Abstract
Understanding what drives environmentally protective or destructive behavior is important to the design and implementation of effective public policies to encourage people's engagement in proenvironmental behavior (PEB). Research shows that a connection to nature is associated with greater engagement in PEB. However, the variety of instruments and methods used in these studies poses a major barrier to integrating research findings. We conducted a meta‐analysis of the relationship between connection to nature and PEB. We identified studies through a systematic review of the literature and used Comprehensive Meta‐Analysis software to analyze the results from 37 samples (n = 13,237) and to test for moderators. A random‐effects model demonstrated a positive and significant association between connection to nature and PEB (r = 0.42, 95% CI 0.36, 0.47, p < 0.001). People who are more connected to nature reported greater engagement in PEB. Standard tests indicated little effect of publication bias in the sample. There was significant heterogeneity among the samples. Univariate categorical analyses showed that the scales used to measure connection to nature and PEB were significant moderators and explained the majority of the between‐study variance. The geographic location of a study, age of participants, and the percentage of females in a study were not significant moderators. We found that a deeper connection to nature may partially explain why some people behave more proenvironmentally than others and that the relationship is ubiquitous. Facilitating a stronger connection to nature may result in greater engagement in PEB and conservation, although more longitudinal studies with randomized experiments are required to demonstrate causation.
Keywords: connectedness to nature, conservation behavior, ecological behavior, human–nature relationship, comportamiento de conservación, comportamiento ecológico, conectividad con la naturaleza, relación humano‐naturaleza, 保护行为, 人与自然的关系, 亲近自然, 生态行为
Short abstract
Article impact statement: Interventions designed to facilitate a stronger connection to nature may result in greater engagement in proenvironmental behavior.
Metaanálisis de la Conexión Humana con la Naturaleza y el Comportamiento a Favor del Ambiental
Resumen
El entendimiento de los conductores del comportamiento de protección o destrucción ambiental es importante para el diseño e implementación de políticas públicas que fomenten la participación de las personas dentro del comportamiento a favor del ambiente (PEB, en inglés). Las investigaciones muestran que la conexión con la naturaleza está asociada con una mayor participación en el PEB. Sin embargo, la variedad de instrumentos y métodos que utilizados en estos estudios presentan una barrera importante para la integración de los resultados de las investigaciones. Realizamos un metaanálisis de la relación entre la conexión con la naturaleza y el PEB. Identificamos estudios por medio de una revisión sistemática de la literatura y utilizamos software de Metaanálisis Completo para analizar los resultados de 37 muestras (n = 13,237) y para examinar a los moderadores. Un modelo de efectos azarosos demostró una asociación positiva y significativa entre la conexión con la naturaleza y el PEB (r = 0.42, 95% CI 0.36, 0.47, p < 0.001). Las personas que están más conectadas con la naturaleza reportaron una mayor participación en el PEB. Las pruebas estándares indicaron un efecto menor del sesgo de publicación en la muestra. Hubo una heterogeneidad significativa entre las muestras. Los análisis univariados categóricos mostraron que las escalas usadas para medir la conexión con la naturaleza y el PEB fueron moderadores significativos y explicaron la mayoría de la varianza entre estudios. La ubicación geográfica de un estudio, la edad de los participantes, y el porcentaje de mujeres en un estudio no fueron moderadores significativos. Encontramos que una conexión más profunda con la naturaleza puede explicar parcialmente por qué algunas personas se comportan más a favor del ambiente que otras y que dicha relación es ubicua. La facilitación de una conexión más fuerte con la naturaleza puede resultar en una mayor participación en el PEB y en la conservación, aunque se requieren estudios más longitudinales con experimentos aleatorios para demostrar la causalidad
摘要
理解保护环境和破坏环境行为背后的驱动力, 对于设计和实施有效的公共政策以鼓励人们参与环保行动至关重要。已有研究表明, 与自然的联系和环保行动参与度之间存在相关性。然而, 这些研究使用了各种各样的工具和方法, 导致这些研究结果难以整合。因此, 我们对人与自然的联系和环保行为之间的关系进行了荟萃分析。我们通过系统文献综述筛选出相关研究, 并用综合荟萃分析软件分析了 37 个样本 (n = 13,237) 的研究结果以检验干涉变量。随机效应模型显示, 亲近自然与环保行为之间有显著的正相关关系 (r = 0.42, 95% CI 0.36, 0.47, p < 0.001) 。更亲近自然的人自我报告的环保行为参与度也更高。标准检测分析表明, 样本中的研究发表偏差的影响很小, 而各样本间存在显著的异质性。单变量分类分析结果显示, 用于测定人与自然的联系和环保行为的尺度是重要的干涉变量, 解释了大部分的研究间差异。而研究地点、参与者年龄和性别都不是显著的干涉变量。我们还发现, 与自然更深层次的联系可能部分解释了为什么有些人更环保, 而且其间的相关性普遍存在。促进人与自然更紧密的联系或能推动人们更多地参与环保和保护行动, 尽管仍需要更多包含随机实验的纵向研究来证明因果关系。【翻译: 胡怡思; 审校: 聂永刚】
Introduction
Environmental degradation, perhaps humanity's greatest current challenge (e.g., IPCC 2014), is largely anthropogenic and driven by habitat destruction, invasive species, pollution, overharvesting, and human overpopulation (Wilson 2002). Attempts to promote sustainable lifestyles or conservation behaviors must therefore focus on changing people's behavior (Ehrlich & Kennedy 2005; Schultz 2011). Understanding what motivates environmentally protective or destructive behavior can be used to encourage proenvironmental behavior (PEB) and conservation and inform government policies requiring broad‐based public support.
An individual's connection to nature may motivate their engagement in PEB (Mayer & Frantz 2004). Conversely, a lack of connection to nature has been blamed for people's apathy toward environmental degradation and protection (Pyle 2003). In humans, as the closeness of relationships between individuals increases, empathy and willingness to help increases (Cialdini et al. 1997), and this phenomenon may extend to human–nature relationships. That is, a close bond with nature may foster empathy for the natural world, which in turn may motivate caring and altruistic behavior (Schultz 2000).
Individuals’ subjective evaluations of their relationships with nature can be conceptualized as connection to nature. The theoretical basis of connection to nature comes from the biophilia hypothesis (Fromm 1964; Wilson 1984), ecopsychology (Roszak 1995; Bragg 1996), and psychological research into interpersonal relationships. Expanding self‐identity to include the natural environment and experiences of belonging with nature are key elements in defining connection to nature. Connection to nature can be considered a values‐based attitude (Brügger et al. 2011), and it has qualities similar to personality traits in that it differs between individuals and groups, is relatively stable over time and in different situations, but can change (Mayer & Frantz 2004; Nisbet et al. 2009). Connection to nature can also be a state. It can be increased or decreased in the short‐term, for example, with exposure to nature (Mayer et al. 2009). However, change may require long‐term or repeated exposure (Schultz & Tabanico 2007).
One might expect a person with a strong connection to nature to behave proenvironmentally. However, even though there is generally a high level of concern about environmental problems and support for environmental protection, there has not been widespread movement toward more sustainable lifestyles (Fox et al. 2006). This gap is partially explained by barriers a person encounters when engaging in PEB, such as personal cost (finance and time) and lack of structural support (recycling schemes and efficient public transport), or knowledge (e.g., type of PEB that is useful). It is unclear whether the same gap exists with connection to nature and PEB.
Operationalization of Connection to Nature and PEB
There are at least 17 different scales that measure connection to nature (Tam 2013a). The items in these scales reflect 3 interrelated dimensions of human relationships with nature: affect (feelings toward nature), cognition (knowledge and beliefs about nature), and behavior (actions and experiences in nature). Some scales measure connection to nature as a single dimension. These unidimensional scales can emphasize an emotional attachment to nature (Kals et al. 1999; Mayer & Frantz 2004; Perkins 2010). For example, the connectedness‐to‐nature scale was designed to assess people's affective sense of connectedness or kinship with nature (Mayer & Frantz 2004). In contrast, Schultz (2002) argues that connection to nature is inherently cognitive and defines it as the “extent to which an individual includes nature within his/her cognitive representation of self” (Schultz 2002:67). Other scales are multidimensional. For example, the nature relatedness scale (Nisbet et al. 2009) has 3 dimensions (NR‐self, NR‐perspective, and NR‐experience) that encompass affective, cognitive, and experiential aspects of the human–nature relationship. The scales have 1–40 items. Most are self‐report surveys requiring responses on a 5‐ to 7‐point Likert‐type scale, and 3 include pictorial components (Schultz 2002; Dutcher et al. 2007; Davis et al. 2009). These different scales are highly correlated and can be loaded onto a single factor and correlate similarly with criterion variables (Tam 2013a). Thus, the scales may measure the same underlying construct of connectedness to nature. There is a small amount of divergence among some scales in their association with criterion variables, such as subjective well‐being and PEB. Differences between cognitive and noncognitive components of connection to nature may explain this divergence (Tam 2013a).
The scales measuring connection to nature are positively associated with meaningful differences in individual personality traits, such as conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and openness (Nisbet & Zelenski 2013; Tam 2013a), well‐being (e.g., Capaldi et al. 2014), environmental attitudes (e.g., Schultz et al. 2004; Brügger et al. 2011), and PEB (e.g., Mayer & Frantz 2004; Nisbet & Zelenski 2013; Whitburn et al. 2018). Most of investigations into connection to nature and PEB have been in adults. However, there is a small body of evidence that shows similar results with children (Roczen et al. 2014; Collado et al. 2015; Otto & Pensini 2017).
We defined PEB broadly as “actions which contribute to environmental protection and/or conservation” (Axelrod 1993:153). Scales measuring PEB aim to capture whether an individual generally behaves proenvironmentally and includes self‐reported and objectively measured PEB. Objectively measured PEB is rare because of the difficulty in discreetly observing behavior, but it has been achieved in measuring energy usage (e.g., Frantz & Mayer 2014) and recycling behavior (Whitburn et al. 2018). The most common way to measure PEB is through self‐report surveys adapted from other scales (e.g., Kaiser 1998; Schultz & Zelezny 1998; Whitmarsh & O'Neill 2010). The scales measuring PEB vary from 6 to 97 items, contain ≥1 dimension, and vary in their internal reliability and factor analysis of their multiple dimensions (Markle 2013).
Although all studies report a positive association between connection to nature and PEB, the strength of this relationship varies (r = 0.14–0.68). The diversity of scales poses a barrier to integrating results across studies and scales. Further, it is unclear how variation in scales measuring connection to nature and PEB affect the relationship between these 2 variables.
There have been 2 meta‐analyses (Hines et al. 1987; Bamberg & Möser 2007) of sociopsychological determinants of PEB, but they did not include connection to nature. We sought to provide a quantitative synthesis of the current research on the relationship between connection to nature and PEB and to examine the effect of moderators on this relationship.
Through meta‐analysis, we aimed to provide an estimate of the size of the relationship between connection to nature and PEB, determine whether there was significant variability across our sample, and examine possible moderators. Moderators instruments used to measure connection to nature and PEB; geographic location of samples (the strength of the relationship can vary with location [Tam 2013a]); and age and gender of study participants (being older and female is associated with greater environmental attitude and PEB [Kollmuss & Agyeman 2002; Whitmarsh & O'Neill 2010]). We tested data for publication bias to determine whether the sample was broadly representative of the extant research.
Methods
We searched electronic databases (Web of Science, Science Direct, ProQuest Science and Technology, PSYCHInfo, ProQuest Dissertations, and Theses Global) for combinations of the following keywords: connection to/with nature, connectedness to nature, nature relatedness, inclusion of nature in self, disposition to connect with nature, environmental identity, emotional affinity toward nature, connectivity to nature, commitment to nature and connection to nature index; pro‐environmental, environmental, ecological, environmentally friendly, environmentally responsible, and conservation and sustainable behavior. Abstracts of promising studies were examined and the full‐text located of studies that merited further investigation. We manually examined reference sections of papers that met the inclusion criteria for additional studies. We contacted study authors if connection to nature and PEB were measured but their relationship was not reported.
Studies were included in the meta‐analysis if they examined the relationship between connection to nature and PEB; included an effect size or information to calculate an effect size (Pearson's correlation coefficients were preferred. However, Fischer's Z scores, used to calculate the effect size, can also be calculated from sample size and significance level in studies, where the correlation coefficient is not provided [e.g., Gosling & Williams 2010]); and included a self‐report assessment of explicit connection to nature and an assessment of PEB.
Connection to nature was measured by 12 different scales (Table 1). When ≥1 scale of connection to nature was used, we selected the scale that was the main focus of the study rather than alternative scales used to provide the evidence of convergent validity (e.g., Mayer & Frantz 2004; Davis et al. 2009; Brügger et al. 2011). We also selected explicit rather than implicit scales (Geng et al. 2015) because the implicit measure of connection to nature is poorly correlated with other scales and may measure a different concept (Brügger et al. 2011). We selected the nature‐relatedness scale from Tam's (2013a) comparative study because he concluded it was perhaps the most reliable scale. This scale and its short form (NR‐6) were treated as a single scale when calculating the overall and moderator effects because NR‐6 is derived from and strongly correlated to the nature‐relatedness scale and both scales show very similar associations with PEB (Nisbet & Zelenski 2013). This decision was supported by the results of our moderator analyses. The nature‐relatedness scale had a correlation with PEB of 0.51, the NR‐6 of 0.50, and when the scales were analyzed as a single entity, r = 0.51.
Table 1.
Author | Scale | Country of origin |
---|---|---|
Kals et al. 1999 | emotional affinity toward nature | Germany |
Schultz 2002 | inclusion of nature in self | U.S.A. |
Clayton 2003 | environmental identity | U.S.A. |
Mayer & Frantz 2004 | connectedness to nature | U.S.A. |
Dutcher et al. 2007 | connectivity with nature | U.S.A. |
Davis et al. 2009 | commitment to the natural environment | U.S.A. |
Nisbet et al. 2009 | nature relatedness | Canada |
Perkins 2010 | love and care for nature | Australia |
Brügger et al. 2011 | disposition to connect with nature | Switzerland |
Nisbet & Zelenski 2013 | NR‐6 (nature relatedness – short form) | Canada |
Tam 2013b | dispositional empathy with nature | China |
Beery & Wolf‐Watz 2014 | environmental connectedness | Sweden |
We came to a consensus on how to rate and classify the content of each scale and also considered the authors’ description of the scale. Each item was categorized as cognitive (associated with thoughts and beliefs, e.g., “I have a deep understanding of how my actions affect the natural world.” [Mayer & Frantz 2004] or nature is part of a person's cognitive representation of self [Schultz 2002]); affective (associated with emotions, e.g., “I feel a deep love for nature.” [Perkins 2010] or “I feel very connected to all living things and the earth.” [Nisbet et al. 2009]); or behavioral or experiential (associated with experiences in nature, e.g., “I take notice of wildlife wherever I am.” [Nisbet et al. 2009] or “I get up early to watch the sunrise.” [Brügger et al. 2011]).
Researchers measured PEB with 8 different scales, including multidimensional latent constructs of self‐reported PEB, single‐dimensional self‐reported PEB, and observed PEB. The multidimensional scales contained items that measure PEB in the areas of energy and water conservation, waste avoidance, and recycling, transport, purchasing (anticonsumerist behavior), self‐education, and social or political actions in various combinations. Studies with one‐dimensional scales, which measure similar behaviors, were combined for the analyses. These scales were self‐reported behavioral measures, such as native vegetation protection in farmland (Gosling & Williams 2010) and proenvironmental gardening and farming (Sanguinetti 2014; Dresner et al. 2015). When authors reported ≥1 PEB measure, we selected for analysis the scale that most closely reflected multidimensional self‐reported PEB. For example, actual commitment to ecological behavior (which measured self‐reported PEB) was selected over verbal commitment (Nisbet & Zelenski 2013) and environmentalism (Mayer & Frantz 2004; Nisbet & Zelenski 2013). Perkins (2010) and Beery and Wolf‐Watz (2014) reported correlation coefficients between connection to nature and individual items of their PEB scales. The weighted mean of the effect size of the PEBs was calculated to avoid double‐counting participants. Some studies measured the relationship of connection to nature and behavioral intentions or willingness to engage in PEB. Although the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen 1991) incorporates intention as the immediate antecedent of behavior, there is only a moderate correlation between behavioral intentions and behavior (Bamberg & Moser 2007; Grimmer & Miles 2017). Behavioral intentions may capture a different concept than self‐reported behavior. Therefore, studies reporting intention or willingness to engage or an interest in engaging in PEB were excluded. Samples were independent (i.e., individual participants were included only once). Longitudinal interventions required reporting the effect size before an intervention was implemented.
The following information was collected (if available) for each sample: authors’ names, publication year, geographic region (North America, South America, Europe, Australasia, and Asia), measure of connection to nature and PEB, effect size, sample size, significance of effect, percentage of female, and age of participants. A number of potentially eligible studies did not report statistics needed for the meta‐analysis, and data could not be obtained from the authors.
We assessed the possibility of publication bias by examining funnel‐plot symmetry, which displays effect sizes (as Fischer's Z) plotted against SE. An unbiased sample shows a symmetrical cloud of data points around the overall effect size in a pattern resembling a funnel (Borenstein 2005). We used Egger et al.’s (1997) regression test to quantify asymmetry in the funnel plot. We undertook a trim and fill analysis (Duval & Tweedie 2000) to determine the adjusted effect, taking into account bias seen in the funnel plot. Finally, we calculated the fail‐safe Ns. Rosenthal's (1991) N is the number of missing samples with an effect size of 0 that are needed to render the current overall effect nonsignificant. Orwin's N (1983) takes into account samples that have a negative effect size.
Data Analyses
Comprehensive Meta‐Analysis software (version 3) (Borenstein 2014) was used to calculate the overall effect size and to assess whether the effect size depended on any moderator variables. Fischer Z transformations were used to determine the effect size (Hedges & Olkin 1985) because some of the reported correlations between connection to nature and PEB were >0.30 (Borenstein et al. 2009). The Fischer's Z scores were converted to correlation coefficients for ease of use.
Random effects models were used because we expected the effect size to vary randomly among studies (Hedges & Vevea 1998). Separate univariate categorical analyses were used to obtain an effect size for each subgroup of moderator variables. Moderators included measures of connection to nature and PEB, geographic location of samples, age group of participants, and percentage of female participants. We selected pooled variance for the moderator analyses because we expected variance to be comparable across subgroups, and because there were <5 samples in some of the subgroups, pooling increased accuracy of the τ 2 (variance of true effect sizes across studies) (Borenstein et al. 2009). Random effects metaregression with method‐of‐moments estimation was used to assess whether the continuous covariate, percentage of females, moderated the relationship between connection to nature and PEB.
No outliers were identified in the sample (Hanson & Bussiѐre 1998). Although Cochran's Q was significant (Q = 406.59), when the sample with the lowest (r = 0.14 [Beery & Wolf‐Watz 2014]) or highest (r = 0.66 [Otto & Pensini 2017]) effect size was removed from the meta‐analysis, the Q statistics did not decrease by 50% (Q = 264.66 and 375.59, respectively).
Results
Sample Characteristics
Thirty‐seven independent samples were identified from 26 studies to 3 June 2018. The total sample was 13,237 individuals (adults, university students, and children) from 11, mostly western, countries (Table 2).
Table 2.
Studya | n | Connection‐to‐nature scale | Dimensions in connection‐to‐nature scalesb | Proenvironmental behavior scalec | Location | Age group | % femaled | Sourcee |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Clayton 2003 | 73 | environmental identity | A + C + B | PEB | U.S.A. | student | – | B |
Mayer & Frantz 2004 S2 | 65 | connectedness to nature | A + C | PEB | U.S.A. | student | 58.8 | J |
Mayer & Frantz 2004 S4 | 135 | connectedness to nature | A + C | PEB | U.S.A. | adult | 74.2 | J |
Mayer & Frantz 2004 S5 | 57 | connectedness to nature | A + C | PEB | U.S.A. | student | – | J |
Schultz et al. 2004 | 98 | inclusion of nature in self | C | PEB | U.S.A. | student | 60 | J |
Raudsepp 2005 | 987 | emotional affinity toward nature | A | PEB | Estonia | adult | – | B |
Dutcher et al. 2007 | 513 | connectivity with nature | A + C | PEB | U.S.A. | adult | 21 | J/T |
Davis et al. 2009 S1 | 71 | commitment to the natural environment | A + C | GEB* | U.S.A. | student | 63.4 | J |
Gosling & Williams 2010 | 131 | connectedness to nature and connectivity with nature | A + C | vegetation protection on farm | Australia | adult | 14 | J |
Perkins 2010 S4 | 235 | love and care for nature | A | PEB | Australia | adult | 58 | J |
Brügger et al. 2011 | 1186 | disposition to connect with nature | A + B | GEB* | Switzerland | adult | 45.2 | J |
Hoot & Friedman 2011 | 195 | connectedness to nature | A + C | PEB | U.S.A. | adult | 46.2 | J |
Nisbet & Zelenski 2013 S1 | 184 | nature relatedness | A + C + B | ecology scale** | Canada | student | 67.4 | J |
Nisbet & Zelenski 2013 S3 | 354 | nature relatedness | A + C + B | ecology scale** | Canada | student | 59.9 | J |
Nisbet & Zelenski 2013 S4 A | 84 | NR‐6 (nature relatedness short form) | A + C | ecology scale** | Canada | adult | 78.6 | J |
Nisbet & Zelenski 2013 S4 B | 123 | NR‐6 | A + C | ecology scale** | Canada | student | 77.2 | J |
Tam 2013a S1 | 322 | nature relatedness | A + C + B | ecological behavior^ | Hong Kong | student | 45.3 | J |
Tam 2013a S2 | 185 | nature relatedness | A + C + B | ecological behavior^ | U.S.A. | adult | 63.8 | J |
Tam 2013b S1 | 288 | dispositional empathy with nature | A + C | ecological behavior^ | Hong Kong | student | 45.1 | J |
Tam 2013b S2 | 172 | dispositional empathy with nature | A + C | ecological behavior^ | U.S.A. | adult | 62.2 | J |
Tam 2013b S3 | 104 | dispositional empathy with nature | A + C | ecological behavior^ | Hong Kong | student | 66.3 | J |
Tam 2013b S4 | 175 | dispositional empathy with nature | A + C | ecological behavior^ | Hong Kong | student | 47.4 | J |
Tam 2013b S5 | 78 | dispositional empathy with nature | A + C | ecological behavior^ | Hong Kong | student | 30.8 | J |
Beery & Wolf‐Watz 2014 | 1374 | environmental connectedness | A + C | PEB | Sweden | adult | – | J |
Roczen et al. 2014 | 1907 | disposition to connect with nature | A + B | GEB* | Germany | adolescent | 57 | J/PC |
Sanguinetti 2014 | 477 | inclusion of nature in self | C | gardening, farming, or animal husbandry | U.S.A. | adults | 60 | J |
Dresner et al. 2015 | 165 | environmental identity | A + C + B | proenvironmental gardening | U.S.A. | adult | 50 | J |
Geng et al. 2015 | 113 | connectedness to nature | A + C | CSEBQ* | China | student | 44.3 | J |
Pereira & Forster 2015 | 74 | connectedness to nature | A + C | PEB | Australia | student | 75.0 | J |
Collado et al. 2015 | 107 | emotional affinity toward nature | A | PEB | Spain | children | 54.9 | J |
Barbaro & Pickett 2016 S1 | 308 | connectedness to nature | A + C | PEB*** | U.S.A. | student | 68 | J |
Barbaro & Pickett 2016 S2 | 296 | connectedness to nature | A + C | PEB*** | U.S.A. | adult | 60 | J |
Soliman et al. 2017 | 230 | connectedness to nature | A + C + B | observed PEB | Canada | student | 70.2 | J |
Forstmann & Sagioglou 2017 | 1487 | nature relatedness | PEB*** | U.S.A. | adult | 61.4 | J/PC | |
Otto & Pensini 2017 | 255 | disposition to connect with nature | A + B | GEB* | Germany | children | – | J/PC |
Whitburn et al. 2018 | 405 | disposition to connect with nature | A + B | GEB* | New Zealand | adult | 62 | J |
Rosa et al. 2018 | 224 | connectedness to nature | A + C | PEB | Brazil | students | 62.5 | J |
Abbreviations: S, study; Sx (A, community sample; B, undergraduate student sample).
Abbreviations: A, affect; C, cognition; B, behavior or experience.
Abbreviations and symbols: PEB, proenvironmental behavior; GEB, general‐ecological‐behavior scale; CSEBQ, college student environmental behavior questionnaire; *, adapted from Kaiser (1998); **, ecology scale, actual commitment (Maloney et al. 1975); ^, adapted from Kaiser (1998) and Schultz & Zelezny (1998); ***, adapted from Whitmarsh and O'Neill (2010).
Data unavailable (–).
Abbreviations: J, peer‐reviewed journal; R, report; T, PhD dissertation; B, book chapter; PC, personal communication with author.
Relationship Between Connection to Nature and PEB
The relationship between connection to nature and PEB was positive and moderately sized across samples: r = 0.42; 95% CI 0.36, 0.47; p < 0.001 (Fig. 1). Thus, connection to nature was relatively higher among people who reported greater participation in PEB. Sample heterogeneity was significant among effect sizes: Q(36) = 406.59, p < 0.001. A substantial portion of total variance was attributable to systematic differences in effect size between samples (I 2 = 91.15); hence, it was important to investigate moderator variables.
Publication bias had only a minor influence on observed effect size. The distribution of samples around the observed effect was approximately symmetrical with an absence of a few samples on the mid to low right‐hand side of the funnel plot. Egger's regression coefficient was not significant (intercept = −0.20; 95% CI −2.56, 2.15; t(35) = 0.17; p = 0.86 [2 tailed]), indicating a lack of bias in the data. Rosenthal's fail safe N indicated that an additional 8952 samples with a 0 effect size would be required for our effect size to be nonsignificant. Orwin's fail‐safe N showed that 293 missing samples would be needed to make the correlation nonsignificant. The trim and fill analysis inputted 2 samples to the right of the mean and produced an adjusted effect of r = 0.45 (95% CI 0.43, 0.46), which is slightly stronger than our observed effect.
Relationship Moderators
The scale used for connection to nature significantly moderated the overall effect (Q[10] = 36.29, p < 0.001, n = 37) and explained 69% of the between‐sample variation (Table 3). Mean effect sizes for the types of connection to nature ranged from 0.14 to 0.60 (Table 3). The relationship was strongest for commitment to the environment (r = 0.60) and weakest for inclusion of nature in self (r = 0.25). Environmental connectedness included 0 in the 95% CIs, indicating a nonsignificant relationship. Results for moderator subgroups with few samples can be problematic. We repeated the moderator analysis for connection‐to‐nature scales with ≥3 samples per subgroup. The results were robust to the change in threshold of inclusion: r = 0.45, Q(3) = 13.62, p = 0.001, R 2 = 0.37, n = 27.
Table 3.
Effect size | 95% CI | Test null (2 tailed) | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Moderatorsa | k | n | r | lower | upper | Z | p b | Test of model |
Connection to nature scales | ||||||||
commitment to the natural environment | 1 | 71 | 0.60 | 0.37 | 0.76 | 4.44 | 0.001 | |
disposition to connect with nature | 4 | 3753 | 0.53 | 0.45 | 0.60 | 10.98 | 0.001 | |
nature relatedness | 7 | 2739 | 0.51 | 0.44 | 0.57 | 12.33 | 0.001 | |
environmental identity scale | 2 | 238 | 0.44 | 0.27 | 0.58 | 4.82 | 0.001 | |
love and care for nature | 1 | 235 | 0.42 | 0.21 | 0.59 | 3.78 | 0.001 | |
connectedness to nature scale | 11 | 1828 | 0.39 | 0.32 | 0.45 | 10.50 | 0.001 | |
dispositional empathy with nature | 5 | 817 | 0.36 | 0.26 | 0.46 | 6.58 | 0.001 | |
connectivity with nature | 1 | 513 | 0.32 | 0.12 | 0.50 | 3.07 | 0.001 | |
emotional affinity toward nature | 2 | 1094 | 0.28 | 0.13 | 0.43 | 3.52 | 0.001 | |
inclusion of nature in self | 2 | 575 | 0.25 | 0.08 | 0.40 | 2.92 | 0.001 | |
environmental connectedness | 1 | 1374 | 0.14 | –0.06 | 0.33 | 1.38 | 0.17* | |
test of model | 37 | 13,237 | Q(10) = 36.29, p < 0.001, R 2 = 0.69 | |||||
Dimensions of connection to nature scales | ||||||||
affect + behavior | 6 | 3960 | 0.52 | 0.43 | 0.60 | 9.97 | 0.001 | |
affect + cognition + behavior | 7 | 2770 | 0.50 | 0.41 | 0.57 | 10.06 | 0.001 | |
affect + cognition | 19 | 4603 | 0.37 | 0.31 | 0.43 | 11.27 | 0.001 | |
affect | 3 | 1329 | 0.33 | 0.18 | 0.47 | 4.20 | 0.001 | |
cognition | 2 | 575 | 0.25 | 0.05 | 0.43 | 2.45 | 0.011 | |
test of model | 37 | 13,237 | Q(4) = 15.90, p < 0.001, R 2 = 0.46 | |||||
Number of items in scales of connection to nature | ||||||||
1–9 | 10 | 3176 | 0.32 | 0.22 | 0.41 | 6.04 | 0.001 | |
10–19 | 18 | 4108 | 0.43 | 0.38 | 0.48 | 14.11 | 0.001 | |
20–29 | 7 | 2860 | 0.57 | 0.50 | 0.64 | 12.44 | 0.001 | |
30+ | 2 | 3093 | 0.49 | 0.34 | 0.62 | 5.67 | 0.001 | |
test of model | 37 | 13,237 | Q(3) = 28.60, p < 0.001, R 2 = 0.47 | |||||
PEB scales | ||||||||
PEB (Whitmarsh & O'Neill)* | 3 | 2091 | 0.51 | 0.39 | 0.61 | 7.26 | 0.001 | |
ecology scale** | 4 | 745 | 0.49 | 0.37 | 0.59 | 7.23 | 0.001 | |
general ecological behavior*** | 13 | 5261 | 0.46 | 0.40 | 0.52 | 12.54 | 0.001 | |
PEB general^ | 13 | 4137 | 0.36 | 0.29 | 0.43 | 9.13 | 0.001 | |
observed PEB | 1 | 230 | 0.29 | 0.02 | 0.52 | 2.13 | 0.03 | |
pro‐environmental gardening or farming | 3 | 773 | 0.25 | 0.09 | 0.36 | 3.12 | 0.001 | |
test of model | 37 | 13,237 | Q(5) = 15.53, p = 0.01, R 2 = 0.52 | |||||
Age group | ||||||||
children | 3 | 2269 | 0.51 | 0.32 | 0.48 | 8.96 | 0.001 | |
students | 18 | 2941 | 0.41 | 0.33 | 0.49 | 9.17 | 0.001 | |
adults | 16 | 8027 | 0.40 | 0.32 | 0.48 | 8.96 | 0.001 | |
test of model | 37 | 13,237 | Q(2) = 1.29, p = 0.53, R 2 = 0.00 | |||||
Mean age (years) | ||||||||
test of model | 29 | Q(1) = 0.00, p = 0.96, R 2 = 0.00 | ||||||
Geographic location | ||||||||
North America | 20 | 5272 | 0.44 | 0.37 | 0.51 | 10.76 | 0.001 | |
South America | 1 | 3045 | 0.44 | 0.09 | 0.69 | 2.45 | 0.01 | |
Europe | 6 | 5816 | 0.41 | 0.28 | 0.53 | 5.68 | 0.001 | |
Australasia | 4 | 845 | 0.38 | 0.20 | 0.53 | 4.05 | 0.001 | |
Asia | 6 | 1080 | 0.35 | 0.20 | 0.48 | 4.47 | 0.001 | |
test of model | 37 | 13,237 | Q(4) = 1.78, p = 0.78, R 2 = 0.00 | |||||
Percent female | ||||||||
test of model | 32 | Q(1) = 3.70, p = 0.06, R 2 = 0.10 |
Multidimensional scales that contained affect and behavior or affect and cognition and behavior had the strongest relationship with PEB (r = 0.52 and 0.50, respectively) (Table 3). These included disposition‐to‐connect‐with‐nature, nature‐relatedness, and environmental‐identity scales. The one‐dimensional cognitive scale had the weakest relationship (r = 0.25). The content and dimensions of the connection‐to‐nature scales (affective, cognitive, or behavioral) significantly moderated the relationship between connection to nature and PEB: Q(4) = 15.90, p < 0.001, R 2 = 0.46, n = 37. This result held when we tested subgroups with ≥3 samples: r = 0.46, Q(2) = 12.21, p < 0.001, R 2 = 0.40, n = 27.
Number of items in a scale also moderated the relationship. Scales with a moderate number of items (20–29) (i.e., nature relatedness and environmental identity) had the strongest relationship with PEB (r = 0.55); those with <9 items had the weakest (r = 0.29) (environmental connectedness, connectivity with nature, and inclusion of nature in self) (Table 3): Q(3) = 28.6, p < 0.001, R 2 = 0.47, n = 37.
Scales used to measure PEB also moderated the overall relationship between connection to nature and PEB (Q[5] = 15.53, p < 0.01, n = 37) and explained 52% of the between‐sample variance (Table 3). The relationship with connection to nature for the types of PEB ranged from 0.25 to 0.51. The scales based on Whitmarsh and O'Neill (2010) had the strongest relationship with connection to nature (r = 0.51). Other self‐reported PEB scales (ecology, general ecological behavior, and general PEB scales) fell in the midrange (r = 0.36–0.49) and observed PEB (Soliman et al. 2017) was at the low end of the range (r = 0.29). Results were robust to change in the threshold of inclusion of ≥3 samples: r = 0.42, Q(4) = 14.4, p = 0.01, R 2 = 0.53, n = 36. Scales used to measure connection to nature and PEB together explained 75% of between‐study variance: Q(15) = 53.75, p = 0.001.
The correlation between connection to nature and PEB was strongest for children (r = 0.51) and weakest for students (r = 0.41) and adults (r = 0.40). Neither age group (Q[2] = 1.29, p = 0.53, n = 37) (Table 3) nor participant mean age (Q[1] = 0.00, p = 0.96, n = 37) were significant moderators. All age subgroups had ≥3 samples.
The relationship between connection to nature and PEB was strongest in samples from North and South America (r = 0.44) and Europe (r = 0.41) and weakest for Asia (r = 0.35) and Australasia (r = 0.38). However, the geographic location of samples did not influence strength of the relationship between connection to nature and PEB: Q(4) = 1.78, p = 0.78, n = 37 (Table 3). Similarly, metaregression showed the percentage of females in the samples was not a moderator of the relationship: Q(1) = 3.70, p = 0.06, n = 32.
Discussion
The relationship between connection to nature and PEB was positive, significant, and moderately sized (r = 0.42). Individuals more strongly connected to nature demonstrated a greater engagement in self‐reported PEB. This relationship held across gender, geographic location, and age group. Our findings support the stance that feeling deeply connected to nature influences how individuals treat it and has implications for increasing general PEB and biodiversity conservation. Our meta‐analysis complements Capaldi et al.’s (2014) meta‐analysis, which quantified the relationship between connection to nature and happiness. They found connection to nature had a small, positive association with vitality and life satisfaction. Together, these meta‐analyses suggest that a close connection with nature is beneficial for human and environmental well‐being. Publication bias did not greatly influence observed effect in our analysis; thus, we have confidence in the observed effect we found.
The large amount of heterogeneity in the data set was largely explained by the scales used to measure connection to nature and PEB (which together explained 75% of between‐study variance). It is somewhat surprising that scales measuring connection to nature moderated the relationship with PEB. If, as Tam (2013a) demonstrated, these scales measure an underlying core construct of connectedness to nature, one would expect the scales to demonstrate a consistent association with PEB. The multidimensional scales with a moderate number of items and affective and behavioral or affective, behavioral, and cognitive dimensions had the strongest association with PEB. The disposition‐to‐connect‐with‐nature and full nature‐relatedness scales incorporated these qualities. The single‐item, cognitive scale (inclusion of nature in self) had one of the lowest associations with PEB. Our findings support Tam's suggestion that the distinction between cognitive and noncognitive scales may be important. Furthermore, multidimensional scales may capture more of what it means to be connected to nature and therefore better predict PEB. Multidimensional scales also allow a detailed analysis of how their various dimensions relate to PEB and how they are affected by interventions aimed to increase connection to nature. Our results indicated that cognition, affect, and behavior were important components of connection to nature in its relationship with PEB. Addressing participant knowledge and beliefs alongside building an emotional and experiential connection to nature may be required in interventions to motivate greater PEB.
The way PEB was operationalized was also a significant moderator of the relationship. The multidimensional scales that measured general PEB and covered several domains of behavior had the strongest association with connection to nature. There was a fairly consistent relationship with connection to nature across these multidimensional scales (means fell within 95% CI of the scale with the highest association with connection to nature, 0.39–0.61). People do not consistently engage with a wide range of PEBs, and some behaviors (e.g., recycling) generally require less effort than others (political activism or using public transport). Multidimensional scales can reduce measurement error and produce generalizable results (Epstein 1983; Kirkpatrick 2003). Aggregating PEBs across several dimensions can capture a more realistic picture of a person's general PEB than measuring a single behavior.
In contrast to what we expected based on the literature, the demographics we tested were not significant moderators of the relationship between connection to nature and PEB. The age group or mean age in years of participants and the percentage of females did not affect the strength of the relationship. This was unexpected because generally being older and female is associated with greater PEB (Kollmus & Agyeman 2002). Undergraduates are often used as a convenient population in research. There are some concerns that, as a population, undergraduates may not be representative of the general adult population and study results may therefore not be generalizable. We found no significant difference in results for students and adults, indicating that these concerns may be unwarranted in this area of research. The lack of geographic location as a moderator contradicted cross‐cultural studies that report that the way individuals relate to the natural environment is culturally patterned (Milfont & Schultz 2016), perhaps because extant research centers on urban populations in western countries. Nature connectedness needs to be investigated in nonwestern cultures.
Our study has some limitations. The correlation we found does not mean causation. A stronger connection to nature may indeed motivate greater PEB. Conversely, individuals who participate in more PEB may develop a stronger connection to nature, or other variables may be involved. However, our findings do consolidate existing research and provide a solid grounding to justify future longitudinal research into changing PEB by strengthening individuals’ connection to nature (Schultz 2011). Differences in the strength of the relationship between connection to nature and engagement in PEB may also be attributable to items in the scales that indirectly measure PEB, which could result in a stronger correlation. For example, several scales include an item on how a person's actions affect the environment (nature‐relatedness, environmental‐identity, and commitment‐to‐nature, and connectedness‐to‐nature scales). These scales did correlate more strongly with PEB than the scales without such items.
Connection to nature and PEB are measured by self‐report. Reliance on self‐reports threatens construct validity (i.e., metrics used may not measure what they are intended to) and can lead to inflated associations between variables measured via the same method due to shared method variance. However, observing actual PEB is difficult because most actions are not carried out in public and observing a single behavior is not a valid measure of general PEB (Frantz & Mayer 2014). We focused on the direct relationship between connection to nature and PEB. However, the relationship may be indirect. For example, the relationship between connection to nature and PEB is partially mediated by environmental attitudes, the use of nature for psychological restoration (Whitburn et al. 2018), and environmental values (Pereira & Forster 2015) and fully mediated by biospheric concerns (Gosling & Williams 2010). It is useful to identify mediators that facilitate the relationship between connection to nature and PEB to determine types of interventions that may motivate greater PEB.
Conservation science has successfully identified the biological values and processes affected by anthropogenic activity, and has successfully mitigated some human impacts. However, conservation efforts are continually undermined by human behavior (Fox et al. 2006). Conservation actions are human behaviors (Schultz 2011); therefore, it is vital to understand how social and psychological factors influence such behaviors (Mascia et al. 2003). Our findings support Schultz's (2011) suggestion that promoting stronger connections to nature could increase individuals’ proenvironmental and conservation behaviors. Our results showed that people with a stronger connection to nature are more likely to engage with a range of PEBs, including conservation of energy and water, anticonsumerism, proenvironmental political activism, and financial support for environmental organizations. Future longitudinal studies (ideally randomized experiments) could provide evidence of a causative relationship between connection to nature and PEB. A recent longitudinal study demonstrated the amount of time spent in nature as a 6‐year‐old is related to environmental attitudes and behavior as a young adult (Evans et al. 2018). Although time in nature is not connection to nature, it may be indicative of the relationship one might expect if connection to nature is causative of PEB. The usefulness of existing interventions, such as environmental education, aimed at strengthening connection to nature as a way to motivate greater engagement in PEB needs evaluation.
Acknowledgments
We are very grateful to M. Forstmann, S. Otto, and N. Roczen for taking the time to review their original research and send us the correlation coefficients that were not available in their articles. We also thank the reviewers, whose constructive and helpful comments greatly improved our manuscript, and Victoria University of Wellington for financial support via J.W.’s doctoral scholarship.
Article impact statement: Interventions designed to facilitate a stronger connection to nature may result in greater engagement in proenvironmental behavior.
Literature Cited
- Studies included in the meta‐analyis are marked with an asterisk.
- Ajzen I. 1991. The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 50:179–211. [Google Scholar]
- Axelrod LJ. 1993. Responding to environmental concern: What factors guide individual action? Journal of Environmental Psychology 13:149–159. [Google Scholar]
- Bamberg S, Möser G. 2007. Twenty years after Hines, Hungerford, and Tomera: a new meta‐analysis of psycho‐social determinants of pro‐environmental behaviour. Journal of Environmental Psychology 27:14–25. [Google Scholar]
- *Barbaro N, Pickett SM. 2016. Mindfully green: examining the effect of connectedness to nature on the relationship between mindfulness and engagement in pro‐environmental behavior. Personality and Individual Differences 93:137–142. [Google Scholar]
- *Beery TH, Wolf‐Watz D. 2014. Nature to place: rethinking the environmental connectedness perspective. Journal of Environmental Psychology 40:198–205. [Google Scholar]
- Borenstein M. 2005. Software for publication bias Pages 193–220 in Rothstein HR, Sutton AJ, Borenstein M, editors. Publication bias in meta‐analysis—prevention, assessment and adjustments. Wiley, Chichester. [Google Scholar]
- Borenstein M. 2014. Comprehensive meta‐analysis software. Version 3. Biostat, Englewood, New Jersey. [Google Scholar]
- Borenstein M, Hedges L, Higgins J, Rothstein H. 2009. Introduction to meta‐analysis. Wiley, United Kingdom. [Google Scholar]
- Bragg EA. 1996. Towards ecological self: deep ecology meets constructionist self‐theory. Journal of Environmental Psychology 16:93–108. [Google Scholar]
- *Brügger A, Kaiser FG, Roczen N. 2011. Connectedness to nature, inclusion of nature, environmental identity, and implicit association with nature. European Psychologist 16:324–333. [Google Scholar]
- Capaldi CA, Dopko RL, Zelenski JM. 2014. The relationship between nature connectedness and happiness: a meta‐analysis. Frontiers in Psychology 5:1–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cialdini R, Brown S, Lewis B, Luce C, Neuberg S. 1997. Reinterpreting the empathy–altruism relationship: when one into one equals oneness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 73:481–494. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Clayton S. 2003. Environmental identity: a conceptual and operational definition Pages 45–65 in Clayton S, Opotow S, editors. Identity and the natural environment: the psychological significance of nature. MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts. [Google Scholar]
- *Collado S, Evans GW, Corraliza JA, Sorrel MA. 2015. The role played by age on children's pro‐ecological behaviors: an exploratory analysis. Journal of Environmental Psychology 44:85–94. [Google Scholar]
- *Davis JL, Green JD, Reed A. 2009. Interdependence with the environment: commitment, interconnectedness, and environmental behavior. Journal of Environmental Psychology 29:173–180. [Google Scholar]
- *Dresner M, Handelman C, Braun S, Rollwagen‐Bollens G. 2015. Environmental identity, pro‐environmental behaviors, and civic engagement of volunteer stewards in Portland area parks. Environmental Education Research 21:991–1010. [Google Scholar]
- *Dutcher DD, Finley JC, Luloff AE, Johnson JB. 2007. Connectivity with nature as a measure of environmental values. Environment and Behavior 39:474–493. [Google Scholar]
- Duval S, Tweedie R. 2000. A nonparametric trim and fill method of accounting for publication bias in meta‐analysis. Journal of the American Statistical Association 95:89–98. [Google Scholar]
- Egger M, Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C. 1997. Bias in meta‐analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. British Medical Journal 31:629–634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ehrlich PR, Kennedy D. 2005. Millennium assessment of human behavior. Science 309:562–563. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Epstein S. 1983. Aggregation and beyond: some basic issues on the prediction of behavior. Journal of Personality 51:360–392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans GW, Otto S, Kaiser FG. 2018. Childhood origins of young adult environmental behavior. Psychological Science 29:679–687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forstmann M, Sagioglou C. 2017. Lifetime experience with (classic) psychedelics predicts pro‐environmental behavior through an increase in nature relatedness. Journal of Psychopharmacology 31:975–988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox HE, Christian C, Nordby JC, Pergams ORW, Peterson GD, Pyke CR. 2006. Perceived barriers to integrating social science and conservation. Conservation Biology 20:1817–1820. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frantz CM, Mayer FS. 2014. The importance of connection to nature in assessing environmental education programs. Studies in Educational Evaluation 41:85–89. [Google Scholar]
- Fromm E. 1964. The heart of man, its genius for good and evil. Harper and Row, New York. [Google Scholar]
- *Geng LN, Xu JK, Ye LJ, Zhou WJ, Zhou KX. 2015. Connections with nature and environmental behaviors. PLOS ONE 10 (e0127247) 10.1371/journal.pone.0127247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Gosling E, Williams KJH. 2010. Connectedness to nature, place attachment and conservation behaviour: testing connectedness theory among farmers. Journal of Environmental Psychology 30:298–304. [Google Scholar]
- Grimmer M, Miles M. 2017. With the best of intentions: a large sample test of the intention‐behaviour gap in pro‐environmental consumer behaviour. International Journal of Consumer Studies 41:2–10. [Google Scholar]
- Hanson RK, Bussière MT. 1998. Predicting relapse: a meta‐analysis of sexual offender recidivism studies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 66:348–362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hedges LV, Olkin I. 1985. Statistical methods for meta‐analysis. Academic Press, San Diego, California.
- Hedges L, Vevea JL. 1998. Fixed‐ and random‐effects models in meta‐analysis. Psychological Methods 3:486–504. [Google Scholar]
- Hines JM, Hungerford H, Tomera AN. 1987. Analysis and synthesis of research on responsible environmental behaviour: a meta‐analysis. Journal of Environmental Education 18:1–8. [Google Scholar]
- *Hoot RE, Friedman H. 2011. Connectedness and environmental behavior: sense of interconnectedness and pro‐environmental behavior. International Journal of Transpersonal Studies 30:89–100. [Google Scholar]
- IPCC (Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change) . 2014. Climate change 2014: synthesis report. IPCC, Geneva. [Google Scholar]
- Kaiser FG. 1998. A general measure of ecological behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 28:395–422. [Google Scholar]
- Kals E, Schumacher D, Montada L. 1999. Emotional affinity toward nature as a motivational basis to protect nature. Environment and Behavior 31:178–202. [Google Scholar]
- Kirkpatrick LA. 2003. Effects of multiple determinancy and measurement error on trait–behavior and behavior–behavior relations: an integrated conceptual mode. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 23:199–209. [Google Scholar]
- Kollmuss A, Agyeman J. 2002. Mind the gap: Why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro‐environmental behavior? Environmental Education Research 8:239–260. [Google Scholar]
- Maloney MP, Ward MP, Braucht GN. 1975. A revised scale for measurement of ecological attitudes and knowledge. American Psychologist 30:787–790. [Google Scholar]
- Markle GL. 2013. Pro‐environmental behavior: Does it matter how it's measured? Development and validation of the pro‐environmental behavior scale (PEBS). Human Ecology 41:905–914. [Google Scholar]
- Mascia MB, Brosius JP, Dobson TA, Forbes BC, Horowitz L, McKean MA, Turner NJ. 2003. Conservation and the social sciences. Conservation Biology 17:649–650. [Google Scholar]
- Mayer FS, Frantz CM, Bruehlman‐Senecal E, Dolliver K. 2009. Why is nature beneficial? The role of connectedness to nature. Environment and Behavior 41:607–643. [Google Scholar]
- *Mayer FS, Frantz CM. 2004. The connectedness to nature scale: a measure of individuals’ feeling in community with nature. Journal of Environmental Psychology 24:503–515. [Google Scholar]
- Milfont TL, Schultz PW. 2016. Culture and the natural environment. Current Opinion in Psychology 8:194–199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Nisbet EK, Zelenski JM. 2013. The NR‐6: a new brief measure of nature relatedness. Frontiers in Psychology 4:1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nisbet EK, Zelenski JM, Murphy SA. 2009. The Nature Relatedness Scale: linking individuals’ connection with nature to environmental concern and behavior. Environment and Behavior 41:715–740. [Google Scholar]
- Orwin RG. 1983. A fail‐safe N for effect size in meta‐analysis. Journal of Educational Statistics 8:157–159. [Google Scholar]
- *Otto S, Pensini P. 2017. Nature‐based environmental education of children: environmental knowledge and connectedness to nature, together, are related to ecological behaviour. Global Environmental Change 47:88–94. [Google Scholar]
- *Pereira M, Forster PM. 2015. The relationship between connectedness to nature, environmental values, and pro‐environmental behaviours. Reinvention 8 Available from http://www.warwick.ac.uk/reinventionjournal/issues/volume8issue2/pereira (Accessed November 2016). [Google Scholar]
- *Perkins HE. 2010. Measuring love and care for nature. Journal of Environmental Psychology 30:455–463. [Google Scholar]
- Pyle RM. 2003. Nature matrix: reconnecting people and nature. Oryx 37:206–214. [Google Scholar]
- *Raudsepp M. 2005. Emotional connection to nature: its socio‐psychological correlates and associations with pro‐environmental attitudes and behavior. Hogrefe & Huber Publishers, Cambridge, Massachusetts. [Google Scholar]
- *Roczen N, Kaiser FG, Bogner FX, Wilson M. 2014. A competence model for environmental education. Environment and Behavior 46:972–992. [Google Scholar]
- *Rosa CD, Profice CC, Collado S. 2018. Nature experiences and adults’ self‐reported pro‐environmental behavoirs: the role of connectedness to nature and childhood nature experiences. Frontiers in Psychology 9 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenthal R. 1991. Meta‐analytic procedures for social research. Revised edition Sage, Newbury Park, California. [Google Scholar]
- Roszak T. 1995. Where psyche meet Gaia Pages 1–20 in Roszak T, Gomes ME, Kanner AD, editors. Ecopsychology: restoring the earth, healing the mind. Sierra Club, San Francisco. [Google Scholar]
- *Sanguinetti A. 2014. Transformational practices in cohousing: enhancing residents’ connection to community and nature. Journal of Environmental Psychology 40:86–96. [Google Scholar]
- Schultz PW. 2000. Empathizing with nature: the effects of perspective taking on concern for environmental issues. Journal of Social Issues 56:391–406. [Google Scholar]
- Schultz PW. 2002. Inclusion with nature: the psychology of human–nature relations Pages 61–78 in Schmuck P, Schultz PW, editors. Psychology of sustainable development. Kluwer Academic, Boston. [Google Scholar]
- Schultz PW. 2011. Conservation is behavior. Conservation Biology 25:1080–1083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Schultz PW, Shriver C, Tabanico JJ, Khazian AM. 2004. Implicit connections with nature. Journal of Environmental Psychology 24:31–42. [Google Scholar]
- Schultz PW, Tabanico J. 2007. Self, identity, and the natural environment: exploring implicit connections with nature. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 37:1219–1247. [Google Scholar]
- Schultz PW, Zelezny L. 1998. Values and proenvironmental behavior: a five‐country survey. Journal of Cross‐Cultural Psychology 29:540–558. [Google Scholar]
- *Soliman M, Peet, J , Davydenko M. 2017. The impact of immersive technology on nature relatedness and pro‐environmental behavior. Journal of Media Psychology‐Theories Methods and Applications 29:8–17. [Google Scholar]
- *Tam KP. 2013a. Concepts and measures related to connection to nature: similarities and differences. Journal of Environmental Psychology 34:64–78. [Google Scholar]
- *Tam KP. 2013b. Dispositional empathy with nature. Journal of Environmental Psychology 35:92–104. [Google Scholar]
- *Whitburn J, Linklater WL, Milfont TL. 2018. Exposure to urban nature and tree planting are related to pro‐environmental behavoir via connection to nature, the use of nature for psychological restoration and environmental attitudes. Environment and Behavior 10.1177/0013916517751009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Whitmarsh L, O'Neill S. 2010. Green identity, green living? The role of pro‐environmental self‐identity in determining consistency across diverse pro‐environmental behaviours. Environmental Psychology 30:305–314. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson EO. 1984. Biophilia. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson EO. 2002. The future of life. Alfred A. Knopf, New York. [Google Scholar]