Skip to main content
Frontiers in Psychiatry logoLink to Frontiers in Psychiatry
. 2020 Feb 14;11:43. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00043

Influence of Socio-Economic Status on Psychopathology in Ecuadorian Children

María Nieves Pérez-Marfil 1, Manuel Fernández-Alcántara 1,2,*, Ahmed F Fasfous 1,3,*, Carlos Burneo-Garcés 1,4, Miguel Pérez-García 1, Francisco Cruz-Quintana 1
PMCID: PMC7034357  PMID: 32116858

Abstract

The socioeconomic status (SES) of parents has been reported to have a crucial impact on emotional competence in childhood. However, studies have largely been carried out in developed countries and in children in a specific age range, and it is not clear whether the effect of the SES of parents varies by age. The objective of this study was to investigate the psychopathological profile (including externalizing and internalizing problems) of children aged 7, 9, and 11 years old with low SES in a developing country (Ecuador). The study included 274 children (139 boys and 135 girls), who were divided between medium-SES (n = 133) and low-SES (n = 141) groups. Data were gathered on socioeconomic and anthropometric variables of the children, and the parents completed the Child Behavior Check-List (CBCL). In comparison to the medium-SES group, children in the low-SES group obtained higher scores for internalizing and externalizing symptoms and for total problems, and they obtained lower scores for social competence skills. The housing risk index and school competence were the two main predictors of internalizing and externalizing problems in this population.

Keywords: socioeconomic status, psychopathology, internalizing, externalizing, anthropometric measures, child behavior

Introduction

Neurodevelopment is a dynamic inter-relationship among genetic, cerebral, cognitive, emotional, and behavioral processes over life (1). Emotional competence and skills can be influenced by nutritional, infectious, and toxic factors and by upbringing practices and patterns (2), as well as by the socio-economic status (SES) of parents (36).

SES consists of numerous factors, including family income, parental education and occupation, psychological and physical health status, physical conditions at home, upbringing practices, stressful situations suffered by child and/or parents, physical or psychological abuse, and nutritional status (711). It has been reported that low SES can be a risk factor for inadequate socio-emotional development and can increase vulnerability to development problems (1214). Various theoretic models have been proposed for the impact of SES on neurodevelopment, considering different explanatory variables. Thus, a low SES has been associated with: external factors, related to the environment with which the child interacts; and internal factors, more related to interactions with parental figures.

External factors are considered in the model by Evans, among others, in which a low SES is associated with greater exposure to environmental risk, which in turn increases the risk of physical and psychological disorders. The review by Evans and Kantrowitz (15) identified a large number of environmental stressors that can indirectly influence the physical and emotional health of children living in poverty, including: lower air quality, larger presence of environmental toxins, higher ambient noise, greater residential crowding, lower housing quality, and worse educational facilities.

Numerous theoretic models have focused on the influence of low SES and poverty on the interaction between parents and children, acknowledged to be a crucial factor (16, 17). Although no unified model has been published that considers all stressors associated with a low SES, certain key factors have been identified (18), including parental investment, parental practices, levels of chronic stress, and coping (8, 19, 20). Parental investment refers to the quality of cognitive stimulation at home, including the availability of books, electronic devices, and toys and the amount of time spent watching television. Parental practices refer to the interaction or involvement of parents in their children's care. It has been suggested that these practices are characterized by greater hostility and conflict in a low SES environment, with less care and support of the children and inconsistent punishment and reward patterns, favoring the development of internalizing and externalizing problems in the children (21). Finally, children with low SES have been found to have lower skills to deal with these stressors, which can accumulate over time and interact with each other, as well greater difficulty in controlling their emptions (19).

Middle childhood (7–11 years) appears to be a crucial stage for the regulation of emotions and for the onset of psychopathologic disorders It is a time when social skills develop, sexual differences are maximized, and important interactions between environmental and genetic factors can be observed (22). In this pre-adolescent stage, there is a marked development of executive function and self-regulatory capacities (23) but also a greater risk of psychopathologic problems. It has been reported that psychopathologies appearing during infancy start to increase at the age of 5–6 years and reach their main peak in middle childhood (24) and that the likelihood of such problems at this stage is increased in a low SES environment (25).

Recent studies on the association between a low SES and psychopathologic aspects showed that children from poorer backgrounds experience greater difficulties in controlling impulses and delaying gratification (26). It has also been observed that the accumulation of multiple risks (e.g., deficits in education, mistreatment, or lack of parental attention) during early infancy can predict a lower capacity for emotional self-regulation, with difficulties in controlling emotions and in co-operating with peers and independent play (10, 2729).

Special interest has been shown in the relationship between SES and the development of mental health problems in children and adolescents, specifically internalizing and externalizing problems, (4, 25). It is estimated that around 20% of children and adolescents worldwide have mental health problems (30), which are considered to be two- to three-fold more prevalent in children of families with low SES (31). The influence of this condition on the short- and long-term development of mental health problems is considered to be more marked during the first months of life (31). However, it has also been found that children and adolescents growing up in a low SES environment have more internalizing (e.g., anxiety and depression) and externalizing (e.g., aggressiveness, opposition, and hyperactivity) symptoms, although findings have not been consistent (4).

There have been attempts to identify the mechanisms underlying the effect of SES on the mental health of children. Bøe and colleagues (4) reported that the emotional wellbeing of parents and their upbringing practices may act as mediator of the interaction between SES and mental health problems in children. They observed that the family economy and the educational level of parents affected the mental health of children in different ways. Thus, the family economy was related to the presence of externalizing problems through the emotional wellbeing of parents and upbringing practices, while the educational level of the mother was related to externalizing problems through negative disciplinary methods. In the case of internalizing problems, direct and indirect associations were found with the family economy, mediated by maternal emotional wellbeing and upbringing practices (4).

However, research to date has been limited. Despite the clear relationship between SES and mental health problems in children, few data are available on the prevalence of these problems, on the most frequent types of disorder, or on possible comorbidities (4, 31). One objective of developmental psychopathology has been to analyze the heterogeneity of symptoms presented by children with behavior problems, with the aim of identifying subgroups with similar difficulties for specific treatment approaches (14). However, there has been inadequate consideration of the cultural setting or the country's level of development, with most research being conducted in developed nations (11, 32). In addition, most studies have focused more on the economic aspects of SES and less on the possible influence of other important factors, such as the educational level of parents (14). The family economy and educational level of parents may have differential influences on family processes and children's socio-emotional adjustment. The upbringing of children may be directly affected by the educational level of parents and indirectly affected by the family economy via effects on parental emotional wellbeing and mental health. Analysis of individual SES components is needed to determine their specific contribution to the socio-emotional adjustment of children. Thus, Merz et al. (25) reported that anxiety and depression levels of children were negatively related to the educational level of parents but were not associated with family income. Finally, the duration of exposure to a low SES environment can determine the likelihood of psychopathologic problems, so that it is important to take the age of children into account. Various reviews have concluded that experiences of poverty during early childhood can have a damaging effect on both cognitive and emotional development (21). In this line, recent studies have described significant interactions between the age of children and SES levels in verbal memory, phonetic fluency, abstract reasoning, and inhibitory control (13).

Limited data are available on the relationship of SES with the development of psychopathologies during middle childhood. Therefore, the aim of this study was to investigate socio-emotional development in children of 7, 9, and 11 years of age exposed to low or medium SES in a city in a developing country (Guayaquil, Ecuador). The study hypothesis was that the children with low SES would have more emotional (externalizing and internalizing) problems in comparison to the children with medium SES.

Method

Participants

The study included 274 Spanish-speaking schoolchildren from Guayaquil, the most populous city of Ecuador, divided among 7-year-olds (45 boys, 44 girls), 9-year-olds (45 boys, 46 girls), and 11-year-olds (47 boys, 45 girls). These ages were selected to allow investigation of the relationship between SES and psychopathologic changes throughout middle childhood.

The study population was divided by SES between: a medium-SES group (n = 133) containing 45 7-year-olds (23 boys, 22 girls), 44 9-year-olds (22 boys, 22 girls), and 44 11-year-olds (22 boys, 22 girls); and a low-SES group (n = 141) containing 46 7-year-olds (24 boys, 22 girls), 47 9-year-olds (23 boys, 24 girls), and 48 11-year-olds (25 boys, 23 girls).

Sampling Procedure

The study was conducted in primary schools in the city, selected to provide a balanced representation of areas with predominantly low-SES or medium-SES populations. Random sampling was conducted among the 7-, 9-, and 11-year-old children registered at each participating school.

The characterization of school catchment areas as low or medium SES and their inclusion in the study was based on multiple factors, including: the private, subsidized, or public funding of the school; basic services in the area; and income and employment levels, among others. Following these criteria, a selection was made of three medium-SES schools (one public, one subsidized, and one private) in the north/center of the city and two low-SES schools (two public schools) in an area at the southern edge of the city “Isla Trinitaria,” surrounded by an inlet of the sea. The population of Isla Trinitaria has considerably increased over the past 20 years, with a major influx of people from other parts of the country, and it currently has around 350,000 inhabitants. It is considered to be one of the poorest zones in the metropolitan area of Guayaquil and did not have access to basic services until 2011, when power lines and a fresh drinking water system were installed, followed by an expansion of the sewage network in 2013. However, major public investment is required in health, education, security, roads, and public transport, among others, before the population of this area can enjoy the same quality of life and opportunities as the majority of the city dwellers (medium-SES).

Inclusion Criteria

Study inclusion criteria were: a) age of 7, 9, or 11-years at the time of assessment; b) regular attendance at one of the participating schools; c) absence of physical, psychological, or cognitive impairment diagnosed by a specialist or reported by teachers or parents; and d) informed consent signed by parent/guardian. Before evaluation of the selected children, interviews were conducted with their teachers and with their parents/guardians to verify that the above inclusion criteria were met, confirming that none had diagnosed or apparent physical or psychological disorders or evidenced major behavioral problems. The availability of an appropriate room for interviews with the children was also established. Out of the eligible children enrolled in the study, 24 were subsequently excluded due to the withdrawal of consent (n = 4) or because conditions for the assessment were not adequate due to interruptions for school activities (n = 20).

Instruments

Socioeconomic Status

Socioeconomic Survey

The questionnaire was developed by the School of Nursing of the University of Guayaquil (33) as part of its Child and Adolescent Care Program and was designed to gather data on the socioeconomic level of the families of children. This questionnaire was administered to the parents/guardians in interviews held at the school of their children (afternoon sessions).This instrument classifies families according to raw scores for maternal level of education level (score range of 1–4) and social class of the head of household (score range of 1–4), and a transformed housing risk index, including house structure, overcrowding, water supply, garbage disposal, toilet availability, and sewage disposal (score range of 1–3). A higher questionnaire score indicates lower socioeconomic level.

Anthropometric Measurements

Measurements were taken of the height, weight, and cranial and abdominal circumferences of the children, using: a SECA wall plastic height scale, model 206 (Hamburg, Germany) with measurement range of 0–220 cm; a SECA digital floor scale, model 803 (Hamburg, Germany) with a limit of 150 kg in 100 g increments; and a SECA measuring tape, model 201 (Hamburg, Germany), an ergonomic and flexible band to measure circumferences, with a range of 0–250 cm in 1 mm increments.

Psychopathology

Child Behavior Checklist in 6- to 18-Year-Olds

The Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) (34) was used to obtain information from the usual guardians of the children on the children's skills or competences (Social Competence Scale), problematic behaviors (Problems Scale), and Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM)-oriented problems. The social competence items yield scores for three narrow-band scales (activities, social, and school) and one broad-band scale (total social competence). The Problems Scale evaluates eight syndromes: somatic complaints, anxiety, depression, social problems, thought problems, attention problems, rule-breaking behavior, and aggressive behavior. It also allows assessment of two large groups of syndromes: internalizing problems (combining withdrawal, somatic complaints, and anxiety/depression) and externalizing problems (combining rule-breaking behavior and aggressive behavior). The DSM-oriented problems include affective problems, anxiety problems, somatic problems, attention deficit/hyperactivity problems, oppositional defiant problems, conduct problems, sluggish cognitive tempo, obsessive-compulsive problems, and post-traumatic problems. Administration of the CBCL takes 25–30 minutes. Correlation coefficients of .90 were obtained for mean scores between different examiners and between two parental reports separated by 7 days (test-retest reliability). Correlation coefficients for the repeated parental reports were .87 for the Social Competence Scale and.89 for the Problems Scale. The CBCL has been adapted to a wide number of Spanish-speaking countries including Spain, Chile, Mexico, and Puerto Rico (35). In the present study the Spanish version of the CBCL was used (35, 36). To stablish the cut-off scores in the present population, the recommendations of Achenbach and Rescorla (37) were considered.

Procedure

A team of six trained evaluators carried out the fieldwork during a 4-month period. Interviews and anthropometric measurements of the children were conducted at school during the morning in a room with adequate physical conditions for this purpose. Parents attended a 30-min interview in the afternoon at their children's school to record their socioeconomic data and complete the CBCL. Written consent was obtained from the parents/guardians of the children for their participation in the study, which was approved by the ethical committee of the local University (Ref: A3/042954/11).

Data Analysis

After descriptive analysis of the data, ANOVAs were conducted with 2x2x3 factorial design considering two SES groups (medium and low), two sex groups (boy and girl), and three age groups (7, 9, and 11 years) as independent variables and CBCL subscales, parental socioeconomic survey subscales, and anthropometric variables as dependent variables, followed by application of the post-hoc Bonferroni test. The chi-square test was also applied to evaluate differences among groups in the percentage of clinical problems in each CBCL scale. Finally, linear regression analyses were performed to identify the SES components and social competences with greatest influence on internalizing, externalizing, and total problems of the CBCL. Given the need for multiple comparisons, the Bonferroni correction was applied to reduce the probability of a type I error, establishing the significance threshold at ≤ 0.002 for ANOVAs and ≤ 0.006 for linear regressions. Partial eta-squared was used as effect size measure.

Results

Before the statistical analyses were performed, the SES classification of participants was tested by considering their maternal education, home risk index, and the social class of head of household. Results confirmed that the classification of the children was appropriate, with the low-SES group scoring significantly higher (i.e., lower SES) for maternal education level [F(1,260) = 249.04, p < .001; partial η2 = .522], home risk index [F(1,260) = 104.91, p < .001; partial η2 = .290], and social class of head of household [F(1,260) = 256.19, p < .001; partial η2 = .502]. No significant differences were observed in these scores as a function of the child's age (see Table 1 ).

Table 1.

Group, age, and sex differences and interaction on socioeconomic, anthropometric, and nutritional measures.

Measures Group 7 years 9 years 11 years p Post hoc
Medium-SES
(n = 133)
Low-SES
(n = 141)
Boy (n = 45)
ME (SD)
Girl (n = 44)
ME (SD)
Boy (n = 45)
ME (SD)
Girl (n = 46)
ME (SD)
Boy (n = 47)
ME (SD)
Girl (n = 45)
ME (SD)
Socioeconomic characteristics
Maternal
education level
Medium
Low
1.38 (0.50)
3.43 (1.08)
1.76 (0.70)
3.30 (1.08)
1.55 (0.75)
3.61 (1.37)
1.57 (0.75)
3.67 (1.17)
1.68 (0.78)
3.80 (1.23)
1.62 (0.59)
3.35 (0.78)
Group** ME < L
Housing risk index Medium
Low
1.10 (0.30)
1.87 (0.46)
1.24 (0.44)
1.85 (0.37)
1.27 (0.46)
1.74 (0.45)
1.38 (0.50)
1.83 (0.48)
1.23 (0.43)
1.68 (0.48)
1.10 (0.30)
1.65 (0.49)
Group** ME < L
Social class of head of household Medium
Low
1.76 (0.70)
3.48 (0.67)
2.05 (0.74)
3.15 (0.37)
1.68 (0.65)
3.09 (0.67)
1.95 (0.67)
3.38 (0.58)
2.18 (0.96)
3.32 (0.56)
2.05 (0.81)
3.30 (0.70)
Group** ME < L
Anthropometric characteristics
Height Medium
Low
1,244.45 (68.90)
1,233.65 (113.75)
1,228.95 (57.56)
1,207.25 (47.86)
1,376.45 (50.39)
1,350.48 (73.55)
1,358.95 (70.25)
1,294.58 (58.81)
1,464.05 (73.39)
1,402.68 (74.51)
1,475.55 (77.30)
1,458.87 (80.55)
Group**
Age**
AgexSex**
ME > LP
7 < 9 < 11
9**
11*
Weight Medium
Low
28.75 (6.52)
23.11 (3.33)
27.01 (5.90)
22.88 (3.20)
38.51 (8.87)
33.01 (7.76)
36.80 (10.89)
27.66 (6.48)
45.55 (9.83)
37.88 (11.34)
46.05 (10.16)
39.43 (12.52)
Group**
Age**
ME > L
7 < 9 < 11
Abdominal circumference Medium
Low
596.95 (75.78)
529.35 (24.91)
591.32 (77.95)
521.25 (35.79)
680.14 (91.13)
601.00 (89.90)
657.50 (99.76)
549.04 (72.18)
710.82 (104.60)
634.08 (97.49)
693.82 (89.44)
612.30 (98.27)
Group**
Age**
Sex*
ME> L
7 < 9 < 11
B > G
Head circumference Medium
Low
516.77 (14.29)
508.96 (15.22)
506.23 (12.21)
499.85 (16.42)
529.18 (9.59)
513.00 (19.80)
518.59 (20.29)
499.46 (22.55)
530.64 (12.09)
514.24 (16.08)
526.41 (15.91)
511.57 (16.77
Group**
Age
Sex
ME> L
7< 9 < 11
B > G

M, mean; SD, standard deviation; ME, medium; L, low; B, boy; G, girl; **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05.

Differences in Anthropometric Variables Between Groups

Results for anthropometric characteristics were as follows: a) weight varied according to the SES, F(1,258) = 38.54, p < .001 (partial η2 = .130), and age, F(2,258) = 83.12, p < .001 (partial η2 = .392); b) height varied according to the SES, F(1,258) = 14.29, p < .001 (partial η2 = .052), and age, F(2,258) = 207.64, p < .001 (partial η2 = .617); c) the abdominal circumference varied according to the SES, F(1,258) = 62.46, p < .001 (partial η2 = .195), and age, F(2,258) = 34.19, p < .001 (partial η2 = .210); and d) the cranial circumference varied according to the SES, F(1,258) = 45.38, p < .001 (partial η2 = .150), and age, F(2,258) = 13.55, p < .001 (partial η2 = .095) (see Table 1 ). In all cases, values were lower for the children in the low-SES group than for the children in the medium-SES group. In regard to age, post-hoc analyses showed differences among the three age groups, with lower scores for the younger than older children. There were also differences for the main effect of the sex variable on head circumference, whose values were higher in boys than in the girls. Finally, the age x sex interaction was significant for height, with differences between boys and girls in the 9- and 11-year-old age groups.

Differences Between Socioeconomic Status Groups in Psychopathology and Social Competence

A significant difference between SES groups were found for all syndromes gathered in the CBCL except for thought problems ( Table 2 ). In comparison to the medium-SES group, the low-SES group obtained higher scores for depression, F(1,258) = 39.234, p < .001 (partial η2 = .132); somatic complaints, F(1,258) = 21.021, p < .001 (partial η2 = .075); social, F(1,258) = 35.566, p < .001 (partial η2 = .121); attention problems, F(1,258) = 49.792, p < .001 (partial η2 = .162); rule-breaking behavior, F(1,258) = 38.436, p < .001 (partial η2 = .130); and aggressive behavior, F(1,258) = 47.404, p < .001 (partial η2 = .155).

Table 2.

Group, age, and sex differences and interaction on syndrome scales [Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL)].

Scales Group 7 years 9 years 11 years p Post hoc
Medium-SES
(n = 133)
Low-SES
(n = 141)
Boy (n = 45)
M (SD)
Girl (n = 44)
M (SD)
Boy (n = 45)
M (SD)
Girl (n = 46)
M (SD)
Boy (n = 47)
M (SD)
Girl (n = 45)
M (SD)
CBCL
Anxiety Medium
Low
4.30 (3.43)
4.96 (4.25)
3.59 (3.47)
5.90 (4.23)
4.36 (3.35)
4.65 (3.10)
6.14 (4.12)
5.50 (3.19)
3.36 (3.35)
6.56 (4.80)
4.76 (2.43)
6.57 (3.88)
Group** ME < L
Depression Medium
Low
1.83 (1.80)
2.48 (1.97)
1.59 (2.22)
3.70 (3.39)
1.68 (1.67)
3.09 (2.61)
2.45 (2.41)
4.50 (3.61)
1.50 (1.74)
5.52 (3.75)
1.81 (1.75)
3.61 (3.07)
Group**
AgexSex*
ME < L
9: B < G
11: B > G
Somatic complaints Medium
Low
1.43 (1.67)
2.65 (2.99)
0.82 (1.30)
3.25 (3.57)
1.09 (1.38)
2.39 (2.62)
1.86 (1.96)
2.79 (2.84)
1.64 (2.38)
3.60 (3.91)
2.14 (2.03)
2.91 (2.52)
Group** ME < L
Social problems Medium
Low
2.26 (1.98)
4.35 (3.54)
3.00 (2.81)
4.40 (2.98)
2.27 (1.75)
4.17 (3.23)
3.59 (2.75)
5.21 (3.16)
1.77 (2.16)
5.48 (3.45)
2.43 (1.96)
3.96 (2.95)
Group** ME < L
Thought problems Medium
Low
1.43 (1.56)
1.39 (1.90)
1.23 (1.90)
1.15 (1.90)
1.05 (1.17)
1.30 (1.33)
1.64 (2.08)
1.75 (2.38)
1.23 (1.27)
1.64 (1.76)
0.67 (1.02)
0.82 (1.40)
AgexSex* 9: B < G
11: B > G
Attention problems Medium
Low
370 (3.25)
5.48 (3.32)
2.95 (2.08)
7.20 (4.54)
2.45 (2.37)
7.78 (5.63)
3.27 (2.73)
4.96 (5.39)
3.45 (2.02)
8.44 (5.79)
2.95 (2.89)
5.22 (3.90)
Group** ME < L
Rule-breaking behavior Medium
Low
2.00 (1.48)
3.91 (4.37)
1.05 (1.56)
4.30 (3.64)
0.95 (1.13)
3.52 (4.08)
1.14 (1.83)
2.46 (1.82)
1.82 (1.82)
3.52 (3.58)
0.71 (0.85)
2.00 (2.32)
Group**
Sex*
ME < L
B > G
Aggressive behavior Medium
Low
7.04 (5.05)
9.09 (7.25)
4.68 (4.19)
9.55 (6.64)
5.23 (4.48)
11.78 (8.48)
5.00 (3.68)
8.46 (6.63)
4.09 (3.07)
10.76 (8.70)
4.33 (1.96)
10.13 (4.08)
Group** ME < L

M, mean; SD, standard deviation; ME, medium; L, low; B, boy; G, girl; **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05.

As shown in Table 3 (CBCL psychopathologic scales), the low-SES group obtained higher scores than the medium-SES group in internalizing problems F(1,258) = 30.757, p < .001 (partial η2 = .107); externalizing problems, F(1,258) = 52.174, p < .001 (partial η2 = .168); and total problems, F(1,258) = 61.362, p < .001 (partial η2 = .192).

Table 3.

Group, age, and sex differences and interaction on internalizing, externalizing, and total problems [Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL)].

Scales Group 7 years 9 years 11 years p Post hoc
Medium-SES
(n = 133)
Low-SES
(n = 141)
Boy (n = 45)
M (SD)
Girl (n = 44)
M (SD)
Boy (n = 45)
M (SD)
Girl (n = 56)
M (SD)
Boy (n = 47)
M (SD)
Girl (n = 45)
M (SD)
CBCL
Internalizing problems Medium
Low
7.57 (4.50)
10.09 (7.17)
6.00 (4.67)
12.85 (8.86)
7.14 (5.29)
10.13 (6.73)
10.45 (6.88)
12.79 (7.43)
6.50 (5.40)
15.68 (10.79)
8.71 (4.42)
13.09 (7.83)
Group** ME < L
Externalizing problems Medium
Low
9.04 (5.67)
13.00 (11.10)
5.73 (5.33)
13.85 (9.55)
6.18 (5.05)
15.30 (11.92)
6.14 (4.85)
10.92 (7.99)
5.91 (4.56)
14.28 (11.64)
5.05 (3.37)
12.13 (5.18)
Group** ME < L
Total problems Medium
Low
28.96 (15.67)
40.30 (23.47)
22.95 (16.59)
45.75 (22.60)
23.55 (12.90)
44.00 (24.94)
30.23 (16.11)
40.71 (22.62)
23.41 (12.51)
51.52 (27.61)
23.29 (9.46)
40.57 (15.27)
Group** ME < L

M, mean; SD, standard deviation; ME, medium; L, low; B, boy; G, girl; **p < 0.01.

As shown in Table 4 (CBCL social competence scales), the low-SES group obtained lower scores than the medium-SES group in social activities, F(1,253) = 12.114, p < .001 (partial η2 = .045); school, F(1,256) = 64.122, p < .001 (partial η2 = .200); and total social competence, F(1,249) = 35.289, p < .001 (partial η2 = .124).

Table 4.

Group, age, and sex differences and interaction on Social Competence Scales [Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL)].

Scales Group 7 years 9 years 11 years p Post hoc
Medium-SES
(n = 133)
Low-SES
(n = 141)
Boy (n = 45)
M (SD)
Girl (n = 44)
M (SD)
Boy (n = 45)
M (SD)
Girl (n = 46)
M (SD)
Boy (n = 47)
M (SD)
Girl (n = 45)
M (SD)
CBCL
Activities Medium
Low
5.89 (2.95)
5.39 (1.73)
7.05 (3.00)
5.37 (2.41)
7.18 (1.94)
5.30 (1.64)
5.71 (2.94)
6.57 (2.41)
7.30 (2.44)
6.00 (2.95)
6.86 (2.31)
6.04 (1.94)
Group** ME > L
Social Medium
Low
5.26 (2.05)
4.32 (2.23)
5.23 (1.69)
3.95 (1.96)
5.62 (1.24)
4.39 (2.04)
4.82 (1.74)
4.33 (1.79)
5.30 (1.80)
4.96 (1.95)
5.57 (1.67)
5.00 (2.28)
Group** ME > L
School Medium
Low
5.22 (0.45)
4.65 (0.93)
5.05 (0.59)
4.70 (0.86)
5.09 (0.43)
4.13 (0.87)
5.18 (0.59)
4.30 (1.11)
5.13 (0.71)
3.88 (1.05)
5.19 (0.60)
4.65 (0.65)
Group** ME > L
Total social competence Medium
Low
16.59 (4.07)
14.68 (2.93)
17.86 (4.17)
14.16 (3.64)
18.57 (2.06)
14.17 (2.98)
16.00 (4.25)
14.95 (3.30)
17.95 (3.90)
15.13 (3.96)
17.81 (3.45)
15.87 (3.61)
Group** ME > L

M, mean; SD, standard deviation; ME, medium; L, low; B, boy; G, girl; **p < 0.01.

As shown in Table 5 (CBCL DSM-oriented Scales), the low-SES group obtained higher scores than the medium-SES group in all problems except for anxiety and obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) problems, as follows: affective problems, F(1,258) = 36.531, p < .001 (partial η2 = .124); somatic problems, F(1,258) = 14.155, p < .001 (partial η2 = .052); ADHD problems, F(1,258) = 26.749 (partial η2 = .094), p < .001; conduct problems, F(1,258) = 46.219, p < .001 (partial η2 = .152); sluggish cognitive tempo (SCT) problems, F(1,258) = 23.543, p < .001 (partial η2 = .084); and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) problems, F(1,258) = 28.824, p < .001 (partial η2 = .100).

Table 5.

Group, age, and sex differences and interaction on Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM)-oriented scales [Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL)].

Scales Group 7 years 9 years 11 years p Post hoc
Medium-SES
(n = 133)
Low-SES
(n = 141)
Boy (n = 45)
M (SD)
Girl (n = 44)
M (SD)
Boy (n = 45)
M (SD)
Girl (n = 46)
M (SD)
Boy (n = 47)
M (SD)
Girl (n = 45)
M (SD)
CBCL
Affective problems Medium
Low
1.61 (1.83)
3.17 (2.46)
2.18 (2.40)
3.85 (2.94)
1.32 (1.52)
3.52 (2.56)
2.59 (2.70)
3.75 (2.80)
1.95 (1.50)
5.04 (3.96)
1.71 (1.45)
3.13 (2.53)
Group**
AgexSex*
ME < L
9: B < G
11: B > G
Anxiety problems Medium
Low
3.04 (2.48)
3.52 (2.48)
2.55 (2.32)
2.95 (2.19)
2.95 (2.26)
2.57 (2.54)
4.09 (2.81)
3.63 (2.20)
2.09 (2.09)
3.60 (2.71)
3.29 (1.79)
3.95 (2.92)
Somatic problems Medium
Low
0.70 (0.88)
1.43 (2.27)
0.41 (0.80)
1.85 (2.13)
0.64 (0.90)
1.04 (1.64)
0.95 (1.76)
1.46 (1.91)
0.86 (1.49)
1.92 (2.52)
1.19 (1.54)
2.00 (2.49)
Group** ME < L
ADHD problems Medium
Low
4.57 (3.98)
6.13 (3.36)
3.59 (3.11)
6.60 (4.08)
3.00 (2.20)
6.74 (4.21)
4.18 (3.35)
4.17 (3.42)
3.95 (2.50)
6.44 (4.32)
2.86 (3.31)
5.30 (3.34)
Group** ME < L
Oppositionalproblems Medium
Low
3.09 (2.48)
3.13 (2.83)
2.23 (2.02)
3.60 (3.05)
1.91 (2.04)
4.17 (3.26)
2.00 (1.85)
2.75 (2.63)
1.82 (1.74)
3.88 (3.33)
1.29 (1.49)
3.35 (1.67)
Group** ME < L
Conduct problems Medium
Low
1.91 (1.47)
4.65 (5.77)
1.00 (1.48)
5.05 (4.49)
0.95 (1.36)
4.74 (5.99)
0.95 (1.68)
3.08 (2.90)
1.82 (2.15)
4.44 (4.93)
0.76 (0.77)
2.83 (2.81)
Group** ME < L
SCT Medium
Low
0.57 (1.12)
0.78 (1.41)
0.68 (0.89)
1.50 (1.88)
0.36 (0.66)
2.00 (2.49)
1.00 (1.31)
1.96 (2.49)
0.55 (0.96)
2.24 (2.37)
0.71 (0.96)
1.26 (1.60)
Group** ME < L
OCD Medium
Low
1.61 (1.90)
1.35 (1.37)
1.23 (1.57)
1.55 (1.61)
1.77 (2.20)
1.26 (1.18)
1.95 (2.15)
1.13 (1.42)
1.14 (1.32)
2.36 (2.25)
1.43 (1.33)
1.74 (1.60)
GroupxAge* 9: ME > L
11: ME < L
PTSD Medium
Low
4.26 (2.96)
5.65 (3.82)
3.18 (2.34)
6.00 (3.28)
4.32 (3.08)
5.48 (4.52)
5.18 (3.39)
6.38 (4.57)
2.77 (1.77)
7.92 (5.26)
3.81 (2.09)
6.35 (4.14)
Group**
GroupxAge*
ME < L
7: ME < L
9: ME = L
11: ME < L

M, mean; SD, standard deviation; ME, medium; L, low; B, boy; G, girl; SCT, sluggish cognitive tempo; OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder; PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder; **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05.

Children were classified into three groups according to their T-score in each CBCL scale following proposals of the CBCL authors (34): a) no problems/normal, T-score < 65; b) borderline, typical score of 65–69; and c) clinical problems, T-score > 69. Children were also classified into three groups according to their scores in internalizing, externalizing, and total problem scales: a) no problems/normal, T-score < 60; b) borderline, T-score of 60–63; and c) clinical problems, T-score > 63. These results are exhibited in Tables 6 8 , which report on the number of children in each group, the percentage, and the between-group differences evaluated using the chi-square test. In comparison to the medium-SES group, the low-SES group had a significantly and markedly higher percentage of children with clinical problems in all scales with the exception of anxiety and thought syndromes, DSM-oriented anxiety problems and OCD scales, and a significantly higher percentage of children with clinical internalizing (21.9 vs. 8.3%) and externalizing (18.5 vs. 2.6%) problems. These values were especially high for the depression scale (40.8 vs. 22.3%) ( Tables 6 8 ).

Table 6.

Percentages of children with and without clinical problems in Syndrome scales [Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL)] and analysis of differences between medium- and low-socioeconomic status (SES) groups.

Scales Group
Medium-SES
(n = 133)
Low-SES
(n = 141)
Without problem
n (%)
Borderline
n (%)
Clinical
n (%)
χ2 p
Anxiety Medium
Low
104 (32.9%)
97 (36.6%)
15 (5.7%)
25 (9.4%)
8 (3%)
16 (6%)
4.962 0.084
Depression Medium
Low
53 (20%)
24 (9.1%)
15 (5.7%)
6 (2.3%)
59 (22.3%)
108 (40.8%)
28.749 <0.001**
Somatic complaints Medium
Low
115 (43.4%)
103 (38.9%)
10 (3.8%)
19 (6%)
2 (0.8%)
16 (6%)
13.910 0.001**
Social problems Medium
Low
119 (44.9%)
107 (40.4%)
5 (1.9%)
16 (6%)
3 (1.1%)
15 (5.7%)
13.967 0.001**
Thought problems Medium
Low
124 (46.8%)
129 (48.7%)
2 (0.8%)
7 (2.6%)
1 (0.4%)
2 (0.8%)
2.758 0.252
Attention problems Medium
Low
122 (46%)
96 (36.2%)
5 (1.9%)
19 (7.2%)
0 (0%)
23 (8.7%)
33.869 <0.001**
Rule-breaking behavior Medium
Low
124 (46.8%)
105 (39.6%)
2 (0.8%)
12 (4.5%)
1 (0.4%)
21 (7.9%)
26.490 <0.001**
Aggressive behavior Medium
Low
118 (44.5%)
89 (33.6%)
7 (2.6%)
22 (8.3%)
2 (0.8%)
27 (10.2%)
32.973 <0.001**

**p < 0.01.

Table 8.

Percentages of children with and without clinical problems in Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM)-oriented scales [Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL)] and analysis of differences between medium- and low-socioeconomic status (SES) groups.

Scales Group
Medium-SES
(n = 133)
Low-SES
(n = 141)
Without problem
n (%)
Borderline
n (%)
Clinical
n (%)
χ2 p
Affective problems Medium
Low
113 (42.6%)
97 (36.6%)
11 (4.2%)
18 (6.8%)
3 (1.1%)
23 (8.7%)
17.868 <0.001**
Anxiety problems Medium
Low
87 (32.8%)
89 (33.6%)
19 (7.2%)
18 (6.8%)
21 (7.9%)
31 (11.7%)
1.519 0.468
Somatic problems Medium
Low
119 (44.9%)
111 (41.9%)
5 (1.9%)
12 (4.5%)
3 (1.1%)
15 (5.7%)
10.722 0.005**
ADHD problems Medium
Low
113 (42.6%)
94 (35.5%)
10 (3.8%)
22 (8.3%)
4 (1.5%)
22 (8.3%)
18.280 <0.001**
Oppositional problems Medium
Low
115 (43.4%)
105 (39.6%)
9 (3.4%)
8 (3%)
3 (1.1%)
25 (9.4%)
17.372 <0.001**
Conduct problems Medium
Low
123 (46.4%)
93 (35.1%)
4 (1.5%)
20 (7.5%)
0 (0%)
25 (9.4%)
39.445 <0.001**
SCT Medium
Low
119 (44.9%)
103 (38.9%)
4 (1.5%)
9 (3.4%)
4 (1.5%)
26 (9.8%)
18.785 <0.001**
OCD Medium
Low
118 (44.5%)
134 (50.6%)
3 (1.1%)
0 (0%)
6 (2.3%)
4 (1.5%)
3.966 0.138
PTSD Medium
Low
119 (44.9%)
99 (37.4%)
6 (2.3%)
25 (9.4%)
2 (0.8%)
14 (5.3%)
22.061 <0.001**

SCT, sluggish cognitive tempo; OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder; PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder **p < 0.01.

Table 7.

Percentages of children with and without clinical problems in internalizing, externalizing, and total problem scales [Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL)] and analysis of differences between medium- and low-socioeconomic status (SES) groups.

Scales Group
Medium-SES
(n = 133)
Low-SES
(n = 141)
Without problem
n (%)
Borderline
n (%)
Clinical
n (%)
χ2 p
Internalizing problems Medium
Low
86 (32.5%)
62 (23.4%)
19 (7.2%)
18 (6.8%)
22 (8.3%)
58 (21.9%)
19.696 <0.001**
Externalizing problems Medium
Low
106 (40%)
75 (28.3%)
14 (5.3%)
14 (5.3%)
7 (2.6%)
49 (18.5%)
36.416 <0.001**
Total problems Medium
Low
104 (32.9%)
68 (25.7%)
17 (6.4%)
22 (8.3%)
6 (2.3%)
48 (18.1%)
40.456 <0.001**

**p < 0.01.

Table 9 displays the results of simple linear regression models for the main CBCL variables (internalizing problems, externalizing problems, and total problems). All models showed statistically significant differences (p < .001) in all variables. The housing risk index (standardized β = 0.204, p = .005) and school competence (standardized β = −0.214, p = .001) emerged as a significant predictor of internalizing problems and school competence as a significant predictor of externalizing problems (standardized β = −0.403, p < .001) and total problems (standardized β = −0.404, p < .001).

Table 9.

Linear regression models using score for internalizing, externalizing, and total problems as criteria and sex, age variables, socioeconomic status (SES) dimensions, and social competences as predictor.

Scales Variables Standardized β t p Inferior 95% CI Superior 95% CI F model R2 Adjusted R2
Internalizing problems F(8, 253) = 5.962*** .163 .136
Age 0.084 1.406 0.161 −0.156 0.937
Sex 0.098 1.656 0.099 −0.279 3.220
MLE −0.048 −0.634 0.527 −1.111 0.570
HRI 0.204 2.862 0.005** 0.942 5.101
SCL 0.143 1.819 0.070 −0.093 2.334
Activities 0.002 0.026 0.979 −0.375 0.385
Social 0.061 0.959 0.339 −0.248 0.718
School −0.214 −3.317 0.001** −2.941 −0.750
Externalizing problems F(8,253) = 9.251*** 0.232 0.207
Age −0.094 −1.647 0.101 −1.094 0.098
Sex −0.054 −0.960 0.338 −2.838 0.977
MLE −0.043 −0.589 0.556 −1.191 0.643
HRI 0.089 1.311 0.191 −0.759 3.777
SCL 0.116 1.542 0.124 −0.287 2.360
Activities −0.025 −0.412 0.681 −0.501 0.328
Social −0.008 −0.125 0.900 −0.561 0.493
School −0.0403 −6.529 <0.001*** −5.157 −2.767
Total problems F(8,253) = 11.768*** 0.278 0.254
Age −0.040 −0.721 0.472 −1.959 0.909
Sex 0.006 0.115 0.908 −4.321 4.858
MLE −0.043 −0.605 0.546 −2.883 1.528
HRI 0.170 2.567 0.011* 1.656 12.568
SCL 0.153 2.092 0.037* 0.198 6.566
Activities −0.017 −0.297 0.767 −1.147 0.847
Social 0.074 1.251 0.212 −0.463 2.073
School −0.404 −6.736 <0.001*** −12.709 −6.958

MLE, maternal level of education; HRI, Housing Risk Index; SCL, social class level of head of household, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Discussion

This study examined the impact of a low SES on the psychopathology of children living in a developing country, considering three age groups (7, 9, and 11 years). In comparison to children in a medium-SES environment, those in a low-SES environment had more internalizing and externalizing problems, with a higher prevalence of most syndromes studied and lower scores in social competence skills. A larger percentage of children in the low- versus medium-SES group had clinical problems in scales related to internalizing and externalizing problems. Finally, the housing risk index and school competence were the two main predictors of internalizing and externalizing problems in this population.

Higher scores were obtained by the low- versus medium-SES group in the three CBCL psychopathology scales (externalizing, internalizing, and total problems). According to the present findings, this type of problem remains present at the ages of 7, 9, and 11 years old in children with low SES, with negative effects on all psychopathological functions and an evident presence of emotional problems. It has also been suggested that malnourished children are more likely to suffer from post-traumatic stress, chronic fatigue syndrome, and depression, among other psychopathological manifestations (38).

In line with the results of previous studies, not all SES factors were associated with the emergence of emotional problems to the same degree (14, 25). The factors that best predicted the presence of emotional problems in our study were the housing risk indicator and school competence deficits. The remaining variables did not demonstrate a significant relationship, although a non-significant trend was observed for the social class of the head of the household. We highlight that the educational level of the mother was not related to the risk of emotional problems in our regression model, which may be attributable to the similarly low educational level of the mothers in both low- and medium-SES groups.

The children in the low-SES group were much more vulnerable to internalizing and externalizing clinical problems in comparison to those in the medium-SES group, and a large proportion of them suffered from clinical depressive disorders. Among previous studies on the risk of low SES for the mental health of children and adolescents, some found that the SES had a greater impact on externalizing disorders (31), whereas we found a generalized association with all problems as well as social competence.

In comparison to the medium-SES group, a greater effect size was observed for the sub-scales related to externalizing behavior in the low-SES group. Previous studies have shown that toxins, ambient noise, and neighbor quality, among many other environmental conditions, are associated with a larger number of these types of problem, including impaired impulse control and higher levels of aggression (15).

Finally, although the children under study had no previous diagnosis of psychological or cognitive impairment, the results for the low-SES group showed very high values in subscales for depression and aggressive behavior, among others. As previously reported (30), it appears to be difficult to correctly identify severe externalizing and internalizing symptoms in children, hampering their receipt of appropriate treatments or interventions.

The present data underline the need for early interventions in infancy to reduce mental health problems among children and adolescents in situations of chronic poverty, as previously proposed (3948). These should consider multiple aspects related to the well-being of children, including performance at school and housing conditions. It is also desirable to involve parents and to adjust interventions to the reality of family life in socially disadvantaged settings. Shonkoff, Richter, van der Gaag, and Bhutta (49) reviewed interventions designed to improve the survival and development of children with low SES and concluded that the combination of nutritional interventions and psychosocial stimulation was the most widely supported approach.

Given that this was not a longitudinal study, it was not possible to rule out the effect of other variables on the differences observed among the three age groups. Future longitudinal studies are needed to control the effect of the exposure to low-SES, as well as other central variables regarding the SES such as physical conditions at home, family income, and the perceived level of stress. A further limitation was the inability to differentiate among the effects on socio-emotional development of specific aspects of low-SES (e.g., poverty, malnutrition, infant abuse, etc.). However, we contribute the first report of this nature in a population of Ecuadorian children, and the results will help in the development of programs to screen for mental health problems in children in disadvantaged settings and to detect indicators of cognitive, emotional, and social vulnerability.

Investigation of the different factors underlying the relationship between a disadvantaged environment and mental health is beyond the scope of our study. Various hypotheses have been developed, including the benefits for learning of a more stimulating and protected environment (50) and the negative effects of a low SES on the development of brain circuits and metabolism-regulating systems, increasing the likelihood of long-term problems in learning, behavior, mental, and physical health (51). It has been observed that the experience of multiple social and economic stressors generates emotional problems related to fear and anxiety in young people, increasing disruptive behavior and alterations in executive function and self-regulation (52). Further research is warranted to elucidate these issues.

Conclusion

In conclusion, schoolchildren with low SES in a developing country had more emotional disorders, including externalizing and internalizing problems, in comparison to those with medium SES. Housing risk index and school competence emerged as the main predictors of the children's CBCL scores. These findings support the need for short-term and long-term preventive programs to counter the negative effects of social deprivation. Future research is required on emotional variables in children with low SES and on related aspects, including the influence of genetics and the role of specific brain mechanisms. It is of particular interest to determine whether the trend to a greater impact of low SES at higher age (11 vs. 7 years of age) detected in this study continues up to adulthood.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding authors.

Ethics Statement

Written consent was obtained from the parents/guardians of the children for their participation in the study, which was approved by the ethical committee of the local University (Ref: A3/042954/11).

Author Contributions

MNP-M, CB-G, FC-Q, and MP-G designed the tasks and collected the data. MNP-M, MF-A, AF, and CB-G undertook the statistical analysis, and MNP-M wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported by the Spanish Agency for International Development Cooperation (AECID) [A3/042954/11] (PI: FC-Q) and Conselleria d'Educació, Investigació, Cultura I Esport de la Generalitat Valenciana (R+D+i projects developed by emerging research groups) [GV/2017/166] (PI: MF-A).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to Richard Davies and Layla Davies-Jiménez for assistance with the English version.

References

  • 1. Boivin MJ, Kakooza AM, Warf BC, Davidson LL, Grigorenko EL. Reducing neurodevelopmental disorders and disability through research and interventions. Nature (2015) 19(527):S155–60. 10.1038/nature16029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Harmony T. Factores que inciden en el desarrollo del sistema nervioso del niño. In: Corsi M, editor. Aproximaciones de las neurociencias a la conducta, 2nd ed. México DF: Editorial Manual Moderno; (2004). p. 148–63. [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Aber JL, Jones SM, Cohen J. The impact of poverty on the mental health and development of very young children. In: Zeanah CH, editor. Handbook of infant mental health. New York: Guilford Press; (2000). p. 113–28. [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Bøe T, Sivertsen B, Heiervang E, Goodman R, Lundervold AJ, Hysing M. Socioeconomic status and child mental health: The role of parental emotional well-being and parenting practices. J Abnormal Child Psychol (2014) 42(5):705–15. 10.1007/s10802-013-9818-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Brito NH, Noble KG. Socioeconomic status and structural brain development. Front In Neurosci (2014) 8:1–12. 10.3389/fnins.2014.00276 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Ghosh S, Chowdhury SD, Chandra AM, Ghosh T. Grades of under nutrition and socioeconomic status influence cognitive development in school children of Kolkata. Am J Phys Anthropol (2015) 156:274–85. 10.1002/ajpa.22648 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Finch JE, Obradovic J. Unique effects of socioeconomic and emotional parental challenges on children's executive functions. J Appl Dev Psychol (2017) 52:126–37. 10.1016/j.appdev.2017.07.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Hackman DA, Farah MJ, Meaney MJ. Socioeconomic status and the brain: mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nat Rev Neurosci (2010) 11(9):651. 10.1038/nrn2897 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Lipina S, Posner MI. The impact of poverty on the development of brain networks. Front In Hum Neurosci (2012) 6:238. 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00238 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Sektnan M, McClelland MM, Acock A, Morrison FJ. Relations between early family risk, children's behavioral regulation, and academic achievement. Early Childhood Res Q (2010) 25(4):464–79. 10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.02.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Valsilyeva M, Dearing E, Ivanova A, Shen C, Kardanova F. Testing the family investment in Russia: Estimating indirect effects of SES and parental beliefs on the literacy skills of first-grades. Early Childhood Res Q (2018) 42:11–20. 10.1016/j.ecresq.2017.08.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Bromberger JT, Schott LL, Matthews KA, Kravitz HM, Harlow SD, Montez JK. Childhood socioeconomic circumstances and depressive symptom burden across 15-years of follow-up during midlife: Study of Women's Health Across the Nation (SWAN). Arch Womens Ment Health (2017) 20:495–504. 10.1007/s00737-017-0747-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Burneo-Garcés C, Cruz-Quintana F, Pérez-García M, Fernández-Alcántara M, Fasfous A, Pérez-Marfil MN. Interaction between socioeconomic status and cognitive development in children aged 7, 9, and 11 years: a cross-sectional study. Dev Neuropsychol (2019) 44(1):1–16. 10.1080/87565641.2018.1554662 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Mills-Koonce WR, Willoughby M, Garrett-Peters P, Wagner N, Vernon-Feagans L, The Familiy Lifer Project Key Investigation The interplay among socioeconomic status, household chaos, and parenting in the prediction of child conduct problems and callous-unemotional behaviours. Dev Psychopathol (2016) 28:757–71. 10.1017/S0954579416000298 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Evans GW, Kantrowitz E. Socioeconomic status and health: the potential role of environmental risk exposure. Annu Rev Public Health (2002) 23(1):303–31. 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.23.112001.112349 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Brooks-Gunn J, Duncan GJ. The effects of poverty on children. Future Children (1997) 7(2):55–71. 10.2307/1602387 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Kumar M, Amugune B, Madeghe B, Wambua GN, Osok J, Polkonikova-Wamoto A, et al. Mechanisms associated with maternal adverse childhood experiences on offspring's mental health in Nairobi informal settlements: a mediational model testing approach. BMC Psychiatry (2018) 18(1):381. 10.1186/s12888-018-1953-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Korzeniowski C, Ison MS, Difabio H. Principales predictores del desarrollo neurocognitivo en niños de contextos socialmente vulnerables. In: Saforcadaand E, Fariña O, editors. Neurociencias Aplicadas: Medioambiente, desarrollo humano y bienestarcomunitario. Nuevostiempos: Universidad Nacional de Avellaneda; (2017). p. 65–88. [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Evans GW, Kim P. Childhood poverty, chronic stress, self-regulation, and coping. Child Dev Perspect (2013) 7(1):43–8. 10.1111/cdep.12013 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Hackman DA, Gallop R, Evans GW, Farah MJ. Socioeconomic status and executive function: Developmental trajectories and mediation. Dev Sci (2015) 18(5):686–702. 10.1111/desc.12246 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Duncan GJ, Magnuson K, Kalil A, Ziol-Guest K. The importance of early childhood poverty. Soc Indic Res (2012) 108(1):87–98. 10.1007/s11205-011-9867-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. DelGiudice M. Middle childhood: an evolutionary-developmental synthesis. In: Halfon N, Forrest C, Lerner R, Faustman E, editors. Handbook of Life Course Health Development. Cham: Springer; (2018). p. 95–107. 10.1007/978-3-319-47143-3_5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol (2013) 64:135–68. 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Deutz MH, Vossen HG, De Haan AD, Deković M, Van Baar AL, Prinzie P. Normative development of the child behavior checklist dysregulation profile from early childhood to adolescence: associations with personality pathology. Dev Psychopathol (2018) 30(2):437–47. 10.1017/S0954579417000955 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Merz EC, Tottenham N, Noble KG. Socioeconomic status, amygdale volume, and internalizing symptoms in children and adolescents. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol (2018) 47(2):312–23. 10.1080/15374416.2017.1326122 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Miller BL, Benson DF, y Johnson JK. Frontal lobes: Clinical and anatomic aspects. In Feinberg TE, Farah MJ, editors. Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychology. New York: McGraw Hill; (2003). P. 385–99. [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Evans GW, English K. The environment of poverty: multiple stressor exposure, psychophysiological stress, and socioemotional adjustment. ChildDevelopment (2002) 73:1238–48. 10.1111/1467-8624.00469 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Raver CC. Placing emotional self-regulation in sociocultural and socioeconomic contexts. Child Dev (2004) 75(2):346–53. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00676.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Wanless SB, McClelland MM, Tominey SL, Acock AC. The Influence of Demographic Risk Factors on Children's Behavioral Regulationin Prekindergarten and Kindergarten. Early Educ Dev (2011) 22(3):461–88. 10.1080/10409289.2011.536132 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. World Health Organization World health statistics: a snapshot of global health. (2012). (No. WHO/IER/HSI/12.1). Ginebra: World Health Organization. 10.7591/cornell/9780801450655.003.0001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Reiss F. Socioeconomic inequalities and mental health problems in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Soc Sci & Med (2013) 90:24–31. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.04.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Manrique-Millones DL, Ghesquière P, Van Leeuwen K. (2014). Relationship among parenting behavior, SES, academic achievement and psychosocial functioning in peruvian children. Universitas Psychologica, 13(2):639–50. 10.11144/Javeriana.UPSY13-2.rpba [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Hinojosa-Fierro ES. (2015). Relación entre niveles de hemoglobina y estado nutricional en niños y niñas de 3 a 5 añosbeneficiarios del Proyecto Compassion Internacional Guaranda 2014 [Relationship between hemoglobin levels and nutritional status in children aged 3 to 5 years beneficiaries of the Compassion International Guaranda Project2014] (Bachelor thesis, Escuela Superior Politécnica de Chimborazo). Retrieved from http://dspace.espoch.edu.ec/handle/123456789/7733.
  • 34. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms & Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families; (2001). [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Lacalle-Sisteré M, Domènech-Massons JM, Granero-Pérez R, Ezpeleta-Ascaso L. Validity of the DSM-oriented scales of the child behavior checklist and youth self-report. Psicothema (2014) 26(3):364–71. 10.7334/psicothema2013.342 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Sardinero E, Pedreira JL, Muñiz J. El cuestionario CBCL de Achenbach: adaptación española y aplicaciones clínicoepidemiológicas [The Achenbach CBCL questionnaire: Spanish adaptation and clinical and epidemiological applications]. Clínica y Salud (1997) 8(3):447–80. [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Multicultural Supplement to the Manual for the ASEBA Preschool Forms & Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families; (2010). [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Kajantie E. Fetal origins of stress-related adult disease. Ann New York Acad Sci (2006) 1083:11–27. 10.1196/annals.1367.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Arruabarrena I, de Paúl J. Early intervention programs for children and families: theoretical and empirical bases supporting their social and economic efficiency. Psychosocial Intervention (2012) 21:117–27. 10.5093/in2012a18 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Heckman J. Skill formation and the economics of investing in disadvantaged children. Science (2006) 312:1900–2. 10.1126/science.1128898 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Heckman J. School, skills, and synapses. Econ Inq (2008) 46:289–324. 10.1111/j.1465-7295.2008.00163.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Knudsen E, Heckman J, Cameron J, Shonkoff J. Economic, neurobiological and behavioral perspectives on building America's Future Workforce. PNAS (2006) 103:10155–62. 10.1073/pnas.0600888103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. National Scientific Council on the Developing Child Young children develop in an environment of relationships (Working paper 1). (2004). U. S. A. Harvard: Harvard University. [Google Scholar]
  • 44. National Scientific Council on the Developing Child (2005/2014). Excessive Stress Disrupts the Architecture of the Developing Brain: Working Paper 3. Updated Edition. USA: Harvard University; Retrieved from http://www.developingchild.harvard.edu. [Google Scholar]
  • 45. National Scientific Council on the Developing Child The timing and quality of early experiences combine to shape brain architecture (Working paper 5). (2007). Harvard University. [Google Scholar]
  • 46. RAND Corporation (2005). Proven benefits of early childhood interventions. (USA: Rand Corporation; ). Retrieved from http://www.rand.org/content/dam/rand/pubs/research_briefs/2005/RAND RB9145.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  • 47. RAND Corporation (2008). What does economics tell us about early childhood policy? USA: Rand Corporation; Retrieved from http://www.rand.org/content/dam/rand/pubs/research_briefs/2008/RAND RB9352.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Washington State Institute on Public Policy (2012). Return on Investment: Evidence-based options to improve statewide outcomes. April Update. (USA: Washington State Institute on Public Policy; ). Retrieved from http://www.wsipp.wa.gov/rptfiles/12-04-1201.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Shonkoff JP, Richter L, van der Gaag J, Bhutta ZA. An integrated scientific framework for child survival and early childhood development. Pediatrics (2012) 129(2):e460–72. 10.1542/peds.2011-0366 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Ursache A, Merz EC, Melvin S, Meyer J, Noble KG. Socioeconomic status, hair cortisol and internalizing symptoms in parents and children. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2017) 78:142–50. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.01.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Shonkoff JP. Building a new biodevelopmental framework to guide the future of early childhood policy. Child Dev (2010) 81(1):357–67. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01399.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Shonkoff JP, Philips DA. From Neurons to Neighborhoods. The Science of Early Childhood Development. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; (2000). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding authors.


Articles from Frontiers in Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES