Abstract
Research has shown that a comparable truth baseline (CTB) approach elicits more cues to deception and results in higher accuracy rates than a small talk baseline. Past research focused on laypeople’s accuracy rates. We examined whether the CTB also has a positive effect on law enforcement personnel accuracy. In this study, 95 police officers judged 10 interviews, whereby half of the senders told the truth, and the other half lied about a mock undercover mission. Half of the interviews included only questioning about the event under investigation, whereas the other half also included questioning aimed at creating a CTB. Total and truth accuracy did not differ, but observers who watched interviews with a CTB obtained higher lie detection accuracy rates than those who watched interviews without the baseline questioning. Signal detection analyses showed that this effect could be attributed to a decreased response bias in the CTB condition.
Key words: baseline interviewing, comparable truth baseline, credibility assessment, deception detection, interrogation, interviewing techniques, police officers’ accuracy
Introduction
Cues to deception and interviewing techniques
Detecting deception is a flourishing research area, from both a theoretical (Bond, Levine, & Hartwig, 2015; Caso, Maricchiolo, Livi, Vrij, & Palena, 2018; Vrij, Hartwig, & Granhag, 2018; Walczyk, Harris, Duck, & Mulay, 2014) and an applied perspective (Vrij & Fisher, 2016). Research initially focused on searching for reliable cues to deceit that liars display spontaneously (see Vrij, 2008, for a comprehensive review of this research). This research has proven to be largely unsuccessful: Cues to deception in this setting are typically faint and unreliable (DePaulo et al., 2003; Vrij, 2008).
Due the paucity of support for the search for reliable cues to deception that liars display spontaneously, scholars have started to examine whether such cues can be elicited or enhanced through specific interviewing protocols (Vrij, 2014; Vrij & Granhag, 2012). Several interview protocols have emerged. In the Strategic Use of Evidence technique the available evidence is presented to interviewees in such a way that it elicits within-statement and between-statements inconsistencies in liars and elicits admissions from them (Granhag & Hartwig, 2008, 2015; May, Granhag, & Tekin, 2017). The Cognitive Credibility Assessment approach builds on the research findings that lying is usually more mentally taxing than telling the truth (Vrij, Fisher, & Blank, 2017). This approach is based on the idea that questions can be asked, or instructions can be given, that elicit different mental processes in truth tellers and liars (Vrij, 2015; Vrij, Fisher, Blank, Leal, & Mann, 2016). Another approach, the Assessment Criteria Indicative of Deception technique, is a (semi-)structured interview that builds on the memory-enhancement techniques (mnemonics) that are part of the Cognitive Interview (Fisher & Geiselman, 1992). Such memory-enhancing techniques have a different impact on truth tellers and liars when considering cues such as response length, amount of details and coherence (Colwell et al., 2009; Colwell, Hiscock-Anisman, Memon, Taylor, & Prewett, 2007). This so-called interviewing to detect deception approach has proven to be successful, and several of the techniques proposed and tested in this area are ready to be implemented in real life (Vrij, 2018; Vrij & Fisher, 2016).
Observers’ lie detection accuracy
Another line of research examines observers’ accuracy at evaluating interviewees’ credibility. The most comprehensive meta-analysis published to date showed that people –laypersons and professionals alike – are poor at this task. The average accuracy, obtained by over 24,000 observers, was 54%, which is only just above the level of chance (50%; Bond & DePaulo, 2006). However, the studies presented in the meta-analysis did not account for the effect of strategic interviewing, which is worth exploring. Luke et al. (2016) found that training law enforcements in the Strategic Use of Evidence technique increased their accuracy rates up to 65%. Similarly, another study found that police officers trained in the Cognitive Credibility Assessment approach asked more effective questions and improved their accuracy rates up to 74% (Vrij, Leal, Mann, Vernham, & Brankaert, 2015). Finally, Colwell et al. (2009) found that observers trained in the Assessment Criteria Indicative of Deception technique obtained accuracy rates up to 77%. These pictures show strong potential for training practitioners into strategic interviewing.
The baseline approach
Another interview technique is based on the rationale that if the observer has previous knowledge of the sender’s truthful behaviour, this can be used as a truthful baseline to inform the observer’s decisions (see Vrij, 2008). Indeed, research has supported this claim, as it was found that being familiar with the sender increases the observer’s performance (Brandt, Miller, & Hocking, 1980, 1982). Feeley, de Turck, and Young (1995) found a positive linear relationship between the level of familiarity and the observers’ performance; that is, the more the observer was familiar with the senders’ truthful behaviour, the more she or he was accurate in detecting deception. However, research also shows that familiarity only works when the baseline is truthful. For example, Garrido and Masip (2001) provided observers with a baseline behaviour of the sender, which could be either truthful or deceitful. The authors found that observers benefited from the baseline exposure only when this was truthful.
The type of baselining reported above is different from the one that one can expect in investigative interviewing. The suspect and the interviewer are often strangers; therefore, such familiarity baseline is difficult to obtain (if possible at all) during investigations. Instead, investigators must obtain the baseline during the first phases of the interview. There have been some suggestions on how to create a baseline on the spot.
Initially, it was suggested to create such a baseline by asking neutral, non-threatening questions (Frank, Yarbrough, & Ekman, 2006) but both theoretical reasons and experimental results have revealed that this method is ineffective (Ewens, Vrij, Jang, & Jo, 2014; Moston & Engelberg, 1993; Palena, Caso, Vrij, & Orthey, 2018). This approach is thought to be a striking misuse of psychological theory (Moston & Engelberg, 1993; Vrij, Leal, Jupe, & Harvey, 2018). The problem is that the baseline and target (which concerns the event under investigation) sections are fundamentally different in such an approach. For example, the stakes are higher in the target than in the baseline section, and the interviewee’s engagement is often different between the two sections. As a result, not only liars but also truth tellers change their behaviour when the baseline and target responses are compared (Ewens et al., 2014; Palena et al., 2018; Vrij, 2016). Therefore, both theoretical explanations (Moston & Engelberg, 1993; Vrij, 2016; Vrij, Leal, et al., 2018) and experimental findings (Caso, Palena, Vrij, & Gnisci, 2019; Ewens et al., 2014) support the assumption that a baseline created through small talk (i.e., small talk baseline) is ineffective.
Vrij (2016) states that a different baselining approach, the creation of a comparable truth baseline (CTB), may be more fruitful. In the CTB approach factors such as engagement, emotion, cognitive load and context are kept comparable between the baseline and the target sections of the interview. To keep such factors comparable, baseline questioning should pertain to the same topic of that discussed when posing a question concerning the event under investigation. Similarly, questions should not be posed in a way that creates different emotional arousal. Two recent studies found that using a CTB, liars changed their behaviour and speech more than truth tellers (Palena et al., 2018) and that observers provided with a CTB obtained higher accuracy rates than those provided with a small talk baseline (Caso et al., 2019).
The main aim of the present study was to explore whether a CTB would also be effective with law enforcement personnel as observers. Involving law enforcement personnel as participants in studies is important as it may make it more likely that they will endorse the findings and, if the findings are positive, start to use the techniques themselves (Vrij & Granhag, 2012).
We decided to compare the CTB approach to a ‘no baseline’ rather than to a small talk baseline for two main reasons. First, we wanted to test the hypothesis that having a comparable baseline behaviour of the interviewee makes the observers more accurate than not having such reference. This builds on the idea that accuracy is increased because a comparable baseline reduces the effect of interpersonal differences (Vrij, 2016). Second, we did not want to compare the CTB with the small talk baseline because: (a) research has already shown that such an approach does not work because truth tellers and liars appear equally deceptive (Ewens et al., 2014; Palena et al., 2018); and (b) research with laypersons has shown that observers provided with a CTB outperform those provided with a small talk baseline (Caso et al., 2019). This is unlikely to change with practitioners, as the inefficacy of the small talk baseline approach depends on its underpinnings rather than on the observer: Both truth tellers and liars display differences between baseline and target periods with this approach (Ewens et al., 2014). Therefore, no reliable cue to deception appears, and neither laypersons nor practitioners can benefit from the small talk baseline approach.
Building on previous results on objective cues to deception elicited with a CTB (Palena et al., 2018) and on the rationale presented above, we expected that practitioners in the CTB condition would achieve higher total (Hypothesis 1), truth (Hypothesis 2) and lie (Hypothesis 3) accuracy rates than practitioners in the no-baseline condition.
Method
Participants
A total of 95 practitioners (88 men and seven women) took part in the experiment. Of them, 42 belonged to the state police, 28 to the financial and economic crimes police, and 25 to the Italian Military police (Carabineri). Age ranged from 28 to 58 years, with a mean of M = 45.39 (SD = 6.71). Professional experience ranged from five to 38 years, with a mean of M = 23.84 (SD = 7.82). One participant was excluded from the analyses because he did not follow the instructions.
Design
The experiment utilised a 2 (baseline: no baseline vs. CTB, between subjects) × 2 (veracity: truth tellers vs. liars, within-subjects) mixed design. For the factor baseline, observers in the no-baseline condition just saw suspects being questioned about the event under investigation. On the other hand, observers in the CTB condition saw interviews where suspects were also questioned about an event other than – but comparable to – that under investigation. For the factor veracity, half of the senders told the truth, whereas the other half lied. For the CTB condition, all senders truthfully reported the additional event, which served as the CTB. The dependent variables were the three accuracy rates obtained by the observers: total accuracy, truth accuracy and lie accuracy. Accuracy rates were obtained using the formula:
Procedure
The first and the second authors made an appointment with the prosecutor (who is the person in charge for recruiting participants for any research project with practitioners) and presented him with an outline of the study. The importance of conducting research with practitioners was stressed. The prosecutor was interested in the project and proceeded to contact high-rank officials belonging to the three organisations (State Police, Economic and Financial Police, and Military Police), who were informed about the research goals. High-rank officers then provided the authors with a list of participants from the three police organisations. The participants were then contacted to take part in the study. Data were collected in three different places, one for each police organisation. Each participant took part in the experiment individually. Upon arrival, she or he was welcomed and briefed about the aim of the study. They were informed that they were going to watch a series of interviews with different people and then decide for each interviewee whether she or he was lying or telling the truth. They were not informed about the number of interviews they would see and about the truth/lie telling ratio in the interviews to avoid these aspects affecting their decisions. In total, each judge evaluated every sender (10 in total).
Participants in the no-baseline condition were not given further information. Participants in the CTB condition were informed that interviews were split in two sections. The first section was the baseline; the second was the target section of the interview. They were also informed that senders were always honest in the baseline section, and therefore they had to take a decision about the sender’s (dis)honesty only regarding the target section. They were invited to examine deviations from the baseline to inform their decisions on the rationale that the more a sender changed his or her behaviour and speech between the two phases, the more likely it was that she or he was lying. Information about which behaviours or speech patterns might be indicative of deceit was not provided. The participants were not informed about the aims of the study (comparing accuracy of practitioners in the two experimental conditions).
In the experiment, veracity decisions were made answering the dichotomous question ‘Do you think the interviewee was . . . “lying” or “telling the truth”?’. After the experimenter felt confident that the participant understood the instruction, she or he was left alone to carry out the lie detection test, which lasted on average about 30 minutes. Once the participant had finished the test, they were thanked and debriefed.
Stimulus material
Senders depicted in the video clips had performed a mock undercover mission. The video clips were obtained from a previous study (Caso et al., 2019). The mock crime started with participants receiving an envelope from the experimenter that contained the password for accessing a laptop. Once logged in, the sender read a Word document file that informed him or her to look for a CD-ROM in a backpack that was placed in the room. The CD-ROM depicted a man informing the sender to look for a key in the same backpack, which would open a safe deposit box placed near the window in the same room. The safe deposit box contained further written instructions, asking the participant to send an email to a specific address and to wait for a person to collect them from the room. Everything until that moment was part of the CTB, whereas everything that happened after this point was part of the target event, the event under investigation.
After a short while the person (a confederate) arrived and gave a newspaper to the sender, informing him or her to read it for further instructions. These instructions informed the sender that she or he had to go to an adjacent room and to look for a USB stick hidden behind a coat hook. The sender had to take that USB stick and put it in place of a second USB stick that was hidden in a book placed in a wardrobe. The sender then had to leave the newspaper next to the book and keep the second USB (the one found in the book) until the end of the experiment. Once these tasks were completed, the sender had to come back to the first room and wait for the interviewer.
We used 10 senders in total. For the CTB, the observer watched both the baseline and target sections, which were composed of the sender answering a free recall baseline question and a free recall target question. All 10 senders answered the baseline question truthfully, whereas for the target question, five of them told the truth, and five lied. The veracity status was counterbalanced. For the condition without the baseline, the observers only saw the target question and answers.
The 10 clips in the ‘no-baseline’ condition lasted 76.70 s on average (SD = 17.36); those in the CTB lasted 170.10 s on average (SD = 20.51). This difference is due to the presence of the baseline.
Results
Total accuracy
To test Hypothesis 1, an analysis of variance (ANOVA) was carried out with baseline (no baseline vs. CTB) as factor, and the total accuracy rate as dependent variable. No significant effect appeared for the baseline factor, F(1, 92) = 1.554, p = .21, d = −0.26, 95% confidence interval, CI [−0.66, 0.15], post hoc achieved power .24. Mean accuracy for participants in the no-baseline condition (M = 49.58; SD = 13.67, 95% CI [45.61, 53.55]) was similar to that of participants in the CTB (M = 53.26; SD = 14.91, 95% CI [48.83, 57.69]). Hypothesis 1 was thus rejected.
Truth accuracy
Preliminary tests assessing ANOVA assumptions showed that homoscedasticity was not respected, F(1, 92) = 7.110, p = .01. Consequently, we ran a Mann–Whitney U test to test Hypothesis 2. The truth accuracy for participants in the no-baseline condition (M = 60.00; SD = 17.01, 95% CI [55.06; 64.93]) did not differ from that of participants in the CTB condition (M = 51.73; SD = 22.93, 95% CI [44.93, 58.55]), U = 863.50, p = .058, post hoc achieved power .48. Therefore, Hypothesis 2 was also rejected.
Lie accuracy
To test Hypothesis 3, an ANOVA was carried out with baseline as factor and lie accuracy as the dependent variable. The baseline effect was significant, F(1, 92) = 17.16, p < .001, d = −0.85, 95% CI [−1.27, −0.43], post hoc achieved power .98. Supporting Hypothesis 3, observers in the CTB condition (M = 54.78; SD = 20.41, 95% CI [48.72, 60.84]) outperformed those in the no-baseline condition (M = 39.16; SD = 15.95, 95% CI [34.53, 43.80]). In addition, more observers (63%) in the CTB condition than in the control condition (25%) obtained an accuracy rate of at least 60%, χ2(1, N = 94) = 13.82, p < .001, Cramer’s V = .38. Figure 1 depicts a density plot of such values.
Signal detection analyses
Our results supported Hypothesis 3. However, lie detection accuracy for participants in the CTB condition did not differ from chance, t(45) = 1.589, p = .12, Bayes Factor 10 (BF10) = 0.513, d = 0.23, 95% CI [−0.06, 0.52], post hoc achieved power .33. This makes an alternative explanation possible. Since with a baseline approach (including the CTB) both truth tellers and liars appear to behave and speak differently between the two phases of the interview (Palena et al., 2018; Vrij, 2016), the increased lie accuracy may be partly due to a lie bias. We tested this possibility by exploring participants’ response bias. Although historically the β value was the preferred measure for bias, some scholars have suggested to use c instead of β (see Stanislaw & Todorov, 1999) as the former is less biased than the latter. c is described as deviations, in standard deviation units, from the neutral point (where neither answer is preferred, i.e., there is no bias), which is set at 0. If the c value is greater than 0, there is a bias toward responding ‘no’ (in our context, ‘truth teller’); if the value is less than 0 there is a bias toward responding ‘yes’ (in our context, ‘liar’). A one-sample t test with c as the dependent variable and 0 as test score, was significant for participants in the no-baseline condition, t(47) = 7.803, p < .001, d = 1.13, 95% CI [0.76, 1.49], post hoc achieved power 1, but not significant for participants in the CTB condition, t(45) = −0.555, p = .58, d = −0.08, 95% CI [−0.37, 0.21], BF01 = 5.404, post hoc achieved power .08. An independent sample t test with baseline (no baseline vs. CTB) as the factor and c as the dependent variable showed that participants in the ‘no-baseline’ condition obtained higher scores (M = 0.29; SD = 0.26, 95% CI [0.22, 0.37]) than those in the CTB condition (M = −0.05, SD = 0.61, 95% CI [−0.23, 0.13]), t(60.62) = 3.547, p = .001, d = 0.73, 95% CI [0.31, 1.14], post hoc achieved power .94. Results for the c score therefore suggest that participants in the ‘no-baseline’ condition were truth biased, whereas those in the CTB displayed no bias. Table 1 displays the rates of truth and lie judgments in each condition.
Table 1.
Baseline |
Decides truth |
Decides lie |
||
---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | |
No baseline | 60.42 | 9.22 | 39.58 | 9.22 |
Comparable truth baseline | 48.48 | 15.77 | 51.52 | 15.77 |
Given the response bias results, we also analysed d′ scores, which is a measure of sensitivity reported in standard deviation units (Stanislaw & Todorov, 1999). A d′ score of 0 indicates an inability to distinguish between the two stimuli (truth telling vs. lying), whereas scores greater than 0 indicate that participants were able to make such a distinction. Neither participants in the ‘no-baseline’ condition (M = −0.002, SD = 0.82, 95% CI [−0.24, 0.23]), t(47) = −0.021, p = .98, BF01 = 6.376, post hoc achieved power .05, nor those in the CTB condition (M = 0.24, SD = 0.99, 95% CI [−0.05, 0.53]), t(45) = 1.620, p = .11, BF01 = 1.864, post hoc achieved power .36, were able to discriminate truth tellers from liars. Considering the analyses on response bias and sensitivity, we can conclude that the difference in accuracy between the two conditions was driven by the difference in response bias.
Discussion
In this experiment, we tested Italian police officers’ ability to evaluate credibility when provided with a CTB compared to when no baseline was presented. We found support for Hypothesis 3, as officers in the CTB condition (54.78%) out-performed those in the no-baseline condition (39.16%) in terms of lie accuracy. However, further analyses showed that this was the result of officers in the CTB condition being less biased rather than being more accurate.
Our results are in alignment with the truth default theory (Levine, 2014), which predicts that observers usually tend to believe others, unless ‘deceptive triggers’ appear and make the observer consider the possibility of deception. In our context, such ‘deceptive triggers’ may originate from behavioural differences that appear – for both truth tellers and liars – between the baseline and the target phase of the interview. However, the idea of humans having a cognitive default (truth) bias has been questioned (see Street, 2015). Future studies should try to disentangle this issue.
The baseline approach comes with several issues. First, truth tellers may be classified as liars because they also often change their behaviour throughout the interview. Second, the problems with the CTB are also shown in Palena et al. (2018), who found that it only worked for one cue (spatial details) when comparisons between baseline and target periods were made. The authors noted that their result was probably due to the task performed by the interviewees, which was mainly spatial. Indeed, participants in Palena’s et al. (2018) study had to commit a mock crime, which requested them to explore different rooms and interact with several objects, making the statements rich in spatial details. This makes clear that the cues to be used with the CTB approach are tightly connected to the content of the story itself.
Third, obtaining a CTB in laboratory settings is easy as the experimenters exert full control over baseline veracity. However, in real life it may be difficult to obtain a CTB that is really truthful and comparable, as a ground truth is often missing. And, in the case of the baseline being a lie, it loses its efficacy (Garrido & Masip, 2001).
In sum, the positive result for CTB concerning lie accuracy was the result of reduced probability of guessing truth, and no differences were found for total and truth accuracy. This shows no real positive effect for using a CTB. Previous research on the effectiveness of various interview techniques reached better results than those we found in the current experiment. This may be partly due to the fact that such techniques are more active approaches – the interviewer conducts the interview actively – whereas the baseline approach is more passive. The only thing the interviewer has to do is to create the baseline. It has already been suggested that active strategic interviewing is more effective for deception detection (Vrij, 2014; Vrij & Granhag, 2012).
Limitations
There were some limitations in the present study. First, our stimulus material was relatively low stakes. This may have affected the results, although it is not straightforward to predict how. However, baseline research relies in part on behavioural patterns, and such patterns are affected by stakes (Hartwig & Bond, 2014). It is therefore important to start examining the baseline technique also in higher stakes situations. Second, observers only watched 10 senders, which may not represent variations in senders’ performance adequately. Third, it appeared that police officers participating in this study do not use small talk baselining or any other type of baselining in their daily practice. Given the problems associated with baselining, this cannot be considered a bad thing. Rather, they reported that when they interview real suspects, they tend to base their decision on available evidence. Yet, we did not provide our participants with evidence.
Correction Statement
This article has been republished with minor changes. These changes do not impact the academic content of the article.
Ethical standards
Declaration of conflicts of interest
Letizia Caso has declared no conflicts of interest
Nicola Palena has declared no conflicts of interest
Elga Carlessi has declared no conflicts of interest
Aldert Vrij has declared no conflicts of interest
The authors declare that all measures and conditions have been reported, that one participant was excluded from the analyses because he did not follow the instructions, and that the sample size was determined in order to have at least 30 participants per cell, accepting any further available participant.
Ethical approval
All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee (the Department of Human and Social Sciences of University of Bergamo) and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Informed consent
Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank the Prosecutor’s office, the State Police Headquarters, the Financial and Economic Police and the Italian Military Police Corp, ‘Arma dei Carabinieri’, all from the city of Bergamo (Italy), for their participation in this research.
References
- Bond C.F. Jr., & DePaulo B.M. (2006). Accuracy of deception judgments. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(3), 214–234. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr1003_2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bond C.F. Jr., Levine T.R., & Hartwig M. (2015). New findings in non-verbal lie detection In Granhag P. A., Vrij A., & Vershuere B. (Eds.), Detecting deception: Current challenges and cognitive approaches (pp. 37–58). Chichester: John Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Brandt D.R., Miller G.R., & Hocking J.E. (1980). The truth-deception attribution: Effects of familiarity on the ability of observers to detect deception. Human Communication Research, 6(2), 99–110. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2958.1980.tb00130.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Brandt D.R., Miller G.R., & Hocking J.E. (1982). Familiarity and lie detection: A replication and extension. Western Journal of Communication (Includes Communication Reports), 46(3), 276–290. doi: 10.1080/10570318209374086 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Caso L., Maricchiolo F., Livi S., Vrij A., & Palena N. (2018). Factors affecting observers’ accuracy when assessing credibility: The effect of the interaction between media, senders’ competence and veracity. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 21, E49. doi: 10.1017/sjp.2018.54 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caso L., Palena N., Vrij A., & Gnisci A. (2019). Observers’ performance at evaluating truthfulness when provided with Comparable Truth or Small Talk Baselines. Psychiatry, Psychology, and Law, 1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colwell K., Hiscock-Anisman C.K., Memon A., Colwell L.H., Taylor L., & Woods D. (2009). Training in assessment criteria indicative of deception to improve credibility judgments. Journal of Forensic Psychology Practice, 9(3), 199–207. doi: 10.1080/15228930902810078 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Colwell K., Hiscock-Anisman C.K., Memon A., Taylor L., & Prewett J. (2007). Assessment criteria indicative of deception (ACID): An integrated system of investigative interviewing and detecting deception. Journal of Investigative Psychology and Offender Profiling, 4(3), 167–180. doi: 10.1002/jip.73 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- DePaulo B.M., Lindsay J.J., Malone B.E., Muhlenbruck L., Charlton K., & Cooper H. (2003). Cues to deception. Psychological Bulletin, 129(1), 74–118. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.74 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ewens S., Vrij A., Jang M., & Jo E. (2014). Drop the small talk when establishing baseline behaviour in interviews. Journal of Investigative Psychology and Offender Profiling, 11(3), 244–252. doi: 10.1002/jip.1414 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Feeley T.H., de Turck M.A., & Young M.J. (1995). Baseline familiarity in lie detection. Communication Research Reports, 12(2), 160–169. doi: 10.1080/08824099509362052 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher R.P., & Geiselman R.E. (1992). Memory enhancing techniques for investigative interviewing: The cognitive interview. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas Publisher. [Google Scholar]
- Frank M.G., Yarbrough J.D., & Ekman P. (2006). Investigative interviewing and the detection of deception In Williamson T. (Ed.), Investigative interviewing: Rights, research and regulation (pp. 229–255). Cullompton, Devon: Willan. [Google Scholar]
- Garrido E., & Masip J. (2001). Previous exposure to the sender’s behavior and accuracy at judging credibility In Roesch R., Corrado R. R., & Dempster R. J. (Eds.), Psychology in the courts: International advances in knowledge (pp. 271–287). London: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Granhag P.A., & Hartwig M. (2008). A new theoretical perspective on deception detection: On the psychology of instrumental mind reading. Psychology, Crime & Law, 14(3), 189–200. doi: 10.1080/10683160701645181 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Granhag P.A., & Hartwig M. (2015). The strategic use of evidence technique: A conceptual overview In Granhag P. A., Vrij A., & Verschuere B. (Eds.), Detecting deception: Current challenges and cognitive approaches (pp. 231–252). Chichester: John Wiley and Sons. [Google Scholar]
- Hartwig M., & Bond C.F. Jr.(2014). Lie detection from multiple cues: A meta-analysis. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28(5), 661–676. doi: 10.1002/acp.3052 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Levine T.R. (2014). Truth-Default Theory (TDT): A theory of human deception and deception detection. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 33(4), 378–392. doi: 10.1177/0261927X14535916 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Luke T.J., Hartwig M., Joseph E., Brimbal L., Chan G., Dawson E., … Granhag P.A. (2016). Training in the strategic use of evidence technique: Improving deception detection accuracy of American law enforcement officers. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 31(4), 270–278. doi: 10.1007/s11896-015-9187-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- May L., Granhag P.A., & Tekin S. (2017). Interviewing suspects in denial: On how different evidence disclosure modes affect the elicitation of new critical information. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1154. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01154 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moston S.J., & Engelberg T. (1993). Police questioning techniques in tape recorded interviews with criminal suspects. Policing and Society, 6, 61–75. doi: 10.1080/10439463.1993.9964670 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Palena N., Caso L., Vrij A., & Orthey R. (2018). Detecting deception through small talk and comparable truth baselines. Journal of Investigative Psychology and Offender Profiling, 15(2), 124–132. doi: 10.1002/jip.1495 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stanislaw H., & Todorov N. (1999). Calculation of signal detection theory measures. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 31(1), 137–149. doi: 10.3758/BF03207704 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Street C.N. (2015). ALIED: Humans as adaptive lie detectors. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 4(4), 335–343. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2015.06.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Vrij A. (2008). Detecting lies and deceit: Pitfalls and opportunities. Chichester: John Wiley and Sons. [Google Scholar]
- Vrij A. (2014). Interviewing to detect deception. European Psychologist, 19(3), 184–194. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000201 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Vrij A. (2015). A cognitive approach to lie detection In Granhag P. A., Vrij A., & Verschuere B. (Eds.), Detecting deception: Current challenges and cognitive approaches (pp. 205–230). Chichester: John Wiley and Sons. [Google Scholar]
- Vrij A. (2016). Baselining as a lie detection method. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 30(6), 1112–1119. doi: 10.1002/acp.3288 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Vrij A. (2018). Verbal lie detection tools from an applied perspective In Rosenfeld J. P. (Eds.), Detecting concealed information and deception (pp. 297–327). London, UK: Elsevier Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Vrij A., Fisher R.P., Blank H., Leal S., & Mann S. (2016). A cognitive approach to elicit verbal and nonverbal cues to deceit In Van Prooijen J.W. & Van Lange P. A. M. (Eds.), Cheating, corruption, and concealment: The roots of dishonesty (pp. 284–302). Cambridge: Cambridge University press. [Google Scholar]
- Vrij A., & Fisher R.P. (2016). Which lie detection tools are ready for use in the criminal justice system? Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 5(3), 302–307. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2016.06.014 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Vrij A., Fisher R.P., & Blank H. (2017). A cognitive approach to lie detection: A meta-analysis. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 22(1), 1–21. doi: 10.1111/lcrp.12088 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Vrij A., & Granhag P.A. (2012). Eliciting cues to deception and truth: What matters are the questions asked. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 1(2), 110–117. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2012.02.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Vrij A., Hartwig M., & Granhag P.A. (2018). Reading lies: Nonverbal communication and deception. Annual Review of Psychology, 70(1), 295–317. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103135 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vrij A., Leal S., Jupe L., & Harvey A. (2018). Within‐subjects verbal lie detection measures: A comparison between total detail and proportion of complications. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 23(2), 265–279. doi: 10.1111/lcrp.12126 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Vrij A., Leal S., Mann S., Vernham Z., & Brankaert F. (2015). Translating theory into practice: Evaluating a cognitive lie detection training workshop. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 4(2), 110–120. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2015.02.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Walczyk J.J., Harris L.L., Duck T.K., & Mulay D. (2014). A social-cognitive framework for understanding serious lies: Activation-decision-construction-action theory. New Ideas in Psychology, 34, 22–36. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2014.03.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]