Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Feb 25.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2018 Aug;30(3):787–806. doi: 10.1017/S0954579418000470

An experimental test of the fetal programming hypothesis: Can we reduce child ontogenetic vulnerability to psychopathology by decreasing maternal depression?

Elysia Poggi Davis 1,2, Benjamin L Hankin 3, Danielle A Swales 1, M Camille Hoffman 4
PMCID: PMC7040571  NIHMSID: NIHMS1559955  PMID: 30068416

Abstract

Maternal depression is one of the most common prenatal complications, and prenatal maternal depression predicts many child psychopathologies. Here, we apply the Fetal Programming Hypothesis as an organizational framework to address the possibility that fetal exposure to maternal depressive symptoms during pregnancy affects fetal development of vulnerabilities and risk mechanisms, which enhance risk for subsequent psychopathology. We consider four candidate pathways through which maternal prenatal depression may affect the propensity of offspring to develop later psychopathology across the lifespan: brain development, physiological stress regulation (hypothalamic pituitary adrenocortical axis), negative emotionality, and cognitive (effortful) control. The majority of past research has been correlational, so potential causal conclusions have been limited. We describe an ongoing experimental test of the fetal programming influence of prenatal maternal depressive symptoms using a randomized controlled trial (RCT) design. In this RCT, Interpersonal Therapy (IPT) is compared to enhanced usual care among distressed pregnant women to evaluate whether reducing prenatal maternal depressive symptoms has a salutary impact on child ontogenetic vulnerabilities and thereby reduces offspring’s risk for emergence of later psychopathology.

Keywords: Maternal, prenatal, depression, development, intervention


Maternal depression is one of the strongest, most well-replicated risks for increased vulnerability to many negative child outcomes, including a wide range of psychopathologies (Goodman & Gotlib, 1999). Indeed, meta-analytic evidence suggests that children of depressed mothers have a substantially elevated likelihood of developing general psychopathology (k = 39, r = 0.24) including higher levels of internalizing (k = 121, r = 0.23) and externalizing (k = 111, r = 0.21) disorders (Goodman et al., 2011)). While this link between maternal depression and poor mental health in her children is well established, the vulnerabilities and mechanisms that explain this intergenerational transmission of risk remain unclear. The most commonly cited processes related to the intergenerational transmission of risk most typically include genetic inheritance, neuroregulatory mechanisms, learning from and exposure to maternal behaviors, cognitions, and affect, and the stressful context of children’s lives (Goodman & Gotlib, 1999). These factors certainly contribute to the intergenerational transmission of risk, but do not fully account for the impact of maternal depression on her offspring. A likely and yet rarely considered pathway is the impact of maternal prenatal depression on the intrauterine environment (i.e., fetal programming) and the ensuing consequences for the developing fetus. The Fetal Programming Hypothesis posits that during sensitive windows of fetal development the environment can exert lasting influences on health and well being across the lifespan (Barker, 1998). This paper articulates the role of maternal depression as a programming agent that may influence mental health via ontogenetic vulnerabilities in the offspring.

The Perinatal Depression Taskforce of the American College of Obstetricians and Gynecologists (2008) highlighted the high rates of maternal depression during pregnancy and the need for early screening and targeted intervention. An estimated 8.5 to 11% of women meet diagnostic criteria for major or minor depression during pregnancy (Gaynes et al., 2005). Even more women report elevated levels of prenatal depressive symptoms as demonstrated in a large study of women screened (n = 3472) with the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CESD) in obstetric clinics, of whom 20% reported symptoms above the cut off (Marcus, Flynn, Blow, & Barry, 2003). Rates of depression vary based on the population investigated and are higher among high-risk samples of pregnant women, such as those exposed to contextual risk factors such as poverty, racism, and violence. For example, one study found that 25% of impoverished pregnant women who resided in inner city areas met diagnostic criteria for minor or major depression (Hobfoll, Ritter, Lavin, Hulsizer, & Cameron, 1995). Depression is one of the most common prenatal complications and rates of depression may be even higher during pregnancy as compared to postpartum. In a longitudinal sample of over 9000 women, more scored above the cutoff on the Edinburgh Postpartum Depression Scale (EPDS) during pregnancy (13.5%) compared to postpartum (9.1%) (Evans, Heron, Francomb, Oke, & Golding, 2001).

The pervasiveness of prenatal depression presents a major public health challenge, as prenatal depressive symptoms have profound and persisting consequences for both the mother and her offspring. Longitudinal naturalistic studies illustrate that fetal exposure to maternal depressive symptoms predicts multiple child ontogenetic vulnerabilities and risk mechanisms that may contribute to later child psychopathology (Barker, Jaffee, Uher, & Maughan, 2011; Buss et al., 2012; Davis & Pfaff, 2014; Davis & Sandman, 2012; Korhonen, Luoma, Salmelin, & Tamminen, 2012; O’Donnell, Glover, Barker, & O’Connor, 2014; Sandman, Buss, Head, & Davis, 2015). Scientific inquiry into the impact of prenatal maternal depressive symptoms on her child can shed light on the mechanisms which drive the development of offspring psychopathology across the life course, and can inform interventions to reduce such risk to offspring.

Many excellent observational longitudinal studies have demonstrated that maternal prenatal depression predicts an increased risk of her offspring developing manifold manifestations of psychopathology. However, this body of knowledge “is inherently limited for drawing causal conclusions” given reliance on correlational designs; “experimental control gained through intervention” is thus needed (O’Connor, Monk, & Fitelson, 2014). In this paper, we address the question of whether experimentally reducing maternal prenatal depressive symptoms, using a randomized controlled trial (RCT) design, can reduce ontogenetic vulnerabilities and the development of risk mechanisms for subsequent psychopathology in the offspring. We review literature relevant to this question. We focus on research evaluating prenatal maternal depression, although we recognize and acknowledge that depression strongly co-occurs with a myriad of other psychopathologies and stressful contexts (Hammen, 2005; Kessler et al., 2005). There are undoubtedly many factors and pathways (multifinality and equifinality) associated with maternal depression that contribute to offspring risk. It is unlikely that maternal depression is the sole avenue transferring intergenerational risk and ontogenetic vulnerability to offspring. In this paper we focus on maternal depression, and we do not explicitly consider co-occurring maternal anxiety, contextual stress, or other comorbid psychopathologies, because the preponderance of empirical attention has investigated maternal depression as the primary independent predictor of offspring risk and psychopathology.

First, we review evidence showing that maternal prenatal depression predicts offspring mental health outcomes. Second, we introduce the Fetal Programming Hypothesis and its empirical support across species that guides our experimental test of the impact of prenatal maternal depression on child development. Third, we review four candidate pathways through which maternal prenatal depression may affect the propensity of offspring to develop later psychopathology across the lifespan: brain development, physiological stress regulation via the hypothalamic pituitary adrenocortical (HPA) axis, negative emotionality, and cognitive (effortful) control. Fourth, we review evidence that mental health interventions can decrease maternal depressive symptoms and, may in turn, have a salubrious effect on offspring. To date, the vast majority of clinical trials have shown that treating mothers’ depression can reduce children’s symptoms, but most studies have not examined development of risk mechanisms in offspring. The extent to which ameliorating prenatal depressive symptoms can reduce early emergence of offspring ontogenetic vulnerabilities, and thus later development of psychopathologies, is important but largely unknown. Fifth, we review interventions for prenatal maternal depression (antidepressant medication and psychotherapy) and present evidence for Interpersonal Therapy (IPT) as a specific intervention approach that holds promise for reducing maternal prenatal depressive symptoms. Finally, we introduce the Care Project, the RCT we are conducting presently, which is an empirical test of the question: Does reducing prenatal maternal depressive symptoms benefit child ontogenetic processes thereby reducing subsequent risk for psychopathology.

Prenatal maternal depression and offspring psychopathology

Compelling evidence from several prospective studies demonstrates prenatal maternal depression as a pathway contributing to the intergenerational transmission of psychopathology (Barker et al., 2011; Clemens et al., 2015; Hay, Pawlby, Waters, Perra, & Sharp, 2010; Korhonen et al., 2012; Luoma et al., 2001; Plant, Barker, Waters, Pawlby, & Pariante, 2013). These studies assess maternal depressive symptoms during pregnancy and then prospectively ascertain offspring psychiatric outcomes later in adolescence and adulthood. This corpus of research illustrates that prenatal maternal depression (diagnosis as well as subclinical symptom elevations) forecast both internalizing and externalizing psychopathology in the offspring even after accounting for paternal depression, postnatal maternal depression and other contextual risk factors. Capron and colleagues (2015) followed participants from a large longitudinal birth cohort (n = 4,303). Elevated maternal report of prenatal depressive symptoms predicted increased likelihood of experiencing an anxiety diagnosis in the offspring (adj. OR = 1.75). Prenatal paternal depression was not associated with child outcomes. This study covaried paternal and maternal postnatal depression as well as sociodemographic factors, and showed that these alternative pathways did not account for these findings. Similar increases in offspring vulnerability to depression have been observed in several large longitudinal cohort studies. Prenatal maternal depression increased the likelihood that her offspring would be diagnosed with depression at age 18 (n = 2847) even after accounting for postnatal maternal depressive symptoms (Pearson et al., 2013). Similarly, Plant and colleagues report that offspring exposed to clinical maternal depression in pregnancy were 3.4 times more likely to have a diagnosed depressive disorder at 18 to 25 years of age after accounting for postnatal maternal depression and sociodemographic risk factors (Plant, Pariante, Sharp, & Pawlby, 2015).

Prenatal maternal depression is not only associated with internalizing problems, but also with externalizing disorders, including antisocial behavior, conduct disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Prenatal maternal diagnosis of depression in either the second or third trimester predicted a diagnosis of conduct disorder in the offspring at 16 years of age after covarying indices of the family environment, the child’s later exposure to maternal depression, the mother’s smoking and drinking during pregnancy, and the parents’ antisocial behavior (Hay et al., 2010). When prenatal maternal depressive symptoms were prospectively assessed in a large sample of 3,298 mother child dyads, prenatal maternal depressive symptoms predicted externalizing behavior problems at 7 to 8 years of age (Barker et al., 2011), a finding that replicates prior research with a smaller sample (Luoma et al., 2001). Consistent with these prospective longitudinal studies, a large Finnish population-based study found that mothers’ reports of depression during pregnancy predicted antisocial behavior in offspring 30 years later (Mäki et al., 2003). When adjusted for mother’s marital status, smoking, wantedness of the pregnancy, place of residence, socioeconomic status and perinatal complications, maternal depression predicted likelihood of engaging in violent crimes (adj. OR = 0.6 females and 1.4 males) and non violent crimes (adj. OR=1.7 females and 1.6 males). Finally, a large population based study showed that prenatal exposure to maternal major depressive disorder (MDD) is associated with increased risk for an ADHD diagnosis during childhood (Clemens et al., 2015).

In summary, the evidence shows that maternal depression broadly predicts offspring risk to multiple forms of psychopathology (Downey & Coyne, 1990; Goodman et al., 2011), including many of the individual putatively discrete DSM-defined psychiatric disorders. Thus, there is not a simple, one-to-one prediction from maternal depression to youth depression, but rather to youth psychopathology broadly. It is well-known that many psychiatric disorders, especially depression, overlap and co-occur with other emotional and behavioral disorders, and this comorbidity is not organized and classified effectively in current categorical psychiatric nosological systems, such as DSM (Rutter & Uher, 2012). Newer structural models that represent dimensional psychopathology have emerged showing that a bifactor model of psychopathology best represents the organization of common emotional, behavioral and thought problems in psychopathology in children, adolescents and adults (Caspi et al., 2014; Hankin et al., 2016; Lahey, Krueger, Rathouz, Waldman, & Zald, 2017; Snyder, Young, & Hankin, 2017). Briefly, the bifactor model of psychopathology includes all of the covariance that is in common across all measured forms of emotional, behavioral and thoughts problems in one general, latent psychopathology factor (i.e., p factor; Caspi et al., 2014), and independent latent internalizing and externalizing factors then capture the remaining unique variance. This new latent dimensional bifactor model (hereafter referred to as the “p factor model”) can more succinctly and parsimoniously organize the many discrete categorical psychiatric disorders into a simpler, empirically supported structural model. We highlight this point concerning the p factor model, which includes the p factor as well as independent, unique internalizing and externalizing dimensions, because having a reliable, valid, and optimal classification system for psychopathology is important for advancing understanding of processes by which maternal depression affects offspring risk to later psychopathology. Our review of evidence regarding the four ontogenetic vulnerabilities and associations with youth psychopathology summarizes research that has predominantly been conducted using DSM-oriented syndromes and diagnoses because most extant research has used a DSM-based psychopathology outcome system, although we note newer research that specifically relates these vulnerabilities to the psychopathology dimensions identified in the p factor model when possible. As maternal depression is broadly related to many manifestations of DSM-oriented psychopathologies in offspring, and not merely to depression specifically, the p factor model offers a promising structural framework to study these questions.

Fetal Programming Hypothesis as an Organizational Framework: How Does Prenatal Maternal Depression Contribute to Ontogenetic Vulnerabilities and Offspring Psychopathology?

These studies provide compelling evidence for the influence of prenatal maternal depression, both at diagnostic and subclinical levels, on subsequent emergence of psychopathology in the offspring. Analytic approaches that covary prenatal and postnatal paternal depression, postnatal maternal depression and sociodemographic factors lends support to the likelihood that in utero exposure to maternal depressive symptoms shapes the development of fetal neural systems, and in turn leads to increased risk of subsequent psychopathology. These studies highlight the need for prospective investigations of prenatal maternal depression and its impact on child ontogenetic processes and mechanisms that contribute to subsequent psychopathology.

What is Fetal Programming?

A key motivation for focusing on the prenatal period to understand pathways by which maternal depression influence the development of psychopathology in her children derives from the Fetal Programming Hypothesis. Broadly, during periods of rapid fetal development or change, the organism is hypothesized to be particularly susceptible to environmental influences, and these effects are postulated to have persisting consequences for health and disease risk across the lifespan (Barker, 1998). The genesis of this hypothesis emerged from Dr. David Barker’s observations, in which birth records were linked with death records, and he noted that a disproportionate number of deaths from coronary heart disease occurred among individuals with low birth weight (Barker, 2003). A large literature now indicates that small size at birth is associated with increased risk for a range of health outcomes including heart disease, diabetes, and obesity (Barker, Eriksson, Forsen, & Osmond, 2002; Nathanielsz, 1999) as well as multiple psychopathologies later in life (Class, Rickert, Larsson, Lichtenstein, & D’Onofrio, 2014; Somhovd, Hansen, Brok, Esbjorn, & Greisen, 2012). Size at birth, however, is only a gross indicator of prenatal perturbations, and prospective investigations are needed to understand the processes and mechanisms by which the prenatal environment influences subsequent health. Both experimental animal research and observational human studies have demonstrated that nutrition is one such programming signal and illustrate lifelong consequences of malnutrition during the prenatal period (Gluckman & Hanson, 2004; Lumey, Stein, & Susser, 2011). Prenatal maternal mental health, and specifically depression, likely serve as an additional programming signal that influences birth outcomes and risk processes for subsequent psychopathology in offspring.

Prenatal maternal depression contributes to adverse birth outcomes

Maternal mental health, and specifically maternal depression impacts the fetal environment and shapes the fetal developmental trajectory in ways that contribute to both adverse birth outcome and subsequent vulnerability to psychopathology (Accortt, Cheadle, & Schetter, 2015; Hoffman, Mazzoni, Wagner, Laudenslager, & Ross, 2016; Sandman, Class, Glynn, & Davis, 2016). Several recent meta-analyses provide important evidence that prenatal maternal depression is associated with preterm birth (Grigoriadis, VonderPorten, Mamisashvili, Tomlinson, et al., 2013; Grote et al., 2010) and low birth weight (Grote et al., 2010). In a test of the impact of prenatal depression a recent meta-analysis specifically assessed untreated prenatal maternal depression and found that prenatal maternal depression is associated with a significantly increased risk of preterm delivery (adj OR = 1.56) and low birth weight (adj OR = 1.96) (Jarde et al., 2016). While important and suggestive as evidence, small size at birth is not likely to be a direct mechanism contributing to subsequent psychopathology. Rather, small birth size serves as a proximal marker that reflects perturbations to the intrauterine environment that impact developing fetal physiological systems that may confer risk to the emergence of subsequent psychopathology.

Animal models illustrate ways that early life environmental signals influence developmental trajectories in ways that both confer adaptation to the subsequent environment as well as risks and vulnerabilities. The western spade foot toad displays a remarkable developmental adaptation to early adversity (Denver, 1997). When its pool of water desiccates rapidly, the tadpole adapts to this harsh environment by accelerating metamorphosis into a toad that is able to survive outside of water. However, a consequence of this accelerated developmental trajectory is that the toad is both smaller and less successful at foraging and reproducing as compared to a toad that had a more optimal environment allowing for a protracted developmental course. The human fetus similarly incorporates signals from the maternal host environment into its developmental trajectory. Maternal stress signals are associated with fetal growth restriction, but also accelerate development of systems necessary for survival (e.g. lungs) in the face of shortened gestation (Dunkel Schetter, 2009; Glynn, Dunkel Schetter, Hobel, & Sandman, 2008). These adaptations are advantageous if the fetus is born preterm, but may also have long-term costs (Davis, Glynn, Waffarn, & Sandman, 2011; Stout, Espel, Sandman, Glynn, & Davis, 2015). In this review, we focus on maternal prenatal depression as a maternal signal that may influence the trajectory of fetal development. We discuss the specific ontogenetic processes and risk mechanisms that may be impacted by prenatal maternal depression and contribute to the increased vulnerability to psychopathology among offspring exposed to prenatal maternal depression.

Processes and risks by which prenatal maternal depression may affect offspring ontogenetic vulnerabilities and risk mechanisms that may contribute to later psychopathology

Fetal Brain Development

The transformation from a zygote to the human newborn is a dramatic process involving rapid cell division and differentiation to produce all biological systems including the brain. Human brain development begins early in gestation and follows an orchestrated series of events including processes such as proliferation, migration, and differentiation. At times during gestation neurogenesis proceeds at a rate of 250,000 cells per minute (Cowan, 1979). By the end of the second trimester 200 billion neurons have been produced (Bourgeois, 1997; Bourgeois, Goldman-Rakic, & Rakic, 1994). Cell migration initiates during the first trimester and when neurons reach their final destination, they arborize and branch in an attempt to establish appropriate connections (Sidman and Rakic, 1973). Synaptogenesis commences around 16 gestational weeks and neural circuits begin to organize (Bourgeois et al., 1994; Kostovic, Judas, Rados, & Hrabac, 2002). The rapid pace of fetal brain development continues during the last third of human pregnancy as the fetal brain forms secondary and tertiary gyri and exhibits neuronal differentiation, dendritic arborization, axonal elongation, synapse formation and collateralization, and myelination (Bourgeois, 1997; Volpe, 2008). Synaptogenesis accelerates during the third trimester to a rate of 40,000 synapse per minute (Bourgeois et al., 1994). Transcriptome studies indicate that the majority of genes are expressed in the human brain during the fetal period and that there is approximately twofold greater gene expression in the fetal brain compared with the adult (Johnson et al., 2009). These findings clearly illustrate that prenatal life is a time of enormous change and thus, is a sensitive window of increased plasticity and vulnerability to environmental signals.

Prenatal maternal depressive symptoms and fetal brain development

Family history of depression strongly predicts neural signatures of risk for psychopathology even prior to the onset of the disorder (Chen, Hamilton, & Gotlib, 2010; Swartz, Williamson, & Hariri, 2015). The Fetal Programming Hypothesis suggests the possibility that alterations to developing brain systems during the prenatal period may be one of the key processes underlying this intergenerational transmission of neural risk mechanisms underlying vulnerability to psychopathology.

In order to evaluate the specific impact of prenatal maternal depression on brain structure and function it is necessary to assess the brain during the prenatal or neonatal period, before the potentially confounding impact of postpartum experiences. A cutting-edge approach that could provide new insight into the impact of prenatal maternal depression on fetal brain development involves the use of imaging techniques to assess the fetal brain. Using fMRI, developmental changes in functional connectivity of the human fetus have been characterized (Thomason et al., 2015). During the third trimester, left amygdala connectivity is characterized first by local circuitry and then by the formation of connections to ipsilateral regions in the frontal and temporal lobe followed by connections to the contralateral amygdala. The rapid development of these neural circuits during the third trimester likely renders this circuitry particularly susceptible to maternal signals, including maternal depression (Scheinost et al., 2017). Notably, individual differences in brain development that predict meaningful outcomes can be observed during the prenatal period. Fetuses that go on to deliver preterm display reduced connectional integrity as compared to age-matched fetuses who go on to deliver at term (Thomason et al., 2017). The detection of connectivity differences among at risk fetuses prior to birth provides important evidence that the fetal brain may be susceptible to prenatal influences.

To our knowledge, prenatal maternal depressive symptoms have not been directly linked to indices of fetal brain development. However, recent studies that imaged the infant shortly after birth reveal that prenatal maternal depressive symptoms predicts brain structure and function during infancy. Demonstration of an association between prenatal maternal depressive symptoms and neonatal brain development is important as it provides evidence for an association between prenatal maternal depression and brain development independent of the postnatal environment. Elevated prenatal maternal depressive symptoms are associated with microstructure of the right and left amygdala (lower fractional anisotropy and axial diffusivity) during the neonatal period (Rifkin-Graboi et al., 2013). Further, elevated levels of prenatal maternal depression predicted connectivity between prefrontal and limbic regions in the newborn (Posner et al., 2016). Both diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and resting state fMRI revealed atypical amygdala-prefrontal cortex (PFC) connectivity in the newborn. Interestingly, fetal heart responses to a laboratory challenge (a maternal stress task) mediated the association between prenatal maternal depression and newborn neural functioning (Posner et al., 2016). A finding that is consistent with the hypothesis that the prenatal environment contributed to this association as neurodevelopmental associations with maternal depression are observed before birth. Further, evidence with 6-month-old infants indicates that even after covarying postnatal maternal depressive symptoms, elevated prenatal maternal depression is associated with functional connectivity of the amygdala to the temporal cortex, insula, anterior cingulate and medial orbitofrontal and ventromedial prefrontal cortices (Qiu et al., 2015). These studies provide evidence that fetal exposure to maternal depressive symptoms shapes the trajectory of brain development and that developing prefrontal and limbic regions may be particularly susceptible.

Prospective longitudinal studies show that the impact of prenatal maternal depressive symptoms on brain structure and function extends beyond infancy and into childhood. Prenatal maternal depressive symptoms predict elevated amygdala response to negative emotional stimuli in 6 to 9 year old children (van der Knaap et al., 2017). Prenatal maternal depressive symptoms predict a thinner frontal cortex during childhood, primarily in the right frontal cortex, a finding that has now been replicated in two independent samples and after covarying postnatal depression (Lebel et al., 2016; Sandman et al., 2015). This pattern of reduced right cortical thickness is one that has been observed in children, adolescents and adults with depression as well as those at risk for depression (Boes, McCormick, Coryell, & Nopoulos, 2008; Ducharme et al., 2013; Foland-Ross, Gilbert, Joormann, & Gotlib, 2015; Peterson et al., 2009). Further evidence for the importance of prenatal exposure comes from a study assessing both maternal and paternal depression during the prenatal and postnatal periods (El Marroun et al., 2016). Maternal, but not paternal, depression predicted child brain morphology at 6 to 9 years after covarying postnatal maternal depression and paternal depression. Specifically, prenatal maternal depressive symptoms predicted a thinner superior frontal gyrus in the left hemisphere in childhood. Determining a direct impact of prenatal maternal depressive symptoms on fetal brain development is a critical next step. We will test this in our RCT by evaluating the degree to which reducing prenatal maternal depressive symptoms influences offspring brain development from birth to one year of age.

Neural mechanisms and links with subsequent offspring psychopathology

The literature associating neural mechanisms with child, adolescent and adult psychopathology is voluminous, and a full review of this extensive literature is beyond the scope of this paper. We briefly summarize key points from this corpus of research with a focus here on structure, function, and connectivity across brain areas and multiple forms of common psychopathologies (e.g., depression, anxiety, ADHD, conduct problems). Our primary conclusion is that some of the same neural regions in offspring that may be affected by fetal exposure to maternal prenatal depressive symptoms are regions that are commonly observed and indicated as likely neural mechanisms contributing to risk for the development of psychopathology. Moreover, an intriguing possibility supported by newer inquiries, is that many of these brain areas do not show a one-to-one specific correspondence with unique DSM-defined psychiatric disorders. Rather, compelling recent research suggests that a few brain areas may be broadly, transdiagnostically associated with risk to general, common forms of psychopathology (p factor), and some other neural regions may show some relative specificity to particular aspects of psychopathology. The newer work that supports this possibility is particularly notable in light of the evidence that maternal depression broadly predicts offspring risk to multiple forms of psychopathology (Downey & Coyne, 1990; Goodman et al., 2011). The p factor model is particularly intriguing in light of the established finding that maternal depression predicts many forms of psychopathology broadly, and not one specific discrete psychiatric disorder, and newer, suggestive data illustrating that a few neural areas confer risk to many discrete psychiatric disorders more generally. In sum, these different lines of scholarship and evidence point to the possibility that maternal prenatal depression may affect offspring neurodevelopment of key brain areas and networks, and in turn, these neural components may confer risk to general psychopathology broadly (i.e., the p factor) as well as some specific psychopathology dimensions (e.g., unique internalizing factor).

A few neural areas have demonstrated relations with various DSM-defined traditional categorical psychiatric disorders. When groups of adult psychiatric patients are compared to healthy controls, meta-analyses generally show reduced grey matter volume in certain common areas, including the prefrontal cortex (e.g., dorsolateral, ventrolateral and orbitofrontal areas) and limbic regions (e.g., amygdala, medial temporal lobe structures, and anterior insula) across disorders including major depression, bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, anxiety, conduct and antisocial behavior problems (Aoki, Inokuchi, Nakao, & Yamasue, 2013; Haijma et al., 2012; Rogers & De Brito, 2016; Shang et al., 2014; Wise et al., 2017). The implicated prefrontal regions are part of a network involved in cognitive control/executive functioning (Niendam et al., 2012). Limbic and paralimbic areas are involved in affective processes, including detecting and responding to affectively salient information (amygdala), emotional memory (medial temporal lobe structures), and visceral/autonomic functions (anterior insula). These neural networks interact, and dysregulation in either system (affective or cognitive control) may contribute to psychopathology (Cole, Repovš, & Anticevic, 2014). In addition to brain structure, functional imaging shows dysregulation in many of these neural areas. Meta-analyses and reviews of functional connectivity in common forms of psychopathology similarly show abnormal activity in circuits that include many of these brain regions (e.g., prefrontal and limbic areas) (Etkin & Wager, 2007; Grupe & Nitschke, 2013; Kaiser, Andrews-Hanna, Wager, & Pizzagalli, 2015; McCarthy, Skokauskas, & Frodl, 2014; Miller, Hamilton, Sacchet, & Gotlib, 2015).

These many studies show a pattern in which reduced activity in PFC and greater activity in limbic areas is associated with various discrete DSM-determined psychopathologies. More recent research suggests that a more limited number of brain areas may be implicated in risk to multiple forms of psychopathology. Of particular interest, a recent meta-analysis focused on grey matter volume reductions showed that only a few common neural substrates, namely the dorsal anterior cingulate and the insula, were related to multiple, supposedly discrete, psychiatric disorders in adults, including schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, depression, anxiety disorders, and substance use disorders (Goodkind et al., 2015). In addition to this meta analysis, recent evidence suggests that common neural regions are associated with manifestations of multiple psychopathologies in a transdiagnostic manner (Buckholtz & Meyer-Lindenberg, 2012; Zald & Lahey, 2017). We recently provided a direct test of this hypothesis with children (ages 6–9). We found that the general psychopathology latent p-factor was associated with reduced grey matter volume in PFC areas, whereas the specific internalizing factor was related to reduced grey matter volume in limbic areas (Snyder, Hankin, Sandman, Head, & Davis, 2017).

These findings in children provide the first direct demonstration that latent dimensional psychopathology factors, as parsimoniously organized via the p-factor model, relate to key neural regions known to confer risk to broad forms of common psychopathologies. Moreover, these prefrontal and limbic regions are both associated with prenatal exposure to maternal depression and implicated in the other ontogenetic vulnerabilities and risk mechanisms highlighted in this paper (stress physiology, negative emotionality and cognitive control). Taken together, such work supports the intriguing possibility of linkages across levels and units of analysis (e.g., emotionality, executive functioning, cognition, stress physiology, neural risk) that may more parsimoniously connect maternal prenatal depression with offspring ontogenetic vulnerabilities and risk to general, broad psychopathology.

Stress physiology: HPA axis regulation

The HPA axis is one of the body’s primary stress regulatory systems that broadly impacts physiological systems contributing to physical and mental health. The HPA axis is a commonly proposed mechanism by which the prenatal environment is thought to impact health and development across the lifespan. HPA axis dysregulation may serve as one pathway by which prenatal maternal depressive symptoms transfers risk for psychopathology to the offspring. The orchestrated response of the HPA axis is initiated by the stress-activated neuropeptide corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH). CRH was first discovered in the hypothalamus and it is now known to also be expressed in neuronal populations in the amygdala and hippocampus (Joels & Baram, 2009). CRH is released in response to stress from axon terminals in the hypothalamic median eminence and acts on receptors in the pituitary where CRH promotes the release of adrenocorticotropin releasing hormone (ACTH) from the pituitary, which activates the release of the glucocorticoid cortisol from the adrenal cortex. The HPA axis is regulated by a negative feedback loop, such that circulating cortisol inhibits further secretion of CRH and limits the magnitude and duration of the stress response (S. M. Smith & Vale, 2006). During pregnancy, CRH additionally is released from the placenta into both the maternal and fetal circulations and plays a central role in integrating maternal stress signals and regulating fetal development and timing of birth (R. Smith, Mesiano, & McGrath, 2002).

HPA axis dysregulation as a consequence of prenatal maternal depression and as a risk to psychopathology can be studied across all components of the HPA axis. CRH and ACTH have been examined, although the majority of research has focused on cortisol, which can be measured non invasively in saliva and thus, is well suited to studies with children and with repeated sampling (e.g., Gunnar & Vazquez, 2006). Here we consider typically-studied approaches that evaluate HPA regulation, including diurnal cortisol rhythms (cortisol typically peaks 30–45 minutes after awakening and then declines across the day), the cortisol awakening response (CAR), and cortisol reactivity to challenge. Before considering empirical work demonstrating cortisol dysregulation, we note that both under (hypo) and over (hyper) activity of the HPA axis are associated with psychopathology and disease (Gunnar & Vazquez, 2006; McEwen, 2004).

Prenatal maternal depressive symptoms and HPA axis regulation

The animal literature has been fairly consistent in demonstrating that prenatal and early life stress impacts HPA axis functioning over the lifetime; even when postnatal experiences and genetics are experimentally controlled (Glover, O’Connor, & O’Donnell, 2010). In humans, postnatal maternal depression, experienced during infancy, predicts HPA axis dysregulation in offspring assessed in adolescence and adulthood (Barry et al., 2015; Murray et al., 2010). Prenatal maternal depression was not measured in these studies and that less is known about the consequences of maternal depressive symptoms during the prenatal period. Existing research evaluating the link between prenatal maternal depression and offspring HPA axis regulation has evaluated the offspring baseline or diurnal regulation as well as responses to stress. Although this review focuses on prenatal maternal depression, it is worth noting that when maternal prenatal distress broadly is considered, prenatal distress is linked to disruptions in both baseline and circadian HPA axis functioning from the neonatal period through adolescence (O’Donnell et al., 2013; Van den Bergh, Van Calster, Smits, Van Huffel, & Lagae, 2008). Several studies report that neonates of mothers with prenatal depression or with any mental health diagnosis during pregnancy have higher cortisol concentrations as compared to neonates born to mothers without a psychiatric diagnosis (Field et al., 2004; Kaplan, Evans, & Monk, 2008; Lundy et al., 1999). Associations with baseline HPA axis regulation are observed in childhood as well. In a prospective longitudinal investigation prenatal maternal depressive symptoms were associated with a larger cortisol awakening response in the offspring at 10 to 12 years of age, after covarying for postnatal factors including maternal depression (Vänskä et al., 2016).

In addition to relations with baseline and circadian functioning, maternal prenatal psychological distress has been linked to heightened cortisol reactivity in response to stress in offspring (Grant et al., 2009; Tollenaar, Beijers, Jansen, Riksen-Walraven, & de Weerth, 2011). A study of 6-month-olds showed that perinatal MDD predicted greater cortisol reactivity in response to laboratory challenge (Brennan et al., 2008). Similarly, in a prospective investigation, children of mothers who experienced more severe depressive symptoms during pregnancy were more likely to display a larger cortisol response to a painful stressor (i.e. inoculation) (Fernandes, Stein, Srinivasan, Menezes, & Ramchandani, 2015). Although this association is not observed in all studies (Braithwaite, Murphy, & Ramchandani, 2016; Davis et al., 2011), the combination of animal and human research indicates that the prenatal environment impacts the development of stress regulatory systems.

HPA axis regulation and links with subsequent offspring psychopathology

Dysregulated HPA-axis activity is associated, concurrently and longitudinally, with various expressions of psychopathology across the lifespan including depression, anxieties, PTSD, and externalizing problems. Depression (both symptoms and diagnosis) in adolescents and adults has been associated with increased cortisol reactivity to a challenge, flatter diurnal cortisol rhythm, and a larger cortisol awakening response (e.g., Burke, Davis, Otte, & Mohr, 2005; Guerry & Hastings, 2011; Lopez-Duran, Kovacs, & George, 2009; Pariante & Lightman, 2008). More recent longitudinal studies show that cortisol dysregulation predicts the subsequent emergence of internalizing disorders. A higher cortisol awakening response in adolescence predicts first onset and recurrence of major depressive episode (Adam et al., 2010; Vrshek-Schallhorn et al., 2013) as well as future anxiety (Adam et al., 2014). In contrast, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is associated with attenuated cortisol production (Yehuda, 2002). Hypocortisolism is not only found in adults with a PTSD diagnosis but also in offspring of mothers with a PTSD diagnosis (Danielson, Hankin, & Badanes, 2015). Hypocortisolism also has been demonstrated in children at risk for antisocial personality disorder (Susman, 2006), and low cortisol awakening response predicted longitudinal trajectories of externalizing behaviors from ages 5 to 15 (Haltigan, Roisman, Susman, Barnett-Walker, & Monahan, 2011). Finally, moving past a focus on specific mental health disorders, a recent meta analytic review demonstrated that a flatter diurnal cortisol slope is associated with multiple psychopathologies, including depression, general externalizing and internalizing problems, and broad mental health problems across ages; these significant associations were found for both concurrent and prospective relations with most mental health syndromes (Adam et al., 2017).

In summary, considerable evidence across multiple psychopathologies, age groups, and patterns of abnormal cortisol response show that HPA-axis dysregulation is an ontogenetic vulnerability for psychopathologies later in life and may be implicated in the pathophysiology linking prenatal maternal depressive symptoms to later risk for psychopathology in offspring. Indeed, longitudinal research demonstrates that maternal prenatal emotional distress and internalizing problems predict a pattern of high, flattened diurnal cortisol in adolescent offspring (ages 14 to 15), which in turn was related to depressive symptoms in adolescent girls (Van den Bergh, Van Calster, Pinna Puissant, & Van Huffel, 2008). Thus, the available evidence suggests that HPA axis dysregulation may be a risk process by which prenatal exposure to maternal depression may impact offspring risk for subsequent psychopathology.

Negative emotionality

Negative emotionality may be another pathway by which prenatal maternal depression confers risk for the development of psychopathology in the offspring. Negative emotionality is a broad temperament factor that has been called the single most important individual difference risk factor in behavioral public health, given the general and nonspecific linkages with diverse physical and mental health outcomes (Lahey, 2009). Similar to its related adult personality trait counterpart of neuroticism in standard trait models, negative emotionality is conceptualized as a tendency toward experiencing negative emotions (e.g., fear, sadness, frustration, anger) more quickly and intensely and to react more easily to stressful life events (Rothbart & Bates, 2006). Conceptually and empirically, the broad temperament factor of negative emotionality overlaps with similar individual difference constructs, such as negative affectivity (Rothbart, Ahadi, Hersey, & Fisher, 2001) and behavioral inhibition (Kagan, Reznick, & Snidman, 1987). Broadly, individual differences in temperament, such as negative emotionality, are traditionally hypothesized to emerge in early life and are expected to exhibit relative stability across development (Rothbart & Bates, 2006).

Associations with prenatal maternal depression

The presence of elevated maternal depressive symptoms during pregnancy has been shown to shape negative emotionality in the offspring (Bhat et al., 2015; Davis et al., 2007; Werner et al., 2007). Prospective longitudinal studies show that prenatal maternal depressive symptoms predict greater negative emotionality in the offspring, assessed by both laboratory observation and maternal report (Davis et al., 2007; Davis et al., 2004). Further, these studies demonstrate that these associations are not fully accounted for by postnatal exposures, as these associations remain after controlling for pertinent postnatal factors, such as maternal postpartum psychological state. For example, in a longitudinal study of 247 maternal child dyads assessed from pregnancy through 2 months postpartum, prenatal maternal depressive symptoms predicts maternal report of greater offspring negative affectivity at age 2 after covarying current maternal depression (Davis et al., 2007). Similarly, within a sample of 123 mothers and their infants, maternal reports of depressive symptoms during pregnancy, but not postpartum, were associated with maternal ratings of greater negative affectivity in her child (Huot, Brennan, Stowe, Plotsky, & Walker, 2004). Further in a high-risk sample of women with a history of MDD a similar association between prenatal depression and child negative emotionality was observed after covarying postnatal influences including maternal depression and socio demographic factors (Rouse & Goodman, 2014). These studies indicate an association between prenatal maternal depressive symptoms and infant negative emotionality that cannot be fully explained by the postnatal environment.

Negative emotionality and links with subsequent offspring psychopathology

Neuroticism, as the adult personality trait, as well as negative emotionality as the temperament factor in infants, children and adolescents, are well-established and long studied individual differences that have been shown to associate with and predict many aspects of poor physical and mental health (Lahey, 2009). Consistent with the view that negative emotionality broadly confers risk to many mental health problems, a meta-analytic review of cross-sectional associations between neuroticism and the most common 11 mental disorders, as defined by DSM, found that individuals carrying these diagnoses reported higher negative emotionality (mean Cohen’s D = 1.65, a large effect size; Kotov et al., 2010). Still, there was variability in the effects, such that anxiety disorders demonstrated the strongest relation with neuroticism (e.g. panic disorder: d = 1.92), with depressive disorders next, (e.g. major depressive disorder: d = 1.33), and substance use disorders considerably lower (e.g. alcohol abuse: d = 0.77).

Consistent with a vulnerability model of temperament as a predisposing factor, Ormel and colleagues (2013) conducted a meta-analytic review of the prospective data. Negative emotionality clearly predicts later mental health disorders, especially for internalizing symptoms and disorders and broad psychological distress (mean d = 0.63, range 0.48–0.87), whereas longitudinal relations with substance use symptoms and disorders are considerably weaker (d = 0.26). Conceptually related to negative emotionality, behavioral inhibition (Hirshfeld-Becker et al., 2008) and inhibited-approach traits (Caspi, Henry, McGee, Moffitt, & Silva, 1995) predict the later development of anxiety disorders. Negative emotionality not only prospectively predicts later depression and anxiety but also behavior problems, such as aggression, conduct and oppositional defiant disorders (e.g., Bates, Pettit, Dodge, & Ridge, 1998; Olson, Bates, Sandy, & Lanthier, 2000; Renken, Egeland, Marvinney, Mangelsdorf, & Sroufe, 1989). Individuals characterized by life-course persistent antisocial behavioral problems exhibit high negative emotionality early in childhood (Moffitt, Caspi, Dickson, Silva, & Stanton, 1996) and extending into adulthood (White, Bates, & Buyske, 2001). Other longitudinal studies have noted prospective associations connecting earlier negative emotionality with later broad internalizing and externalizing problems, such as observed in a four-year follow-up of 6 year old children (Eisenberg et al., 2009) and in longitudinal predictions of externalizing behaviors across childhood and into adolescence (Gjone & Stevenson, 1997). Late adolescents’ self-reported ratings of high negative emotionality at age 18 predicted higher rates of diagnosed affective, anxiety, substance use, and antisocial personality disorders at age 21 (Krueger, 1999). Observed temperament in preschool aged children predicts later psychopathology. For example, temperamental dysphoria at 3 predicted later depression and oppositional behavior, and fear at 3 predicted anxiety disorders (Dougherty, Klein, Durbin, Hayden, & Olino, 2010). Extending over a longer prospective interval, Caspi and colleagues (1996) followed a cohort of children from age 3 to 21 years of age and found that observations of negative emotionality at age 3 predicted diagnoses of depressive disorder and alcohol use in adulthood.

Finally, this brief review clearly shows that negative emotionality is associated with multiple forms of common psychopathologies. Consistent with this general predictive linkage, recent research has demonstrated that negative emotionality indeed is related to general psychopathology, as instantiated via the latent p factor, as well as unique latent psychopathology dimensions. Negative emotionality is associated with the p factor in children and adolescents ages 9–17 (Tackett et al., 2013) and preschoolers (Olino, Dougherty, Bufferd, Carlson, & Klein, 2014). Among adults, higher negative emotionality was associated with the p factor (Caspi et al., 2014), and after taking into account the p factor, there was no longer any association between negative emotionality and externalizing problems, whereas the association between negative emotionality and internalizing problems remained significant. Finally, in two samples of youth, one of pre-adolescents (ages 5–11) and the second comprised of children and adolescents (ages 9–17), negative emotionality related to the p factor as well as the specific internalizing dimension (Hankin et al., 2017). Thus, in addition to serving as a broad transdiagnostic risk factor (i.e., associating with the p factor), negative emotionality also serves as risk for the specific internalizing psychopathology dimension, and these patterns appear robust across different ages. Negative emotionality may serve as an ontogenetic vulnerability for psychopathology and be one pathway by which prenatal exposure to maternal depression leads to subsequent psychopathology in the offspring.

Cognitive (effortful) control

Cognitive, or effortful, control, describes an interrelated set of processes whereby the individual seeks to modify internal emotional states and thoughts as well as adaptively change behavior to achieve goals (Nigg, 2017). Although effortful control, cognitive control, executive function, and self-regulation are terms that convey somewhat distinct constructs and processes from disparate foundational literatures, at the same time these terms convey sufficient overlap and characterize conceptually similar core processes (Nigg, 2017). Cognitive (effortful) control plays an important role in self-regulating emotions and affecting risk to emergence of psychopathology through executive functioning constituent processes, such as attentional processes, inhibitory control, planning, updating and shifting. Given this strong conceptual overlap, we selectively review literature showing that maternal prenatal depression predicts offspring’s cognitive (effortful) control and that youth cognitive (effortful) control deficits confer risk to the development of psychopathology broadly.

Prenatal maternal depression and cognitive (effortful) control

Various lines of evidence support the connection between maternal depression and children’s cognitive functioning, although few longitudinal investigation of maternal prenatal depression on children’s cognitive control exist. Several large prospective investigations, have demonstrated that prenatal maternal depression is associated with measures of general cognitive functioning, such as the presence of developmental delays at 18 months and child IQ at 8 years of age, independent of postnatal maternal depression (Evans et al., 2012). Few existing studies evaluate a link between prenatal depression and cognitive control. Infants born to women diagnosed with prenatal depression (Gerardin et al., 2011) or elevated depressive symptoms (Pacheco & Figueiredo, 2012) show poorer state regulation measured with the Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale (NBAS) providing evidence for an impact of prenatal maternal depression on infant self-regulatory abilities. Parallel findings from 2 large cohort studies (ALSPAC and generation R) show that prenatal maternal depression predicted child attentional problems at 3 years of age after covarying postpartum maternal depression and paternal depression (Van Batenburg-Eddes et al., 2013). Similarly prenatal maternal depression predicted executive function problems and problems shifting attention at 4 years of age (El Marroun et al., 2017). These large population studies provide evidence for an association between prenatal maternal depression and child cognitive control. Additional evidence supporting a link between prenatal maternal depression and cognitive control comes from a large population based study showing that prenatal exposure to maternal MDD is associated with increased risk for an ADHD diagnosis during childhood (Clemens et al., 2015). Although our focus is on prenatal maternal depression, it is worth noting that prospective longitudinal studies have linked prenatal maternal anxiety to cognitive control abilities, including behavioral measures of attentional control, through adolescence (Van den Bergh & Marcoen, 2004; Van den Bergh et al., 2005). These large population studies provide evidence for an association between prenatal maternal depression and child cognitive control.

Poor cognitive (effortful) control and links with subsequent offspring psychopathology

An extensive, literature has demonstrated longitudinal associations between effortful control and later emotional and behavioral psychopathologies in children and adolescents (for reviews, see (Hankin et al., 2016; Nigg, 2017; Snyder, Miyake, & Hankin, 2015). This body of research shows that poor cognitive (effortful) control predicts all common forms of psychopathology, including ADHD, depression and mood disorders (e.g., bipolar disorder), anxiety disorders, conduct problems and broader externalizing disorders, substance use and alcohol problems, and thought disorders (e.g. schizophrenia).

With respect to cognitive control, it is notable that low cognitive (effortful) control is associated with most forms of individual psychopathologies, and this replicated pattern suggests that poor cognitive control may be a transdiagnostic risk and, thus, linked with the general, broad latent psychopathology p factor (e.g., Beauchaine & Thayer, 2015; Hankin et al., 2016; Snyder, Hankin, et al., 2017). Indeed recent evidence suggests this is the case. Two independent samples of youth showed that poor effortful control associates with the p factor, capturing broad common psychopathology, as well as unique externalizing behaviors (Hankin et al., 2017). Moreover, considerable research shows that poor cognitive control is significantly related with reduced structural and neural activity in prefrontal cortices (Banich, 2009), and numerous studies show that reduced activity in PFC correlates with various discrete psychopathologies (Buckholtz & Meyer-Lindenberg, 2012; Goodkind et al., 2015). Taken together, these studies suggest linkages between poor cognitive (effortful) control with general, broad psychopathology across levels of analysis (e.g., temperament, executive function tasks, neural indices).

Are reductions in maternal depression associated with improvements in children’s symptom level and functioning?

It has only recently been considered that treating maternal depression could have a positive influence on the well-being and functioning of her children (Gunlicks & Weissman, 2008). Although longitudinal studies show that variations in parents’ depressive symptoms across time are prospectively associated with changes in children’s depressive symptoms and functioning (e.g., Garber, Ciesla, McCauley, Diamond, & Schloredt, 2011), experimental designs are needed to determine if reducing maternal depression causes changes in child outcomes. RCTs, in which participants are randomized to either intervention or a comparison condition, are considered the gold standard to evaluate potential causality. Several RCTs have now demonstrated that reducing maternal depressive symptoms via intervention can affect her children’s psychopathological symptoms. A recent meta-analysis of nine RCTs, primarily focusing on postpartum depression, compared the effect of psychotherapy versus control groups (treatment as usual; waiting list) for mothers’ depression and her offspring’s mental health outcomes (Cuijpers et al., 2014). Psychotherapy conducted only with the mother both reduced her depression levels (Hedge’s g = .66; medium effect size) and improved mental health outcomes in her children (g = .40, small effect size). For example, a RCT focused on clinically depressed mothers and their children showed that depressed mothers treated with Interpersonal Therapy (IPT) had lower depressive symptoms and better functioning at 3 and 9 months after treatment. Notably, the children of mothers in the IPT group exhibited significantly lower depressive symptoms at the 9-month follow-up (Swartz et al., 2008). Importantly, in this trial they demonstrated that maternal depression improvement preceded the depressive symptoms change in the children. The available evidence supports the notion that treating maternal depression is associated with positive outcomes (e.g., reduced symptoms) in her offspring. It is important to note that the preponderance of these studies were conducted with postpartum depressed women; few have studied the effect that reducing maternal depression during pregnancy has on offspring outcomes.

Research investigating prenatal interventions focused on reducing maternal depression is limited (O’Connor et al., 2014). In our current research, we are building logically on the prior empirical work demonstrating that psychotherapeutic interventions aimed at reducing mothers’ depressive symptoms can have a salubrious effect on her children’s mental health and symptom reduction. The current evidence base shows that treating maternal depression is associated with positive outcomes, especially depressive symptom reduction in the children, but these studies have not evaluated change in the four vulnerabilities and risk mechanisms in the offspring we have highlighted in this review. Moreover, the fetal programming hypothesis suggests that the fetal period is critical for conferring ontogenetic vulnerability to the child, beyond those due to postnatal experiences. Thus, it follows that the influence of prenatal maternal depression on the offspring must involve risks and mechanisms other than postnatal psychosocial exposures (e.g., parental sensitivity, family conflict). At present, reasons for the children’s reduction in symptoms as a result of treating maternal depression are not well understood. It is possible, and perhaps even likely, that complex interactions between postnatal (e.g., mother-infant attachment) and prenatal influences (maternal stress physiology) synergistically influence youth outcomes (e.g., Bergman et al., 2010). An enhanced understanding of the processes through which elevated risk is transmitted intergenerationally, consistent with a fetal programming hypothesis perspective, is needed.

We advocate inquiry focused on improving knowledge on potential mechanisms and moderators (cf., Goodman & Gotlib, 1999) that may augment intergenerational risk transmission, especially during the prenatal period when maternal risk for developing depression is high (Evans et al., 2001; Gavin et al., 2005) and fetal neurodevelopment is rapidly changing. We seek to extend such work to distressed pregnant women and examine empirically whether by reducing prenatal maternal depression we can impact infant risk factors and mechanisms, such as those reviewed earlier (e.g., neural circuitry stress physiology, negative emotionality, and cognitive control), that past work suggests contributes to the emergence of psychopathology in children and adolescents. Our primary notion is that experimentally reducing maternal depression during the prenatal period, a time of rapid fetal development, will reduce ontogenetic vulnerabilities in the offspring and thus, diminish the likelihood that these offspring will develop subsequent psychopathology. With this in mind, we now turn to considering pharmacological and psychotherapeutic options that have been investigated.

Interventions to reduce maternal prenatal depression

Antidepressant medications

CDC data show that antidepressants are the third most prescribed medication of all medications prescribed and used in the United States, with 9% of the total U.S. population (11.8% of women) having an antidepressant prescription in the last 30 days (2009–2012; http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/hus/contents2015.htm#080). Antidepressant medication has demonstrated efficacy in reducing depressive symptoms among adults (Hollon, 2016 Psychiatry Online, accessed 2017). Consistent with the studies described above using psychotherapy, postnatal reduction of maternal depression using antidepressant medication has benefits for her children. In the longitudinal Sequenced Treatment Alternatives to Relieve Depression (STAR*D) trial, depressed women were treated to remission through a sequenced set of pharmacological interventions. Women’s depression remission was associated with reductions in offspring symptoms and diagnoses (Weissman et al., 2006). A one-year follow-up showed that the children of women whose depression remitted exhibited significant decreases in internalizing and externalizing symptoms, whereas those children whose mothers’ depression did not remit did not display symptom reduction (Pilowsky et al., 2008; Wickramaratne et al., 2011).

In contrast to the generally accepted benefit of postnatal antidepressant treatment, considerable debate, among scholars and clinicians as well as the general public, exists regarding the safety of antidepressant medication usage during pregnancy for the developing fetus and later child outcomes. Antidepressant medication use and prescription is prevalent and the most common treatment for depression among women of childbearing age (Ko, Farr, Dietz, & Robbins, 2012). Antidepressants cross the placenta and the fetal blood brain barrier (Mulder, Ververs, De Heus, & Visser, 2011; Nulman et al., 2012). SSRIs, the most commonly used antidepressants, impact serotonin transmission in the developing fetal brain (Hendrick et al., 2003; Olivier et al., 2013). Serotonin receptors are present as early as 5 gestational weeks. Therefore, it is plausible that prenatal antidepressants affect fetal development. Here, we briefly review the impact that antidepressant medication use has on the developing fetus and later child outcomes. We seek to provide a balanced perspective on the potential risks and benefits of antidepressant usage during pregnancy. There are known consequences of maternal depression on birth and child outcomes. Much of the controversy in the literature centers around whether antidepressant medication increases adverse outcomes for the developing fetus and then the child beyond the direct, main effect impact that exposure to maternal depression confers.

Observational human research has linked prenatal SSRI exposure to adverse outcomes including preterm birth, shortened gestation, lower birth weight, congenital malformations, persistent pulmonary hypertension, septal heart defects, poor neonatal adaptation syndrome (PNAS) and respiratory distress (Grigoriadis, VonderPorten, Mamisashvili, Eady, et al., 2013). These findings have raised concern about the impact of prenatal antidepressant use among pregnant women. Recent meta-analyses, as well as a large cohort study comparing siblings with and without exposure to antidepressant medication, document a link between antidepressant use and birth outcomes, particularly gestational length and preterm birth (Eke, Saccone, & Berghella, 2016; Ross et al., 2013; Viktorin et al., 2016). Notably associations remain, even when a stringent comparison of depressed and medicated mothers to depressed and non-medicated mothers was employed. Overall these meta-analyses and findings from a genetically informed population based study suggest a small, but meaningful impact of prenatal antidepressants on preterm birth and gestational length. It is worth noting that a recent cohort study found that although overall maternal depression was associated with preterm birth, maternal depression was not related to gestational length or preterm birth among the 160 (of the 831 total) who screened positive for depression and also received prenatal treatment with antidepressant medication (Venkatesh et al., 2016), thus supporting the need for further research to elucidate this issue.

Perhaps even more controversial than the impact on birth outcome is the question of whether there are consequences of prenatal antidepressant medication for neurodevelopmental disorders such as ADHD and autism spectrum disorders in the offspring (Man et al., 2017; Sujan et al., 2017). A possible link between prenatal antidepressant exposure and autism spectrum disorders has proved particularly controversial. Several large studies and meta-analyses report significant associations (Boukhris, Sheehy, Mottron, & Berard, 2016; Croen, Grether, Yoshida, Odouli, & Hendrick, 2011), whereas other large studies fail to replicate (Harrington, Lee, Crum, Zimmerman, & Hertz-Picciotto, 2014; Hviid, Melbye, & Pasternak, 2013). These studies have raised concern that antidepressant use during the prenatal period impacts fetal brain development and could affect subsequent emergence of child neurodevelopmental disorders. Recent studies have shown that prenatal SSRI exposure is linked to neonatal brain development (Jha et al., 2016) and alterations in sensitive windows (Weikum, Oberlander, Hensch, & Werker, 2012). In contrast to the conflicting or limited evidence in the human literature, rodent research using pharmacological manipulations provides compelling evidence that serotonin exposure early in life exerts persisting effects on neural functioning as well as on stress physiology (Avitsur et al., 2016; Borue, Chen, & Condron, 2007; Gingrich et al., 2017).

Given the impact of prenatal maternal depression on both maternal and infant morbidity, the literature is struggling to characterize the risks and benefits of antidepressant medication. Psychotherapy provides an alternative treatment option that may alleviate the consequences of maternal depression without the concerns related to antidepressant medication.

Psychotherapy

Approximately 75% of patients report a preference for psychotherapy as compared to medication (Cuijpers, Cristea, Karyotaki, Reijnders, & Huibers, 2016). It is well known that multiple psychotherapies, including Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT), Interpersonal Therapy (IPT), and brief psychodynamic therapy (as well as others) have demonstrated efficacy for treating depression among adults (e.g., Cuijpers et al., 2016; Hollon, 2016; Solomonov & Barber, 2016). The large depression treatment literature indicates that evidence based interventions have similar efficacy. Although CBT is frequently discussed as the “gold standard” psychotherapy treatment for depression, this notion has been questioned and challenged (Leichsenring & Steinert, 2017).

Despite the availability of evidence-based and efficacious interventions for perinatal women, only a minority of clinically depressed women (18%) (Marcus, 2009) seek treatment during the prenatal and postpartum periods. A recent meta-analysis for the U.S. Preventive Services Task Force (O’Connor, Rossom, Henninger, Groom, & Burda, 2016) reviewed 18 trials, the majority of which included RCTs that recruited women with postpartum depression (only 3 trials were included that recruited women during pregnancy). They demonstrated a medium to large effect size in decreasing depressive symptoms. This meta-analysis included mostly interventions using CBT or nondirective counseling; no interpersonally-based intervention was included. An earlier meta-analysis of treatments for perinatal depression, that included IPT, (Sockol, Epperson, & Barber, 2011) showed that women assigned to intervention conditions reported significantly less depression compared to controls (Hedge’s G = .65). Moreover, Sockol and colleagues’ (2011) review concluded that psychological interventions that included interpersonal components more significantly reduced depression compared to control conditions and CBT-based interventions. Similarly, a recent Cochrane review of 28 preventive interventions aimed at postpartum depression showed that women who receive a psychosocial or psychological intervention, compared to standard care, were significantly less likely to experience depressive symptoms; interventions based on interpersonal psychotherapy were particularly promising (Dennis & Dowswell, 2013).

Interpersonal Therapy

In our ongoing RCT to reduce prenatal maternal depressive symptoms we are implementing IPT for several reasons. First, women prefer psychotherapy to medication (Cuijpers et al., 2016). Second, numerous published RCTs testing IPT reveal that women with perinatal depression, or those at-risk (e.g., high distress), who received IPT showed a significant reduction in depressive symptoms compared to controls (Clark, Tluczek, & Wenzel, 2003; Grote et al., 2015; Grote et al., 2009; J. E. Johnson et al., 2016; Lenze & Potts, 2017; Mulcahy, Reay, Wilkinson, & Owen, 2010; O’Hara, Stuart, Gorman, & Wenzel, 2000; Spinelli & Endicott, 2003; Swartz et al., 2008; Zlotnick, Miller, Pearlstein, Howard, & Sweeney, 2006; Zlotnick, Tzilos, Miller, Seifer, & Stout, 2016). In particular, recently published studies show that IPT is efficacious (medium effect size) with socio-economically disadvantaged pregnant women of diverse cultural backgrounds, especially after the women participate in an initial engagement session to problem solve common barriers to treatment (Grote et al., 2015; Lenze & Potts, 2017). IPT significantly reduced depressive symptoms, compared to an active control group (parenting education program), among depressed pregnant women (Spinelli, Endicott, Goetz, & Segre, 2016). Third, an attitudes survey of pregnant women showed a preference for IPT (Dimidjian & Goodman, 2014). In summary, while CBT and other interventions can effectively reduce depressive symptoms in pregnant women, we selected IPT because of the strong evidence base in studies of pregnancy and women’s greater preference for it.

Although the corpus of research demonstrates that IPT reduces maternal depression during pregnancy (Grote et al., 2015; Melville et al., 2014), it is not known whether decreasing prenatal maternal depression will impact fetal neurodevelopment and infant ontogenetic vulnerability. Thus, our RCT serves as an experimental test of fetal programming of maternal depression. The efficacious intervention we are employing is known as “MOMCare” (Grote et al., 2015). MOMCare is a culturally relevant, collaborative care intervention that provides brief IPT via approximately 8 sessions after an initial engagement session. In their most recent and largest study, Grote and colleagues completed a RCT that establishes efficacy of MOMCare, a version of brief IPT designed for pregnant women, with socio-economically disadvantaged pregnant women of diverse cultural backgrounds. MOMCare (n=83) was compared to enhanced usual care (n=85). Women randomized to MOMCare showed significantly higher rates of depression remission and lower depressive symptom severity as compared to controls. These effects persisted through 18 months postpartum (effect sizes = .35 for severity of depressive symptoms and .36 for depression remission).

The Care Project: An experimental test of fetal programming of maternal depression

The authors (Davis, Hankin as co-Primary Investigators) are presently conducting a RCT (called The Care Project). This study will reduce pregnant women’s levels of depressive symptoms via an established intervention to more rigorously test the Fetal Programming Hypothesis with respect to the impact of exposure to maternal depressive symptoms and its influence on later development of infant risk mechanisms. Specifically, this project will implement MOMCare, as compared to enhanced usual care, to examine empirically the impact of reducing maternal depressive symptoms on offspring ontogenetic vulnerabilities and risk mechanisms (see Figure 1). Prior to beginning IPT sessions, a manualized pre-therapy engagement session is implemented to help resolve barriers to care (e.g., logistical, emotional, cultural). Thereafter, MOMCare providers continue with manualized brief IPT. Brief IPT focuses on psychoeducation and interpersonal skill building to decrease interpersonal conflict and increase interpersonal support and competence. Women are educated about the link between feelings and interpersonal interactions and are taught communication and interpersonal strategies that they can apply to different people in their lives. Moreover and consistent with a collaborative care model, women in MOMCare may be prescribed antidepressant medications, as needed and as determined based on women’s preference and in collaborative consultation with medical experts (e.g., maternal fetal medicine obstetrician, psychiatrist).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

The Care Project: A schematic overview of the study design of the Care Project. The Care Project is a randomized controlled trial (RCT) in which women with elevated levels of depressive symptoms (operationalized as EPDS scores >9) are recruited early in pregnancy. At baseline and prior to the intervention, maternal measures are collected. Then half of the women are randomized to the IPT intervention group (MOMCare) and the other half receive enhanced usual care. Maternal measures will be collected longitudinally during pregnancy through 12 months postpartum. Infants will be evaluated during the neonatal period and at 6- and 12-months. Infants will be assessed across four risk processes: 1) brain development 2) physiological stress regulation, 3) negative emotionality, and 4) cognitive (effortful) control as well as multiple infant psychopathological symptoms.

In the Care Project, we plan to recruit 240 socio-economically and ethnically diverse pregnant women with elevated levels of depressive symptoms (operationalized as EPDS scores >9). At baseline and prior to the intervention, maternal measures are collected. Then half of the women are randomized to MOMCare, including an initial engagement session, and the other half receive enhanced usual care. Maternal measures will be collected longitudinally during pregnancy through 12 months postpartum. Infants will be evaluated during the neonatal period and at 6- and 12-months, adjusted for gestational length for infants born preterm. Infants will be assessed across four risk processes: 1) brain development (volume and thickness via structural MRI; white matter fiber tracts via DTI; functional connectivity via resting state MRI; and EEG/ERP measures), 2) physiological stress regulation (cortisol), 3) negative emotionality (eye-tracking tasks, observation of negative emotionality and maternal report of negative emotionality), and 4) cognitive (effortful) control (eye-tracking tasks, observation of effortful control and maternal report of infant effortful control) as well as multiple infant psychopathological symptoms. The prospective evaluations from birth to 1 year will allow for a direct test of the impact of fetal experiences on development. The inclusion of neonatal assessments (e.g., MRI, cortisol) will allow for a direct test of the impact of our prenatal intervention on the fetus before postnatal influences are expected to exert a strong influence. Continuous assessments over the first postnatal year will allow for longitudinal assessment of the persisting impact of the prenatal intervention. We recognize that prenatal IPT may influence child outcomes via both prenatal and postnatal (e.g., parenting) pathways. Thus, we will assess postnatal factors, such as parenting, maternal mood, and contextual factors that may mediate or moderate the impact of the prenatal intervention on child outcomes.

Finally, we emphasize that the Care Project is an empirical test of a set of a priori hypotheses grounded in the fetal programming with translational implications regarding offspring ontogenetic vulnerability and mental health outcomes. As such, this project represents a “risky test” (Meehl, 1978) of these specific hypotheses. Still, as is the case in most scientific investigations, we may be wrong. Importantly, though, we as a field learn from failure, and from disconfirmation of our a priori hypotheses, so long as the research investigation is well-designed and executed (Firestein, 2015). In this vein, our project is sufficiently powered to detect reliable, robust findings and, thus, provide meaningful conclusions regarding our specific project aims, whether the hypotheses are corroborated or not. We believe that the findings, whether supportive (or not) of our hypotheses, will provide useful information for the literature, especially the fields of fetal programming and developmental psychopathology. We further note that if our eventual findings refute our a priori hypotheses, these study specific results do not necessarily disconfirm the larger fetal programming model that has been studied and well-supported across species and for other developmental processes and outcomes.

In summary, the Care Project, by using a RCT with an evidence-based intervention to reduce depressive symptoms in a sample of distressed women during pregnancy, will provide an experimental test of aspects of the Fetal Programming Hypothesis. We anticipate being able to make inferences with respect to important infant outcomes and a select set of key ontogenetic vulnerabilities and risk mechanisms in offspring that have been shown to enhance vulnerability to the development of later psychopathology. A pillar of developmental psychopathology is the study of normal development informing abnormal functioning and processes, and investigation of abnormal populations likewise informing normal development. Our hope is that the Care Project will inform both basic developmental science, by providing an experimental test of aspects of the Fetal Programming Hypothesis, and will enlighten translational knowledge and hold potential policy implications regarding how intergenerational transmission of psychopathology through development of key risk mechanisms may be reduced among offspring of distress women.

Acknowledgements

Supported by the National Institutes of Health (R01 MH109662 and P50 MH096889)

References

  1. Accortt EE, Cheadle A, & Schetter CD (2015). Prenatal depression and adverse birth outcomes: an updated systematic review. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 19(6), 1306–1337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Adam EK, Doane LD, Zinbarg RE, Mineka S, Craske MG, & Griffith JW (2010). Prospective prediction of major depressive disorder from cortisol awakening responses in adolescence. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35(6), 921–931. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Adam EK, Doane LD, Zinbarg RE, Mineka S, Craske MG, & Griffith JW (2017). Prospective prediction of major depressive disorder from cortisol awakening responses in adolescence. Psychoneuroendocrinology. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Adam EK, Vrshek-Schallhorn S, Kendall AD, Mineka S, Zinbarg RE, & Craske MG (2014). Prospective associations between the cortisol awakening response and first onsets of anxiety disorders over a six-year follow-up–2013 Curt Richter Award Winner. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 44, 47–59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Aoki Y, Inokuchi R, Nakao T, & Yamasue H (2013). Neural bases of antisocial behavior: A voxel-based meta-analysis. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(8), 1223–1231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Avitsur R, Grinshpahet R, Goren N, Weinstein I, Kirshenboim O, & Chlebowski N (2016). Prenatal SSRI alters the hormonal and behavioral responses to stress in female mice: possible role for glucocorticoid resistance. Hormones and Behavior, 84, 41–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Banich MT (2009). Executive function: The search for an integrated account. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(2), 89–94. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01615.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  8. Barker DJ (1998). In utero programming of chronic disease. Clin Sci (Lond), 95(2), 115–128. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9680492 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Barker DJ (2003). The midwife, the coincidence, and the hypothesis. BMJ, 327(7429), 1428–1430. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7429.1428 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Barker DJ, Eriksson JG, Forsen T, & Osmond C (2002). Fetal origins of adult disease: strength of effects and biological basis. International Journal of Epidemiology, 31(6), 1235–1239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Barker ED, Jaffee SR, Uher R, & Maughan B (2011). The contribution of prenatal and postnatal maternal anxiety and depression to child maladjustment. Depression and anxiety, 28(8), 696–702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Barry TJ, Murray L, Pasco Fearon RM, Moutsiana C, Cooper P, Goodyer IM,… Halligan SL (2015). Maternal postnatal depression predicts altered offspring biological stress reactivity in adulthood. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 52, 251–260. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.12.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bates JE, Pettit GS, Dodge KA, & Ridge B (1998). Interaction of temperamental resistance to control and restrictive parenting in the development of externalizing behavior. Developmental psychology, 34(5), 982. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Beauchaine TP, & Thayer JF (2015). Heart rate variability as a transdiagnostic biomarker of psychopathology. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 98(2), 338–350. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.08.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bhat A, Chowdayya R, Selvam S, Khan A, Kolts R, & Srinivasan K (2015). Maternal prenatal psychological distress and temperament in 1–4 month old infants–A study in a non-western population. Infant Behavior and Development, 39, 35–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Boes AD, McCormick LM, Coryell WH, & Nopoulos P (2008). Rostral anterior cingulate cortex volume correlates with depressed mood in normal healthy children. Biol Psychiatry, 63(4), 391–397. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&dopt=Citation&list_uids=17916329 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Borue X, Chen J, & Condron BG (2007). Developmental effects of SSRIs: lessons learned from animal studies. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 25(6), 341–347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Boukhris T, Sheehy O, Mottron L, & Berard A (2016). Antidepressant use during pregnancy and the risk of autism spectrum disorder in children.. JAMA Pediatrics, 170(2), 117–124. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.3356 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Bourgeois JP (1997). Synaptogenesis, heterochrony and epigenesis in the mammalian neocortex. Acta Paediatr Suppl, 422, 27–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Bourgeois JP, Goldman-Rakic PS, & Rakic P (1994). Synaptogenesis in the prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkeys. Cereb Cortex, 4(1), 78–96. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8180493 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Buckholtz JW, & Meyer-Lindenberg A (2012). Psychopathology and the human connectome: Toward a transdiagnostic model of risk for mental illness. Neuron, 74(6), 990–1004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Burke HM, Davis MC, Otte C, & Mohr DC (2005). Depression and cortisol responses to psychological stress: a meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30(9), 846–856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Buss C, Davis EP, Shahbaba B, Pruessner JC, Head K, & Sandman CA (2012). Maternal cortisol over the course of pregnancy and subsequent child amygdala and hippocampus volumes and affective problems. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 109(20), E1312–1319. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1201295109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Capron LE, Glover V, Pearson RM, Evans J, O’Connor TG, Stein A,… Ramchandani PG (2015). Associations of maternal and paternal antenatal mood with offspring anxiety disorder at age 18 years. Journal of affective disorders, 187, 20–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Caspi A, Henry B, McGee RO, Moffitt TE, & Silva PA (1995). Temperamental origins of child and adolescent behavior problems: From age three to age fifteen. Child development, 66(1), 55–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Caspi A, Houts RM, Belsky DW, Goldman-Mellor SJ, Harrington H, Israel S,… Poulton R (2014). The p factor: One general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clinical Psychological Science, 2(2), 119–137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Newman DL, & Silva PA (1996). Behavioral observations at age 3 years predict adult psychiatric disorders: Longitudinal evidence from a birth cohort. Archives of general psychiatry, 53(11), 1033–1039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Chen MC, Hamilton JP, & Gotlib IH (2010). Decreased hippocampal volume in healthy girls at risk of depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(3), 270–276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Clark R, Tluczek A, & Wenzel A (2003). Psychotherapy for postpartum depression: a preliminary report. Am J Orthopsychiatry, 73(4), 441–454. doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.73.4.441 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Class QA, Rickert ME, Larsson H, Lichtenstein P, & D’Onofrio BM (2014). Fetal growth and psychiatric and socioeconomic problems: population-based sibling comparison. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 205(5), 355–361. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Clemens CC, Castro VM, Blumenthal SR, Rosenfield HR, Murphy SN, Fava M, & Robinson EB (2015). Prenatal antidepressant exposure is associated with risk for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder but not autism spectrum disorder in a large health system. Molecular Psychiatry, 20(6), 727–734. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.90 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Cole MW, Repovš G, & Anticevic A (2014). The frontoparietal control system: A central role in mental health. The Neuroscientist, 20(6), 652–664. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Cowan WM (1979). The development of the brain. Sci Am, 241(3), 113–133. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Croen LA, Grether JK, Yoshida CK, Odouli R, & Hendrick V (2011). Antidepressant use during pregnancy and childhood autism spectrum disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68(11), 1104–1112. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.73 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Cuijpers P, Cristea IA, Karyotaki E, Reijnders M, & Huibers MJH (2016). How effective are cognitive behavior therapies for major depression and anxiety disorders? A meta-analytic update of the evidence. World Psychiatry, 15(3), 245–258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Cuijpers P, Karyotaki E, Weitz E, Andersson G, Hollon SD, & van Straten A (2014). The effects of psychotherapies for major depression in adults on remission, recovery and improvement: A meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 159, 118–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Danielson CK, Hankin BL, & Badanes LS (2015). Youth offspring of mothers with posttraumatic stress disorder have altered stress reactivity in response to a laboratory stressor. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 53, 170–178. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.01.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Davis EP, Glynn LM, Schetter CD, Hobel C, Chicz-Demet A, & Sandman CA (2007). Prenatal exposure to maternal depression and cortisol influences infant temperament. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 46(6), 737–746. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e318047b775 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Davis EP, Glynn LM, Waffarn F, & Sandman CA (2011). Prenatal maternal stress programs infant stress regulation. J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 52(2), 119–129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Davis EP, & Pfaff D (2014). Sexually dimorphic responses to early adversity: implications for affective problems and autism spectrum disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 49, 11–25. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.06.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Davis EP, & Sandman CA (2012). Prenatal psychobiological predictors of anxiety risk in preadolescent children. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37(8), 1224–1233. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.12.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Davis EP, Snidman N, Wadhwa PD, Dunkel Schetter C, Glynn L, & Sandman CA (2004). Prenatal maternal anxiety and depression predict negative behavioral reactivity in infancy. Infancy, 6(3), 319–331. [Google Scholar]
  43. Dennis C, & Dowswell T (2013). Psychosocial and psychological interventions for preventing postpartum depression. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews(2). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Denver RJ (1997). Environmental stress as a developmental cue: corticotropin-releasing hormone is a proximate mediator of adaptive phenotypic plasticity in amphibian metamorphosis. Hormones and Behavior, 31(2), 169–179. doi:S0018–506X(97)91383–0 [pii] 10.1006/hbeh.1997.1383 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Dimidjian S, & Goodman SH (2014). Preferences and attitudes toward approaches to depression relapse/recurrence prevention among pregnant women. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 54, 7–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Dougherty LR, Klein DN, Durbin CE, Hayden EP, & Olino TM (2010). Temperamental positive and negative emotionality and children’s depressive symptoms: A longitudinal prospective study from age three to age ten. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 29(4), 462–488. [Google Scholar]
  47. Downey G, & Coyne JC (1990). Children of depressed parents: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 108(1), 50–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Ducharme S, Albaugh MD, Hudziak JJ, Botteron KN, Nguyen T, Truong C,… Byars AW (2013). Anxious/depressed symptoms are linked to right ventromedial prefrontal cortical thickness maturation in healthy children and young adults. Cerebral cortex, 24(11), 2941–2950. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Dunkel Schetter C (2009). Stress Processes in Pregnancy and Preterm Birth. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(4), 204–209. [Google Scholar]
  50. Eisenberg N, Valiente C, Spinrad TL, Cumberland A, Liew J, Reiser M,… Losoya SH (2009). Longitudinal relations of children’s effortful control, impulsivity, and negative emotionality to their externalizing, internalizing, and co-occurring behavior problems. Developmental psychology, 45(4), 988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Eke A, Saccone G, & Berghella V (2016). Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) use during pregnancy and risk of preterm birth: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BJOG: An International Journal of Obstetrics & Gynaecology, 123(12), 1900–1907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. El Marroun H, Tiemeier H, Muetzel RL, Thijssen S, der Knaap NJF, Jaddoe VWV,… White TJH (2016). Prenatal exposure to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors and non-verbal cognitive functioning in childhood. Depression and Anxiety, 33(7), 658–666.27163186 [Google Scholar]
  53. El Marroun H, White TJ, Fernandez G, Jaddoe VWV, Verhulst FC, Stricker BH, & Tiemeier H (2017). Prenatal exposure to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors and non-verbal cognitive functioning in childhood. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 31(3), 346–355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Etkin A, & Wager TD (2007). Functional neuroimaging of anxiety: A meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and specific phobia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164(10), 1476–1488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Evans J, Heron J, Francomb H, Oke S, & Golding J (2001). Cohort study of depressed mood during pregnancy and after childbirth. BMJ, 323(7307), 257–260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Evans J, Melotti R, Heron J, Ramchandani P, Wiles N, Murray L, & Stein A (2012). The timing of maternal depressive symptoms and child cognitive development: a longitudinal study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 53(6), 632–640. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02513.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Field T, Diego M, Dieter J, Hernandez-Reif M, Schanberg S, Kuhn C,… Bendell D (2004). Prenatal depression effects on the fetus and the newborn. Infant Behavior and Development, 27(2), 216–229. [Google Scholar]
  58. Firestein S (2015). Failure : Why science is so successful. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  59. Foland-Ross LC, Gilbert BL, Joormann J, & Gotlib IH (2015). Neural markers of familial risk for depression: an investigation of cortical thickness abnormalities in healthy adolescent daughters of mothers with recurrent depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(3), 476. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Garber J, Ciesla JA, McCauley E, Diamond G, & Schloredt KA (2011). Remission of depression in parents: Links to healthy functioning in their children. Child Development, 82(1), 226–243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Gavin NI, Gaynes BN, Lohr KN, Meltzer-Brody S, Gartlehner G, & Swinson T (2005). Perinatal depression: a systematic review of prevalence and incidence. Obstet Gynecol, 106(5 Pt 1), 1071–1083. doi: 10.1097/01.AOG.0000183597.31630.db [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Gaynes BN, Gavin N, Meltzer-Brody S, Lohr KN, Swinson T, Gartlehner G,… Miller WC (2005). Perinatal depression: Prevalence, screening accuracy, and screening outcomes: Summary. Retrieved from [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  63. Gerardin P, Wendland J, Bodeau N, Galin A, Bialobos S, Tordjman S,… Cohen D (2011). Depression during pregnancy: is the developmental impact earlier in boys? a prospective case-control study. J Clin Psychiatry, 72(3), 378–387. doi: 10.4088/JCP.09m05724blu [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Gingrich JA, Malm H, Ansorge MS, Brown A, Sourander A, Suri D,… Weissman MM (2017). New insights into how serotonin selective reuptake inhibitors shape the developing brain. Birth Defects Research, 109(12), 924–932. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Gjone H, & Stevenson J (1997). A longitudinal twin study of temperament and behavior problems: Common genetic or environmental influences? Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(10), 1448–1456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Glover V, O’Connor TG, & O’Donnell K (2010). Prenatal stress and the programming of the HPA axis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(1), 17–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Gluckman PD, & Hanson MA (2004). Living with the past: evolution, development, and patterns of disease. Science, 305(5691), 1733–1736. doi: 10.1126/science.1095292 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Glynn LM, Dunkel Schetter C, Hobel C, & Sandman CA (2008). Pattern of perceived stress and anxiety in pregnancy predict preterm birth. Health Psychology, 27(1), 42–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Goodkind M, Eickhoff SB, Oathes DJ, Jiang Y, Chang A, Jones-Hagata LB,… Korgaonkar MS (2015). Identification of a common neurobiological substrate for mental illness. Journal of the American Medical Association Psychiatry, 72(4), 305–315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Goodman SH, & Gotlib IH (1999). Risk for psychopathology in the children of depressed mothers: a developmental model for understanding mechanisms of transmission. Psychol Rev, 106(3), 458–490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Goodman SH, Rouse MH, Connell AM, Broth MR, Hall CM, & Heyward D (2011). Maternal depression and child psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clinical child and family psychology review, 14(1), 1–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Grant K, McMahon C, Austin M, Reilly N, Leader L, & Ali S (2009). Maternal prenatal anxiety, postnatal caregiving and infants’ cortisol responses to the still-face procedure. Developmental Psychobiology, 51(8), 625–637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Grigoriadis S, VonderPorten EH, Mamisashvili L, Eady A, Tomlinson G, Dennis C,… Cheung A (2013). The effect of prenatal antidepressant exposure on neonatal adaptation: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Clin Psychiatry, 74(4), e309–e320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Grigoriadis S, VonderPorten EH, Mamisashvili L, Tomlinson G, Dennis CL, Koren G,… Ross LE (2013). The impact of maternal depression during pregnancy on perinatal outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 74(4), e321–e341. doi: 10.4088/JCP.12r07968 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Grote NK, Bridge JA, Gavin AR, Melville JL, Iyengar S, & Katon WJ (2010). A meta-analysis of depression during pregnancy and the risk of preterm birth, low birth weight, and intrauterine growth restriction. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(10), 1012–1024. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Grote NK, Katon WJ, Russo JE, Lohr MJ, Curran M, Galvin E, & Carson K (2015). Collaborative care for perinatal depression in socioeconomically disadvantaged women: A randomized trial. Depress Anxiety, 32(11), 821–834. doi: 10.1002/da.22405 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Grote NK, Swartz HA, Geibel SL, Zuckoff A, Houck PR, & Frank E (2009). A randomized controlled trial of culturally relevant, brief interpersonal psychotherapy for perinatal depression. Psychiatr Serv, 60(3), 313–321. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.60.3.313 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Grupe DW, & Nitschke JB (2013). Uncertainty and anticipation in anxiety: An integrated neurobiological and psychological perspective. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(7), 488–501. doi: 10.1038/nrn3524 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Guerry JD, & Hastings PD (2011). In search of HPA axis dysregulation in child and adolescent depression. Clinical child and family psychology review, 14(2), 135–160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Gunlicks ML, & Weissman MM (2008). Change in child psychopathology with improvement in parental depression: a systematic review. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(4), 379–389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Gunnar MR, & Vazquez D (2006). Stress neurobiology and developmental psychopathology.
  82. Haijma SV, Van Haren N, Cahn W, Koolschijn PC, Hulshoff Pol HE, & Kahn RS (2012). Brain volumes in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis in over 18 000 subjects. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 39(5), 1129–1138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Haltigan JD, Roisman GI, Susman EJ, Barnett-Walker K, & Monahan KC (2011). Elevated trajectories of externalizing problems are associated with lower awakening cortisol levels in midadolescence. Developmental Psychology, 47(2), 472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Hammen C (2005). Stress and depression. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 293–319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Hankin BL, Davis EP, Snyder H, Young JF, Glynn LM, & Sandman CA (2017). Temperament factors and dimensional, latent bifactor models of child psychopathology: Transdiagnostic and specific associations in two youth samples. Psychiatry Research, 252, 139–146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Hankin BL, Snyder HR, Gulley LD, Schweizer TH, Bijttebier P, Nelis S,… Vasey MW (2016). Understanding comorbidity among internalizing problems: Integrating latent structural models of psychopathology and risk mechanisms. Development and Psychopathology, 28(4), 987–1012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Harrington RA, Lee L-C, Crum RM, Zimmerman AW, & Hertz-Picciotto I (2014). Prenatal SSRI use and offspring with autism spectrum disorder or developmental delay. Pediatrics, 133(5), e1241–e1248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Hay DF, Pawlby S, Waters CS, Perra O, & Sharp D (2010). Mothers’ antenatal depression and their children’s antisocial outcomes. Child Dev, 81(1), 149–165. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01386.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Hendrick V, Stowe ZN, Altshuler LL, Hwang S, Lee E, & Haynes D (2003). Placental passage of antidepressant medications. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(5), 993–996. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.5.993 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Hirshfeld-Becker DR, Micco J, Henin A, Bloomfield A, Biederman J, & Rosenbaum J (2008). Behavioral inhibition. Depression and Anxiety, 25(4), 357–367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Hobfoll SE, Ritter C, Lavin J, Hulsizer MR, & Cameron RP (1995). Depression prevalence and incidence among inner-city pregnant and postpartum women. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology, 63(3), 445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Hoffman MC, Mazzoni SE, Wagner BD, Laudenslager ML, & Ross RG (2016). Measures of Maternal Stress and Mood in Relation to Preterm Birth. Obstet Gynecol, 127(3). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Hollon SD (2016). The efficacy and acceptability of psychological interventions for depression: Where we are now and where we are going. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 25(4), 295–300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Huot R, Brennan P, Stowe Z, Plotsky P, & Walker E (2004). Negative affect in offspring of depressed mothers is predicted by infant cortisol levels at 6 months and maternal depression during pregnancy, but not postpartum. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1032(1), 234–236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Hviid A, Melbye M, & Pasternak B (2013). Use of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors during pregnancy and risk of autism. New England Journal of Medicine, 369(25), 2406–2415. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1301449 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Jarde A, Morais M, Kingston D, Giallo R, MacQueen G, Giglia L,… McDonald SD (2016). Neonatal outcomes in women with untreated antenatal depression compared with women without depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA psychiatry, 73(8), 826–837. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.0934 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Jha SC, Meltzer-Brody S, Steiner RJ, Cornea E, Woolson S, Ahn M,… Styner M (2016). Antenatal depression, treatment with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, and neonatal brain structure: A propensity-matched cohort study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 253, 43–53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Joels M, & Baram TZ (2009). The neuro-symphony of stress. Nat Rev Neurosci, 10(6), 459–466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Johnson JE, Price AB, Kao JC, Fernandes K, Stout R, Gobin RL, & Zlotnick C (2016). Interpersonal psychotherapy (IPT) for major depression following perinatal loss: A pilot randomized controlled trial. Archives of Women’s Mental Health, 19(5), 845–859. doi: 10.1007/s00737-016-0625-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Johnson MB, Kawasawa YI, Mason CE, Krsnik Z, Coppola G, Bogdanovic D,… Sestan N (2009). Functional and evolutionary insights into human brain development through global transcriptome analysis. Neuron, 62(4), 494–509. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Kagan J, Reznick JS, & Snidman N (1987). The physiology and psychology of behavioral inhibition in children. Child Development, 58(6), 1459–1473. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Kaiser RH, Andrews-Hanna JR, Wager TD, & Pizzagalli DA (2015). Large-scale network dysfunction in major depressive disorder: A meta-analysis of resting-state functional connectivity. Journal of the American Medical Association Psychiatry, 72(6), 603–611. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Kaplan LA, Evans L, & Monk C (2008). Effects of mothers’ prenatal psychiatric status and postnatal caregiving on infant biobehavioral regulation: can prenatal programming be modified? Early Hum Dev, 84(4), 249–256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Merikangas KR, & Walters EE (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(6), 593–602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Ko JY, Farr SL, Dietz PM, & Robbins CL (2012). Depression and treatment among US pregnant and nonpregnant women of reproductive age, 2005–2009. Journal of Women’s Health, 21(8), 830–836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Korhonen M, Luoma I, Salmelin R, & Tamminen T (2012). A longitudinal study of maternal prenatal, postnatal and concurrent depressive symptoms and adolescent well-being. J Affect Disord, 136(3), 680–692. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.10.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Kostovic I, Judas M, Rados M, & Hrabac P (2002). Laminar organization of the human fetal cerebrum revealed by histochemical markers and magnetic resonance imaging. Cereb Cortex, 12(5), 536–544. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11950771 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Krueger RF (1999). The structure of common mental disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56(10), 921–926. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Lahey BB (2009). Public health significance of neuroticism. American Psychologist, 64(4), 241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Lahey BB, Krueger RF, Rathouz PJ, Waldman ID, & Zald DH (2017). A hierarchical causal taxonomy of psychopathology across the life span. Psychological Bulletin, 143(2), 142–186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Lebel C, Walton M, Letourneau N, Giesbrecht GF, Kaplan BJ, & Dewey D (2016). Prepartum and postpartum maternal depressive symptoms are related to children’s brain structure in preschool. Biol Psychiatry, 80(11), 859–868. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.12.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Leichsenring F, & Steinert C (2017). Is cognitive behavioral therapy the gold standard for psychotherapy? The need for plurality in treatment and research. Journal of the American Medical Association Psychiatry, 318(14), 1323–1324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Lenze SN, & Potts MA (2017). Brief interpersonal psychotherapy for depression during pregnancy in a low-income population: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Affective Disorders, 210, 151–157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Lopez-Duran NL, Kovacs M, & George CJ (2009). Hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis dysregulation in depressed children and adolescents: A meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(9), 1272–1283. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Lumey LH, Stein AD, & Susser E (2011). Prenatal famine and adult health. Annu Rev Public Health, 32, 237–262. doi: 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-031210-101230 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Lundy BL, Jones NA, Field T, Nearing G, Davalos M, Pietro PA,… Kuhn C (1999). Prenatal depression effects on neonates. Infant Behavior and Development, 22(1), 119–129. [Google Scholar]
  117. Luoma I, Tamminen T, Kaukonen P, Laippala P, Puura K, Salmelin R, & Almqvist F (2001). Longitudianl study of maternal depressive symptoms and child well-being. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(12), 1367–1374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Mäki P, Veijola J, Räsänen P, Joukamaa M, Valonen P, Jokelainen J, & Isohanni M (2003). Criminality in the offspring of antenatally depressed mothers: a 33-year follow-up of the Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort. Journal of Affective Disorders, 74(3), 273–278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Man KK, Chan EW, P., I., Coghill D, Simonoff E, Chan PK, & Wong IC (2017). Prenatal antidepressant use and risk of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in offspring: population based cohort study. Bmj, 357. doi: 10.1136/bmj.j2350 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Marcus SM (2009). Depression during pregnancy: rates, risks and consequences - Motherisk Update 2008. Can J Clin Pharmacol, 16(1), 15–22. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Marcus SM, Flynn HA, Blow FC, & Barry KL (2003). Depressive symptoms among pregnant women screened in obstetrics settings. J Womens Health (Larchmt), 12(4), 373–380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. McCarthy H, Skokauskas N, & Frodl T (2014). Identifying a consistent pattern of neural function in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 44(4), 869–880. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. McEwen BS (2004). Protection and damage from acute and chronic stress: allostasis and allostatic overload and relevance to the pathophysiology of psychiatric disorders. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1032(1), 1–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Meehl PE (1978). Theoretical risks and tabular asterisks: Sir Karl, Sir Ronald, and the slow progress of soft psychology. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46(4), 806–834. [Google Scholar]
  125. Melville JL, Reed SD, Russo J, Croicu CA, Ludman E, LaRocco-Cockburn A, & Katon W (2014). Improving care for depression in obstetrics and gynecology: A randomized controlled trial. Obstetrics and Gynecology, 123(6), 1237–1246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Miller CH, Hamilton JP, Sacchet MD, & Gotlib IH (2015). Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging of major depressive disorder in youth. Journal of the American Medical Association Psychiatry, 72(10), 1045–1053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Dickson N, Silva P, & Stanton W (1996). Childhood-onset versus adolescent-onset antisocial conduct problems in males: Natural history from ages 3 to 18 years. Development and psychopathology, 8(2), 399–424. [Google Scholar]
  128. Mulcahy R, Reay RE, Wilkinson RB, & Owen C (2010). A randomised control trial for the effectiveness of group Interpersonal Psychotherapy for postnatal depression. Arch Womens Ment Health, 13(2), 125–139. doi: 10.1007/s00737-009-0101-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Mulder E, Ververs F, De Heus R, & Visser G (2011). Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors affect neurobehavioral development in the human fetus. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36(10), 1961–1971. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.67 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Murray L, Arteche A, Fearon P, Halligan S, Croudace T, & Cooper P (2010). The effects of maternal postnatal depression and child sex on academic performance at age 16 years: a developmental approach. J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 51(10), 1150–1159. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02259.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Nathanielsz PW (1999). Life in the Womb: The Origin of Health and Disease. Ithaca, NY: Promethean Press. [Google Scholar]
  132. Niendam TA, Laird AR, Ray KL, Dean YM, Glahn DC, & Carter CS (2012). Meta-analytic evidence for a superordinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive functions. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 12(2), 241–268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Nigg JT (2017). Annual Research Review: On the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(4), 361–383. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Nulman I, Koren G, Rovet J, Barrera M, Pulver A, Streiner D, & Feldman B (2012). Neurodevelopment of children following prenatal exposure to venlafaxine, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, or untreated maternal depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 169(11), 1165–1174. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11111721 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. O’Connor TG, Monk C, & Fitelson EM (2014). Practitioner review: maternal mood in pregnancy and child development--implications for child psychology and psychiatry. J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 55(2), 99–111. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12153 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. O’Donnell KJ, Glover V, Barker ED, & O’Connor TG (2014). The persisting effect of maternal mood in pregnancy on childhood psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol, 26(2), 393–403. doi: 10.1017/s0954579414000029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. O’Donnell KJ, Glover V, Jenkins J, Browne D, Ben-Shlomo Y, Golding J, & O’Connor TG (2013). Prenatal maternal mood is associated with altered diurnal cortisol in adolescence. Psychoneuroendocrinology. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.01.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. O’Hara MW, Stuart S, Gorman LL, & Wenzel A (2000). Efficacy of interpersonal psychotherapy for postpartum depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57(11), 1039–1045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. O’Connor E, Rossom RC, Henninger M, Groom HC, & Burda BU (2016). Primary care screening for and treatment of depression in pregnant and postpartum women: Evidence report and systematic review for the US Preventive Services Task Force. Journal of the American Medical Association, 315(4), 388–406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Olino TM, Dougherty LR, Bufferd SJ, Carlson GA, & Klein DN (2014). Testing models of psychopathology in preschool-aged children using a structured interview-based assessment. Journal of abnormal child psychology, 42(7), 1201–1211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Olivier JD, Åkerud H, Kaihola H, Pawluski J, Skalkidou A, Högberg U, & Sundström-Poromaa I (2013). The effects of maternal depression and maternal selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor exposure on offspring. Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience, 7(73), 1–15. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2013.00073 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Olson SL, Bates JE, Sandy JM, & Lanthier R (2000). Early developmental precursors of externalizing behavior in middle childhood and adolescence. Journal of abnormal child psychology, 28(2), 119–133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Ormel J, Jeronimus BF, Kotov R, Riese H, Bos EH, Hankin B,… Oldehinkel AJ (2013). Neuroticism and common mental disorders: meaning and utility of a complex relationship. Clin Psychol Rev, 33(5), 686–697. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.04.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Pacheco A, & Figueiredo B (2012). Mother’s depression at childbirth does not contribute to the effects of antenatal depression on neonate’s behavioral development. Infant Behavior and Development, 35(3), 513–522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Pariante CM, & Lightman SL (2008). The HPA axis in major depression: classical theories and new developments. Trends in neurosciences, 31(9), 464–468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Pearson RM, Evans J, Kounali D, Lewis G, Heron J, Ramchandani PG,… Stein A (2013). Maternal depression during pregnancy and the postnatal period: risks and possible mechanisms for offspring depression at age 18 years. JAMA psychiatry, 70(12), 1312–1319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Peterson BS, Warner V, Bansal R, Zhu H, Hao X, Liu J,… Weissman MM (2009). Cortical thinning in persons at increased familial risk for major depression. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 106(15), 6273–6278. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805311106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Pilowsky DJ, Wickramaratne P, Talati A, Tang M, Hughes CW, Garber J,… Sood AB (2008). Children of depressed mothers 1 year after the initiation of maternal treatment: Findings from the STAR*D-Child Study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(9), 1136–1147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Plant DT, Barker ED, Waters CS, Pawlby S, & Pariante CM (2013). Intergenerational transmission of maltreatment and psychopathology: the role of antenatal depression. Psychological Medicine, 43(3), 519–528. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Plant DT, Pariante CM, Sharp D, & Pawlby S (2015). Maternal depression during pregnancy and offspring depression in adulthood: role of child maltreatment. Br J Psychiatry, 207(3), 213–220. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.114.156620 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Posner J, Cha J, Roy A, Peterson B, Bansal R, Gustafsson H,… Monk C (2016). Alterations in amygdala–prefrontal circuits in infants exposed to prenatal maternal depression. Translational psychiatry, 6(11), e935. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Qiu A, Anh TT, Li Y, Chen H, Rifkin-Graboi A, Broekman BF,… Meaney MJ (2015). Prenatal maternal depression alters amygdala functional connectivity in 6-month-old infants. Transl Psychiatry, 5, e508. doi: 10.1038/tp.2015.3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Renken B, Egeland B, Marvinney D, Mangelsdorf S, & Sroufe LA (1989). Early childhood antecedents of aggression and passive-withdrawal in early elementary school. Journal of personality, 57(2), 257–281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Rifkin-Graboi A, Bai J, Chen H, Hameed WB, Sim LW, Tint MT,… Qiu A (2013). Prenatal maternal depression associates with microstructure of right amygdala in neonates at birth. Biol Psychiatry, 74(11), 837–844. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.06.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Rogers JC, & De Brito SA (2016). Cortical and subcortical gray matter volume in youths with conduct problems: A meta-analysis. Journal of the American Medical Association Psychiatry, 73(1), 64–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Ross LE, Grigoriadis S, Mamisashvili L, VonderPorten EH, Roerecke M, Rehm J,… Mousmanis P (2013). Selected pregnancy and delivery outcomes after exposure to antidepressant medication: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA psychiatry, 70(4), 436–443. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.684 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Hersey KL, & Fisher P (2001). Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: The Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Child Development, 72(5), 1394–1408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Rothbart MK, & Bates JE (2006). Temperament. Handbook of child psychology. [Google Scholar]
  159. Rouse MH, & Goodman SH (2014). Perinatal depression influences on infant negative affectivity: Timing, severity, and co-morbid anxiety. Infant Behavior and Development, 37(4), 739–751. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Rutter M, & Uher R (2012). Classification issues and challenges in child and adolescent psychopathology. International Review of Psychiatry, 24(6), 514–529. doi: 10.3109/09540261.2012.719862 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Sandman CA, Buss C, Head K, & Davis EP (2015). Fetal exposure to maternal depressive symptoms is associated with cortical thickness in late childhood. Biol Psychiatry, 77(4), 324–334. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.06.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Sandman CA, Class QA, Glynn LM, & Davis EP (2016). Neurobehavioral disorders and developmental origins of health and disease In Rosenfeld C (Ed.), The Epigenome and Developmental Origins of Health and Disease (pp. 236–267): Academic Press/Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
  163. Scheinost D, Sinha R, Cross SN, Kwon SH, Sze G, Constable RT, & Ment LR (2017). Does prenatal stress alter the developing connectome? Pediatric Research, 81(1–2), 214–226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Shang J, Fu Y, Ren Z, Zhang T, Du M, Gong Q,… Zhang W (2014). The common traits of the ACC and PFC in anxiety disorders in the DSM-5: Meta-analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies. PLOS ONE, 9(3), 1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Smith R, Mesiano S, & McGrath S (2002). Hormone trajectories leading to human birth. Regulatory Peptides, 108(2–3), 159–164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  166. Smith SM, & Vale WW (2006). The role of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in neuroendocrine responses to stress. Dialogues in clinical neuroscience, 8(4), 383–395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  167. Snyder HR, Hankin BL, Sandman CA, Head K, & Davis EP (2017). Distinct patterns of reduced prefrontal and limbic gray matter volume in childhood general and internalizing psychopathology. Clinical Psychological Science, 5(6), 1001–1013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  168. Snyder HR, Miyake A, & Hankin BL (2015). Advancing understanding of executive function impairments and psychopathology: Bridging the gap between clinical and cognitive approaches. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(328), 1–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  169. Snyder HR, Young JF, & Hankin BL (2017). Strong homotypic continuity in common psychopathology, internalizing, and externalizing factors across 18 months in adolescents. Clinical Psychological Science, 5(1), 98–110. doi: 10.1177/2167702616651076 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  170. Sockol LE, Epperson CN, & Barber JP (2011). A meta-analysis of treatments for perinatal depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(5), 839–849. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  171. Solomonov N, & Barber JP (2016). What we know, what we do not know, and where are we heading? Efficacy and acceptability of psychological interventions for depression. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 25(4), 301–308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  172. Somhovd MJ, Hansen BM, Brok J, Esbjorn BH, & Greisen G (2012). Anxiety in adolescents born preterm or with very low birthweight: a meta-analysis of case-control studies. Dev Med Child Neurol, 54(11), 988–994. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2012.04407.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  173. Spinelli MG, & Endicott J (2003). Controlled clinical trial of interpersonal psychotherapy versus parenting education program for depressed pregnant women. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(3), 555–562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  174. Spinelli MG, Endicott J, Goetz RR, & Segre LS (2016). Reanalysis of efficacy of interpersonal psychotherapy for antepartum depression versus parenting education program: initial severity of depression as a predictor of treatment outcome. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 77(4), 535–540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  175. Stout SA, Espel EV, Sandman CA, Glynn LM, & Davis EP (2015). Fetal programming of children’s obesity risk. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 53, 29–39. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.12.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  176. Sujan AC, Rickert ME, Öberg AS, Quinn PD, Hernández-Díaz S, Almqvist C,… D’Onofrio BM (2017). Associations of maternal antidepressant use during the first trimester of pregnancy with preterm birth, small for gestational age, autism spectrum disorder, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in offspring. Jama, 317(15), 1553–1562. doi: 10.1001/jama.2017.3413 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  177. Susman EJ (2006). Psychobiology of persistent antisocial behavior: Stress, early vulnerabilities and the attenuation hypothesis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(3), 376–389. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.08.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  178. Swartz HA, Frank E, Zuckoff A, Cyranowski JM, Houck PR, Cheng Y,… Shear MK (2008). Brief interpersonal psychotherapy for depressed mothers whose children are receiving psychiatric treatment. Am J Psychiatry, 165(9), 1155–1162. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07081339 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  179. Swartz JR, Williamson DE, & Hariri AR (2015). Developmental change in amygdala reactivity during adolescence: effects of family history of depression and stressful life events. Am J Psychiatry, 172(3), 276–283. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14020195 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  180. Tackett JL, Lahey BB, Van Hulle C, Waldman I, Krueger RF, & Rathouz P. J. h. (2013). Common genetic influences on negative emotionality and a general psychopathology factor in childhood and adolescence. Journal of abnormal psychology, 122(4), 1142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  181. Thomason ME, Grove LE, Lozon TA, Vila AM, Ye Y, Nye MJ,… Yeo L (2015). Age-related increases in long-range connectivity in fetal functional neural connectivity networks in utero. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, 96–104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  182. Thomason ME, Scheinost D, Manning JH, Grove LE, Hect J, Marshall N,… Yeo L (2017). Weak functional connectivity in the human fetal brain prior to preterm birth. Scientific Reports, 7, 39286. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  183. Tollenaar MS, Beijers R, Jansen J, Riksen-Walraven JM, & de Weerth C (2011). Maternal prenatal stress and cortisol reactivity to stressors in human infants. Stress, 14(1), 53–65. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&dopt=Citation&list_uids=20666659 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  184. Van Batenburg-Eddes T, Brion MJ, Henrichs J, Jaddoe VWV, Hofman A, Verhulst FC,… Tiemeier H (2013). Parental depressive and anxiety symptoms during pregnancy and attention problems in children: A cross-cohort consistency study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(5), 591–600. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  185. Van den Bergh BR, & Marcoen A (2004). High antenatal maternal anxiety is related to ADHD symptoms, externalizing problems, and anxiety in 8- and 9-year-olds. Child Dev, 75(4), 1085–1097. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00727.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  186. Van den Bergh BR, Mennes M, Oosterlaan J, Stevens V, Stiers P, Marcoen A, & Lagae L (2005). High antenatal maternal anxiety is related to impulsivity during performance on cognitive tasks in 14- and 15-year-olds. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 29(2), 259–269. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.10.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  187. Van den Bergh BR, Van Calster B, Pinna Puissant S, & Van Huffel S (2008). Self-reported symptoms of depressed mood, trait anxiety and aggressive behavior in post-pubertal adolescents: Associations with diurnal cortisol profiles. Horm Behav, 54(2), 253–257. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.03.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  188. Van den Bergh BR, Van Calster B, Smits T, Van Huffel S, & Lagae L (2008). Antenatal maternal anxiety is related to HPA-axis dysregulation and self-reported depressive symptoms in adolescence: a prospective study on the fetal origins of depressed mood. Neuropsychopharmacology, 33(3), 536–545. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301450 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  189. van der Knaap NJF, Klumpers F, El Marroun H, Mous S, Schubert D, Jaddoe V,… White T (2017). Maternal depressive symptoms during pregnancy are associated with amygdala hyperresponsivity in children. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  190. Vänskä M, Punamäki R, Lindblom J, Tolvanen A, Flykt M, Unkila-Kallio L,… Tiitinen A (2016). Timing of early maternal mental health and child cortisol regulation. Infant and Child Development, 25(6), 461–483. [Google Scholar]
  191. Viktorin A, Lichtenstein P, Lundholm C, Almqvist C, D’Onofrio BM, Larsson H,… Magnusson PKE (2016). Selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitor use during pregnancy: Association with offspring birth size and gestational age. International Journal of Epidemiology, 45(1), 170–177. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyv351 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  192. Volpe JJ (2008). Neurology of the newborn (5 ed.). Philedelphia: Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
  193. Vrshek-Schallhorn S, Doane LD, Mineka S, Zinbarg RE, Craske MG, & Adam EK (2013). The cortisol awakening response predicts major depression: predictive stability over a 4-year follow-up and effect of depression history. Psychological medicine, 43(3), 483–493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  194. Weikum WM, Oberlander TF, Hensch TK, & Werker JF (2012). Prenatal exposure to antidepressants and depressed maternal mood alter trajectory of infant speech perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(Supplement 2), 17221–17227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  195. Weissman MM, Pilowsky DJ, Wickramaratne PJ, Talati A, Wisniewski SR, Fava M,… King CA (2006). Remissions in maternal depression and child psychopathology: A STAR*D-child report. Journal of the American Medical Association, 295(12), 1389–1398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  196. Werner EA, Myers MM, Fifer WP, Cheng B, Fang Y, Allen R, & Monk C (2007). Prenatal predictors of infant temperament. Developmental psychobiology, 49(5), 474–484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  197. White RH, Bates ME, & Buyske S (2001). Adolescence-limited versus persistent delinquency: Extending Moffitt’s hypothesis into adulthood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110(4), 600–609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  198. Wickramaratne P, Gameroff MJ, Pilowsky DJ, Hughes CW, Garber J, Malloy E,… Alpert JE (2011). Children of depressed mothers 1 year after remission of maternal depression: Findings from the STAR*D-Child study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(6), 593–602. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  199. Wise T, Radua J, Via E, Cardoner N, Abe O, Adams TM,… Perico CAM (2017). Common and distinct patterns of grey-matter volume alteration in major depression and bipolar disorder: Evidence from voxel-based meta-analysis. Molecular Psychiatry, 22(10), 1455–1463. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  200. Yehuda R (2002). Current status of cortisol findings in post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 25(2), 341–368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  201. Zald DH, & Lahey BB (2017). Implications of the hierarchical structure of psychopathology for psychiatric neuroimaging. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 2(4), 310–317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  202. Zlotnick C, Miller IW, Pearlstein T, Howard M, & Sweeney P (2006). A preventive intervention for pregnant women on public assistance at risk for postpartum depression. Am J Psychiatry, 163(8), 1443–1445. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.8.1443 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  203. Zlotnick C, Tzilos G, Miller I, Seifer R, & Stout R (2016). Randomized controlled trial to prevent postpartum depression in mothers on public assistance. Journal of Affective Disorders, 189, 263–268. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.09.059 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES