanatomical substrates of altered reward processing in major depressive disorder revealed by a dopaminergic probe. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2005;62:1228–1236. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.11.1228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kornstein SG, et al. Gender differences in chronic major and double depression. J. Affect. Disord. 2000;60:1–11. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(99)00158-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Angst J. AD-M. Do the diagnostic criteria determine the sex ratio in depression? J. Affect. Disord. 1984;7:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(84)90040-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Young MA, Fogg LF, Scheftner WA, Keller MB, Fawcett JA. Sex differences in the lifetime prevalence of depression: does varying the diagnostic criteria reduce the female/male ratio? J. Affect. Disord. 1990;18:187–192. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(90)90035-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Joffe H, Cohen LS. Estrogen, serotonin, and mood disturbance: where is the therapeutic bridge? Biol. Psychiatry. 1998;44:798–811. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00169-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Rubinow DR, Schmidt PJ, Roca CA. Estrogen-serotonin interactions: implications for affective regulation. Biol. Psychiatry. 1998;44:839–850. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00162-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Sternbach H. Age-associated testosterone decline in men: clinical issues for psychiatry. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1998;155:1310–1318. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.10.1310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Wang C, et al. Testosterone replacement therapy improves mood in hypogonadal men—a clinical research center study. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 1996;81:3578–3583. doi: 10.1210/jcem.81.10.8855804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Seney ML, et al. The role of genetic sex in affect regulation and expression of GABA-related genes across species. Front. Psychiatry. 2013;4:104. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Viau V, Meaney MJ. Variations in the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal response to stress during the estrous cycle in the rat. Endocrinology. 1991;129:2503–2511. doi: 10.1210/endo-129-5-2503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Kessler RC, McGonagle KA, Swartz M, Blazer DG, Nelson CB. Sex and depression in the National Comorbidity Survey. I: Lifetime prevalence, chronicity and recurrence. J. Affect. Disord. 1993;29:85–96. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(93)90026-g. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Castro-Costa E, Lima-Costa MF, Carvalhais S, Firmo JO, Uchoa E. Factors associated with depressive symptoms measured by the 12-item General Health Questionnaire in community-dwelling older adults (The Bambui Health Aging Study) Rev. Bras. Psiquiatr. 2008;30:104–109. doi: 10.1590/s1516-44462008005000007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Arnold AP. The organizational-activational hypothesis as the foundation for a unified theory of sexual differentiation of all mammalian tissues. Horm. Behav. 2009;55:570–578. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.03.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.McCarthy MM, Arnold AP. Reframing sexual differentiation of the brain. Nat. Neurosci. 2011;14:677–683. doi: 10.1038/nn.2834. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Arnold AP, Chen X. What does the “four core genotypes” mouse model tell us about sex differences in the brain and other tissues? Front. Neuroendocrinol. 2009;30:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2008.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Seney ML, Walsh C, Stolakis R, Sibille E. Neonatal testosterone partially organizes sex differences in stress-induced emotionality in mice. Neurobiol. Dis. 2012;46:486–496. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2012.02.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Seney ML, Ekong KI, Ding Y, Tseng GC, Sibille E. Sex chromosome complement regulates expression of mood-related genes. Biol. Sex. Differ. 2013;4:20. doi: 10.1186/2042-6410-4-20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Puralewski R, Vasilakis G, Seney ML. Sex-related factors influence expression of mood-related genes in the basolateral amygdala differentially depending on age and stress exposure. Biol. Sex Differ. 2016;7:50. doi: 10.1186/s13293-016-0106-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Dolen G, Darvishzadeh A, Huang KW, Malenka RC. Social reward requires coordinated activity of nucleus accumbens oxytocin and serotonin. Nature. 2013;501:179–184. doi: 10.1038/nature12518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Franklin, K. B. J. & Paxinos, G. The Mouse Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates, compact 3rd edn (Elsevier, New York, NY, USA, 2008).
- 29.Kim D, et al. TopHat2: accurate alignment of transcriptomes in the presence of insertions, deletions and gene fusions. Genome Biol. 2013;14:R36. doi: 10.1186/gb-2013-14-4-r36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Anders S, Pyl PT, Huber W. HTSeq-a Python framework to work with high-throughput sequencing data. Bioinformatics. 2015;31:166–169. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btu638. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Law CW, Chen Y, Shi W, Smyth GK. voom: Precision weights unlock linear model analysis tools for RNA-seq read counts. Genome Biol. 2014;15:R29. doi: 10.1186/gb-2014-15-2-r29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Ritchie ME, et al. limma powers differential expression analyses for RNA-sequencing and microarray studies. Nucleic Acids Res. 2015;43:e47. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkv007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Plaisier SB, Taschereau R, Wong JA, Graeber TG. Rank-rank hypergeometric overlap: identification of statistically significant overlap between gene-expression signatures. Nucleic Acids Res. 2010;38:e169. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkq636. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Cahill KM, Huo Z, Tseng GC, Logan RW, Seney ML. Improved identification of concordant and discordant gene expression signatures using an updated rank-rank hypergeometric overlap approach. Sci. Rep. 2018;8:9588. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-27903-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Zhang B, Horvath S. A general framework for weighted gene co-expression network analysis. Stat. Appl. Genet. Mol. Biol. 2005;4:Article17. doi: 10.2202/1544-6115.1128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Langfelder P, Zhang B, Horvath S. Defining clusters from a hierarchical cluster tree: the Dynamic Tree Cut package for R. Bioinformatics. 2008;24:719–720. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btm563. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Langfelder P, Luo R, Oldham MC, Horvath S. Is my network module preserved and reproducible? PLoS Comput. Biol. 2011;7:e1001057. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1001057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Zhang B, et al. Integrated systems approach identifies genetic nodes and networks in late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Cell. 2013;153:707–720. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.03.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Margolin AA, et al. ARACNE: an algorithm for the reconstruction of gene regulatory networks in a mammalian cellular context. BMC Bioinformatics. 2006;7(Suppl. 1):S7. doi: 10.1186/1471-2105-7-S1-S7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Bagot RC, et al. Circuit-wide transcriptional profiling reveals brain region-specific gene networks regulating depression susceptibility. Neuron. 2016;90:969–983. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.04.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Seney ML, et al. Opposite molecular signatures of depression in men and women. Biol. Psychiatry. 2018;84:18–27. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.01.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Albert FW, et al. Genetic architecture of tameness in a rat model of animal domestication. Genetics. 2009;182:541–554. doi: 10.1534/genetics.109.102186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Diez M, et al. Identification of gene regions regulating inflammatory microglial response in the rat CNS after nerve injury. J. Neuroimmunol. 2009;212:82–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroim.2009.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Kang HJ, et al. Decreased expression of synapse-related genes and loss of synapses in major depressive disorder. Nat. Med. 2012;18:1413–1417. doi: 10.1038/nm.2886. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Labonte B, et al. Sex-specific transcriptional signatures in human depression. Nat. Med. 2017;23:1102–1111. doi: 10.1038/nm.4386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Zhang, H., et al. alpha1- and beta3-adrenergic receptor-mediated mesolimbic homeostatic plasticity confers resilience to social stress in susceptible mice. Biol. Psychiatry85, 226–236 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 47.Hultman R, et al. Brain-wide electrical spatiotemporal dynamics encode depression vulnerability. Cell. 2018;173:166–180 e114. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2018.02.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Muir J, et al. In vivo fiber photometry reveals signature of future stress susceptibility in nucleus accumbens. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2018;43:255–263. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Han MH, Nestler EJ. Neural substrates of depression and resilience. Neurotherapeutics. 2017;14:677–686. doi: 10.1007/s13311-017-0527-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Laine MA, et al. Brain activation induced by chronic psychosocial stress in mice. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:15061. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-15422-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Drysdale AT, et al. Resting-state connectivity biomarkers define neurophysiological subtypes of depression. Nat. Med. 2017;23:28–38. doi: 10.1038/nm.4246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Kronfol Z, et al. Impaired lymphocyte function in depressive illness. Life Sci. 1983;33:241–247. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(83)90382-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Maes M, et al. Depression-related disturbances in mitogen-induced lymphocyte responses and interleukin-1 beta and soluble interleukin-2 receptor production. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 1991;84:379–386. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1991.tb03163.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Evans DL, et al. Mood disorders in the medically ill: scientific review and recommendations. Biol. psychiatry. 2005;58:175–189. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Haapakoski R, Mathieu J, Ebmeier KP, Alenius H, Kivimaki M. Cumulative meta-analysis of interleukins 6 and 1beta, tumour necrosis factor alpha and C-reactive protein in patients with major depressive disorder. Brain Behav. Immun. 2015;49:206–215. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2015.06.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Maes M, et al. Leukocytosis, monocytosis and neutrophilia: hallmarks of severe depression. J. Psychiatr. Res. 1992;26:125–134. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(92)90004-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Khera A, et al. Race and gender differences in C-reactive protein levels. J. Am. Coll. Cardiol. 2005;46:464–469. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2005.04.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Birur B, Amrock EM, Shelton RC, Li L. Sex Differences in the Peripheral Immune System in Patients with Depression. Front. psychiatry. 2017;8:108. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Eisenberger NI, Inagaki TK, Rameson LT, Mashal NM, Irwin MR. An fMRI study of cytokine-induced depressed mood and social pain: the role of sex differences. Neuroimage. 2009;47:881–890. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Moieni M, et al. Sex differences in depressive and socioemotional responses to an inflammatory challenge: implications for sex differences in depression. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:1709–1716. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Tyssowski KM, et al. Different neuronal activity patterns induce different gene expression programs. Neuron. 2018;98:530–546 e511. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.04.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Beckmann AM, Davidson MS, Goodenough S, Wilce PA. Differential expression of Egr-1-like DNA-binding activities in the naive rat brain and after excitatory stimulation. J. Neurochem. 1997;69:2227–2237. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1997.69062227.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Worley PF, et al. Constitutive expression of zif268 in neocortex is regulated by synaptic activity. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 1991;88:5106–5110. doi: 10.1073/pnas.88.12.5106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Herdegen T, Leah JD. Inducible and constitutive transcription factors in the mammalian nervous system: control of gene expression by Jun, Fos and Krox, and CREB/ATF proteins. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 1998;28:370–490. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(98)00018-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Cullinan WE, Herman JP, Battaglia DF, Akil H, Watson SJ. Pattern and time course of immediate early gene expression in rat brain following acute stress. Neuroscience. 1995;64:477–505. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(94)00355-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Kim S, Kim H, Um JW. Synapse development organized by neuronal activity-regulated immediate-early genes. Exp. Mol. Med. 2018;50:11. doi: 10.1038/s12276-018-0025-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Minatohara K, Akiyoshi M, Okuno H. Role of immediate-early genes in synaptic plasticity and neuronal ensembles underlying the memory trace. Front Mol. Neurosci. 2015;8:78. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2015.00078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Chandra R, Lobo MK. Beyond neuronal activity markers: select immediate early genes in striatal neuron subtypes functionally mediate psychostimulant addiction. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2017;11:112. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Taniguchi M, et al. HDAC5 and its target gene, Npas4, function in the nucleus accumbens to regulate cocaine-conditioned behaviors. Neuron. 2017;96:130–144 e136. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.09.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Manning CE, Williams ES, Robison AJ. Reward network immediate early gene expression in mood disorders. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2017;11:77. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Covington HE, et al. Antidepressant effect of optogenetic stimulation of the medial prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:16082–16090. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1731-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Xu Y, et al. Inhibition of phosphodiesterase 2 reverses impaired cognition and neuronal remodeling caused by chronic stress. Neurobiol. Aging. 2015;36:955–970. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.08.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Monsey MS, et al. Chronic corticosterone exposure persistently elevates the expression of memory-related genes in the lateral amygdala and enhances the consolidation of a Pavlovian fear memory. PLoS One. 2014;9:e91530. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0091530. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Matsumoto K, Ono K, Ouchi H, Tsushima R, Murakami Y. Social isolation stress down-regulates cortical early growth response 1 (Egr-1) expression in mice. Neurosci. Res. 2012;73:257–262. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2012.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Okada R, Matsumoto K, Tsushima R, Fujiwara H, Tsuneyama K. Social isolation stress-induced fear memory deficit is mediated by down-regulated neuro-signaling system and Egr-1 expression in the brain. Neurochem. Res. 2014;39:875–882. doi: 10.1007/s11064-014-1283-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Hodges TE, Green MR, Simone JJ, McCormick CM. Effects of social context on endocrine function and Zif268 expression in response to an acute stressor in adolescent and adult rats. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 2014;35:25–34. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2014.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Spiegel I, et al. Npas4 regulates excitatory-inhibitory balance within neural circuits through cell-type-specific gene programs. Cell. 2014;157:1216–1229. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.03.058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Furukawa-Hibi Y, Nagai T, Yun J, Yamada K. Stress increases DNA methylation of the neuronal PAS domain 4 (Npas4) gene. Neuroreport. 2015;26:827–832. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Furukawa-Hibi Y, Yun J, Nagai T, Yamada K. Transcriptional suppression of the neuronal PAS domain 4 (Npas4) gene by stress via the binding of agonist-bound glucocorticoid receptor to its promoter. J. Neurochem. 2012;123:866–875. doi: 10.1111/jnc.12034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Sugathan A, Waxman DJ. Genome-wide analysis of chromatin states reveals distinct mechanisms of sex-dependent gene regulation in male and female mouse liver. Mol. Cell Biol. 2013;33:3594–3610. doi: 10.1128/MCB.00280-13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Yen A, Kellis M. Systematic chromatin state comparison of epigenomes associated with diverse properties including sex and tissue type. Nat. Commun. 2015;6:7973. doi: 10.1038/ncomms8973. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Kong MM, et al. Regulation of D1 dopamine receptor trafficking and signaling by caveolin-1. Mol. Pharm. 2007;72:1157–1170. doi: 10.1124/mol.107.034769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Shmuel M, Nodel-Berner E, Hyman T, Rouvinski A, Altschuler Y. Caveolin 2 regulates endocytosis and trafficking of the M1 muscarinic receptor in MDCK epithelial cells. Mol. Biol. Cell. 2007;18:1570–1585. doi: 10.1091/mbc.E06-07-0618. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Luoma JI, Boulware MI, Mermelstein PG. Caveolin proteins and estrogen signaling in the brain. Mol. Cell Endocrinol. 2008;290:8–13. doi: 10.1016/j.mce.2008.04.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Lorsch ZS, et al. Estrogen receptor alpha drives pro-resilient transcription in mouse models of depression. Nat. Commun. 2018;9:1116. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-03567-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Garrett JE, Wellman CL. Chronic stress effects on dendritic morphology in medial prefrontal cortex: sex differences and estrogen dependence. Neuroscience. 2009;162:195–207. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.04.057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Cook SC, Wellman CL. Chronic stress alters dendritic morphology in rat medial prefrontal cortex. J. Neurobiol. 2004;60:236–248. doi: 10.1002/neu.20025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Pittenger C, Duman RS. Stress depression, and neuroplasticity: a convergence of mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:88–109. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Qiao H, et al. Dendritic spines in depression: what we learned from animal models. Neural Plast. 2016;2016:8056370. doi: 10.1155/2016/8056370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Seney ML, et al. Opposite molecular signatures of depression in men and women. Biol. Psychiatry. 2018;84:18–27. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.01.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Bollinger JL, Bergeon Burns CM, Wellman CL. Differential effects of stress on microglial cell activation in male and female medial prefrontal cortex. Brain Behav. Immun. 2016;52:88–97. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2015.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Hung LW, et al. Gating of social reward by oxytocin in the ventral tegmental area. Science. 2017;357:1406–1411. doi: 10.1126/science.aan4994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Nardou R, et al. Oxytocin-dependent reopening of a social reward learning critical period with MDMA. Nature. 2019;569:116–120. doi: 10.1038/s41586-019-1075-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Hodes GE, et al. Sex differences in nucleus accumbens transcriptome profiles associated with susceptibility versus resilience to subchronic variable stress. J. Neuroscience. 2015;35:16362–16376. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1392-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.