Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Apr 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Psychiatr Res. 2020 Jan 29;123:95–101. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2020.01.016

Sluggish Cognitive Tempo and ADHD Symptoms in Relation to Task-Unrelated Thought: Examining Unique Links with Mind-Wandering and Rumination

Joseph W Fredrick 1, Michael J Kofler 2, Matthew A Jarrett 3, G Leonard Burns 4, Aaron M Luebbe 1, Annie A Garner 5, Sherelle L Harmon 6, Stephen P Becker 7,8
PMCID: PMC7047632  NIHMSID: NIHMS1559438  PMID: 32045730

Abstract

Recent theoretical and empirical evidence highlights associations between attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms and task-unrelated thought, including mind-wandering and rumination. However, it has been hypothesized that sluggish cognitive tempo (SCT), characterized by daydreaming and staring behaviors, may uniquely relate to task-unrelated thought. The purpose of the present study was to test whether SCT symptoms are associated with greater mind-wandering and rumination, and whether this association remains when controlling for ADHD and internalizing symptoms. Participants (N = 4,679; 18–29 years; 69% female; 80.9% White) enrolled in six universities in the United States completed measures of SCT, ADHD symptoms, internalizing symptoms, and rumination, as well as two scales used to assess mind-wandering. Although ADHD symptoms were correlated with greater self-reported mind-wandering and rumination, relations with mind-wandering on the daydreaming frequency scale, reflective rumination, and brooding rumination were attenuated when controlling for SCT and internalizing symptoms. Above and beyond other psychopathology dimensions, SCT symptoms were uniquely associated with greater self-reported mind-wandering and both reflective and brooding rumination. Additionally, SCT symptoms were more strongly associated than other psychopathology dimensions with the mind-wandering measure of daydreaming frequency. Results provide the first empirical support for unique and robust associations between SCT symptoms and task-unrelated thought, while suggesting that the link between ADHD and mind-wandering may be less robust than previously suggested.

Keywords: ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, daydreaming, mind wandering, rumination, sluggish cognitive tempo


There has been growing interest in the relation between psychopathology and task-unrelated thought, the pattern and content of thoughts that are unrelated to the task at hand (Christoff, Irving, Fox, Spreng, & Andrews-Hanna, 2016). Two forms of task-unrelated thought, mind-wandering (e.g., decoupling of thought from external environment) and rumination (e.g., thoughts marked by negatively-valenced content), were recently theorized as underlying symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Bozhilova, Michelini, Kuntsi, & Asherson, 2018). However, the extent to which specific ADHD dimensions are related to these types of task-unrelated thought remains unclear (Lanier, Noyes, & Biederman, 2019). Furthermore, no study has examined whether sluggish cognitive tempo (SCT) symptoms are associated with task-unrelated thought. This is surprising since SCT is highly correlated with ADHD symptoms and is characterized in part by behaviors such as excessive daydreaming, mental confusion, and staring into space (Becker et al., 2016). Accordingly, the present study examined both ADHD and SCT symptoms in relation to multiple types of task-unrelated thought.

ADHD and Task-Unrelated Thought

Research has recently begun to investigate ADHD and task-unrelated thought. Specifically, ADHD symptoms have been associated with greater self-reported mind-wandering (Biederman et al., 2019; Franklin et al., 2017; Seli, Smallwood, Cheyne, & Smilek, 2015), excessive daydreaming (Jonkman, Markus, Franklin, & Van Dalfsen, 2017), and task-unrelated thoughts during experimental tasks (Jonkman et al., 2017; Van Den Driessche et al., 2017). ADHD inattentive symptoms may be more consistently and strongly associated with mind-wandering relative to ADHD hyperactive-impulsive symptoms (Biederman et al., 2019; Jonkman et al., 2017), though other studies have found both ADHD dimensions to be independently associated with increased reports of mind-wandering (Biederman et al., 2019; Mowlem, Agnew-Blais, Pingault, & Asherson, 2019; Mowlem, Skirrow, et al., 2019). In sum, research supports a link between ADHD symptomatology and mind-wandering, although there is inconclusive evidence on specific links with ADHD dimensions.

Compared to mind-wandering, there has been little interest in the relation between ADHD symptomatology and rumination. Research has demonstrated significant correlations between ADHD symptoms and self-reported ruminative responses to stress (Yeguez, Hill, Buitron, & Pettit, 2018), in addition to greater self-reported rumination among college students with elevated inattentive symptoms (Jonkman et al., 2017). There is thus preliminary evidence for an association between ADHD inattention and increased rumination.

Overall, although research to date has shown that ADHD symptoms (particularly inattention) are associated with task-unrelated thought, a simultaneous body of research has emerged that highlights a unique collection of attentional difficulties that may better explain ADHD’s association with task-unrelated thought.

SCT and Task-Unrelated Thought

Sluggish cognitive tempo (SCT) is characterized by excessive daydreaming, inconsistent alertness, and underactive behavior. Although initially viewed as a specifier of, or even redundant with, ADHD inattentive symptoms, a meta-analysis found strong support for SCT symptoms being separable from ADHD inattentive symptoms across child, adolescent, and adult populations (Becker et al., 2016). It is important to investigate the association between SCT and task-unrelated thought given its potential to advance theory and identify mechanisms underlying the SCT phenotype. SCT includes both slow/sluggish behaviors in addition to daydreaming/inconsistent alertness behaviors (Becker et al., 2016; Fenollar Cortés, Servera, Becker, & Burns, 2017; Kamradt, Momany, & Nikolas, 2018), with the latter characteristics being conceptualized as an excessive form of poor attentional control and mind-wandering (Adams, Milich, & Fillmore, 2010). This is an especially intriguing possibility given that SCT symptoms are at least in part defined by internal distractibility (e.g., daydreaming) while ADHD symptoms are in part defined by external distractibility (e.g., extraneous stimuli) (Becker, Burns, Leopold, Olson, & Willcutt, 2018). Moreover, poor internal attentional control, including an inability to disengage from thoughts, has been linked to greater rumination (Whitmer & Gotlib, 2013). In fact, a recent study found that adolescent-reported SCT symptoms, but not ADHD inattentive symptoms, were uniquely associated with greater reflective and brooding rumination (Becker, Burns, Smith, & Langberg, 2019). It is thus surprising that more studies have yet to directly examine SCT in relation to task-unrelated thought, particularly given the growing interest in whether mind wandering is a core feature or correlate of ADHD symptoms.

Distinguishing between Types of Task-Unrelated Thought

Before testing relations between ADHD and SCT symptoms and task-unrelated thought, it is important to distinguish between different types of thought. In this study, we focus on mind-wandering and rumination given that they are both task-unrelated thoughts but also differ in their constraints and content. As described within a dynamic framework of mind-wandering, “both mind-wandering and rumination tend to be stimulus independent and unrelated to the current task…However, although thoughts during mind-wandering are free to ‘move hither and thither’, thoughts during rumination tend to remain fixed on a single theme or topic” (Christoff et al., 2016). Thus, mind-wandering is a type of spontaneous thought, whereas rumination is a type of thought with strong automatic constraints (Christoff et al., 2016). In addition, mind-wandering has been conceptualized as containing a variety of broad content (e.g., thoughts/ideas unrelated to a current task), while rumination unique focuses on negative-valenced content (e.g., thinking about past and current difficulties) (Christoff et al., 2016).

Specifically, mind-wandering is a universal experience pertaining to the decoupling of thought from the external environment toward task-unrelated thoughts (Jonkman et al., 2017). Though mind-wandering is characterized by both deliberate and spontaneous thought, most research has focused on the latter (Seli et al., 2015). Daydreaming, conceptualized as a marker of mind-wandering with a focus on internal, often imaginative, stimuli (McMillan, Kaufman, & Singer, 2013), has been correlated with higher self-reported mind-wandering (Chiorri & Vannucci, 2019; Forster & Lavie, 2014), less self-reported present moment awareness (Marchetti, Van De Putte, & Koster, 2014), and higher task-unrelated thoughts assessed during sustained attention tasks (Mrazek, Smallwood, & Schooler, 2012; Stawarczyk, Majerus, Van Der Linden, & D’Argembeau, 2012). Although mind-wandering and daydreaming have been linked to adaptive outcomes (e.g., creativity, problem-solving), research has also documented associations with increased internalizing symptoms (Marchetti, Koster, Klinger, & Alloy, 2016) and reduced performance during cognitively challenging tasks (Lanier et al., 2019).

In contrast to mind-wandering, rumination is characterized by perseveration on negative affective states and is often subdivided into reflective rumination (e.g., effectively thinking about solutions) and brooding rumination (e.g., passively focusing on distress) (Nolen-Hoeksema, Wisco, & Lyubomirsky, 2008; Treynor, Gonzalez, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2003). Although originally viewed as adaptive (Treynor et al., 2003), emerging research indicates that reflective rumination may be linked to maladaptive outcomes such as heightened suicide risk (Surrence, Miranda, Marroquín, & Chan, 2009). Conversely, brooding rumination has been consistently associated with depressive and anxiety symptoms (McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011). Considered together, mind-wandering and rumination are both types of task-unrelated thought that are related to a variety of maladaptive outcomes.

Present Study

The present study used a large, multi-site sample of college students to examine ADHD and SCT symptoms in relation to mind-wandering and rumination. The study had two objectives:

  1. The first objective was to evaluate ADHD and SCT symptoms in relation to multiple self-report measures of task-unrelated thought. Specifically, correlations were conducted to examine SCT, inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity in relation to mind-wandering, reflective rumination, and brooding rumination. We separated hyperactive and impulsive dimensions following inconsistent research on whether these dimensions are associated with mind-wandering (Lanier et al., 2019) and research supporting a three-factor structure of ADHD in adults (Martel, Levinson, Langer, & Nigg, 2016). We expected all four psychopathology dimensions to be significantly correlated with greater mind-wandering, reflective rumination, and brooding rumination. We hypothesized SCT and inattention to have significantly stronger correlations than hyperactivity and impulsivity with task-unrelated thought. There was not an a priori hypothesis for hyperactive and impulsive symptoms given mixed findings from past research.

  2. The second objective was to examine whether SCT and ADHD symptoms were uniquely associated with task-unrelated thought. For this objective, the task-unrelated thought measures were regressed on the SCT, ADHD inattention, ADHD hyperactivity, and ADHD impulsivity dimensions. Both anxiety and depressive symptoms were also included in these models given their established associations with task-unrelated thought (McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011). We hypothesized that SCT, ADHD inattention, depression, and anxiety would each be uniquely associated with task-unrelated thought. We did not make an a priori hypothesis regarding the ADHD hyperactive and impulsive dimensions.

Methods

Participants

Participants were 4,679 undergraduates enrolled in six universities across regions of the United States. Participants’ ages ranged from 18 to 29 years (M = 19.08, SD = 1.36) and 69.8% identified as female, 30% as male, and 0.2% as other. There were 80.9% of participants who identified as White, while the remaining identified as Black/African American (7.1%), Asian, (6.2%), Biracial/Multiracial (4.8%), American Indian/Alaska Native (0.5%), or Native Hawaiian/Other Pacific Islander (0.2%); 10% identified as Hispanic or Latino. Most of the participants reported as being in their first year of college (58.3%), while the others reported being in their second year (21.8%), third year (12.5%), or fourth year or later (7.1%) year. Thirteen percent of the participants reported clinically significant symptoms of ADHD (≥ 5 symptoms rated as ‘often’ or ‘very often’; 5.6% reported elevated inattentive symptoms, 4.7% reported elevated hyperactive-impulsive symptoms, and 2.7% reported elevated inattentive and hyperactive-impulsive symptoms). See Dvorsky et al. (2018) and Swope et al. (2019) for additional details.

Procedures

This study was approved at each of each institution’s local Institutional Review Board (IRB). Data were conducted during the 2016–2017 academic year. Participants at five of the universities provided consent and completed the survey anonymously using a Qualtrics platform. The sixth university instructed participants to select individual timeslots to complete in-person consent, followed by the online survey. Participants across all universities received course credit for following completing the study, which lasted an hour on average.

Measures

SCT symptoms.

The Adult Concentration Inventory (ACI; Becker, Burns, Garner, et al., 2018) is an adult self-report measure of SCT symptoms developed following a meta-analysis that identified optimal items distinguishing SCT from ADHD-IN and internalizing symptoms (Becker et al., 2016). Factor analyses resulted in 10 SCT items that demonstrated convergent and discriminative validity from both ADHD-IN and internalizing symptomatology (Becker, Burns, Garner, et al., 2018). Participants rated on a four-point scale (0 = not at all, 3= very often) how often each symptom (e.g., “I stare off into space”) occurred in the past six months. To eliminate potential contamination of the SCT scale with the Imaginal Process Inventory daydreaming measure of mind-wandering described below, one item (“I daydream”) was removed from the SCT scale in the present study. In the present study, the nine-item SCT scale demonstrated acceptable internal consistency (α = .87).

ADHD symptoms.

The Barkley Adult ADHD Rating Scale-IV (BAARS-IV; Barkley, 2011) was used to measure adult self-reported ADHD symptoms. The 18 items assess the inattentive and hyperactive-impulsive symptoms of ADHD in the DSM-5. Participants complete each item using a four-point scale (0 = not at all, 3 = very often). Factor analyses from Barkley’s (2011) nationally representative sample and a large sample of college students (Becker, Langberg, Luebbe, Dvorsky, & Flannery, 2014) found support for separate inattention (9 items), hyperactivity (5 items), and impulsivity (4 items) dimensions. In the present study, αs = .88, .72 and .79 for inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity, respectively.

Depressive and anxiety symptoms.

The depression and anxiety subscales of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scale-21 (DASS; Antony, Bieling, Cox, Enns, & Swinson, 1998; Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995) were used to assess adult self-reported depressive and anxiety symptoms. The DASS-21 has demonstrated strong reliability estimates in clinical and nonclinical populations (Antony et al., 1998). Participants responded to each item on the seven-item depression (e.g., “I was unable to become enthusiastic about anything”) and seven-item anxiety subscale (e.g., “I felt scared without any good reason”) in reference to the past week using a four-point scale (0 = did not apply to me at all, 3 = applied to me very much or most of the time). None of the DASS-21 items overlap with items on the rumination scale described below. In the present study, αs = .90 and .79 for depression and anxiety, respectively.

Mind-wandering.

Different scales have been used to assess mind-wandering, and two measures were included in this study to include as comprehensive a measurement of mind-wandering as possible and to allow for a comparison of findings across scales with different item content. Specifically, the newly-developed Mind Excessively Wandering Scale (MEWS; Mowlem, Skirrow, et al., 2019) and the historical Imaginal Process Inventory (IPI; Singer & Antrobus, 1966) were both used as separate measures of self-reported mind-wandering.

The MEWS was originally developed to identify the mental phenomena of mind-wandering that is present among individuals with ADHD (Mowlem, Skirrow, et al., 2019). The MEWS has demonstrated acceptable internal consistency, acceptable factor loadings, and convergent validity with other measures of mind-wandering and ADHD symptoms in clinical and non-clinical adult populations (Mowlem, Agnew-Blais, et al., 2019; Mowlem, Skirrow, et al., 2019). Participants respond to each of the 12 items (e.g., “I have difficulty controlling my thoughts”) on a four-point scale (0 = not at all or rarely, 3 = nearly all of the time or constantly). Internal consistency in the present study was good (α = .93).

The IPI daydreaming frequency subscale is frequently used as a measure of mind-wandering (Forster & Lavie, 2014; Mrazek et al., 2012). Scores on the IPI daydreaming subscale are correlated with task-unrelated thought during sustained attention tasks (Mrazek et al., 2012; Stawarczyk et al., 2012). Additionally, the daydreaming frequency subscale has demonstrated strong internal consistency and test-retest reliability (Chiorri & Vannucci, 2019; Marchetti et al., 2014). Participants responded to each of the 12 items (e.g., “In the past five days, I lost myself in active daydreaming”) on a five-point scale. In the present study, internal consistency was good (α = .95).

Rumination.

The Ruminative Response Scale (RRS; Treynor et al., 2003) was used to measure adult self-reported ruminative responses. The RRS includes three factors to assess for rumination (brooding, reflection, and depression-related). The five-item brooding scale (e.g., “When you feel down, sad, or depressed, how often do you think, why do I always react this way?”) and the five-item reflection subscale (e.g., “When you feel down, sad, or depressed, how often do you go someplace alone to think about your feelings?”) were used in the present study. Internal consistency for reflection (α = .84) and brooding (α = .87) was satisfactory.

Analytic Approach

Data quality check.

