Skip to main content
Annals of Clinical and Translational Neurology logoLink to Annals of Clinical and Translational Neurology
. 2020 Feb 7;7(2):219–228. doi: 10.1002/acn3.50987

Cognition in multiple system atrophy: a single‐center cohort study

Sabine Eschlböck 1, Margarete Delazer 2, Florian Krismer 1, Thomas Bodner 2, Alessandra Fanciulli 1, Beatrice Heim 1, Antonio Heras Garvin 3, Christine Kaindlstorfer 1, Elfriede Karner 2, Katherina Mair 1, Christoph Rabensteiner 1, Cecilia Raccagni 1, Klaus Seppi 1, Werner Poewe 1, Gregor K Wenning 1,
PMCID: PMC7034507  PMID: 32031752

Abstract

Objective

Cognitive impairment in multiple system atrophy (MSA) is common, but remain poorly characterized. We evaluated cognitive and behavioral features in MSA patients and assessed between‐group differences for MSA subtypes and the effect of orthostatic hypotension (OH) on cognition.

Methods

This retrospective study included 54 patients with clinical diagnosis of possible and probable MSA referred to the Department of Neurology at Medical University of Innsbruck between 2000 and 2018. Neurological work‐up included comprehensive neuropsychological testing including Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD‐plus) test battery, Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB), digit span test (DST), clock drawing task (CLOX1), and Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS‐D).

Results

The mean MMSE score was 27.6 points. Overall, slight to moderate cognitive impairment was noted in up to 40% of patients, with predominant impairment of executive function and verbal memory. Patients with the cerebellar variant performed significantly worse than patients with the parkinsonian type (P < 0.05) in a screening of executive functions (FAB) and in phonemic verbal fluency. Depression and anxiety scores were elevated in 28% and 22% of MSA patients, respectively. Cognitive profile, depression, and anxiety levels were comparable between patients with and without OH.

Interpretation

Cognitive deficits are relatively frequent in MSA and primarily affect executive functions and verbal memory. Future comparative studies including Parkinson dementia, Lewy body disease, and MSA cases with and without OH are required to elucidate disease‐specific cognitive profiles in these synucleinopathies and to examine the influence of cardiovascular autonomic dysfunction on cognitive function in MSA.

Introduction

Multiple system atrophy (MSA) is a rare neurodegenerative disorder clinically defined by severe autonomic failure, parkinsonism, and/or cerebellar ataxia.1, 2 Neuropathologically, glial cytoplasmic inclusions (GCIs) are found particularly in the striatonigral and olivopontocerebellar systems of MSA brains.3 According to the predominant motor phenotype, MSA can be subclassified into a parkinsonian (MSA‐P) and a cerebellar variant (MSA‐C).4 In recent years, nonmotor and nonautonomic symptoms have been increasingly recognized in MSA.5, 6 The degree of cognitive impairment in MSA patients ranges from normal to moderate decline and affects memory, executive, attentional, and visuospatial functions.7, 8, 9 Accordingly, cognitive impairment has been reported in up to 37 % of neuropathologically proven MSA cases.9, 10, 11 In contrast, severe cognitive decline that significantly disrupts daily living is uncommon in MSA; hence, dementia has been regarded as a nonsupporting feature in the current diagnostic criteria.4 Comparative studies on cognitive impairment in both motor subtypes have been conducted but reported heterogeneous results.8, 12, 13, 14, 15, 16, 17 Kawai and colleagues observed a multidomain cognitive decline in MSA‐P but not in MSA‐C.12 Another study reported a more pronounced executive and verbal memory dysfunction in MSA‐C,14 and a third found comparable cognitive profiles in both variants.8 Cognitive deficits in MSA may be explained by underlying degeneration of basal ganglia and to secondarily disrupted striato‐pallido‐thalamocortical circuits.7, 18 In patients with cerebellar disorders, deficits in executive, memory, visuospatial, and language domains have been shown, thus indicating that the cerebellum plays an important role in higher order functions,19 and further support the concept of "subcortical dementia." Beyond that, it is also suggested that with disease progression a primary cortical involvement becomes apparent.9, 20, 21

It is well established that anxiety and depression are both frequently found in MSA patients22, 23, 24, 25 and possibly influence cognitive function. Additionally, orthostatic hypotension (OH) may also be associated with cognitive impairment in MSA, although the underlying mechanisms have not been established yet.26, 27

A recent study identified Dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) as the most common misdiagnosis of MSA during life,9 highlighting the need for better characterization of MSA‐specific patterns of cognitive dysfunction. Previous reports on cognitive and behavioral features in MSA patients have produced inconsistent results, in part due to different definitions of cognitive impairment, nonstandardized assessment methods or lack of standardized norms. Therefore, the objective of this study was the evaluation of cognitive and behavioral features in patients with MSA by the use of a standardized and validated neuropsychological test battery with age‐ and education‐specific norms. Moreover, we aimed to assess group differences between MSA subtypes as well as between patients with and without OH.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

We retrospectively reviewed the medical records of 54 MSA patients referred to the movement disorder unit at the Department of Neurology, Medical University of Innsbruck, between January 2000 and May 2018. Patients were clinically diagnosed by movement disorder specialists with possible or probable MSA‐P or MSA‐C according to revised Gilman criteria.4 At time of neuropsychological examination, 18.5% of patients fulfilled the diagnostic criteria of possible MSA. However, all these patients fulfilled the criteria for probable MSA 24 months later at last available follow‐up and therefore were included in the final analysis. The following demographic and clinical information were abstracted from medical records: gender, education level, age at symptom onset and neuropsychological examination, subjective response to levodopa, disease duration, global disability (Unified Multiple System Atrophy Rating Scale UMSARS part IV,28 Hoehn and Yahr (H&Y) stage29), and medication. Median disease duration was selected to define a cut‐off value for comparison of the cognitive profile of early versus late disease course. Additionally, results from head‐up‐tilt or, if not available, from standing test, were abstracted to analyze the effect of orthostatic hypotension and supine hypertension on cognition. Age of symptom onset was defined as occurrence of either motor symptoms (parkinsonism and/or cerebellar symptoms) or autonomic dysfunction (symptoms of OH and/or urogenital features). OH was defined as a systolic blood pressure (BP) drop of at least 20 mmHg and/or a reduction in diastolic blood pressure of at least 10 mmHg within 3 min of standing or head‐up tilt.30, 31 Neurogenic supine hypertension (SH) was defined as systolic BP ≥ 140 mmHg and/or diastolic BP ≥ 90 mmHg in supine position.32 Moreover, mean standing BP after 3 min of head‐up tilt or standing test, mean BP change after 3 min of head‐up tilt or standing test, and mean supine BP were calculated. Given the retrospective nature of the following study, the quality of medical records was inconsistent. Therefore, inadequate documentation was considered as exclusion criteria. Patients not able to read and write as well as severe psychiatric comorbidity were also excluded.

This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Medical University of Innsbruck and conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Neuropsychological and behavioral assessment

The following standardized and validated neuropsychological test battery was applied by neuropsychologists. Test scores were obtained from medical records.

CERAD‐plus battery

All participants performed the CERAD‐plus battery33, 34 assessing global cognition, object naming, verbal memory (learning, recall, and recognition), constructive abilities (copying geometrical shapes), figural memory (reproduction of geometrical shapes), semantic (animals/minute) and phonological (s‐words/minute) word fluency, psychomotor speed (TMT‐A), and cognitive flexibility (Trail Making B/Trail Making A). TMT (B/A) ratio was calculated to diminish the influence of motor impairment on part B providing a purer measure of executive function.35 When patients could not finish TMT‐B, a maximum score of 400 sec was assumed and entered in the database. The CERAD‐plus battery provides norms adjusted for age, education, and gender, allowing the transformation of the single scales’ raw scores into z‐scores based on a large and representative standardization sample. In this study, cognitive impairment was defined as a z‐score below −1.28, collapsing slight to severe deficits, which has the best discriminative power.36

DST

All subjects performed the digit span test37 to evaluate attention span (digit span forward, DSF) and working memory (digit span backward, DSB). Performance was evaluated according to age‐scaled norms.

CLOX1

The clock drawing task38 was performed in 52 patients and was used to evaluate executive functions. The cut‐off was set at ≤10.

FAB

The Frontal Assessment Battery39 is a screening test to evaluate frontal‐executive and behavioral functions composed of six subtests and a maximum possible score of 18. An age and education‐specific cut‐off was used (<5th percentile) to define patients that performed in the impaired range40. It has to be noted that data were available for 43 patients.

HADS

The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale was applied in 50 patients to evaluate anxiety and depression.41 Cut‐off scores for detection of anxiety or depression measured by The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) were ≥11/21.42

Statistics

All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS (IBM SPSS Statistics, Chicago, IL, USA, Version 24). A chi‐square or Fisher's exact test was used to calculate differences in categorical data as appropriate. For analyses of continuous variables and demographic data, Mann–Whitney U test or t‐test was conducted according to the distribution of data. The significance level was set at P < 0.05. Pearson’ s correlation test was used to evaluate the relationship between raw scores of the CERAD‐plus battery, DST, FAB, CLOX1, and clinical variables, including disease duration, age of onset, UMSARS part IV, Hoehn and Yahr, orthostatic hypotension, supine hypertension, mean standing BP, mean supine BP, mean BP change, anticholinergics, depression, and anxiety. Due to multiple testing, P values were corrected by the Bonferroni method.

Results

Demographic and clinical data

In this study we enrolled 54 patients of which 55.6% (n = 30) were men. Thirty‐nine (72.2%) patients predominantly had parkinsonian symptoms (MSA‐P), and 15 (27.8%) mainly cerebellar features (MSA‐C). Average age of disease onset was 60 years without any significant difference between MSA‐P and MSA‐C patients (P = 0.827). 81.5% (n = 44) fulfilled diagnostic criteria for probable MSA. The mean disease duration between disease onset and neuropsychological assessment was 4 years (±2.9). Patients were educated on average for 10 years, which was comparable between MSA‐P and MSA‐C (P = 0.161). In 57.4% of individuals, cardiovascular autonomic failure was confirmed by tilt table testing or standing test. A high proportion of MSA‐P patients (76.9%) received levodopa therapy, in contrast to a minority of 13.3% in the MSA‐C group (P < 0.001). No patient had a persistent subjective response to levodopa. Additionally, subjects were treated with antidepressants (53.7%), anticholinergics (20.4%), and hypnotic agents (22.2%). Demographic and clinical features are summarized in Table 1.

Table 1.

Demographic and clinical characteristics of patients with multiple system atrophy.

  Overall MSA‐P MSA‐C P
n (%) 54 (100) 39 (72.2) 15 (27.8)  
Gender
Male, n (%) 30 (55.6) 20 (51.3) 10 (66.7) NS
Diagnostic certainty
Possible, n (%) 10 (18.5) 6 (15.4) 4 (26.7) NS
Probable, n (%) 44 (81.5) 33 (84.6) 11 (73.3) NS
Clinical features
Education in years, mean (SD) 10.2 (2.8) 9.9 (2.6) 11.0 (3.0) NS
Age symptom onset, mean (SD) 59.8 (7.5) 59.7 (7.6) 60.1 (7.6) NS
Age at neuropsychological assessment, mean (SD) 63.9 (6.5) 64.2 (6.2) 63.3 (7.5) NS
UMSARS part IV, mean (SD) 3.2 (1.0) 3.3 (1.0) 3.1 (1.0) NS
H&Y, mean (SD) 3.3 (1.0) 3.3 (1.0) 3.4 (1.0) NS
Orthostatic hypotension, n (%) 31 (57.4) 25 (64.1) 6 (40.0) NS
Supine hypertension, n (%) 23 (42.6) 18 (46.2) 5 (33.3) NS
Medication
Levodopa, n (%) 32 (59.3) 30 (76.9) 2 (13.3) <0.001
Anticholinergics, n (%) 11 (20.4) 10 (25.6) 1 (6.7) NS
Antidepressants, n (%) 29 (53.7) 21 (53.8) 8 (53.3) NS
Hypnotic agents, n (%) 12 (22.2) 8 (20.5) 4 (26.7) NS

MSA‐C, multiple system atrophy cerebellar subtype; MSA‐P, multiple system atrophy Parkinsonism subtype; UMSARS, Unified Multiple System Atrophy Rating Scale part IV; H&Y, Hoehn and Yahr; SD, standard deviation.

P‐values are shown in domains with significant group differences (ns not significant).

Neurocognitive assessment

Comparison of raw scores

Raw scores are summarized in detail in Table 2. MSA‐C patients performed significantly worse compared to MSA‐P in the FAB score (P = 0.036) and in phonemic verbal fluency (P = 0.024).

Table 2.

Neuropsychological test battery (raw scores) and between‐group differences for MSA‐P vs. MSA‐C and OH + vs. OH−.

  Overall MSA‐P MSA‐C P OH+ OH− P
Global cognitive status, mean (SD)
MMSE 27.6 (2.4) 27.6 (2.6) 27.7 (1.9) NS 27.4 (2.8) 28.0 (1.8) NS
Memory function, mean (SD)
CERAD word list
Wordlist learning sum 17.9 (3.9) 17.6 (3.8) 18.7 (3.9) NS 17.6 (3.9) 18.3 (3.7) NS
Wordlist delayed recall 5.8 (2.0) 5.7 (1.8) 6.1 (2.5) NS 5.7 (2.0) 6.0 (2.0) NS
Wordlist savings (%) 78.3 (22.0) 77.6 (23.2) 80.4 (19.2) NS 76.1 (24.7) 81.4 (17.8) NS
Wordlist recognition 9.1 (1.1) 9.1 (1.2) 9.0 (1.1) NS 9.1 (1.2) 9.1 (1.1) NS
False positive 0.3 (0.7) 0.3 (0.6) 0.3 (0.8) NS 0.4 (0.8) 0.2 (0.5) NS
CERAD constructional praxis recall
Constructional praxis recall 8.4 (2.3) 8.5 (2.2) 8.0 (2.4) NS 8.1 (1.9) 8.8 (2.7) NS
Constructional praxis savings (%) 80.2 (22.5) 82.2 (20.0) 75.1 (28.2) NS 77.6 (19.1) 83.7 (26.3) NS
Executive function, mean (SD)
CERAD verbal fluency
Animal naming 18.0 (6.1) 18.6 (6.6) 16.3) (4.4) NS 17.7 (6.6) 18.4 (5.5) NS
S‐words 10.4 (4.4) 11.3 (4.5) 8.2 (3.4) 0.024 10.8 (4.6) 9.9 (4.2) NS
Trail Making A 58.3 (23.1) 54.7 (21.7) 66.3 (24.8) NS 57.2 (23.6) 59.5 (23.0) NS
Trail Making B/A 3.6 (4.1) 3.9 (4.8) 2.8 (1.1) NS 4.3 (5.2) 2.6 (0.6) NS
CLOX 1 11.4 (1.9) 11.5 (1.9) 11.1 (2.0) NS 11.4 (1.8) 11.4 (2.0) NS
FAB 14.7 (2.7) 15.2 (2.7) 13.3 (2.6) 0.036 14.8 (2.9) 14.5 (2.4) NS
Attention span, mean (SD)
Digit span forwards 6.5 (1.8) 6.7 (1.8) 6.1 (1.5) NS 6.8 (2.0) 6.2 (1.4) NS
Working memory, mean (SD)
Digit span backwards 5.1 (1.3) 5.3 (1.2) 4.6 (1.3) NS 5.1 (1.3) 5.0 (1.2) NS
Language functions, mean (SD)
Boston Naming Test 13.8 (1.3) 13.7 (1.4) 14.1 (1.2) NS 13.8 (1.2) 13.8 (1.5) NS
Visuospatial functions, mean (SD)
Constructional praxis copy 10.3 (0.9) 10.3 (1.0) 10.3 (0.8) NS 10.3 (1.1) 10.3 (0.8) NS
Mood and behavior, mean (SD)
HADS‐D anxiety 7.6 (4.2) 7.8 (4.3) 7.1 (3.9) NS 8.0 (4.5) 7.0 (3.7) NS
HADS‐D depression 7.4 (4.0) 7.7 (3.8) 6.8 (4.5) NS 8.2 (4.2) 6.2 (3.6) NS

MMSE, The Mini‐Mental State Examination; CERAD, The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease; CLOX, The clock drawing task; FAB, The Frontal Assessment Battery; HADS‐D, The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; MSA‐C, multiple system atrophy cerebellar subtype; MSA‐P, multiple system atrophy parkinsonism subtype; OH+, with orthostatic hypotension; OH−, without orthostatic hypotension; SD, standard deviation.

Classification according to age‐, gender‐, and education adjusted z‐scores

Following age‐, gender‐, and education adjusted z‐scores (CERAD‐plus), global cognition (MMSE) was impaired in 35.2% (n = 19) of MSA cases with a substantial cognitive decline in four patients (7.4%). Deficits in verbal memory (learning, recall, and recognition) were noted in up to 37.7% (n = 20) as demonstrated by word list learning. 23.1% (n = 12) of patients showed deficits in reproduction of geometrical shapes, evaluating figural memory. Attention span and working memory were widely maintained and reduced in only 11.1% (n = 6) and 14.8% (n = 8), respectively. Results of the FAB indicated executive dysfunction in 41.9% (n = 18/43). Semantic (animals/minute) and phonological (s‐words/minute) verbal fluency exhibited a decrease in 24.1% (n = 13) and 21.3% (n = 10), respectively. Psychomotor speed was impaired in 32.7% (n = 16) and cognitive flexibility in 21.3% (n = 10). Difficulties in the clock drawing test was noted in 23.1% (n = 12) and in object naming in 20.4% (n = 11) of patients. Constructional praxis was preserved in most cases (impaired in 13.2%; n = 7).

Comparison MSA‐C and MSA‐P subtype

Interestingly, MSA‐C patients showed impairment in the FAB battery more frequently than MSA‐P (80% vs. 30%, P = 0.003). Figure 1 shows the proportion of MSA‐C and MSA‐P patients scoring in the impaired range. Comparison of raw scores yielded also a significant difference in phonemic word fluency, which, however, was not found in age‐scaled norms. In addition, to compare the cognitive profile of early versus late disease course a cut‐off value of 3 years has been selected. Adjusted for age, gender, and education a significant difference was only present for global cognition (defined as MMSE), which was impaired in 21% of patients with short disease duration compared to 50% of patients with disease duration over three years (P = 0.045). Therefore, with exception of global cognition status, deficits were already present in early stage of disease and no significant difference has been found between MSA‐P and MSA‐C.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Percentage of multiple system atrophy (MSA) patients with cognitive impairment for MSA subtypes (MSA‐C vs. MSA‐P). The proportion of patients scoring in the impaired range in the FAB battery was significant higher in patients with MSA‐C compared to MSA‐P (80% vs. 30%, P = 0.003). No significant difference was found in the other subtests. For definition of cut‐off score see method section. MSA‐C, multiple system atrophy cerebellar subtype; MSA‐P, multiple system atrophy parkinsonism subtype; MMSE, The Mini‐Mental State Examination; CLOX, The clock drawing task; FAB, The Frontal Assessment Battery; *P < 0.05.

Comparison OH+ and OH−

The neuropsychological profile (raw scores and z scores) of patients with preserved blood pressure regulation (without orthostatic hypotension and supine hypertension (OH−/SH−) was similar to patients with orthostatic hypotension and with/without supine hypertension (OH+/SH−, OH+/SH+) (Table 2 and Fig. 2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Percentage of multiple system atrophy (MSA) patients with cognitive impairment for patients with and without preserved blood pressure regulation (OH− vs. OH+/SH− vs. OH+/SH+). The cognitive profile was comparable in patients with and without cardiovascular autonomic failure. For definition of cut‐off score see method section. MSA‐C, multiple system atrophy cerebellar subtype; MSA‐P, multiple system atrophy parkinsonism subtype; OH, orthostatic hypotension; MMSE, The Mini‐Mental State Examination; CLOX, The clock drawing task; FAB, The Frontal Assessment Battery; SH, supine hypertension.

Assessment of anxiety and depression

Average score of HADS‐D was 7.6 (±4.2) for anxiety and 7.4 (±4.0) for depression. Assessment of anxiety and depression showed an increase in 30% of patients (HADS‐D anxiety 22% n = 11, HADS‐D depression 28% n = 14). No differences were observed between MSA‐P and MSA‐C or OH + and OH−.

Cognitive impairment and clinical variables

Correlation analyses indicated that UMSARS part IV and H&Y were inversely correlated with the following neuropsychological subtests: MMSE (r = −0.429, P = 0.001; r = −0.409, P = 0.002) and semantic verbal fluency (r = −0.493, P = 0.001, r = −0.508, P = 0.001). Age of onset correlated positively with Trail making test A (r = 0.475, P = 0.001). We found no significant association between cognitive deficits and depression, anxiety, anticholinergics, mean standing BP, mean BP change, mean supine BP, the presence of OH (see Table 2) or SH.

Discussion

In the last decade, awareness of cognitive dysfunction and neuropsychiatric features as non‐motor features in MSA has increased. Several studies found a broad spectrum of deficits in cognition and behavior in MSA.7 However, cognitive patterns in MSA remain poorly characterized, as differences in definitions, methods, and study designs have led to inconsistent results.7

The present cohort showed a relatively good performance of general cognitive ability (as indicated by MMSE) that was only mildly to moderately impaired in 35% of patients. These findings are supported by previous studies that have shown mild cognitive deficits in MSA patients.43, 44, 45, 46, 47 In accordance with data from postmortem studies,48, 49, 50, 51 a more pronounced cognitive impairment, with MMSE ≤24 points, was present in 7.4% of cases, indicating dementia in a small group of patients. Nonetheless, it has to be mentioned that the sensitivity of MMSE to detect executive dysfunction and language deficits remains controversial.52 Therefore, in this study a comprehensive neuropsychological test battery including the CERAD‐plus test battery,53 the digit span test,37 the Frontal Assessment Battery,54 the clock drawing task,38 and the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale42 were used. Our study shows that approximately 40% of patients had a documented slight to moderate cognitive impairment in at least one domain, wherein executive functions were most often affected in line with previous findings.8, 46, 54, 55, 56 In detail, executive dysfunction in MSA comprised deficits in semantic and phonemic verbal fluency12, 56, 57, 58 as well as processing speed, cognitive flexibility, and working memory,14, 16, 45 being impaired in up to 42% in this cohort. Whereas verbal memory was affected in a similar percentage, which is in agreement with previous reports,8, 43, 56 figural memory and attention span were widely preserved. With regards to visuospatial functions, previous studies reported conflicting results, with some suggesting preserved function43, 59 and others reporting difficulties in this domain.12, 45, 56 In this study, only a small proportion of MSA patients showed impaired visuospatial function. Confirming previous reports, 20% of MSA patients showed deficits in object naming.45, 55, 60 The following results emphasize that patients with MSA and DLB show a similar pattern of neurocognitive deficits, which still remains a diagnostic dilemma. A comparison of cognitive profile in patients with synucleinopathies has been performed, which has shown that executive functions, memory, and visuospatial functions were impaired in both groups.45 Nonetheless, the level of cognitive impairment was severe in patients with DLB and intermediate in patients with MSA,45 which was also confirmed in the following study. Nonetheless, the cardinal characteristics of the natural history of disease in patients with DLB remain fluctuations in cognition and the presence of visual hallucinations.7 Although this was beyond the scope of the following study and therefore has not been assessed, these major distinctive features are usually not present in patients with MSA.7

So far, few studies have addressed the possible differences between patients with predominant cerebellar or parkinsonian presentation,12, 13, 14, 15, 16, 17 and the focus has been mainly on MSA‐P patients. In this study, a comparison of raw scores revealed lower performance of MSA‐C patients than MSA‐P patients in a screening of executive functions and in phonological verbal fluency. After adjustment for age, education, and gender, a greater proportion of patients with MSA‐C compared to MSA‐P patients were impaired on the FAB. In concordance with these results, cognitive deficits in executive function tests8, 14, 15, 55 including verbal fluency14, 43 and TMT ‐B14 have been reported in MSA‐C. Nonetheless, there are conflicting results and heterogeneous data have been published regarding the differences between MSA‐C and MSA‐P. Previous studies suggested a comparable performance,8, 17, 45 a more prominent decline of executive function and verbal memory in MSA‐C14, 15, 26, 55 or a more pronounced cognitive dysfunction in MSA‐P.12, 13

Although the neuropathological underpinnings of cognitive deficits in MSA remain incompletely understood, it is assumed that due to degeneration of subcortical structures, circuits from the frontal cortex to basal ganglia and thalamus are disrupted,7 leading to cognitive deficits.61 While deafferentation of subcortical structures impact the cognitive decline to a large extent, there is increasing evidence that with disease progression intrinsic cortical pathology becomes more apparent.7 Recently, neuropathological studies reported an association of neuronal cytoplasmatic inclusions in neocortex or limbic regions and cognitive impairment in MSA.9, 20, 21 Additionally, imaging data also suggest that beside atrophy of subcortical regions, thinning of neocortices may contribute to cognitive impairment.61, 62, 63 In this study, MSA‐C patients showed to be more frequently impaired in a screening of executive function, possibly resulting from disruption of cerebrocerebellar circuits. This phenomenon has also been observed in other cerebellar disorders, as the cerebellum functionally modulates higher cognition. Typical symptoms encompass deficits in executive function, language, spatial cognition, affect regulation and they are subsumed under the term "cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome."19

Furthermore, it has to be noted that cognition performance may be related to a number of factors such as mood disturbances26 or blood pressure fluctuations.27 In this cohort, symptoms of depression and anxiety were present in 28% and 22% of MSA patients, respectively. These results are consistent with previous findings reporting a prevalence for depression ranging from 20 to 80%,8, 9, 17, 22, 44, 45 and for anxiety up to 40%.24, 45 As reported previously,8, 12 we did not find any difference in depression and anxiety between the two MSA variants and no significant correlations were observed between cognitive impairment and mood. Thus, cognitive impairments observed in this study cannot be attributed to depressive or anxiety disorders.

This study also evaluated the impact of OH on cognition. Studies in PD suggested that OH is associated with cognitive impairment64, 65, 66, 67 and may underlie a posture‐mediated exacerbation.68, 69 To our knowledge, only few studies have addressed this issue in MSA with controversial findings.16, 46, 51, 70 In this study, we could not identify any difference in cognition in MSA patients with and without OH, and we did not find any correlation between cognitive impairment and cardiovascular dysfunction. Hence, it may be speculated that the influence of cardiovascular autonomic failure on cognition is rather acute and reversible. Nonetheless, due to the retrospective design and incomplete information on duration of orthostatic symptoms in each patient, we could not investigate eventual time‐dependent cumulative effects to this end.

One of the main strengths of this study represents the large sample size of MSA patients that has been recruited from one single center. Moreover, the neuropsychological assessment performed in this study included the CERAD‐plus battery, which allows a standardized assessment of various cognitive functions, an evaluation according to age‐, gender‐, and education‐matched norms and which may be easily used for comparison purposes in other study cohorts. Moreover, to our knowledge, only CERAD subtests have occasionally been applied in previous studies in MSA so far. Nonetheless, some methodological issues, which may have an influence on our conclusions, have to be acknowledged and merit discussion. First, the risk of a selection bias limits this study since, following second consensus criteria, patients with severe cognitive deficits may have been excluded. Moreover, patients were clinically diagnosed and neuropathological diagnosis is not available, possibly leading to a misclassification of a subgroup of patients. Second, given the retrospective nature of the study, no standardized study protocol was available and therefore, a reporting bias cannot be excluded. Third, neuropsychological tests were performed in the sitting position, where patients usually do not experience OH symptoms. Thus, posture‐mediated changes were not assessed.

Conclusion

Despite these inevitable shortcomings, this study reaffirms previous findings of a wide spectrum of cognitive and behavioral impairments in MSA. Slight to moderate deficits frequently involve executive functions and verbal memory, whereas global cognitive ability (as indicated by the MMSE screening) remains intact in the majority of cases. Further comparative studies including PDD (Parkinson dementia), LBD (lewy body disease) and MSA cases and a standardized neuropsychological test battery are required to improve diagnosis in clinical practice.

Conflict of Interest

Sabine Eschlböck: nothing to declare. Margarete Delazer: nothing to declare. Florian Krismer: Research Grant MSA coalition. Thomas Bodner: nothing to declare. Alessandra Fanciulli: nothing to declare. Beatrice Heim: nothing to declare. Antonio Heras Garvin: nothing to declare. Christine Kaindlstorfer: nothing to declare. Elfriede Karner: nothing to declare. Katherina Mair: nothing to declare. Christoph Rabensteiner: nothing to declare. Cecilia Raccagni: Travel Grant MSA coalition. Klaus Seppi: Klaus Seppi reports personal fees from Teva, UCB, Lundbeck, AOP Orphan Pharmaceuticals AG, Roche, Grünenthal and Abbvie, honoraria from the International Parkinson and Movement Disorders Society, research grants from FWF Austrian Science Fund, Michael J. Fox Foundation, and International Parkinson and Movement Disorder Society, outside the submitted work. Werner Poewe: Werner Poewe has received consultancy and lecture fees in relation to clinical drug programs for PD from AbbVie, AstraZeneca, BIAL, Biogen, Biohaven, Britannia, Grünenthal, Intec, Ipsen, Lundbeck, Novartis, Neuroderm, Orion Pharma, Oxford Biomedica, Prexton, Regenera, Roche, Sunovion, Sun Pharma, Takeda, Teva, UCB and Zambon. Royalties: Thieme, Wiley Blackwell, Oxford University Press and Cambridge University Press. Gregor K. Wenning: Gregor Wenning has received consultancy and lecture fees in relation to clinical drug programs for MSA from AbbVie, AstraZeneca, Biogen, Lundbeck, Novartis, Takeda, UCB and Zambon.

Author Contributions

Sabine Eschlböck: Execution of the project, statistical analysis and interpretation of data, writing of the first manuscript. Margarete Delazer: Execution of neuropsychological test battery, execution of the project, statistical analysis and interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript. Florian Krismer: Statistical analysis and interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript, contribution of patients. Thomas Bodner: Execution of neuropsychological test battery, critical revision of the manuscript. Alessandra Fanciulli: Interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript, contribution of patients. Beatrice Heim, Antonio Heras Garvin, Katherina Mair: Conception of the study, Critical revision of the manuscript. Elfriede Karner: Execution of neuropsychological test battery, critical revision of the manuscript. Christoph Rabensteiner: Execution of the project, critical revision of the manuscript. Cecilia Raccagni: Critical revision of the manuscript, contribution of patients. Klaus Seppi, Christine Kaindlstorfer, Werner Poewe: Conception of the study, Critical revision of the manuscript, contribution of patients. Gregor K. Wenning: Conception and organization of the project, statistical analysis and interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript.

Acknowledgments

We thank our patients and families for supporting this research.

Funding Information

None funding information provided.

References

  • 1. Wenning GK, Ben Shlomo Y, Magalhães M, et al. Clinical features and natural history of multiple system atrophy. An analysis of 100 cases. Brain 1994;117(Pt 4):835–845. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Fanciulli A, Wenning G. Multiple‐system atrophy. N Engl J Med 2015;372:249–263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Papp M, Kahn J, Lantos P. Glial cytoplasmic inclusions in the CNS of patients with multiple system atrophy (striatonigral degeneration, olivopontocerebellar atrophy and Shy‐Drager syndrome). J Neurol Sci 1989;94:79–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Gilman S, Wenning G, Low P, et al. Second consensus statement on the diagnosis of multiple system atrophy. Neurology 2008;71:670–676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Jecmenica‐Lukic M, Poewe W, Tolosa E, Wenning GK. Premotor signs and symptoms of multiple system atrophy. Lancet Neurol 2012;11:361–368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Wenning G, Geser F, Krismer F, et al. The natural history of multiple system atrophy: a prospective European cohort study. Lancet Neurol 2013;12:264–274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Stankovic I, Krismer F, Jesic A, et al. Cognitive impairment in multiple system atrophy: a position statement by the Neuropsychology Task Force of the MDS Multiple System Atrophy (MODIMSA) study group. Mov Disord 2014;29:857–867. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Siri C, Duerr S, Canesi M, et al. A cross‐sectional multicenter study of cognitive and behavioural features in multiple system atrophy patients of the parkinsonian and cerebellar type. J Neural Transm 2013;120:613–618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Koga S, Parks A, Uitti RJ, et al. Profile of cognitive impairment and underlying pathology in multiple system atrophy. Mov Disord 2017;32:405–413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Wenning G, Tison F, Ben Shlomo Y, et al. Multiple system atrophy: a review of 203 pathologically proven cases. Mov Disord 1997;12:133–147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Koga S, Aoki N, Uitti RJ, et al. When DLB, PD, and PSP masquerade as MSA. Neurology 2015;85:404–412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Kawai Y, Suenaga M, Takeda A, et al. Cognitive impairments in multiple system atrophy. Msa‐P vs. Msa‐C. Neurology 2008;70(16 Pt 2):1390–1396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Gatto E, Demey I, Sanguinetti A, et al. Cognition in a multiple system atrophy series of cases from Argentina. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2014;72:773–776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Chang CC, Chang YY, Chang WN, et al. Cognitive deficits in multiple system atrophy correlate with frontal atrophy and disease duration. Eur J Neurol 2009;16:1144–1150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Kawahara Y, Ikeda Y, Deguchi K, et al. Simultaneous assessment of cognitive and affective functions in multiple system atrophy and cortical cerebellar atrophy in relation to computerized touch‐panel screening tests. J Neurol Sci 2015;351:24–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Barcelos LB, Saad F, Giacominelli C, et al. Neuropsychological and clinical heterogeneity of cognitive impairment in patients with multiple system atrophy. Clin Neurol Neurosurg 2018;164:121–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Stanzani‐Maserati M, Gallassi R, Calandra‐Buonaura G, et al. Cognitive and sleep features of multiple system atrophy: review and prospective study. Eur Neurol 2014;72:349–359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Brown R, Marsden C. “Subcortical dementia”: the neuropsychological evidence. Neuroscience 1988;25:363–387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Schmahmann J, Sherman J. The cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. Brain 1998;121(Pt 4):561–579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Homma T, Mochizuki Y, Komori T, Isozaki E. Frequent globular neuronal cytoplasmic inclusions in the medial temporal region as a possible characteristic feature in multiple system atrophy with dementia. Neuropathology 2016;36:421–431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Cykowski MD, Coon EA, Powell SZ, et al. Expanding the spectrum of neuronal pathology in multiple system atrophy. Brain 2015;138:2293–2309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Benrud‐Larson L, Sandroni P, Schrag A, Low P. Depressive symptoms and life satisfaction in patients with multiple system atrophy. Mov Disord 2005;20:951–957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Schrag A, Geser F, Stampfer‐Kountchev M, et al. Health‐related quality of life in multiple system atrophy. Mov Disord 2006;21:809–815. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Schrag A, Sheikh S, Quinn N, et al. A comparison of depression, anxiety, and health status in patients with progressive supranuclear palsy and multiple system atrophy. Mov Disord 2010;25:1077–1081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Tison F, Yekhlef F, Chrysostome V. Depression and self‐reported depressive symptoms in multiple system atrophy compared to Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 2006;21:1056–1057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Balas M, Balash Y, Giladi N, Gurevich T. Cognition in multiple system atrophy: neuropsychological profile and interaction with mood. J Neural Transm 2010;117:369–375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Udow SJ, Robertson AD, Macintosh BJ, et al. “Under pressure”: Is there a link between orthostatic hypotension and cognitive impairment in α‐synucleinopathies? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2016;87:1311–1321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Wenning GK, Tison F, Seppi K, et al. Development and validation of the Unified Multiple System Atrophy Rating Scale (UMSARS). Mov Disord 2004;19:1391–1402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Hoehn M, Yahr M. Parkinsonism: onset, progression and mortality. Neurology 1967;17:427–442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Freeman R, Wieling W, Axelrod FB, et al. Consensus statement on the definition of orthostatic hypotension, neurally mediated syncope and the postural tachycardia syndrome. Auton Neurosci 2011;161:46–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. The Consensus Committee of the American Autonomic Society and the American Academy of Neurology . Consensus statement on the definition of orthostatic hypotension, pure autonomic failure and multiple system atrophy. Clin Auton Res 1996;6:125–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Fanciulli A, Jordan J, Biaggioni I, et al. Consensus statement on the definition of neurogenic supine hypertension in cardiovascular autonomic failure by the American Autonomic Society (AAS) and the European Federation of Autonomic Societies (EFAS): Endorsed by the European Academy of Neurology. Clin Auton Res 2018;28:355–362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Morris J, Heyman A, Mohs R, et al. The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer's disease. Neurology 1989;39:1159–1165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Berres M, Monsch A, Bernasconi F, et al. Normal ranges of neuropsychological tests for the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. Stud Heal Technol Inform 2000;77:195–199. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Arbuthnott K, Frank J. Trail making test, part B as a measure of executive control: validation using a set‐switching paradigm. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2000;22:518–528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Aebi C. Validierung der neuropsychologischen Testbatterie CERAD‐NP. Eine Multi‐Center Studie [Dissertation]. Univ Basel.2002.
  • 37. Wechsler D. Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation, 1981. [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Royall D, Cordes J, Polk M. CLOX: an executive clock drawing task. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1998;64:588–594. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Dubois B, Slachevsky A, Litvan I, Pillon B. The FAB: a Frontal Assessment Battery at bedside. Neurology 2000;55:1621–1626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Benke T, Karner E, Delazer M. FAB‐D: German version of the frontal assessment battery. J Neurol 2013;260:2066–2072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Herrmann‐Lingen C, Buss U, Snaith RP. HospitalAnxiety and Depression Scale ‐ Deutsche Version (HADS‐D) (3., aktualisierte und neu normierte Auflage). Bern: Hans Huber. Manuel, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Hinz A, Brähler E. Normative values for the hospital anxiety and depression scale (HADS) in the general german population. J Psychosom Res 2011;71:74–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Bürk K, Daum I, Rüb U. Cognitive function in multiple system atrophy of the cerebellar type. Mov Disord 2006;21:772–776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Fetoni V, Soliveri P, Monza D, et al. Affective symptoms in multiple system atrophy and Parkinson’s disease: response to levodopa therapy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999;66:541–544. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Kao AW, Racine CA, Quitania LC, et al. Cognitive and neuropsychiatric profile of the synucleinopathies: Parkinson’s disease, dementia with Lewy bodies and multiple system atrophy. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2009;23:365–370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Brown RG, Lacomblez L, Landwehrmeyer BG, et al. Cognitive impairment in patients with multiple system atrophy and progressive supranuclear palsy. Brain 2010;133:2382–2393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Fiorenzato E, Weis L, Falup‐Pecurariu C, et al. Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) and Mini‐Mental State Examination (MMSE) performance in progressive supranuclear palsy and multiple system atrophy. J Neural Transm 2016;123:1435–1442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Kitayama M, Wada‐Isoe K, Irizawa Y, Nakashima K. Assessment of dementia in patients with multiple system atrophy. Eur J Neurol 2009;16:589–594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Kim H, Jeon B, Kim Y, et al. Clinical and imaging characteristics of dementia in multiple system atrophy. Park Relat Disord 2013;19:617–621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Wenning GK, Hughes A, Wenning GK, et al. What clinical features are most useful to distinguish definite multiple system atrophy from Parkinson ’ s disease? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2000;68:434–440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. O’Sullivan SS, Massey LA, Williams DR, et al. Clinical outcomes of progressive supranuclear palsy and multiple system atrophy. Brain 2008;131:1362–1372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Feher E, Mahurin R, Doody R, et al. Establishing the limits of the Mini‐Mental State. Examination of “subtests”. Arch Neurol 1992;49:87–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Karrasch M, Laatu S, Martikainen K, Marttila R. CERAD test performance and cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Acta Neurol Scand 2013;128:409–413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Paviour DC, Winterburn D, Simmonds S, et al. Can the frontal assessment battery (FAB) differentiate bradykinetic rigid syndromes? Relation of the FAB to formal neuropsychological testing. Neurocase 2005;11:274–282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Cao B, Zhao B, Wei Q‐Q, et al. The global cognition, frontal lobe dysfunction and behavior changes in chinese patients with multiple system atrophy. PLoS One 2015;10:e0139773. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Santangelo G, Cuoco S, Pellecchia MT, et al. Comparative cognitive and neuropsychiatric profiles between Parkinson’s disease, multiple system atrophy and progressive supranuclear palsy. J Neurol 2018;265:2602–2613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Dujardin K, Defebvre L, Krystkowiak P, et al. Executive function differences in multiple system atrophy and Parkinson’s disease. Park Relat Disord 2003;9:205–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Hong HJ, Song SK, Lee PH, et al. Cognitive impairments in multiple system atrophy of the cerebellar type. J Mov Disord 2011;4:41–45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Bak TH, Caine D, Hearn VC, Hodges JR. Visuospatial functions in atypical parkinsonian syndromes. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2006;77:454–456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Lyoo CH, Jeong Y, Ryu YH, et al. Effects of disease duration on the clinical features and brain glucose metabolism in patients with mixed type multiple system atrophy. Brain 2008;131:438–446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Fiorenzato E, Weis L, Seppi K, et al. Brain structural profile of multiple system atrophy patients with cognitive impairment. J Neural Transm 2017;124:293–302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Lee MJ, Shin J‐H, Seoung J‐K, et al. Cognitive impairments associated with morphological changes in cortical and subcortical structures in multiple system atrophy of the cerebellar type. Eur J Neurol 2016;23:92–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Kim JS, Yang JJ, Lee DK, et al. Cognitive impairment and its structural correlates in the parkinsonian subtype of multiple system atrophy. Neurodegener Dis 2015;15:294–300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Allcock LM, Kenny RA, Mosimann UP, et al. Orthostatic hypotension in Parkinson ’ s disease : association with cognitive decline? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2006;21:778–783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Hohler A, Zuzuárregui J, Katz D, et al. Differences in motor and cognitive function in patients with Parkinson’s disease with and without orthostatic hypotension. Int J Neurosci 2012;122:233–236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Bae HJ, Lim JH, Cheon SM. Orthostatic hypotension and cognitive impairment in de novo patients with Parkinson’s disease. J Mov Disord 2014;7:102–104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Pilleri M, Facchini S, Gasparoli E, et al. Cognitive and MRI correlates of orthostatic hypotension in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol 2013;260:253–259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Peralta C, Stampfer‐Kountchev M, Karner E, et al. Orthostatic hypotension and attention in Parkinson’s disease with and without dementia. J Neural Transm 2007;114:585–588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Centi J, Freeman R, Gibbons CH, et al. Effects of orthostatic hypotension on cognition in Parkinson disease. Neurology 2017;88:17–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Deguchi K, Takeuchi H, Sasaki I, et al. Impaired novelty P3 potentials in multiple system atrophy ‐ correlation with orthostatic hypotension. J Neurol Sci 2001;190:61–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Annals of Clinical and Translational Neurology are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES