Abstract
Background
Older African Americans are more likely to develop Alzheimer’s disease (AD) than older Caucasians, and this difference cannot be readily explained by cerebrovascular and socioeconomic factors alone. We previously showed that mild cognitive impairment and AD dementia were associated with attenuated increases in the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) levels of total and phosphorylated tau in African Americans compared to Caucasians, even though there was no difference in beta-amyloid 1–42 level between the two races.
Methods
We extended our work by analyzing early functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) biomarkers of the default mode network in older African Americans and Caucasians. We calculated connectivity between nodes of the regions belonging to the various default mode network subsystems and correlated these imaging biomarkers with non-imaging biomarkers implicated in AD (CSF amyloid, total tau, and cognitive performance).
Results
We found that race modifies the relationship between functional connectivity of default mode network subsystems and cognitive performance, tau, and amyloid levels.
Conclusion
These findings provide further support that race modifies the AD phenotypes downstream from cerebral amyloid deposition, and identifies key inter-subsystem connections for deep imaging and neuropathologic characterization.
Keywords: Alzheimer’s disease, Cognitive impairment, Functional connectivity, Default mode network, Disparities
Introduction
It is not well understood why older African Americans are twice as likely to develop Alzheimer’s disease (AD) as older non-Hispanic Caucasian Americans (abbreviated as Caucasian hereafter) [1, 2]. While vascular disease [3–5] has been speculated to contribute to the disparities in AD risks, genome-wide association and clinical studies suggest race/ethnicity (hereafter referred to as race) also independently modifies the molecular pathways implicated in the development and manifestation of AD pathology. For example, the APOE ε4 allele confers lower AD risks for African Americans than Caucasians [6, 7], the ABCA7 risk allele confers greater AD risks for African Americans than Caucasians [8], and AD is associated with less amnestic baseline performance and slower longitudinal decline in African Americans than Caucasians on neuropsychological analysis [9]. These cohort-level differences may reflect intrinsic biological differences between race, lower correlation between clinically-suspected and pathologically-confirmed AD (~ 75% accurate), recruitment bias in one or both races, or a combination of these factors [10, 11]. Data-driven strategies are therefore necessary to provide mechanistic correlates of observed race-associated differences to more clearly understand AD disparity.
One such approach is to use etiologic biomarkers associated with hallmark AD pathology to enhance the likelihood that those clinically suspected to have AD indeed have the pathology. We recently showed that in a group of older adults with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or AD dementia, African Americans had lower cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) levels of tau-related biomarkers than Caucasians [12]. This is despite similar changes in CSF levels beta-amyloid 1–42 (Aβ42). We interpreted these findings as preliminary evidence for divergent biomarker trajectories and these differences have now been validated in one independent cohort in St. Louis as well as an independent younger cohort in Atlanta [13, 14].
Because we have not identified a difference in atrophy patterns on MRI between African Americans and Caucasians with AD, we hypothesized that resting-state functional MRI (rsfMRI) would be a more sensitive approach to identify the effect of race on AD-related neurological changes. We are particularly interested in resting state functional connectivity, as alterations in connectivity can be detected well before symptom onset [15] and track disease progression [16]. To explore brain changes associated with AD which may differ between races, we analyzed functional connectivity (hereafter referred to as connectivity) in the default mode network (DMN) using rsfMRI. The DMN is considered a potentially useful imaging biomarker for AD that is more widely available than amyloid PET [17–22].
In older adults, the DMN is broadly defined as correlated Blood Oxygen Level Dependent (BOLD) signal among the precuneus, posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), the inferior parietal lobule (IPL) and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) [20, 23]. The DMN overlaps with anatomical sites vulnerable to amyloid deposition and atrophy in early AD [24], and reduced connectivity between DMN nodes (intra-network connectivity) mirrors the stage-wise tau deposition on PET imaging [25, 26] even before there is detectable atrophy [3]. The trajectory of AD functional connectivity changes is complex. The overwhelming majority of studies examining four DMN nodes reported reduced connectivity in AD (dementia) [27–31], with an exception reporting increased connectivity during early MCI [27]. However, few studies used etiologic biomarkers to distinguish between cognitive impairment due to AD, psychiatric illness, or cerebrovascular disease [32]. DMN hyperconnectivity has also been observed in asymptomatic APOE ε4 carriers when compared to non-carriers [33, 34], sometimes decades before symptom onset [35]. DMN connectivity may therefore have different relationships with AD risks (including risk genes), pathologic markers, clinical phenotypes, and disease stage, making inclusion of etiologic and clinical biomarkers in AD-related DMN analysis critical to ensure the consistency of findings.
As research on the DMN progresses, further fractionation of this complex network has revealed synchronous BOLD activity in regions outside traditional definitions of the DMN. Core subsystems [36] (dorsomedial, medial temporal lobe, and midline core) have been proposed to each contain key regions which work in tandem to support cognitive processes in learning and memory, retrieval of autobiographical information, self-referential processes [37], and social processing [38]. Dividing the DMN into its subcomponents has thus far provided more sensitive timelines for disease progression in AD and other neurological disorders [28]. Studies have shown that connectivity within the medial temporal lobe, rather than average DMN connectivity between the four core nodes, more consistently relates to cognitive impairment in AD [16, 30]; increases in connectivity within the anterior subsystems during early AD is more consistently identified in studies analyzing DMN subsystems [30, 34]; and memory impairment can be associated with decreased intra-subsystem connectivity within the medial temporal lobe [28] but increased connectivity between dorsomedial and midline core subsystems [39, 40].
The vast majority of studies analyze connectivity changes within diagnostic categories of normal cognition (NC), MCI, and AD dementia. Given that differences in cognitive impairment between NC and MCI and between MCI and AD dementia can sometimes be small, a continuous measure of cognition is preferred [41, 42] especially when it remains controversial whether current diagnostic algorithms are valid in African Americans (even with race-adjusted norms) [3, 43]. Thus, we also use a composite measure of cognitive performance derived from neuropsychological tests [12] to serve as a continuous, rather than categorical, measure of disease burden. We hypothesized that race modifies the relationship between connectivity and AD-related cognitive impairment, and between connectivity and two CSF AD biomarkers (Aβ42 [44] and t-Tau [45]). Furthermore, we specifically tested the generalizability of AD-associated connectivity changes between DMN nodes and between DMN subsystems to extend the AD biomarker phenotype in African Americans.
Methods
Participants
This study analyzed previously collected data from a study that recruited self-reported Non-Hispanic Caucasians and African Americans over the age of 65 across the diagnostic spectrum of Alzheimer’s disease dementia including individuals with normal cognition (NC), individuals with mild cognitive impairment (MCI), and individuals with Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) [12]. The study was approved by the Emory University Institutional Review Board. Each participant underwent a detailed interview for demographic information, self-reported race (Caucasians of Hispanic or Latino ethnicity were not included in this study), vascular risk factors (coronary artery disease, congestive heart failure, atrial fibrillation, hypertension, hyperlipidemia, diabetes, suspected transient ischemic attack), other medical comorbidities (e.g., cancer), and medications (e.g., use of angiotensin-converting enzyme inhibitors or angiotensin II receptor blockers). Each participant was then assigned a diagnosis according to consensus criteria including those for NC, MCI, and AD dementia (global Clinical Dementia Rating of 1 or 2.) Cognitively impaired subjects suspected of having a non-AD dementia (vascular, Lewy body, and frontotemporal dementia) were excluded. While our cohort was not age and gender matched specifically, we did not find significant differences in age or gender between races (Table 1). As previously reported, diabetes and hypertension were more common in African Americans than Caucasians, but African Americans had lower brain total white matter hyperintensity (WMH) volumes than Caucasians. Demographic data in Table 1 refer to individuals who passed MRI quality control standards (n = 137) as described below.
Table 1.
Overall (N = 137) | Normal cognition | MCI | AD | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Caucasian (n = 72) | African American (n = 65) | Caucasian (n = 31) | African American (n = 27) | Caucasian (n = 25) | African American (n = 28) | Caucasian (n = 16) | African American (n = 10) | |
Age, years (SD) | 70.90 (7.84) | 69.20 (7.49) | 71.65 (8.39)* | 67.48 (6.17)* | 71.52 (5.82) | 70.07 (7.74) | 68.50 (9.41) | 71.4 (9.65) |
Women (Men) | 35 (37) | 43 (29) | 19 (12) | 17 (10) | 15 (10) | 12 (16) | 9 (7) | 6 (4) |
Years of Education (SD) | 16.32 (2.85) | 16.15 (2.85) | 16.81 (2.69) | 15.78 (2.66) | 17.08 (2.58) | 16.29 (2.81) | 14.19 (1.94) | 16.80 (3.55) |
MMSE (SD) | 26.32 (4.90) | 26.91 (3.57) | 28.84 (0.86) | 28.00 (1.86) | 27.64 (1.68) | 26.67 (2.34) | 21.37 (3.38) | 20.22 (6.18) |
Cognitive Z Scores (SD) | −0.78 (1.12) | −0.72 (0.99) | 0.07 (0.51) | −0.11 (0.51) | − 0.88 (0.86) | − 0.67 (0.59) | −2.24 (0.66) | −2.36 (1.02) |
White Matter Hyperintensity volume (mm3) | 3984.40 (4110.10) | 3886.03 (4964.93) | 3306.81 (3337.32) | 2440.93 (2576.15) | 4018.66 (4080.03) | 4313.70 (5330.60) | 5243.68 (5333.14) | 6730.21 (7607.48) |
Having ABCA7 risk allele, (%) | 23.67* | 43.10* | 29.00 | 44.40 | 24.00 | 50.00 | 12.50 | 30.00 |
Have Diabetes, (%) | 5.67** | 33.80** | 6.50** | 33.33** | 0* | 28.60* | 12.50* | 50.00* |
Have Hypertension (%) | 45.83** | 72.31** | 45.16 | 62.96 | 56.00 | 78.57 | 31.25* | 80.00* |
CSF Aβ42, pg/ml (SD) | 148.35 (95.02) | 168.48 (128.86) | 199.93 (132.30) | 165.27 (89.8) | 199.11 (141.02) | 134.69 (90.99) | 77.65 (47.39) | 133.55 (140.71) |
CSF t-Tau, pg/ml (SD) | 72.84 (49.63)** | 40.92 (20.79)** | 51.68 (30.96)** | 36.04 (11.63)** | 63.99 (33.71)* | 35.44 (13.67)* | 108.48 (77.43) | 75.8 (33.52) |
CSF p-Tau181, pg/ml (SD) | 17.58 (8.97)** | 25.81 (12.47)** | 23.47 (9.71)** | 14.05 (5.07)** | 25.01 (13.42)* | 17.74 (7.21)* | 33.53 (14.64) | 24.54 (10.27) |
CSF t-Tau/Aβ42 | 0.73 (0.91)* | 0.24 (0.22)* | 0.46 (0.74)* | 0.15 (0.12)* | 0.44 (0.42)* | 0.22 (0.14)* | 1.36 (1.16) |
0.55 (0.41) |
Note: T-tests were performed to compare races in the whole cohort and within each diagnostic category. *p < .05, **p < .001. MMSE and CogZ scores were significantly different between diagnostic categories such, NC having highest MMSE scores and AD having lowest (MMSE: NC vs MCI, p = .007, MCI vs AD, p < .0001, CogZ: NC vs MCI, p < .0001, MCI vs AD, p < .0001)
Cognitive, genetic, and CSF biomarkers
Neuropsychological analysis was performed as previously described [12]. Briefly, each subject underwent a detailed neurologic examination and neuropsychological analysis. These included (1) memory (Consortium to Establish A Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease word list delayed recall, Brief Visual Memory Test–Revised [BVMT-R] delayed recall), (2) executive function (Trail Making Test B, reverse digit span [RD], Symbol Digit Substitution Test, and letter-guided fluency), (3) language (Boston Naming Test [60 items], category fluency), and (4) visuospatial function (Judgment of Line Orientation [JOLO], Rey-Osterrieth complex figure test). With the exception of BVMT-R, JOLO, and RD, subtest Z-scores were calculated according to published normative data, adjusting for age, sex, education, and race. Z-scores for these three subtests were calculated using the same norms in Caucasians, but calculated using Atlanta-based, cognitively normal African Americans because published norms generated mean Z-scores of > 2. Domain-specific Z-scores were calculated by averaging subtest Z-scores, and Z-scores for the four domains were averaged to generate composite cognitive Z-scores. Subjects with MCI and AD dementia had lower MMSE and cognitive Z-scores than subjects with NC (p < 0.01 for all comparisons, Table 1). In addition, each subject underwent standardized collection of blood (for APOE and ABCA7 genotyping) and CSF without overnight fasting according to a modified Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) protocol as previously described [46].
MRI acquisition and preprocessing
Each subject underwent MRI scanning using a modified ADNI protocol on a 3 T scanner (Siemens AG) which included a T1-weighted 3D MPRAGE sequence (TR/TI/TE = 1620/950/3 msec, flip angle = 30o, matrix = 192 × 256 × 160, and voxel size = 0.98 × 0.98 × 1 mm3) and a 6 min eyes-open resting state functional MRI scan (TR/TE = 3000 ms/32 ms flip angle = 90o, field of view (FOV) = 200 × 200 mm2, acquisition matrix = 64 × 64, voxel size = 3.1 × 3.1 × 3.5 mm3, slice = 33, time point = 124) at the Emory Center for Systems Imaging. For rsfMRI, we used the DPABI v4.0.190305 toolbox to preprocess the image data [47] after discarding the first 10 volumes to allow the magnetization to approach a dynamic equilibrium, and to allow for more time for our participants to get comfortable inside the scanner [31, 48]. Individual echo-planar imaging (EPI) data were slice time corrected. Participants whose head motion exceeded 3.0 mm in translation or 3° in rotation were excluded. We further reduced the confound of head motion by higher-order regression based on Friston’s 24-parameter model [49], and the effect of physiological artifacts by covarying signals from CSF space and white matter [50]. EPI data were normalized to a study specific template generated using the DARTEL algorithm in DPABI that is better suited for populations with larger amounts of atrophy than standard normalization to the MNI template [51]. A spatial filter of 6 mm full width at half maximum Gaussian kernel was used. Subsequently, a band pass temporal filter (0.01–0.08 Hz) was applied to reduce the low-frequency drifts and high-frequency noise.
MRI quality control
To be eligible for this analysis, participants must have had a T1 suitable for use in segmentation, as well as a usable resting state scan. To further eliminate confounds from head motion, we removed anyone whose mean framewise displacement (MWFD) was 3 mm and higher [52]. Among 145 subjects, 8 (5%) had rsfMRI that did not pass quality control and were excluded from DMN analysis. Table 1 displays demographic data only for individuals included in the MRI analysis (n = 137), and Table 2 shows demographic data for individuals not included in the analysis. Compared to those included the analysis, those excluded did not differ significantly in age, gender, diagnosis, or race. There was no significant difference in motion according to race or diagnosis.
Table 2.
Overall (N = 8) | Normal cognition | MCI | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Caucasian (n = 5) | African American (n = 3) | Caucasian (n = 4) | African American (n = 2) | Caucasian (n = 1) | African American (n = 1) | |
Age, years (SD) | 69.78 (5.83) | 65.20 (10.25) | 71.65 (8.39) | 67.48 (6.17) | 71.52 | 70.07 |
Years of Education (SD) | 16.82 (2.85) | 16.25 (2.63) | 16.84 (2.69) | 16.78 (2.66) | 17.08 | 16.29 |
MMSE (SD) | 27.82 (4.90) | 28.91 (3.57) | 28.94 (0.86) | 27.75 (1.86) | 27.64 | 26.67 |
Cognitive Z Scores | 0.89 (2.62) | 0.82 (1.89) | −0.02 (0.62) | 0.01 (0.71) | 0.84 | 0.75 |
Having ABCA7 risk allele, (number) | 1 | 2 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 |
Have Diabetes, (number) | 0 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0.0 | 0 |
Have Hypertension (number) | 2 | 2 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 |
CSF Aβ42, pg/ml (SD) | 133.34 (95.02) | 139.81 (140.36) | 200.93 (132.40) | 164.27 (98.6) | 201.11 | 134.69 |
CSF t-Tau, pg/ml (SD) | 72.84 (49.63) | 40.92 (20.79) | 51.68 (30.96) | 36.04 (11.63) | 63.99 | 35.438 |
CSFp-Tau 181, pg/ml (SD) | 42.71 (20.98) | 29.90 (15.60) | 37.44 (16.29) | 22.95 (8.46) | 29.65 | 17.25 |
CSF t-Tau/Aβ42 | 0.79 (0.81) | 0.26 (0.21) | 0.85 (0.87) | 0.15 (0.16) | 0.46 | 0.22 |
RsfMRI independent component analysis
We used a data driven approach (Independent Component Analysis; ICA) using the Group ICA of fMRI Toolbox v4.0b (GIFT) to identify large-scale brain networks [53, 54]. We first performed independent components analysis with model-order of 80 to empirically derive our regions of interest which enabled us to break the DMN into its various subregions, while still maintaining appropriate degrees of freedom. ICA is a data driven approach that allows for more adaptation to individual subject variability, which is essential in special populations, particularly those with atrophy as in our sample. The DMN and its subnetworks are relatively robust, and can easily be identified in a higher order ICA model [16, 55]. We chose an ICA approach as it can be more sensitive to sample characteristics, such as brain atrophy in older populations, than standard atlas based seed-regions while still accurately identifying regions of interest [56, 57].
To identify our regions of interest, we correlated all non-artifactual components [58] with templates of the DMN and chose components with the highest correlation values to the templates (0.80 cutoff threshold). Using the default mode network subdivisions and coordinates outlined by Andrews-Hanna [36], we identified 11 components that contained our regions of interest for the DMN subsystems. Components were manually confirmed using the xjview toolbox (http://www.alivelearn.net/xjview/) to ensure that they contained only our regions of interest. Regions included the temporal pole (TP), lateral temporal cortex (2 regions; ventrolateral (vlTC) and dorsolateral (dlTC), dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC), and the temporal parietal junction (TPJ) which comprised the dorsomedial subsystem; the parahippocampal gyrus (pHG), hippocampus, and posterior inferior parietal lobule (pIPL) comprised the medial temporal lobe subsystem; finally the precuneus, posterior cingulate (PCC), and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) comprised the midline core subsystem (Fig. 1). We then calculated functional connectivity in GIFT by correlating the time courses of signal fluctuations between the chosen components, and obtained a correlation value for each region pair for a total of 55 measures of pairwise connectivity.
Statistical analyses
Statistical analysis was performed in IBM SPSS 24.0 (Armonk, NY) and R version 3.3.3 [59]. MANCOVA was used to determine if race modifies DMN connectivity according to cognition. First, we analyzed baseline connectivity differences (only within controls). Measures of intra-network connectivity between the DMN nodes were the dependent variables; cognitive scores, race, gender, age, and MFWD were independent variables. Next, we analyzed data from all participants using the same model, but included a higher order interaction term (race X cognitive scores). Separate models to additionally account for effects of APOE ε4, ABCA7 risk allele, hypertension, total WMH volume, cardiovascular risk score, and diabetes on DMN connectivity were also analyzed. For race-dependent connectivity changes, we accounted for multiple comparisons through the Benjamin-Hochberg method [60]. False discovery rate was limited to 10% given our sample size and the number of nominally significant interactions with race.
The same analysis was repeated according to Aβ42 levels in all subjects. Because there is significant overlap in t-Tau levels between NC and AD, we performed a third analysis according to t-Tau levels only in subjects with reduced Aβ42 levels (< 192 pg/mL) [61]consistent with cerebral amyloid deposition [62]. Compared to using uncorrected nominal p < 0.05 as a threshold, we reduced the number of race-dependent node pairs from 23 to ten (from six to four for cognition, from ten to two for CSF Aβ42, and from seven to four for CSF t-Tau). Because we observed an over-representation of race’s effect on inter-subsystem connectivity between nodes belonging to the midline core and dorsomedial (midline-dorsomedial) subsystems regardless of the measure used for AD (cognition, Aβ42, t-Tau), we used bootstrapping (see below) to test whether the midline-dorsomedial connectivity was preferentially modified by race in AD compared to intra-subsystem and other inter-subsystem node pairs. Finally, as confirmation, we used analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) to determine whether race influenced the mean midline-dorsomedial connectivity, midline-temporal connectivity, and dorsomedial-temporal connectivity adjusting for diagnosis, age, and gender. Mean subsystem connectivity [28] was calculated by averaging, for each individual, all pairwise inter-subsystem node pair connectivity between the two subsystems in question (15 pairs in midline-dorsomedial, 9 pairs in midline-temporal, and 15 pairs in dorsomedial-temporal).
Bootstrapping
We developed a novel simulation-based approach to test whether there was empirical enrichment, or over-representation, for race modifying connectivity between midline core and dorsomedial subsystems. To determine the likelihood of a concentration of significant interaction terms occurring by chance alone, we first obtained p-values for all Race x Cognitive Z-score interaction term for all potential node pairings (n = 55; all subjects), and repeated the process for Aβ42 (n = 55; all subjects) and t-Tau (n = 55; only in subjects with Aβ42 < 192 pg/mL).
As these AD features are inter-related, we pooled all 165 (55 × 3) p-values together, and used bootstrapping analysis (“boot” package in R [63], with replacement) to create 1500 simulated 3 × 5 (size of midline-dorsomedial matrix) matrices of p-values. The number of matrices (out of 1500) with three or more significant p-values is thus the probability of an observed concentration in any random 3 × 5 matrix of node pairs resulting from chance alone. At the same time, because this probability can be artificially reduced by a more stringent threshold at the matrix level (e.g., four or more significant p-values), we created a second set of 1500 simulated p-value matrices through the same bootstrapping process to represent the range of possible midline-dorsomedial p-values. Instead of drawing from all potential p-values, these 1500 matrices were then only sampled from p-values pooled from the 45 interaction p-values between midline-dorsomedial node pairs (n = 15 each for Race x Cognitive Z-score, Race x Aβ42, and Race x t-Tau) (Fig. 2). The probability of having three or more significant p-value in each matrix in this second bootstrap is then compared with the first using Chi-squared test. The null hypothesis for this test was that the number of samples that contained more than three significant p values would not differ between the midline-dorsomedial bootstrap and the chance-only bootstrap. We elected to use 1500 as the bootstrap size as it is well within the commonly recommended threshold [64], but still a tiny fraction of all possible combinations.
Results
Baseline connectivity differences
We first compared baseline connectivity profiles between older African Americans and Caucasians with NC (n = 58, Fig. 3). Compared to Caucasians, African Americans had lower connectivity between the precuneus and the ventrolateral temporal cortex (by 0.31, 95% CI 0.16− 0.46, p = 0.01), the inferior parietal lobule and parrahippocampal gyrus (by − 0.15, 95% -0.28, − 0.03, p = 0.01), and the temporal pole and hippocampus (by 0.19, 95% CI 0.33− 0.04-0.33, p = 0.01; Table 3). There were otherwise no baseline connectivity differences in the remaining 52 inter-nodal connectivity values between the two racial groups.
Table 3.
Connectivity | B (95% Confidence interval) | Unadjusted p | Storey’s q |
---|---|---|---|
Temporal Pole to Hippocampus | −0.19(− 0.33, − 0.04) | 0.01 | 0.137 |
Ventrolateral Temporal Cortex to Precuneus | −0.31(− 0.46, − 0.16) | 0.01 | < 0.001 |
Inferior Parietal Lobule to Parahippocampal gyrus | − 0.15(− 0.28, − 0.03) | 0.01 | 0.495 |
Race-independent changes centered in the medial temporal lobe subsystem of DMN
Because AD is characterized by reduced CSF Aβ42, increased CSF t-Tau, and cognitive impairment, we first analyzed the relationship between DMN connectivity, AD biomarkers (cognitive Z-score, Aβ42, t-Tau), and race to determine when race did not modify the relationship between AD biomarker and connectivity. In both African Americans and Caucasians, lower (more abnormal) Aβ42 levels correlated with decreased connectivity between the inferior parietal lobule and the parrahippocampal gyrus (B = −0.01, t (167) = − 2.14, p = 0.02). Because there is overlap in CSF t-Tau and p-Tau181 levels between controls and AD even though their levels are elevated at the group level, we restricted t-Tau-related analysis to those with Aβ42 levels consistent with AD (< 192 pg/mL). This also showed higher (more abnormal) t-Tau levels to correlate with decreased connectivity between multiple region pairs within the DMN, including hippocampus-temporal pole (B =0.04, t (167) = 1.58, p = 0.02) (Fig. 4). Connectivity correlated with cognitive impairment regardless of race appeared to occur between the medial temporal lobe and the midline core subsystems, and between the medial temporal lobe and the dorsomedial subsystems (Table 4, Fig. 4).
Table 4.
Subsystems | Connectivity | Factor | B (95% Confidence Interval) | Unadjusted p | Storey’s q |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
MTL to MTL |
Posterior IPL to Hippocampus | Race | 0.019(− 0.29, − 0.05) | 0.387 | |
t-Tau | 0.002(0.00, 0.003) | < 0.001 | < 0.001 | ||
Posterior IPL to Parahippocampal gyrus | Race | 0.32(−0.14, .25) | 0.710 | ||
Aβ42 | 0.02 (0.008, 0.40) | 0.010 | 1.000 | ||
MTL to midline core | Posterior IPL to Ventromedial prefrontal cortex | Race | −0.05(−0.30, 0.20) | 0.675 | |
t-Tau | 0.002(0.00, 0.003) | 0.001 | 0.004 | ||
Parahippocampal gyrus to Precuneus | Race | −0.10(−0.30, 0.23) | 0.358 | ||
t-Tau | 0.002 (0.00, 0.002) | 0.008 | 0.147 | ||
MTL to dorsomedial | Temporal Pole to Hippocampus | Race | −0.10(−0.30, 0.10) | 0.337 | |
t-Tau | 0.002(0.00, 0.003) | 0.009 | 0.247 |
Unadjusted p-values which remain significant after Benjamini-Hochberg step-up correction for multiple comparisons are bolded. Storey’s q-values are also shown with FDR < 10%. MTL Medial Temporal Lobe, IPL inferior parietal lobule
Race selectively modified the relationship between AD biomarkers and connectivity only between the MTL and Dorsomedial subsystem nodes
We next examined node pairs whose connectivity relationship with AD biomarkers was modified by race (Table 5, Fig. 5). In Caucasians, greater cognitive impairment was associated with decreased DMN connectivity between the precuneus and lateral temporal cortex, and between the precuneus and the temporal pole. However, the opposite is true in African Americans, with greater cognitive impairment associated with increased connectivity between these same regions. Similarly, lower (more abnormal) Aβ42 levels correlated with greater connectivity between the precuneus and both lateral temporal cortex and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex only in African Americans. Higher t-Tau levels (in those with Aβ42 levels < 192 pg/mL) also correlated with greater connectivity between the lateral temporal cortex and precuneus, and between the temporal pole and both vmPFC and precuneus, and between the hippocampus and PCC, again only in African Americans. Adjusting for risk genes (ABCA7, APOE) and other factors (hypertension, cardiovascular risk score, white matter hyperintensities, and diabetes) did not significantly influence connectivity values and race-associated differences persisted in connectivity relationship.
Table 5.
Subsystem | Connectivity Pair | Variable Name | B(95% Confidence Interval) | Unadjusted p | Storey’s q |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Dorsomedial to midline core | Dorsomedial Prefrontal Cortex to Precuneus | Race | −0.02(−0.30, 0.25) | 0.861 | |
Cognitive impairment | 0.04(−0.13, 0.04) | 0.292 | |||
Race * Cognitive impairment | −0.14(0.07, 0.25) | 0.005 | 0.092 | ||
Ventrolateral Temporal Cortex to Precuneus | Race | −0.24(− 0.59, 0.11) | 0.170 | ||
Cognitive impairment | −0.08(− 0.02, 0.19) | 0.103 | |||
Race * Cognitive impairment | 0.18(0.02, 0.17) | 0.001 | 0.055 | ||
Race | −0.05(−0.32, 0.21) | 0.695 | |||
t-Tau | −0.003(− 0.02, − 0.002) | 0.712 | |||
Race * t-Tau | 0.02(−0.03, − 0.0002) | 0.001 | 0.055 | ||
Dorsomedial Prefrontal cortex to Posterior Cingulate Cortex | Race | 0.29(0.08, 0.50) | 0.007 | ||
Cognitive Impairment | 0.05(−0.15, 0.03) | 0.407 | |||
Race * Cognitive impairment | 0.18(0.02, 0.17) | 0.007 | 0.096 | ||
Temporal Pole to Precuneus | Race | 0.05(−0.18, −0.29) | 0.667 | ||
Aβ42 | 0.0003(0.00, 0.01) | 0.345 | |||
Race * Aβ42 | −0.001(−0.002, − 0.00001) | 0.004 | 0.073 | ||
Race | −0.80(− 0.29, 0.13) | 0.442 | |||
t-Tau | −0.002(− 0.01, − 0.001) | 0.736 | |||
Race * t-Tau | 0.003(0.001, 0.006) | 0.005 | 0.069 | ||
Dorsolateral Temporal Cortex to Precuneus | Race | −0.21(−0.12, 0.36) | 0.112 | ||
Aβ42 | 0.0002(−0.0001, 0.01) | 0.345 | |||
Race * Aβ42 | −0.002(− 0.003, − 0.00001) | 0.002 | 0.055 | ||
Race | −0.21(− 0.43, 0.02) | 0.070 | |||
t-Tau | −0.001(− 0.002, − 0.02) | 0.066 | |||
Race * t-Tau | 0.004(0.001, 0.006) | 0.003 | 0.083 | ||
Race | −0.074(− 0.20,0.54) | 0.004 | |||
Cognitive impairment | 0.27(−0.04,0.09) | 0.001 | |||
Race * Cognitive impairment | 0.20(0.05, 0.25) | 0.002 | 0.055 | ||
MTL to midline core | Posterior Cingulate to Hippocampus | Race | −0.001(−0.22, 0.22) | 0.900 | |
t-Tau | −0.001(− 0.03, 0.002) | 0.199 | |||
Race * t-Tau | 0.003(0.001, 0.052) | 0.004 | 0.073 |
Unadjusted p-values which remain significant after Benjamini-Hochberg step-up correction for multiple comparisons are bolded. Storey’s q-values are also shown with FDR < 10%. MTL Medial Temporal Lobe, IPL inferior parietal lobule
Visualizing race-independent (Fig. 4) and race-dependent (Fig. 5) DMN changes in AD, we observed a pattern of race-specific changes involving connectivity between two subsystems. Whereas race-independent connectivity occurred between each pair of subsystems, nine out of ten race-dependent connectivity changes were between the midline core and dorsomedial subsystems. For each subject, we calculated a mean connectivity value by averaging the all node-pair connectivity values between two subsystems. ANCOVA adjusting for diagnosis, age, and gender showed a main effect for race (F (2, 119) = 3.255, p = 0.074) for mean midline-dorsomedial connectivity, but not for mean midline-temporal connectivity (F (2,119) = 0.061, p = 0.8060) or mean dorsomedial-temporal connectivity (F (2,119) = 1.418, p = 0.236).
We further tested whether the midline-dorsomedial connectivity had an over-representation of node pairs whose connectivity was modified by race compared to the rest of DMN, we used bootstrapping (with replacement) to create 1500 simulated 3 × 5 matrices drawn from midline-dorsomedial node pairs and 1500 simulated matrices drawn from all node pairs. We found that drawing from the midline-dorsomedial matrices was more likely to result in identifying at least three significant race X AD biomarker effect than drawing from all node pairs: 791/1500 in the midline-dorsomedial sample vs. 192/1500 in the chance-only sample, X2 (2, N = 3000) = 487.53, p = 0.00001.
Discussion
Consistent with previous work, we found AD to alter connectivity between the medial temporal lobe and dorsomedial subsystems, but we identified race-specific changes associated with these alterations [ 65, 66]. Importantly, we extend the effect of race on AD-related connectivity from the inter-nodal level to the inter-subsystem level through a novel analytical strategy. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first attempt to statistically identify enrichment of a factor’s effect on connectivity between two subsystems across multiple related measures (cognition, Aβ42, t-Tau). The implication of this inter-subsystem effect is not well understood. Other conditions previously observed to confer similar specificity on inter-subsystem connectivity include PTSD [36], depression [67], and schizophrenia [68]. Interestingly, some of these conditions show racial disparities (schizophrenia [69] and PTSD [70] are more common in African Americans than Caucasians). The inter-subsystem specificity may reflect shared vulnerability to neuropsychiatric disorders in African Americans, existence of disease subtypes, or divergent disease-associated pathways. We discuss these possibilities in the context of AD in African Americans below.
In contrast to a uniformly slow disease process in African Americans, it is possible that the different pathologic processes in AD may not proceed at the same pace in African Americans. In post-mortem studies of AD (involving primarily Caucasians), Aβ42-rich neuritic plaques are found early in the medial temporal as well as neocortical regions [24]. In contrast, tau-related changes appear in the medial temporal lobe before a stage-wise involvement of the frontal and then parietal cortical regions [24]. If we can interpret these observations as early co-localization of neuritic plaques and neurofibrillary tangles in the medial temporal lobe, the race-independent effect on inter-subsystem connectivity involving this region is in keeping with shared early AD changes by older African Americans and Caucasians when CSF Aβ42 alterations are detectable. The attenuation of midline-dorsomedial connectivity in African Americans could then be interpreted as early compensation when AD is mild, or as pathological hyper-connectivity [71, 72]. This would support the diminished cognitive reserve hypothesis in African Americans (potentially due to vascular disease [73]), and the prevailing longitudinal models that African Americans have slower decline in the presence of AD pathology [3, 74]. At the same time, the correspondence between ante-mortem DMN connectivity changes and post-mortem lesional mapping is known to be imperfect. For example, we found connectivity involving the posterior inferior parietal lobule (pIPL, a node in the medial temporal lobe subsystem) to be affected by AD independent of race. This may suggest pIPL to be a locus of early AD pathology, but neurofibrillary tangles do not appear in this region until later in AD [75].
For example, other than milder AD-related tau pathology, the selectivity of race for midline-dorsomedial connectivity could result from non-AD pathologies outside these two subsystems or neuro-protective changes along the tracts connecting two subsystems. Limited autopsy studies have shown African Americans more likely than Caucasians to have mixed AD and vascular lesions [76], and we previously showed in this cohort that African Americans experienced greater cognitive impact than Caucasians from the same degree of WMH [12]. In the current study, we did not find total WMH volume to be related to race and connectivity. However, the impact of regionally specific WMH has yet to be examined. The baseline differences in connectivity suggest existing differences in brain function separate from disease mechanisms that could be related to vascular disease, but the nature of these differences is not well understood, and the inclusion of vascular disease in our regression models did not alter our results. Although hypertension was more prevalent in our African American cohort and African Americans had elevated cardiovascular risk scores, including this in our analysiswhen we included this variable in our analyses, it did not explain the variability associated with race. Our identification of race-associated changes in midline-dorsomedial connectivity would support a search for WMH changes outside of these two subsystems. Alternatively, Caucasians may be more likely to have WMH between these two subsystems [77]. The vascular load in our cohort was mild to moderate, as it is not feasible, or ecologically valid to recruit older patients with minimal vascular disease. There are a variety of risk factors and contributing comorbidities for Alzheimer’s Disease. It is possible that the various risk factors associated with AD may be different across different ethnic groups, such that AAs may have an increased vascular component of AD, while exhibiting AD pathology sufficient to meet diagnostic thresholds. WMH and AD are not mutually exclusive, and many have stated that WMH are a core feature of AD [78] [79],, and a better predictor of disease burden in African Americans [80]. Future research will explore region-specific WMH between races and whether these differences relate to observed connectivity biomarker relationships.
It would be remissive to not explore social factors which may contribute to these biological disparities. The current work is the first to establish AD-related connectivity difference between races, and extends the neurobiological phenotype of AD in African Americans beyond a higher prevalence. How historical and current social inequalities may interact with genetic and environmental risks to give rise to these biological endpoints remains unknown. A variety of social disparities including income (amount vs. purchasing power), education (length vs. quality), and discrimination may additively or synergistically converge on the same biological endpoints. When analyzed separately, these factors may individually correlate with racial disparity but fail to capture the entire range of exposures facing different groups. For example, individuals who experience racial discrimination and perceive it as such are more likely to have higher blood pressure and increased psychological distress [81–83], which in turn are risk factors for AD [84]. Chronic stress also increases connectivity between the DMN and other networks at least in young adults [85], and may in part account for baseline and AD-related connectivity differences between the two racial groups. We did not include household income as a surrogate measure of lifelong socioeconomic status because the two measures poorly correlate in retired people, and the sample size limited our ability to interpret results when we introduced a measure such as the Area Deprivation Index [86]. A larger sample size will be necessary to test mediation effects between discrimination, stress, cardiovascular disease, and negative health outcomes, and cohort studies need to explore biologically meaningful methods to characterize individual and group-based experiences of injustice.
While we present the first biomarker-informed analysis of DMN inter-subsystem connectivity in African Americans, there are a number of limitations to our study. We tested two common AD risk genotypes as mediators for race-associated differences, but we did not perform extensive genomic association analysis because of sample size. While we observed multiple race-associated differences in DMN connectivity using ICA, we did not perform seed-based analysis of other large-scale brain networks (e.g., salience network). This cohort’s African American participants had similar years of education and socioeconomic status as their Caucasian counterparts, but other medical, psychiatric, or psychosocial differences could contribute to inter-subsystem connectivity differences. We did not identify a modifying effect of race on mean connectivity strength between the MTL and dorsomedial subsystem. Lastly, both racial groups include heterogeneous genetic backgrounds and in some cases mixed genetic heritage, so our results should be interpreted at the cohort level rather than the individual level. Nevertheless, we present additional evidence that AD is associated with systematic biomarker differences between older African Americans and Caucasians. Because CSF t-Tau-related findings similar to ours were replicated in a separate US cohort [87], independent replication of these DMN findings will further highlight the importance of diversity, inclusion, and disparities in on-going effort to elucidate mechanism-related biomarkers in AD.
Conclusions
We previously identified that African Americans and Caucasians share the same AD-associated CSF alterations related to amyloid deposition, but different CSF tau biomarker levels regardless of AD status [14]. Here we extend our findings to show older African Americans and Caucasians have similar AD-associated subsystem connectivity changes involving the medial temporal subsystems. However, we also demonstrate race-specific patterns of connectivity between the midline core and dorsomedial subsystems, that are in-line with current studies that suggest divergent tau relationships between races. We thus propose adding DMN connectivity to the list of biomarkers with race-dependent alterations in AD. Similar to CSF, rsfMRI profiles for AD established in pre-dominantly Caucasian cohorts may under-diagnose the disease when applied directly to African Americans, and negatively impact the interpretation of clinical trial outcomes when rsfMRI is used as surrogate marker of AD. The current work further provides specific regions of interest for imaging-based and molecular investigation of disease mechanisms.
Acknowledgements
The authors acknowledge the following persons for assistance with enrollment: James Lah, MD, PhD; Allan Levey, MD, PhD; Chadwick Hales, MD, PhD; Angela Ashley, MD; Felicia Goldstein, PhD; and Andrea Kippels, CNP.
Abbreviations
- AA
African American
- AD
Alzheimer’s Disease
- CSF
Cerebrospinal fluid
- MCI
Mild cognitive impairment
- MRI
Magnetic resonance imaging
- NC
Normal cognition
- NHW
Non-Hispanic White
- rsfMRI
Resting state functional MRI
- WMH
White matter hyperintensities
Authors’ contributions
MM, JAT, WTH, MWP, and DQ were responsible for the conception and design of the study. JCH, MWP, and WTH were responsible for acquisition of the data. MM, JAT, and WTH were responsible for analysis, and interpretation of data, drafting the manuscript and revising it critically for important intellectual content. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Funding
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health grants AG43885, AG42856, AG25688, and AG61660.
Availability of data and materials
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, upon reasonable request.
Ethics approval and consent to participate
This study was approved by Emory University’s institutional review board.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
WTH has a patent (assignee, Emory University) on the use of CSF p/t-Tau ratio in the evaluation of frontotemporal lobar degeneration, has received research support from Avid Pharmaceuticals, and has received travel support from Eli Lilly and Hoffman-La Roche. DQ has received research support from Medtronic and Siemens Healthcare.
Contributor Information
Maria B. Misiura, Email: mariamisiura@gmail.com
William T. Hu, Email: William.hu@emory.edu
References
- 1.Fitzpatrick AL, et al. Incidence and prevalence of dementia in the cardiovascular health study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2004;52:195–204. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52058.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Potter GG, et al. Cognitive performance and informant reports in the diagnosis of cognitive impairment and dementia in African Americans and whites. Alzheimers Dement. J. Alzheimers Assoc. 2009;5:445–453. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2009.04.1234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Barnes LL, Bennett DA. Alzheimer’s disease in African Americans: risk factors and challenges for the future. Health Aff Proj Hope. 2014;33:580–586. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.2013.1353. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Turner AD, James BD, Capuano AW, Aggarwal NT, Barnes LL. Perceived stress and cognitive decline in different cognitive domains in a cohort of older African Americans. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry Off J Am Assoc Geriatr Psychiatry. 2017;25:25–34. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2016.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Wilson RS, Capuano AW, Sytsma J, Bennett DA, Barnes LL. Cognitive aging in older black and white persons. Psychol Aging. 2015;30:279–285. doi: 10.1037/pag0000024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Hendrie HC, et al. APOE ε4 and the risk for Alzheimer disease and cognitive decline in African Americans and Yoruba. Int Psychogeriatr IPA. 2014;26:977–985. doi: 10.1017/S1041610214000167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Logue MW, et al. Two rare AKAP9 variants are associated with Alzheimer disease in African Americans. Alzheimers Dement. J Alzheimers Assoc. 2014;10:609–618.e11. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.06.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Hollingworth Paul, Harold Denise, Sims Rebecca, Gerrish Amy, Lambert Jean-Charles, Carrasquillo Minerva M, Abraham Richard, Hamshere Marian L, Pahwa Jaspreet Singh, Moskvina Valentina, Dowzell Kimberley, Jones Nicola, Stretton Alexandra, Thomas Charlene, Richards Alex, Ivanov Dobril, Widdowson Caroline, Chapman Jade, Lovestone Simon, Powell John, Proitsi Petroula, Lupton Michelle K, Brayne Carol, Rubinsztein David C, Gill Michael, Lawlor Brian, Lynch Aoibhinn, Brown Kristelle S, Passmore Peter A, Craig David, McGuinness Bernadette, Todd Stephen, Holmes Clive, Mann David, Smith A David, Beaumont Helen, Warden Donald, Wilcock Gordon, Love Seth, Kehoe Patrick G, Hooper Nigel M, Vardy Emma R L C, Hardy John, Mead Simon, Fox Nick C, Rossor Martin, Collinge John, Maier Wolfgang, Jessen Frank, Rüther Eckart, Schürmann Britta, Heun Reiner, Kölsch Heike, van den Bussche Hendrik, Heuser Isabella, Kornhuber Johannes, Wiltfang Jens, Dichgans Martin, Frölich Lutz, Hampel Harald, Gallacher John, Hüll Michael, Rujescu Dan, Giegling Ina, Goate Alison M, Kauwe John S K, Cruchaga Carlos, Nowotny Petra, Morris John C, Mayo Kevin, Sleegers Kristel, Bettens Karolien, Engelborghs Sebastiaan, De Deyn Peter P, Van Broeckhoven Christine, Livingston Gill, Bass Nicholas J, Gurling Hugh, McQuillin Andrew, Gwilliam Rhian, Deloukas Panagiotis, Al-Chalabi Ammar, Shaw Christopher E, Tsolaki Magda, Singleton Andrew B, Guerreiro Rita, Mühleisen Thomas W, Nöthen Markus M, Moebus Susanne, Jöckel Karl-Heinz, Klopp Norman, Wichmann H-Erich, Pankratz V Shane, Sando Sigrid B, Aasly Jan O, Barcikowska Maria, Wszolek Zbigniew K, Dickson Dennis W, Graff-Radford Neill R, Petersen Ronald C, van Duijn Cornelia M, Breteler Monique M B, Ikram M Arfan, DeStefano Anita L, Fitzpatrick Annette L, Lopez Oscar, Launer Lenore J, Seshadri Sudha, Berr Claudine, Campion Dominique, Epelbaum Jacques, Dartigues Jean-François, Tzourio Christophe, Alpérovitch Annick, Lathrop Mark, Feulner Thomas M, Friedrich Patricia, Riehle Caterina, Krawczak Michael, Schreiber Stefan, Mayhaus Manuel, Nicolhaus S, Wagenpfeil Stefan, Steinberg Stacy, Stefansson Hreinn, Stefansson Kari, Snædal Jon, Björnsson Sigurbjörn, Jonsson Palmi V, Chouraki Vincent, Genier-Boley Benjamin, Hiltunen Mikko, Soininen Hilkka, Combarros Onofre, Zelenika Diana, Delepine Marc, Bullido Maria J, Pasquier Florence, Mateo Ignacio, Frank-Garcia Ana, Porcellini Elisa, Hanon Olivier, Coto Eliecer, Alvarez Victoria, Bosco Paolo, Siciliano Gabriele, Mancuso Michelangelo, Panza Francesco, Solfrizzi Vincenzo, Nacmias Benedetta, Sorbi Sandro, Bossù Paola, Piccardi Paola, Arosio Beatrice, Annoni Giorgio, Seripa Davide, Pilotto Alberto, Scarpini Elio, Galimberti Daniela, Brice Alexis, Hannequin Didier, Licastro Federico, Jones Lesley, Holmans Peter A, Jonsson Thorlakur, Riemenschneider Matthias, Morgan Kevin, Younkin Steven G, Owen Michael J, O'Donovan Michael, Amouyel Philippe, Williams Julie. Common variants at ABCA7, MS4A6A/MS4A4E, EPHA1, CD33 and CD2AP are associated with Alzheimer's disease. Nature Genetics. 2011;43(5):429–435. doi: 10.1038/ng.803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Mez J, et al. Dysexecutive versus amnestic Alzheimer’s disease subgroups: analysis of demographic, genetic, and vascular factors. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2013;27:218–225. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e31826a94bd. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Beach TG, Monsell SE, Phillips LE, Kukull W. Accuracy of the clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer disease at National Institute on Aging Alzheimer disease centers, 2005–2010. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2012;71:266–273. doi: 10.1097/NEN.0b013e31824b211b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Bonner Gloria J., Darkwa Osei K., Gorelick Philip B. Autopsy Recruitment Program for African Americans. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders. 2000;14(4):202–208. doi: 10.1097/00002093-200010000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Howell JC, et al. Race modifies the relationship between cognition and Alzheimer’s disease cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2017;9:88. doi: 10.1186/s13195-017-0315-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Ozturk T, et al. Linked CSF reduction of phosphorylated tau and IL-8 in HIV associated neurocognitive disorder. Sci Rep. 2019;9:8733. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-45418-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Wharton Whitney, Kollhoff Alexander L., Gangishetti Umesh, Verble Danielle D., Upadhya Samsara, Zetterberg Henrik, Kumar Veena, Watts Kelly D., Kippels Andrea J., Gearing Marla, Howell J. Christina, Parker Monica W., Hu William T. Interleukin 9 alterations linked to alzheimer disease in african americans. Annals of Neurology. 2019;86(3):407–418. doi: 10.1002/ana.25543. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Beason-Held LL, et al. Changes in brain function occur years before the onset of cognitive impairment. J Neurosci. 2013;33:18008–18014. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1402-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Damoiseaux JS, Prater KE, Miller BL, Greicius MD. Functional connectivity tracks clinical deterioration in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging. 2012;33:828.e19–828.e8.28E30. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.06.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Brier MR, et al. Loss of Intranetwork and internetwork resting state functional connections with Alzheimer’s disease progression. J Neurosci. 2012;32:8890. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5698-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Damoiseaux JS, et al. Reduced resting-state brain activity in the “default network” in normal aging. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18:1856–1864. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Greicius MD, Srivastava G, Reiss AL, Menon V. Default-mode network activity distinguishes Alzheimer’s disease from healthy aging: evidence from functional MRI. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101:4637–4642. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0308627101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Hafkemeijer A, van der Grond J, Rombouts SARB. Imaging the default mode network in aging and dementia. Imaging Brain Aging Neurodegener Dis. 2012;1822:431–441. doi: 10.1016/j.bbadis.2011.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.He X, et al. Abnormal salience network in normal aging and in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35:3446–3464. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Simioni AC, Dagher A, Fellows LK. Compensatory striatal–cerebellar connectivity in mild–moderate Parkinson’s disease. NeuroImage Clin. 2016;10:54–62. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Uddin Lucina Q., Clare Kelly A.M., Biswal Bharat B., Xavier Castellanos F., Milham Michael P. Functional connectivity of default mode network components: Correlation, anticorrelation, and causality. Human Brain Mapping. 2008;30(2):625–637. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20531. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Braak H, Braak E. Frequency of stages of Alzheimer-related lesions in different age categories. Neurobiol Aging. 1997;18:351–357. doi: 10.1016/S0197-4580(97)00056-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Palmqvist S, Schöll M, Strandberg O, et al. Earliest accumulation of β-amyloid occurs within the default-mode network and concurrently affects brain connectivity. Nat Commun. 2017;8:1214. 10.1038/s41467-017-01150-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 26.Seemiller J, Tahmasian M, Yakushev I, Drzezga A. In vivo tau-PET indicates expansion of tau pathology within the default mode network in Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement J Alzheimers Assoc. 2015;11:P109–P110. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2015.06.186. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Badhwar A, et al. Resting-state network dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Alzheimers Dement Amst Neth. 2017;8:73–85. doi: 10.1016/j.dadm.2017.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Qi H, Liu H, Hu H, He H, Zhao X. Primary disruption of the memory-related subsystems of the default mode network in Alzheimer’s disease: resting-state functional connectivity MRI study. Front Aging Neurosci. 2018;10:344. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00344. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Zhu DC, Majumdar S, Korolev IO, Berger KL, Bozoki AC. Alzheimer’s disease and amnestic mild cognitive impairment weaken connections within the default-mode network: a multi-modal imaging study. J Alzheimers Dis. 2013;34:969–984. doi: 10.3233/JAD-121879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Grieder M, Wang DJJ, Dierks T, Wahlund L-O, Jann K. Default mode network complexity and cognitive decline in mild Alzheimer’s disease. Front Neurosci. 2018;12:770. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00770. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Nuttall R, Pasquini L, Scherr M, Sorg C. Degradation in intrinsic connectivity networks across the Alzheimer’s disease spectrum. Alzheimers Dement Amst Neth. 2016;5:35–42. doi: 10.1016/j.dadm.2016.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Qi Z, et al. Impairment and compensation coexist in amnestic MCI default mode network. NeuroImage. 2010;50:48–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Dickerson BC, et al. Increased hippocampal activation in mild cognitive impairment compared to normal aging and AD. Neurology. 2005;65:404–411. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000171450.97464.49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Fleisher AS, et al. Resting-state BOLD networks versus task-associated functional MRI for distinguishing Alzheimer’s disease risk groups. NeuroImage. 2009;47:1678–1690. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Filippini N, et al. Distinct patterns of brain activity in young carriers of the APOE-epsilon4 allele. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106:7209–7214. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811879106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Andrews-Hanna JR, Reidler JS, Sepulcre J, Poulin R, Buckner RL. Functional-anatomic fractionation of the Brain’s default network. Neuron. 2010;65:550–562. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Andrews-Hanna JR, Smallwood J, Spreng RN. The default network and self-generated thought: component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2014;1316:29–52. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Gusnard DA, Akbudak E, Shulman GL, Raichle ME. Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98:4259–4264. doi: 10.1073/pnas.071043098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Vipin A, et al. Cerebrovascular disease influences functional and structural network connectivity in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2018;10:82. doi: 10.1186/s13195-018-0413-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Bai F, et al. Abnormal whole-brain functional connection in amnestic mild cognitive impairment patients. Behav Brain Res. 2011;216:666–672. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Busse A, Angermeyer MC, Riedel-Heller SG. Progression of mild cognitive impairment to dementia: a challenge to current thinking. Br J Psychiatry. 2006;189:399–404. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.105.014779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Allegri RF, Glaser FB, Taragano FE, Buschke H. Mild cognitive impairment: believe it or not? Int Rev Psychiatry. 2008;20:357–363. doi: 10.1080/09540260802095099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Gamaldo AA, Allaire JC, Sims RC, Whitfield KE. Assessing mild cognitive impairment among older African Americans. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010;25:748–755. doi: 10.1002/gps.2417. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Wang L, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid Aβ42, phosphorylated Tau181, and resting-state functional ConnectivityCSF Aβ42, Tau181, and resting-state ConnectivityCSF Aβ42, Tau181, and resting-state connectivity. JAMA Neurol. 2013;70:1242–1248. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2013.2934. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Li K, et al. Aberrant functional connectivity network in subjective memory complaint individuals relates to pathological biomarkers. Transl Neurodegener. 2018;7:27. doi: 10.1186/s40035-018-0130-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Hu William T., Watts Kelly D., Shaw Leslie M., Howell Jennifer C., Trojanowski John Q., Basra Sundeep, Glass Jonathan D., Lah James J., Levey Allan I. CSF beta-amyloid 1-42 - what are we measuring in Alzheimer's disease? Annals of Clinical and Translational Neurology. 2014;2(2):131–139. doi: 10.1002/acn3.160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Yan C, Zang Y. DPARSF: a MATLAB toolbox for ‘pipeline’ data analysis of resting-state fMRI. Front Syst Neurosci. 2010;4:13. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Wang B, et al. Decreased complexity in Alzheimer’s disease: resting-state fMRI evidence of brain entropy mapping. Front Aging Neurosci. 2017;9:378. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00378. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Yan C-G, et al. A comprehensive assessment of regional variation in the impact of head micromovements on functional Connectomics. NeuroImage. 2013;76:183–201. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Murphy K, Birn RM, Handwerker DA, Jones TB, Bandettini PA. The impact of global signal regression on resting state correlations: are anti-correlated networks introduced? NeuroImage. 2009;44:893–905. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Ashburner J. A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. NeuroImage. 2007;38:95–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage. 2012;59:2142–2154. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Calhoun VD, Adali T, Pearlson GD, Pekar JJ. A method for making group inferences from functional MRI data using independent component analysis. Hum Brain Mapp. 2001;14:140–151. doi: 10.1002/hbm.1048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Calhoun VD, Liu J, Adali T. A review of group ICA for fMRI data and ICA for joint inference of imaging, genetic, and ERP data. NeuroImage. 2009;45:S163–S172. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Dipasquale O, et al. High-dimensional ICA analysis detects within-network functional connectivity damage of default-mode and sensory-motor networks in Alzheimer’s disease. Front Hum Neurosci. 2015;9:43. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Yu Q, et al. Comparing brain graphs in which nodes are regions of interest or independent components: a simulation study. J Neurosci Methods. 2017;291:61–68. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2017.08.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Calhoun VD, De Lacy N. Ten Key observations on the analysis of resting-state functional MR imaging data using independent component analysis. Neuroimaging Clin N Am. 2017;27:561–579. doi: 10.1016/j.nic.2017.06.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Allen EA, et al. A baseline for the multivariate comparison of resting-state networks. Front Syst Neurosci. 2011;5:2. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.R: A language and environment for statistical computing. (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2015).
- 60.Benjamini Yoav, Hochberg Yosef. Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological) 1995;57(1):289–300. [Google Scholar]
- 61.Palmqvist S, Mattsson N, Hansson O. & Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative Cerebrospinal fluid analysis detects cerebral amyloid-β accumulation earlier than positron emission tomography. Brain J Neurol. 2016;139:1226–1236. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Fletcher E, et al. Staging of amyloid β, t-tau, regional atrophy rates, and cognitive change in a nondemented cohort: results of serial mediation analyses. Alzheimers Dement. Amst. Neth. 2018;10:382–393. doi: 10.1016/j.dadm.2018.04.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Canty, Angelo & Ripley, Brian. boot: Bootstrap R (S-Plus) Functions. (2017).
- 64.Raschka, S. Model Evaluation, Model Selection, and Algorithm Selection in Machine Learning. ArXiv181112808 Cs Stat (2018).
- 65.Kenny ER, Blamire AM, Firbank MJ, O’Brien JT. Functional connectivity in cortical regions in dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2012;135:569–581. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Wang L, et al. Changes in hippocampal connectivity in the early stages of Alzheimer’s disease: evidence from resting state fMRI. NeuroImage. 2006;31:496–504. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Zhu X, Zhu Q, Shen H, Liao W, Yuan F. Rumination and default mode network subsystems connectivity in first-episode, Drug-Naive Young Patients with Major Depressive Disorder. Sci Rep. 2017;7:43105. doi: 10.1038/srep43105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Du Y, et al. Interaction among subsystems within default mode network diminished in schizophrenia patients: a dynamic connectivity approach. Schizophr Res. 2016;170:55–65. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.11.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Blokland Gabriëlla A.M., del Re Elisabetta C., Mesholam-Gately Raquelle I., Jovicich Jorge, Trampush Joey W., Keshavan Matcheri S., DeLisi Lynn E., Walters James T.R., Turner Jessica A., Malhotra Anil K., Lencz Todd, Shenton Martha E., Voineskos Aristotle N., Rujescu Dan, Giegling Ina, Kahn René S., Roffman Joshua L., Holt Daphne J., Ehrlich Stefan, Kikinis Zora, Dazzan Paola, Murray Robin M., Di Forti Marta, Lee Jimmy, Sim Kang, Lam Max, Wolthusen Rick P.F., de Zwarte Sonja M.C., Walton Esther, Cosgrove Donna, Kelly Sinead, Maleki Nasim, Osiecki Lisa, Picchioni Marco M., Bramon Elvira, Russo Manuela, David Anthony S., Mondelli Valeria, Reinders Antje A.T.S., Falcone M. Aurora, Hartmann Annette M., Konte Bettina, Morris Derek W., Gill Michael, Corvin Aiden P., Cahn Wiepke, Ho New Fei, Liu Jian Jun, Keefe Richard S.E., Gollub Randy L., Manoach Dara S., Calhoun Vince D., Schulz S. Charles, Sponheim Scott R., Goff Donald C., Buka Stephen L., Cherkerzian Sara, Thermenos Heidi W., Kubicki Marek, Nestor Paul G., Dickie Erin W., Vassos Evangelos, Ciufolini Simone, Reis Marques Tiago, Crossley Nicolas A., Purcell Shaun M., Smoller Jordan W., van Haren Neeltje E.M., Toulopoulou Timothea, Donohoe Gary, Goldstein Jill M., Seidman Larry J., McCarley Robert W., Petryshen Tracey L. The Genetics of Endophenotypes of Neurofunction to Understand Schizophrenia (GENUS) consortium: A collaborative cognitive and neuroimaging genetics project. Schizophrenia Research. 2018;195:306–317. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.09.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Alexander AC, et al. Racial differences in posttraumatic stress disorder vulnerability following hurricane Katrina among a sample of adult cigarette smokers from New Orleans. J Racial Ethn Health Disparities. 2017;4:94–103. doi: 10.1007/s40615-015-0206-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Hird Megan, Churchill Nathan, Fischer Corinne, Naglie Gary, Graham Simon, Schweizer Tom. Altered Functional Brain Connectivity in Mild Cognitive Impairment during a Cognitively Complex Car Following Task. Geriatrics. 2018;3(2):20. doi: 10.3390/geriatrics3020020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Hillary FG, et al. Hyperconnectivity is a fundamental response to neurological disruption. Neuropsychology. 2015;29:59–75. doi: 10.1037/neu0000110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Wang P, Zhou B, Yao H, et al. Aberrant intra- and inter-network connectivity architectures in Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment. Sci Rep. 2015;5:14824. 10.1038/srep14824. Published 6 Oct 2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 74.Miles TP, Froehlich TE, Bogardus ST, Inouye SK. Dementia and race: are there differences between African Americans and Caucasians? J Am Geriatr Soc. 2001;49:477–484. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2001.49096.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Braak H, Del Tredici K. Spreading of tau pathology in sporadic Alzheimer’s disease along Cortico-cortical top-down connections. Cereb Cortex N Y N. 2018;1991(28):3372–3384. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Meier IB, et al. White matter predictors of cognitive functioning in older adults. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2012;18:414–427. doi: 10.1017/S1355617712000227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Sun Y, et al. Abnormal functional connectivity in patients with vascular cognitive impairment, no dementia: a resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Behav Brain Res. 2011;223:388–394. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Alosco ML, et al. A Clinicopathological investigation of white matter Hyperintensities and Alzheimer’s disease neuropathology. J Alzheimers Dis JAD. 2018;63:1347–1360. doi: 10.3233/JAD-180017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Brickman AM, et al. Regional white matter hyperintensity volume, not hippocampal atrophy, predicts incident Alzheimer’s disease in the community. Arch Neurol. 2012;69:1621–1627. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2012.1527. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Zahodne LB, et al. Structural MRI predictors of late-life cognition differ across African Americans, Hispanics, and whites. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2015;12:632–639. doi: 10.2174/1567205012666150530203214. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Krieger N, Sidney S. Racial discrimination and blood pressure: the CARDIA study of young black and white adults. Am J Public Health. 1996;86:1370–1378. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.86.10.1370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Sawyer PJ, Major B, Casad BJ, Townsend SSM, Mendes WB. Discrimination and the stress response: psychological and physiological consequences of anticipating prejudice in interethnic interactions. Am J Public Health. 2012;102:1020–1026. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2011.300620. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Berger M, Sarnyai Z. “More than skin deep”: stress neurobiology and mental health consequences of racial discrimination. Stress. 2015;18:1–10. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2014.989204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Bisht K, Sharma K, Tremblay M-È. Chronic stress as a risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease: roles of microglia-mediated synaptic remodeling, inflammation, and oxidative stress. Neurobiol Stress. 2018;9:9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2018.05.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Soares JM, et al. Stress impact on resting state brain networks. PLoS One. 2013;8:e66500. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0066500. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Kind AJH, Buckingham WR. Making neighborhood-disadvantage metrics accessible — the neighborhood atlas. N Engl J Med. 2018;378:2456–2458. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp1802313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Morris JC, et al. Assessment of racial disparities in biomarkers for Alzheimer DiseaseAssessment of racial disparities in biomarkers for Alzheimer DiseaseAssessment of racial disparities in biomarkers for Alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurol. 2019;76:264–273. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2018.4249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, upon reasonable request.