In order to ensure quality responses, an instructional manipulation check (IMC; Oppenheimer, Meyvis, & Davidenko, 2009), trap questions (e.g., “Please click on the response ‘sometimes’”), and questions assessing for participants’ reported effort were used. Participants were only able to proceed with the survey if they answered the IMC correctly, which asked the participant to select a specific answer for the question. Successful completion of trap questions were designated with a 50% accuracy or higher and effort ratings of 5 or higher on a 10 point scale (0 = not much effort, 10 = my best effort). 4,679 (94%) of the total 4,955 participants met the designated criteria. See Dvorsky et al. (2018) and Swope et al. (2019) for additional details.

Primary Analyses.

All analyses were conducted in Mplus v8.2 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2018). Latent variables were used for all constructs with each item specified to load on its respective factor. Item ratings were treated as ordered-categories. Analyses used the robust weighted least squares means and variance (WLSMV) estimator. There was little missing information (covariance coverage was approximately 99%; the WLSMV uses a pairwise approach to missing information). First, correlations were conducted to examine the psychopathology dimensions (i.e., SCT, inattention, hyperactivity, impulsivity, depression, anxiety) in relation to the task-unrelated thought domains (i.e., reflection rumination, brooding rumination, IPI mind-wandering, MEWS mind-wandering). In addition, the Mplus model constraint procedure was used to test for significant differences among factor correlations for significant differences. Second, structural regression analyses were conducted to examine the unique relations between psychopathology dimensions and task-unrelated thought. Statistical significance was set at p < .001 for all analyses given the sample size; we also focused on effect magnitude.

Results

Correlation Analyses

Descriptive statistics of the study variables are provided in Table 1. Participant characteristics (i.e., age, sex, race, year in school) were not significantly associated or were only negligibly associated with the mind-wandering or rumination variables (all rs < .10) and were therefore not considered further. The task-unrelated thought variables were significantly correlated with each other (all ps < .001). IPI mind-wandering and MEWS mind-wandering were strongly associated (r = .57). Brooding was strongly associated with reflection rumination (r = .83). Reflective and brooding rumination were strongly associated with MEWS mind-wandering (rs = .50 and .61, respectively) and moderately associated with IPI mind-wandering (rs = .37 and .34, respectively). As shown in Table 2, the psychopathology dimensions were all significantly correlated with each other (all ps < .001), with most correlations large in magnitude.

Table 1.

Descriptive Statistics of Manifest Study Variables

Variable Mean SD Range Skew Kurtosis
SCT 1.19 0.59 0 – 3 0.65 0.34
ADHD Inattention 1.68 0.54 1 – 4 1.07 1.23
ADHD Hyperactivity 1.75 0.55 1 – 4 0.88 0.62
ADHD Impulsivity 1.62 0.6 1 – 4 1.22 1.34
Depression 0.54 0.61 0 – 3 1.53 2.05
Anxiety 0.48 0.51 0 – 3 1.46 2.18
RRS Reflection 9.69 3.69 4 – 20 0.56 −0.42
RRS Brooding 10.51 3.91 4 – 20 0.49 −0.54
IPI Mind-wandering 2.76 0.91 1 – 5 0.21 −0.60
MEWS Mind-wandering 1.01 0.65 0 – 3 0.53 −0.003

Note. N = 4,679. ADHD = attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. IPI = Imaginal Process Inventory. MEWS = Mind Excessively Wandering Scale. RRS = Ruminative Response Scale. SCT = sluggish cognitive tempo. SD = standard deviation.

Table 2.

Intercorrelations among Latent Psychopathology Dimensions

1 2 3 4 5 6
1. SCT --
2. ADHD inattention .80 --
3. ADHD hyperactivity .55 .63 --
4. ADHD impulsivity .43 .54 .63 --
5. Depression .59 .63 .43 .28 --
6. Anxiety .60 .62 .60 .41 .76 --

Note. N = 4,679. ADHD = attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. SCT = sluggish cognitive tempo. All correlations significant at p < .001.

Table 3 shows the correlations of the psychopathology dimensions with task-unrelated thought variables (all ps < .001), with superscripts used to indicate significant differences between the magnitude of the bivariate correlations within each task-unrelated thought dimension. Regarding the MEWS mind-wandering scale, all six psychopathology dimensions were strongly associated with greater mind-wandering (all rs > .40), with SCT (r = .65) and inattention (r = .67) having the strongest associations with MEWS mind-wandering scores. For the IPI mind-wandering scale, SCT symptoms were most strongly associated with greater IPI mind-wandering (r = .64), followed next by inattention (r = .52), which was in turn more strongly associated with greater IPI mind-wandering than the remaining psychopathology dimensions. Finally, for both rumination dimensions, a stepwise pattern in correlation magnitude differences was found. Specifically, depressive symptoms were most strongly correlated with greater rumination (rs = .60 and .67 for reflection and brooding rumination, respectively), followed in order by anxiety, SCT and inattention (which did not differ from each other), hyperactivity, and impulsivity.

Table 3.

Correlation Analyses Examining Sluggish Cognitive Tempo, ADHD, and Internalizing Dimensions in Relation to Task-Unrelated Thought

MEWS Mind-Wandering IPI Mind-Wandering (Daydreaming) RRS Reflection RRS Brooding
Correlation SE Correlation SE Correlation SE Correlation SE
SCT 0.65a 0.01 0.64a 0.01 0.41a 0.01 0.49a 0.01
ADHD inattention 0.67a 0.01 0.52b 0.01 0.38a 0.02 0.48a 0.01
ADHD hyperactivity 0.57d,e 0.01 0.38c 0.02 0.31b 0.02 0.38b 0.02
ADHD impulsivity 0.43e 0.02 0.27d 0.02 0.18c 0.02 0.26c 0.02
Depression 0.59 c,d 0.01 0.39c 0.01 0.60d 0.01 0.67d 0.01
Anxiety 0.62 b,c 0.01 0.41c 0.01 0.52e 0.01 0.62e 0.01

Note. N = 4,679. Within each task-unrelated thought variable (column), latent correlation coefficients with different superscripts differ significantly in relation to the outcome variable at p < .001. ADHD = attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. MEWS = Mind Excessively Wandering Scale. IPI = Imaginal Process Inventory. RRS = Ruminative Response Scale. SCT = sluggish cognitive tempo. SE = standard error. All correlations significant at p < .001.

Structural Regression Analyses

Table 4 shows the unique effects (standardized partial regression coefficients) of the psychopathology dimensions in relation to the task-unrelated thought dimensions. Regarding MEWS mind-wandering, five of the six psychopathology dimensions were uniquely associated with higher mind-wandering scores, with impulsivity the only dimension not uniquely associated with mind-wandering (β = 0.03; see Table 4). The unique relative associations with MEWS mind-wandering were similar for the five significant psychopathology dimensions (βs = 0.15 – 0.22). On the other hand, SCT was the only psychopathology dimension to be uniquely and significantly associated with greater IPI mind-wandering. Whereas higher SCT symptoms were strongly associated with greater IPI mind-wandering (β = 0.59), the other psychopathology dimensions were negligibly, non-significantly associated with IPI mind-wandering (βs = −0.03 – 0.04).

Table 4.

Structural Regression Analyses Examining Sluggish Cognitive Tempo, ADHD, and Internalizing Dimensions in Relation to Task-Unrelated Thought

MEWS Mind-Wandering IPI Mind-Wandering (Daydreaming) RRS Reflection RRS Brooding
β SE β SE β SE β SE
SCT 0.22* 0.02 0.59* 0.02 0.12* 0.03 0.14* 0.03
ADHD inattention 0.21* 0.03 0.02 0.03 −0.12* 0.03 −0.07 0.03
ADHD hyperactivity 0.15* 0.02 0.03 0.03 0.05 0.03 0.02 0.03
ADHD impulsivity 0.03 0.02 −0.03 0.02 −0.03 0.02 0.01 0.02
Depression 0.15* 0.02 −0.01 0.03 0.49* 0.03 0.47* 0.03
Anxiety 0.15* 0.03 0.04 0.03 0.13* 0.03 0.20* 0.03

Note. N = 4,679. ADHD = attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. MEWS = Mind Excessively Wandering Scale. IPI = Imaginal Process Inventory. RRS = Ruminative Response Scale. SCT = sluggish cognitive tempo. SE = standard error.

*

p < .001.

In examining rumination dimensions, depression, anxiety, and SCT symptoms were each significantly (p < .001) associated with increased reflective and brooding rumination, with depression being most strongly associated with both types of rumination (βs = 0.49 and 0.47 for reflection and brooding rumination, respectively). Conversely, in the regression analyses, inattentive symptoms were no longer significantly related to brooding rumination and were significantly associated with lower reflective rumination. Finally, ADHD hyperactivity and impulsivity symptoms were no longer significantly associated with either type of rumination.1

Discussion

Recent theoretical (Bozhilova et al., 2018) and empirical (Lanier et al., 2019) research has generated an interest in ADHD and task-unrelated thought. However, no study has incorporated SCT symptoms when testing these relations, despite previous suggestions that task-unrelated thought may be a hallmark feature of SCT (Adams et al., 2010; Barkley, 2014). To add to the existing literature, the current study used a large, multi-site sample of college students and two domains of task-unrelated thought: mind-wandering (i.e., shift in thoughts away from an ongoing task) and rumination (i.e., pattern of thoughts fixed on negatively-valenced content). Results of the present study provide the first empirical support for unique and robust associations between SCT symptoms and task-unrelated thought, while suggesting that the link between ADHD and task-unrelated thought may be less robust than previously suggested.

SCT and Other Psychopathology Dimensions in Relation to Mind-wandering

When controlling for other psychopathology domains, SCT symptoms remained uniquely associated with both measures of mind-wandering. However, a differential pattern of findings emerged when comparing the strength of these effects with other psychopathologies. Similar to the bivariate results, when examining mind-wandering as assessed with the MEWS, the magnitude of associations was similar for SCT and other psychopathology dimensions, with the exception of impulsivity. Conversely, SCT symptoms were strongly associated with greater self-reported mind-wandering on the IPI whereas the other psychopathology domains were not uniquely associated with IPI-assessed mind-wandering. These findings are the first to provide empirical support for these possibilities and demonstrate that SCT symptoms, even when controlling for ADHD and internalizing symptoms domains, are uniquely associated with self-reported mind-wandering.

Findings of the present study also suggest that previous reports regarding ADHD’s association with mind-wandering may be less robust or more nuanced than previously thought. Though ADHD symptom domains were correlated with greater mind-wandering, these relations became non-significant or attenuated when controlling for SCT and internalizing symptoms. Specifically, inattentive and hyperactive symptoms remained uniquely associated with greater MEWS-assessed mind-wandering, albeit effects were smaller in magnitude, and these dimensions were not significantly associated with IPI-assessed mind-wandering in the regression analyses. The findings for inattentive and hyperactive symptoms are consistent with a previous study finding both symptom domains, but not impulsivity, to be linked with mind-wandering (Biederman et al., 2019). However, our findings also demonstrate that studies examining the association between ADHD and mind-wandering should in tandem consider SCT symptoms and incorporate multiple measures of mind-wandering.

Of note, recent studies have found SCT symptoms to be uniquely associated with greater openness to experience (Becker, Schmitt, et al., 2018) and greater anticipation of upcoming rewarding experiences (Swope et al., 2020). Interestingly, the personality trait of openness to experience has been linked to greater positive-constructive daydreaming (e.g., elaboration on positive experiences; Blouin-Hudon & Zelenski, 2016). Thus, individuals with SCT symptoms may have a tendency to be more imaginative and insightful. This possibility also parallels evidence from the mind-wandering literature that points to both maladaptive and adaptive correlates of mind-wandering (Mooneyham & Schooler, 2013; Smallwood & Andrews-Hanna, 2013).

It is important to acknowledge the diverging pattern of findings for the two different mind-wandering measures. SCT symptoms were more strongly associated than all other psychopathology dimensions with scores on the IPI measure of mind-wandering. The IPI scale focused on daydreaming frequency and, importantly, the “daydreaming” item was removed from the SCT scale before conducting analyses, suggesting this finding is not due to item overlap. In contrast, using the MEWS, the magnitude of associations between SCT and mind-wandering was similar to the associations of most other psychopathology dimensions with mind-wandering. Whereas the IPI mind-wandering scale is specific to daydreaming frequency, the MEWS includes more wide-ranging content, including difficulties with focus (e.g., “I can only focus my thoughts on one thing at a time with considerable effort”), mental organization (e.g., “My thoughts are disorganized and ‘all over the place’), attentional control (e.g., “I have difficulty slowing my thoughts down and focusing on one thing at a time”). Thus, the MEWS item content may be a non-specific measure of mind-wandering in relation to psychopathology domains. Importantly, whereas IPI mind-wandering scale scores are significantly correlated with laboratory-measured mind-wandering using the sustained attention to response task (SART; Stawarczyk et al., 2012), we are unaware of any studies examining the MEWS in relation to SART-assessed mind-wandering. Thus, there is less evidence for the validity of the MEWS as a measure of mind-wandering, and this is an especially important area for research given the increasing interest in the intersection between mind-wandering and psychopathology. It will be important for future research to further examine these two measures of mind-wandering, for example by examining these scales in relation to deliberate or spontaneous mind-wandering (Seli, Carriere, & Smilek, 2014) or using person centered analyses (e.g., latent profile analysis) to identify patterns of mind-wandering within individuals.

SCT and Other Psychopathology Dimensions in Relation to Rumination

Only one previous study (Becker et al., 2019) has documented significant correlations between SCT symptoms and ruminative responses. In the present study, SCT symptoms were uniquely associated with higher reflective and brooding rumination as hypothesized, though smaller in magnitude compared to depressive symptoms. In contrast, ADHD inattentive, hyperactive, and impulsive symptoms were negligibly or even negatively associated with rumination in the structural regression analyses. Although inattentive symptoms have been previously linked with ruminative responses (Jonkman et al., 2017), findings from the present study replicate the Becker et al. (2019) study in suggesting that this may be due in large part to the co-occurrence of inattention with internalizing and SCT symptoms, given the change in results from bivariate to multivariate analyses.

An interesting finding was that inattentive symptoms became associated with less reflective rumination and were unrelated to brooding rumination in the regression analyses, whereas SCT symptoms remained uniquely positively associated with both types of rumination. These findings provide some evidence of recent proposals that excessive internal distractibility may be a feature that distinguishes SCT from ADHD inattentive symptoms (Becker, Burns, Leopold, et al., 2018). Rumination has been theorized to be one mechanism explaining the relation between SCT and internalizing psychopathology (Becker & Willcutt, 2019). Given that brooding, and to a lesser extent reflective rumination, has been identified as a key feature of internalizing psychopathology and suicidal behavior (Hsu et al., 2015; Surrence et al., 2009), rumination may be the mechanism linking SCT with these outcomes. An important area of future research will be to investigate whether rumination mediates the association between SCT symptoms and internalizing psychopathology and other negative outcomes (e.g., social withdrawal, suicidal behavior).

Limitations and Future Directions

Strengths of the current study include a large sample size recruited from six universities and examination of both reflective and brooding rumination as well as multiple self-report measures of mind-wandering. Despite these strengths, several limitations are worth noting. The cross-sectional nature of the study precludes drawing causal interferences. Future research should use experimental and longitudinal designs to investigate the relation between SCT symptoms and task-unrelated thought over time. Longitudinal studies would also be well-positioned to examine whether task-unrelated thought explains the relations between SCT and internalizing psychopathologies and functional impairment. Moreover, future research could then test whether the frequency of mind-wandering exacerbates relations between psychopathology symptoms and areas of impairment. In addition, although we included two self-report measures of mind-wandering, laboratory-assessed mind-wandering during the SART or similar paradigms was not possible given the constraints of the study and will be important to include in future research (Christoff et al., 2016). It will also be important to examine whether SCT symptoms are associated with default mode network activity, a set of brain regions including the medial temporal lobe, that are consistently active during rest and are implicated in mind-wandering (Christoff et al., 2016). Additionally, the relatively homogenous sample prevents generalizing findings to varying age groups and clinical populations. For instance, it is possible that relations between ADHD and task-unrelated thought would be stronger in a clinical sample of adults with ADHD. Similar to the broader research on mind-wandering (Lanier et al., 2019), research would benefit from testing relations of SCT with task-unrelated thought in younger samples as SCT symptoms tends to increase following childhood (Leopold et al., 2016). Finally, recent research has identified a differential pattern of outcomes among distinct valences of rumination (e.g., sadness, anger; Harmon, Stephens, Repper, Driscoll, & Kistner, 2019). Future research should consider testing whether these unique valences of rumination are differentially associated with SCT and other psychopathologies.

Conclusion

Findings from the present study are the first to demonstrate that SCT symptoms are uniquely associated with measures of task-unrelated thought while controlling for ADHD and internalizing symptom domains. Specifically, SCT symptoms were uniquely associated with greater mind-wandering, reflective rumination, and brooding rumination. Additionally, SCT symptoms were more strongly associated than other psychopathology dimensions with mind-wandering as assessed by a measure of daydreaming frequency. These findings suggest that task-unrelated thought may be a key feature of the SCT construct and point to the importance of additional studies examining SCT in relation to mind-wandering and rumination.

Highlights.

  • No study has examined sluggish cognitive tempo (SCT) and mind-wandering.

  • SCT symptoms were uniquely associated with mind-wandering and rumination.

  • ADHD symptoms were less clearly associated with mind-wandering or rumination.

  • Important to include SCT in studies of task-unrelated thought and psychopathology.

  • Studies using experimental thought-probe tasks and longitudinal designs are needed.

Acknowledgement:

Stephen Becker is currently supported by award K23MH108603 from the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health (NIH).

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

1

Analyses were re-run using a combined hyperactivity-impulsivity factor as opposed to separate hyperactivity and impulsivity factors. The overall pattern of findings was unchanged from those reported in Table 4. The only exception is that when a combined hyperactivity-impulsivity factor was used, ADHD inattention was significantly negatively associated with brooding rumination (β = −.07, SE = .02, p = .02). The combined hyperactivity-impulsivity factor was not significantly associated with IPI mind-wandering, reflection ruination, or brooding rumination (ps > .05) but was significantly associated with higher MEWS mind-wandering scores (β = .15, SE = .02, p < .001).

Declarations of Interest: None.

References

  1. Adams ZW, Milich R, & Fillmore MT (2010). A case for the return of attention-deficit disorder in DSM-5. The ADHD report, 18(3), 1–6. [Google Scholar]
  2. Antony MM, Bieling PJ, Cox BJ, Enns MW, & Swinson RP (1998). Psychometric properties of the 42-item and 21-item versions of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales in clinical groups and a community sample. Psychological Assessment, 10(2), 176–181. [Google Scholar]
  3. Barkley RA (2011). Barkley Adult ADHD Rating Scale-IV (BAARS-IV): Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  4. Becker SP, Burns GL, Garner AA, Jarrett MA, Luebbe AM, Epstein JN, & Willcutt EG (2018). Sluggish cognitive tempo in adults: Psychometric validation of the Adult Concentration Inventory. Psychological Assessment, 30(3), 296–310. doi: 10.1037/pas0000476 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Becker SP, Burns GL, Leopold DR, Olson RK, & Willcutt EG (2018). Differential impact of trait sluggish cognitive tempo and ADHD inattention in early childhood on adolescent functioning. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 59(10), 1094–1104. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12946 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Becker SP, Burns GL, Smith ZR, & Langberg JM (2019). Sluggish cognitive tempo in adolescents with and without ADHD: Differentiation from adolescent-reported ADHD inattention and unique associations with internalizing domains. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 1–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Becker SP, Langberg JM, Luebbe AM, Dvorsky MR, & Flannery AJ (2014). Sluggish Cognitive Tempo is Associated With Academic Functioning and Internalizing Symptoms in College Students With and Without Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 70(4), 388–403. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Becker SP, Leopold DR, Burns GL, Jarrett MA, Langberg JM, Marshall SA, … Willcutt EG (2016). The Internal, External, and Diagnostic Validity of Sluggish Cognitive Tempo: A Meta-Analysis and Critical Review. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 55(3), 163–178. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2015.12.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Becker SP, Schmitt AP, Jarrett MA, Luebbe AM, Garner AA, Epstein JN, & Burns GL (2018). Sluggish cognitive tempo and personality: Links to BIS/BAS sensitivity and the five factor model. Journal of Research in Personality, 75, 103–112. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2018.06.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Becker SP, & Willcutt EG (2019). Advancing the study of sluggish cognitive tempo via DSM, RDoC, and hierarchical models of psychopathology. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(5), 603–613. doi: 10.1007/s00787-018-1136-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Biederman J, Lanier J, DiSalvo M, Noyes E, Fried R, Woodworth KY, … Faraone SV (2019). Clinical correlates of mind wandering in adults with ADHD. J Psychiatr Res, 117, 15–23. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2019.06.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Blouin-Hudon E-MC, & Zelenski JM (2016). The daydreamer: Exploring the personality underpinnings of daydreaming styles and their implications for well-being. Consciousness and Cognition, 44, 114–129. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.07.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bozhilova NS, Michelini G, Kuntsi J, & Asherson P (2018). Mind wandering perspective on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 92, 464–476. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.07.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chiorri C, & Vannucci M (2019). Replicability of the Psychometric Properties of Trait-Levels Measures of Spontaneous and Deliberate Mind Wandering. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 35(4), 459–468. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000422 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Christoff K, Irving ZC, Fox KCR, Spreng RN, & Andrews-Hanna JR (2016). Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: a dynamic framework. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(11), 718–731. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.113 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fenollar Cortés J, Servera M, Becker SP, & Burns GL (2017). External Validity of ADHD Inattention and Sluggish Cognitive Tempo Dimensions in Spanish Children With ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 21(8), 655–666. doi: 10.1177/1087054714548033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Forster S, & Lavie N (2014). Distracted by your mind? Individual differences in distractibility predict mind wandering. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(1), 251–260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Franklin MS, Mrazek MD, Anderson CL, Johnston C, Smallwood J, Kingstone A, & Schooler JW (2017). Tracking Distraction. Journal of Attention Disorders, 21(6), 475–486. doi: 10.1177/1087054714543494 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Harmon SL, Stephens HF, Repper KK, Driscoll KA, & Kistner JA (2019). Children’s rumination to sadness and anger: Implications for the development of depression and aggression. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 48(4), 622–632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Hsu KJ, Beard C, Rifkin L, Dillon DG, Pizzagalli DA, & Björgvinsson T (2015). Transdiagnostic mechanisms in depression and anxiety: The role of rumination and attentional control. Journal of Affective Disorders, 188, 22–27. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.08.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Jonkman LM, Markus CR, Franklin MS, & Van Dalfsen JH (2017). Mind wandering during attention performance: Effects of ADHD-inattention symptomatology, negative mood, ruminative response style and working memory capacity. PLOS ONE, 12(7), e0181213. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0181213 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kamradt JM, Momany AM, & Nikolas MA (2018). Sluggish Cognitive Tempo Symptoms Contribute to Heterogeneity in Adult Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 40(2), 206–223. doi: 10.1007/s10862-017-9631-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Lanier J, Noyes E, & Biederman J (2019). Mind Wandering (Internal Distractibility) in ADHD: A Literature Review. Journal of Attention Disorders, 108705471986578. doi: 10.1177/1087054719865781 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Leopold DR, Christopher ME, Burns GL, Becker SP, Olson RK, & Willcutt EG (2016). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and sluggish cognitive tempo throughout childhood: temporal invariance and stability from preschool through ninth grade. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(9), 1066–1074. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12505 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Lovibond PF, & Lovibond SH (1995). The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(3), 335–343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Marchetti I, Koster EHW, Klinger E, & Alloy LB (2016). Spontaneous Thought and Vulnerability to Mood Disorders. Clinical Psychological Science, 4(5), 835–857. doi: 10.1177/2167702615622383 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Marchetti I, Van De Putte E, & Koster EHW (2014). Self-generated thoughts and depression: from daydreaming to depressive symptoms. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 131. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00131 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Martel MM, Levinson CA, Langer JK, & Nigg JT (2016). A Network Analysis of Developmental Change in ADHD Symptom Structure From Preschool to Adulthood. Clinical Psychological Science, 4(6), 988–1001. doi: 10.1177/2167702615618664 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. McLaughlin KA, & Nolen-Hoeksema S (2011). Rumination as a transdiagnostic factor in depression and anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49(3), 186–193. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.12.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. McMillan RL, Kaufman SB, & Singer JL (2013). Ode to positive constructive daydreaming. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 626. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00626 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Mooneyham BW, & Schooler JW (2013). The costs and benefits of mind-wandering: A review. 67(1), 11–18. doi: 10.1037/a0031569 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Mowlem FD, Agnew-Blais J, Pingault J-B, & Asherson P (2019). Evaluating a scale of excessive mind wandering among males and females with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder from a population sample. Scientific Reports, 9(1). doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-39227-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Mowlem FD, Skirrow C, Reid P, Maltezos S, Nijjar SK, Merwood A, … Asherson P (2019). Validation of the Mind Excessively Wandering Scale and the Relationship of Mind Wandering to Impairment in Adult ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 23(6), 624–634. doi: 10.1177/1087054716651927 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Mrazek MD, Smallwood J, & Schooler JW (2012). Mindfulness and mind-wandering: finding convergence through opposing constructs. Emotion, 12(3), 442–448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Muthén L, & Muthén B (1998–2018). BO 1998–2018. Mplus User’s Guide. [Google Scholar]
  36. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Wisco BE, & Lyubomirsky S (2008). Rethinking Rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(5), 400–424. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Oppenheimer DM, Meyvis T, & Davidenko N (2009). Instructional manipulation checks: Detecting satisficing to increase statistical power. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(4), 867–872. [Google Scholar]
  38. Seli P, Carriere JSA, & Smilek D (2015). Not all mind wandering is created equal: Dissociating deliberate from spontaneous mind wandering. Psychological Research, 79, 750–758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Seli P, Smallwood J, Cheyne JA, & Smilek D (2015). On the relation of mind wandering and ADHD symptomatology. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 629–636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Singer JL, & Antrobus JS (1966). Imaginal process inventory. Center for Research in Cognition and Affect. CUNY copyright. [Google Scholar]
  41. Smallwood J, & Andrews-Hanna J (2013). Not all minds that wander are lost: the importance of a balanced perspective on the mind-wandering state. Frontiers in psychology, 4, 441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Stawarczyk D, Majerus S, Van Der Linden M, & D’Argembeau A (2012). Using the Daydreaming Frequency Scale to Investigate the Relationships between Mind-Wandering, Psychological Well-Being, and Present-Moment Awareness. Frontiers in Psychology, 3. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00363 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Surrence K, Miranda R, Marroquín BM, & Chan S (2009). Brooding and reflective rumination among suicide attempters: Cognitive vulnerability to suicidal ideation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 803–808. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.06.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Swope AK, Fredrick JW, Becker SP, Burns GL, Garner AA, Jarrett MA, … Luebbe AM (2020). Sluggish cognitive tempo and positive valence systems: Unique relations with greater reward valuation but less willingness to work. Journal of Affective Disorders, 261, 131–138. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.10.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Treynor W, Gonzalez R, & Nolen-Hoeksema S (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive therapy and research, 27(3), 247–259. [Google Scholar]
  46. Van Den Driessche C, Bastian M, Peyre H, Stordeur C, Acquaviva É, Bahadori S, … Sackur J (2017). Attentional Lapses in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Blank Rather Than Wandering Thoughts. Psychological Science, 28, 1375–1386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Whitmer AJ, & Gotlib IH (2013). An attentional scope model of rumination. Psychological Bulletin, 139(5), 1036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Yeguez CE, Hill RM, Buitron V, & Pettit JW (2018). Stress Accounts for the Association Between ADHD Symptoms and Suicide Ideation When Stress-Reactive Rumination Is High. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 42(4), 461–467. doi: 10.1007/s10608-018-9910-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES