Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2019 Dec 6;61(2):85–92. doi: 10.1111/jsap.13085

A review of maternal behaviour in dogs and potential areas for further research

N R Santos 1,, A Beck 2, A Fontbonne 1
PMCID: PMC7027574  PMID: 31808169

Abstract

Here we review information on maternal behaviour in dogs, defined as a combination of all the acts of the mother towards her offspring, which begins before parturition and continues until weaning. In dogs, maternal care is measured using the most commonly observed behaviours, such as the time spent in contact, licking/grooming and nursing of the puppies. Since newborn puppies have a very limited capacity for movement, maternal interaction is essential to their survival, nourishment and protection. It is also an important element of the bonding process between puppies and the bitch and is thought to play a role in the social development of the puppies. Nevertheless, some questions still need to be clarified, such as the best way to quantify factors that may interfere with maternal behaviour. In recent studies, maternal care, or maternal style, was measured using a scoring system and found to be influenced by factors such as litter size, breed and parity, or even human interaction. However, the impact of the emotional state of the bitch and the quality of maternal behaviour on puppy survival and development remain unclear. The long‐lasting effects of mother–puppy interactions on puppy behaviour during their adult life are still poorly understood, despite their importance for breeders who wish to prevent future problem behaviours.

INTRODUCTION

The first example of human‐animal bonding was the interaction with canids, which led to the domestication of dogs. Based on genetic material, dog domestication is thought most likely to have occurred around 5400 to 16,300 years ago (Pang et al. 2009). However, archaeological studies found dog‐like fossils among human fossils as early as 31,000 years ago (Germonpré et al. 2009). The continuous breeding of dogs has led to over 400 breeds with morphological and functional diversity (Shearman & Wilton 2011).

Although selective breeding has resulted in a large variation in phenotype, function and size of dogs, its effect on basic instincts such as maternal behaviour appears to be marginal. The majority of dog breeds still show similar basic maternal behaviour. This includes integrated elements such as nest‐building, reluctance to leave the nest, genital and overall licking of the newborn, nursing and direct contact with the litter. Nest building is generally observed before parturition and nursing from birth to around 5 to 10 weeks postpartum, for both the domestic bitch (Scott & Fuller 1965, Pal 2005, 2008) and all canid species (Lord et al. 2013). Nevertheless, to our knowledge, characterisation of maternal behaviour by breed has not been studied. Breeds that have been used to study maternal behaviour include beagle (Rheingold 1963, Scott & Fuller 1965, Grant 1987, Guardini et al. 2016, Orfanou et al. 2016); cocker spaniel (Rheingold 1963, Scott & Fuller 1965); German shepherd dog (Foyer et al. 2016, Bray et al. 2017a, 2017b); Labrador and golden retriever (Bray et al. 2017a, 2017b); mixed dog breeds (Guardini et al. 2015, Czerwinski et al. 2017, Guardini et al. 2017) and crossbreeds (Korda and Brewinska, 1977 1977a,1977b).

One of the first studies on the interaction between the bitch and her puppies was performed by Scott (1958) during weaning. The same research group later focused on the genetics and social behaviour of dogs (Scott & Fuller 1965). Maternal behaviour and interactions with puppies at the time of parturition and during the early postpartum period have since been studied by many groups (e.g. Bleicher 1962; Freak, 1962, Rheingold 1963 and Hart 1979; Markwell & Thorne 1987; Grant 1987). The number of investigations aiming to characterise maternal care in domestic dogs during the first postpartum weeks is limited (Scott & Marston 1950; James 1952; Bleicher 1962; Freak, 1962, Rheingold 1963). However, since it was demonstrated that adult dogs' character traits can be affected by maternal, litter‐related and seasonal variables experienced early in life (Foyer et al. 2013), a considerable amount of available data has been published in this area (Guardini et al. 2015, Czerwinski et al. 2016, Foyer et al. 2016, Guardini et al. 2016, Orfanou et al. 2016, Czerwinski et al. 2017, Guardini et al. 2017, Bray et al. 2017a, 2017b, Dietz et al. 2018). The research methodology varies greatly, depending on the available technology, but there is an increasing trend towards the use of video recording, which allows the same sample to be evaluated multiple times (Table 1).

Table 1.

Methods used to observe maternal behaviour in recently published studies (2015 to 2017)

Reference Environment – subjects Type and time of observation
Bray et al. (2017a) Guide dogs kennel – 21 bitches (German shepherds, Labrador retrievers and golden retriever) Video recording: 3 days per week, two 10‐minute sessions in the morning and two 10‐minute sessions in the afternoon, during the first 3 weeks postpartum.
Czerwinski et al. (2017) Bitches and puppies of private owners – 6 bitches (Border terrier, English Staffordshire terrier, greyhound, Labrador and whippet) Video recording: over 96 hours on post‐natal days 3, 6, 9 and 12 (24 hours per day).
Foyer et al. (2016) Breeding program for military working dogs – 22 bitches (German shepherd dog) Video recording: 1 day per week (1st, 7th, 14th and 21st day postpartum), continuously every second hour over a 24 hour period during the first 3 weeks postpartum.
Guardini et al. (2015 and 2017) Bitches and their puppies living in home environments – 10 bitches (2015), 12 bitches (2017), (Belgian shepherd Groenendael, Border collie, boxer, cross‐breed, German shepherd dog, Labrador retriever, short haired dachshund and Weimaraner) Video recording: every day for 15 minutes in the morning, starting when the mother returned to the whelping box after her first morning urination and/or defaecation, from day 1 to day 21 after birth.
Guardini et al. (2016) Bitches and puppies living in laboratory conditions – 8 bitches (beagle) Video recording: every day for 15 minutes in the morning once the mother returned from her walk, from day 1 to day 21 after birth.

The objective of this review is to describe the repertoire of mother‐pup interactions and the common activities associated with maternal behaviour in the dog, during both the preparation for whelping and the first 3 weeks postpartum. In addition, the factors that may influence maternal behaviour and the effect of maternal care on puppy development are discussed. Recognising behaviours that can be observed around the time of birth contributes to veterinary advice to dog breeders. Moreover, an increased knowledge of factors likely to impact future puppy development may aid in minimising behavioural problems in adult dogs. Finally, this review aims to highlight areas where future research is still needed.

MATERNAL BEHAVIOUR REPERTOIRE

Maternal interaction with pups commences with preparation for whelping and includes the series of events resulting in the birth, survival and development of a social and healthy dog. Generally, researchers have concentrated on interactions that are easiest to measure. Oro‐nasal interactions such as anogenital licking are observed shortly after birth and are essential during the first 3 weeks of life to stimulate urination and defaecation in puppies (Scott 1958). Maternal behaviour during the neonatal period is defined in most studies as the amount of time that the bitch spends with the puppies (time spent in contact and time spent in the whelping area), the extent of the oro‐nasal interaction and time spent nursing. The neonatal period has been defined as the period from birth to 12 days postnatally (Scott & Fuller 1965), or from day 3 to day 16 postpartum (Battaglia 2009). It is followed by a transition period from 15 to 21 days postpartum which is associated with the opening of the eyes, developing the sense of hearing and neurological development (Scott & Fuller 1965, Battaglia 2009). By the end of the transition period, there is a shift from total dependence on the bitch to a degree of independence through neurological development and growth of the puppies. These two periods should be carefully evaluated since the complete dependence of the puppies on the mother means it is likely that the character of the dam or her maternal style during this time might influence subsequent development of the puppies. Thermal and tactile stimulation are important in determining neonatal reactions to the environment (Fox, 1972, Hoffman et al. 2004), and it has been suggested that inadequate or poor maternal stimulation during these periods may alter the animal's behaviour later in life (Czerwinski et al. 2016).

Preparation for parturition

One or 2 days beforehand, the bitch starts showing distinct signs of impending parturition (Wells 2009, Linde‐Forsberg 2010), such as restlessness and reduced appetite 12 to 24 hours before whelping. Although domestic dogs should have similar instincts during the peripartum to those observed in wild canids, such as digging and nesting behaviour (Kleiman 1968), the manifestation of these behaviours depends on individual, environmental factors and levels of human contact (Udell et al. 2010). In a questionnaire‐based evaluation of pre partum behaviour in the bitch, owners described changes in behaviour in approximately 50% of the bitches with at least one of: lack of attention, drowsiness, aggression, anxiety, fickleness, capriciousness, irritation, increase in attention requests and a change of appetite (Ferrari & Monteiro‐Filho 2004). Prewhelping behaviours, as described above, have been attributed to the sharp decline in blood progesterone during the 24‐hour pre partum period (Concannon 2000). However, the isolation commonly observed in wild canids (Kleiman 1968, AZA Canid Taxon 2012) is not always observed in the domestic dog. Bitches may either seek seclusion (Bleicher 1962, Wells 2009) or search excessively for human attention (Freak, 1962, Hart 1979 and Linde‐Forsberg 2005). Similarly, building a nest is not always observed in the domesticated bitch. Some bitches will shred paper, blankets and bedding in an attempt to build a nest (Bleicher 1962, Hart 1979, Beaver 1999, Wells 2009). The nesting behaviour appears to be initiated by the increasing frequency and intensity of uterine contractions and is also associated with shivering, perhaps as a means to increase body temperature (Bleicher 1962, Hart 1979, Linde‐Forsberg 2010) following the progesterone‐driven temperature drop. In 71% of primiparous and 80% of multiparous bitches an attempt to build a nest was observed by the owner (Ferrari & Monteiro‐Filho 2004). As far as we know, a potential correlation between the intensity of nesting behaviour and the future maternal behaviour in bitches has not been studied (Rooney et al. 2016).

Postpartum: Oro‐nasal interaction

The first contact of the bitch with newly born puppies is through oro‐nasal interaction. During eutocia, the bitch will bite and tear the foetal membranes and cut the umbilical cord (Schweizer & Meyers‐Wallen 2000, Wykes & Olson 2003), which is crucial to puppy survival. Because of the type of placentation (endotheliochorial zonary placenta) with the allantochorion attached to the uterus and the amniotic sac freely loose inside, the majority of puppies are born surrounded by the amniotic sac. Therefore, if the puppy is not released from it soon after birth, it will die from asphyxiation. At birth, the bitch breaks the foetal membrane, licks the head and the mouth of the puppy, which results in the stimulation of respiration until the birth of another puppy and this cycle is repeated until the end of parturition (Bleicher 1962). The act of licking is rough, and it is thought likely that the subsequent movements and sounds of the neonate prevent the mother from eating the puppy itself (Noirot 1972, Peters & Kristal 1983, Kristal 2009). In addition, licking the pup's head also appears to guide it to the mammary glands. This licking activity also serves to clean the puppy, the whelping box and the bitch's own anogenital area (Bleicher 1962).

Oro‐nasal interaction is also very important in stimulating the excretion process of the puppies (Rheingold 1963, Wilsson 2016). During the first 21 days of life, the puppies rely on the stimulus of licking by the mother (Fig 1) to defaecate and urinate (Grant 1987, Linde‐Forsberg 2005). The frequency of licking declines slowly over time (Korda & Brewinska 1977a, Grant 1987, Foyer et al. 2016, Guardini et al. 2016, Czerwinski et al. 2017, Guardini et al. 2017, Bray et al. 2017a). In mixed breed dogs, licking activity decreases from day 2 to day 13 to 14 (Korda & Brewinska 1977a) and by days 13 to 20 puppies become less reliant on this stimulation (Grant 1987). By the end of the third week, puppies do not require stimulation in order to trigger urination and defaecation (Scott 1958).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Licking the puppies is part of maternal care and, among other functions, is essential to stimulate the excretory behaviour of puppies during the first 3 weeks of life

The frequency of licking activity has been used to score the quality of maternal behaviour (Foyer et al. 2016, Guardini et al. 2016, Bray et al. 2017a), but the methodology can modify the results: when short time interval sampling methods were used, the frequency and the duration of anogenital licking were highly inconsistent (Czerwinski et al. 2017).

Time of contact

The time the bitch spends in contact with her puppies is important during the first weeks after birth in order to regulate the temperature of the nest and keep the puppies warm (Fig 2). There is a dramatic drop in body temperature of puppies immediately after birth, reaching a nadir within 40 minutes postpartum (van der Weyden et al. 1989). Rectal temperature then gradually increases to 35 to 37°C – the normal temperature for newborn puppies throughout the first week of life (Lawler 2008). Hypothermia is a serious risk in newborn pups because it suppresses all bodily functions, including respiration, heart rate and gastrointestinal motility. Newborn pups have poor thermoregulatory ability (Linde‐Forsberg 2010) and are dependent on behavioural heat‐seeking to maintain adequate body temperature (Grundy 2006). They rely totally on the mother and the environment to keep them warm, especially during the first few days postpartum. As puppies become less dependent on the dam and other external heat sources to maintain body temperature, the bitch spends more time away from the whelping box (Grant 1987). Although this has not been studied, the temperature of the maternity area may influence how long the bitch spends with her puppies.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

During the first days postpartum, close contact with the dam helps to keep the puppies warm, since they are unable to regulate their own body temperature

Nurturing activity

A large proportion of the mother‐pup interaction takes place during feeding sessions (Fig 3). Soon after birth, puppies are likely to nurse while the dam is still whelping (Hart 1980), but feeding the puppies appears not to be a priority of the bitch around this time (Bleicher 1962). There are several aspects of nurturing to consider, such as the stimulation to nurse, the nursing activity itself, the intake of colostrum and the position of the bitch during this activity.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

The duration and frequency of suckling is at its highest after birth and decreases over the postpartum period

Nursing is controlled by a combination of hormonal influences and the central nervous system (CNS), which acquires a suitable level of selective reactivity to the puppies' needs during lactation (Korda & Brewinska 1977b). The duration and frequency of nursing decrease and change over time. In the first 3 days postpartum, bitches were observed to nurse their puppies almost continuously (Grant 1987, Pal 2005). The act of nursing appears to be guided by a synergy between the bitch and the puppies. From day 1 to day 21 postpartum, suckling is stimulated by the bitch and the frequency decreases over time, in particular during the night (Grant 1987). The bitch approaches the puppies, lies down and licks them to stimulate suckling (Hart 1980, Grant 1987). The preference for a particular mammary gland or teat appears to be variable (Arteaga et al. 2012, Mila 2015, Orfanou et al. 2016). Each puppy regulates the number of suckling acts and their duration. Once well fed, the puppy will simply release the nipple, but as long as they remain attached, the bitch almost never leaves the nest during the first 48 hours after birth. From around day 13 postpartum, the bitch actively interrupts the nursing act (Korda & Brewinska 1977b). The nursing behaviour appears to be affected by the maturity of the pups. When 15‐day‐old puppies were replaced with 1‐ to 4‐day‐old puppies the bitch automatically increased the nursing activity and fed the younger puppies more often (Korda & Brewinska 1977b). Another reason for the decrease in the duration and frequency of nursing over time is that suckling probably becomes more efficient as the puppies get stronger (Grant 1987). The prevalence of individual puppies trying to feed when the bitch is resting increases around the 5th to 6th week postpartum, which may cause the bitch to leave the puppies and to start the weaning process (Grant 1987). Although not a general rule, there are instances when bitches have allowed puppies from other bitches to nurse (Hart 1980, Pal 2005, Paul et al. 2014).

It has been proposed that differences in the nursing posture adopted by the dam could potentially affect the development of the puppies. However, only one study has attempted to relate the maternal nursing style to the outcome for young adult guide dogs. Ventral nursing (mother lying on stomach) was associated with more puppies released from the program, whereas vertical nursing (mother sitting or standing position) were related to program success (Bray et al. 2017b). When mothers nursed ventrally their static position and the proximity of the nipples to the puppies' faces reduced the level of effort to suckle. In contrast, when mothers nursed vertically, while sitting or standing, suckling led to effortful endeavour by the puppies. The ventral nursing style fails to challenge the puppies and therefore deprives them of the opportunity to acquire a certain degree of independence (and/or problem solving), leading to an increased incidence of anxiety‐related behaviours and a lower success rate in becoming a guide dog as a consequence (Bray et al. 2017b). In another study, the dam was more commonly observed to adopt a sitting position during daytime nursing and a lying position at night (Grant 1987). The effect of the posture of the bitch during nursing requires further evaluation.

Puppy interaction with maternal behaviour

Newborns have a strong tendency to locate warm places and they instinctively detect the teat and suckle as soon as possible after birth (Scott 1967). Puppies' activities in the first 2 weeks of life consist of sleeping, heat‐seeking and nursing, and the direction of the interaction is almost always from the mother to the puppies (Grant 1987). During these first weeks, puppies spend almost 90% of the time sleeping (Domingos et al. 2008). Puppies will also crawl close to each other and the dam (Hoffman et al. 2004, Domingos et al. 2008), assumedly to maintain optimal body temperature. Puppies are attracted to the mammary gland by olfactory cues (Hart 1980) a behaviour which facilitates milk intake (Schaal 2010). Once puppies open their eyes and are able to move, their contact time with the mother increases again because now they can follow the bitch (Rheingold 1963). At this point the pattern of nursing is more variable (Grant 1987). By day 21, puppies can stand up for the first time (Orfanou et al. 2016) and actively seek out the bitch or the teat (Grant 1987).

A normal puppy should cry when experiencing any state of discomfort. However, the awareness and response of the bitch to the puppies' crying change during the peripartum period. At birth, the bitch appears to be insensitive to the crying of the puppy (Bleicher 1962), and only begins to respond to their crying after all puppies are born (Freak, 1962). However, in order to survive, puppies should be capable of signalling in any distressful situation (whether hungry or cold) by whining, which should immediately alert the dam to their needs. Whining and yelping decrease with age (Elliot & Scott 1961), as well as the response of the dam to the puppies' demands. During the first week after whelping, bitches were observed to be more attentive to the crying of a younger foster puppy than to their own older puppies (Korda & Brewinska 1977a).

MATERNAL CARE OVER TIME

All of the interactions characterised as maternal care occur at high frequency during the first weeks of a pup's life and then progressively decline over time (Rheingold 1963, Guardini et al. 2015, Foyer et al. 2016, Guardini et al. 2016, Guardini et al. 2017, Bray et al. 2017a, 2017b). In free‐roaming dogs, as well as in several of the wild canids, maternal care starts to be replaced by pack care (with the help of adult female and male dogs related to the puppies) at around 3 to 4 weeks. This increases at the time of weaning, giving the bitch the opportunity to recover her body condition (Pal 2005) and hence increase her chances of successful reproduction in the future.

In most breeding facilities, wet diet is provided to the bitch and her puppies starting at 3 weeks postpartum. At this stage, the puppies are capable of digesting food. They start to eat, partly encouraged by the appetising smell and the example of the dam, partly because the wet diet is often mixed with milk replacer or minced meat as a further incentive. As puppies become more independent, maternal care decreases in intensity. At around 3 weeks of age, puppies begin to leave the nest box and defaecate and urinate without stimulation from the dam (Houpt 2018). In addition, milk production normally ceases in the dam between 7 and 10 weeks after giving birth (Scott & Fuller 1965). As the food intake of the pups increases, a parallel decrease in nursing activity occurs, eventually leading to weaning. Beyond the purely nutritional process, the psychological satisfaction of suckling itself should not be underestimated in social animals like the dog (Malm & Jensen 1996).

FACTORS INFLUENCING MATERNAL BEHAVIOUR

Very little information is available to help define the factors influencing maternal behaviour. Although maternal behaviour differs among bitches (Rheingold 1963, Czerwinski et al. 2014, Guardini et al. 2015, Bray et al. 2017a), it is quite constant within one animal during the course of a particular motherhood/lactation (Foyer et al. 2016, Bray et al. 2017a). Conversely, no study has compared if mothering style is consistent across litters. Some environmental factors are known to influence maternal behaviour. For example, time spent with the puppies varied depending on the material used in the whelping box (Wilsson & Sundgren 1998a), or on the ambient temperature, or on the season of birth (Welker 1959, Wilsson & Sundgren 1998a, van der Waaij et al. 2008, Foyer et al. 2013, Foyer et al. 2016).

The effect of parity in the bitch is not clear. Maternal behaviour in primiparous and multiparous females appears to be similar, based on a descriptive assessment of behaviour (Bleicher 1962, Hart 1979), from the results of a questionnaire sent to dog owners (Ferrari & Monteiro‐Filho 2004), and on cumulative data on military service dogs (Foyer et al. 2013). However, an experienced mother responds more promptly to the needs of the newborn and is less disturbed by the physiological changes around parturition (Hart 1979, Foyer et al. 2016). Guardini et al. (2015), reporting on 10 small‐to‐medium size females, found that experienced bitches showed a much higher level of maternal care than primiparous bitches during the first few days postpartum, but maternal care increased in primiparous bitches over the next few weeks, eventually surpassing the multiparous bitches. Evaluation of the records of German shepherd dogs from the Swedish armed forces demonstrated that puppies from more experienced bitches scored better for confidence and physical engagement, based on principal component analyses, when tested as young adult dogs (Foyer et al. 2013). In contrast, in a study using bitches from a guide dog facility, mothers who whelped fewer litters had significantly higher maternal behaviour scores than more experienced mothers (Bray et al. 2017a). The effect of parity therefore needs further exploration.

Litter size may also influence the care provided by the bitch. Mothers with fewer puppies (1 to 5) appear to dedicate more time to individual puppies, thus improving maternal care (Foyer et al. 2016), and scoring higher in maternal style assessments (Bray et al. 2017a). Although gender bias has been described in different situations among canids (Wilsson & Sundgren 1998a, Wilsson & Sundgren 1998b, Beerda et al. 1999a, Beerda et al. 1999b, Svartberg 2002, Courreau & Langlois 2005, van der Waaij et al. 2008, Foyer et al. 2013, Foyer et al. 2016), evaluation of maternal care in relation to the gender of the puppy has not specifically been investigated although, in general, no specific or significant differences based on the puppy‐bitch interactions have been observed in relation to the gender of the puppy. Maternal care appears to be displayed in the same manner to the entire litter regardless the sex of the puppies (Foyer et al. 2016, Bray et al. 2017a).

Even though still poorly understood in dogs, genetics may play an important role in maternal behaviour and justifies attention when selecting breeding stock. Removal of bitches from the breeding program that have poor maternal behaviour appears to be a positive strategy to improve maternal behaviour overall (Houpt 2012). In general, bitches will provide less care towards sick or fading puppies, but poor maternal behaviour in relation to healthy pups should be considered in the breeding selection program. In addition, although hard to prove, it is possible that bitches with a genetic predisposition for high maternal care could at the same time be more likely to have small litters (Foyer et al. 2016). Human interference could also have a negative impact on maternal behaviour (Hart 1979), since bitches that have a stronger bond with humans might conceivably display worse maternal care and relinquish their puppies. Furthermore, if these puppies are then hand‐raised, they are more likely to be included in the breeding program, hence biasing the selection process even further towards poor mothers.

The importance of pregnancy hormones, such as prolactin, in sensitising the neural circuits controlling parental interactions, and inducing timely activation of maternal behaviours immediately after parturition has been well described in rodents (Brown et al. 2017). Likewise, the effect of oxytocin on maternal behaviour has been well documented in humans (Nagasawa et al. 2012) and sheep (Kendrick et al. 1987), but poorly in dogs. We know that luteal progesterone is essential for maintenance of pregnancy as well as for the signs of false pregnancy (pseudocyesis) in non‐pregnant bitches (Tsutsui et al. 2007). Consistently higher plasma oxytocin levels have been observed during the expulsive stage of labour than during the gestation period (Olsson et al. 2003). It has also been suggested that oxytocin increases maternal behaviour in primiparous bitches during the first 21 days postpartum (Guardini et al. 2015). Although synthetic oxytocin has been commonly used in veterinary practice to treat apparent uterine inertia, the inter‐relationships between the secretion pattern of oxytocin, the intensity of uterine contractions, the progress of foetal expulsion (Klarenbeek et al. 2007), and the effect on maternal care in dogs need further exploration. The use of intranasal oxytocin appears to improve maternal behaviour in bitches after Caesarean section (Mason 2016). According to Houpt (2012) a drop in oestrogen and progesterone, an increase in oxytocin (and possibly prolactin), cervical stimulation and the presence of a small creature with a tiny snout that is wet with amniotic fluid are all factors involved in maternal behaviour. However, how the hormonal levels interact to influence this behaviour and whether this mechanism can be manipulated to improve maternal care is unknown and requires further study. Also, the influence of in utero conditions related to maternal hormones, especially the levels of androgens and corticosteroids, still need to be evaluated in dogs. To our knowledge, no studies have been done to describe the foetal programming hypothesis in dogs as described in humans and other mammals (Meise et al. 2016).

IMPACT OF MATERNAL BEHAVIOUR ON PUPPY DEVELOPMENT

Although it has been suggested that the nursing style and the amount of interactions from the bitch can be important predictors of the future behaviour of the puppies when adult (Dietz et al. 2018), the impact of maternal care on the litter needs further evaluation. The results so far are equivocal, or at least seem to depend on the breed. In German shepherd puppies, a higher maternal score (defined as the duration of physical contact: including a combination of nursing, licking, sniffing or poking the pup) was associated with superior results in tests for military aptitude. These tests evaluated physical engagement with humans and inanimate objects, social engagement and lack of aggression at 18 months of age. Pups from litters with a higher maternal‐puppy interaction scored higher for engagement in social activities with humans, but also displayed more physical engagement and aggression, compared with puppies from less attentive mothers (Foyer et al. 2016). Similarly, beagle puppies at 8 weeks of age that had received greater maternal care (i.e. longer daily duration of maternal care) performed better in tests measuring exploratory behaviour and showed fewer signs of stress, such as increased locomotion patterns and vocalisation during isolation (Guardini et al. 2016). In a questionnaire‐based study, dog owners were asked to relate the behaviour of the adult dog to maternal care quality (based on a 7‐point scale). Owners were unequivocal in associating fearful behaviour in adult dogs with poor quality maternal care during puppyhood (Tiira & Lohi 2015). However, there is no quantitative evidence as to how much impact maternal style has on the subsequent behaviour of the pups, whether the effect is similar in all breeds, and what the long‐term effects of maternal behaviour are. Even if high maternal quality plays a role in performance tests at 2 months (Guardini et al. 2016) and at 15 months (Foyer et al. 2016), little or no correlation has been found between the results of temperament tests of dogs as puppies and later as adults, suggesting that the phenotypic variation in puppy behaviour does not predict the adult phenotype (Wilsson & Sundgren 1998a). Maternal behaviour most likely shapes the character of puppies, leading to a set of reactions and adaptations that will affect the behaviour of the puppy and probably also the adult dog. Although higher maternal care allows young puppies to cope better with stress (Guardini et al. 2015), which persists in young adult military dogs (Foyer et al. 2016), the same outcome was not observed for guide dogs. Bray et al. (2017b) reported that puppies raised by bitches with high scores regarding maternal care, displayed higher activity when isolated, a short latency before vocalising when presented with a novel object, and poor performance and perseverance during a problem‐solving task, leading to a failure to be selected for use as guide dogs.

According to Houpt (2018), the socialisation period from 4 to 14 weeks appears to be the most important from a behavioural standpoint because during this time puppies are capable of learning from the interactions with their littermates, the bitch and humans. There are no studies dealing with the effect of human or interspecies interactions during the neonatal period, usually limited to the first 2 to 3 weeks of life (Indrebo et al. 2007) on socialisation in dogs. Although susceptible to external influences during the perinatal period, the developing brain appears to be protected from the potential negative impact of high levels of adrenocorticotropic hormone and glucocorticoid during the period described as the stress hypo‐responsive period (SHRP) (Sapolsky & Meaney 1986, Rincón‐Cortés & Sullivan 2014). Described in rats, the SHRP also appears to occur in dogs, since brief maternal separation does not elicit a physiological stress response (cortisol increase) in pups of 3 to 4 weeks of age, but leads to an increase in urinary cortisol at the age of 5 to 6 weeks (Nagasawa et al. 2014). Therefore, it is possible that until 4 or 5 weeks of age, either the puppies are too young to react to maternal separation or the SHRP‐effect prevents them from the negative impact of poor maternal behaviour. In addition, during the first 2 weeks of life, the CNS is immature. Brain activity, measured by electroencephalography, is low (Fox 1971) and, as already mentioned, hearing and vision are limited or absent (Scott 1958).

In future, it might be interesting to evaluate if there is correlation between maternal style and oxytocin secretion in the bitch, examine whether the quality of maternal care is associated with the ability to cope and adjust after exposure to a major stress or trauma (resilience) in puppies (Pfau & Russo 2015) and, if so, when this influence is critical. It has been shown that, during the socialisation period (from 3.5 to approximately 12 weeks of age; Scott & Marston 1950; Scott 1958 and Scott & Fuller 1965), a greater amount and variation of stimuli will produce dogs that are more sociable to humans, and more capable of coping with challenging situations (Case 2005). However, more research is needed in this area to help understand if the maternal hormone profile during pregnancy might play a role in foetal programming in dogs, because hormones can cross the placenta.

It remains to be seen, given the complex interplay of genetics and environment, how reversible or irreversible are the consequences of early life experiences (Dietz et al. 2018) and if adequate stimulation during the socialisation period can overcome the effects of poor‐quality maternal care. The plasticity and adaptability of dog behaviour has been well‐documented in many situations, such as when introducing an adult dog from a shelter into a home environment. Scott & Fuller (1965) reported on how poor intraspecies socialisation in a puppy raised by humans for the first 9 weeks of life was overcome after contact with other puppies. Even isolation at an early age from other dogs and humans can be overcome by mimicking the play‐fighting behaviour normally experienced with its littermates, leading previously isolated dogs to respond to humans like properly socialised individuals (Fuller 1964, Scott & Fuller 1965, Fox 1967). Nevertheless, behavioural problems and phobias are one of the major reasons for relinquishment of pet dogs, as well as failure of adoption from shelters or euthanasia (Lambert et al. 2015). Therefore, it is important that more research is conducted to better understand the effect of maternal behaviour during each phase of puppy development, and the overall impact on adult dog brain plasticity, in order to improve the overall welfare of dogs in modern society.

Conflict of interest

None of the authors of this article has a financial or personal relationship with other people or organisations that could inappropriately influence or bias the content of the paper.

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful to Lexicon Scientific for their editing contribution to this paper.

References

  1. Arteaga, L. , Rödel, H. G. , Elizalde, M. T. , et al (2012) The pattern of nipple use before weaning among littermates of the domestic dog. Ethology 118, 1‐8 [Google Scholar]
  2. AZA Canid Taxon Advisory Group (2012) Large Canid (Canidae) Care Manual. Association of Zoos and Aquariums, Silver Spring, MD, USA: p 138 [Google Scholar]
  3. Battaglia, C. L. (2009) Periods of early development and the effects of stimulation and social experiences in the canine. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 4, 203‐210 [Google Scholar]
  4. Beaver, B. (1999) Canine Behavior. A Guide for Veterinarians. W.B. Saunders, Philadelphia, PA, USA: pp 205‐221 [Google Scholar]
  5. Beerda, B. , Schilder, M. B. H. , van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. , et al (1999a) Chronic stress in dogs subjected to social and spatial restriction. I. Behavioral responses. Physiology & Behavior 66, 233‐242 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Beerda, B. , Schilder, M. B. H. , Bernardina, W. , et al (1999b) Chronic stress in dogs subjected to social and spatial restriction. II. Hormonal and immunological responses. Physiology & Behavior 66, 243‐254 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bleicher, N. (1962) Behavior of the bitch during parturition. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 140, 1076‐1082 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bray, E. E. , Sammel, M. D. , Cheney, D. L. , et al (2017a) Characterizing early maternal style in a population of guide dogs. Frontiers in Psychology 8, 1‐13 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bray, E. E. , Sammel, M. D. , Cheney, D. L. , et al (2017b) Effects of maternal investment, temperament, and cognition on guide dog success. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 114, 9128‐9133 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Brown, R. S. E. , Aoki, M. , Ladyman, S. R. , et al (2017) Prolactin action is required for maternal behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114, 10779‐10784. 10.1073/pnas.1708025114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Case, L. P. (2005) The Dog: Its Behavior, Nutrition and Health. 2nd edn Blackwell, Oxford, UK: [Google Scholar]
  12. Concannon, P. W. (2000) Canine pregnancy: predicting parturition and timing events of gestation. International Veterinary Information Service, p. 1‐7. http://www.ivis.org Accessed August 15, 2018
  13. Courreau, J. F. & Langlois, B. (2005) Genetic parameters and environmental effects which characterise the defence ability of the Belgian shepherd dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 91, 233‐245 [Google Scholar]
  14. Czerwinski, V. H. , Hynd, P. I. , Smith, B. P. , et al (2014) Maternal care differs between litters of labradors: a pilot study. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 9, e16‐e17 [Google Scholar]
  15. Czerwinski, V. H. , Smith, B. P. , Hynd, P. I. , et al (2016) The influence of maternal care on stress‐related behaviors in domestic dogs: what can we learn from the rodent literature? Journal of Veterinary Behavior 14, 52‐59. 10.1016/j.jveb.2016.05.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. Czerwinski, V. H. , Smith, B. P. , Hynd, P. I. , et al (2017) Sampling maternal care behaviour in domestic dogs: What's the best approach? Behavioural Processes 140, 41‐46 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dietz, L. , Arnold, A. K. , Goerlich, V. C. , et al (2018) The importance of early life experiences for the development of behavioural disorders in domestic dogs. Behaviour 155, 83‐114 [Google Scholar]
  18. Domingos, T. C. S. , Rocha, A. A. & Cunha, I. C. N. (2008) Basic care with pregnant and neonate canine and feline: review of literature. Journal of Benefit‐Cost Analysis 1, 94‐120 [Google Scholar]
  19. Elliot, O. & Scott, J. P. (1961) The development of emotional distress reactions to separation, in puppies. The Journal of Genetic Psychology 99, 3‐22 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Ferrari, J. B. , Monteiro‐Filho, E. L. A. (2004) Canis familiaris ‐ comparative analysis of pre and postpartum behavioral patterns. Thesis – final dissertation – Análise comparativa dos padrões comportamentais pré e pós parto de Canis familiaris; 2004; Trabalho de Conclusão de Curso; (Graduação em Ciências Biológicas) ‐ Universidade Federal do Paraná; Orientador: Emygdio Leite de Araujo Monteiro Filho, 37p.
  21. Fox, M. W. (1967) The effects of short‐term social and sensory isolation upon behavior, EEG and averaged evoked potentials in puppies. Physiology and Behavior 2, 145‐151 [Google Scholar]
  22. Fox, M. W. (1971) Integrative Development of Brain and Behavior in the Dog. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, USA: p 348 [Google Scholar]
  23. Fox, M. W. (1972) Understanding your dog: everything you want to know about your dog but haven't been able to ask him. (3rd ed). Coward, McCann & Geoghegan, New York. [Google Scholar]
  24. Foyer, P. , Wilsson, E. , Wright, D. , et al (2013) Early experiences modulate stress coping in a population of German shepherd dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 146, 79‐87. 10.1016/j.applanim.2013.03.013 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Foyer, P. , Wilsson, E. & Jensen, P. (2016) Levels of maternal care in dogs affect adult offspring temperament. Scientific Reports 6, 1‐8. 10.1038/srep19253 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Freak, M. J . (1962) Abnormal conditions associated with pregnancy and parturition in the bitch. Veterinary Record 74, 1323‐1339 [Google Scholar]
  27. Fuller, J. L. (1964) Effects of experimental deprivation upon behavior in animals In: Transaction of the Third World Conference in Psychiatry, Montreal (1961). University of Toronto Press, Toronto, Canada: pp 223‐227 [Google Scholar]
  28. Germonpré, M. , Sablin, M. V. , Stevens, R. E. , et al (2009) Fossil dogs and wolves from Palaeolithic sites in Belgium, the Ukraine and Russia: osteometry ancient DNA and stable isotopes. Journal of Archaeological Science 36, 473‐490 [Google Scholar]
  29. Grant, T. R. (1987) A behavioural study of a beagle bitch and her litter during the first three weeks of lactation. Journal of Small Animal Practice 28, 992‐1003. 10.1111/j.1748-5827.1987.tb01323.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. Grundy, S. A. (2006) Clinically relevant physiology of the neonate. Veterinary Clinics of North America: Small Animal Practice 36, 443‐459. 10.1016/j.cvsm.2005.12.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Guardini, G. , Bowen, J. , Raviglione, S. , et al (2015) Maternal behaviour in domestic dogs: a comparison between primiparous and multiparous dogs. Dog Behavior 1, 23‐33. 10.4454/db.v1i1.4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  32. Guardini, G. , Mariti, C. , Bowen, J. , et al (2016) Influence of morning maternal care on the behavioural responses of 8‐week‐old beagle puppies to new environmental and social stimuli. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 181, 137‐144. 10.1016/j.applanim.2016.05.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. Guardini, G. , Bowen, J. , Mariti, C. , Fatjó J., Sighieri C., Gazzano A. (2017) Guardini G, Bowen J, Mariti C, Fatjó J, Sighieri C, Gazzano A. Influence of maternal care on Behavioural development of domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) living in a home environment. Animals 7, 93 10.3390/ani7120093 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hart, B. L. (1979) Maternal behavior in the twentieth century. Canine Practice 6, 18‐22 [Google Scholar]
  35. Hart, B. L. (1980) Postparturient maternal responses and mother‐young interections. Canine Practice 7, 10‐13 [Google Scholar]
  36. Hoffman, L. , Kelley, R. & Waltz, D. (2004) Managing puppy and kitten growth for a healthy adulthood. Proceedings of the pre‐congress symposium at the 29th world congress of the world small animal veterinary association. World small animal veterinary association, October 6 – 9, Island of Rhodes. Greece, 85‐90 [Google Scholar]
  37. Houpt, K. A. (2012) Small Animal Maternal Behavior and its Aberrations. In: Recent Advances in Companion Animal Behavior Problems, Houpt K.A. (Ed.). International Veterinary Information Service, Ithaca NY (http://www.ivis.org). http://www.applecrossvet.com.au/Portals/applecrossvet/Intranasal%20oxytocin.pdf Accessed September 23, 2018
  38. Houpt, K. A. (2018) Development of Behavior in: Domestic Animal Behavior for Veterinarians and Animal Scientists. Wiley‐Blackwell, NJ, USA: pp 127‐162 [Google Scholar]
  39. Indrebo, A. , Trangerud, C. & Moe, L. (2007) Canine neonatal mortality in four large breeds. Acta Veterinaria Scandinavica 49, S2 [Google Scholar]
  40. James, W. T. (1952) Observation on behavior of new‐born puppies: method of measurement and types of behavior involved. The Journal of Genetic Psychology 80, 65‐73 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kendrick, K. M. , Keverne, E. B. & Baldwin, B. A. (1987) Intracerebroventricular oxytocin stimulates maternal behaviour in the sheep. Neuroendocrinology 46, 56‐61. 10.1159/000124796 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Klarenbeek, M. , Okkens, A. C. , Kooistra, H. S. , et al (2007) Plasma oxytocin concentrations during late pregnancy and parturition in the dog. Theriogenology 68, 1169‐1176. 10.1016/j.theriogenology.2007.08.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kleiman, D. (1968) Reproduction in the Canidae. International Zoo Yearbook 8, 3‐8. 10.1111/j.1748-1090.1968.tb00419.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Korda, P. & Brewinska, J. (1977a) The effect of stimuli emitted by sucklings on tactile contact of the bitches with sucklings and on number of licking acts. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis 37, 99‐115 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Korda, P. & Brewinska, J. (1977b) The effect of stimuli emitted by sucklings on the course of their feeding by bitches. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis 37, 117‐130 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kristal, M. B. (2009) The biopsychology of maternal behavior in nonhuman mammals. ILAR Journal 50, 51‐63. 10.1093/ilar.50.1.51 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Lambert, K. , Coe, J. , Niel, L. , et al (2015) A systematic review and meta‐analysis of the proportion of dogs surrendered for dog‐related and owner‐related reasons. Preventive Veterinary Medicine 118, 148‐160 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Lawler, D. F. (2008) Neonatal and pediatric care of the puppy and kitten. Theriogenology 70, 384‐392 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Linde‐Forsberg, C. (2005) Abnormalities in pregnancy, parturition, and the periparturient period In: Textbook of Veterinary Internal Medicine. Eds Ettinger S. J. et al W.B. Saunders, Philadelphia, PA, USA: pp 1655‐1667 [Google Scholar]
  50. Linde‐Forsberg, C. (2010) Pregnancy diagnosis, normal pregnancy and parturition in the bitch. England G., von Heimendahl A. (Eds.), BSAVA Manual of Canine and Feline Reproduction and Neonatology, BSAVA, Gloucester, UK, 89‐97. [Google Scholar]
  51. Lord, K. , Feinstein, M. , Smith, B. , et al (2013) Variation in reproductive traits of members of the genus Canis with special attention to the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Behavioural Processes 92, 131‐142 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Malm, K. & Jensen, P. (1996) Weaning in dogs: within‐and between‐litter variation in milk and solid food intake. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 49, 223‐235 [Google Scholar]
  53. Markwell, P. J. & Thorne, C. J. (1987) Early behavioural development of dogs. Journal of Small Animal Practice 28, 984‐989 [Google Scholar]
  54. Mason, S. (2016) The use of intranasal oxytocin therapy for bitches post caesarean section. Proceedings of the Australian Reproduction Veterinarians Seminar, Treatment updates for specific disease conditions, July 9‐10, Surfers Paradise, Queensland. http://www.applecrossvet.com.au/Portals/applecrossvet/Intranasal%20oxytocin.pdf. Accessed September 30, 2018
  55. Meise, K. , von Engelhardt, N. , Forcada, J. , et al (2016) Offspring hormones reflect the maternal prenatal social environment: potential for Foetal programming? PLoS One 11, e0145352 10.1371/journal.pone.0145352 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Mila, H. (2015) Neonatal period in the dog: Immunological and nutritional determinants for survival. Thèse de doctorat en Pathologie, Toxicologie, Génétique et Nutrition. École doctorale Sciences écologiques, vétérinaires, agronomiques et bioingénieries (Toulouse).
  57. Nagasawa, M. , Okabe, S. , Mogi, K. , et al (2012) Oxytocin and mutual communication in mother‐infant bonding. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6, 31 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Nagasawa, M. , Shibata, Y. , Yonezawa, A. , et al (2014) The behavioral and endocrinological development of stress response in dogs. Developmental Psychobiology 56, 726‐733. 10.1002/dev.21141 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Noirot, E. (1972) The onset and development of maternal behavior in rats, hamsters and mice: a selective review In: Advances in the study of behavior, Vol. 4 Eds Lehrman D. S., Hinde R. A. and Shaw E. New York Academic Press, New York, NY, USA: pp 107‐145 [Google Scholar]
  60. Olsson, K. , Bergström, A. , Kindahl, H. , et al (2003) Increased plasma concentrations of vasopressin, oxytocin, cortisol and the prostaglandin F2α metabolite during labour in the dog. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica 179, 281‐287 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Orfanou, C. , Gougoulis, D. A. , Ververidis, H. N. , et al (2016) Patterns of maternal ‐ offspring behaviour of dogs and potential association with mammary health. Proceedings of the 8th International Symposium on Canine and Feline Reproduction ISCFR, June 22‐25, Paris, France, p 184.
  62. Pal, S. K. (2005) Parental care in free‐ranging dogs Canis familiaris . Applied Animal Behaviour Science 90, 31‐47 [Google Scholar]
  63. Pal, S. K. (2008) Maturation and development of social behaviour during early ontogeny in free‐ranging dog puppies in West Bengal. India Applied Animal Behaviour Science 111, 95‐107 [Google Scholar]
  64. Pang, J. F. , Kluetsch, C. , Zou, X. J. , et al (2009) MtDNA data indicate a single origin for dogs south of yangtze river, less than 16,300 years ago from numerous wolves. Molecular Biology and Evolution 26, 2849‐2864 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Paul, M. , Majumder, S. & Bhadra, A. (2014) Grandmotherly care: a case study in Indian free‐ranging dogs. Journal of Ethology 32, 75‐82 [Google Scholar]
  66. Peters, L. C. & Kristal, M. B. (1983) The suppression of infanticide in mother rats. Journal of Comparative Psychology 97, 167‐177 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Pfau, M. L. & Russo, S. J. (2015) Peripheral and central mechanisms of stress resilience. Neurobiology of Stress 1, 66‐79 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Rheingold, H. L. (1963) Maternal behavior in the dog In: Maternal Behavior. Eds Mammals I. and Rheingold H. L. John Wiley & Sons, New York, NY, USA: pp 169‐202 [Google Scholar]
  69. Rincón‐Cortés, M. & Sullivan, R. M. (2014) Early life trauma and attachment: immediate and enduring effects on neurobehavioral and stress axis development. Frontiers in Endocrinology 5, 33 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Rooney, N. J. , Clark Corinna, C. A. & Casey, R. A. (2016) Minimising fear and anxiety in working dogs: a review. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 16, 53‐64 [Google Scholar]
  71. Sapolsky, R. M. & Meaney, M. J. (1986) Maturation of the adrenocortical stress response: neuroendocrine control mechanisms and the stress hyporesponsive period. Brain Research Reviews 11, 65‐76 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Schaal, B. (2010) Mammary odor cues and pheromones: mammalian infant‐directed communication about maternal state, mammae and milk. Vitamins and Hormones 83, 83‐136 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Schweizer, C. M. & Meyers‐Wallen, V. N. (2000) Medical management of dystocia and indications for cesarean section in the bitch In: Current Veterinary Therapy XIII. Ed Bonagura J. W.B. Saunders Co, Philadelphia, PA, USA: pp 933‐939 [Google Scholar]
  74. Scott, J. P. (1958) Critical periods in the development of social behavior in puppies. Psychosomatic Medicine 20, 42‐54. 10.1097/00006842-195801000-00005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Scott, J. P. (1967) The evolution of social behavior in dogs and wolves. Zoologist 7, 373‐381 [Google Scholar]
  76. Scott, J. P. & Fuller, J. L. (1965) Genetics and the Social Behavior of the Dog. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, USA: pp 89‐108, 110‐112, 117‐150, 293 [Google Scholar]
  77. Scott, J. P. & Marston, M. V. (1950) Critical periods affecting the development of normal and mal‐adjustive social behavior of puppies. Pedagogical Seminar: The Journal of General Psychology 77, 25‐60 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Shearman, J. R. & Wilton, A. N. (2011) Origins of the domestic dog and the rich potential for gene mapping. Genetics Research International 2011, 579308 10.4061/2011/579308 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Svartberg, K. (2002) Shyness–boldness predicts performance in working dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 79, 157‐174 [Google Scholar]
  80. Tiira, K. & Lohi, H. (2015) Early life experiences and exercise associate with canine anxieties. PLoS One 10, e0141907 10.1371/journal.pone.0141907 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Tsutsui, T. , Kirihara, N. , Hori, T. , et al (2007) Plasma progesterone and prolactin concentrations in overtly pseudopregnant bitches: a clinical study. Theriogenology 67, 1032‐1038. 10.1016/j.theriogenology.2006.05.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Udell, M. A. , Dorey, N. R. & Wynne, C. D. L. (2010) What did domestication do to dogs? A new account of dogs' sensitivity to human actions. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society 85, 327‐345 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. van der Waaij, E. H. , Wilsson, E. & Strandberg, E. (2008) Genetic analysis of results of a Swedish behavior test on German shepherd dogs and Labrador retrievers. Journal of Animal Science 86, 2853‐2861 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Welker, W. I. (1959) Factors influencing aggregation of neonatal puppies. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 52, 376‐380 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Wells, D. (2009) Behaviour of dogs In: The Ethology of Domestic Animals: An Introductory Text. Ed Jensen P. Cabi Publishing, Linkoping, Sweden: pp 192‐203 [Google Scholar]
  86. van der Weyden, G. C. , Taverne, M. A. , Dieleman, S. J. , et al (1989) Physiological aspects of pregnancy and parturition in dogs. Journal of Reproduction and Fertility. Supplement 39, 211‐224 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Wilsson, E. (2016) Nature and nurture—how different conditions affect the behavior of dogs. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 16, 45‐52 [Google Scholar]
  88. Wilsson, E. & Sundgren, P. E. (1998a) Behavior test for 8‐week‐old puppies—heritabilities of tested behavior traits and its correspondence to later behavior. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 58, 151‐162 [Google Scholar]
  89. Wilsson, E. & Sundgren, P. E. (1998b) Effects of weight, litter size and parity of mother on the behaviour of the puppy and the adult dog. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 56, 245‐254. 10.1016/S0168-1591(97)00094-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  90. Wykes, P. M. & Olson, P. N. (2003) Vagina, vestibule, and vulva In: Textbook of Small Animal Surgery. Ed Slatter D. H. WB Saunders, Philadelphia, PA, USA: pp 1502‐1510 [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Small Animal Practice are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES