Short abstract
For many, colours convey affective meaning. Popular opinion assumes that perception of colour is crucial to influence emotions. However, scientific studies test colour–emotion relationships by presenting colours as patches or terms. When using patches, researchers put great effort into colour presentation. When using terms, researchers have much less control over the colour participants think of. In this between-subjects study, we tested whether emotion associations with colour differ between terms and patches. Participants associated 20 emotion concepts, loading on valence, arousal, and power dimensions, with 12 colours presented as patches (n = 54) or terms (n = 78). We report high similarity in the pattern of associations of specific emotion concepts with terms and patches (r = .82), for all colours except purple (r = .−23). We also observed differences for black, which is associated with more negative emotions and of higher intensity when presented as a term than a patch. Terms and patches differed little in terms of valence, arousal, and power dimensions. Thus, results from studies on colour–emotion relationships using colour terms or patches should be largely comparable. It is possible that emotions are associated with colour concepts rather than particular perceptions or words of colour.
Keywords: colour, affect, semantic associations, metaphors, Geneva Emotion Wheel
Introduction
Across languages and cultural traditions, we use colour to express and convey emotional states. We feel blue, see red, or we are green with envy; we wear white to weddings and black to funerals; and we give red hearts to our loved ones on Valentine’s Day. It seems that colour–emotion associations are ubiquitous (e.g., Adams & Osgood, 1973; Allen & Guilford, 1936; Hupka, Zaleski, Otto, Reidl, & Tarabrina, 1997; Ou et al., 2018; Valdez & Mehrabian, 1994). One detail should, however, not be neglected: The first examples are concerned with affective colour expressions, omnipresent in language (Soriano & Valenzuela, 2009), while the remaining examples are concerned with colour perceptions. Actual research into colour–emotion associations have used both colour patches (e.g., Valdez & Mehrabian, 1994) and colour terms (e.g., Adams & Osgood, 1973). When using patches, great effort is put into how colours appear by controlling colour presentation (see Valdez & Mehrabian, 1994). Researchers can carefully control the three colour dimensions of hue, saturation, and lightness (Hunt & Pointer, 2011) and test a myriad of colours. When using terms, researchers have little control over the colour participants think of. In such studies, researchers can present fewer colours and the colours are presented as words. Considering these methodological differences, we have little a priori knowledge to predict whether and, if so, which colour–emotion relationships would be the same or different, when a person is presented with patches or terms.
The roles of language versus perception have been considered in various theoretical frameworks. The conceptual metaphor theory emphasises the role of language in colour–emotion associations (Lakoff & Johnson, 1999). This theory suggests that abstract concepts like affect (i.e., emotions, mood, evaluations, preferences) are metaphorically, or metonymically, linked to more concrete perceptual experiences such as colour (see also Meier & Robinson, 2005; Soriano & Valenzuela, 2009). This link would help people to better understand and describe their affective experiences. Meier and Robinson (2005) used this framework to explain the omnipresent association between positivity and lightness, which manifests in metaphorical expressions like bright day and dark thoughts (Adams & Osgood, 1973; Lakens, Fockenberg, Lemmens, Ham, & Midden, 2013; Meier, Robinson, Crawford, & Ahlvers, 2007; Specker et al., 2018). Meier and Robinson (2005) additionally argued that such metaphorical associations further reinforce these links across time, through language and cultural learning. At some point, the metaphors might function independently from the original perceptual associations and so dissociate language and perception (e.g., there is no clear perceptual base for expressions like green with envy, feeling blue, and yellow-bellied).
Other frameworks are less readily apt to explain colour–emotion associations through affective metaphors. If we take the example of yellow1 and joy, this association is widely spread (Burkitt & Sheppard, 2014; Dael, Perseguers, Marchand, Antonietti, & Mohr, 2016; Jonauskaite, Althaus, Dael, Dan-Glauser, & Mohr, 2019; Kaya & Epps, 2004; Lindborg & Friberg, 2015; Sutton & Altarriba, 2016) but has no equivalent metaphorical expression, at least not in English, German, French, Lithuanian, Dutch, or Spanish. Rather, yellow is metaphorically associated with negative emotions in different languages (e.g., yellow-bellied—to be cowardly or easily scared; Gelb vor Neid sein—to be envious, rire jaune—forced laughter to hide embarrassment). Similarly, despite the expression feeling blue signifying sadness in English, blue has been repeatedly associated with positive emotions (Adams & Osgood, 1973; Jonauskaite & Mohr, 2019; Kaya & Epps, 2004; Valdez & Mehrabian, 1994; Wexner, 1954) and is generally a liked colour (Eysenck, 1941; Jonauskaite, Dael, et al., 2019; Palmer & Schloss, 2010) in English- and non-English-speaking countries. In such cases, visual colour perception may play a more important role than language. These associations might be rooted in repeated perceptual associations between a colour and an emotional situation, such as feeling joyful when the sun is shining or feeling good when looking at clear blue water. Such propositions have been made to explain colour preferences (Ecological Valence Theory, Palmer & Schloss, 2010) and various cross-modal relationships (structural or statistical correspondence, C. Spence, 2011).
When appreciating the implications of the various frameworks, we would have to expect that certain colour–emotion associations might be more strongly reinforced by the linguistic system and colour metaphors (e.g., the associations between blue, and between sadness and yellow and negative emotions) and others through the perceptual system (e.g., the associations between yellow, and between joy and blue and positive emotions). In the former case, the actual colour presentation might play a more important role, while in the latter case, colour presentation might play a less important role. Moreover, there might be colour–emotion associations reinforced through both systems. For instance, the metaphorical expression seeing red associates red with feelings of anger, which would indicate a linguistic influence. Nonetheless, when one gets angry, blood rushes to the face (Benitez-Quiroz, Srinivasan, & Martinez, 2018), and so the perception of red faces in an angry situation might further strengthen the association between red and anger.
One possible approach for investigating which system, perceptual or linguistic, reinforces colour–emotion associations to a greater extent is to compare emotion associations with colour presented as patches (i.e., perceptual stimuli) versus as terms (i.e., linguistic stimuli). Despite the large body of empirical studies on colour–emotion associations, few studies have compared these methods directly (see Wang, Shu, & Mo, 2014 for a notable exception). More commonly, researchers worked separately with either colour patches (Allen & Guilford, 1936; D’Andrade & Egan, 1974; Fugate & Franco, 2019; Hanada, 2018; Kaya & Epps, 2004; Manav, 2007; Palmer, Schloss, Xu, & Prado-Leon, 2013; Valdez & Mehrabian, 1994) or colour terms (Adams & Osgood, 1973; Hupka et al., 1997; Soriano & Valenzuela, 2009; Sutton & Altarriba, 2016). Researchers working with perceptual colour stimuli have criticised research studies that used linguistic colour stimuli on the basis of vagueness and imprecision (e.g., Fugate & Franco, 2019; Valdez & Mehrabian, 1994). In other words, when presenting linguistic stimuli, unlike when presenting perceptual stimuli, one does not know the exact colour (how light, how saturated) participants visualised. Thus, it is unclear if emotions are attached to particular physical properties of colours, particular colour terms, or instead to particular colour concepts (i.e., abstract representations of colour combining colour perceptions with colour terms).
Furthermore, different methodologies of colour assessment might tap into different associative mechanisms. For instance, Wang et al. (2014) studied which colour–emotion associations are natural (i.e., arise due to perceptual pairing) and which are social (i.e., arise due to linguistic and cultural pairing) in Chinese participants. They tested implicit valence associations with colour terms and colour patches of red and blue. The authors demonstrated that red was evaluated both positively and negatively when presented as a patch. When red was presented as a term, it was evaluated exclusively positively. The authors suggested that the association between red and negative emotions (e.g., anger) is natural. Thus, it is present when a red patch is perceived. When red is treated linguistically, however, such pairing may be overshadowed by the exclusively positive connotations of red in Chinese culture (i.e., good fortune, success, beauty, joy, etc.; Toulson, 2013)—the social associations. Hence, to obtain a more complete picture, emotion associations should be tested with both colour terms and patches.
In this study, we investigated the extent to which colour–emotion associations are comparable between colour patches and colour terms by asking participants to associate as many or as few emotion concepts (Geneva Emotion Wheel [GEW]; Scherer, 2005; Scherer, Shuman, Fontaine, & Soriano, 2013) with 12 colours. The emotion concepts differentially loaded on the emotion dimensions of valence (positive–negative), arousal (high arousal–low arousal), and power (strong–weak; Fontaine, Scherer, Roesch, & Ellsworth, 2007). Thus, we were able to analyse colour associations with specific emotions and with emotion dimensions. We also tested emotion intensity. Crucially, one group of participants associated emotion concepts with colour terms (Experiment 1), while the other group associated emotion concepts with colour patches (Experiment 2). We chose the 11 basic colour terms in French plus turquoise for the term condition and focal colours that best matched each term for the patch condition (Lindsey & Brown, 2014). The focal colours are representative members of colour categories (Abbott, Griffiths, & Regier, 2016), thus participants were likely to imagine focal colours when presented with colour terms. We expected some degree of dissimilarity between colour terms and colour patches, especially for colours that might have divergent meanings as a term and as a patch (e.g., blue, yellow).
Method
Participants
We recruited 132 first-year university students (23 males) with a mean age of 20.52 years (95% confidence interval [CI] = [20.21, 20.82]). Seventy-eight participants (15 males) took part in Experiment 1 (Mage = 21.19, 95% CIage = [20.88, 21.51], range: 19–24) and a new group of 54 participants (8 males) took part in Experiment 2 (Mage = 19.60, 95% CIage = [19.10, 20.01], range: 18–26). An independent samples t test showed that participants in Experiment 1 (M = 21.19, standard deviation = 1.40) were slightly older than in Experiment 2 (M = 19.56, standard deviation = 1.66), t(130) = 6.13, p < .001, d = 1.06. This difference occurred because participants in Experiment 1 participated at the end of the academic year, while participants in Experiment 2 participated at the beginning of another academic year. The gender distribution was comparable in both experiments, χ2(1) = .432, p = .51, V = .057.
We performed a sample size power analysis (Mayr, Buchner, Erdfelder, & Faul, 2007) for a 2 × 12 mixed-design analysis of variance (ANOVA) based on the number of emotions (broadness). This analysis suggested that at an alpha level of .050 and a beta level of .950, and assuming a correlation between repeated measures of .5 and epsilon of 1, the total sample size of 54 is sufficient to detect a medium effect size of .25. We set 27 (i.e., 54/2) as a minimum number of participants for each colour presentation condition and recruited participants over 2 months (April–May) for Experiment 1, and over 2 months (October–November) for Experiment 2. We included all participants who volunteered in this time window (total N = 173; n = 98 in Experiment 1, n = 75 in Experiment 2).
We subsequently excluded participants in case of self-reported (n = 3 in Experiment 1) or tested (n = 1 in Experiment 2; Ishihara, 1993) colour blindness or who were not native French speakers (n = 13 in Experiment 1; n = 20 in Experiment 2). We recruited participants from the same student pool but coming from different academic years. Consequently, participants’ age, gender, the level of education, and the native language were matched between the two experiments. Finally, as the data for Experiment 1 were Internet-based, we excluded participants who were too quick or too slow (i.e., took ≤3 or ≥90 minutes; n = 4) or did not show minimal engagement with the experiment (i.e., spent ≤20 seconds on the first four colour terms; n = 0; see also, Jonauskaite, Dael, et al., 2019).
Participation was voluntary and was rewarded with course credit. Both experiments were conducted in accordance with the principles expressed in the Declaration of Helsinki (World Medical Association, 2013). No specific ethical clearance was received for this study, as the law of Canton of Vaud, Switzerland did not require it.
Colour Stimuli
We used 12 colour stimuli—red, orange, yellow, green, turquoise, blue, purple, pink, brown, white, grey, and black—labelling the principal colour categories (Biggam, 2012). They were presented as colour terms written in black ink in French (Experiment 1) or as colour patches (Experiment 2). Turquoise covers the blue–green range and the remaining 11 stimuli represent French basic colour categories (Morgan, 1993; Table S1). For patches, we displayed the best exemplars of each colour category (i.e., focal colours, Table 1; Lindsey & Brown, 2014), which are largely universally recognised (Regier, Kay, & Cook, 2005).
Table 1.
Colour term (Experiment 1) | Colour patch (Experiment 2) |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Munsell colour order system |
CIE1931 coordinates |
|||||
Hue | Value | Chroma | Y (cd/m2) | x | y | |
Red | 5.00 R | 4 | 14 | 12.00 | .57 | .31 |
Orange | 5.00 YR | 6 | 12 | 30.05 | .51 | .42 |
Yellow | 5.00 Y | 8 | 14 | 59.44 | .45 | .48 |
Green | 2.50 G | 5 | 12 | 20.99 | .27 | .50 |
Turquoise | 7.50 BG | 6 | 8 | 30.38 | .22 | .33 |
Blue | 10.00 B | 6 | 10 | 30.05 | .20 | .24 |
Purple | 7.50 P | 4 | 10 | 12.00 | .31 | .22 |
Pink | 7.50 RP | 7 | 8 | 43.07 | .37 | .31 |
Brown | 7.50 YR | 3 | 6 | 6.55 | .49 | .42 |
White | 10.00 RP | 9.5 | 0 | 90.01 | .31 | .33 |
Grey | 10.00 RP | 6 | 0 | 30.05 | .31 | .33 |
Black | 10.00 RP | 1.5 | 0 | 2.02 | .31 | .33 |
Grey (background) | 10.00 RP | 5 | 0 | 18.58 | .31 | .32 |
Note. Munsell values for colour patches taken from Lindsey and Brown (2014). The last columns show the CIE1931 xyY values for our patches.
Emotion Assessment
The GEW 3.0 (Figure 1; Scherer, 2005; Scherer et al., 2013) is a self-report measure of the feeling component of emotion. Twenty emotion concepts (Table S2) are represented along the circumference of a wheel, organised around two axes—valence, also known as evaluation or pleasantness (horizontal: positive vs. negative), and power, also known as control, dominance, or potency (vertical: strong vs. weak). Emotion concepts similar in valence and power are placed close to each other on the GEW. Emotion concepts can further be categorised in terms of arousal, also known as activation (high arousal vs. low arousal) based on complementary research studies (Fontaine, 2013; Soriano et al., 2013). Circles of increasing size connect the centre of the wheel with the circumference of the wheel. These circles denote five degrees of emotion intensity, coded from 1 (smallest circle; weakest intensity) to 5 (biggest circle; strongest intensity), or 0 if no emotion is chosen (little square). The Swiss Centre for Affective Sciences provides the validated French version of the GEW (Table S1).
Procedure: Experiment 1 (Colour Terms)
Data for Experiment 1 were collected online from the first-year psychology student pool of the local university. The experiment started with an information page. Participants were informed that they expressed their consent to participate by continuing to the next page. Then, participants were explained the task and performed a manipulation check exercise indicating that they understood the task. Participants were asked to correct the responses of an imaginary person (Peter) and were given feedback. During the experiment, participants saw the 12 colour terms written in black ink (Table 1) presented sequentially and in random order above the GEW on a neutral grey background (see Figure 2). Participants associated one, several, or none of the GEW emotion concepts with each colour term and rated the respective emotion intensity by choosing circles of different sizes (later converted to 1–5 ratings). After the main experiment, participants provided demographic information and were debriefed. They also saw results from a previous related marketing experiment in a graphic form. Participation took on average 12.3 minutes.
We collected the current data as part of a larger on-going International Colour-Emotion Survey online (https://www2.unil.ch/onlinepsylab/colour/main.php; Mohr, Jonauskaite, Dan-Glauser, Uusküla, & Dael, 2018). Given the continuous nature of the survey, we reported part of these data in our study on gendered colours (Jonauskaite, Dael, et al., 2019, Study 3).
Procedure: Experiment 2 (Colour Patches)
Data for Experiment 2 were collected in the laboratory from the first-year psychology student pool. Upon arrival, participants signed a written informed consent form. We next used a paper version of Ishihara (1993) colour blindness test to assess participants’ colour vision. Afterwards, participants were invited to individual testing rooms, which were dark and illuminated only by a computer monitor. Participants were presented with a colour patch (15° × 15° viewing angle) on a neutral grey background (see Table 1) for a minimum of 5 seconds and instructed to focus on the colour patch. Participants chose when to move to the subsequent page. In analogy to Experiment 1, they associated one, several, or none of the GEW concepts with the target colour patch and rated the intensity of each associated emotion concept. While associating emotions, participants could see the target colour on small GEW squares as well as on the chosen intensity circles (Figure 2(b); Experiment 2). There were three colour patches as practice trials at the beginning of the experiment, with 12 total experimental colour patches presented in randomised order (Table 1, values adapted for each monitor, see “Apparatus” section). We collected these data in the laboratory to ensure accurate colour presentation. Experimenters were available for questions at any point during the experiment. After the main experiment, participants provided demographic information on a paper questionnaire and completed another unrelated experiment, not reported here (choosing focal colours). Finally, they were debriefed. The experiment took approximately 15 minutes (Figure 2).
Apparatus
The task was performed on four similar monitors (Colour Edge CG243W 24.1″ Widescreen LCD display), which were linearised with an in-built sequence before each session. We used the Konica Minolta CS-100A Chroma Meter to measure the parameters of red, green, and blue guns of each monitor. We report here the white points of the monitors in Commission Internationale de l’Eclairage [CIE] xyY colour space (Monitor 1: .318, .334, 73; Monitor 2: .325, .340, 113; Monitor 3: .321, .336, 109; Monitor 4: .318, .323, 128). Although the four monitors had substantially different luminance values for the white points, these differences should not affect our study given that colour presentation was calculated for each monitor separately. Monitor 1 could not produce a luminance value large enough to represent the white patch (see Table 1) and thus it displayed a slightly darker version. Observers’ eyes would always adapt to this maximum value and thus they did not perceive any difference. The gamma curves were estimated from luminance increments of each of the three guns using a standard protocol (Brainard, Pelli, & Robson, 2002). The measured primaries in CIE xyY of Monitor 1 were red (R) = (.697, .300, 18.9), green (G) = (.189, .698, 50.9), and blue (B) = (.141, .026, 2.13), Monitor 2—R = (.696, .301, 29.6), G = (.192, .701, 78.9), and B = (.141, .026, 3.17), Monitor 3—R = (.695, .301, 28.7), G = (0.191, 0.699, 76.8), and B = (.141, .028, 3.30), and Monitor 4—R = (.696, .301, 34.9), G = (.188, .694, 89.5), and B = (.141, .027, 4.25). These measurements were then used to convert colour values from monitor-independent xyY system to monitor-dependent RGB system and display them in each screen. This step was necessary in order to keep the photometric characteristics of colour stimuli constant across monitors. Viewing was unrestrained and the viewing distance was approximately 70 cm.
Design and Data Analysis
We employed a mixed design to establish which emotions were associated with which colours (terms and patches together; within-subjects design) and to compare the emotion associations between colour terms and patches (between-subjects design). The independent variables were (a) colour presentation mode (between-subjects; colour term or colour patch), (b) colour (within-subjects, 12 levels, see “Colour Stimuli” section), and (c) emotion (within-subjects, 20 levels, see “Emotion Assessment” section) or emotion dimensions of valence, arousal, and power (within-subjects, 2 levels, see later).
Specific emotions
We started the analyses by investigating the specific emotion concepts associated with colours. We calculated the proportion of participants who associated a specific colour with a specific emotion concept by dividing the number of participants who chose each emotion concept for each colour by the total number of participants. The proportion of participants was calculated for each colour presentation condition separately, as well as for both conditions together. The proportion was the first dependent variable (DV1). DV1 varied from 0 (very unlikely association, no one chose it) to 1 (very likely association, everyone chose it).
We identified the most and least prominent colour–emotion associations with a two-step autocluster analysis using Schwarz’s Bayesian Criterion (Bacher, Wenzig, & Vogler, 2004) on the proportion values of terms and patches together (DV1). To compare the pattern of emotion associations, we created two 12 × 20 (Colours × Emotions) representation matrices using the proportion values. Then, we employed representation similarity analyses based on Pearson matrix-to-matrix correlations (Kriegeskorte, 2008) to compare the colour term matrix (12 × 20) with the colour patch matrix (12 × 20), also known as a Pattern Similarity Index (PSI). PSI reflects the degree of similarity in the pattern of colour–emotion associations across terms and patches. A PSI score of 1 indicates perfect pattern similarity, and a PSI score of 0 indicates complete pattern dissimilarity. Furthermore, to compare the similarity of emotion associations between terms and patches for each colour, we calculated PSIcolour. PSIcolour was estimated per colour using Pearson correlations (12 correlations, each time 1 × 20 colour term vector correlated to 1 × 20 colour patch vector).
To test whether the proportion of participants endorsing an association differed between terms and patches, we used a mixed-design 2 × 240 ANOVA on proportion values, which accounted for the repeated-measures design. This test indicated whether emotion associations were overall more likely for terms or patches. To identify the source of any dissimilarity, we used Fisher’s (1922) exact tests to compare the proportion of participants endorsing a particular colour–emotion association (yes/no; n = 240) for terms and patches.
Emotion dimensions
We derived emotion dimensions associated with colours from the number of emotion concepts associated with each colour. For each colour, we counted how many positive and negative (valence), high and low arousal (arousal), and strong and weak (power) emotion concepts each participant chose (Table S2). The number of emotions indicated the broadness of associations and was our second dependent variable (DV2). DV2 varied from 0 to 10 for each level of valence, arousal, and power. We conducted a mixed-design 2 × 2 × 12 multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) model with broadness of valence, arousal, and power as dependent variables, and levels of emotion dimensions (positive-negative, high-low arousal, and strong-weak), colour presentation mode, and colour as independent variables.
Emotion intensity
The third dependent variable (DV3)—emotion intensity—was calculated by averaging intensity ratings assigned to emotion concepts associated with each colour. Emotion intensity varied from 1 (weak) to 5 (strong), unless no emotion concept was chosen (coded as missing value). A series of independent-samples t tests compared emotion intensity ratings (DV3) between terms and patches overall, and for each colour separately. We used t tests to preserve statistical power due to missing values.
Across the statistical tests, where appropriate, we controlled familywise error (type I error) using false discovery rate (FDR) correction and marked the corrected p values as pFDR (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995). Alpha level was set at .05; all analyses were two-tailed. We performed analyses and created graphs with the R v.3.4.0 and SPSS v.25.
Results
Specific Emotions
Across terms and patches, the cluster analysis indicated three clusters of specific colour–emotion associations with a satisfactory goodness of fit (silhouette measure of cohesion and separation was 0.7, Bayesian Information Criterion = 64.9). Cluster 1 included prominent associations (n = 26), endorsed by many participants (38%–73%). The most prominent specific colour–emotion associations were red-anger (73%), red-love (68%), red-hate (51%), red-pleasure (39%), orange-joy (48%), orange-amusement (41%), yellow-joy (61%), yellow-amusement (44%), turquoise-joy (45%), turquoise-pleasure (41%), blue-relief (38%), pink-pleasure (63%), pink-love (63%), pink-joy (55%), pink-amusement (42%), brown-disgust (50%), white-relief (44%), grey-sadness (61%), grey-regret (55%), grey-disappointment (54%), black-disappointment (48%), black-hate (47%), black-sadness (45%), black-regret (45%), black-fear (45%), and black-contempt (43%). Green and purple had no specific emotion associations in the cluster of the most prominent associations. Cluster 2 included occasional associations (n = 77) endorsed by 17% to 37% of participants (e.g., blue-sadness, green-amusement). Cluster 3 included rare associations (n = 137) endorsed by 2% to 17% of participants (e.g., pink-pride, blue-love, red-amusement). We show the colour–emotion associations as proportions in Table S3 for terms and patches together, in Table S4 for terms, in Table S5 for patches, and graphically in Figure 3.
After having established the specific colour–emotion associations, we compared the associations between terms and patches. The PSI comparing the patterns of colour–emotion associations with matrix-to-matrix correlations indicated a high degree of similarity (r = .82, R2 = .672, p < .001) between terms and patches (Figure 3(a): terms, Figure 3(b): patches). This result means that similar emotion concepts were associated with colours as terms and as patches. Furthermore, colour-specific PSIcolour were high (r = .79–.96, R2 = .624–.922) for all colours except purple (r = −.23, R2 = .053, pFDR = .340, see Table 2), indicating that the similarity between terms and patches held across all colours except purple. The specific emotion concept associations with purple as a term were unrelated to those with purple as a patch.
Table 2.
Colour | PSI |
---|---|
Red | .92*** |
Orange | .94*** |
Yellow | .90*** |
Green | .88*** |
Turquoise | .88*** |
Blue | .79*** |
Purple | −.23 |
Pink | .92*** |
Brown | .88*** |
White | .91*** |
Grey | .96*** |
Black | .81*** |
Note. All p values are FDR corrected for multiple comparisons. PSI = Pattern Similarity Index
***p < .001, PSI = 1 suggests perfect similarity.
When looking at the proportion of participants endorsing colour–emotion associations, the mixed-design ANOVA on the proportion values showed a main effect of presentation mode, F(1, 239) = 33.05, p < .001, = .121. A higher proportion of participants endorsed emotion associations with colour terms than with colour patches. Fisher’s exact tests identified that the associations between black and hate, pFDR < .001, odds ratio [OR] = 8.64, 95% CI = [3.59, 22.48]; black and anger, pFDR = .007, OR = 5.13, 95% CI = [2.05, 14.26]; black and regret, pFDR = .007, OR = 4.48, 95% CI = [1.98, 10.66]; and black and sadness, pFDR = .022, OR = 3.85, 95% CI = [1.72, 9.00] were more often chosen when black was a term than a patch. No other comparisons were significant (see Figure 3(c)).
Emotion Dimensions
The mixed-design MANOVA estimating the number of emotion concepts (broadness) was overall significant; Pillai’s trace value = .55, F(1, 130) = 160.0, p < .001, = .552. The MANOVA indicated a main effect of colour, Pillai’s trace value = .52, F(11, 120) = 11.70, p < .001, = .517, demonstrating that the number of associated emotion concepts varied by colour. This effect was significant for individual mixed-design ANOVAs on valence, F(11, 1430) = 15.62, p < .001, = .107, arousal, F(11, 1430) = 15.62, p < .001, = .107, and power, F(11, 1430) = 15.62, p < .001, = .107. Planned deviation contrasts indicated that red, yellow, pink, and black yielded a greater number and purple, brown, and white yielded a smaller number of emotion concepts than expected on average (ps ≤ .012; Table 3).
Table 3.
Colour | Colour (term and patch) |
Colour term |
Colour patch |
t Value | Cohen’s d | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | 95% CI | Range | M | 95% CI | Range | M | 95% CI | Range | |||
Red | 4.70 | [3.98, 5.43] | 0–20 | 5.00 | [4.02, 5.98] | 1–20 | 4.28 | [3.18, 5.37] | 0–17 | 0.98 | 0.17 |
Orange | 3.45 | [2.86, 4.03] | 0–20 | 3.59 | [2.72, 4.46] | 0–20 | 3.24 | [2.54, 3.94] | 0–9 | 0.62 | 0.11 |
Yellow | 3.96 | [3.32, 4.61] | 0–20 | 3.97 | [3.09, 4.86] | 0–20 | 3.94 | [2.99, 4.90] | 0–16 | 0.05 | 0.01 |
Green | 3.64 | [3.01, 4.27] | 0–20 | 3.92 | [2.99, 4.86] | 0–20 | 3.24 | [2.47, 4.01] | 0–11 | 1.13 | 0.19 |
Turquoise | 3.48 | [2.87, 4.1] | 0–20 | 3.47 | [2.60, 4.35] | 0–20 | 3.50 | [2.67, 4.33] | 0–11 | −0.04 | 0.01 |
Blue | 3.95 | [3.33, 4.58] | 0–20 | 4.17 | [3.24, 5.10] | 0–20 | 3.65 | [2.89, 4.41] | 1–11 | 0.86 | 0.15 |
Purple | 3.26 | [2.65, 3.87] | 0–20 | 3.56 | [2.64, 4.48] | 0–20 | 2.81 | [2.12, 3.51] | 0–10 | 1.30 | 0.22 |
Pink | 4.23 | [3.6, 4.85] | 0–20 | 4.42 | [3.54, 5.30] | 0–20 | 3.94 | [3.06, 4.83] | 1–12 | 0.77 | 0.13 |
Brown | 2.53 | [1.9, 3.16] | 0–20 | 2.50 | [1.58, 3.42] | 0–20 | 2.57 | [1.76, 3.39] | 0–14 | −0.12 | 0.02 |
White | 3.01 | [2.4, 3.61] | 0–20 | 3.45 | [2.52, 4.37] | 0–20 | 2.37 | [1.73, 3.01] | 0–12 | 1.91 | 0.32 |
Grey | 3.70 | [3.1, 4.31] | 0–20 | 4.13 | [3.22, 5.03] | 0–20 | 3.09 | [2.39, 3.80] | 0–13 | 1.80 | 0.31 |
Black | 4.51 | [3.87, 5.14] | 0–20 | 5.56 | [4.64, 6.49] | 0–20 | 2.98 | [2.34, 3.63] | 0–10 | 4.59*** | 0.77 |
Overall | 3.70 | [3.52, 3.88] | 0–20 | 3.98 | [3.72, 4.24] | 0–20 | 3.30 | [3.07, 3.53] | 0–17 | 1.04 | 0.19 |
Note. All p values are FDR corrected. CI = confidence interval.
***pFDR < .001.
There was no main effect for colour presentation mode, Pillai’s trace value = .01, F(1, 130) = 1.38, p = .242, = .011. This result means that the same number of emotions, on average, was associated with terms and patches. Nonetheless, there was a significant interaction between colour and colour presentation mode, Pillai’s trace value = .27, F(11, 120) = 3.97, p < .001, = .267. This interaction was present for all three individual mixed-design ANOVAs, namely on valence, F(11, 1430) = 5.59, p < .001, = .041, arousal, F(11, 1430) = 5.59, p < .001, = .041, and power, F(11, 1430) = 5.59, p < .001, = .041. Post hoc comparisons revealed that a greater number of emotion concepts was associated with black when presented as a term than as a patch (pFDR < .001). The term versus patch comparisons were not significant for other colours (all psFDR ≥ .30; Table 3).
Valence
Following the results of the mixed-design MANOVA with a mixed-design ANOVA on valence, there was the main effect of valence, F(1, 130) = 70.82, p < .001, = .353, indicating a positivity bias. Participants overall associated more positive (M = 2.10, 95% CI = [1.80, 2.40]) than negative (M = 1.54, 95% CI = [1.26, 1.83]) emotion concepts with colours. A significant interaction between valence and colour presentation mode, F(1, 130) = 5.40, p = .022, = .040, indicated that this positivity bias was only present for patches (pFDR = .012; terms: pFDR = .181).
A significant interaction between valence and colour, F(11, 1430) = 76.39, p < .001, = .370, indicated that colours differed in valence. To break down this interaction, we used a series of post hoc t tests, FDR adjusted for multiple comparisons, to compare the number of positive and negative emotion concepts associated with each colour (so-called valence bias). Pink, white, green, orange, blue, yellow, and turquoise were all significantly biased towards positive associations (all psFDR < .001), while black, grey, and brown (all psFDR < .001) were significantly biased towards negative associations. Red (pFDR = .974) and purple (pFDR = .765) exhibited no valence bias, meaning that the same number of positive and negative emotion concepts was on average associated with these colours (Figure 4(a) and (b), Table S 6).
A significant three-way interaction between valence, colour, and colour presentation mode, F(11, 1430) = 3.70, p < .001, = .028, suggested that the valence bias varied by colour presentation mode. A series of FDR-corrected t tests revealed that a greater number of negative emotion concepts were associated with black when presented as a term (M = 4.92, 95% CI = [4.26, 5.59]) than as a patch (M = 2.74, 95% CI = [2.10, 3.38], pFDR < .001). No other comparisons were significant (all psFDR ≥ .085, Figure 4).
Arousal
Following the results of the mixed-design MANOVA with a mixed-design ANOVA on arousal, there was no main effect of arousal, F(1, 130) = 1.37, p = .245, = .010. Participants associated the same number of emotion concepts of high (M = 1.85, 95% CI = [1.56, 2.14]) and low arousal (M = 1.80, 95% CI = [1.51, 2.08]) with colours. A significant two-way interaction between arousal and colour presentation mode was observed, F(1, 130) = 9.79, p = .002, = .070. However, no statistically significant differences were seen in post hoc tests.
A significant two-way interaction between arousal and colour, F(11, 1430) = 57.69, p < .001, = .307, showed that colours differed in how arousing they were (arousal bias). FDR-corrected post hoc tests demonstrated that red (pFDR < .001), pink (pFDR < .001), yellow (pFDR < .001), and orange (pFDR = .001) were associated with a greater number of high compared with low arousal emotion concepts. Grey (pFDR < .001), brown (pFDR < .001), white (pFDR < .001), black (pFDR < .001), and blue (pFDR = .012) yielded a greater number of low compared with high arousal emotion concepts. Purple (pFDR = .084), turquoise (pFDR = .327), and green (pFDR = .867) did not differ in terms of arousal (see Figure 4(a); Table S6). The three-way interaction between arousal, colour, and colour presentation mode was not significant, F(11, 1430) = 1.63, p = .085, = .012.
Power
Following the results of the mixed-design MANOVA with a mixed-design ANOVA on power, there was no main effect of power, F(1, 130) = 2.16, p = .144, = .016, indicating that participants associated the same number of strong (M = 1.85, 95% CI = [1.57, 2.14]) and weak (M = 1.79, 95% CI = [1.50, 2.08]) emotion concepts with colours. Despite a two-way interaction effect between power and colour presentation mode, F(1, 130) = 7.05, p = .009, = .051, no statistically significant differences were seen in post hoc tests.
A significant two-way interaction effect between power and colour, F(11, 1430) = 31.69, p < .001, = .196, demonstrated that colours differed in power (power bias). FDR-corrected post hoc tests demonstrated that yellow (pFDR < .001), red (pFDR < .001), orange (pFDR < .001), green (pFDR < .001), and turquoise (pFDR = .027) were associated with a greater number of strong emotion concepts than weak emotion concepts. Grey (pFDR < .001), white (pFDR < .001), and blue (pFDR = .002) were associated with a greater number of weak compared with strong emotion concepts. Purple (pFDR = .341), black (pFDR = .576), pink (pFDR = .904), and brown (pFDR = .905) did not differ in terms of power (see Figure 4(b); Table S6). The three-way interaction effect between power, colour, and colour presentation mode was not significant, F(11, 1430) = 1.37, p = .181, = .010.
Emotion Intensity
An independent samples t test showed that participants associated emotion concepts of higher intensity with terms than with patches, t(130) = 2.32, p = .022, d = 0.41. Analyses for individual colours, after the FDR correction for multiple comparisons, indicated that this difference was only present for black (Table 4).
Table 4.
Colour | Colour (term and patch) |
Colour term |
Colour patch |
N | t value | Cohen’s d | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | 95% CI | Range | M | 95% CI | Range | M | 95% CI | Range | ||||
Red | 3.86 | [3.70, 4.01] | 1.9–5.0 | 3.99 | [3.82, 4.17] | 1.9–5.0 | 3.65 | [3.38, 3.93] | 1.0–5.0 | 130 | 2.16 | 0.39 |
Orange | 3.32 | [3.15, 3.49] | 1.0–5.0 | 3.36 | [3.16, 3.57] | 1.0–5.0 | 3.25 | [2.95, 3.56] | 1.0–5.0 | 120 | 0.62 | 0.11 |
Yellow | 3.55 | [3.39, 3.70] | 1.0–5.0 | 3.55 | [3.34, 3.76] | 1.0–5.0 | 3.54 | [3.30, 3.79] | 1.0–5.0 | 129 | 0.04 | 0.01 |
Green | 3.24 | [3.07, 3.41] | 1.0–5.0 | 3.38 | [3.20, 3.56] | 1.0–5.0 | 3.05 | [2.72, 3.38] | 1.0–5.0 | 124 | 1.90 | 0.35 |
Turquoise | 3.51 | [3.32, 3.71] | 1.0–5.0 | 3.66 | [3.42, 3.90] | 1.0–5.0 | 3.31 | [2.99, 3.63] | 1.0–5.0 | 118 | 1.80 | 0.34 |
Blue | 3.44 | [3.29, 3.59] | 1.0–5.0 | 3.49 | [3.28, 3.70] | 1.0–5.0 | 3.37 | [3.15, 3.60] | 1.3–5.0 | 128 | 0.74 | 0.13 |
Purple | 3.21 | [3.02, 3.40] | 1.0–5.0 | 3.32 | [3.09, 3.55] | 1.0–5.0 | 3.07 | [2.75, 3.40] | 1.0–5.0 | 118 | 1.26 | 0.23 |
Pink | 3.56 | [3.39, 3.74] | 1.0–5.0 | 3.72 | [3.50, 3.95] | 1.0–5.0 | 3.34 | [3.06, 3.62] | 1.0–5.0 | 129 | 2.13 | 0.38 |
Brown | 3.34 | [3.14, 3.53] | 2.0–5.0 | 3.33 | [3.10, 3.57] | 2.0–5.0 | 3.34 | [3.01, 3.68] | 1.0–5.0 | 108 | −0.07 | −0.01 |
White | 3.56 | [3.36, 3.76] | 1.7–5.0 | 3.69 | [3.46, 3.92] | 1.7–5.0 | 3.37 | [3.01, 3.73] | 1.0–5.0 | 116 | 1.56 | 0.30 |
Grey | 3.55 | [3.37, 3.72] | 1.8–5.0 | 3.61 | [3.38, 3.84] | 1.8–5.0 | 3.45 | [3.17, 3.74] | 1.0–5.0 | 128 | 0.85 | 0.15 |
Black | 3.62 | [3.45, 3.79] | 1.8–5.0 | 3.86 | [3.67, 4.04] | 1.8–5.0 | 3.26 | [2.95, 3.57] | 1.0–5.0 | 127 | 3.55** | 0.64 |
Overall | 3.49 | [3.37, 3.61] | 1.8–5.0 | 3.61 | [3.46, 3.75] | 1.8–5.0 | 3.33 | [3.12, 3.53] | 1.7–4.8 | 132 | 2.32* | 0.41 |
Note. Significant differences, after the FDR correction, in emotion intensity between colour presentation modes are flagged as *pFDR < .050 and **pFDR < .010. CI = confidence interval.
Discussion
Research on colour–emotion associations has used colour patches or colour terms. In the former case, the perceptual attributes of colour are considered decisive. In the latter, perceptual attributes are little controlled and linguistic features are considered decisive. In this study, we tested whether colour–emotion associations were comparable when using colour terms and colour patches (similar to Wang et al., 2014). Our French-speaking participants associated a large number of emotion concepts (Scherer, 2005; Scherer et al., 2013) with a representative number of either colour terms or patches and rated the intensity of the associated emotion concepts. In our analyses, we accounted for colour associations with (a) specific emotions, (b) emotion dimensions (valence, arousal, and power; Fontaine et al., 2007), as well as (c) emotion intensity.
Cluster analysis indicated that colour associations with specific emotions differed in terms of frequency. Some associations were frequent (e.g., red-anger, red-love, yellow-joy), other associations occurred occasionally (e.g., blue-sadness), and still other associations were rare (e.g., blue-love). Some colours were associated with a single specific emotion (brown-disgust and white-relief), while others with several specific emotions (red-love, red-anger; yellow-joy, and yellow-amusement). Further emotion associations were better described in terms of emotion dimensions. For instance, black was associated with mainly negative emotions, grey with negative and weak emotions, blue with positive emotions, and green with positive and powerful emotions. Important to our study, colour associations with specific emotions were very similar (similarity coefficient of .82) when contrasting the associations between colour terms and patches. Similarities ranged from .79 (blue) to .96 (grey), with the exception of low similarity for purple (−.23). The degree of dissimilarity is likely due to additional perceptual or linguistic factors, or perhaps noise in the data. In this study, we observed some systematic dissimilarities between colour terms and colour patches. Participants, overall and particularly for black, (a) were more likely to select an emotion concept for terms than for patches and (b) selected emotion concepts of higher intensity for terms than for patches. Participants also associated more negative emotions with black when the colour was presented as a term than as a patch.
Our study showed a high degree of similarity in colour associations with specific emotions and emotion dimensions between colour terms and colour patches, at least in a French-speaking population. This similarity might indicate that emotions are associated with an abstract representation of colour (i.e., a colour concept; Abbott et al., 2016). This abstract representation can be accessed via colour perceptions, at least when they are close to focal colours, or basic colour terms. Potentially, similar shades of colour named by the same colour term might be associated with more comparable emotions than similar shades of colour named by different colour terms. For instance, when the colour term red denoted both a typical red (i.e., potentially focal) and a dark shade of red, the same emotion associations emerged (i.e., anger and love; Fugate & Franco, 2019). In contrast, in our study, focal red (i.e., named red) was associated with positive and negative emotions (i.e., love, anger, and hate), while light red (i.e., named pink) was exclusively associated with positive emotions (i.e., love, joy, and pleasure; see also Fugate & Franco, 2019; Gil & Le Bigot, 2014; Jonauskaite, Dael, et al., 2019; Kaya & Epps, 2004; Sutton & Altarriba, 2016). Despite these observations, lightness and saturation might play a more important role than hue when shades of colour are drastically different from focal colours (e.g., Dael et al., 2016; Palmer et al., 2013; Specker et al., 2018; Valdez & Mehrabian, 1994).
High similarity in emotion associations with colour terms and colour patches held for almost all colours. For instance, we replicated the widely observed association between lightness and positivity (Allan, 2009; Lakens et al., 2013; Meier et al., 2007; Specker et al., 2018; Valdez & Mehrabian, 1994). Whether lightness was perceived or only imagined, white was associated with exclusively positive emotions and black and grey with exclusively negative emotions. Other colours were associated with both known and new emotions. For instance, in addition to associating yellow with joy (e.g., Fugate & Franco, 2019; Sutton & Altarriba, 2016) and brown and disgust (e.g., Fugate & Franco, 2019), our participants also associated yellow with amusement, orange with joy and amusement, and turquoise with joy and pleasure.
The only colour that exhibited different specific emotion associations when presented as a term and as a patch was purple. While the pattern of emotion associations with purple was uncorrelated between terms and patches, purple did not carry widely shared associations with emotion concepts. No emotion concept was chosen for purple by more than 20% of participants. Other empirical studies have already suggested that purple carries idiosyncratic emotion connotations (Fugate & Franco, 2019; Hemphill, 1996; Hupka et al., 1997; Sutton & Altarriba, 2016). Hence, purple might be the most affectively neutral or the most affectively ambiguous colour.
Another colour that was somewhat different between terms and patches was black. However, black did not differ in terms of which emotions were associated, only in terms of how likely the associations were. Whether black was presented as a term or a patch, it was associated with almost all of the given negative emotions (i.e., sadness, guilt, regret, disappointment, fear, disgust, contempt, hate, and anger; see also Adams & Osgood, 1973; Burkitt & Sheppard, 2014; Fugate & Franco, 2019; Hanada, 2018; Specker et al., 2018; Valdez & Mehrabian, 1994). When black was a term, however, the negative associations were stronger and more intense, especially with hate, anger, regret, and sadness. Potentially, terms evoke colour percepts that differ from patches. Uusküla and Eessalu (2018) showed that the supposedly black glossy Munsell colour chip was named as black only by a minority of their participants in a colour naming study. If our black colour patch was not black enough, participants might have for that reason associated the patch with fewer negative emotions. Another possibility is the difference between black in the linguistic and perceptual contexts. Black might evoke more negative associations linguistically (e.g., black magic, blackmail, etc., Allan, 2009) rather than perceptually, the latter triggering notions of sophistication and elegance (Labrecque & Milne, 2012). Hence, while being associated with similar negative emotions perceptually and linguistically, black was even more negative in the linguistic context.
Based on different theoretical frameworks, we expected that certain colour–emotion associations might be more prevalent in frequency and intensity when conveyed linguistically (i.e., blue and sadness, yellow and negative emotions), while others might be more prevalent in frequency and intensity when conveyed perceptually (i.e., yellow and joy, blue and positive emotions). Our data demonstrated that yellow was associated with joy (see also Jonauskaite, Althaus, et al., 2019; Kaya & Epps, 2004; Sutton & Altarriba, 2016), which is likely explained by its link to sunshine (Jonauskaite, Abdel-Khalek, et al., 2019). Yellow was not associated with any negative emotion, as some colour expressions would have predicted (e.g., yellow-bellied or rire jaune [yellow laughter]). Our data also showed that blue was associated with positive emotions (see also Adams & Osgood, 1973; Kaya & Epps, 2004; Manav, 2007; Valdez & Mehrabian, 1994; Wexner, 1954), potentially related to experiences of a clear sky or clean water (Palmer & Schloss, 2010). Blue was not in general associated with any negative emotion. The only negative association with blue was sadness, endorsed by 27% of our participants (see also Barchard, Grob, & Roe, 2017; Palmer et al., 2013; Sutton & Altarriba, 2016). This association can be related to colour expressions like feeling blue in English and avoirs des bleus à l’âme (the soul being bruised, meaning feeling sad, and melancholic) in French. Hence, these examples suggest that—relatively speaking—conceptual colour–emotion associations might have been more strongly reinforced by the perceptual rather than the linguistic systems. A more systematic investigation with a greater number of colours and emotions and a greater number of colour metaphors would be useful to examine this conjecture further.
High emotion similarity between colour terms and colour patches cannot provide equivocal support to the theories favouring the role of language (e.g., conceptual metaphor theory; Lakoff & Johnson, 1999) or perception (e.g., Ecological Valence Theory; Palmer & Schloss, 2010). Of course, these theories have not focussed on colour–emotion associations but instead propose generic association mechanisms. More specific to colour, Wang et al. (2014) proposed a distinction between natural and social colour–emotion associations. Natural associations are believed to be reinforced by perceptual pairings and social associations through linguistic and cultural pairings. They suggested that the association between red and negative emotions is natural and apparent when red is a patch, while an association between red and positive emotions is social and apparent when red is a term. We did not replicate the same distinction in the Swiss French-speaking population neither for red nor for any other colour. The valence of all 12 studied colours did not differ between terms and patches. It is unclear to what extent cultural or methodological differences could account for the discrepancy. For instance, in a related study linking 12 colour terms with 20 emotion concepts (Jonauskaite, Wicker, et al., 2019), Chinese participants chose a large number of positive emotions (especially, love and joy) for the term red but also associated red with anger. Hence, colour–emotion associations might be reinforced by perceptual, linguistic, and cultural systems, and the weight of these factors might vary by culture.
Our results might inform theories of embodied cognition (e.g., Barsalou, 1999), providing evidence for an overlapping representation of linguistic and perceptual stimuli. For instance, the classic Stroop effect (Stroop, 1935) demonstrates how naming the ink of letters is hampered when the letters spell an inconsistent colour term. Similarly, auditory presentation of task-unrelated colour terms has been shown to interfere with discrimination performance for colour patches (Richter & Zwaan, 2009). Linguistic reference to red, by including the word red in the description of a person, increased their perceived attractiveness (Pazda & Elliot, 2017). Similarly, linguistic reference to red, when this word appeared on the exam sheet, hampered students’ intellectual performance (Lichtenfeld, Maier, Elliot, & Pekrun, 2009). Equivalent effects on attractiveness and intellectual performance have been reported for perceptual experiences of red (Lehmann, Elliot, & Calin-Jageman, 2018; M. A. Maier, Elliot, & Lichtenfeld, 2008; Mehta & Zhu, 2009). A recent psychophysiological study showed that the words darkness and brightness triggered comparable pupillary responses to perceptual stimuli (Mathôt, Grainger, & Strijkers, 2017). Imaging studies have also demonstrated shared neural networks of colour perception and colour knowledge in the left fusiform gyrus (Simmons et al., 2007; Slotnick, 2009) and the left lingual gyrus (Hsu, Frankland, & Thompson-Schill, 2012). On the other hand, the existence of distinct neurological conditions such as colour anomia (i.e., inability to name visually presented colours; Davidoff & Ostergaard, 1984), colour agnosia (i.e., inability to recognise colours; Davidoff, 1996), and cerebral achromatopsia (i.e., complete colour blindness after cortical damage; Zeki, 1990) provide evidence for (at least partly) separated neural networks. Hence, it appears that colour perception and colour semantics engage to some degree overlapping neural networks.
In conclusion, our results demonstrate that conceptual associations between emotions and colours were very similar when a colour was presented perceptually (i.e., patch of a focal colour) and linguistically (i.e., a basic colour term). Hence, studies associating emotions with colour terms or patches can be compared and their results integrated (with some caution taken for purple and black). Future studies can choose to study colour terms or colour patches based on their experimental design and not under the assumption that one method would give more accurate results than the other. Our results would indicate that emotion concepts are associated with a colour concept—an abstract representation of colour—rather than specific perceptual or linguistic properties of colour. This suggestion is true at least when perceived colours relate to focal colours. Future studies may investigate emotion associations with colour patches that are difficult to name (e.g., that border between two neighbouring colour terms; Parraga & Akbarinia, 2016) and with colour patches that vary in lightness and saturation but are named by the same basic term. Our results could also be replicated in a within-subject design to ensure that similarity between colour terms and patches holds within groups. Note that our study cannot determine whether language or perception drives colour–emotion relationships in the first place: We do not know whether colour patches were named or colour terms imagined or both. To disentangle the origins of colour–emotion associations, it would be necessary to move towards less typical populations. For instance, studies in populations that possess different numbers of colour categories would be informative (see M. Maier & Abdel Rahman, 2018). Here, we demonstrated that, once acquired, conceptual colour–emotion associations depend little on how colour is presented.
Supplemental Material
Supplemental material, IPE902484 Supplemental material for Feeling Blue or Seeing Red? Similar Patterns of Emotion Associations With Colour Patches and Colour Terms by Domicele Jonauskaite, C. Alejandro Parraga, Michael Quiblier and Christine Mohr in i-Perception
Acknowledgements
The authors wish to thank Nele Dael for conceptual support, Jean-Philippe Antonietti and Ahmad Abu-Akel for statistical support, Amer Chamseddine for programming Experiment 1, Guillaume Sierro for programming Experiment 2, Richard Klein for contributing to the translation of the instructions, Carole Biggam for insightful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript, and anonymous reviewers for their helpful suggestions.
Author Biographies
Domicele Jonauskaite is a doctoral student at the Institute of Psychology, University of Lausanne, Switzerland. Her doctoral position is financially supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation with a personal Doc.CH grant. Within its framework, she investigates the importance of perceptual versus linguistic routes in affective connotations of colour. She holds a BSc degree in psychology (Royal Holloway, University of London, UK) and MSc degree in neuroscience (University of Geneva, Switzerland). Together with Christine Mohr, they have founded the Colour Experience (colourexperience.ch) platform designed to disseminate scientifically validated empirical research on psychological effects of colour.
C. Alejandro Parraga received his first degree in physics from the Universidad Nacional de Tucumán, Argentina and his PhD degree in visual perception from the University of Bristol, UK. In 2008, he was recipient of the “Ramón y Cajal” fellowship, Spain. Since 2012, he works in the Computer Vision Centre/Computer Science Department of the Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona on biologically plausible computational models of visual perception, colour vision, and psychophysics.
Michael Quiblier is a third-year bachelor student in psychology at the University of Lausanne, Switzerland. Since 2017, he has been working as a research assistant in affective colour psychology at the local Cognitive and Affective Regulation Laboratory. Michael is currently an exchange student at the Boise State University, USA, where he went via the International Student Exchange Program. He intends to pursue his further academic training through a master in psychology.
Christine Mohr is a professor for cognitive psychology at the University of Lausanne, Switzerland. She graduated with a psychology degree from the University of Konstanz, Germany. Subsequently, she completed her PhD at the Institute of Psychology at the University of Zürich, Switzerland. After postdoctoral studies in Geneva, Switzerland, and Edmonton, Canada, she continued her academic career as a lecturer and later senior lecturer at the University of Bristol, UK. Now in Lausanne, she focusses on two major research lines: (a) colour psychology with a particular interest in the relationship between colour and affect and (b) cognitive and affective factors in adult belief and its formation. By today, she authored or coauthored over 120 peer-reviewed research contributions.
Note
Colour terms in italics (e.g., yellow) refer to both a colour patch (i.e., a yellow perceptual stimulus) and a colour term (i.e., jaune being yellow in French, because we conducted this study in French).
Contributor Information
Domicele Jonauskaite, Institute of Psychology, University of Lausanne, Switzerland.
C. Alejandro Parraga, Computer Science Department, Computer Vision Center, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain.
Authors’ Contributions
D. J. and C. M.: conception and design experiment; D. J.: data analyses; D. J. and M. Q.: data collection; C. A. P. and C. M.: supervision; D. J. and C. M.: manuscript writing (original draft); D. J., M. Q., C. A. P., and C. M.: manuscript writing (editing).
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Funding
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This research was supported with a Doc.CH fellowship grant to D. J. (P0LAP1_175055) and a research grant to C. M. (100014_182138) from the Swiss National Science Foundation. Its initiation was possible, thanks to AkzoNobel, Imperial Chemical Industries Limited, in particular Dr. David Elliott and Dr. Tom Curwen, Color R&I team, Slough, UK, and Stephanie Kraneveld, Sassenheim, the Netherlands, who supported this empirical work on colour preferences and emotions. C. A. P. was funded by grant 2017-SGR-649 and grant DPI2017-89867-C2-1-R from the Catalan and the Spanish Science Ministries.
ORCID iD
Domicele Jonauskaite https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7513-9766
Supplementary Material
Supplementary material for this article is available online at: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/suppl/10.1177/2041669520902484.
References
- Abbott J. T., Griffiths T. L., Regier T. (2016). Focal colors across languages are representative members of color categories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113, 11178–11183. doi:10.1073/pnas.1513298113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adams F. M., Osgood C. E. (1973). A cross-cultural study of the affective meanings of color. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 4, 135–157. doi:10.1177/002202217300400201 [Google Scholar]
- Allan K. (2009). The connotations of English colour terms: Colour-based X-phemisms. Journal of Pragmatics, 41, 626–637. doi:10.1016/j.pragma.2008.06.004 [Google Scholar]
- Allen E. C., Guilford J. P. (1936). Factors determining the affective values of color combinations. The American Journal of Psychology, 48, 643–648. Retrieved from https://www.jstor.org/stable/1416516 [Google Scholar]
- Bacher J., Wenzig K., Vogler M. (2004). SPSS TwoStep cluster—A first evaluation. In Recent developments and applications in social research methodology: Proceedings of the RC33 Sixth International Conference on Social Science Methodology. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Barbara Budrich Publishers; Retrieved from https://nbn-resolving.org/urn:nbn:de:0168-ssoar-327153 [Google Scholar]
- Barchard K. A., Grob K. E., Roe M. J. (2017). Is sadness blue? The problem of using figurative language for emotions on psychological tests. Behavior Research Methods, 49, 443–456. doi:10.3758/s13428-016-0713-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barsalou L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–660. doi:10.1017/S0140525X99252144 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benitez-Quiroz C. F., Srinivasan R., Martinez A. M. (2018). Facial color is an efficient mechanism to visually transmit emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115, 3581–3586. doi:10.1073/pnas.1716084115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benjamini Y., Hochberg Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B (Methodological), 57, 289–300. doi:10.2307/2346101 [Google Scholar]
- Biggam C. P. (2012). Basic colour categories. In The semantics of colour: A historical approach (pp. 58–69). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
- Brainard D. H., Pelli D. G., Robson T. (2002). Display characterization In Hornak J. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of imaging science and technology (pp. 172–188). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. doi:10.1002/0471443395.img011 [Google Scholar]
- Burkitt E., Sheppard L. (2014). Children’s colour use to portray themselves and others with happy, sad and mixed emotion. Educational Psychology, 34, 231–251. doi:10.1080/01443410.2013.785059 [Google Scholar]
- Dael N., Perseguers M.-N., Marchand C., Antonietti J.-P., Mohr C. (2016). Put on that colour, it fits your emotion: Colour appropriateness as a function of expressed emotion. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 1619–1630. doi:10.1080/17470218.2015.1090462 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D’Andrade R., Egan M. (1974). The colors of emotion. American Ethnologist, 1, 49–63. doi:10.1525/ae.1974.1.1.02a00030 [Google Scholar]
- Davidoff J. B. (1996). Lewandowsky’s case of object-colour agnosia In Code C., Wallesch C. W., Joanette Y., Lecours A. R. (Eds.), Classic cases in neuropsychology (pp. 133–144). Hove, England: Psychology Press Ltd. [Google Scholar]
- Davidoff J. B., Ostergaard A. L. (1984). Colour anomia resulting from weakened short-term colour memory: A case study. Brain, 107, 415–431. doi:10.1093/brain/107.2.415 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eysenck H. J. (1941). A critical and experimental study of colour preferences. The American Journal of Psychology, 54, 385–394. doi:10.2307/1417683 [Google Scholar]
- Fisher R. A. (1922). On the interpretation of χ2 from contingency tables, and the calculation of P. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, 85, 87–94. doi:10.2307/2340521 [Google Scholar]
- Fontaine J. R. J. (2013). Dimensional, basic emotion, and componential approaches to meaning in psychological emotion research In Fontaine J., Scherer K., Soriano C. (Eds.), Components of emotional meaning: A sourcebook (pp. 31–45). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199592746.003.0003 [Google Scholar]
- Fontaine J. R. J., Scherer K. R., Roesch E. B., Ellsworth P. C. (2007). The world of emotions is not two-dimensional. Psychological Science, 18, 1050–1057. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02024.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fugate J. M. B., Franco C. L. (2019). What color is your anger? Assessing color-emotion pairings in English speakers. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1–17. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00206 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gil S., Le Bigot L. (2014). Seeing life through positive-tinted glasses: Color-meaning associations. PLoS One, 9, e104291. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0104291 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanada M. (2018). Correspondence analysis of color–emotion associations. Color Research and Application, 43, 224–237. doi:10.1002/col.22171 [Google Scholar]
- Hemphill M. (1996). A note on adults’ color–emotion associations. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 157, 275–280. doi:10.1080/00221325.1996.9914865 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hsu N. S., Frankland S. M., Thompson-Schill S. L. (2012). Chromaticity of color perception and object color knowledge. Neuropsychologia, 50, 327–333. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.12.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hunt R. W. G., Pointer M. R. (2011). Colour order systems. In Measuring colour (4th ed., pp. 155–195). Chichester, England: John Wiley.
- Hupka R. B., Zaleski Z., Otto J., Reidl L., Tarabrina N. V. (1997). The colors of anger, envy, fear, and jealousy. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 28, 156–171. doi:10.1177/0022022197282002 [Google Scholar]
- Ishihara S. (1993). Album-test pour la recherche des dyschromatopsies congenitales, 38 plates edn [Ishihara’s test for colour deficiency: 38 plates edition]. Tokyo, Japan: Kanehara. (Original work published 1917) [Google Scholar]
- Jonauskaite, D., Abdel-Khalek, A. M., Abu-Akel, A., Al-Rasheed, A. S., Antonietti, J.-P., Ásgeirsson, Á. G., … Mohr, C. (2019). The sun is no fun without rain: Physical environments affect how we feel about yellow across 55 countries. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 66, 101350. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.101350 [Google Scholar]
- Jonauskaite D., Althaus B., Dael N., Dan-Glauser E., Mohr C. (2019). What color do you feel? Color choices are driven by mood. Color Research & Application, 44, 272–284. doi:10.1002/col.22327 [Google Scholar]
- Jonauskaite D., Dael N., Chèvre L., Althaus B., Tremea A., Charalambides L., Mohr C. (2019). Pink for girls, red for boys, and blue for both genders: Colour preferences in children and adults. Sex Roles, 80, 630–642. doi:10.1007/s11199-018-0955-z [Google Scholar]
- Jonauskaite, D., & Mohr, C. (2019). A commentary: The sun is no fun without rain: Reply to “The sun and how do we feel about the color yellow? Methodological concerns. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 101379. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.101379 [Google Scholar]
- Jonauskaite D., Wicker J., Mohr C., Dael N., Havelka J., Papadatou-Pastou M., … Oberfeld D. (2019). A machine learning approach to quantify the specificity of colour–emotion associations and their cultural differences. Royal Society Open Science, 6, 190741. doi:10.1098/rsos.190741 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaya N., Epps H. H. (2004). Relationship between color and emotion: A study of college students. College Student Journal, 38, 396–406. Retrieved from https://nzdis.org/projects/attachments/299/colorassociation-students.pdf [Google Scholar]
- Kriegeskorte N. (2008). Representational similarity analysis – connecting the branches of systems neuroscience. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 2, 1–28. doi:10.3389/neuro.06.004.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Labrecque L. I., Milne G. R. (2012). Exciting red and competent blue: The importance of color in marketing. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 40, 711–727. doi:10.1007/s11747-010-0245-y [Google Scholar]
- Lakens D., Fockenberg D. A., Lemmens K. P. H., Ham J., Midden C. J. H. (2013). Brightness differences influence the evaluation of affective pictures. Cognition and Emotion, 27, 1225–1246. doi:10.1080/02699931.2013.781501 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lakoff G., Johnson M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenges to Western thought. New York, NY: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
- Lehmann G. K., Elliot A. J., Calin-Jageman R. J. (2018). Meta-analysis of the effect of red on perceived attractiveness. Evolutionary Psychology, 16, 1–27. doi:10.1177/1474704918802412 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lichtenfeld S., Maier M. A., Elliot A. J., Pekrun R. (2009). The semantic red effect: Processing the word red undermines intellectual performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 1273–1276. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.06.003 [Google Scholar]
- Lindborg P., Friberg A. K. (2015). Colour association with music is mediated by emotion: Evidence from an experiment using a CIE Lab interface and interviews. PLoS One, 10, e0144013. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0144013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lindsey D. T., Brown A. M. (2014). The color lexicon of American English. Journal of Vision, 14, 17. doi:10.1167/14.2.17 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maier M., Abdel Rahman R. (2018). Native language promotes access to visual consciousness. Psychological Science, 29, 1757–1772. doi:10.1177/0956797618782181 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maier M. A., Elliot A. J., Lichtenfeld S. (2008). Mediation of the negative effect of red on intellectual performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1530–1540. doi:10.1177/0146167208323104 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manav B. (2007). Color-emotion associations and color preferences: A case study for residences. Color Research and Application, 32, 144–150. doi:10.1002/col.20294 [Google Scholar]
- Mathôt S., Grainger J., Strijkers K. (2017). Pupillary responses to words that convey a sense of brightness or darkness. Psychological Science, 28, 1116–1124. doi:10.1177/0956797617702699 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayr S., Buchner A., Erdfelder E., Faul F. (2007). A short tutorial of GPower. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 3, 51–59. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.32.4.932 [Google Scholar]
- Mehta R., Zhu R. (2009). Blue or red? Exploring the effect of color on cognitive task performances. Science, 323, 1226–1229. doi:10.1126/science.1169144 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meier B. P., Robinson M. D. (2005). The metaphorical representation of affect. Metaphor and Symbol, 20, 239–257. doi:10.1207/s15327868ms2004_1 [Google Scholar]
- Meier B. P., Robinson M. D., Crawford L. E., Ahlvers W. J. (2007). When “light” and “dark” thoughts become light and dark responses: Affect biases brightness judgments. Emotion, 7, 366–376. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.366 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mohr C., Jonauskaite D., Dan-Glauser E. S., Uusküla M., Dael N. (2018). Unifying research on colour and emotion: Time for a cross-cultural survey on emotion associations with colour terms In MacDonald L. W., Biggam C. P., Paramei G. V. (Eds.), Progress in colour studies: Cognition, language, and beyond (pp. 209–222). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins. doi:10.1075/z.217.11moh [Google Scholar]
- Morgan G. (1993). Basic colour terms: Comparative results for French and Russian. Journal of French Language Studies, 3, 1–17. doi:10.1017/S0959269500000326 [Google Scholar]
- Ou L. C., Yuan Y., Sato T., Lee W. Y., Szabó F., Sueeprasan S., Huertas R. (2018). Universal models of colour emotion and colour harmony. Color Research and Application, 43, 736–748. doi:10.1002/col.22243 [Google Scholar]
- Palmer S. E., Schloss K. B. (2010). An ecological valence theory of human color preference. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 8877–8882. doi:10.1073/pnas.0906172107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palmer S. E., Schloss K. B., Xu Z., Prado-Leon L. R. (2013). Music-color associations are mediated by emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 8836–8841. doi:10.1073/pnas.1212562110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parraga C. A., Akbarinia A. (2016). NICE: A computational solution to close the gap from colour perception to colour categorization. PLoS One, 11, e0149538. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0149538 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pazda A. D., Elliot A. J. (2017). Processing the word red can enhance women’s perceptions of men’s attractiveness. Current Psychology, 36, 316–323. doi:10.1007/s12144-016-9420-8 [Google Scholar]
- Regier T., Kay P., Cook R. S. (2005). Focal colors are universal after all. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 8386–8391. doi:10.1073/pnas.0503281102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richter T., Zwaan R. A. (2009). Processing of color words activates color representations. Cognition, 111, 383–389. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.02.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scherer K. R. (2005). What are emotions? And how can they be measured? Social Science Information, 44, 695–729. doi:10.1177/0539018405058216 [Google Scholar]
- Scherer K. R., Shuman V., Fontaine J. R. J., Soriano C. (2013). The GRID meets the Wheel: Assessing emotional feeling via self-report In Fontaine J. R. J., Scherer K. R., Soriano C. (Eds.), Components of emotional meaning: A sourcebook (pp. 281–298). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. doi:10.13140/RG.2.1.2694.6406 [Google Scholar]
- Simmons W. K., Ramjee V., Beauchamp M. S., McRae K., Martin A., Barsalou L. W. (2007). A common neural substrate for perceiving and knowing about color. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2802–2810. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.05.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slotnick S. D. (2009). Memory for color reactivates color processing region. NeuroReport, 20, 1568–1571. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e328332d35e [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Soriano C., Fontaine J. R. J., Scherer K. R., Akırmak G. A., Alarcón P., Alonso-Arbiol I., … Zitouni A. (2013). Cross-cultural data collection with the GRID instrument In Fontaine J., Scherer K., Soriano C. (Eds.), Components of emotional meaning: A sourcebook (pp. 98–105). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199592746.003.0007 [Google Scholar]
- Soriano C., Valenzuela J. (2009). Emotion and colour across languages: Implicit associations in Spanish colour terms. Social Science Information, 48, 421–445. doi:10.1177/0539018409106199 [Google Scholar]
- Specker E., Leder H., Rosenberg R., Hegelmaier L. M., Brinkmann H., Mikuni J., Kawabata H. (2018). The universal and automatic association between brightness and positivity. Acta Psychologica, 186, 47–53. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.04.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spence C. (2011). Crossmodal correspondences: A tutorial review. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 73, 971–995. doi:10.3758/s13414-010-0073-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stroop J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662. doi:10.1037/h0054651 [Google Scholar]
- Sutton T. M., Altarriba J. (2016). Color associations to emotion and emotion-laden words: A collection of norms for stimulus construction and selection. Behavior Research Methods, 48, 686–728. doi:10.3758/s13428-015-0598-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toulson R. E. (2013). The meanings of red envelopes: Promises and lies at a Singaporean Chinese funeral. Journal of Material Culture, 18, 155–169. doi:10.1177/1359183513483909 [Google Scholar]
- Uusküla M., Eessalu M. (2018). Glossy black is not actually ‘black’: Evidence from psycholinguistic colour-naming studies in 14 European languages. Cultura e Scienza Del Colore—Color Culture and Science Journal, 09, 39–44. doi:10.23738/ccsj.i92018.04 [Google Scholar]
- Valdez P., Mehrabian A. (1994). Effects of color on emotions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 394–409. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.123.4.394 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang T., Shu S., Mo L. (2014). Blue or red? The effects of colour on the emotions of Chinese people. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 17, 152–158. doi:10.1111/ajsp.12050 [Google Scholar]
- Wexner L. B. (1954). The degree to which colors (hues) are associated with mood-tones. Journal of Applied Psychology, 38, 432–435. doi:10.1037/h0062181 [Google Scholar]
- World Medical Association. (2013). World Medical Association declaration of Helsinki. Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. The Journal of the American Medical Association, 310, 2191–2194. doi:10.1001/jama.2013.281053 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeki S. (1990). A century of cerebral achromatopsia. Brain, 113, 1721–1777. doi:10.1093/brain/113.6.1721 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
How to cite this article
- Jonauskaite D., Parraga C. A., Quiblier M., Mohr C. (2020). Feeling blue or seeing red? Similar patterns of emotion associations with colour patches and colour terms. i-Perception, 11(1), 1–24. doi:10.1177/2041669520902484 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Supplemental material, IPE902484 Supplemental material for Feeling Blue or Seeing Red? Similar Patterns of Emotion Associations With Colour Patches and Colour Terms by Domicele Jonauskaite, C. Alejandro Parraga, Michael Quiblier and Christine Mohr in i-Perception