Abstract
Chronic pain is common in children and increases their risk for developing a chronic pain condition in adulthood, yet relatively little is known about early parental psychosocial factors that predict the development of chronic pain in childhood. We examined the extent to which chronic pain frequency in a community sample of 6-year-old children was related to frequency of chronic pain in their parents, and was prospectively predicted by early maternal risk (i.e., depressive symptoms) and promotive (i.e., hope) factors. Fifty primary caregivers (94% mothers) of 6-year-old twin children who were enrolled in a larger study during children’s infancy were randomly selected to complete a telephone interview regarding their own, their partner’s, and their children’s pain symptoms and functioning. Pain symptom scores were derived by summing the number of seven possible body areas that were painful at least monthly during the prior 6 months. Pain symptoms at three or more sites were coded as multisite pain. Prior maternal depressive symptoms and hope were assessed when children were aged 12-months. Pain symptom scores were positively correlated within families, and risk of child pain increased in a dose-response fashion according to whether neither, one, or both parents experienced multisite pain. Maternal hope but not depressive symptoms prospectively predicted fewer painful body regions in children five years later. Findings suggest that pain runs in families and pain in childhood may be influenced by early maternal psychosocial factors. Future research should focus on how parents’ own health and psychological attributes influence risk for children’s chronic pain.
Keywords: children, chronic pain, depressive symptoms, hope
Introduction
Chronic pain is common among children and is associated with concurrent disability as well as a higher risk for having a chronic pain syndrome in adulthood (Brattberg, 2004). In line with commonly used definitions, we refer to recurrent or persistent pain exceeding three months in duration as chronic pain (King et al., 2011). Median prevalence rates of weekly to monthly chronic pain in community samples of children range from 11-38% (King et al., 2011). However, rates can be substantially higher; in one community sample of children aged 6 to 13, 64% reported pain occurring at least monthly (Petersen, Brulin, & Bergström, 2006). Childhood chronic pain is associated with poorer quality of life, including interference with academics, peer relationships, and family functioning (Palermo, 2000). Furthermore, the literature suggests that childhood chronic pain is stable and persists into adulthood (Brattberg, 2004). Thus, identifying psychosocial factors associated with risk for childhood chronic pain is critical to informing prevention and treatment efforts.
One risk factor for chronic pain in children is parental pain. Children who have neither, one, or two parents with chronic pain experience increased risk of pain in a dose-response pattern (Hoftun, Romundstad & Rygg, 2013). Although the mechanisms underlying the parent-to-child transmission of chronic pain remain unclear, environmental influences may be especially impactful during infancy and early childhood, as rapid brain development represents a sensitive developmental period (Fox, Levitt, & Nelson, 2010) during which learning and formation of health habits occur (Sallis & Nader, 1988). Maternal psychosocial characteristics are one environmental factor thought to influence the development of children’s physical (Taylor et al., 2004) and emotional well-being (Bowlby, 2008). Maternal depression has been associated with increased pain experiences in children, particularly recurrent headaches and abdominal pain (Zuckerman, Stevenson, & Bailey, 1987). The causal direction of these relations is unclear, but evidence suggests that maternal depression may increase risk of benign pain in children through coercive parenting styles, modeling of somatization, and/or reinforcement of children’s illness behaviors (Feldman et al., 2010; Zuckerman et al., 1987).
Little is known about whether positive maternal attributes prevent the development of chronic pain in children, but some findings suggest that maternal positive attributes are indirectly related to better pain-related child outcomes through parenting behaviors (Stone & Wilson, 2016). For example, parental optimism toward their child’s pain condition is linked with lower levels of catastrophizing about their child’s pain (Noel, Beals-Erickson, Law, Alberts & Palermo, 2016), a known risk factor for pediatric chronic pain (Stone & Wilson, 2016).
A related but less studied promotive factor is hope, defined as “the perceived capability to derive pathways to desired goals and to motivate oneself via agency thinking to use those pathways” (Snyder, 2002). Though closely related to optimism, hope is a better predictor of life satisfaction than is optimism (Bailey, Eng, Frisch & Snyder, 2007). Hope represents expectations about efficacy to carry out goals whereas optimism represents expectations about outcomes (Bailey et al., 2007). Hope is linked with higher pain tolerance and lower pain severity among healthy adults (Snyder et al., 2005), and more effective pain management among adults with chronic pain (Laird, 1992; Tennen & Affleck, 1999). Based on these findings, we posited that early maternal hope would predict fewer child pain symptoms. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to examine early maternal hope as a predictor of later child pain outcomes.
The present study interviewed mothers regarding themselves, their partners, and their twin offspring to investigate whether: 1) pain symptoms are associated among family members, 2) presence of parental multisite pain predicts offspring pain in a dose-response fashion, and 3) maternal depressive symptoms and hope measured at child age 12-months predict pain at child age 6 years.
Methods
Participants and Procedure
The sample comprised 50 primary caregivers and their 6-year-old twins (N=100) randomly selected from a larger ongoing study to participate in an assessment of pain in parents and their children. The larger study consisted of 303 families recruited from state birth records, enrolled during twins’ infancy, and represent families living in both rural and urban environments. The current study focused on chronic pain in children, and was not powered to determine the genetic contributions to children’s health.
The sample was balanced in terms of sex (51% female) and was ethnically diverse (60% Caucasian, 24% Latino, 10% multi-racial, 2% African American, 2% Asian, 2% American Indian). Primary caregivers averaged 36.56 years (SD = 4.48) when the children were age 6, and reported a median annual household income of $50,000 and a median education level of a four-year college degree. The 50 families were similar to the remaining families in the parent study on child gender and ethnicity (χ 2 < 1.44, ns), and parent education and family income level (ts < −1.93, ns). The study protocol was approved by the institution’s review board. All 50 families contacted by phone agreed to participate, provided verbal consent, and were compensated for their participation. Data regarding maternal psychological attributes were obtained from questionnaires completed during an earlier assessment that occurred at child age 12-months. Single-parent households made up 12% (n=6) of the sample. Parental informants of pain symptoms primarily consisted of mothers (n=47; 94%), and most families used the same parental informant at child age 12-months and child age-6 years (n=47; 94%).
Measures
Pain and functioning.
An adapted structured pain questionnaire previously used to assess back pain, headache, stomach pain, and facial pain in a population-based sample of 11 to 17 year old children and their parents was used (Saunders, Von Korff, LeResche & Mancl, 2007). Adaptations included adding items to assess limb, throat, and other pain, and adding a frequency rating adopted from another community-based study of pain in children ages 9 to 11 (Mikkelsson, Salminen & Kautiainen, 1997). Mothers were asked to rate their own, their partner’s, and each of their children’s pain frequency over the past 6 months for each of the seven body regions. Items were rated on the following 5-point scale: 1 (daily), 2 (more than once per week), 3 (weekly), 4 (monthly), 5 (rarely or never). Informants were asked to include only pain episodes that lasted a whole day or more, or that occurred several times in a year (Saunders et al., 2007). Pain symptom scores were computed by summing the number of possible body regions in which pain was rated as occurring at least monthly over the prior six months, yielding scores that could range from 0 to 7, with higher scores reflecting greater number of body regions with at least monthly pain. Consistent with Hoftun et al. (2013), presence of multisite pain was coded for mothers and fathers if three or more body regions were experienced as painful at least monthly over the prior six months. Pain intensity was not assessed.
Mothers also indicated whether pain experiences had interfered with attending school/work/volunteer duties ( 0 = no, 1 = yes) for themselves, their partners, and their children, and how many times they sought medical treatments for their own pain, their partners’ pain, and their children’s pain during the prior 6 months.
Maternal depressive symptoms.
Symptoms of maternal depression were assessed by self-report at child age 12-months using the 20-item Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977). The frequency of experiencing each depression symptom over the past week was rated on a scale ranging from 0 (rarely or none of the time) to 3 (most or almost all of the time). Items were summed, yielding a total score that could range from 0 to 60. The measure had excellent internal consistency in the current sample (α = .91).
Maternal hope.
Maternal hope was assessed by self-report at child age 12-months using the Temporality & Future (10 items; “I believe that each day has potential”) and Positive Readiness (10 items; “I can see a light even in a tunnel”) subscales of the Herth Hope Scale (Herth, 1991). Participants rated how often in the past week each item applied to them on scale that ranged from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Items were summed, yielding a total score that could range from 20 to 100, with higher scores reflecting higher levels of hope. The measure had excellent internal consistency in the current sample (α = .92).
Analytic Strategy
Pearson correlations were computed to evaluate the strength of associations between parents’ and twins’ pain symptoms. Because of the hierarchical data structure, we used multilevel modeling (Gelman & Hill, 2006) with twins clustered within families (controlling for twin dependence) to evaluate relations: between the number of parents with multisite pain and child number of areas of pain, and between early maternal depressive symptom and hope scores and subsequent child pain. Models included child sex and ethnicity (0=boys, 1=girls; 0=Caucasian, 1=other) as covariates, and the latter model additionally controlled for maternal pain at child age 6-years. Dependence between children within families was ignored for correlations and descriptive statistics. All analyses were conducted using SPSS Version 22.
Results
Descriptive Statistics, Correlations, and Pain Prevalence
Table 1 lists the descriptive statistics and correlations of the primary study variables. The CESD maternal depression mean score assessed at child age 12-months was 10.72, falling below the cut-off score of 16 that indicates clinical levels of depression (Weissman et al., 1977). Nevertheless, 20% of mothers had CESD scores of 16 or greater, a range consistent with clinically significant depression. As expected, maternal hope was inversely related to maternal depressive symptoms, maternal pain, paternal pain, and child pain. Maternal depressive symptoms were positively related to maternal pain and child pain but were unrelated to paternal pain.
Table 1.
Variable | M | SD | Skewness | Kurtosis | Min, Max | Correlations | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | ||||||
1 Maternal hope | 82.91 | 11.47 | −0.55 | −0.38 | 56, 100 | −.58** | −.43** | −.30** | −.42** |
2 Maternal depressive symptoms | 10.72 | 9.68 | 1.50 | 1.60 | 0, 39 | - | .55** | .22 | .24* |
3 Maternal pain | 2.08 | 1.43 | 0.19 | −0.69 | 0, 5 | - | - | .37** | .21* |
4 Paternal pain | 1.71 | 1.44 | 0.42 | −0.91 | 0, 5 | - | - | - | .34** |
5 Child pain | 1.02 | 1.19 | 0.95 | −0.16 | 0, 4 | - | - | - | - |
Note: Maternal hope and depressive symptoms assessed at child age 12 months; all pain variables assessed at child age 6 years. Pain scores are sum of seven possible body regions in which pain was experienced at least monthly during the prior six months, with higher scores reflecting greater number of body regions with at least monthly pain.
p < 0.05;
p < 0.01.
The most common types of at least monthly pain in children were abdominal pain (33%), limb pain (21%), and headache (19%); in mothers were headache (64%), backache (54%), and limb pain (26%); and in fathers were backache (52%), headache (40%), and stomach and limb pain (both 29%). For children, the prevalence of at least monthly pain in one or more regions was 54%, in two or more regions was 29%, and in three or more regions was 15%. In mothers and fathers, the prevalence rates of at least monthly pain in one or more regions were 82% and 80%, two or more regions were 66% and 58%, and in three or more regions were 38% and 26%, respectively. Regarding the interference of pain in daily life during the prior 6 months, 16% of children missed school due to pain, and 20% of parents missed work or other scheduled activities due to pain. Regarding treatment seeking, 39% of parents and 31% of children sought medical treatment for pain over the last 6 months.
Prediction of Pain Symptoms
Pain symptom scores correlated between parents (r=.37), between siblings (r=.68), and between children and their mothers (r=.21) and fathers (r=.34), all ps < .05. Risk of child pain increased in a dose–response fashion according to whether none, one, or both parents reported multisite pain (F(1,39) = 5.30, p < .05), controlling for child sex and ethnicity. The relation between parent multisite pain and child pain is depicted in Figure 1. Mean number of body regions in children with at least monthly pain increased from 0.74 (SD =1.08) to 1.03 (SD =1.14) to 1.92 (SD =0.79) according to whether neither, one, or both parents reported multisite pain, respectively.
Table 2 presents findings of the regression model examining the relations between maternal depressive symptoms and hope at child age 12-months, and child pain scores at age 6 years. Maternal hope but not depressive symptoms prospectively predicted fewer painful body regions in children (F(1,40) = 7.44, p < .01). In a model predicting child pain at age 6 years from early maternal depressive symptoms (without maternal hope), depressive symptoms remained non-significant (F(1,43) = 3.24, p = .079).
Table 2.
Variable | Betas | Std. Error | p-value |
---|---|---|---|
Predictors | |||
Maternal hope (child-12 mo) | −.042 | .015 | .009 |
Maternal depressive symptoms (child-12 mo) | −.004 | .020 | .851 |
Covariates | |||
Current maternal paina | .008 | .126 | .949 |
Child sex (0=male; 1=female) | .122 | .200 | .543 |
Child ethnicity (0=Caucasian; 1=other) | −.677 | .308 | .034 |
Child and maternal pain scores are the sum of seven possible body regions in which pain was experienced at least monthly during the prior 6 months, with higher scores reflecting greater number of body regions with at least monthly pain.
Discussion
This study examined the prevalence, consequences, and clustering of chronic pain within families in a community sample of 6-year old children and their parents, and examined whether maternal depressive symptoms and hope prospectively predicted child pain. Findings indicate that chronic pain in children and their parents was common, with over half of children and three-quarters of parents having at least monthly pain in one or more body regions. These prevalence rates are consistent with those observed in a community sample of adolescents and their mothers (Saunders et al., 2007), suggesting that young children are experiencing prevalence rates of at least monthly pain that are similar to those seen in samples of adolescents.
Findings also suggest that there are meaningful consequences of childhood chronic pain, including interference with daily living activities and utilization of health care services to specifically address pain. Consistent with the literature, pain in this sample was linked to interference with attending school (Palermo, 2000; Smith et al., 2001) and with medical attention seeking to manage child pain (Huguet & Miró, 2008). Together, the emerging evidence suggests that chronic pain in early to middle childhood may have immediate impacts on daily functioning and may impose meaningful costs on families and the healthcare system.
Pain symptoms were significantly and positively associated between siblings, parents, and parents and their offspring, indicating clustering of pain within families. The association between parental pain and child pain was such that the risk of pain in children increased according to whether neither, one, or both parents experienced multisite chronic pain, similar to the pattern observed previously in adolescents (Hoftun et al., 2013). Thus, the increased risk for child pain that is associated with parent pain may begin well before adolescence.
We examined the contributions of parental psychosocial factors to child pain by testing the extent to which maternal characteristics assessed when children were 12-months old related to child pain at age six. Maternal hope prospectively predicted fewer painful body regions in offspring, a relation sustained even when controlling for maternal depressive symptoms, current maternal pain, child sex, and child ethnicity. To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine whether positive maternal attributes evident during children’s infancy predict pain later in childhood. One possible mechanism accounting for this association is the mother’s own stress and pain management, as hope is related to greater agency and ability to problem-solve (Snyder et al., 2005), as well as more effective chronic pain management (Laird, 1992; Tennen & Affleck, 1999). Another possibility is that better family functioning and parenting practices mediate the relation between maternal hope and child pain, as poor family functioning, family conflict, maternal stress, and coercive parenting styles have been linked with more pain in children (Feldman et al., 2010; Juang, Wang, Fuh, Lu & Chen, 2004; Lewandowski, Palermo, Stinson, Handley & Chambers, 2010). Although the literature is limited, one study reported that parental hope is linked with better family functioning and more parental warmth in families with children with externalizing disorders (Kashdan et al., 2002). Additional research is needed to both replicate and explain the relation between maternal hope and subsequent child pain.
In contrast to hope, maternal depressive symptoms assessed in children’s infancy were not related to pain at age six, whereas other investigations have reported cross-sectional relations between maternal depression and child pain (Zuckerman et al., 1987). The lack of correspondence of findings between studies is not likely due to low levels of depressive symptoms in the current sample. Indeed, 20% of mothers in the present study had depression scores in the clinical range, a rate that is slightly higher than the rates seen in other community samples (Knight, Williams, McGee, & Olaman, 1997), but in line with literature suggesting that mothers of twins may have higher rates of depression than mothers of singletons (Thorpe, Golding, MacGillivray & Greenwood, 1991). One notable difference between the two maternal factors, hope and depressive symptoms, is that hope is trait-like and less subject to change, whereas depressive symptoms may be more variable (Matthey, Barnett, Ungerer & Waters, 2000; Snyder et al., 1991). Therefore, maternal hope may represent a more reliable predictor of child pain assessed five years later than depressive symptoms. Together, the current findings highlight the value of considering not only risk but also promotive attributes of parents as factors that may contribute to subsequent child pain.
This study has a number of important strengths. First, the community-based sample was diverse and included participants from a range of ethnic and socioeconomic backgrounds. Moreover, recruitment of community residents rather than individuals seeking treatment made it possible to assess the full range of pain symptoms in both parents and children. Finally, the longitudinal design allowed us to test the prospective relations between maternal hope and depressive symptoms and child pain, a rare opportunity in research on the development of pain in early childhood.
Although intriguing, the findings should be interpreted with the methodological limitations in mind. First, the sample size was small, limiting our ability to test complex developmental models or evaluate heritability of pain. Second, reports of children’s and parents’ pain were provided by a single reporter, leaving open the possibility that common method variance accounts for at least part of the familial clustering of pain reports. Of note, parent reports of child symptoms are commonly used, as young children lack sufficient cognitive skills to be able to answer questions effectively (De Leeuw, 2011). Finally, the causes of family members’ pain experiences, which could include disease, injury, or other factors (Treede et al., 2015), were unknown, precluding examination of whether findings varied as a function of pain etiology. Future work replicating these findings in a larger sample using multiple reporters and more detailed methods of assessment is needed.
In summary, pain is common in early childhood and clusters within families. Child pain is related in a dose-response fashion to the presence of parental multisite pain, highlighting the need to uncover mechanisms involved in the intergenerational transmission of pain. One avenue we explored was maternal depressive symptoms and hope during infancy as predictors of subsequent child pain. Our findings point to the potential importance of early maternal attributes that promote healthy development, rather than solely risk, for targeted efforts to prevent the development of chronic pain in childhood.
Footnotes
Conflict of Interest: The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Ethical Approval: “All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.”
Informed Consent: Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
Reference List
- Bailey TC, Eng W, Frisch MB, & Snyder CR (2007). Hope and optimism as related to life satisfaction. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 2(3), 168–175. [Google Scholar]
- Bowlby J (2008). A secure base: Parent-child attachment and healthy human development. Basic books. [Google Scholar]
- Brattberg G (2004). Do pain problems in young school children persist into early adulthood? A 13-year follow-up. European Journal of Pain, 8(3), 187–199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Leeuw ED (2011). Improving data quality when surveying children and adolescents: Cognitive and social development and its role in questionnaire construction and pretesting. In Report prepared for the Annual Meeting of the Academy of Finland: Research programs public health challenges and health and welfare of children and young people (pp. 10–12). [Google Scholar]
- Feldman JM, Ortega AN, Koinis-Mitchell D, Kuo AA, & Canino G (2010). Child and family psychiatric and psychological factors associated with child physical health problems: results from the Boricua youth study. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 198(4), 272. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox SE, Levitt P, & Nelson CA III. (2010). How the timing and quality of early experiences influence the development of brain architecture. Child Development, 81(1), 28–40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gelman A, & Hill J (2006). Data analysis using regression and multilevel/hierarchical models. Cambridge university press. [Google Scholar]
- Herth K (1991). Development and refinement of an instrument to measure hope. Research and Theory for Nursing Practice, 5(1), 39. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoftun GB, Romundstad PR, & Rygg M (2013). Association of parental chronic pain with chronic pain in the adolescent and young adult: family linkage data from the HUNT Study. JAMA Pediatrics, 167(1), 61–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huguet A, & Miró J (2008). The severity of chronic pediatric pain: an epidemiological study. The Journal of Pain, 9(3), 226–236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Juang KD, Wang SJ, Fuh JL, Lu SR, & Chen YS (2004). Association between adolescent chronic daily headache and childhood adversity: a community-based study. Cephalalgia, 24(1), 54–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kashdan TB, Pelham WE, Lang AR, Hoza B, Jacob RG, Jennings JR, … & Gnagy EM. (2002). Hope and optimism as human strengths in parents of children with externalizing disorders: Stress is in the eye of the beholder. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 21(4), 441–468. [Google Scholar]
- King S, Chambers CT, Huguet A, MacNevin RC, McGrath PJ, Parker L, & MacDonald AJ (2011). The epidemiology of chronic pain in children and adolescents revisited: a systematic review. Pain, 152(12), 2729–2738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knight RG, Williams S, McGee R, & Olaman S (1997). Psychometric properties of the Centre for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) in a sample of women in middle life. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(4), 373–380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laird SP (1994). A preliminary investigation into the role of prayer as a coping technique for adult patients with arthritis.
- Lewandowski AS, Palermo TM, Stinson J, Handley S, & Chambers CT (2010). Systematic review of family functioning in families of children and adolescents with chronic pain. The Journal of Pain, 11(11), 1027–1038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthey S, Barnett B, Ungerer J, & Waters B (2000). Paternal and maternal depressed mood during the transition to parenthood. Journal of Affective Disorders, 60(2), 75–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mikkelsson M, Salminen JJ, & Kautiainen H (1997). Non-specific musculoskeletal pain in preadolescents. Prevalence and 1-year persistence. Pain, 73(1), 29–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noel M, Beals-Erickson SE, Law EF, Alberts N, & Palermo TM (2016). Characterizing the pain narratives of parents of youth with chronic pain. The Clinical Journal of Pain, 32(10), 849. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palermo TM (2000). Impact of recurrent and chronic pain on child and family daily functioning: a critical review of the literature. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petersen S, Brulin C, & Bergström E (2006). Recurrent pain symptoms in young schoolchildren are often multiple. Pain, 121(1-2), 145–150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radloff LS (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1(3), 385–401. [Google Scholar]
- Sallis JF, & Nader PR (1988). Family determinants of health behaviors In Health Behavior (pp. 107–124). Springer, Boston, MA. [Google Scholar]
- Saunders K, Von Korff M, LeResche L, & Mancl L (2007). Relationship of common pain conditions in mothers and children. The Clinical Journal of Pain, 23(3), 204–213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith BH, Elliott AM, Chambers WA, Smith WC, Hannaford PC, & Penny K (2001). The impact of chronic pain in the community. Family Practice, 18(3), 292–299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snyder CR (2002). Hope theory: Rainbows in the mind. Psychological Inquiry, 13(4), 249–275. [Google Scholar]
- Snyder CR, Berg C, Woodward JT, Gum A, Rand KL, Wrobleski KK, … & Hackman A. (2005). Hope against the cold: Individual differences in trait hope and acute pain tolerance on the cold pressor task. Journal of Personality, 73(2), 287–312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snyder CR, Harris C, Anderson JR, Holleran SA, Irving LM, Sigmon ST, … & Harney P. (1991). The will and the ways: Development and validation of an individual-differences measure of hope. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(4), 570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stone AL, & Wilson AC (2016). Transmission of risk from parents with chronic pain to offspring: an integrative conceptual model. Pain, 157(12), 2628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor SE, Lerner JS, Sage RM, Lehman BJ, & Seeman TE (2004). Early environment, emotions, responses to stress, and health. Journal of Personality, 72(6), 1365–1394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tennen H, & Affleck G (1999). Finding benefits in adversity. Coping: The psychology of what works, 279–304. [Google Scholar]
- Thorpe K, Golding J, MacGillivray I, & Greenwood R (1991). Comparison of prevalence of depression in mothers of twins and mothers of singletons. British Journal of Medicine, 302(6781), 875–878. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Treede RD, Rief W, Barke A, Aziz Q, Bennett MI, Benoliel R, … & Giamberardino MA. (2015). A classification of chronic pain for ICD-11. Pain, 156(6), 1003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weissman MM, Sholomskas D, Pottenger M, Prusoff BA, & Locke BZ (1977). Assessing depressive symptoms in five psychiatric populations: a validation study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 106(3), 203–214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zuckerman B, Stevenson J, & Bailey V (1987). Stomachaches and headaches in a community sample of preschool children. Pediatrics, 79(5), 677–682. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]