Abstract
National culture has been overlooked in discussions related to research productivity and impact owing to individual, socio-political structure, and economic factors. This study shows the relationships between the dimensions of cultural value orientation of the nation and research performance indicators. More than 60 countries were included and Pearson correlation analysis was employed. The variables were taken from Geert Hofstede and Scimago Journal & Country Rank worksheets. This study found that (1) Individualism has significant correlations with the majority of the indicators; (2) Power distance and indulgence correlate with a country’s research impact in the form of citation per document; (3) Masculinity, long term orientation, and uncertainty avoidance do not correlate with the indicators. Owing to the fact that the national culture is relatively enduring, countries need to measure their elasticity of hopes and action plans in an effort to boost research productivity and impact, by integrating the national culture in the estimate.
Keywords: research impact, research productivity, national culture, individualism, indulgence, power distance, citations per document, self citations
Introduction
Makri (2018) recently released a report on the increasing number of publications in various countries. She stated that it’s unclear what has triggered and driven the strong gains in Egypt and Pakistan. Throughout the report, various variables believed to be responsible for the increasing number of publications, such as indexation duration, funding, global engagement, international collaboration, and political policies on science and higher education, are explained.
Several predictors of research productivity and impact had been identified, i.e. author characteristics, co-authorship networks, citation history, journal impact factors, twits ( Xiaomei et al., 2017), cohort effects (in terms of scientific discipline), age, career stages, gender, the country of origin of the PhD holders, and reward structure of the research enactment ( Claudia & Francisco, 2007). They are mostly at the individual and institutional level. At the country level, the predictors are the number of universities, GDP per capita, control of corruption, civil liberties ( Mueller et al., 2016), country’s wealth and population size, country’s value of research tradition, tenure and promotion criterion, experimental costs, IRB (Institutional Review Boards) review flexibility, language barrier, and the training of new young researchers ( Demaria, 2009).
However, national cultural orientation is yet to be analyzed, with the present study assuming that individual, institutional, and structural factors are also influenced by the cultural values of a nation. Hofstede Insights (2019) defined culture as the collective mental programming of the human mind which distinguishes one group of people from another, consisting of six dimensions, i.e. (1) power distance (PD) – acceptance on the unequal power distribution in a society; (2) uncertainty avoidance (UA) – intolerance of ambiguity and uncustomary thoughts and practices; (3) individualism (IND) – projection of individuals’ “I” in a society rather than “we” (collectivism); (4) masculinity (MAS) – the toughness and competitiveness rather than the tenderness and cooperativeness (femininity) orientation; (5) long term orientation (LTO) – the society’s preference of time-honored rather than pragmatic approaches (short term normative orientation); and (6) indulgence (IVR) – the society facilitation towards a fun and enjoyable life rather than restraint (suppression of needs gratification by strict social norms).
National culture is relatively stable ( Maseland & van Hoorn, 2017) and is widely used to explain various performances at the country level, such as learning and academic performance ( Signorini et al., 2009). The present study hypothesized that there are correlations between the national culture dimensions and research performance indicators. The research performance is assumed to be mediated by research culture, and the culture experiences stimulations and challenges from the national culture; as happened in China, the bureaucratic (high power distance) and nepotistic culture suppresses an innovative and meritocratic research culture ( Shi & Rao, 2010).
Methods
All following data were retrieved on December 18, 2018 and compiled into a worksheet (see Underlying data ( Abraham, 2019)) as the material of this present analysis. Countries’ research impact (citations per document/CPD, citations, self-citations) and productivity (total documents) were obtained from the Scimago Journal & Country Rank ( https://www.scimagojr.com/countryrank.php?out=xls), while national cultural orientations ( pdi=power distance, idv=individualism, mas=masculinity, uai=uncertainty avoidance, ltowvs=long term orientation, ivr=indulgence) were acquired from Geert Hofstede web site ( https://geerthofstede.com/wp-content/uploads/2016/08/6-dimensions-for-website-2015-08-16.xls). Pearson product-moment correlation analysis was conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics version 20 for Windows.
Results
The descriptive and correlational statistics are presented in Table 1 and Table 2. As shown, among 68 countries, IND is positively correlated with CPD ( r=0.506, p<0.01), total documents ( r=0.351, p<0.01), citations ( r=0.405, p<0.01), and self-citations ( r=0.304, p<0.05). MAS, UA, and LTO do not correlate with these four. PD ( r=-0.555, p<0.01, N=68) and IVR ( r=0.480, p<0.01, N=91) correlate with CPD. Total documents, citations, and self-citations correlate with each other with r>0.90, p<0.01, N=239. CPD is positively, but weakly, correlated with total documents, total citations and total self-citations, with r<0.20, p<0.05, N=239.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of research impact (CPD, CIT, SELF) and productivity (DOC) as well as national culture dimensions (PD, IND, MAS, UA, LTO, IVR).
M | SD | N | |
---|---|---|---|
DOC | 204893.92 | 878342.00 | 239 |
CIT | 3558474.20 | 19007535.44 | 239 |
SELF | 1076044.63 | 8178012.17 | 239 |
CPD | 13.35 | 6.15 | 239 |
PD | 59.12 | 22.02 | 68 |
IND | 43.85 | 24.16 | 68 |
MAS | 48.60 | 19.96 | 68 |
UA | 67.13 | 23.26 | 68 |
LTO | 46.05 | 24.30 | 90 |
IVR | 45.41 | 22.56 | 91 |
DOC = Total documents; CIT = Total citations; SELF = Total self-citations; CPD = Citations per document; PD = Power distance; IND = Individualism; MAS = Masculinity; UA = Uncertainty avoidance; LTO = Long term orientation; IVR = Indulgence
Table 2. Correlations results between national cultures dimensions and research impact and productivity.
DOC | CIT | SELF | CPD | PD | IND | MAS | UA | LTO | IVR | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
DOC | r | 1 | 0.958 ** | 0.933 ** | 0.153 * | -0.144 | 0.351 ** | .213 | -0.194 | 0.124 | 0.088 |
p | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.018 | 0.241 | 0.003 | 0.082 | 0.113 | 0.246 | 0.405 | ||
N | 239 | 239 | 239 | 239 | 68 | 68 | 68 | 68 | 90 | 91 | |
CIT | r | 1 | 0.977 ** | 0.196 ** | -0.209 | 0.405 ** | 0.165 | -0.174 | 0.040 | 0.146 | |
p | 0.000 | 0.002 | 0.088 | 0.001 | 0.178 | 0.156 | 0.710 | 0.168 | |||
N | 239 | 239 | 239 | 68 | 68 | 68 | 68 | 90 | 91 | ||
SELF | r | 1 | 0.142 * | -0.135 | 0.304 * | 0.146 | -0.157 | -0.004 | 0.107 | ||
p | 0.028 | 0.273 | 0.012 | 0.235 | 0.200 | 0.970 | 0.311 | ||||
N | 239 | 239 | 68 | 68 | 68 | 68 | 90 | 91 | |||
CPD | r | 1 | -.555 ** | 0.506 ** | -0.084 | -0.185 | -0.084 | 0.480 ** | |||
p | .000 | 0.000 | 0.494 | 0.130 | 0.430 | 0.000 | |||||
N | 239 | 68 | 68 | 68 | 68 | 90 | 91 |
DOC = Total documents; CIT = Total citations; SELF = Total self-citations; CPD = Citations per document; PD = Power distance; IND = Individualism; MAS = Masculinity; UA = Uncertainty avoidance; LTO = Long term orientation; IVR = Indulgence
**. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).
*. Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).
Among 54 countries, even after controlling the Log GDP per capita (taken from the World Happiness Report), the correlations between IND and CPD ( r=0.439, p<0.01), total documents ( r=0.268, p<0.05), and citations ( r=0.320, p<0.05), between PD and CPD ( r=-0.504, p<0.01), as well as between IVR and CPD ( r=0.411, p<0.01) persist (see Table 3).
Table 3. Partial correlations results between national cultures dimensions and research impact and productivity, controlling Log GDP per capita (LGDP).
Control Variables | DOC | CIT | SELF | CPD | PD | IND | IVR | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
LGDP | DOC | r | 1.000 | 0.954 | 0.940 | 0.218 | -0.019 | 0.268 | 0.031 |
p | . | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.106 | 0.887 | 0.046 | 0.819 | ||
N | 0 | 54 | 54 | 54 | 54 | 54 | 54 | ||
CIT | r | 1.000 | 0.984 | 0.303 | -0.083 | 0.320 | 0.099 | ||
p | . | 0.000 | 0.023 | 0.541 | 0.016 | 0.468 | |||
N | 0 | 54 | 54 | 54 | 54 | 54 | |||
SELF | r | 1.000 | 0.222 | -0.047 | 0.253 | 0.079 | |||
p | . | 0.100 | 0.730 | 0.060 | 0.564 | ||||
N | 0 | 54 | 54 | 54 | 54 | ||||
CPD | r | 1.000 | -0.504 | 0.439 | 0.411 | ||||
p | . | 0.000 | 0.001 | 0.002 | |||||
N | 0 | 54 | 54 | 54 |
DOC = Total documents; CIT = Total citations; SELF = Total self-citations; CPD = Citations per document; PD = Power distance; IND = Individualism; MAS = Masculinity; UA = Uncertainty avoidance; LTO = Long term orientation; IVR = Indulgence
Discussion
The positive correlations between IND and CPD, total documents, citations, and self-citations could be explained using the findings of Deschacht & Maes (2017). They found that in countries with more individualistic cultures: (1) the scientists prioritize their self-development, (2) the records of scientific work are historically longer (usually Western countries), and (2) self-citations flourish more. This does not necessarily mean that there have been citation abuses, but that self-citation is used to refer to their prior works, thereby, preventing unnecessary repetitions of ideas in newer works ( Deschacht, 2017). Although IND and collaboration are often contested (e.g. Kemp, 2013), a “collaborative individualism” ( Limerick & Cunnington, 1993) – stressing both working together and self-emancipation – is possible, explaining the positive correlation.
PD has a negative correlation with CPD. In this case, PD might manifest itself in academic writing in the form of rigid, authoritative, defensive, and dogmatic styles ( Koutsantoni, 2005). In addition, PD negatively correlates with democracy ( Maleki & Hendriks, 2014). The lower level of democracy reduces the opportunity of the academic community to exchange and market (in the broad sense) scientific information, as well as debate openly. Likewise, democracy that does not flourish deters the use of research results in creating public policies. In addition, science is co-opted or used as just a tool to achieve exclusive interests by ideologues, pundits and political leaders; they ignore the state-of-the-art nature of the research ( Branscomb & Rosenberg, 2012). All the conditions could reduce CPD.
IVR is positively correlated with CPD; this may be because IVR facilitates academic freedom ( Ohmann, 2011) and manifests itself in a “lovely” academic writing style ( Kiriakos & Tienari, 2018). This style is not dry and cold, but rather dialogical, humanistic, more reflexive, and capable of showing authors’ courage and vulnerability. Compelling insights are more easily born from the writings that embody those qualities; as mentioned, “a thin line exists between interesting insights and self-indulgence” ( Nadin & Cassell, 2006, p. 214). Scientific authors who read such works would be attracted to cite them, leading to an increase in the works’ CPD. In addition, “strategic indulgence” is possible and known to be a creative process that enables one to balance academic activity (such as writing) with non-academic ones ( Jia et al., 2018) – fostering insight.
The weak correlation between CPD and other research performance indicators shows that CPD is more difficult to manipulate or be an object of the author’s “engineering”.
Conclusion
National culture dimensions, especially individualism, power distance, and indulgence, are pivotal variables that are to be considered in justifying research impact and productivity. National culture can be integrated as a moderating variable in the predictive relationship between GDP per capita and research impact and productivity. Diversification of this study – based on the document and authors’ collaboration types, the indexing databases, the disciplines, the open science practices, as well as the history and development of the research in a country – is a future opportunity for further study.
Data availability
Source data
Geert Hofstede: Dimension data matrix. https://geerthofstede.com/research-and-vsm/dimension-data-matrix/ ( Hofstede et al., 2010)
Scimago Journal & Country Rank: Download data. https://www.scimagojr.com/countryrank.php?out=xls ( Scimago Lab, 2018)
World Happiness Report 2018: Chapter 2: online data. https://s3.amazonaws.com/happiness-report/2018/WHR2018Chapter2OnlineData.xls ( Helliwell et al., 2018)
All source data was accessed and retrieved on the 18/12/2018
Underlying data
Figshare: National culture, research performance indicators, and log GDP Per capita. https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.7723211 ( Abraham, 2019)
Data are available under the terms of the Creative Commons Zero "No rights reserved" data waiver (CC0 1.0 Public domain dedication).
Funding Statement
The author(s) declared that no grants were involved in supporting this work.
[version 1; peer review: 2 approved with reservations]
References
- Abraham J: National culture, research performance indicators, and log GDP per capita. [Data set]. Figshare. 2019. 10.6084/m9.figshare.7723211 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Branscomb LM, Rosenberg AA: Science and democracy.2012. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
- Claudia GB, Francisco MV: The determinants of research output and impact: A study of Mexican researchers. Res Policy. 2007;36(7):1035–1051. 10.1016/j.respol.2007.03.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Demaria A: Research productivity among nations. J Am Coll Cardiol. 2009;54(25):2460–2462. 10.1016/j.jacc.2009.11.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deschacht N, Maes B: Cross-cultural differences in self-promotion: A study of self-citations in management journals. J Occup Organ Psychol. 2017;90(1):77–94. 10.1111/joop.12162 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Deschacht N: Me, myself, and I: self-citation rates are higher in individualist cultures than in collectivist cultures.2017. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
- Helliwell J, Layard R, Sachs J: World Happiness Report 2018. Sustainable Development Solutions Network, New York,2018. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
- Hofstede G, Hofstede GJ, Minkov M: Cultures and Organizations: Software of The Mind. McGraw Hill, New York,2010. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
- Hofstede Insights: National culture.2019. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
- Jia L, Hirt ER, Koh AH: How to have your cake and eat it too: strategic indulgence in big-time collegiate sports among academically successful students. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2018. 10.1177/1948550618789403 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kemp AT: Collaboration vs. individualism: what is better for the rising academic? Qual Rep. 2013;18(50):1–8. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
- Kiriakos CM, Tienari J: Academic writing as love. Manag Learn. 2018;49(3):263–277. 10.1177/1350507617753560 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Koutsantoni D: Greek cultural characteristics and academic writing. J Mod Greek Stud. 2005;23(1):97–138. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
- Limerick D, Cunnington B: Collaborative Individualism and The End of The Corporate Citizen. In: Limerick D, Cunnington B (eds). Managing The New Organisation Chapter 4. Business and Professional Publishing, Chatswood,1993. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
- Makri A: Pakistan and Egypt had highest rises in research output in 2018. Nature. 2018. 10.1038/d41586-018-07841-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Maleki A, Hendriks F: The relation between cultural values and models of democracy: a cross-national study. Democratization. 2014;22(6):981–1010. 10.1080/13510347.2014.893426 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Maseland R, van Hoorn A: Culture at the country level. In: van Herk H, Torelli CJ (eds.). Cross Cultural Issues in Consumer Science and Consumer Psychology Springer, New York,2017;7–32. 10.1007/978-3-319-65091-3_2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mueller CE, Gaus H, Konradt I: Predicting research productivity in international evaluation journals across countries. J Multidiscip Eval. 2016;12(27):79–92. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
- Nadin S, Cassell C: The use of a research diary as a tool for reflexive practice: Some reflections from management research. Qualitative Research in Accounting & Management. 2006;3(3):208–217. 10.1108/11766090610705407 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ohmann R: Academic freedom’s best days. Inside Higher Ed. 2011. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
- Scimago Lab: Scimago journal & country rank. [Data set].2018. https://www.scimagojr.com/countryrank.php [Google Scholar]
- Shi Y, Rao Y: China’s research culture. Science. 2010;329(5996):1128. 10.1126/science.1196916 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Signorini P, Wiesemes R, Murphy R: Developing alternative frameworks for exploring intercultural learning: a critique of Hofstede's cultural difference model. Teach High Educ. 2009;14(3):253–264. 10.1080/13562510902898825 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Xiaomei B, Hui L, Fuli Z, et al. : An overview on evaluating and predicting scholarly article impact. Information. 2017;8(3):73 10.3390/info8030073 [DOI] [Google Scholar]