Abstract
Finger tapping is sensitive to motor slowing and emerging symptoms in individuals at Clinical High-Risk for psychosis (CHR). A sensitive, computerized finger tapping task would be beneficial in early psychosis screening batteries. The study included 41 CHR and 32 healthy volunteers, who completed a computerized finger tapping task and clinical interviews. This computerized finger tapping task was sensitive to slowing in the CHR group compared to healthy volunteers, and as expected negative but not positive symptoms related to motor slowing. Computerized finger tapping tasks may be an easily dispersible tool for early symptom detection battery relevant to emerging negative symptoms.
Introduction
Schizophrenia (SZ) is a major cause of disability worldwide, and in spite of existing treatment, patients continue to demonstrate significant disability [1, 2], which may be related to the duration of untreated psychosis [3, 4]. As a result, early detection and intervention in individuals at clinical high-risk (CHR) is critical for early intervention in order to improve the trajectory of psychosis progression. However, extant CHR screenings require clinical training and are impractical for widespread dissemination of CHR detection methods. Advancement and validation of online neuropsychological batteries, that engage mechanisms underlying emerging symptoms, may be a beneficial tool in CHR screening in clinical practice.
The finger tapping task is a well-established neuropsychological measure of timing and motor deficits that has been validated for online use [5, 6]. Critically, patients with psychosis show motor slowing [7, 8] that appears early in the course of psychosis development [9–17]. This motor slowing in psychosis may be related to coordination among basal ganglia, cerebellar, and cortico-motor circuits [18–20], which is disrupted across disease stage [10, 21–24]. The extant literature demonstrates that motor abnormalities are predictive of future conversion to psychotic disorders [25] and future symptom progression [26–28]. Relatedly, there is emerging evidence that speaks to an important role for slowing in CHR individuals [11] and strong links with negative symptoms in schizophrenia [29–31]. These clinical features make motor abnormalities a great candidate for early risk detection. Despite this potential for clinical utility, typical motor assessment approaches require trained administrators or expensive equipment that is not easily distributable, which limits the utility of motor slowing metrics in clinical practice. The present study examines the possibility that online finger tapping tasks may be sensitive to risk for psychosis, which would provide a time and cost-efficient measure that is easily distributed for clinical use.
Methods
Participants
A total of 73 participants (59% female; CHR=41, healthy volunteers (HV)=32) age 15–24 (M=20.66, SD=2.14) completed a computerized neuropsychiatry test battery and clinical interviews. All participants provided informed consent and data was collected in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments. The majority of the sample was (90.4%) right-handed. All subjects were administered the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I disorders (SCID) to rule out a psychosis diagnosis and assess history of other disorders [32]. The Structured Interview Structured Interview for Psychosis-Risk Syndromes (SIPRS) was administered to diagnose CHR subjects and rule out symptoms in HV [33]. Medication with antipsychotics was an exclusionary criterion as antipsychotics have been related to motor slowing [34], and so none of the current sample of CHR subjects were medicated with antipsychotics.
Finger Tapping Task
Participants completed the Computerized Finger-tapping Test (CTAP) [5, 35] from the Penn Computerized Neurocognitive Battery (CNB). They were instructed to press the spacebar as quickly as possible moving only the index finger for 10 seconds after their first button press. During the task, trial blocks alternate between the dominant and non-dominant hand over a total of 10 trials (5 trials per hand). Mean finger tapping behavior will be compared across groups; if there are group differences in finger tapping, then potential relationship to symptoms will be explored in a single model. These analyses were completed using SPSS 25.0.
Results
Participants
There were not significant differences in sex (CHR: 22F/19M; HV: 21F/11M), χ2=1.06, p=.30, or handedness χ2=0.56, p=.46, by group. There was not a significant difference in age between groups, t(71)=0.22, p=.82, and age did not significantly relate to finger tapping performance, F(1,71)=1.69, p=.20. As expected, the CHR group showed significantly higher total positive symptoms, t(71)=14.18, p<.001, and total negative symptoms, t(71)=5.92, p<.001, on the SIPRS compared to the HV group, Figure 1B.
Computerized Finger Tapping (CTAP) by Group
Finger tapping behavior was analyzed in a repeated-measure general linear model, which compared motor speed a cross hands (dominant, non-dominant) as the within-subject factor with group (CHR, HC) and sex (female, male) as between-subject factors. Sex was included as a factor because of known sex differences in handedness [5, 36]. There was a significant main effect of group, F(1,69)=4.80, p=.03, ɳ2p=.07 with the CHR group (M=52.58, SEM=.91) showing more motor slowing compared to the HV group (M=55.68, SEM=1.08), Figure 1B. The group difference was present across hands and reflected a general motor slowing as there was no difference in handedness condition by group, p=.70. As expected, there was a significant hand by gender interaction, F(1,69)=4.33, p=.04, ɳ2p=.06, driven by a greater motor speed difference across hands in women compared to males, but no significant main effect of gender was found, p=.08. There were no other significant main or interactive effects, p’s>.57. Follow-up analyses, found no main or interactive impact of block related to group, and that age had no impact on the significance or direction of the findings when added as a covariate.
Computerized Finger Tapping (CTAP) by Symptoms
Within the CHR group, a repeatedmeasure general linear model, which compared motor speed across hands (dominant, non-dominant) as the within-subject factor and sex (female, male), total positive symptoms, and total negative symptoms as between-subject factors. Motor speed was not related to SIPRS total positive symptom score, F(1,40)=3.19, p=.08, r= −.29, ɳ2p=.08; which was consistent across hands with no interaction by hand, p=.92. Motor speed was related to SIPRS total negative symptom score, F(1,40)=5.75, p=.03, r=.37, ɳ2p=.13; which was consistent across hands with no interaction by hand, p=.93. Again, there was a significant hand by gender interaction, F(1,40)=4.10, p=.05, ɳ2p=.10. This interaction was driven by female participants showing a greater difference in tapping across hands compared to males, but no significant main effect of gender was found, p=.98. There were no other significant main or interactive effects, p’s>.92.
Discussion
CHR individuals showed significant motor slowing, i.e., a reduced number of button presses, compared to healthy volunteers regardless whether the hand was dominant or non-dominant, which is consistent previous psychosis literature [9, 13–16]. Additionally, female participants showed greater difference between dominant and non-dominant hands compared to males [5, 36], which did not vary by group. Within the CHR group, negative but not positive symptoms related to motor slowing. This finding is in line with other work from our lab [10, 11, 24], and others [15, 16], suggesting that motor slowing appears to be tied unique to negative symptoms. This is also consistent with a cognitive dysmetria framework [37, 38].
In summary, finger tapping was sensitive to the presence of attenuated symptoms in the CHR group, and reflected the magnitude of negative symptoms after accounting for positive symptoms. Although, motor slowing related to total negative symptom score, but it is possible that this relationship may reflect deficits in overlapping basal ganglia, cerebellar, and cortico-motor circuits [24, 39, 40]. Critically, these circuits underlie both motor and cognitive processes; disruptions in these circuits may result in motor and cognitive deficits that comprise negative symptoms [10, 24, 39, 40]. Future studies should examine the extent to which computerized finger tapping may be reflect underlying basal ganglia, cerebellar, and cortico-motor circuit dysconnectivity [10, 27].
Notably, this task is brief, and can be feasibly administered on any home computer keyboard (all that is required is an Internet connection and a keyboard). Therefore computerized, online finger tapping tasks may be a critical and easily dispersible tool for clinical practice that is also relevant to current emerging negative symptoms. As previously noted, motor symptoms are predictive of psychosis onset and symptom progression [25, 26, 28]. Accordingly, finger tapping may be also be a predictive, clinical tool that marks conversion. The utility of finger tapping as a predictive tool should be explored in future longitudinal analyses of conversion and symptom progression.
The current study highlights several important topics, but there are several limitations to consider. The current sample does not contain conversion or follow-up information, which could assess the sensitivity of motor slowing in predicting future symptoms. The sample size of the current study is consistent with much of the extant literature (Becker et al., 2010; Gschwandtner et al., 2006; Keefe et al., 2006; Lencz et al., 2006; Niendam et al., 2007, 2006; Woodberry et al., 2013), future studies would benefit from larger sample sizes. The computerized finger tapping task has been validated in other studies [6, 35], the current study did not validate the motor slowing with alternate finger tapping tasks within the current sample. The current study also focuses on a single, behavioral approach to assessing motor slowing; future studies may benefit from a multimodal approach to examine biological mechanisms. Finally, future research should examine if this finger tapping task can effectively distinguish individuals at clinical high-risk in large, population screenings.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the National Institutes of Mental Health (1R01MH112545-01, R21 MH110374, R21/R33 Award, MH103231. We have no conflicts to disclose.
References
- 1.Brown S (1997) Excess mortality of schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. The British Journal of Psychiatry 171:502–508. 10.1192/bjp.171.6.502 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Olfson M, Gerhard T, Huang C, et al. (2015) Premature Mortality Among Adults With Schizophrenia in the United States. JAMA Psychiatry 72:1172–1181. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.1737 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Cechnicki A, Cichocki Ł, Kalisz A, et al. (2014) Duration of untreated psychosis (DUP) and the course of schizophrenia in a 20-year follow-up study. Psychiatry Research 219:420–425. 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.05.046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Penttilä M, Jääskeläinen E, Hirvonen N, et al. (2014) Duration of untreated psychosis as predictor of long-term outcome in schizophrenia: systematic review and meta-analysis. The British journal of psychiatry: the journal of mental science 205:88–94. 10.1192/bjp.bp.113.127753 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Gur RC, Richard J, Hughett P, et al. (2010) A cognitive neuroscience-based computerized battery for efficient measurement of individual differences: Standardization and initial construct validation. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 187:254–262. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2009.11.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Gur RC, Ragland JD, Moberg PJ, et al. (2001) Computerized Neurocognitive Scanning: I. Methodology and Validation in Healthy People. Neuropsychopharmacology 25:766–776. 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00278-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.D’Reaux RA, Neumann CS, Rhymer KN (2000) Time of day of testing and neuropsychological performance of schizophrenic patients and healthy controls. Schizophrenia Research 45:157–167. 10.1016/S0920-9964(99)00196-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Gur RC, Braff DL, Calkins ME, et al. (2015) Neurocognitive performance in family-based and case-control studies of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 163:17–23. 10.1016/j.schres.2014.10.049 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Becker HE, Nieman DH, Wiltink S, et al. (2010) Neurocognitive functioning before and after the first psychotic episode: does psychosis result in cognitive deterioration? Psychological Medicine 40:1599–1606. 10.1017/S0033291710000048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Dean DJ, Walther S, Bernard JA, Mittal VA (2018) Motor Clusters Reveal Differences in Risk for Psychosis, Cognitive Functioning, and Thalamocortical Connectivity: Evidence for Vulnerability Subtypes. Clinical Psychological Science 6:721–734. 10.1177/2167702618773759 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Dean DJ, Mittal VA (2015) Spontaneous parkinsonisms and striatal impairment in neuroleptic free youth at ultrahigh risk for psychosis. NPJ Schizophr 1:14006 10.1038/npjschz.2014.6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Dickson H, Cullen AE, Reichenberg A, et al. (2014) Cognitive impairment among children at-risk for schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatric Research 50:92–99. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.12.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Dickson H, Laurens KR, Cullen AE, Hodgins S (2012) Meta-analyses of cognitive and motor function in youth aged 16 years and younger who subsequently develop schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine 42:743–755. 10.1017/S0033291711001693 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Gschwandtner U, Pflüger M, Aston J, et al. (2006) Fine motor function and neuropsychological deficits in individuals at risk for schizophrenia. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 256:201–206. 10.1007/s00406-005-0626-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Niendam TA, Bearden CE, Zinberg J, et al. (2007) The Course of Neurocognition and Social Functioning in Individuals at Ultra High Risk for Psychosis. Schizophr Bull 33:772–781. 10.1093/schbul/sbm020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Niendam TA, Bearden CE, Johnson JK, et al. (2006) Neurocognitive performance and functional disability in the psychosis prodrome. Schizophrenia Research 84:100–111. 10.1016/j.schres.2006.02.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Damme KSF, Gallagher N, Vargas T, et al. (2019) Motor sequence learning and pattern recognition in youth at clinical high-risk for psychosis. Schizophrenia Research 208:454–456. 10.1016/j.schres.2019.03.023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Moritz CH, Haughton VM, Cordes D, et al. (2000) Whole-brain Functional MR Imaging Activation from a Finger-tapping Task Examined with Independent Component Analysis. American Journal of Neuroradiology 21:1629–1635 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Rao SM, Bandettini PA, Binder JR, et al. (1996) Relationship between Finger Movement Rate and Functional Magnetic Resonance Signal Change in Human Primary Motor Cortex. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 16:1250–1254. 10.1097/00004647-199611000-00020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Witt ST, Meyerand ME, Laird AR (2008) Functional neuroimaging correlates of finger tapping task variations: An ALE meta-analysis. Neuroimage 42:343–356. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Bernard JA, Orr JM, Mittal VA (2017) Cerebello-thalamo-cortical networks predict positive symptom progression in individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis. NeuroImage: Clinical 14:622–628. 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.03.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Bernard JA, Mittal VA (2014) Cerebellar-Motor Dysfunction in Schizophrenia and Psychosis-Risk: The Importance of Regional Cerebellar Analysis Approaches. Front Psychiatry 5:. 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00160 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Schröder J, Essig M, Baudendistel K, et al. (1999) Motor Dysfunction and Sensorimotor Cortex Activation Changes in Schizophrenia: A Study with Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. NeuroImage 9:81–87. 10.1006/nimg.1998.0387 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Walther S, Mittal VA (2017) Motor System Pathology in Psychosis. Curr Psychiatry Rep 19:97 10.1007/s11920-017-0856-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Mittal VA, Walker EF (2007) Movement abnormalities predict conversion to Axis I psychosis among prodromal adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 116:796–803. 10.1037/0021-843X.116.4.796 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Dean DJ, Kent JS, Bernard JA, et al. (2015) Increased postural sway predicts negative symptom progression in youth at ultrahigh risk for psychosis. Schizophrenia Research 162:86–89. 10.1016/j.schres.2014.12.039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Mittal VA, Dean DJ, Bernard JA, et al. (2014) Neurological Soft Signs Predict Abnormal Cerebellar-Thalamic Tract Development and Negative Symptoms in Adolescents at High Risk for Psychosis: A Longitudinal Perspective. Schizophr Bull 40:1204–1215. 10.1093/schbul/sbt199 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Mittal VA, Hasenkamp W, Sanfilipo M, et al. (2007) Relation of neurological soft signs to psychiatric symptoms in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 94:37–44. 10.1016/j.schres.2007.04.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Heinz A, Knable MB, Coppola R, et al. (1998) Psychomotor slowing, negative symptoms and dopamine receptor availability—an IBZM SPECT study in neuroleptic-treated and drug-free schizophrenic patients. Schizophrenia Research 31:19–26. 10.1016/S0920-09964(98)00003-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Bermanzohn PC, Siris SG (1992) Akinesia: A syndrome common to parkinsonism, retarded depression, and negative symptoms of schizophrenia. Comprehensive Psychiatry 33:221–232. 10.1016/0010-440X(92)90045-R [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Bervoets C, Docx L, Sabbe B, et al. (2014) The nature of the relationship of psychomotor slowing with negative symptomatology in schizophrenia. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 19:36–46. 10.1080/13546805.2013.779578 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Gibbon M, Spitzer RL, Benjamin LS, First MB (1997) Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 (SCID-5). https://www.appi.org/products/structured-clinical-interview-for-dsm-5-scid-5. Accessed 2 May 2019
- 33.Miller TJ, McGlashan TH, Rosen JL, et al. (2003) Prodromal Assessment With the Structured Interview for Prodromal Syndromes and the Scale of Prodromal Symptoms: Predictive Validity, Interrater Reliability, and Training to Reliability. Schizophr Bull 29:703–715. 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Weickert TW, Goldberg TE (2005) First- and second-generation antipsychotic medication and cognitive processing in schizophrenia. Curr Psychiatry Rep 7:304–310. 10.1007/s11920-005-0085-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Coleman AR, Moberg PJ, Ragland JD, Gur RC (1997) Comparison of the Halstead-Reitan and Infrared Light Beam Finger Tappers. Assessment 4:277–286. 10.1177/107319119700400307 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Da Silva FN, Irani F, Richard J, et al. (2012) More than just tapping: index finger-tapping measures procedural learning in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 137:234–240. 10.1016/j.schres.2012.01.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Andreasen NC, Paradiso S, O’Leary DS (1998) “Cognitive Dysmetria” as an Integrative Theory of Schizophrenia: A Dysfunction in Cortical-Subcortical-Cerebellar Circuitry? Schizophr Bull 24:203–218. 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033321 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Carroll CA, O’Donnell BF, Shekhar A, Hetrick WP (2009) Timing dysfunctions in schizophrenia as measured by a repetitive finger tapping task. Brain and Cognition 71:345–353. 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.06.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Bernard JA, Mittal VA (2015) Updating the research domain criteria: the utility of a motor dimension. Psychological Medicine 45:2685–2689. 10.1017/S0033291715000872 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Mittal VA, Bernard JA, Northoff G (2017) What Can Different Motor Circuits Tell Us About Psychosis? An RDoC Perspective. Schizophr Bull 43:949–955. 10.1093/schbul/sbx087 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Keefe RSE, Perkins DO, Gu H, et al. (2006) A longitudinal study of neurocognitive function in individuals at-risk for psychosis. Schizophrenia Research 88:26–35. 10.1016/j.schres.2006.06.041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Lencz T, Smith CW, McLaughlin D, et al. (2006) Generalized and Specific Neurocognitive Deficits in Prodromal Schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 59:863–871. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Sawada K, Kanehara A, Sakakibara E, et al. (2017) Identifying neurocognitive markers for outcome prediction of global functioning in individuals with first-episode and ultra-high-risk for psychosis. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 71:318–327. 10.1111/pcn.12522 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Woodberry KA, McFarlane WR, Giuliano AJ, et al. (2013) Change in neuropsychological functioning over one year in youth at clinical high risk for psychosis. Schizophrenia Research 146:87–94. 10.1016/j.schres.2013.01.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]