Abstract
Astronauts on interplanetary missions - such as to Mars - will be exposed to space radiation, a spectrum of highly-charged, fast-moving particles that includes 56Fe and 28Si. Earth-based preclinical studies show space radiation decreases rodent performance in low- and some high-level cognitive tasks. Given astronaut use of touchscreen platforms during training and space flight and given the ability of rodent touchscreen tasks to assess functional integrity of brain circuits and multiple cognitive domains in a non-aversive way, here we exposed 6-month-old C57BL/6J male mice to whole-body space radiation and subsequently assessed them on a touchscreen battery. Relative to Sham treatment, 56Fe irradiation did not overtly change performance on tasks of visual discrimination, reversal learning, rule-based, or object-spatial paired associates learning, suggesting preserved functional integrity of supporting brain circuits. Surprisingly, 56Fe irradiation improved performance on a dentate gyrus-reliant pattern separation task; irradiated mice learned faster and were more accurate than controls. Improved pattern separation performance did not appear to be touchscreen-, radiation particle-, or neurogenesis-dependent, as 56Fe and 28Si irradiation led to faster context discrimination in a non-touchscreen task and 56Fe decreased new dentate gyrus neurons relative to Sham. These data urge revisitation of the broadly-held view that space radiation is detrimental to cognition.
Subject terms: Neuroscience, Stem cells
Introduction
Interplanetary missions - such as to Mars - are a high priority for many space agencies. The crew of future missions will face hazards1–3, such as exposure to galactic cosmic radiation4–7 a spectrum of low and high-(H) atomic number (Z) and high-energy (E) particles such as 56Fe and 28Si. Fast-moving HZE particles cannot be effectively blocked by modern spacecraft shielding8–11. Therefore, it is concerning that studies with laboratory animals generally conclude HZE particles are detrimental to brain and behavior12–14. Such preclinical data suggest HZE particle exposure may be harmful to astronaut cognition and impede mission success.
However, there are reasons to revisit the conclusion that HZE particle exposure is detrimental to cognition. First, age at the time of irradiation matters. Most preclinical data that led to the Probabilistic Risk Assessment of HZE particles being detrimental to cognition were from tests performed on young adult rodents (~2-3 months [mon] at exposure)14; in many cases, age at testing was not reported14. To more accurately reflect the average age of astronauts, NASA now requires ground-based space studies to be performed in mature animals (~6-mon-old at irradiation)14–24. Indeed, some studies now directly compare the cognitive impact of HZE irradiation in early life vs. maturity25–28, although the results are mixed. Second, type of behavioral test matters. Recent work with mature rodents shows HZE particle exposure decreases performance in some - but not all - behavioral tests; even tests that engage similar neural circuits produce distinct results14–20,29. A potential contribution to these task-dependent discrepancies is task-specific testing environment. In humans (including astronauts), automated computerized cognitive assays help control for the influence of testing environments30–32. However, such an approach has not been used to assess cognition in mature rodents after HZE exposure. Third, breadth of testing matters. Preclinical studies on space radiation typically assess one or two cognitive domains33–35. In contrast, astronauts repeatedly undergo test batteries - often on a touchscreen platform - to assess integrity of many cognitive domains over time30,36. To this end, many aspects of neuroscience have employed rodent touchscreen testing, a platform extensively validated for its ability to provide multidimensional assessment of functional integrity of brain circuits in a highly-sensitive and translationally-relevant way37–39. In regard to space radiation, it is known that head-only exposure of young adult rats exposed to protons (a low energy particle) does not change acquisition or reversal learning on a touchscreen line discrimination task40. However, given the power of touchscreen testing, it is surprising that it is not known how whole body exposure of mature rodents to HZE particles influences performance on a battery of rodent touchscreen tests. This is particularly notable as the touchscreen platform permits analysis of many higher cognitive functions - such as pattern separation - which are part of the astronaut’s mission-critical skill set yet which have not been preclinically assessed for their sensitivity to space radiation.
To address these major knowledge gaps, mature C57BL/6J male mice received either Sham irradiation (IRR) or whole body 56Fe particle IRR and were assessed on a battery of touchscreen cognitive tasks to assess complex learning, cognitive flexibility, visuospatial learning, and stimulus-response habit learning39,41–43. This touchscreen battery revealed an unexpected finding: improved pattern separation in 56Fe IRR vs. Sham mice. To assess whether this improvement was dependent on touchscreen testing or on 56Fe IRR, we then assessed separate cohorts of 56Fe IRR vs. Sham mice for pattern separation performance in a non-touchscreen task, contextual discrimination fear conditioning (CDFC), and also assessed the impact of 28Si IRR vs. Sham on CDFC. Irrespective of whether tested on a touchscreen or non-touchscreen platform, or whether CDFC mice received to 56Fe or 28Si IRR, IRR mice had better pattern separation than Sham mice. Taken together, these data show whole body exposure to HZE particles is not detrimental to high level cognition in mature mice and actually enhances performance in certain mission-critical tasks, such as pattern separation.
Results
Mice given whole body 56Fe IRR demonstrate overall normal perceptual discrimination, association learning, and cognitive flexibility in touchscreen testing
Six-mon-old male C57BL/6J mice received either Sham IRR or fractioned (Frac) whole body 20 cGy 56Fe (3 exposures of 6.7 cGy every-other day, total 20 cGy) (Figs. 1a,c,e). This total dose is submaximal to that predicted for a Mars mission44,45, and the fractionation interval (48 hours [hr]) was determined by the inter-fraction period for potential repair processes46. As previously reported47, this dose and these fractionation parameters do not interfere with weight gain or cause hair loss (Fig. S1a).
Beginning 1 mon post-IRR, Sham and 56Fe IRR mice began training on a touchscreen platform extensively validated in rodents39,41,43,48,49(Fig. 1a,b). Mice initially went through five stages of general touchscreen training (Fig. 2a), with performance reflecting instrumental or operant learning. Sham and 56Fe IRR mice completed most stages of the initial operant touchscreen training in similar periods of time (Fig. 2a). The exception was the final stage, Punish Incorrect (PI, where an incorrect trial to timeout); on average, 56Fe IRR mice finished PI in ~40% fewer days versus Sham mice (Fig. 2a, Table S1).
Mice then advanced to pairwise discrimination (PD, visual discrimination) and PD reversal (reversal learning, Fig. 2b,c), tests which reflect perceptual discrimination and association learning as well as cognitive flexibility, respectively, and rely on cortical (prefrontal, orbital frontal, perirhinal) and striatal circuits41,48. On average, both 56Fe IRR and Sham mice completed PD and PD reversal in a similar number of days (Fig. 2d, Table S1). Analysis of the distribution of subjects to reach criteria each day revealed significant difference between Sham and 56Fe IRR mice (Fig. 2e). Specifically, 50% of Sham mice reached PD completion criteria at 9.5 days, while 50% 56Fe IRR mice reached criteria at 12 days. However, Sham and 56Fe IRR mice did not differ with regard to average session length, percent correct, or number of errors (Fig. 2f,h). In PD reversal, the distribution of subjects to reach completion criteria was not different between Sham and 56Fe IRR mice (Fig. 2i), with 50% of Sham and 56Fe IRR mice reaching PD reversal completion criteria at 15 and 14 days, respectively (Fig. 2i). As with PD, Sham and 56Fe IRR mice did not differ in PD reversal average session length, percent correct, or number of errors (Fig. 2j,l).
Mice given whole body 56Fe IRR demonstrate normal visuospatial learning and stimulus-response habit learning in touchscreen testing
A parallel group of mice was used to assess the influence of 56Fe IRR object-location paired associates learning (PAL) and visuomotor conditional learning (VMCL) which reflect visuospatial and stimulus-response habit learning, respectively, and rely on intact circuits of the hippocampus41,43 (PAL) and striatum and posterior cingulate cortex41,43 (VMCL, Fig. 1b). Consistent with results in the first cohort of mice, Sham and 56Fe IRR mice completed most stages of operant touchscreen training in similar periods of time (Fig. 3a), again with the exception of PI where 56Fe IRR mice finished in ~20% fewer days than Sham.
In PAL (Fig. 3b), Sham and 56Fe IRR mice were similar in session length, number of trials, percent correct, and number of errors over the 29-day testing period (Fig. 3c-f). In both VMCL train and test (Fig. 3g,h), Sham and 56Fe IRR mice had similar average days to completion (Fig. 3i). In VMCL train, Sham and 56Fe IRR mice performed similarly in regard to distribution of subjects to reach criteria each training day (50% subjects reached criteria at 10 days in Sham mice vs. 9 days in 56Fe IRR mice), session length, number of trials, percent correct, and number of errors (VMCL train; Fig. 3j-n, Table S1). In VMCL test, Sham and 56Fe IRR mice had similar distribution of subjects to reach criteria each training day (50% of subjects reached criteria at 22 days in Sham mice vs. 23 days in 56Fe IRR mice), session lengthpercent correct, percent missed, and number of errors (VMCL test; Figs. 3o-s, Table S1). However, the time to complete the session on the last day of VMCL test was longer in 56Fe IRR relative to Sham mice (Fig. 3p).
Whole body 56Fe IRR improves pattern separation in an appetitive-based location discrimination touchscreen task
A brain region commonly studied with regard to space radiation-induced deficits in function and activity-dependent processes (i.e. neurogenesis) is the hippocampal dentate gyrus47,50–52. Based on prior work, we hypothesized whole body 56Fe IRR impairs pattern separation, a cognitive function reliant on dentate gyrus integrity53,54. To test this hypothesis, Sham and 56Fe IRR mice were assessed on a touchscreen pattern separation task: location discrimination (LD)39(Fig. 1a). In the LD training portion of the assessment (LD train, Fig. 4a), Sham and 56Fe IRR mice had similar distribution of the proportion of subjects reaching criteria (Fig. 4b), average days to completion, session length, and percent correct (Fig. 4c,e). However, Sham and 56Fe IRR mice differed in LD performance (LD test, Fig. 4f) in several aspects. First, the distribution of proportion of subjects reaching criteria was distinct in 56Fe IRR mice vs. Sham mice (Fig. 4g). 56Fe IRR mice reached criteria at >3x faster rate vs. Sham mice, and 50% of 56Fe IRR mice reached criteria by 4 days versus Sham mice reaching criteria by 6 days. Second, 56Fe IRR mice completed LD test in fewer days than Sham mice (Fig. 4h), although both groups showed similar session length and number of completed trials (Fig. 4i,j). Third, 56Fe IRR mice performed LD test more accurately than Sham mice both overall (Fig. 4k) as well when presented with stimuli separated by either large or small distances (Fig. 4l).
We next behaviorally-probed reasons why 56Fe IRR mice had improved pattern separation relative to Sham mice. For example, the improved location discrimination in 56Fe IRR mice may be reflective of unintentional screen touches, perhaps due to IRR-induced alteration of attention to stimuli or motivation to obtain reward. However, the number of blank touches (Fig. 4m), reward collection latency (Fig. 4n), and choice latency (Fig. 4o,p) were not different between 56Fe IRR mice and Sham mice. Also, since the location of the rewarded stimuli changed daily but maintained within each session, it is possible that pattern separation is progressively improved within a session, particularly on the last test day. Sham and 56Fe IRR mice had similar last day block duration and left/right touches during intertrial interval, but 56Fe IRR mice had a greater percent correct during the 4th 10-trial block relative to Sham mice (Fig. 4q,r,t). In addition, while Sham mice did not differ between the 1st and 4th 10-trial blocks on the last day, 56Fe IRR mice had fewer blank touches in the 4th 10-trial block relative to the 1st 10-trial block. These data suggest that on the last day of LD, 56Fe IRR mice demonstrate within-session enhanced pattern separation (Fig. 4s).
Whole body 56Fe and 28Si IRR improves pattern separation in a foot shock- based contextual discrimination task
To assess whether 56Fe IRR-induced improvement in pattern separation was restricted to appetitive tasks, a parallel cohort of mice was exposed to Sham or 56Fe IRR and tested on pattern separation using a classic pattern separation behavior paradigm: contextual discrimination fear conditioning (CDFC)54,55. To specifically assess whether particle inter-fraction interval influenced behavioral outcome, 6-mon-old C57BL/6J mice received Sham IRR, whole body fractionated 20 cGy (Frac 20 cGy; 3 exposures of 6.7 cGy) 56Fe IRR, or whole body non-fractionated 20 cGy (Non-Frac 20 cGy; 1 exposure of 20 cGy) 56Fe IRR (Fig. 1c). As previously reported47, Sham IRR, Frac 20 cGy, and Non-Frac 20 cGy mice had similar weight changes over time (Fig. S1a).
Beginning ~2-mon post-IRR (8 mon of age), mice underwent CDFC (Figs. 5, S2) to learn that one context (Context A) was paired with a foot shock while another similar context (Context B) was a non-shock context. When tested in CDFC, Sham mice discriminated the two contexts by Days 9–10 (Block 5), as they froze more in the shock-paired context (Context A) compared to the non-shock context (Context B; Fig. 5a, Table S1). However, mice exposed to either Frac 20 cGy or Non-Frac 20 cGy of 56Fe IRR discriminated the contexts by Days 3–4 (Block 2, Fig. 5b,c, Table S1). Direct comparison across treatment groups revealed Frac 20 cGy and Non-Frac 20 cGy mice froze more in Context A vs. Context B in Blocks 2 and 4, earlier than Sham (Fig. 5d,f, Table S1). Possible explanations for these results include differential activity, anxiety, or pain sensitivity in Sham vs. 56Fe IRR mice. To address these possibilities, parallel groups of mice underwent assessment for locomotion (Fig. S1b), dark/light testing (Fig. S1c,d) and pain threshold (Fig. S1e,g). However, Sham, Frac, and Non-Frac mice performed similarly on all these tests (Fig. S1b,g). Thus, both Frac and Non-Frac 20 cGy 56Fe IRR mice learned to pattern separate earlier relative to Sham mice without overt changes in locomotion, anxiety-like behavior, or sensitivity to pain.
To determine if the improvement in CDFC pattern separation generalized to other fear-based hippocampal- and amygdala-based learning, a parallel cohort of mice received Sham or 56Fe IRR and underwent classical contextual fear conditioning (CFC; Fig. 1e, Fig. S3a,b). Sham and 56Fe IRR mice (both Frac and Non-Frac 20 cGy groups) performed similarly in the context test (Fig. S3c) and in the cue test both pre-tone and during tone (Fig. S3d). Importantly, to see if the space radiation-induced improvement in CDFC was dependent on the type of heavy particle used, CDFC was also performed with mice exposed to whole body 28Si IRR (Figs. 1d and 6), a particle with a smaller track structure than 56Fe56. Sham mice spent more time freezing in Context A vs. Context B only on Days 9–10 (Block 5) and Days 15–16 (Block 8, Figs. 6a). Mice exposed to 20 cGy of 28Si discriminated between the two contexts as early as Days 11–12 (Block 6; Fig. 6bf). Notably, mice exposed to 100 cGy of 28Si were able to discriminate between the two contexts as early as Days 5–6 (Block 3; Fig. 6ce,f). Taken together, these data show that exposure to two different HZE particles - either 56Fe or 28Si - results in earlier separation ability relative to Sham mice on the shock-based CDFC pattern separation test.
56Fe IRR decreases dentate gyrus neurogenesis 4 mon post-IRR
Pattern separation ability is dependent on new dentate gyrus neurons as well as dentate gyrus activity, and an inducible increase in adult neurogenesis improves pattern separation54,57,58. To assess whether the IRR-induced improvement in pattern separation reported here was correlated with increased neurogenesis, we used stereology to quantify the number of cells in the dentate gyrus immunoreactive for doublecortin (DCX, Fig. 7a), a widely-accepted marker for neurogenesis59. Although mice exposed to either Frac or Non-Frac 56Fe IRR had improved context discrimination compared to control mice (Fig. 5), these mice had fewer DCX + cells compared to control mice (Fig. 7b,c, Table S1).
Discussion
Astronaut training and in-mission assessment rely on touchscreen testing due to its flexibility in probing a variety of cognitive functions. Rodent touchscreen testing similarly allows researchers to probe the multidimensional functional integrity of brain circuits in a highly-sensitive and translationally-relevant way37,39,41,60, but prior to the present work it was unknown how exposure to the HZE particles that comprise space radiation influences touchscreen performance. Based on the large literature with young animals and the negative impact of HZE particle exposure on the central nervous system13,61, we hypothesized whole-body exposure to ground-based HZE particles would diminish the performance of mice in touchscreen-based behaviors, particularly those behaviors reliant on the dentate gyrus, such as pattern separation. The results of our multi-domain cognitive assessment showed our hypothesis was wrong. Mature mice exposed to either Sham IRR or HZE particles performed similarly in touchscreen tasks of visual discrimination, cognitive flexibility, rule-based learning, and object-spatial associated learning, in classical hippocampal- and amygdala-based tasks (i.e. CFC), and in tasks that detect anxiety-like behavior (i.e. D/L). Surprisingly, IRR mice performed better than Sham IRR mice in pattern separation tasks when assessed on either appetitive (LD test) or aversive (CDFC) platforms. Both 56Fe and 28Si IRR resulted in earlier and more consistent pattern separation in CDFC vs. respective Sham groups, showing that HZE-induced improvement is not specific to a particular HZE particle. While this study was not powered to assess dose-dependence, 20 cGy 56Fe IRR (either fractionated or non-fractiationed) appeared to improve pattern separation in CDFC more effectively than 20 cGy of 28Si and roughly similar to 100 cGy of 28Si, a result which merits additional future study. Taken together, our study suggests whole body exposure of HZE particles in maturity is not detrimental to high-level cognition, and actually enhances performance specifically in the mission-critical task of pattern separation.
There are three aspects of the present results that are notable from the perspective of behavioral neuroscience in general, and multiple memory systems in particular62,63. First, in both humans and rodents, hippocampal damage can actually facilitate behavioral performance on certain tasks64,65. For example, when amnesic patients with partial hippocampal injury are given extended exposure to study materials, they can improve their recognition memory to the level of control subjects. Such an improvement is not seen after severe hippocampal injury. Thus, it is reasonable to consider whether the improved pattern separation ability presented here result from HZE particle-induced partial damage to the hippocampus. This is unlikely, as the HZE particle parameters used here do not induce detectable damage to post-mitotic neurons in the adult rodent brain66,67 or, as shown here, deficits in other tasks that engage the hippocampus (PAL, CFC). Second, as memory mechanisms in the medial temporal lobe (i.e. hippocampus) and basal ganglia (i.e. dorsal striatum) may sometimes compete63, it is possible the improved dentate gyrus-based pattern separation reported here is associated with decreased dorsal striatum-based ‘habit’ learning. However, we find pattern separation is improved in 56Fe relative to Sham mice without a change in VMCL habit learning, suggesting normal dorsal striatal function. Finally, the improved pattern separation reported here is reminiscent of the excessive attention seen in some psychiatric disorders - such as autism or obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) - and in animal models for these disorders68,69. Evaluation of autistic- or OCD-like behavioral patterns after HZE particle exposure using other touchscreen paradigms (i.e. extinction, 5-choice serial reaction time test, 5-choice continuous performance reaction task) would clarify whether the improved pattern separation ability demonstrated here is accompanied by maladaptive behaviors (i.e. impaired attention and increased impulsivity)70,71.
What might be the neural mechanism underlying the improved pattern separation in HZE-irradiated mice reported here? One possibility is an HZE-induced shift in underlying brain circuit activity. In rodents and humans, pattern separation requires the appropriate balance of activity in the entorhinal cortex-dentate gyrus network57,72–74. In aged humans, a decline in pattern separation is proposed to be due to a hypoactive anterolateral enthorhinal cortex and hyperactive dentate gyrus/CA375. Thus, it is possible the HZE-induced improved pattern separation reported here in mouse results from an opposite activity shift: a hyperactive enthorhinal cortex and hypoactive dentate gyrus/CA3. Indeed, in rodents, pattern separation performance is correlated with dentate gyrus activity; better performance results in a hypoactive dentate gyrus, and worse performance results in a hyperactive dentate gyrus54,72. As pattern separation engages distinct hippocampal networks relative to other hippocampal-dependent tests (such as novel object recognition)76,77, such an HZE-induced shift in hippocampal networks may explain why we see improved pattern separation - while other groups see decreased novel object recognition - after HZE exposure.
Another possibility is that the improved pattern separation we report in HZE-irradiated mice is due to HZE-induced conditions in the dentate gyrus that favor “sparse encoding” of entorhinal cortical input. Sparse encoding is the concept that information - a stimulus, context, experience, memory, etc. - is represented by a relatively small number of simultaneously-active neurons78. Sparse encoding in dentate gyrus granule cell neurons is critical for pattern separation, as it minimizes interference between memory representations of similar but not identical experiences79–83. This sparsity is due in part to inhibition of dentate gyrus granule cell neurons by GABAergic interneurons and mossy cells84,85. It is unknown how the HZE particle parameters used here influence dentate gyrus GABAergic interneurons and mossy cells in mature mice. However, exposure to other energetic particles that comprise space radiation alters the inhibitory network in the dentate gyrus and other hippocampal subregions of young adult rodents86,87. In the future, evaluation of GABAergic signaling and other measures relevant to sparse encoding (e.g. number and functionality of hilar interneurons and mossy cells, pattern of memory-induced immediate early gene activation) after Mars-relevant exposure to space radiation would allow testing of the hypothesis that HZE-induced improvement in sparse encoding contributes to the HZE-induced improvement in pattern separation reported here.
A third possibility - and related to conditions that favor sparse encoding - is that HZE particle exposure increases dentate gyrus neurogenesis. In young adult rodents, inducible increase in hippocampal neurogenesis improves pattern separation, while inducible decrease in neurogenesis impairs pattern separation54,58,88. However, here we show that improved pattern separation is not correlated with the number of new hippocampal neurons, at least when examined 4 mon-post IRR (when the touchscreen pattern separation testing began in a parallel group of mice). The present study did not assess the number of new neurons 2 mon-post IRR (when CDFC testing began), and did not assess if IRR influences other measures of neurogenesis, such as synaptic connectivity and dendritic integration. However, these data add to the growing evidence that the number of new neurons does not always predict pattern separation performance, particularly in older rodents89,90. In fact, decreased hippocampal neurogenesis is proposed to diminish sensitivity to memory interference and thus improve performance in certain memory tasks90,91. Computational models support that decreased neurogenesis may enhance sparse encoding92,93, which as mentioned above may explain why we see improved pattern separation after HZE particle exposure yet other groups see decreased performance in their behavioral tests.
The disconnect shown here between pattern separation and hippocampal neurogenesis raises interesting future directions. Although historically tied to learning and memory, hippocampal neurogenesis also plays a role in forgetting94 with high levels of hippocampal neurogenesis facilitating the forgetting of prior memories, resulting in greater cognitive flexibility95. In converse, lower levels of hippocampal neurogenesis - as seen with age - facilitate the persistence of prior memories, lead to more interference with new memory formation, and thus may decrease cognitive flexibility95. As here we show irradiated mice have decreased neurogenesis relative to control mice (4 mon post-IRR), it is possible irradiated mice have consequently decreased forgetting (greater memory persistence) and also experience more proactive interference from past memories and would have less cognitive flexibility. Rodent cognitive flexibility can be directly tested using a reversal learning paradigm similar to the PD reversal learning task presented here. However, this task does not test rodent memory retention, and as we have shown, this relatively simplistic reversal learning is not affected by HZE radiation exposure. If the PD memory load were to be increased - for example, by training with more pairs of images - the rodent’s ability to then perform reversal with this larger number of stimuli would provide a more robust interrogation of cognitive flexibility. Alternatively, future experiments can hone in on dentate gyrus-specific cognitive flexibility via assessed LD reversal39,42,58, which contrasts with the PD reversal reliance on non-dentate gyrus brain regions (primarily PFC, perirhinal cortex, striatal circuits). Specifically, a challenging LD within-session reversal test would provide clarity as to whether IRR mice have decreased dentate gyrus specific-cognitive flexibility relative to controls42. Finally, future experiments could probe the influence of HZE particle exposure on the converse of pattern separation: pattern completion (i.e. formation of an accurate generalization of partial sensory input). Pattern separation and pattern completion abilities have a reciprocal relationship in both mice and aged humans88,96. As we show HZE particle exposure improves pattern separation (fine detail discrimination) and may increase proactive interference (given the decreased neurogenesis), it is possible irradiated mice have improved pattern separation yet worse pattern completion ability. If that were true, we could then further explore the possibility that the functional switch from pattern completion to pattern separation is driven in part by a slowing of the development of adult-generated neurons88,97. However, pattern completion relies on memory recall88, which is assessed in our PAL paradigm41 and is normal in our irradiated mice.
In conclusion, it is understandable that HZE particle exposure is presumed to have a negative influence on some lower and high-level cognitive functions, as many studies support this conclusion12,14,33,40,98,99. However, our study shows this is not universally true. Mature male mice that receive whole-body exposure to two different HZE particles perform similarly to control mice on many high-level cognitive tasks, reflecting the functional integrity of key neural circuits (i.e. PFC-perirhinal cortex-striatum, dorsal striatum, posterior cingulate cortex, hippocampus). Strikingly, mice irradiated with either 56Fe or 28Si actually perform “better” than control mice in both appetitive and aversive pattern separation tasks. Whether this HZE exposure-induced dentate gyrus-selective functional enhancement is compensation to earlier irradiation-induced neuromorphological changes100 remains to be tested, as does the task-, dose-, particle-, and LET-dependence of this functional enhancement. However, our work urges revisitation of the generally-accepted conclusion that space radiation is detrimental to cognition.
Methods
Animals
Animal procedures and husbandry were in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, and performed in IACUC-approved facilities at UT Southwestern Medical Center (Dallas, TX), Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia (Philadelphia, PA), and Brookhaven National Laboratories (BNL, Upton NY). 2-month(mon)-old male C57BL/6J mice (JAX stock #000664) were housed at UTSW and shipped to BNL for irradiation at 6 mon of age. At both facilities, food and water were provided ad libitum except during the appetitive behavior tasks (see also Supporting Information [SI] text).
Particle irradiation (IRR)
Mice received whole body HZE (56Fe or 28Si) particle IRR at BNL’s NASA Space Radiation Laboratory (NSRL). All mice were placed for 15 minutes (min) in modified clear polystyrene cubes (SI text). For 56Fe experiments, mice received Sham IRR (placed in cubes Monday, Wednesday, Friday, but received no IRR) or Fractionated (Frac) 20 cGy 56Fe (600 MeV/n, LET 174 KeV/μ, dose rate 20 cGy/min; placed in cubes and received 6.7 cGy on Monday, Wednesday, and Friday), and some experiments (Fig. 1) also included a group that received Non-Fractionated (Non-Frac) 20 cGy 56Fe (placed in cubes Monday, Wednesday, and Friday but received 20 cGy only on Friday). For 28Si IRR, mice received Sham IRR (placed in cubes, but received no IRR) or a single exposure of either 20 cGy or 100 cGy 28Si (275 MeV/n, LET 72 KeV/μ, dose rate 20 cGy/min or 100 cGy/min).
Overview of behavioral testing
All mice began behavior testing 1-2-mon post-IRR, but within each cohort (Fig. 1a,e), the interval between radiation exposure and behavioral testing was equal for Sham and IRR groups. Parallel groups of mice were tested for appetitive touchscreen behavioral tests (operant touchscreen platform: touchscreen training; Pairwise Discrimination, PD; PD reversal; Location Discrimination, LD; different paired associates learning, PAL; Visuomotor Conditional Learning, VMCL) vs. aversive behavioral tests (contextual fear conditioning, CFC; contextual discrimination fear conditioning, CDFC). Subsets of mice were also tested for general activity (locomotor, LM), anxiety (dark/light box test, D/L) and pain sensitivity (pain threshold, PT; SI text).
Appetitive behavior testing
The touchscreen platform used was Model 80614 made by Lafayette Instruments (Lafayette, IN). Additional touchscreen methods are in SI text.
Aversive behavior testing
CDFC overview is provided below. See Figs. S2,S3 and SI text for additional CDFC information, and for detailed information about CFC.
Contextual discrimination fear conditioning (CDFC)
A modified CDFC behavioral paradigm was utilized in which mice were exposed daily to two contexts (Context A and B) that shared similarities (including a floor pattern, a high-salience contextual feature55,88,101 (SI text). Importantly, Context A was always paired with a foot shock, while Context B was never paired with a foot shock, as described below. Mice were exposed daily to both Context A and Context B for 16 days. The order of exposure to Context A and B alternated between days (Fig. S2).
Tissue collection
After completion of behavioral tests, mice underwent intracardial perfusion, fixation, and tissue sectioning as previously described47,102 with additional detail provided in SI text.
Immunohistochemistry (IHC)
Immunohistochemistry was performed as previously described47,51 with additional detail provided in SI text.
Stereological cell quantification
Unbiased analysis of DCX + cell number was performed via stereologic quantification on a BX51 System Microscope (Olympus America, Center Valley, PA, USA) as previously described47,51.
Statistical analyses
Data are reported as mean ± s.e.m. Testing of data assumptions (for example, normal distribution, similar variation between control and experimental groups, etc.) and statistical analyses were performed in GraphPad Prism (ver. 8.2.0). Statistical approaches and results are provided in Table S1 for main figures and in Table S2 for supplementary figures, and statistical analysis summaries are provided in the figure legends. Analyses with two groups were performed using an unpaired, two-tailed Student’s t-test and analyses with more than two groups and one variable were performed using one-way ANOVA and Bonferroni post hoc test. Analyses with more than two variables were performed using two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc test; repeated measures (RM) were used where appropriate, as indicated in figure legends and Tables S1, S2. For the distribution of subjects reaching criteria between control and experimental groups, the Mantel-Cox test was used, and significance was defined as *p < 0.05. For behavioral studies, mice were randomly assigned to groups. Sample sizes were pre-determined via power analysis and confirmed on the basis of extensive laboratory experience and consultation with CHOP and PennMed statisticians.
Datasets
Raw data are made available to researchers on written request.
Ethics
Human subjects
No
Animal subjects
Yes
Ethics statement
The study was approved by three Ethics committees (the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees at the University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center [UTSW], Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia [CHOP], and Brookhaven National Laboratories [BNL]). Specifically, animal procedures and husbandry were in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, and performed in IACUC-approved facilities at UT Southwestern Medical Center (UTSW, Dallas TX; AAALAC Accreditation #000673, PHS Animal Welfare Assurance D16–00296, Office of Laboratory Animal Welfare [OLAW] A3472-01), Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia (CHOP, Philadelphia, PA; AAALAC Accreditation #000427, PHS Animal Welfare Assurance D16-00280 [OLAW A3442-01]) and Brookhaven National Laboratories (BNL, Upton NY; AAALAC Accreditation #000048, PHS Animal Welfare Assurance D16-00067 [OLAW A3106-01]).
Dual-use research
Not applicable.
Permissions
This manuscript represents original work, and is not a reproduction or modification of any part of an article that has been previously published or submitted to another journal.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
Research supported by NASA grants NNX07AP84G (to BPC and AJE), NNX12AB55G (to AJE and BPC), and NNX15AE09G (to AJE) and NIH grants to AJE DA007290, DA023555, and DA016765. CWW was supported by an NIH Institutional Training grant (DA007290, PI: AJE, David W. Self), and SY was supported by an NIH Institutional Training Grant (MH076690, PI: CA Tamminga), a PENN McCabe award, an 2019 IBRO travel grant and NARSAD Young Investigator Grant from the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation. We thank members of the Eisch, Chen, and Stowe Laboratories for technical support and helpful conversations, particularly Lyles Clark, Fred Kiffer, Guillermo Palchik, Shibani Mukherjee, Vanessa Torres, Angela Walker, and Kielen Zuurbier. We also thank Steve Maren and Craig Stark for their comments that greatly helped our data interpretation. We are deeply grateful to members of the Brookhaven National Laboratory staff including Adam Rusek (Physics team leader), MaryAnn Petry (animal support director), Peter Guida (organization and technical support director) as well as all of their team members and the BNL IACUC committee who help make our experiments possible.
Author contributions
(based on Project CRediT) listed via initials and alphabetically by last name Conceptualization: S.G.B., B.P.C.C., A.J.E., M.J.L., C.W.W., S.Y. Methodology: S.G.B., B.P.C.C., A.J.E., M.J.L., I.S., F.T., C.W.W., S.Y. Software: Not applicable Validation: M.J.L., R.P.R., I.S., C.W.W., S.Y. Formal Analysis: S.G.B., A.J.E., N.I., M.J.L., D.R.R., P.D.R., R.L.R., I.S., H. Y.S, F.T., C.W.W., S.Y. Investigation: S.G.B., A.J.E., N.I., M.J.L., R.L.R., D.R.R., P.D.R., H.Y.S., C.W.W. Resources: S.G.B., A.J.E., AMS Data Curation: A.J.E., M.J.L., R.P.R., C.W.W., S.Y. Writing, original draft: A.J.E., C.W.W., S.Y. Writing, review and editing: S.G.B., A.J.E., I.S, C.W.W., S.Y. Visualization: A.J.E, R.P.R., C.W.W., S.Y. Supervision: A.J.E., A.M.S., S.Y. Projection Administration: A. J.E., A.M.S. Funding Acquisition: B.C.C., A.J.E., S.Y.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: Cody W. Whoolery and Sanghee Yun.
Supplementary information
is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41598-020-59419-z.
References
- 1.Vazquez ME. Neurobiological problems in long-term deep space flights. Adv. Space Res. 1998;22:171–183. doi: 10.1016/S0273-1177(98)80009-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Setlow RB. The hazards of space travel: Before sending out astronauts on an interplanetary mission, we need to investigate how the conditions in space affect human health. The International Space Station is therefore of huge importance to ensure the health of a spaceship crew travelling to other planets. EMBO Rep. 2003;4:1013–1016. doi: 10.1038/sj.embor.7400016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Human Health and Performance Risks of Space Exploration Missions: Evidence Reviewed by the NASA Human Research Program. (Government Printing Office, 2009).
- 4.Chancellor JC, Scott GBI, Sutton JP. Space Radiation: The Number One Risk to Astronaut Health beyond Low Earth Orbit. Life. 2014;4:491–510. doi: 10.3390/life4030491. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine, Health and Medicine Division, Board on Health Sciences Policy & Committee to Review NASA’s Evidence Reports on Human Health Risks. Review of NASA’s Evidence Reports on Human Health Risks: 2016 Letter Report. (National Academies Press, 2017). [PubMed]
- 6.Cucinotta FA, Kim MH, Chappell LJ, Huff JL. How safe is safe enough? Radiation risk for a human mission to Mars. PLoS One. 2013;8:e74988. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0074988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Nelson GA. Space Radiation and Human Exposures, A Primer. Radiat. Res. 2016;185:349–358. doi: 10.1667/RR14311.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Zeitlin C, La Tessa C. The Role of Nuclear Fragmentation in Particle Therapy and Space Radiation Protection. Front. Oncol. 2016;6:65. doi: 10.3389/fonc.2016.00065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Cucinotta FA, Kim M-HY, Ren L. Evaluating shielding effectiveness for reducing space radiation cancer risks. Radiat. Meas. 2006;41:1173–1185. doi: 10.1016/j.radmeas.2006.03.011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Spillantini P, et al. Shielding from cosmic radiation for interplanetary missions: Active and passive methods. Radiat. Meas. 2007;42:14–23. doi: 10.1016/j.radmeas.2006.04.028. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Durante M. Space radiation protection: Destination Mars. Life Sci. Space Res. 2014;1:2–9. doi: 10.1016/j.lssr.2014.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Cucinotta FA, Alp M, Sulzman FM, Wang M. Space radiation risks to the central nervous system. Life Sciences in Space Research. 2014;2:54–69. doi: 10.1016/j.lssr.2014.06.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Jandial R, Hoshide R, Waters JD, Limoli CL. Space-brain: The negative effects of space exposure on the central nervous system. Surg. Neurol. Int. 2018;9:9. doi: 10.4103/sni.sni_250_17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Kiffer F, Boerma M, Allen A. Behavioral effects of space radiation: A comprehensive review of animal studies. Life Sci. Space Res. 2019;21:1–21. doi: 10.1016/j.lssr.2019.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Acharya Munjal M., Baulch Janet E., Klein Peter M., Baddour Al Anoud D., Apodaca Lauren A., Kramár Eniko A., Alikhani Leila, Garcia Camillo, Angulo Maria C., Batra Raja S., Fallgren Christine M., Borak Thomas B., Stark Craig E. L., Wood Marcello A., Britten Richard A., Soltesz Ivan, Limoli Charles L. New Concerns for Neurocognitive Function during Deep Space Exposures to Chronic, Low Dose-Rate, Neutron Radiation. eneuro. 2019;6(4):ENEURO.0094-19.2019. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0094-19.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Raber, J. et al. Combined Effects of Three High-Energy Charged Particle Beams Important for Space Flight on Brain, Behavioral and Cognitive Endpoints in B6D2F1 Female and Male Mice. Frontiers in Physiology vol. 10 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 17.Rabin BM, et al. Effects of exposure to 12C and 4He particles on cognitive performance of intact and ovariectomized female rats. Life Sciences in Space Research. 2019;22:47–54. doi: 10.1016/j.lssr.2019.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Rabin BM, Poulose SM, Bielinski DF, Shukitt-Hale B. Effects of head-only or whole-body exposure to very low doses of 4He (1000 MeV/n) particles on neuronal function and cognitive performance. Life Sci. Space Res. 2019;20:85–92. doi: 10.1016/j.lssr.2019.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Jones Carli, Mange Ami, Granata Lauren, Johnson Benjamin, Hienz Robert, Davis Catherine. Short and Long-Term Changes in Social Odor Recognition and Plasma Cytokine Levels Following Oxygen (16O) Ion Radiation Exposure. International Journal of Molecular Sciences. 2019;20(2):339. doi: 10.3390/ijms20020339. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Howe Alexis, Kiffer Frederico, Alexander Tyler C., Sridharan Vijayalakshmi, Wang Jing, Ntagwabira Fabio, Rodriguez Analiz, Boerma Marjan, Allen Antiño R. Long-Term Changes in Cognition and Physiology after Low-Dose 16O Irradiation. International Journal of Molecular Sciences. 2019;20(1):188. doi: 10.3390/ijms20010188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Belov OV, et al. Neurochemical insights into the radiation protection of astronauts: Distinction between low- and moderate-LET radiation components. Physica Medica. 2019;57:7–16. doi: 10.1016/j.ejmp.2018.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Batmunkh M, Aksenova SV, Bayarchimeg L, Bugay AN, Lkhagva O. Optimized neuron models for estimation of charged particle energy deposition in hippocampus. Phys. Med. 2019;57:88–94. doi: 10.1016/j.ejmp.2019.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kokhan VS, et al. Combined effects of antiorthostatic suspension and ionizing radiation on the behaviour and neurotransmitters changes in different brain structures of rats. Behav. Brain Res. 2017;320:473–483. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.10.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Kokhan VS, Shakhbazian EV, Markova NA. Psycho-emotional status but not cognition is changed under the combined effect of ionizing radiations at doses related to deep space missions. Behav. Brain Res. 2019;362:311–318. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.01.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Carrihill-Knoll KL, Rabin BM, Shukitt-Hale B, Joseph JA, Carey A. Amphetamine-induced taste aversion learning in young and old F-344 rats following exposure to 56Fe particles. Age. 2007;29:69–76. doi: 10.1007/s11357-007-9032-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Rabin BM, Joseph JA, Shukitt-Hale B, Carrihill-Knoll KL. Interaction between age of irradiation and age of testing in the disruption of operant performance using a ground-based model for exposure to cosmic rays. Age. 2012;34:121–131. doi: 10.1007/s11357-011-9226-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Britten RA, et al. Exposure to mission relevant doses of 1 GeV/Nucleon (56)Fe particles leads to impairment of attentional set-shifting performance in socially mature rats. Radiat. Res. 2014;182:292–298. doi: 10.1667/RR3766.1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Rabin BM, Carrihill-Knoll KL, Miller MG, Shukitt-Hale B. Age as a factor in the responsiveness of the organism to the disruption of cognitive performance by exposure to HZE particles differing in linear energy transfer. Life Sciences in Space Research. 2018;16:84–92. doi: 10.1016/j.lssr.2017.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Cekanaviciute, E., Rosi, S. & Costes, S. V. Central Nervous System Responses to Simulated Galactic Cosmic Rays. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 19, (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 30.Moore TM, et al. Validation of the Cognition Test Battery for Spaceflight in a Sample of Highly Educated Adults. Aerosp Med Hum Perform. 2017;88:937–946. doi: 10.3357/AMHP.4801.2017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Basner M, et al. Cognition Test Battery. PsycTESTS Dataset. 2017 doi: 10.1037/t62872-000. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Gershon R. C., Wagster M. V., Hendrie H. C., Fox N. A., Cook K. F., Nowinski C. J. NIH Toolbox for Assessment of Neurological and Behavioral Function. Neurology. 2013;80(Issue 11, Supplement 3):S2–S6. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182872e5f. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Britten RA, Miller VD, Hadley MM, Jewell JS, Macadat E. Performance in hippocampus- and PFC-dependent cognitive domains are not concomitantly impaired in rats exposed to 20cGy of 1GeV/n (56)Fe particles. Life Sci. Space Res. 2016;10:17–22. doi: 10.1016/j.lssr.2016.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Cherry JD, et al. Galactic cosmic radiation leads to cognitive impairment and increased aβ plaque accumulation in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. PLoS One. 2012;7:e53275. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0053275. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Rabin BM, Shukitt-Hale B, Carrihill-Knoll KL. Effects of Age on the Disruption of Cognitive Performance by Exposure to Space Radiation. J. Behav. Brain Sci. 2014;04:297–307. doi: 10.4236/jbbs.2014.47031. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Nasrini, J., Dinges, D., Binsted, K. & Caldwell, B. J. Cognitive performance in long-duration Mars simulations at the Hawaii space exploration analog and simulation (HI-SEAS). in NASA Human Research Program Investigators’ Workshop 1–2 (2017).
- 37.Hvoslef-Eide M, Nilsson SRO, Saksida LM, Bussey TJ. Cognitive Translation Using the Rodent Touchscreen Testing Approach. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 2016;28:423–447. doi: 10.1007/7854_2015_5007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kangas BD, Bergman J. Touchscreen technology in the study of cognition-related behavior. Behav. Pharmacol. 2017;28:623–629. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0000000000000356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Oomen CA, et al. The touchscreen operant platform for testing working memory and pattern separation in rats and mice. Nat. Protoc. 2013;8:2006–2021. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2013.124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Davis CM, DeCicco-Skinner KL, Roma PG, Hienz RD. Individual differences in attentional deficits and dopaminergic protein levels following exposure to proton radiation. Radiat. Res. 2014;181:258–271. doi: 10.1667/RR13359.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Horner AE, et al. The touchscreen operant platform for testing learning and memory in rats and mice. Nat. Protoc. 2013;8:1961–1984. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2013.122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Swan AA, et al. Characterization of the role of adult neurogenesis in touch-screen discrimination learning. Hippocampus. 2014;24:1581–1591. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Delotterie DF, et al. Touchscreen tasks in mice to demonstrate differences between hippocampal and striatal functions. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2015;120:16–27. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2015.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Cucinotta FA, Durante M. Cancer risk from exposure to galactic cosmic rays: implications for space exploration by human beings. Lancet Oncol. 2006;7:431–435. doi: 10.1016/S1470-2045(06)70695-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Hellweg CE, Baumstark-Khan C. Getting ready for the manned mission to Mars: the astronauts’ risk from space radiation. Naturwissenschaften. 2007;94:517–526. doi: 10.1007/s00114-006-0204-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Thames HD. An ‘Incomplete-repair’ Model for Survival after Fractionated and Continuous Irradiations. International Journal of Radiation Biology and Related Studies in Physics, Chemistry and Medicine. 1985;47:319–339. doi: 10.1080/09553008514550461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Whoolery CW, et al. Whole-Body Exposure to (28)Si-Radiation Dose-Dependently Disrupts Dentate Gyrus Neurogenesis and Proliferation in the Short Term and New Neuron Survival and Contextual Fear Conditioning in the Long Term. Radiat. Res. 2017 doi: 10.1667/RR14797.1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Chudasama Y, Bussey TJ, Muir JL. Effects of selective thalamic and prelimbic cortex lesions on two types of visual discrimination and reversal learning. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2001;14:1009–1020. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01607.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Delotterie D, Mathis C, Cassel J-C, Dorner-Ciossek C, Marti A. Optimization of Touchscreen-Based Behavioral Paradigms in Mice: Implications for Building a Battery of Tasks Taxing Learning and Memory Functions. PLoS One. 2014;9:e100817. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0100817. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Casadesus G, et al. Hippocampal neurogenesis and PSA-NCAM expression following exposure to 56Fe particles mimics that seen during aging in rats. Exp. Gerontol. 2005;40:249–254. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2004.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.DeCarolis NA, et al. (56)Fe Particle Exposure Results in a Long-Lasting Increase in a Cellular Index of Genomic Instability and Transiently Suppresses Adult Hippocampal Neurogenesis in Vivo. Life Sci. Space Res. 2014;2:70–79. doi: 10.1016/j.lssr.2014.06.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Raber J, et al. Effects of whole body 56Fe radiation on contextual freezing and Arc-positive cells in the dentate gyrus. Behav. Brain Res. 2013;246:162–167. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.02.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Kesner Raymond P. An analysis of dentate gyrus function (an update) Behavioural Brain Research. 2018;354:84–91. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.07.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Sahay A, et al. Increasing adult hippocampal neurogenesis is sufficient to improve pattern separation. Nature. 2011;472:466–470. doi: 10.1038/nature09817. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.McHugh, et al. Dentate Gyrus NMDA Receptors Mediate Rapid Pattern Separation in the Hippocampal Network. Science. 2007;317:94–99. doi: 10.1126/science.1140263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Schaefer, H. J. & Sullivan, J. J. Atlas of nuclear emulsion micrographs from personnel dosimeters of manned space missions. (1976).
- 57.Zhuo, J.-M. et al. Young adult born neurons enhance hippocampal dependent performance via influences on bilateral networks. Elife5, (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 58.Clelland CD, et al. A functional role for adult hippocampal neurogenesis in spatial pattern separation. Science. 2009;325:210–213. doi: 10.1126/science.1173215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Francis F, et al. Doublecortin is a developmentally regulated, microtubule-associated protein expressed in migrating and differentiating neurons. Neuron. 1999;23:247–256. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80777-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Phillips BU, Lopez-Cruz L, Saksida LM, Bussey TJ. Translational tests involving non-reward: methodological considerations. Psychopharmacology. 2019;236:449–461. doi: 10.1007/s00213-018-5062-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Craven PA, Rycroft MJ. Fluxes of galactic iron nuclei and associated HZE secondaries, and resulting radiation doses, in the brain of an astronaut. Advances in Space Research. 1994;14:873–878. doi: 10.1016/0273-1177(94)90552-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Nadel L. Multiple memory systems: what and why. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1992;4:179–188. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1992.4.3.179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Poldrack RA, Packard MG. Competition among multiple memory systems: converging evidence from animal and human brain studies. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:245–251. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00157-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Reed JM, Hamann SB, Stefanacci L, Squire LR. When amnesic patients perform well on recognition memory tests. Behav. Neurosci. 1997;111:1163–1170. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.111.6.1163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Schwarting RKW, Busse S. Behavioral facilitation after hippocampal lesion: A review. Behav. Brain Res. 2017;317:401–414. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.09.058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Tofilon PJ, Fike JR. The radioresponse of the central nervous system: a dynamic process. Radiat. Res. 2000;153:357–370. doi: 10.1667/0033-7587(2000)153[0357:TROTCN]2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Grosshans DR, Duman JG, Gaber MW, Sawakuchi G. Particle Radiation Induced Neurotoxicity in the Central Nervous System. International Journal of Particle Therapy. 2018;5:74–83. doi: 10.14338/IJPT-18-00026.1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Kheirbek MA, Klemenhagen KC, Sahay A, Hen R. Neurogenesis and generalization: a new approach to stratify and treat anxiety disorders. Nat. Neurosci. 2012;15:1613–1620. doi: 10.1038/nn.3262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Lissek S. Toward an account of clinical anxiety predicated on basic, neurally mapped mechanisms of Pavlovian fear-learning: the case for conditioned overgeneralization. Depress. Anxiety. 2012;29:257–263. doi: 10.1002/da.21922. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Mar AC, et al. The touchscreen operant platform for assessing executive function in rats and mice. Nat. Protoc. 2013;8:1985–2005. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2013.123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Davis GL, et al. Functional coding variation in the presynaptic dopamine transporter associated with neuropsychiatric disorders drives enhanced motivation and context-dependent impulsivity in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 2018;337:61–69. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.09.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Ikrar T, et al. Adult neurogenesis modifies excitability of the dentate gyrus. Front. Neural Circuits. 2013;7:204. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Leal SL, Yassa MA. Integrating new findings and examining clinical applications of pattern separation. Nat. Neurosci. 2018;21:163–173. doi: 10.1038/s41593-017-0065-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Tran T, Bridi M, Koh MT, Gallagher M, Kirkwood A. Reduced cognitive performance in aged rats correlates with increased excitation/inhibition ratio in the dentate gyrus in response to lateral entorhinal input. Neurobiol. Aging. 2019;82:120–127. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.07.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Reagh Zachariah M., Noche Jessica A., Tustison Nicholas J., Delisle Derek, Murray Elizabeth A., Yassa Michael A. Functional Imbalance of Anterolateral Entorhinal Cortex and Hippocampal Dentate/CA3 Underlies Age-Related Object Pattern Separation Deficits. Neuron. 2018;97(5):1187-1198.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.01.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Kubik S, Miyashita T, Guzowski JF. Using immediate-early genes to map hippocampal subregional functions. Learn. Mem. 2007;14:758–770. doi: 10.1101/lm.698107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Barbosa FF, et al. Differential Cortical c-Fos and Zif-268 Expression after Object and Spatial Memory Processing in a Standard or Episodic-Like Object Recognition Task. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2013;7:112. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Olshausen BA, Field DJ. Sparse coding of sensory inputs. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2004;14:481–487. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Leutgeb JK, Leutgeb S, Moser M-B, Moser EI. Pattern separation in the dentate gyrus and CA3 of the hippocampus. Science. 2007;315:961–966. doi: 10.1126/science.1135801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Diamantaki, M., Frey, M., Berens, P., Preston-Ferrer, P. & Burgalossi, A. Sparse activity of identified dentate granule cells during spatial exploration. Elife5 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 81.Dieni CV, Nietz AK, Panichi R, Wadiche JI, Overstreet-Wadiche L. Distinct determinants of sparse activation during granule cell maturation. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:19131–19142. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2289-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Neunuebel JP, Knierim JJ. Spatial firing correlates of physiologically distinct cell types of the rat dentate gyrus. J. Neurosci. 2012;32:3848–3858. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6038-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Danielson NB, et al. Distinct Contribution of Adult-Born Hippocampal Granule Cells to Context Encoding. Neuron. 2016;90:101–112. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.02.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Szabo GG, et al. Extended Interneuronal Network of the Dentate Gyrus. Cell Rep. 2017;20:1262–1268. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2017.07.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Cayco-Gajic NA, Silver RA. Re-evaluating Circuit Mechanisms Underlying Pattern Separation. Neuron. 2019;101:584–602. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.01.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Lee, S.-H. et al. Neurophysiology of space travel: energetic solar particles cause cell type-specific plasticity of neurotransmission. Brain Struct. Funct. 1–13 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 87.Marty VN, et al. Radiation-induced alterations in synaptic neurotransmission of dentate granule cells depend on the dose and species of charged particles. Radiat. Res. 2014;182:653–665. doi: 10.1667/RR13647.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Nakashiba T, et al. Young dentate granule cells mediate pattern separation, whereas old granule cells facilitate pattern completion. Cell. 2012;149:188–201. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2012.01.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Cès A, et al. Age-related vulnerability of pattern separation in C57BL/6J mice. Neurobiol. Aging. 2018;62:120–129. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.10.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Saxe MD, et al. Paradoxical influence of hippocampal neurogenesis on working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2007;104:4642–4646. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0611718104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Becker Suzanna. Neurogenesis and pattern separation: time for a divorce. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. 2016;8(3):e1427. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.O’Reilly RC, Rudy JW. Conjunctive representations in learning and memory: principles of cortical and hippocampal function. Psychol. Rev. 2001;108:311–345. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Finnegan R, Becker S. Neurogenesis paradoxically decreases both pattern separation and memory interference. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2015;9:136. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Akers KG, et al. Hippocampal neurogenesis regulates forgetting during adulthood and infancy. Science. 2014;344:598–602. doi: 10.1126/science.1248903. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Tello-Ramos Maria C., Branch Carrie L., Kozlovsky Dovid Y., Pitera Angela M., Pravosudov Vladimir V. Spatial memory and cognitive flexibility trade-offs: to be or not to be flexible, that is the question. Animal Behaviour. 2019;147:129–136. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2018.02.019. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Vieweg P, Stangl M, Howard LR, Wolbers T. Changes in pattern completion–a key mechanism to explain age-related recognition memory deficits? Cortex. 2015;64:343–351. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.12.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Trinchero MF, et al. High Plasticity of New Granule Cells in the Aging Hippocampus. Cell Rep. 2017;21:1129–1139. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2017.09.064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Wyrobek AJ, Britten RA. Individual variations in dose response for spatial memory learning among outbred wistar rats exposed from 5 to 20 cGy of 56Fe particles. Environ. Mol. Mutagen. 2016;57:331–340. doi: 10.1002/em.22018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Davis CM, DeCicco-Skinner KL, Hienz RD. Deficits in Sustained Attention and Changes in Dopaminergic Protein Levels following Exposure to Proton Radiation Are Related to Basal Dopaminergic Function. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0144556. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0144556. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Parihar VK, et al. Persistent changes in neuronal structure and synaptic plasticity caused by proton irradiation. Brain Struct. Funct. 2015;220:1161–1171. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0709-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Huckleberry KA, Ferguson LB, Drew MR. Behavioral mechanisms of context fear generalization in mice. Learn. Mem. 2016;23:703–709. doi: 10.1101/lm.042374.116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Yun S, et al. Stimulation of entorhinal cortex-dentate gyrus circuitry is antidepressive. Nat. Med. 2018;24:658–666. doi: 10.1038/s41591-018-0002-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.